stress and well being

91
Stress and well-being 1 Stress and Well-Being at Work Mark A. Griffin University of Sheffield Sharon Clarke University of Manchester To appear in S. Zedeck (Ed.). Handbook of Industrial/Organizational Psychology (Volume 3). Washington DC: American Psychological Association August 2009

Upload: joseph-fkk

Post on 01-Nov-2014

50 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 1

Stress and Well-Being at Work

Mark A. Griffin

University of Sheffield

Sharon Clarke

University of Manchester

To appear in S. Zedeck (Ed.). Handbook of Industrial/Organizational Psychology (Volume 3).

Washington DC: American Psychological Association

August 2009

Page 2: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 2

STRESS AND WELL-BEING AT WORK

INTRODUCTION

Stress is the single most common reason given for absence from work in the UK, and one in

six Americans report they are ‘extremely’ stressed (HSE, 2008). However, the general notion of

stress at work can be contentious, and the possibility that work causes illness has been received with

varying degrees of skepticism, indifference, and alarm across sectors of modern society. Regulators,

employers, unions, insurers, and health professionals continue to struggle with the meaning and

management of stress in the workplace.

Although there is much diversity across concepts of work stress, a considerable body of

stress-related theory and practice has developed over the past 40 years. Researchers from

psychology, economics, sociology, public health, engineering and medicine represent just some of

the major disciplines that have addressed the nature and consequences of work stress (Ganster &

Schaubroeck, 1991). Within the field of psychology, Beehr and Franz (1987) identified medical,

clinical psychology, engineering psychology, and organizational psychology approaches. This

chapter focuses on research from the field of organizational psychology but draws from multiple

areas where appropriate.

Work stress is not a single event or a specific psychological state. Rather, work stress

describes a general process in which individuals respond to and manage demands to meet multiple

goals over time. A basic distinction between stressors (e.g., excessive workloads) as the primary

drivers of this process, and strains (e.g., anxiety and depression) as its primary outcomes, has

proved useful building a more complete picture of the stress process (e.g., Caplan, Cobb, French,

Harrison, & Pinneau, 1980; Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, & Rosenthal, 1964; Lazarus, 1966;

McGrath, 1976). Stressors describe the demands experienced by individuals and strains capture a

Page 3: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 3

range of negative outcomes for health and well-being. Nevertheless, most popular writing and many

research studies refer to ‘stress’ as if it was a single outcome, typically as a shorthand for the strain

experience of ‘feeling stressed’.

All theories of stress must grapple with the common observation that although some events

are intrinsically stressful (e.g., experiencing a violent event at work), individuals respond to

stressful events in different ways. At the extreme, the same work environment can be debilitating

and negative for one individual while at the same time it is exciting and challenging for another.

The distinction between stressors and strains has proved useful for addressing this core problem

(Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). A stressor is the perceived demand from the environment and therefore

comprises both external stimuli and the perceptual processes of the individual. Strains are the

psychological, behavioral, and physiological outcomes of this process and include such diverse

responses as anxiety, absenteeism, and illness. Overall, the stressor-strain distinction captures the

elements of a negative transaction between the individual and the environment.

The stressor-strain distinction has proved less useful for articulating how positive and

negative experiences interact in the stress process or for elaborating the way the stress process

unfolds over time. Recent approaches to work stress draw on more fully developed theories of

affect (e.g., Watson, Wiese, Vaidya, & Tellegen, 1999) and give greater attention to systems of

positive psychology (e.g., Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) to build better elaborated models of

well-being and the stress process (Folkman, 2008). Time spans have always been important

considerations in models of stress, but most empirical studies have used cross-sectional designs

(Zapf, Dormann, & Frese, 1996). Recently more attention has been paid to research designs that

capture the unfolding transaction between individuals and their work context. For example, models

of recovery incorporate a cyclical process of resource depletion and repair that unfolds from day to

Page 4: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 4

day and week to week (e.g., Sonnentag & Zijlstra, 2006). Importantly, just as work can have

negative consequences for the health and well-being of individuals, so too can being out of work.

Whilst unemployment may be viewed as a challenge by some people, depending on their personal

agency, it can also be experienced as an extremely stressful experience for others, especially in the

long-term (Fryer, 1986). The stress process associated with unemployment will not be considered

further in this chapter, which focuses on stress related to work (for a review see Fryer, 1998).

The remainder of this chapter is divided into four sections. First, we review seven different

approaches to the stress process, highlighting influential research and theory over the past forty

years. Second, we introduce an integrative framework as a basis for discussing similarities and

differences across the major approaches. The integrated framework also provides a basis for

reviewing the role of time in the stress process and considering implications for different

methodological approaches and research paradigms. Third, we review organizational interventions

to reduce stress and fourth, we conclude with a summary and review some future directions for the

study of the stress process. Excluded from this review are specific sources of work-related stress,

such as international assignments and home-work balance, which are covered elsewhere in this

Handbook (see also Vol. 3, Chapter 23, by Leung and Peterson, on "Managing a Globally

Distributed Workforce: Social and Interpersonal Issues" and Vol. 3, Chapter 14, by Hammer and

Zimmerman, on "Quality of Work Life.")

MAJOR APPROACHES TO THE STRESS PROCESS

Below we review seven different theoretical approaches that have been influential in

shaping understanding of the stress process at work. This coverage is not exhaustive but the

approaches provide an overview of the different issues addressed in the study of stress and illustrate

the different elements of the stress process at work that have been articulated to date.

Page 5: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 5

The socio-cognitive model

The socio-cognitive or transactional model of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) emphasizes

the ongoing interaction between the person and the environment. As such, stress is not located in

the person or the environment, but in the relationship between the environment, individuals’

appraisals of the environment, and ongoing attempts to cope with issues that arise (Cooper, Dewe,

& O'Driscoll, 2001). The model describes two stages of cognitive appraisal. First, primary

appraisal involves appraisal of potential stressors as threatening and posing some kind of threat to

the individual. Then, secondary appraisal involves the evaluation of coping resources and

alternative responses. If an individual perceives that a situation is threatening, but that he or she has

the ability to cope with it, then strain is not experienced. Indeed, the situation may be perceived as

challenging. Coping has been defined as “the thoughts and behaviors used to manage the external

and internal demands of situations that are appraised as stressful” and also shapes emotional

responses (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2004, p. 745). A distinction is commonly made between

problem focused coping, which seeks to solve the demands of a stressor, and emotion focused

coping, which helps the individual to feel better about the stressor (Cooper et al., 2001). Strain

arises when an individual appraises the demands of a particular situation as about to exceed

available resources and, therefore, to threaten their well-being, necessitating a change in individual

functioning to restore the imbalance (Lazarus, 1966). The model suggests that the relationship

between the environment and person is ongoing and reciprocal, as it is the interactions between the

two that determine strain. Interactive effects have been researched extensively and these are

discussed in more detail below in the section titled ‘Longer-term dynamics of the stress process’.

Stable individual differences, such as personality, and within-person fluctuations in mood

can affect both the appraisal of stressors as threatening and the appraisal of the individual’s ability

Page 6: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 6

to cope. For example, moods have been shown to mediate the link between daily stressors and

same-day job performance (Stewart & Barling, 1996), where subjective perceptions of stress

influence mood which in turn impacts on job performance. This study found that the indirect

influence of subjective stress on job performance (mediated by negative mood, B = -.38) was

considerably stronger than the direct effect of subjective stress on job performance (B = -.21).

Relatively stable personal factors, such as self-esteem, can also influence individuals’ appraisals.

Individuals with high self-esteem will be less likely to view a potential stressor as threatening and

more likely to view themselves as able to cope (Rector & Roger, 1996).

The demands-control model

The demands-control (DC) model (Karasek, 1979, 1989; Karasek & Theorell, 1990)

proposes that job demands and job control play the key role in the stressor-strain relationship. Job

demands refer to aspects of the job which require additional or sustained physical, psychological, or

emotional effort (de Jonge & Dormann, 2003); job control, or decision latitude, refers to the degree

of control over decisions concerning the job (Karasek & Theorell, 1990). The model predicts that

excessive job demands increase strain, while high job control mitigates these adverse effects.

Together, these effects constitute the ‘strain hypothesis’. Those in active jobs with high demands

and high control should be able to minimize strain by actively managing job demands. The iso-

strain model (Johnson, Hall, & Theorell, 1989) added the role of support, proposing that social

support also buffers the effects of job demands on strain. Iso-strain occurs in jobs characterized by

high demands, low control and low support. Job control and social support are viewed as resources

because both are aspects of the job which can lead to buffering job demands and related efforts

(Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004). Both the strain and the iso-strain hypotheses have received substantial

Page 7: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 7

support in the literature (van der Doef & Maes, 1999; de Lange, Taris, Kompier, Houtman, &

Bongers, 2003).

The DC model assumes that those in active jobs will take advantage of the high level of

control to actively manage high demands. However, there is research evidence to suggest that there

are individual differences in the way that people react in these situations, such that personal

characteristics moderate the demands-control relationship. Salanova, Peiró, and Schaufeli (2002)

found that for those with high self-efficacy, job control buffered the effect of job demands on strain,

but for low self-efficacy individuals, job control acted as an additional stressor, leading to increased

strain (R2 = .02) Thus, for some individuals high job control can exacerbate, rather than buffer, job

stress in demanding jobs. Parker and Sprigg (1999) found that for those with a highly proactive

personality in a high-control job, as demands increased, job strain decreased (R2 = .02). However,

for passive employees, there was no demands-control interaction, indicating that strain increased

with demands, regardless of the level of control. Meier, Semmer, Elfering, and Jacobshagen (2008)

found for those with an internal locus of control, the predictions of the DC model were supported

(R2 = .07). However, for those with an external locus of control, high demands had a more negative

effect on strain under conditions of high control (rather than low control). Given the difficulty of

finding a substantial three-way interaction in field studies, an additional contribution of up 3% can

be considered of practical significance. Thus, relatively stable personality characteristics have a

significant influence on the impact of job demands and job control on strain, particularly locus of

control.

The role stress

Roles describe the behavioral patterns and expectations of individuals in complex systems

(Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1991), and the process through which work roles create the experience of

model

Page 8: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 8

stress was one of the earliest and most fruitful approaches to work stress (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992).

Kahn et al. (1964) reported the first of a series of studies based at the Institute of Social Research

(ISR) and exploring the nature of role conflict and role ambiguity in the stress process. Role conflict

describes two or more sets of incompatible work demands, whereas role ambiguity describes a lack

of specificity or predictability in role functions and responsibilities (Beehr & Newman, 1978; Kahn

et al., 1964; Katz & Kahn, 1978). Role overload is the third most common form of role stress and is

sometimes viewed as a particular form of role conflict. Role overload is a function of too much

work, time pressures, and a lack of resources to meet commitments and responsibilities (Beehr &

Glazer, 2005). Role conflict and role ambiguity are probably the two most frequently studied

stressors in organizational life (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). There is substantial support for the role

stress model, with large effect sizes (.43 < r < .48) reported for the effect of role stressors on

measures of affective strain (Jackson & Schuler, 1985; Ortqvist & Wincent, 2006; Sauter, Murphy,

& Hurrell, 1990).

The cybernetic model

Cybernetic models define goals and feedback loops as the central feature of the stress

process (Cummings & Cooper, 1979). Through a feedback cycle, individuals monitor the

discrepancy between a preferred or reference state and the actual work conditions as they are

perceived. The perceived discrepancy is a source of strain for the individual and motivates action to

reduce the discrepancy by changing or adapting to the environment in some way. For example, a

discrepancy between perceived and preferred levels of workload leads to an attempt to reduce the

level of workload or to adapt to this level of load. This coping action is then followed by a new

comparison of the discrepancy between preferred and actual states (Edwards, 1992).

Page 9: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 9

The feedback cycle assumes that individual behavior is directed toward a steady state of

balance between the individual and the environment. This drive towards homeostasis creates strain

when homeostasis is disrupted, and it motivates the need for engaging in coping activities (Miller,

1965). Edwards (1992) noted that feedback cycles are part of most theories of stress including

transactional and role stress models. However, he argued that the underlying cybernetic principles

had been given insufficient attention in these models of the stress process. The proposed benefits of

the cybernetic approach include a more systematic definition of the components of the feedback

cycle that can be used to identify and explain a wide range of stressors, strains, and coping

responses.

The cybernetic feedback cycle is hierarchically organized so short-term cycles (e.g., a

disruption to work load) are embedded within longer-term cycles (e.g., achieving life goals). For

each cycle, perceived and actual standards provide a comparison point for determining action

(Carver & Scheier, 1990). The source of preferred states or reference criterion derives from

individual factors such as values and personality (Cummings & Cooper, 1998). Both positive and

negative affect can be elicited by the feedback cycle. The cybernetic model often reflects a highly

dynamic situation as individuals are faced with allocating limited resources in terms of time and

attention to managing multiple goals over time. Schmidt and DeShon (2007) found that resource

allocation is affected by both progress towards goals and the consequences of failing to reach goals,

with avoidance goals (avoiding losses) attracting greater resources in the short term. However, in

the longer term, this may be damaging to health and well-being. Carver and Scheier (1990)

conceptualized a second feedback system that played a meta-monitoring function in the evaluation

of goal progress. Affect is proposed to arise as a function of the speed of progress toward goals.

From this perspective, homeostasis is maintained by the hierarchically nested negative feedback

Page 10: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 10

loop while affect arises from the meta-monitoring of the speed of progress through feedback loops

toward goal attainment.

Although not as well-researched as the previous stress models, the major tenets of the

cybernetic model have received support. The primary prediction of the cybernetic model is that the

discrepancy between a preferred or desired state and actual work conditions will affect strain; as the

discrepancy increases, so too will the adverse effects on well-being. Elsass and Veiga (1997)

operationalized this process by examining the effect of desired goals (in this case, desired control)

over and above the effect of job characteristics (in this case, actual control); they found that desired

control accounted for significant additional variance in job strain, controlling for both job autonomy

(R2 = .04) and desired participation (R2 = .03). Stronger support was demonstrated where desired

states were operationalized as personal goals (ter Doest, Maes, Gebhardt, & Koelewijn, 2006); with

personal goal facilitation accounting for significant additional variance in four measures of job

strain (job satisfaction, personal accomplishment, emotional exhaustion and psychological

symptoms), controlling for demographics and job characteristics (.05 < R2 < .07).

The challenge-hindrance model

LePine, Podsakoff, and LePine (2005) proposed the challenge-hindrance framework, based

on Lazarus’ socio-cognitive model, to account for the inconsistent evidence of the relationship

between stressors and performance. Within this framework, stressors may be appraised as either

challenges or hindrances. When stressors are appraised as challenging, positive emotions are

evoked and active coping strategies, such as problem-solving, are engaged. Challenge stressors

include job and role demands, pressure, time urgency, and workload. Drawing on expectancy

theory, LePine et al. (2005) suggest that challenge stressors are associated with high motivation

and, therefore, lead to better performance because individuals are likely to believe that there is a

Page 11: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 11

positive relationship between effort and expectancy. When stressors are appraised as threatening,

negative emotions ensue and a passive or emotion-focused style of coping is used. These are

described as ‘hindrance stressors’ and include constraints, hassles, role ambiguity, role and

interpersonal conflict, role overload, supervisor-related stress, and organizational politics. These

stressors are not motivating because effort expended to cope with them is unlikely to be successful

or bring valued rewards.

The model predicts that hindrance and challenge stressors are both subject to the same

psychological process (primary appraisal) and both will result in strain. Indeed, LePine et al.

(2005) found that there is some degree of overlap between these types of stressor (r = .33), perhaps

due to the similarity in some of the stressors, e.g., role overload (hindrance) and role demands

(challenge). However, whilst both types of stressor are significantly related to strain (hindrance, β

= .50; challenge, β = .23), other outcomes will differ, as challenge stressors are associated with

positive emotions and attitudes (LePine et al, 2005). Research has supported the distinction between

these two types of stressors, and their differential relationship with job satisfaction (Cavanaugh,

Boswell, Roehling, & Boudreau, 2000). Podsakoff, LePine, and LePine (2007) showed strong

support for the differential relationships between hindrance stressors and job satisfaction (β = -.57)

compared to challenge stressors and job satisfaction (β = -.02). A similar differential relationship

has also been demonstrated with role-based performance (Wallace, Edwards, Arnold, Frazier, &

Finch, 2009). This framework de-emphasizes the need for individual difference explanations as it

assumes a degree of consistency in the perception of stressors, i.e., some will be consistently

appraised as challenging, whilst others will be consistently appraised as hindrances. However,

despite some supporting evidence (LePine et al, 2005; Podsakoff et al, 2007; Wallace et al, 2009),

this model is inconsistent with a substantial body of research that has emphasized the moderating

Page 12: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 12

effect of personality, and other individual difference variables, on how individuals appraise

stressors (see section titled “Individual differences” below).

The conservation of resources model

The Conservation of Resources (COR) approach to stress proposes that people

fundamentally seek to obtain, retain, protect, and restore resources. Resources describe a wide range

of objects (e.g., shelter), personal characteristics (e.g., self-esteem), conditions (e.g., status), or

energies (e.g., knowledge) that are important for adaptive functioning (Hobfoll, 1989). Resources

are valued in their own right or because they lead to other valued resources. Strain results from the

threat of resource loss, the actual loss of resources, or failure to gain sufficient resources.

Individuals also strive to develop surplus resources, which are the source of higher levels of well-

being. Stressors are generally perceived to be negative because they deplete resources. This was

supported by Lee & Ashforth's (1996) meta-analysis which found that five of eight work demands

were strongly related to affective strain (r >.50).

COR proposes that the main motivational process in stress is preventing the loss of

resources. Losing resources creates strains, and coping responses are enacted with the goal of

protecting and restoring resources. COR has most frequently been studied in relation to burnout (see

next section), and Lee and Ashforth (1996) found support for the basic propositions of COR theory

in their meta-analysis of burnout correlates.

The burnout model

The term “burnout” has received substantial attention as a distinct dysfunctional outcome of

the stress process for individuals. Burnout research focuses on strain outcomes of the stress process,

in particular the more extreme forms of strain with long term negative consequences for individuals.

Maslach and Jackson (1981) defined three dimensions of burnout: emotional exhaustion,

Page 13: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 13

depersonalization, and diminished personal accomplishment. Emotional exhaustion describes

feelings of being emotionally overextended, depersonalization refers to cynical and detached

responses to others, and reduced personal accomplishment refers to a decline in efficacy and

feelings of competence and productivity (Maslach, 1998).

Burnout research initially focused on employees working in caregiving and people oriented

roles where high demands arose from working with clients although the approach is widely adopted

in other settings (Cooper et al., 2001). Lee and Ashforth (1996) argued that service providers suffer

strain when their emotional resources are depleted to such an extent that they are no longer able to

meet the demands of interpersonal stressors. Their results supported the link between burnout and a

wide range of individual, job, and organizational stressors.

The three dimensions of burnout are part of a theoretical process whereby exhaustion leads

to actions that distance the individuals from elements of their work that are stressful, resulting in

cynicism and depersonalization. Over time, these actions and withdrawal result in decreased

experiences of efficacy. The causal sequence among dimensions has been controversial and

alternative sequences have been proposed (Golembiewski, Munzenrider, & Carter, 1983), with

some suggesting that personal accomplishment develops somewhat independently of the other two

dimensions (Leiter, 1993). This is supported by meta-analytic findings (Lee & Ashforth, 1996) that

exhaustion and depersonalization are more closely related (ρ = .64) than personal accomplishment

with exhaustion (ρ = -.33) or with depersonalization (ρ = -.36). Exhaustion is the most widely

reported and studied dimension of burnout (Maslach & Leiter, 2008). Lee and Ashforth's (1996)

meta-analysis supports the proposition that exhaustion (mean ρ = .44) and depersonalization (mean

ρ = .33) are more strongly related to job stressors than diminished personal accomplishment (mean

ρ = .10). However, Halbesleben (2006) did not find that social support was a stronger predictor of

Page 14: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 14

exhaustion as proposed in burnout models, although it acted as a moderator. Evidence linking

burnout to individual difference variables is inconsistent, with the exception of an association

between neuroticism and burnout. Personality traits were found to account for significant

additional variance over and above job stressors (R2 = .12), but only neuroticism was a significant

predictor of burnout (β = .32) with those who are high in neuroticism more likely to develop the

symptoms of burnout in response to stressors (Zellars, Perrewe, & Hochwarter, 2000).

INTEGRATING FRAMEWORK

There is much overlap in the issues and constructs addressed by different approaches to

stress, and all of the models presented above have some features in common. Overall, there are few

examples where different theoretical approaches result in different predictions (although see

Edwards, 1992 for examples). Rather, the complexity of the stress process means that different

approaches tend to emphasize some features of the process and place less emphasis on others. For

example, role theories of stress emphasize the demands imposed by the environment, transactional

theories emphasize the appraisal of demands, and burnout theories emphasize the responses and

consequences of demands. Although there is substantial support for components of these stress

models, there is also evidence that focusing on a limited number of stressors (such as job demands

and job control) fails to adequately reflect the complexity of the stress-strain relationship (van

Veldhoven, Taris, de Jonge, & Broersen, 2005).

To provide a comprehensive comparison and review of stress models we present an

integrating framework in Figure 1 that summarizes the ongoing transaction between the person and

the situation. The goal of the overall framework is to provide a broad picture of the relationships

between more complex sub-components or facets. For example, overall models such as Beehr

Page 15: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 15

(2000) and Cox and MacKay (1981) build on specific facet models of stress (Beehr & Newman,

1978; Newman & Beehr, 1979).

Insert Figure 1 about here

The framework highlights two key processes of work stress: a transactional process linking

the person and environment, and a dynamic process that unfolds over time. First, transactional

approaches describe how the individual evaluates and responds to environmental conditions. This

transaction is a feature of many stress models particularly the socio-cognitive model (Lazarus &

Folkman, 1984) and the challenge-hindrance model (LePine et al., 2005) that specify how

environmental features are appraised by individuals; and cybernetic approaches (Edwards &

Cooper, 1990) where the discrepancy between the individual’s preferred state and the perceived

environment is the main motivational process. Other approaches emphasize the role of the

environment more directly. Role theory (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992) describes how specific task

conditions can have an impact on strain outcomes, the demands-control model (Karasek, 1989)

focuses on interaction of specific work characteristics, and the conservation of resources approach

(Hobfoll, 1989) emphasizes the way the environment helps build or deplete resources. Despite the

large role played by the environment in models of stress, Daniels (2006) notes that external features

such as job characteristics are typically assessed by individual self-reports that make it difficult to

disentangle environmental conditions from psychological processes such as bias, mood, and social

interaction.

Second, the stress process is dynamic because the transaction between the person and

environment unfolds over time through a process of mutual influence. The figure incorporates this

dynamic by specifying a short term time cycle of ongoing appraisal, goals, and action within a

Page 16: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 16

longer term cycle of interaction between the individual and the environment. This nesting of

processes recognizes the hierarchical structure of feedback loops in the stress process (Carver &

Scheier, 1990). Stress theories typically incorporate propositions about the temporal ordering of

events, and each major theory described above identifies a primary causal sequence through which

stressors are translated into strains. There is also some attention to stressors that might have long or

short term consequences (Beehr & Franz, 1987). However, the dynamics of mutual influence over

time have proved difficult to specify (Beehr, 2000).

In the following section, we provide a more detailed review of elements that constitute the

longer-term and the shorter-term dynamics of the stress process. We then explore the role of time

for linking short-term and long-term dynamics and conclude this section with a discussion of

methodological implications for studying the stress process.

Longer-term dynamics of the stress

The interaction between the individual and the environment

process

We begin reviewing the specific content of Figure 1 by identifying the relatively longer-

term elements of the person and the environment interaction. We first review studies that inform

main effects of the environment and individual on strain outcomes, then review issues associated

with understanding person-environment interactions.

The environment

The environment creates demands and provides supports in the stress process. The potential

range of environmental factors is vast: almost any event or situation that a person encounters could

be viewed as a source of stress. Most stress models propose that relatively stable job characteristics

such as job demands and control have an impact on workers’ health and well-being. Generic role

stressors and demands have been found to differ significantly across occupations (Sparks & Cooper,

Page 17: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 17

1999). In addition, job characteristics specific to particular occupations can be important predictors

of stress outcomes. For example, Cooper, Clarke, and Rowbottom (1999) identified occupational

characteristics within medical specialties that were specifically relevant for anaesthetists.

Alfredsson and Theorell (1983) developed an assessment of the risks posed by objective job

characteristics for myocardial infarction in 118 occupational groups.

In a review of 63 studies, van der Doef and Maes (1999) reported that two-thirds supported

a relationship between job characteristics and psychological well-being and this conclusion has

been supported in reviews of longitudinal studies (de Lange et al., 2003). Overall, the design of

jobs can have a significant impact on a wide range of positive and negative health outcomes

associated with the stress process (Parker, Turner, & Griffin, 2003).

In addition to job characteristics, organization design factors, such as the structure and

climate of the organization, management style, communication, level of consultation and politics,

influence individuals’ health and well-being. Indeed, a number of studies have found a more

negative effect associated with organizational stressors than stressors more intrinsically related to

the job in occupations including police (Hart, Wearing, & Headey, 1995; Thompson, Kirk-Brown,

& Brown, 2001), ambulance staff (Glendon & Coles, 2001), and teachers (Hart, 1994). For

example, Hart et al (1995) reported that police officers reported higher levels of psychological

distress in relation to organizational hassles (such as administration and paperwork) compared to

operational hassles (such as dealing with victims of crime or facing physical danger).

Sparks, Faragher, and Cooper (2001) identified four sources of stress that have become

increasingly important as employment conditions change: job security; long work hours; control at

work; and, managerial style. In particular, a lack of job security has been found to have detrimental

effects on health and well-being in European (Borg, Kristensen, & Burr, 2000; Domenighetti,

Page 18: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 18

D’Avanzo, & Bisig, 2000) and North American workers (McDonough, 2000; Probst & Brubaker,

2001). Job security can be objective, reflecting the actual likelihood of redundancy, or subjective,

reflecting perceptions of job security.

The social environment at work can constitute a demand or a resource, and can also act as a

moderator of the stressor-strain relationship. Social support has been one of the most prominent

resources investigated in the stress process and was included as an integral part of the extended

demand-control model (Johnson, Hall, & Theorell, 1989; Karasek, 1979). Kahn and Byosiere

(1992) reviewed 22 studies that investigated the link between social support and strain. All but two

reported main effects of social support on strain and, in most cases, the effect was observed for

support from both coworkers and supervisors. Viswesvaran, Sanchez, and Fisher's (1999) meta-

analysis found a medium effect size (ρ = -.21) for the direct relationship between social support and

strain. This negative effect indicates that social support has a reducing effect on strain (independent

of stressors) across most situations. Evidence that social support moderates stressor-strain

relationships is more mixed despite a large number of studies as well as narrative and meta-analytic

reviews assessing the proposed effect. Partial evidence for the moderating effect of social support

was reported by (Viswesvaran et al., 1999), who found an accumulated R2 of 3% across studies in

their meta-analysis. The interaction between support and stressors might be observed more reliably

if assessed at the appropriate level of analysis (Bliese & Castro, 2000) and where the type of

support matches the type of stressor (de Jonge & Dormann, 2006). For example, stressors

associated with the organizational level of analysis might show more consistent effects by taking

account of systematic differences between organizations rather than individuals’ perceptions within

organizations.

Page 19: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 19

The home-work interface is a distinct topic in the stress literature and a chapter in this

Handbook reviews home-work issues in more detail (see also Vol. 3, Chapter 14, by Hammer and

Zimmerman, on "Quality of Work Life"). Meta-analyses have explored the antecedents and

consequences of work-family conflict, generally supporting the proposition that work stressors have

an impact on the extent to which work interferes with family (e.g., Byron, 2005). Stressors at work

can also have an impact on the health of the employee’s family members (Bakker, Demerouti, &

Dollard, 2008).

Physical demands (such as exposure to heat, noise, and toxic substances) have received less

attention than psychological stressors. These demands are sometimes used as a control variable to

estimate the effects of psychological stressors or excluded from consideration as psychological

stressors (e.g., Ganster, 2008). The impact of shift work and work schedules, in contrast, has

received a great deal of attention. Totterdell (2005) provides an extensive review of the impact of

scheduling on factors such as fatigue, disease, and absence. There is widespread agreement that

shift work and long work hours significantly increase the risk of sleep disturbance and fatigue and

also increase the likelihood of some cancers, particularly breast cancer, gastrointestinal disorders

and cardiovascular disease. The increased risk is quite substantial for both breast cancer (50-60%)

and cardiovascular disease (40%) (see Totterdell, 2005, for further discussion).

The role of individual differences

Stable dispositional factors, such as personality, influence the stress-strain process in a

number of different ways, including: exposure to stressors; appraisal and reactivity to stressful

events; consistent use of coping strategies and development of coping style; and, susceptibility to

stress outcomes (Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995; Grant & Langan-Fox, 2006). For example, exposure

to stressors appears to occur more frequently for individuals high in neuroticism because they are

Page 20: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 20

more likely to view a situation as threatening and to use maladaptive coping techniques

(Hemenover, 2003; McCrae & Costa, 1986). In contrast, those high in extraversion and

conscientiousness are more likely to view potential stressors as a challenge and to adopt problem-

solving coping strategies (Watson & Hubbard, 1996). Research on the interactive effects of

personality traits (Grant & Langan-Fox, 2006) highlighted the role of combinations of neuroticism,

extraversion and conscientiousness – specifically that combinations of high neuroticism-high

conscientiousness predicted higher stressor exposure and low neuroticism-high extraversion and/or

high conscientiousness predicted lower stressor exposure. In relation to coping, high extraversion-

high conscientiousness generally predicted higher problem-focused coping and high neuroticism-

low conscientiousness generally predicted lower problem-focused coping. Overall, personality type

explained approximately 8% of the variance in stressor exposure and 11% of the variance in coping.

Low strain was consistently associated with low neuroticism-high extraversion-high

conscientiousness; however, high strain was differentially related to personality type, depending on

the outcome variable (Grant & Langan-Fox, 2006). Physical ill-health was most associated with

‘impulsive’ personality types (high extraversion-high neuroticism-low conscientiousness), whereas

job dissatisfaction was most associated with individuals low on all three personality dimensions.

Low agreeableness in combination with high neuroticism was related to job dissatisfaction, but not

physical ill-health; this finding suggests an affective, rather than a physiological, response is typical

for such individuals.

Kahn and Byosiere (1992) argued that locus of control influences how individuals cope with

stress; those with an internal locus of control will engage more actively to cope with stress, and so

display better health outcomes. An internal locus of control is associated with the belief that

outcomes can be controlled and so individuals are more likely to adopt a proactive approach to

Page 21: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 21

dealing with a negative work environment. Individuals with an internal locus of control are more

likely to prevent the development of stressful conditions and to actively seek out ways of managing

negative situations when they occur. Overall, internals are more likely to perceive work stressors as

manageable and less threatening. For example, Daniels and Guppy (1994) found that job control

moderated the stress-strain relationship, but only for individuals with internal locus of control. Ng,

Sorensen, and Eby (2006) found that internal locus of control was strongly associated with well-

being, including mental well-being (ρ = .36), physical health (ρ = .31) and burnout (ρ = -.27),

predominately through cognitive processes, self-evaluation, motivation, and coping strategies.

Extending locus of control, the concept of core self-evaluation (CSE) reflects an individual’s

overall perception of self-worth and is comprised of: locus of control, plus generalized self-efficacy,

and neuroticism (Judge, Erez, Bono, & Thoresen, 2002; Judge, Locke, & Durham, 1997). CSE can

influence the type of jobs that individuals select, the work environments that they experience and

their perceptions of the environment (Dormann, Fay, Zapf, & Frese, 2006). Feeling able to control

one’s job situation is a central part of CSE and an important aspect of well-being; feeling in control

will lead to satisfaction regardless of whether that control is actually exercised. Therefore, an

internal locus of control can be more important for job satisfaction than available control. In some

situations, an internal locus of control might lead to less engagement with the external environment

to protect mental resources that otherwise would be depleted (Schönpflug, 1983). There are

circumstances, however, where an internal locus of control may have adverse effects, for example,

an internal locus of control might lead one to expend effort to manage a situation that cannot be

controlled, thus depleting resources (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000).

A wide variety of additional factors have been studied as individual antecedents in the stress

process, or as moderators of the stress process, including negative affectivity, hardiness, Type A

Page 22: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 22

personality, and optimism (Cooper et al., 2001). We consider individual factors as moderators in

more detail in the next section.

Interactions between the individual and the environment

A defining feature of the stress process is the way individual characteristics interact with

features of the environment. In this section we review various approaches to describing and

analyzing this interaction from the perspective of more stable person and environment features. The

socio-cognitive approach (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and the demands-control model of stress

(Karasek & Theorell, 1990) include person-environment interactions as a central part of each

theory. Other approaches incorporate this interaction as an implicit link between core constructs

such as person-environment fit (Edwards & Cooper, 1990) and homeostasis (Hobfoll, 1989).

One complexity associated with assessing an interaction between individuals and

environments is the sheer number of interactions that can be considered. All environmental

demands and resources can potentially interact with all individual differences. A further

methodological complexity is the form of the interaction. In its simplest form, an interaction can be

represented by a multiplicative relationship between two variables so that the effect of one variable

depends on the level of the second variable. Extensive research has assessed this interaction using

concepts of moderation, contingency, and buffering. We reviewed some of these issues in the

discussion of social support above. The DC model incorporates interaction as part of its central

propositions and we review this model next in more detail together with other features of the

person-environment interaction.

Many different forms of interaction between demands and control have been tested

empirically (Van Vegchel, De Jonge, & Landsbergis, 2005). Karasek (1989) proposed that the

interaction could simply reflect the joint effect of demands and control on strain via an additive

Page 23: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 23

effect, given that multiplicative terms are difficult to detect in practical situations. Van Vegchel et

al. (2005) found that both multiplicative and ratio interaction terms were significant predictors of

strain; both supported the combination of high demands, low control as high strain, but differed on

other conditions. The results were consistent with Warr's (1994) suggestion that relationships

between environmental characteristics, such as job demands, and well-being are non-linear.

The existence of non-linear relationships is problematic for testing interactional effects.

Inconsistencies in the reporting of interaction effects may also be partially explained by the

‘matching hypothesis’ (de Jonge & Dormann, 2006), whereby interaction effects are more likely to

be demonstrated when there is a match between the type of stressor, resource and strain. de Jonge

and Dormann (2006) found that interactions were more likely to be supported when there was a

triple match between the stressor, resource and strain. For example, physical resources and physical

stressors were more likely to interact in the prediction of physical outcomes compared to other

outcomes.

A review conducted by de Lange et al. (2003) included only longitudinal studies that met a

high quality standard. They found that of 19 studies reviewed, 8 reported support for the joint

effects of job demands, job control or social support on health outcomes over time.

The basic interaction between demands and control has been extended in a variety of ways.

Three-way interactions have been supported for self-efficacy (Salanova et al., 2002; Schaubroeck &

Merritt, 1997), active coping (de Rijk, Le Blanc, Schaufeli, & De Jonge, 1998), proactive

personality (Cunningham & De La Rosa, 2008; Parker & Sprigg, 1999), information (Jimmieson &

Terry, 1999), and locus of control (Meier et al., 2008). Although the theoretical basis for individual

moderators is elaborated in these studies, an overall framework for selecting substantively different

types of moderators has not been established. It is possible failures to demonstrate interactions

Page 24: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 24

between demands and control result from a lack of methodological and theoretical sophistication

(Parker et al., 2003). For example, significant interactions have been found when the level of

analysis at which constructs are theorized is matched to the measurement strategies and analyses

(Bliese & Castro, 2000; Morrison, Payne, & Wall, 2003).

In addition to the DC model, other approaches to the stress process either test an interaction

between the person and the environment directly or describe some form of mutual and

interdependent relationship. The concepts of ‘fit’ and ‘discrepancy’ are particularly important for

conveying this interdependence in conservation of resources, P-E fit and cybernetic models of

stress.

Cummings and Cooper (1979) provide a comprehensive portrayal of the person-

environment interaction in terms of time, information, and feedback. An underlying feature of

interaction arguments is a drive toward homeostasis, where the individual and environment achieve

a state of equilibrium (Hobfoll, 1989). From this perspective, a stressor is a force disrupting a

system beyond its range of stability, and the stress process involves the actions that achieve a new

state of equilibrium.

It is difficult to assess the unique importance of interactions in the stress process

independent of main effects. Interactions are often reported in relation to multiple main effects, null

effects are often observed, and the proportion of variance explained by interactions is frequently

small. Design, measurement, and statistical artifacts can also bias the size of interaction effects

downwards (Aguinis, Beaty, Boik, & Pierce, 2005). Overall, effect sizes for interactions explaining

between 2% and 5% of strain outcomes appear to have a significant and meaningful impact in the

stress process.

Page 25: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 25

Longer-term outcomes of the stress

We now look at the final element of the longer-term time cycle in the Figure, the outcomes

of the stress process. Individuals can vary greatly in their response to stressors. For example, a

heavy workload might cause psychological effects in one person, making them anxious, whilst

another might experience physiological symptoms, such as headaches. There are also differences of

the impact of some stressors on health for men and women. For example, long work hours have a

negative effect on health (Sparks et al., 2001) but the risk is greater for women (Starrin, Larsson,

Brenner, Levi, & Petterson, 1990).

process

Various categorizations of longer-term stress outcomes have been proposed but are

generally grouped as psychological, physiological, or behavioral outcomes (Kahn & Byosiere,

1992). Most research has focused on the psychological outcomes of the stress process and the

strongest empirical effects of stressors have been observed in relation to psychological outcomes.

This stronger effect might be inflated because of common method bias resulting from similar

measurement for stressors and strains (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). It is

possible that experience of one strain can increase vulnerability to other types of strain. For

example, the long-term effects of job dissatisfaction associated with role ambiguity might be

physical ill-health. This causal process would lead to a weaker relationship between stressors and

physical outcomes compared to psychological outcomes.

We now explore the negative outcomes of the stress process and integrate some more

positive features of health and well-being. We review the different categories of outcome in terms

of psychological strain and well-being, physical health, and behavioral outcomes.

Page 26: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 26

Psychological strain and well-being

Kahn and Byosiere (1992) identified over 40 different measures of psychological strain,

although they noted considerable conceptual overlap among categories. Job dissatisfaction was the

most commonly used measure of strain, and other common measures were anxiety, depression, and

generalized strain. Most research reviews indicate that psychological strains, such as anxiety and

depression, are strong correlates of work-related stressors (Jackson & Schuler, 1985; Jex & Beehr,

1991; Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). Viswesvaran et al (1999) estimated a correlation of .43 between the

general construct of work stressors and the general construct of strain.

In the DC model, the benefits of control and support in buffering against the negative health

effects of high demands, via coping, accumulate over time. Control and support have their buffering

effect by facilitating problem-solving. Over time, consistent use of coping mechanisms leads to a

‘coping style’, a cross-situational consistency in coping. Daniels, Beesley, Cheyne, and Wimalasiri

(2008) found that consistent use of problem-solving coping, enacted through support and control,

led to fewer risky decisions, but could have detrimental effects in the short term. These findings

would support the adjustment model, where strain increases proportionately with increases in the

intensity of the stressor until the individual adjusts to the stressor (Frese and Zapf, 1988).

Although it is clear that the stress process is associated with a wide range of psychological

strains, there has often been limited theoretical justification for the specific strains included in a

particular study (Cooper et al., 2001). The study of burnout is an exception, with the three

dimensions of exhaustion, depersonalization, and lower efficacy derived from theoretical

propositions about the aetiology of symptoms.

Further progress toward a more theoretical description of stress outcomes has been made by

integrating the broader construct of well-being. The typical conceptualization of stress covers only a

Page 27: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 27

part of the broader domain of well-being (Warr, 2005). Mental health should not be considered

merely as the absence of stress symptoms, but in terms of the presence of active mental health

(Parker et al., 2003). Such a state would have positive indicators, such as competence, mastery,

aspiration and desire for autonomy (Warr, 1994). These indicators encompass the motivational and

behavioral dimensions of well-being as well as its affective nature.

Ryff & Keyes, (1995) developed a six-factor model of wellness: autonomy, environmental

mastery, personal growth, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. These

aspects of well-being are defined as: a sense of self-determination and the ability to resist social

pressures to think and act in certain ways (autonomy); the capacity to effectively manage one’s life

and the surrounding world (environmental mastery); the sense of continued growth and

development as a person as well as openness to new experiences (personal growth); positive regard

for other people, such as a genuine concern about the welfare of others (positive relations with

others); the belief that one’s life is purposeful and meaningful and that one has something to live for

(purpose in life); and a positive evaluation of oneself and one’s past life (self-acceptance). Warr

(1994) emphasizes the importance not only of these separate dimensions, but also of ‘integrated

functioning’ that represents the person as a whole. The importance of achieving such a balance is

reflected in the definition of ‘happiness’ as the balance between positive and negative affect

(Bradburn, 1969).

Models of burnout have also been extended to include engagement dimensions that

represent positive poles of the burnout dimension (Maslach & Leiter, 2008). Positive mental well-

being has been related to ‘work engagement’, where engaged workers are defined as those who

have “a sense of energetic and effective connection with their work activities and [they] see

themselves as able to deal well with the demands of their job” (Schaufeli, Taris, & van Rhenen,

Page 28: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 28

2008, p. 176). It comprises three aspects: high levels of energy and mental resilience (vigor); a

sense of significance, enthusiasm, inspiration, pride and challenge (dedication); and, being fully

engrossed in work (absorption). The latter element of absorption has also been associated with

workaholism; however, workaholism has been linked with poor well-being (Schaufeli et al., 2008;

Taris, Schaufeli, & Verhoeven, 2005). There is moderate support for a positive relationship

between work engagement and good mental health: Schaufeli & Bakker (2004) found that

emotional exhaustion was negatively correlated with vigor (r = -.40) and dedication (r = -.28), but

had little relationship with absorption (r = -.07). The association with well-being may be explained

by engaged individuals’ response to highly demanding, but highly resourced jobs. Engaged

workers become engrossed in their work, but perceive work as ‘fun’. In contrast, workaholics tend

to work excessively hard in poorly resourced jobs, i.e., those that are highly demanding, but have

poor support (Demerouti, Bakker, de Jonge, Janssen, & Schaufeli, 2001). Thus, motivational

aspects differ: workaholics are driven to work hard, whilst engaged workers are more intrinsically

motivated. However, managing highly demanding jobs over time, even for engaged workers, may

eventually lead to burnout (Schaufeli et al., 2008).

Physical health

Physiological outcomes are studied less often than other outcomes in stress research.

However, there is a substantial body of evidence that the stress process, over time, can have a

significant negative effect on workers’ physical health and immunity (van der Doef & Maes, 1999;

Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Whilst acute and time-limited stressors lead to an adaptive response,

chronic stressors result in decreased potential adaptiveness of the immune system over time

(Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Chandola et al. (2006) assessed data from over 4000 individuals on

four occasions to link work stressors to health outcomes, including a metabolic syndrome

Page 29: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 29

associated with increased risk of heart disease and Type 2 diabetes. The study found that chronic

work stress substantially increased the likelihood of developing the metabolic syndrome: men were

nearly twice as likely and women over five times more likely to have the metabolic syndrome,

compared to those with no exposure to work stress. Heart rate, blood pressure and catecholamines

have been assessed most often as indicators of physiological functioning. Jones et al. (2007) argue

that one mechanism linking job strain to health is through a range of health behaviors. Those most

studied are alcohol and smoking (e.g., Kouvonen, Kivimaki, Cox, Cox, & Vahtera, 2005). Other

studies have shown that job strain is associated with decreased healthy eating or increased body

mass index (Hellerstedt & Jeffery, 1997; Kouvonen et al., 2005; Lallukka et al., 2004; Tsutsumi et

al., 2003).

Studies investigating health outcomes have not reported consistent findings, although Zapf

et al. (1996) argue that the effect size of the stressor-strain relationship is limited, due to the large

number of potential causative factors influencing health and well-being (including environment,

leisure and family stressors, social class, personality, and health behaviors). Segerstrom (2007)

noted that the most psychologically healthy individuals sometimes have the least robust immune

system, further complicating a straightforward link between long-term stressors and health; for

example, unnecessary energy allocated to the immune system makes less energy available for other

systems. When resources are threatened, it could be adaptive for organisms to direct energy away

from the immune system and toward protecting or restoring resources (Segerstrom, 2007).

Work stress has been associated with a range of musculoskeletal diseases and complaints

(Bongers, de Winter, Kompier, & Hildebrandt, 1993; Carayon, Smith, & Haims, 1999).

Biomechanical processes explain some of the reasons that stressors such as workload might lead to

musculoskeletal complaints. However, psychological processes also play a part in shaping the

Page 30: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 30

experience of these factors. Authors have proposed that psychological affective strains might

mediate the link between stressors and musculoskeletal complaints (e.g., Kjellberg & Wadman,

2007). The prevalence of office based work and increasing computer use mean that these problems

are likely to increase. Sprigg, Stride, Wall, Holman, and Smith (2007) found that the impact of

workload on musculoskeletal disorders for call centre employees was partially mediated by the

psychological experiences of anxiety and depression.

Behavioral outcomes

Indirectly, a broad domain of behavioral responses has been linked to the stress process. Jex

and Beehr (1991) divide behaviors into those with significance for the organization, including job

performance, turnover and absenteeism; and those with significance to the individual, including use

of alcohol, smoking, substance use and destructive behaviors. Behaviors can be associated with

stressors via a direct relationship (e.g., work overload causes absence from work) or mediated by an

affective state (e.g., work overload causes anxiety, which in turn causes absenteeism).

The experience of transient positive moods has also been linked to enhanced outcomes (Erez

& Isen, 2002; Totterdell, 2000). Tsai, Chen, and Liu (2007) found that positive mood states

predicted task performance. Furthermore, the study showed that positive moods have a lasting

effect on task performance measured three weeks later. This effect may be facilitated through

interpersonal processes, such as helping coworkers – whereby an employee’s positive mood results

in greater engagement in helping coworkers motivated by desire to maintain his or her positive

mood. Positive moods can be ‘contagious’ as individuals’ helping behavior encourages positive

moods in coworkers. Walter and Bruch (2008) argue that although most research has considered

positive moods and emotion at the individual level, they may be viewed as collective constructs

Page 31: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 31

operating at a group level. George (1990) showed that positive group affect is related to

absenteeism and individual well-being.

Proactive work behavior has also been linked to enhanced well-being (Crant, 2000); this

behavior is defined as “taking initiative in improving current circumstances; it involves challenging

the status quo rather than passively adapting present conditions” (p.436). Examples of proactive

behavior include generating and implementing new ideas and taking the initiative in problem-

solving. Such behavior involves a conscious decision-making process and is affected by the

individual’s level of self-efficacy (need to feel ‘in control’ of a situation) and felt responsibility for

change (motivated to approach initiative and willing to accept responsibility). Parker, Williams,

and Turner (2006) showed that self-efficacy played a significant role in proactive behavior,

highlighting the importance of building employees’ perceptions of their own capability as a means

of enhancing employee proactivity, and subsequently their well-being.

Response to stressors can include a range of work withdrawal behaviors (R. L. Kahn &

Byosiere, 1992). Darr and Johns (2008) reviewed 153 studies and found positive but small

connections among work strain, illness, and absenteeism, challenging the popular workplace

estimates that introduced this chapter. They estimated that work strain accounts for between six and

29% of the variance in absenteeism. In comparing whether absence was primarily a response to a

noxious workplace or a function of illness, they concluded with tentative support for a partial illness

explanation.

Workplace safety is another potentially important outcome of the stress process. Stress is

considered to be responsible for 60-80% of all workplace accidents (Cooper, Liukkonen, &

Cartwright, 1996), yet there is a lack of research examining accidents as an outcome of

occupational stress. Occupational stress is related to increased accident risk in a variety of working

Page 32: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 32

environments. For example, job stress was found to be a small but significant predictor of work

accidents (β = .13, p <.01) in a study of 778 veterinarians (Trimpop, Kirkcaldy, Athanasou, and

Cooper, 2000) and also in a sample of 2500 doctors (β = .09, p <.05; Kirkcaldy, Trimpop, and

Cooper, 1997). Experience of workplace stressors may have direct effects on performance,

increasing accident liability, or the effects may be indirect, mediated by employee health and well-

being. Exposure to long-term stressors that result in psychological and physical symptoms of ill-

health can also lead to increased accident risk. For example, Houston and Allt (1997), in a study of

junior house officers, found that psychological distress was linked to significant medical errors, as

well as everyday errors. Cooper and Cartwright (1994) elaborated on multiple processes through

which individual symptoms can ultimately influence organizational outcomes such as accident

rates. For example, high absenteeism might lead to staff shortages increasing workload on

remaining personnel, and making errors more likely.

Short-term dynamics of the stress

We next view the stress process in terms of psychological systems that dynamically interact

with each other over shorter periods of time. This dynamic process is embedded in the longer term

process described above and is consistent with the notion that feedback cycles at lower levels of

analysis are likely to occur more rapidly than those at higher levels (Campion & Lord, 1982). In

contrast to the longer term cycle, the cognitive, affective, and behavioral processes in this cycle are

more fluid and difficult to differentiate. This cycle is also likely to involve automatic processing

that occurs without conscious responding (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000; Bargh, 1990). Nevertheless,

the theoretical approaches we have introduced to date are relevant to various parts of this process

and we describe them next.

processes

Page 33: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 33

Appraisal Processes

First, the appraisal process involves the perception of environmental stimuli. Different

approaches to stress propose categorization processes that describe how features of the environment

are encoded by individuals. In the appraisal process, the environment becomes salient due to events

that occur in relation to histories and propensities of the individual. Many approaches to stress

emphasize appraisal although the conceptualization and measurement of ongoing appraisals is

problematic (Troup & Dewe, 2002).

The socio-cognitive approach places particular emphasis on appraisal of the environment

(e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Primary appraisal involves the recognition of a potential stressor

and initial evaluation of whether the stressor presents a loss, threat, or challenge. Although a wide

range of factors can be appraised as stressors, the categorization of a situation as threatening and

uncertain is particularly important.

The socio-cognitive approach to appraisal has been further refined through hindrance-

challenge models of stress (Podsakoff, LePine, & LePine, 2007). The central appraisal process in

this approach is whether a potential stressor is categorized as a challenge or a hindrance. The nature

of the stressor is an important determinant of the categorization. As noted above, many individual

and situational characteristics combine to determine how the environment is categorized, although

the hindrance-challenge approach allows for intrinsic features of environmental demand.

The DC model involves initial appraisal of demands and resources to meet the demands and

implies strain related outcomes are derived when demands exceed resources. Cybernetic models

incorporate appraisal directly with perceptions of discrepancy. Individuals appraise the environment

with reference to internal standards and it is these appraisals that motivate subsequent responses.

Page 34: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 34

Surprisingly little research has elaborated the appraisal process by integrating development

in social cognition and cognitive psychology. An exception is provided by Daniels, Harris, and

Briner (2004) who identify automatic and controlled information processing pathways through

which the perception of an event is encoded. Recent biological research investigates the relationship

between the appraisal process and cortical activity. For example, appraisal of a stressor with low

controllability is associated with higher cerebral blood flow in the orbitofrontal and prefrontral

cortices, which in turn were related to peripheral autonomic and immune activities (Ohira et al.,

2008).

In summary, most major approaches to stress incorporate some form of cognitive appraisal

involving an evaluation and categorization of environmental features. Central to this evaluation is

the categorization and comparison of resources and demands.

Goal processes

Next, we consider how appraisal of the environment engages the individual’s goal systems.

Goals are cognitive representations of desired or undesired states (Austin & Vancouver, 1996).

Goals are distinct from the evaluation of referents and discrepancies described in the appraisal

process because they guide the selection and persistence of further action. Goal processes are

generally considered controlled and intentional, though once established they can be activated

quickly and automatically (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000).

Cybernetic models of stress include goals as a central mechanism (Cummings & Cooper,

1979; Edwards, 1992; Miller, 1965). The goal system comprises a negative feedback loop which

acts to minimize discrepancies between environmental characteristics and relevant reference criteria

(Cummings & Cooper, 1979; Edwards, 1992). The cybernetic approach has been criticized for

failing to capture important features of human goal systems such as forethought and the deliberate

Page 35: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 35

creation of discrepancies to motivate action (Locke, 1991, 1994). In response, Edwards (1998)

argued that the hierarchical arrangement of feedback loops explains how effort to resolve

discrepancies at lower levels also helps to resolve discrepancies at higher levels. This system

explains the link between short term discrepancies and the striving to longer term value-based

goals. In effect the discrepancy perceived in the appraisal processes, is incorporated into the goals

system of the hierarchical set of discrepancies. Setting standards might also be considered here, as

this activity involves forethought (Edwards, 1998).

Both cybernetic and socio-cognitive theories of stress describe how perceptions of threat

and discrepancy engage goal systems that motivate further action. The socio-cognitive theory of

stress incorporates aspects of this goal process in terms of secondary appraisal. The evaluation of

response options and resources for coping occurs as part of this appraisal. Daniels et al. (2004)

described a cognitive process through which a mental model of work events is built dynamically to

identify and evaluate progress toward goals.

A further elaboration of the goal process is contained in the distinction between approach

and avoidance goals (Elliot, 2006). Based on the dichotomy between pleasure-pain, the approach

and avoidance distinction has been part of human philosophy for hundreds of years, and theories of

stress incorporate the distinction in various ways. For example, primary appraisal in the socio-

cognitive model includes categorization of an event as harmful, beneficial, or neutral (Lazarus &

Folkman, 1984). In recent years, a more fine-grained development of approach and avoidance

motivations has been developed and integrated with the study of affect, cognition, and behavior

(Carver, 2006; Elliot & Thrash, 2002; Higgins, 1997). Approach motivation is the direction of

behavior toward positive stimuli whereas avoidance motivation is behavior directed away from

negative stimuli (Elliot, 2006). Goals provide cognitive representation of an outcome that guides the

Page 36: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 36

individual toward or away from that outcome (Elliot & Fryer, 2007). This work is important for

extending the study of goals in the stress process in a number of ways.

First, stress research through its focus on negative outcomes is intrinsically concerned with

avoidance goals. However, most stress models incorporate negative feedback or discrepancy

reducing loops (Edwards, 1998). In contrast, the motivational process underlying avoidance

involves discrepancy enlarging loops. Discrepancy enlarging loops differ from discrepancy

reducing loops because the distancing process has no specific direction. That is, a discrepancy

reducing loop helps individuals to home in on a specific goal whereas a discrepancy enlarging loop

simply leads away from the anti-goal. The discrepancy enlarging goal is therefore less directive

(Carver, 2006). In most systems, the enlarging process eventually leads to a positive incentive that

then engages approach motivation. For example, actively avoiding participation in a group with a

negative work climate might bring an individual in contact with a group where the work climate is

positive. Approach goals for the second work group might then guide behavior according to a

discrepancy reduction loop.

Stable individual differences can influence whether approach or avoidance goals are

adopted. For example, Elliot and Thrash (2002, p. 806) described stable temperaments to adopt

either approach or avoidance goals based on “networks of biological sensitivities that are

responsible for immediate affective, cognitive, and behavioral propensities in response to

encountered or imagined stimuli”. Carver and White (1994) identified differential sensitivity among

individuals to the experience of punishment and rewards. Using Gray's (1994) distinction between

behavioral activation system (BAS) and behavioral inhibition systems (BIS) they found that some

individuals were more sensitive.

Page 37: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 37

In general, the BIS system serves to protect the individuals by inhibiting behavior that could

result in aversive consequences (Watson et al., 1999). As yet, few studies have applied this

distinction to stress and the work environment. One example is provided by van der Linden, Taris,

Beckers, & Kindt (2007) who found that punishment sensitivity interacted with job characteristics

to predict fatigue at work, where high BIS-individuals with low job control experienced higher

levels of fatigue (β = -.13, p <.05).

In summary, if an environmental transaction is appraised in positive terms then approach

goals are activated and pursued through a negative feedback loop. On the other hand, a transaction

appraised as threatening can lead to avoidance or approach goals. Avoidance goals are based on a

positive feedback loop that is conceptually distinct and based on different physiological and

neurological processes. The implications of this difference have not been fully explored in the stress

literature and we examine this issue further in the next section of the dynamics process.

Responses in the shorter term

Once a particular goal is activated, individuals are motivated to strive toward achieving the

goal. Cognitive, affective, behavioral, and physiological responses are distinct components of goal

striving and achievement. However, disentangling these components is difficult because they are

interdependent and closely related in any given moment. For example, Grandey (2000) suggested

that keeping a diary of emotional events is a potential strategy for regulating the cognitive and

physical arousal associated with emotional labor at work. While recognizing that the components of

response are interdependent, different perspectives emphasize the role of specific response systems.

We review different approaches in terms of the degree to which they define response as an

active agent in the short term dynamic process. First, we consider “reactive” responses where the

outcome is viewed primarily as a consequence or epiphenomenon of the stress process. For

Page 38: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 38

example, physiological studies of stress often view increased heart rate as a response to threat with

long term consequences for health, but not as part of the ongoing cycle of appraisal and goals.

Reactive responses have a short life span and should be distinguished from outcomes which

develop over a longer period of time. However, reactive responses may accumulate over time to

lead to longer-term health outcomes. Second, we view more “active” responses such as coping

behavior that involves directly changing the elements of the dynamic process in either a positive or

negative way.

Reactive response

Affective and physiological outcomes are the most commonly studied reactive responses in

the stress process. Negative affect is a fundamental response to discrepancy (Edwards, 1998),

mismatch (French, Caplan, & Harrison, 1982), and imbalance (Siegrist, 1996). Although this

negative affect provides the broad motivational basis for addressing this lack of fit, its role is non-

specific. The quality of affect is determined not only by the size of the gap between actual and

desired states, but by the rate of progress towards goals (Carver & Scheier, 1990).

Different affective responses have been proposed for the pursuit of approach or avoidance

goals (Carver, 2006). Carver and White (1994) found differences in individual sensitivity to

activation or inhibition systems were related differentially to the experience of emotion in situations

of threat or reward. For example, people higher in BIS sensitivity showed higher levels of anxiety

in response to threat.

Short term physiological responses within the stress process encompass changes in the

endocrine, immune, cardiovascular, and musculoskeletal system. In the work context, most interest

in physiological systems has focused on how that activation or suppression of these systems leads to

long term negative outcomes such as disease or injury (see next section). Sonnentag and Fritz

Page 39: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 39

(2006) conducted an extensive review of research linking acute and chronic stressors to

catecholamines (adrenaline and noradrenaline) and cortisol. Acute stressors were most commonly

studied in laboratory settings and were clearly related to an increase in adrenaline levels. Tasks that

posed social-evaluative threat and low controllability elicited strongest cortisol response (Dickerson

& Kemeny, 2004). Noradrenaline was more strongly associated with aversive experiences and

demanding tasks. In field studies, workload and control were the most common factors investigated

but results across studies were more variable. In general, more evidence was found for the impact of

acute versus chronic stressors on endocrine responses (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2006).

Physiological responses to work have most often been studied as a means to understand the

negative consequences of stressors. However, Heaphy and Dutton (2008) recently called for the

positive physiological consequences of work to receive more attention and be better integrated

within this stream of research.

Active response

Active responses are viewed as actions that change the unfolding pattern of the dynamic

stress process. Here, the concept of coping with stressors is most relevant. Coping is one of the

most common and important responses studied in the stress process (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).

Coping serves four major functions: approach, avoidance, emotional regulation and reappraisal

(Ferguson & Cox, 1997), although a widely accepted definition of the content and function of

coping responses has proved elusive. However, a distinction between problem focused coping that

seeks to solve the demands of a stressor, emotion focused coping that helps the individual to feel

better about the stressor, and appraisal focused coping that attempts to redefine the stressor, has

been used widely to capture different goals of coping (Cooper et al., 2001).

Page 40: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 40

Although many studies treat coping as a dispositional response, the concept also informs the

more dynamic aspects of the link between the individual and the environment (Folkman & Lazarus,

1988). From a cybernetic perspective, coping behavior is a process of feedback control (Carver &

Scheier, 1990) and discrepancies increase the intensity of coping efforts (Edwards, 1998). Coping is

complex because it is conceived as both a cause and a consequence of the stress process (Kinicki &

Latack, 1990).

Coping behaviors are initiated as a result of primary and secondary appraisal (Lazarus &

Folkman, 1984). Behavioral responses are important because they involve actions that can directly

influence and change the environment. For example, seeking feedback aids problem solving. On the

other hand, emotion focused responses, such as emotional discharge, have generally been

considered escapist strategies that are not beneficial for the individual in the long term. Folkman

and Moskowitz (2004) identified three types of coping strategy that evoked positive emotions:

positive reappraisal; problem-focused coping; and imbuing ordinary events with positive meaning.

Coping strategies that evoke positive emotions, such as humor, may be particularly effective in

coping with stressful situations that generate negative emotions, such as anxiety and depression.

Those individuals who are more aware of their own emotions may be better able to use such coping

mechanisms.

Affective response can actively influence the dynamic stress process by directly influencing

the categorization process. Daniels et al.'s (2004) model of affect and well-being proposed that

affect influences the cognitive processes by which events are categorized and the mental models

that are constructed to create and pursue goals. Broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998)

accounts for the effects of positive emotions in broadening people’s momentary thought-action

repertoires (in contrast to negative emotions which tend to have the opposite effect). The theory

Page 41: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 41

states that “discrete positive emotions…share the ability to broaden people’s momentary thought-

action repertoires and build their enduring personal resources” (Fredrickson, 2001, p.219). The

theory is based on the adaptive role of emotions as triggers for a specific set of behaviors; for

example, experiencing fear leads individuals to make their escape. Most specific actions, however,

are linked to negative emotions; whereas positive emotions lead not to specific actions, but

encourage a broader range of approach behaviors. It is argued that the experience of positive

emotions broadens the scope of attention, cognition and action and leads to the development of

physical, intellectual and social resources over time (Fredrickson, 2001). The building of personal

resources helps individuals to develop psychological resilience when faced with potential stressors

in the future. Furthermore, the enhancement of psychological resilience can lead to improved well-

being over time creating the possibility for an ongoing upwards spiral.

The role of time in the stress

To understand the dynamic features of psychological processes it is necessary to identify

stability and change in constructs, allow for multiple patterns of change over time, and distinguish

recurrent versus ongoing phenomena (Roe, 2008). Neufeld (1999) differentiated five types of time-

dependent systems that researchers have used to describe the stress process: transactional, dynamic,

process like, adaptational, and recursive. Transactional systems are represented by a network of

variables that are closely connected and interdependent, for example ‘reciprocal determinism’

(Bandura, 1978); dynamic systems are characterized by variables that interact with each other over

time (e.g., McGrath & Beehr, 1990; Roe, 2008); process like systems are those where the evolution

of variables over time depends on prior continuous change and so remains in a state of flux (e.g.,

Edwards, 1992); adaptational systems are those in which variables respond and adapt to changes in

each other (e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984); finally, recursive systems reflect a dynamic state of

process

Page 42: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 42

interaction between variables that form a predictable pattern, for example, ‘limit cycles’ which

reflect a stable oscillation, within a given period and amplitude (Neufeld, 1999). Although time has

always played a role in models of stress, few approaches provide such a detailed distinction

between time frames (Beehr, 2000).

Our framework incorporates time by differentiating a longer-time cycle within which is

nested a shorter-term cycle. The longer-term processes involve changes in fairly stable individual

differences (e.g., personality) and environmental factors (e.g., task structures). The short-term cycle

denotes more variable fluctuations such as the effect of daily work patterns on mood changes

(DeLongis, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1988). In reality, the time frame for the stress process ranges on a

continuum from moment-to-moment appraisals of the environment and coping responses (e.g.,

Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007) to the long term effect of chronic stressors such as heart disease which

might be expressed over periods of years (e.g., Chandola, Brunner, & Marmot, 2006). A review of

45 longitudinal studies identified time lags ranging between 28 days and 12 years with

approximately two thirds providing a theoretical rationale for the particular time lag (de Lange et

al., 2003).

Our framework also indicates a cyclical process whereby strain outcomes have an influence

on stressors over both the short term and longer term. For example, an anxiety response in the work

place can lead to a subsequent experience of stressors by contributing to a more fearful

environment. This form of reciprocal determinism is fundamental to social cognitive processes

(Bandura, 1978) but difficult to establish empirically (Lance, Lautenschlager, Sloan, & Varca,

1989). Kohn and Schooler (1978) conducted a 10-year longitudinal study of the reciprocal effects of

work complexity and intellectual flexibility and demonstrated a pattern of reciprocal relationship

between work and personal characteristics. Frese, Garst, and Fay (2007) identified a reciprocal

Page 43: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 43

relationship between personal initiative and work characteristics as a positive feedback loop

occurring over a four-year time lag. It seems likely that this reciprocal influence is weaker than the

alternative path from stressors to strains (de Lange et al., 2003). There is fairly strong evidence

from longitudinal studies that work characteristics can influence stress-related outcomes, and some

high quality longitudinal studies investigating reciprocal relationships have failed to identify reverse

causation (Carayon, 1992, 1993).

The process through which short-term responses shape long term outcomes raises both

theoretical and methodological hurdles. Stress theories are generally better suited to explaining how

short-term dynamics are shaped and constrained by the more stable context of person and situation

characteristics (Griffin, 1997). The mechanism through which short-term dynamics shape long-term

dynamics is likely to involve not only the translation of short-term responses into long term health

outcomes, but also result in change for the person and the situation. Frese and Zapf (1988)

described five types of stress exposure models through which short-term dynamics might be

expressed in terms of long term strain outcomes. First, a stress reaction model describes the case

where strain increases proportionately with increases in the intensity of the stressors. This case

describes the implicit, though largely untested assumption, of most stress models that higher

appraisals of stress over time lead to greater strain outcomes.

Second, an accumulation model proposes that accumulation of stressors might only appear

as strains after a certain ‘breaking point’ and might not disappear when stressors are removed.

Jones, Conner, McMillan, and Ferguson (2007) found that daily fluctuations in job demands and

mood were associated with health-related behaviors such as smoking and caffeine intake which

might result in the later onset of illness.

Page 44: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 44

Third, a dynamic accumulation model where a person is weakened by exposure to stressors

so that an inner dynamic continues to increase strain even after the stressors are removed.

Brosschot, Pieper, and Thayer (2005) describe a process of perseverative cognition where negative

thoughts and emotions initiated by environmental conditions but prolonged by short-term dynamics

such as worry, rumination, and anticipatory stress.

Fourth an adjustment model which begins as with the stress reaction model but only until

the person adjusts to the stressor. An example of this process is provided by recent research on

recovery, which has been described as the opposite to the strain process (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007).

The recovery process restores personal resources that have been depleted through the stress process

(Meijman & Mulder, 1998). Research investigating the daily fluctuations in stress and recovery

suggest an ongoing cyclical process where daily stressors create a need for recovery, and the quality

of recovery from day to day influences the experience of subsequent stressors (Sonnentag, 2003;

Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007). Finally, Frese and Zapf (1988) propose a sleeper effect in which the

strain appears some time after the period of stress exposure as in post-traumatic stress disorder.

More complex processes involving the environment are possible. For example, the support

deterioration model proposes that individuals exposed to high levels of stressors will, over time,

experience reduced social support because the providers of support are unable to maintain the high

level of support required. Longitudinal studies have supported this process (Cieslak, Knoll, &

Luszczynska, 2007). In combination, short-term and long-term approaches to stress suggest

mechanisms by which the daily experience of demands and resources is translated via affect,

behavior, and cognition into long term health outcomes.

Page 45: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 45

Implications for assessing the stress

The above review highlights a range of methodology issues for studying the stress process.

We summarize these issues in relation to three key questions of “what”, “when”, and “how” of

measurement. First “what” is the construct domain that is assessed in the stress process? Our review

shows that domain is extremely broad, ranging from organizational characteristics, through

individual differences, to long term physical health. The vast number of constructs requires

researchers to be selective about what is measured in any particular study. At the same time, it is

important to span dimensions that contribute to strain outcomes (van Veldhoven et al., 2005). The

distinction between situational constructs, such as job characteristics, and individual constructs,

such as personality, provides one basis on which to ensure broad coverage of constructs. Integration

across these domains requires greater attention to multiple levels of analysis (Bliese & Jex, 2002).

The distinction between psychological, behavioral and physiological constructs also identifies

content dimensions that require greater integration.

process

The “when” of measurement is particularly important in our review. Because the stress

process unfolds over time, it is important to assess constructs with appropriate time lags.

Longitudinal studies are increasingly reported in the literature, both for shorter-term and longer-

term dynamics. However, integrating both time frames requires consideration of multiple time lags

in longitudinal studies (Frese & Zapf, 1998). Evaluating the relative importance of different time

frames is difficult because the constructs within each time frame are different. For example,

measures of short-term stressors are often occupation specific (e.g., physical threat) while long-term

stressors tend to be more general (e.g., work overload, Beehr, 2000).

Finally, “how” the stress process is assessed is determined by the available assessment tools

and sources of research data. By far, self-report surveys dominate the assessment of work-stress.

Page 46: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 46

Because individual perceptions are so important within the psychology of the stress process, this

emphasis is understandable. However, the self-report cross-sectional surveys that remain common

in stress research are limited in the extent to which they answer “what” and “when” questions

above. The dynamics of the stress process require multiple measurement approaches including

observational and participative methods (e.g., Rutledge et al., 2009), discourse analysis (e.g.,

Harkness et al., 2005), archival data, and experiments. Within survey methods there is scope to

extend self-report measurement to include ratings by people other than the focal individual such as

co-workers and family members (e.g., Sonnentag & Kruel, 2006). The use of aggregated ratings

also enables properties at the team and organizational level to be assessed (Daniels, 2006).

The “what”, “when”, and “how” of assessment in the stress process is particularly relevant

for understanding how organizations can manage the stress process and enhance the well-being of

employees. To manage stress and well-being, organizations seek to influence the unfolding stress

process for both long-term and short-term outcomes. In the next section, we review research

relevant to various strategies for managing health and well-being.

MANAGING STRESS AND WELL-BEING

Organizations have adopted a variety of strategies to reduce strain and improve well-being.

A distinction between primary, secondary, and tertiary strategies has proved useful for describing

the different approaches to intervention. Stress management that focuses on prevention is referred

to as primary intervention and is most often targeted at the work environment; for example, work

redesign or organizational change and development. Primary interventions that involve preventive

action may also be targeted at the individual; techniques include health promotion programs,

training and education, selection and placement. In addition, many organizations engage tertiary

interventions to deal with those employees who report physical or psychological ill-health as an

Page 47: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 47

outcome of stress. In this case, employees are already suffering stress outcomes and require

treatment activities, such as stress counseling. Secondary interventions are required where

employees report responses to stressors and experience feelings of ‘being unable to cope’. In this

case, action is needed to prevent employees from developing stress-related outcomes; for example

by increasing their capacity to cope with stressors. Secondary interventions include programs that

encourage more healthy lifestyles, such as relaxation classes, or provide education on how to

develop more effective stress management skills.

Tertiary intervention

Tertiary interventions act to mitigate individual stress outcomes, for example, helping

individuals to cope with their anxiety through stress counseling. Organizations often provide

workers with access to Employee Assistance Programs (EAPs) in order to help those who are

experiencing stress outcomes. EAPs, which are often supplied by external providers, offer a

number of services, including counseling, advice and referral to other sources of support and

specialist treatment. Workplace counseling is usually offered as part of an EAP. This process is

aimed at helping people to explore a problem and finding alternative ways of dealing with it so that

something can be done about it. The overriding goal is to help clients to help themselves and to

take responsibility for their own lives. Counseling can improve employees’ psychological well-

being by increasing their confidence and self-esteem (Berridge & Cooper, 1997). Positive effects of

improved self-esteem can feed back into the stress process by boosting individuals’ resilience in the

face of stressors (forming a feedback loop between stress outcomes and the individual depicted in

Figure 1). There have been reports that companies can benefit from good return rates on their

investment in counseling services and EAPs; for example, US figures have demonstrated that

companies made savings to investment returns of between 3:1 and 15:1, following the introduction

Page 48: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 48

of stress counseling (Cooper & Cartwright, 1994), based on subsequent reductions in absenteeism

(Cooper and Sadri, 1995). However, there is a lack of systematic evaluation studies in the

published academic literature to support these findings (Reynolds, 2000). Indeed, reviews of

existing evaluation studies have reported mixed results (Shapiro, Cheesman, & Wall, 1993). From a

sample of 61 police officers, Doctor, Curtis, and Isaacs (1994) evaluated the effectiveness of an

EAP in terms of absenteeism and health but found no significant effects over a 12-week evaluation

period. However, Michie (1992, 1996) examined the effectiveness of an EAP in samples of nurses;

in both cases, significantly reduced anxiety, depression and life satisfaction were observed after six

months. In a sample of UK City Council workers, Reynolds, Taylor, and Shapiro (1993) also found

that workers’ physical and psychological well-being improved as a result of stress counseling.

Cotton and Hart (2003) suggest that EAPs may lead to improved morale amongst employees

leading to participants reporting improved satisfaction, rather than impacting on psychological

distress in terms of clinical outcomes. Other intervention programs have included an EAP as part of

a package of measures, which also included organizational-level interventions (Adkins, Quick, &

Moe, 2000; Peters & Carlson, 1999) – however, these studies did not report on the effectiveness of

individual elements of the package. Thus, there is a need for further research to establish the

effectiveness of EAPs and stress counseling programs.

Secondary intervention

Secondary interventions operate by developing individuals’ skills in identifying and

managing stressors, and by improving their coping strategies and/or by replacing maladaptive

coping styles with more successful ones. As people in stressful situations may resort to

maladaptive behaviour (such as smoking or drinking alcohol to excess) or to inappropriate

responses (such as working harder but making more mistakes or unrealistic promises), the aim of

Page 49: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 49

stress management programs is to educate employees to identify potential stressors (influencing

appraisal processes in Figure 1) and learn to apply adaptive coping strategies (influencing active

responses in Figure 1). Adaptive responses could include: planning, organizing, prioritizing

assignments and enlisting others’ support (Murphy, 1985). There is evidence that both meditation

and relaxation techniques are successful in alleviating negative responses to stress (Toivanen,

Helin, & Hänninen, 1993; Toivanen, Länsimies, Jokela, & Hänninen, 1993; Wiholm, Arnetz, &

Berg, 2000) and enhancing coping strategies (Alexander et al., 1993; Hyman, 1993), with some

indication of long-term improvement (Sheppard, Staggers, & John, 1997). Another common

technique, cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) has demonstrated significant improvements through

increased coping abilities and awareness of stress (Firth-Cozens & Hardy, 1992; Freedy & Hobfoll,

1994; Grønningæter, Hytten, Skauli, Christensen, & Ursin, 1992). In a meta-analytic review,

Richardson and Rothstein (2008) found that CBT was the most effective intervention for reducing

the negative psychological responses to stress, particularly anxiety (Cohen’s d = 1.16). There is

also evidence that stress management programs are associated with reduced absenteeism and lower

medical costs (Cooper, 1986; Giga, Faragher, & Cooper, 2003). Participation in stress management

training should not only reduce stress symptoms during the course of the intervention program, but

also provide skills to help manage stressors in the future. One explanation for the effectiveness of

CBT is its proactive focus, as opposed to the more popular, but less effective (Cohen’s d = 0.51)

techniques of relaxation and meditation, which are essentially passive (Richardson & Rothstein,

2008). While some studies have indicated long term improvements (Kushnir & Malkinson, 1993),

including an impact on general life satisfaction, suggesting that skills may transfer to non-work

domains (Reynolds et al., 1993), others have shown no long term effects (e.g., Whatmore,

Page 50: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 50

Cartwright, Cooper, Kompier, & Cooper, 1999). Techniques are often successfully combined

within stress management programs (Peters & Carlson, 1999; Tsai & Crockett, 1993).

Health promotion programs are broad in scope, ranging from purely educational schemes to

more proactive interventions, such as learning to take one’s own blood pressure, or altering one’s

lifestyle in terms of exercise or diet. These programs are based on research that has linked various

lifestyle and health habits (e.g., physical fitness, sleeping) with a reduction in the impact of work

stress (Steffy, Jones, & Noe, 1990). For example, exercise (aerobic) is generally regarded to be a

valuable technique in combating stress, and it is interesting to note that many body movements

imitate the body’s natural response to stress – for example, action based on ‘flight or fight’.

Exercise has also been found to help in improving self-esteem and mastery and thus can also

contribute to problem-focused coping (Long & Flood, 1993). However, individual differences

mean that regular exercise can help some, but not necessarily all, individuals. A causal link

between physical activity and CHD risk has been suggested (Powell, Thompson, Caspersen, &

Kendrick, 1987), with heart disease almost twice as prevalent in inactive compared with active

individuals. In a review of 95 studies, van Doornen and De Geus (1993) reported that different

methodological strategies make it difficult to differentiate the effects of physical activity and fitness

on CHD risk. However, they found that 84% of studies concluded that physically active or fit

subgroups were significantly better able to deal with the physiological consequences of stress

compared to less active or less fit subgroups. They concluded that individuals do have control over

a number of CHD risk factors, although exercise has to be combined with other beneficial actions

(e.g., diet, attitude to life) to reduce CHD risk significantly. (Daubenmier et al., 2007) found that

multicomponent programs, which included diet, exercise, and stress management, significantly

reduced coronary risk for participants after three months. Health promotion programs may also act

Page 51: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 51

as an avenue for delivering social support, as they are often group-based rather than individual-

based; thus, they can serve as social support interventions, as well as stress-reduction techniques.

Primary intervention

Primary interventions often focus on the reduction of stressors in the workplace and take

place at an organizational level. For example, where the nature of the job is leading to stress, the

task or the work environment might be subject to redesign; or, where the organization’s structure or

climate is the source of stress, a more participative management style might be encouraged. Hart

and Cotton (2003) report that in occupations characterized by unique operational pressures (e.g.,

dealing with victims and danger for police), organizational stressors (such as leadership style and

organizational climate) were the strongest determinants of levels of positive affect, accounting for

approximately 70% of the variance in levels of morale. Such research would suggest that primary

interventions in these cases should focus on workplace strategies (at group or organizational level)

to maintain employee morale.

Interventions are frequently aimed at changing the work environment, but this may not

always be practicable or desirable. Some aspects of the work environment may not be amenable to

change, particularly certain aspects of the job itself, the level of job security or job-related pay and

benefits. In such cases, it may be more appropriate to develop selection and recruitment

procedures; first to ensure that appropriate individuals are attracted and selected for these posts, and

second that appropriate support and training is offered to help post-holders. Training may be

considered as primary prevention, rather than secondary, as it enhances task-related knowledge and

skills. Training can also be useful in building employees’ perceptions of their own capability, in

terms of self-efficacy (Parker et al., 2006). Richardson and Rothstein (2008) found that

Page 52: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 52

interventions that aimed to help participants to build personal resources and improve stress

management skills were highly effective (Cohen’s d = 1.41).

Work redesign

There is a relatively limited literature on stress reduction at the primary level, as few studies

have focused on work redesign or restructuring (Giga et al., 2003; Israel, Baker, Goldenhar,

Heaney, & Schurman, 1996). However, evidence suggests that primary interventions have a

significant impact both on individuals’ health and well-being and on organizational outcomes,

including productivity and absenteeism. Reviewing findings from 1500 projects funded by the

Swedish Working Life Fund, Brulin and Nilsson (1994) found that productivity improved on

average by 10%, including production errors and delivery times. Kawakami, Araki, Kawashima,

Masumoto, and Hayashi (1997) demonstrated significant effects on stress symptoms and absence

levels following a one-year intervention program involving changes to the work environment, job

redesign and training. Another means to reduce employees’ stress levels focuses upon improving

communication. Cartwright, Cooper, and Whatmore (2000) found that improvements in

communications within a UK Government department led to significant enhancements in job

satisfaction and perceptions of control. Evans, Johansson, and Rydstedt (1999) also found

significant reductions in stress symptoms. However, neither of these studies examined

performance.

Participation and autonomy

Many jobs are characterized by high demands, where the nature of the demands cannot be

changed. However, the negative effects of high job demand can be mitigated by enhancing

opportunities for control, according to the DC model of stress. This may be achieved through

employee participation or increased autonomy, As noted earlier, job control can have positive

Page 53: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 53

effects as it leads to the feeling of ‘being in control’ even where that control is not actually

exercised. Job autonomy enhances self-efficacy (Axtell & Parker, 2003; Parker, 1998) which in turn

mediates the link to personal initiative (Frese & Fay, 2001). Increasing job control also leads to

enhanced mastery (Bandura, 2001) enabling individuals to cope more effectively with perceived

stressors. Control can be introduced in a number of ways, such as developing autonomous work

groups. Studies of such work groups have found positive effects on productivity and other work

criteria, but a lack of improvement in well-being, motivation and absenteeism (Goodman, Devadas,

& Hughson, 1988). Changes to the level of skill discretion and authority over decisions are also

outcomes resulting from participation interventions (Theorell, Emdad, Arnetz, & Weingarten,

2001). To relieve job strain, it is suggested that workers should be given greater scope for decision-

making and use of discretion at work but at the same time not to overreach individual capabilities in

the quest to obtain more substantial job responsibilities. As noted earlier, there are individual

differences in employees’ responses to enhanced control – whereas one employee may view

additional autonomy as a challenge, another may view it as a source of stress.

Evaluation studies of interventions using participation and autonomy have had mixed

results. For example, Reynolds et al. (1993) found no significant changes in well-being or

absenteeism in UK public sector workers as the result of a participation intervention. Similarly,

Heaney et al. (1993) found that worker well-being failed to improve, whilst Mikkelsen and

Gundersen (2003) found a short-term improvement in subjective health, but this was not maintained

in the longer term. Other studies have shown that work reorganisation interventions result in

significant reductions in depression (Bond & Bunce, 2001) and improved attitudes towards

innovation (Bunce & West, 1996). Bond and Bunce (2001) demonstrated that job control mediated

the effect of the intervention on employees’ mental health. In a later study, Bond, Flaxman, and

Page 54: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 54

Bunce (2008) further showed that this effect is moderated by the individual difference variable,

psychological flexibility, such that those who were high in psychological flexibility benefited most

from the intervention in terms of mental health.

Whilst interventions that focus on work design alone are relatively uncommon, interventions

combining changes to the work environment with increased participation are increasingly employed

by organizations (Giga et al., 2003). Evidence suggests that this combination is particularly

powerful in terms of improving absence and performance rates (e.g., Bond & Bunce, 2001; Matrajt,

1992). For example, Terra (1995) reported a 50% decrease in sickness absence and improved

productivity following the introduction of job redesign and self-regulating teams.

Increased job control may be not an appropriate means of reducing stress for all individuals.

Although many will find that low control is a source of stress, there are individual differences in the

degree of psychological strain experienced. The increased responsibility and decision latitude

associated with greater autonomy can be perceived as an unwanted burden by some individuals,

indicating that stress reduction measures targeted at increasing worker autonomy will not be

suitable for all workers. Worker participation and careful monitoring of the effects of any changes

is needed when implementing stress reduction measures. Sparks et al. (2001) recommended that

relevant training support be provided; for example, where appropriate, problem-solving sessions

could be held between supervisors and workers to identify job demands or stressors. Parker and

Sprigg (1999) argue that increasing job control for passive employees will have little effect on

strain; for such employees, the most successful strategy would be to reduce demands. Other options

would include training passive employees to become more proactive (using more active coping

strategies and building individuals’ ability to manage demands confidently). Strategies could seek

to increase workers’ perceived control so that they can cope more effectively (Spector, 1998).

Page 55: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 55

Training that enhances workers’ control beliefs may help to protect health and well-being (Meier et

al., 2008). In addition, there is evidence that cultural differences exist in the response of employees

to interventions that increase job control, such as empowerment (Hui, Au, & Fock, 2004).

Support networks

Consistent with the ‘iso-strain hypothesis’, external support networks have been found to

buffer individuals against psychological strain during times of crisis (Rodin & Salovey, 1989).

Individuals with external support tend to rely more on active coping strategies than do those without

such support (Billings & Moos, 1984). Establishing co-worker support groups has been found to

improve psychological well-being (Bagnara, Baldasseroni, & Tartaglia, 1999). Grossman and

Silverstein (1993) evaluated the effectiveness of support groups for healthcare professionals; the

study found that workers reported reduced stress and improved performance at work. However, the

groups experienced high dropout rates, perhaps caused by those workers who most needed help

leaving the group. Greater success was associated with groups who had high autonomy and

decision latitude; these groups perceived their work as more stimulating and received increased

feedback from supervisors (Eriksson, Moser, Unden, & Orth-Gomer, 1992).

Support groups have been successfully combined with individual-level techniques in stress

interventions, such as CBT and relaxation; evaluation studies indicating that in addition to reducing

stress symptoms, participants’ support seeking skills and adequacy of coping also improved (Elliott

& Maples, 1991; Lees & Ellis, 1990; McCue & Sachs, 1991). However, as with individual-level

interventions, there is some doubt concerning long-term effects. In combination with primary

organizational interventions, such as work redesign, training and communication, long-term

maintenance of low levels of stress has been reported (Griffin, Hart, & Wilson-Evered, 2000;

Kalimo & Toppinen, 1999). For example, Kalimo and Toppinen (1999) examined work and health

Page 56: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 56

related factors over a 10-year period for 11,000 forestry industry workers who participated in a

stress intervention program involving work redesign, training and co-worker support groups. The

study found that a majority of staff rated their psychological working capacity as good and that

stress remained low. Caution needs to be exercised in respect of alleged beneficial effects of social

support, because there are occasions when social interaction (and even inappropriate counseling)

can have a detrimental effect upon an individual’s health, for example where conflict and strife is

inherent in social relationships.

Summary of intervention studies

There is substantial evidence to suggest that many stress intervention programs are effective

in mitigating stress symptoms (Berridge & Cooper, 1997; Highley-Marchington, Cooper, & Britain,

1998). However, much of the evidence is focused on secondary and tertiary measures, such as

CBT, relaxation and stress counseling, which are aimed at individuals who feel ‘stressed’ or display

stress outcomes, such as ill-health. Although secondary and tertiary interventions are effective

when targeted at ‘high risk’ individuals or groups, there has been a tendency for organizations to

employ such programs for the workforce as a whole. This has been described as the ‘inoculation

approach’ to stress, as it focuses on the consequences, rather than the sources of stress, reflecting an

approach that stress is an inherent and enduring feature of the working environment. Isolated

programs can have temporary effects in reducing stress (Murphy, 1985): although stress outcomes

are improved as a result of treatment and job perceptions become more positive, these changes can

decay over time if employees return to an unchanged work environment, with the same level of

stressor exposure. There is also the danger that such programs tend to attract the ‘worried well’

(Sutherland & Cooper, 1991), rather than those who most need help, particularly where programs

are used across the workforce, rather than being targeted at ‘high risk’ individuals. The integrative

Page 57: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 57

framework (Figure 1) emphasizes the importance of feedback loops in the stress process, suggesting

that interventions which feed back to influence the work environment and the person (and the P-E

relationship) will be most effective over time, e.g., CBT and skills training which help to build

personal resources.

Reviews of stress intervention programs (Burke, 1993; Cooper & Payne, 1988; Kahn &

Byosiere, 1992; Karasek, 1992) have suggested that such programs often fail to emphasize

prevention at source. However, although still comparatively rare, there are examples of multi-

perspective approaches to stress intervention, which include measures aimed at both prevention and

remedial action (Giga et al., 2003). Figure 1 highlights the multiple facets and complex dynamics

of the stress process, suggesting that interventions targeted at both short-term and longer-term

processes are likely to be the most effective. Such approaches can have a powerful effect, not only

in making long term improvements in mental and physical well-being (Elo, Leppanen, & Sillanpaa,

1998; Munz, Kohler, & Greenberg, 2001), but also in enhancing relationships, industrial relations,

work climate and increasing awareness of stress as an organizational issue (Lourijsen et al, 1999;

Nijhuis et al, 1996; Poelmans et al, 1999; Wynne & Rafferty, 1999). In addition, evaluation studies

report significant reductions in sickness absenteeism (Lourijsen, Houtman, Kompier, &

Grundemann, 1999; Munz et al., 2001; Poelmans, Compernolle, De Neve, Buelens, & Rombouts,

1999), mishaps and suicides (Adkins et al., 2000), and improved productivity (Munz et al., 2001).

Cox et al (2000) recommend that a package of measures is more likely to succeed, particularly

where measures are to be implemented at primary and secondary levels and involve preventive

action in addition to tertiary action. The importance of developing an intervention strategy based

on a risk assessment of the health and safety hazards has been emphasized by a number of

researchers (Clarke & Cooper, 2004; T. Cox et al., 2000; Giga et al., 2003). Nevertheless, there is

Page 58: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 58

currently a lack of evaluation studies that are needed to support the effectiveness of such

intervention strategies, particularly over the long term.

CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Studies of the stress process ask a deceptively simple question: why does work sometimes

have a negative impact on the health and well-being of individuals? We identified two major

themes that highlight why a straightforward answer to this question has not been forthcoming:

reciprocal time cycles, and the integration of positive well-being. We explored these themes in

relation to some of the major theories of the stress process and developed an integrative framework

for comparing time frames and integrating positive health outcomes. We now review each of these

themes in terms of their implication for future research and practice.

Time cycles

First, stress involves an ongoing process of interaction between the individual and the

environment that unfolds over both short-term and longer-term time scales. Short term dynamic

processes, such as cognitive appraisal, are shaped by the long-term context and can also influence

long-term outcomes. This system of embedded cycles means that even sophisticated research

designs will not reflect the multiple time frames and causal sequences of the full stress process.

This research challenge of complex time cycles is not unique to stress-related research, but

does suggest the kinds of questions that are relevant. For example, what is the correct time lag for

estimating the impact of work overload on depression? How do short term-recovery strategies map

onto long term health outcomes such as heart disease?

Researchers have made many inroads into answering these questions and the scope of stress

research is daunting. Research designs that range from laboratory studies measuring endocrinal

function in the workplace to national studies interviewing cohorts of employees over decades are

Page 59: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 59

commendable feats of researcher sweat and inspiration. Nevertheless, cross-sectional studies in

stress are still very common (de Lange et al., 2003; Zapf et al., 1996), and likely to remain popular

as more organizations seek to evaluate the risks associated with stressors and strains in their

workplace. Unfortunately, the scope for these single-time and single-organization surveys to

contribute new theoretical understandings about the stress process itself is diminishing.

Promising developments for understanding more dynamic processes can be seen in the use

of methods that capture unfolding dynamics such as time sampling methods. However, addressing

issues of time is not only a matter of adopting a particular research design. It is also important to

theorize about the role of time cycles in each study, speculate about their impact on the stress

process, and integrate practical outcomes for the management of stress in organizations. We

propose three questions that can be considered in: a) what is the time lag of effects, b) how does

reciprocal causation operate, and, c) what is the link between short term and long term processes?

These three questions can be addressed not only through specific research designs but as part of any

study that incorporates the stress process. Even cross-sectional studies can articulate how constructs

at a single time point reflect the effect of relationships over time. In this way, researchers can

develop a more systematic dialog about time cycles in the stress process.

Integrating positive features of well-being

The second theme concerned integrating positive aspects of health and well-being within

studies of the stress process. The negative outcomes of the transaction are clearly important but, if

considered in isolation, provide a necessarily limited view of the link between work and well-being.

At the extreme, a focus on negative outcomes might actually help to shape the stress process itself

by influencing the scope of research that is conducted and influencing the attention of

policymakers.

Page 60: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 60

Increasingly, research studies are integrating positive dimensions of affect, cognition, and

behavior at various stages of the stress process. For example, both individual and environmental

factors provide positive resources for coping, positive affect shapes the stress process as both an

independent and dependent variable, and active mental health is an important long term outcome of

dynamic processes. The integration of positive dimensions is sometimes the focus of research

studies, such as those integrating engagement and burnout. More often, positive constructs can be

incorporated with a range of constructs to provide a more fully developed model of the stress

process in a particular context.

A potential concern with integrating fully elaborated models of well-being into the stress

process is that the topic of stress might become so broad as to be meaningless. In that case, would it

be better to subsume stress into more general theoretical frameworks such as employee motivation,

well-being, or work design? In part, the answer to this question is ‘yes’. The theoretical frameworks

for these topics continue to develop and draw on broader fields of psychology and other disciplines.

A better understanding of the dynamic processes of which stress is a part might be achieved by

more concerted efforts toward building general motivational frameworks.

In part, the answer is also ‘no’ because there are negative outcomes of the stress process that

are important for different stakeholders to identify and to differentiate from positive outcomes no

matter how desirable the latter might be. In particular, organizations and public bodies with

responsibility for managing risk and harm have a particular interest in the negative consequences of

the stress process.

The above issues denote some ambivalence about the direction of future stress research in

relation to other topics in work motivation. However, the study of stress is a multidisciplinary and

energetic field that will continue to develop in multiple directions. As with our first theme, we

Page 61: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 61

recommend some questions that can be addressed in most studies of the stress process across a

broad domain of research studies: a) can positive and negative processes be differentiated at each

stage of the process, b) how do positive and negative constructs combine and interact with each

other throughout the stress process? Again, these are considerations that can be addressed directly

in research designs or as an intrinsic part of the theoretical development and practical application in

individual studies.

Conclusion

The stress process engages fundamental experiences of work for individuals and has

important management and strategic consequences for organizations. Diverse studies over the past

40 years show the variety of mechanisms through which work can influence employee well being.

The field continues to develop a better understanding of the ongoing transaction between the

individual and their environment. Future directions include greater emphasis on the time cycles in

the stress process and greater integration of positive constructs to understand the sometimes

negative consequences of work.

Page 62: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 62

REFERENCES

Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed

behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 53-63.

Adkins, J. A., Quick, J. C., & Moe, K. O. (2000). Building world class performance in changing

times. In L. R. Muphy & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Healthy and productive work: An

international perspective (pp. 107–131). London: Taylor & Francis.

Aguinis, H., Beaty, J. C., Boik, R. J., & Pierce, C. A. (2005). Effect size and power in assessing

moderating effects of categorical variables using multiple regression: A 30-year review.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 94-106.

Alexander, C. N., Swanson, G. C., Rainforth, M. V., Carlisle, T. W., Todd, C. C., & Oates Jr, R. M.

(1993). Effects of the Transcendental Meditation program on stress-reduction, health, and

employee development in two occupational settings. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 6, 245-

262.

Alfredsson, L., & Theorell, T. (1983). Job characteristics of occupations and myocardial infarction

risk: effect of possible confounding factors. Social Science and Medicine, 17(20), 1497-

1503.

Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and

content. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 338-375.

Axtell, C. M., & Parker, S. K. (2003). Promoting role breadth self-efficacy through involvement,

work redesign and training. Human Relations, 56(1), 112-131.

Bagnara, S., Baldasseroni, A., & Tartaglia, R. (1999). Italy: A school of nursing. In M. A. Kompier

& C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Preventing Stress, Improving Productivity: European case studies in

the workplace (pp. 297-397). London: Routledge.

Page 63: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 63

Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & Dollard, M. F. (2008). How job demands affect partners'

experience of exhaustion: Integrating work-family conflict and crossover theory. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 93(4), 901-910.

Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33(4), 344-

358.

Bandura, A. (2001). Social Cognitive Theory: An Agentic Perspective.

Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of thought and behavior. In E. T.

Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of Motivation and Cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 93-

130). New York: Guilford Press.

Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental

processes.

Beehr, T. (2000). An organizational psychology meta-model of occupational stress. In C. L. Cooper

(Ed.), Theories of Organizational Stress (pp. 6-27). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Beehr, T. A., & Franz, T. M. (1987). The current debate about the meaning of job stress . In J. M.

Ivancevich & D. C. Ganster (Eds.), Job Stress: From Theory to Suggestions (pp. 5-18).

New York: The Haworth Press.

Beehr, T. A., & Glazer, S. (2005). Organizational role stress. In J. Barling, E. K. Kelloway, & M. R.

Frone (Eds.), Handbook of Work Stress (pp. 7-33). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Beehr, T. A., & Newman, J. E. (1978). Job stress, employee health, and organizational

effectiveness: A facet analysis, model, and literature review. Personnel Psychology, 31(4),

665-99.

Berridge, J., & Cooper, C. L. (1997). Employee assistance programmes and workplace counselling.

Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

Page 64: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 64

Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1984). Coping, stress, and social resources among adults with

unipolar depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 877-891.

Bliese, P. D., & Castro, C. A. (2000). Role clarity, work overload and organizational support:

Multilevel evidence of the importance of support. Work and Stress, 14(1), 65-73.

Bliese, P. D., & Jex, S. M. (2002). Incorporating a multilevel perspective into occupational stress

research: Theoretical, methodological, and practical implications. Journal of Occupational

Health Psychology, 7(3), 265-277.

Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying personality in the stress process.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 890-902.

Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2001). Job control mediates change in a work reorganization

intervention for stress reduction. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6(4), 290-

302.

Bond, F. W., Flaxman, P. E., & Bunce, D. (2008). The influence of psychological flexibility on

work redesign: mediated moderation of a work reorganization intervention. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 93(3), 645-654.

Bongers, P. M., de Winter, C. R., Kompier, M. A. J., & Hildebrandt, V. H. (1993). Psychosocial

factors at work and musculoskeletal disease. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment

and Health, 19(5), 297-312.

Borg, V., Kristensen, T. S., & Burr, H. (2000). Work environment and changes in self-rated health:

a five year follow-up study. Stress Medicine, 16(1), 37-47.

Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The Structure of Psychological Well-being. Oxford: Aldine.

Brosschot, J. F., Pieper, S., & Thayer, J. F. (2005). Expanding stress theory: Prolonged activation

and perseverative cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 1043-1049.

Page 65: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 65

Brulin, G., & Nilsson, T. (1994). Arbetsutveckling och förbättrad produktivitet. Stockholm:

Arbetslivsfonden.

Bunce, D., & West, M. A. (1996). Stress management and innovation interventions at work. Human

Relations, 49(2), 209-224.

Burke, R. J. (1993). Organizational-level interventions to reduce occupational stressors. Work and

Stress, 7(1), 77-87.

Byron, K. (2005). A meta-analytic review of work–family conflict and its antecedents. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 67(2), 169-198.

Campion, M. A., & Lord, R. G. (1982). A control systems conceptualization of the goal-setting and

changing process. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 30, 265-287.

Caplan, R. D., Cobb, S., French, J. R. P., Harrison, R., & Pinneau, S. R. (1980). Job demands and

worker health (NIOSH Publication No. 75-160). Washington, DC: Department of Health,

Education, and Welfare.

Carayon, P. (1992). A longitudinal study of job design and worker strain: preliminary results. In

Stress & Well-being at Work: Assessments and Interventions for Occupational Mental

Health, (pp. 19-32). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Carayon, P. (1993). A longitudinal test of Karasek's Job Strain model among office workers. Work

& Stress, 7(4), 299-314.

Carayon, P., Smith, M. J., & Haims, M. C. (1999). Work organization, job stress, and work-related

musculoskeletal disorders. Human Factors, 41(4).

Cartwright, S., Cooper, C. L., & Whatmore, L. (2000). Improving communications and health in a

government. In Healthy and Productive Work: An International Perspective (pp. 67-85).

Page 66: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 66

Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. Motivation

and Emotion, 30(Emotion), 105-110.

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A

Control-Process View. Psychological Review, 97(1), 19-35.

Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective

responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 67(2), 1731-1740.

Cavanaugh, M. A., Boswell, W. R., Roehling, M. V., & Boudreau, J. W. (2000). An empirical

examination of self-reported work stress among U. S. managers. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 85(1), 65-74.

Chandola, T., Brunner, E., & Marmot, M. (2006). Chronic stress at work and the metabolic

syndrome: prospective study. British Medical Journal, 332(7540), 521-525.

Cieslak, R., Knoll, N., & Luszczynska, A. (2007). Reciprocal relations among job demands, job

control, and social support are moderated by neuroticism: A cross-lagged analysis. Journal

of Vocational Behavior, 71(1), 84-96.

Clarke, S., & Cooper, C. L. (2004). Managing the Risk of Workplace Stress: Health and Safety

Hazards. London: Routledge.

Cooper, C. L. (1986). Job distress: recent research and the emerging role of the clinical

occupational psychologist. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 39, 325-331.

Cooper, C. L., & Cartwright, S. (1994). Healthy Mind; Healthy Organization--A Proactive

Approach to Occupational Stress. Human Relations, 47(4), 455.

Cooper, C. L., Clarke, S., & Rowbottom, A. M. (1999). Occupational stress, job satisfaction and

well-being in anaesthetists. Stress Medicine, 15(2), 115-126.

Page 67: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 67

Cooper, C. L., Dewe, P., & O'Driscoll, M. P. (2001). Organizational Stress: A Review and Critique

of Theory, Research, and Applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Cooper, C. L., Liukkonen, P., & Cartwright, S. (1996). Stress prevention in the workplace:

Assessing the costs and benefits to organisations. Luxembourg: Office for official

publications of the European communities.

Cooper, C. L., & Payne, R. (1988). Causes, Coping and Consequences of Stress at Work.

Chichester: Wiley.

Cooper, C. L., & Sadri, G. (1995). The impact of stress counseling at work. In R. Crandall & P. L.

Perrewe (Eds.), Occupational Stress: A Handbook (pp. 271-282). Washington, DC: Taylor

& Francis.

Cotton, P., & Hart, P. M. (2003). Occupational wellbeing and performance: A review of

organisational health research. Australian Psychologist, 38(2), 118-127.

Cox, T., Griffiths, A., Barlowe, C., Randall, K., Thomson, L., & Rial-Gonzalez, E. (2000).

Organizational Interventions for Work Stress: A Risk Management Approach. Sudbury:

HSE Books.

Cox, T., & MacKay, C. (1981). A Transactional Approach to Occupational Stress. In N. J. Corlett

& Richardson (Eds.), Stress, Work Design, and Productivity (pp. 75-95). London: Wiley.

Crant, J. M. (2000). Proactive behavior in organizations. Journal of Management, 26(3), 435.

Cummings, T. G., & Cooper, C. L. (1979). A Cybernetic Framework for Studying Occupational

Stress. Human Relations, 32(5), 395-418.

Cummings, T. G., & Cooper, C. L. (1998). A cybernetic theory of organizational stress. In C. L.

Cooper (Ed.), Theories of Organizational Stress (pp. 101-121). Oxford: Oxford University

Press.

Page 68: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 68

Cunningham, C. J., & De La Rosa, G. M. (2008). The interactive effects of proactive personality

and work-family interference on well-being. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,

13(3), 271-82.

Daniels, K. (2006). Rethinking job characteristics in work stress research. Human Relations, 59(3),

267-290.

Daniels, K., Beesley, N., Cheyne, A., & Wimalasiri, V. (2008). Coping processes linking the

demands-control-support model, affect and risky decisions at work. Human Relations,

61(6), 845-874.

Daniels, K., & Guppy, A. (1994). Occupational stress, social support, job control, and

psychological well-being. Human Relations, 47(12), 1523-1544.

Daniels, K., Harris, C., & Briner, R. B. (2004). Linking work conditions to unpleasant affect:

Cognition, categorization and goals. Journal of Occupational and Organizational

Psychology, 77(3), 343-363.

Darr, W., & Johns, G. (2008). Work strain, health, and absenteeism: A meta-analysis., 13.

Daubenmier, J. J., Weidner, G., Sumner, M. D., Mendell, N., Merritt-Worden, T., Studley, J., et al.

(2007). The contribution of changes in diet, exercise, and stress management to changes in

coronary risk in women and men in the multisite cardiac lifestyle intervention program.

Annals of behavioral medicine: a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 33(1),

57.

DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The impact of daily stress on health and mood:

psychological and social resources as mediators. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 54(3), 486-95.

Page 69: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 69

Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., de Jonge, J., Janssen, P. P. M., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). Burnout

and engagement at work as a function of demands and control. Scandinavian Journal of

Work, Environment and Health, 27(4), 279-286.

Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute Stressors and Cortisol Responses: A Theoretical

Integration and Synthesis of Laboratory Research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 36.

Doctor, R. S., Curtis, D., & Isaacs, G. (1994). Psychiatric morbidity in policemen and the effect of

brief psychotherapeutic intervention: A pilot study. Stress Medicine, 10(3), 151-157.

van der Doef, M., & Maes, S. (1999). The Job Demand-Control (-Support) Model and

psychological well-being: a review of 20 years of empirical research. Work & Stress, 13(2),

87-114.

ter Doest, L., Maes, S., Gebhardt, W. A., & Koelewijn, H. (2006). Personal goal facilitation through

work: Implications for employee satisfaction and well-being. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 55(2), 192-219.

Domenighetti, G., D’Avanzo, B., & Bisig, B. (2000). Health effects of flexible employment among

employees in the swiss general population. International Journal of Health Services, 30,

477-90.

Dormann, C., Fay, D., Zapf, D., & Frese, M. (2006). A state-trait analysis of job satisfaction: On the

effect of core self-evaluations. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 55(1), 27-51.

Edwards, J. R. (1992). A cybernetic theory of stress, coping, and well-being in organizations. The

Academy of Management Review, 17(2), 238-274.

Edwards, J. R. (1998). Cybernetic theory of stress, coping, and well-being: review and extension to

work and family. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of Organizational Stress (pp. 122–152).

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Page 70: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 70

Edwards, J. R., & Cooper, C. L. (1990). The person-environment fit approach to stress: Recurring

problems and some suggested solutions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 11(4), 293-

307.

Elliot, A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motivation. Motivation and

Emotion, 30(2), 111-116.

Elliot, A. J., & Fryer, J. W. (2007). The goal construct in psychology. In J. Y. Shah & W. Gardner

(Eds.), Handbook of Motivation Science. New York: Guilford Press.

Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach–Avoidance Motivation in Personality: Approach

and Avoidance Temperaments and Goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

82(5), 804-818.

Elliott, T. R., & Maples, S. (1991). Stress management training for employees experiencing

corporate acquisition. Journal of Employment Counseling, 28(3), 107-14.

Elo, A. L., Leppanen, A., & Sillanpaa, P. (1998). Applicability of survey feedback for an

occupational health method in stress management. Occupational Medicine, 48(3), 181-188.

Elsass, P. M., & Veiga, J. F. (1997). Job control and job strain: A test of three models. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 2, 195-211.

Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy

motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(6), 1055-1066.

Eriksson, I., Moser, V., Unden, A. L., & Orth-Gomer, K. (1992). Using knowledge and discussion

to decrease stress in Swedish public administration officials. In Conditions of Work Digest

(Vol. 11, pp. 214-214). Geneva: International Labour Office.

Evans, G. W., Johansson, G., & Rydstedt, L. (1999). Hassles on the job: a study of a job

intervention with urban bus drivers. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20(2), 199-208.

Page 71: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 71

Ferguson, E., & Cox, T. (1997). The functional dimensions of coping scale: Theory, reliability and

validity. British Journal of Health Psychology, 2, 109-130.

Firth-Cozens, J., & Hardy, G. E. (1992). Occupational stress, clinical treatment and changes in job

perceptions. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 65(2), 81-88.

Folkman, S. (2008). The case for positive emotions in the stress process. Anxiety Stress Coping,

21(1), 3-14.

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The relationship between coping and emotion: implications

for theory and research. Social Science and Medicine, 26(3), 309-17.

Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: Pitfalls and Promise. Annual Review of

Psychology, 55(1), 745-774.

Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2,

300-319.

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The Role of Positive Emotions in Positive Psychology: The Broaden-

and-Build Theory of Positive Emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218-224.

Freedy, J. R., & Hobfoll, S. E. (1994). Stress inoculation for reduction of burnout: A conservation

of resources approach. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 6(4), 311-325.

French, J. R. P., Caplan, R. D., & Harrison, R. V. (1982). The Mechanisms of Stress and Strain.

Chichester: Wiley.

Frese, M., & Fay, D. (2001). Pesonal initiative: An active performance concept for work in the 21st

century. In B. M. Staw & R. M. Sutton (Eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior (Vol.

23, pp. 133-187). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Page 72: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 72

Frese, M., Garst, H., & Fay, D. (2007). Making things happen: Reciprocal relationships between

work characteristics and personal initiative in a four-wave longitudinal structural equation

model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(4), 1084-1102.

Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1998). Methodological issues in the study of work stress: Objective vs

subjective measurement of work stress and the question of longitudinal studies. In C. L.

Cooper & R. Payne (Eds.), Causes, Coping and Consequences of Stress at Work (pp. 375-

411). Chichester: Wiley.

Fryer, D. (1986). Employment deprivation and personal agency during unemployment: A critical

discussion of Jahoda's explanation of the psychological effects of unemployment. Social

Behaviour, 1(1), 3-23.

Fryer, D. (1998). Labour market disadvantage, deprivation and mental health. . In P. J. D. Drenth,

H. Thierry, & de Wolff (Eds.), Handbook of work and organizational, Vol. 2: Work

psychology (2nd ed., pp. 215-227). Hove, England: Taylor & Francis.

Ganster, D. C. (2008). Measurement challenges for studying work-related stressors and strains.

Human Resource Management Review, 18(4), 259-270.

Ganster, D. C., & Schaubroeck, J. (1991). Work Stress and Employee Health. Journal of

Management, 17(2), 235-271.

George, J. M. (1990). Personality, affect, and behavior in groups. Journal of Applied Psychology,

75(2), 107-116.

Giga, S., Faragher, B., & Cooper, C. L. (2003). Identification of good practice in stress

prevention/management.

In J. Jordan, E. Gurr, S. Tinline, S. Giga, B. Faragher, & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Beacons of

Page 73: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 73

Excellence in Stress Prevention. HSE Research Report No. 133 (pp. 1-45). Sudbury: HSE

Books.

Glendon, I., & Coles, F. (2001). Stress in Ambulance Staff. In P. A. Hancock & P. A. Desmond

(Eds.), Stress, Workload, and Fatigue (pp. 167-199). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Golembiewski, R. T., Munzenrider, R., & Carter, D. (1983). Phases of progressive burnout and

their work site covariants: Critical issues in OD research and praxis. The Journal of Applied

Behavioral Science, 19(4), 461-481.

Goodman, P. S., Devadas, R., & Hughson, T. L. (1988). Groups and productivity: Analyzing the

effectiveness of self-managing teams. In J. P. Campbell & J. R. Campbell (Eds.),

Productivity in organizations (pp. 295-327). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotion regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize

emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 95-110.

Grant, S., & Langan-Fox, J. (2006). Occupational stress, coping and strain: The

combined/interactive effect of the Big Five traits. Personality and Individual Differences,

41(4), 719-732.

Gray, J. A. (1994). Three fundamental emotion systems. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The

Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. (pp. 243-247). New York: Oxford University

Press.

Griffin, M. A. (1997). Interaction Between Individuals and Situations: Using HLM Procedures to

Estimate Reciprocal Relationships. Journal of Management, 23(6), 759.

Griffin, M. A., Hart, P. M., & Wilson-Evered, E. (2000). Using employee opinion surveys to

improve organizational health. Healthy and Productive Work: An International Perspective.

Page 74: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 74

Grønningæter, H., Hytten, K., Skauli, G., Christensen, C. C., & Ursin, H. (1992). Improved health

and coping by physical exercise or cognitive behavioral stress management training in a

work environment. Psychology and Health, 7(2), 147-163.

Grossman, A. H., & Silverstein, C. (1993). Facilitating support groups for professionals working

with people with AIDS. Social Work, 38(2), 144-51.

Halbesleben, J. (2006). Sources of social support and burnout: A meta-analytic test of the

conservation of resources model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(5), 1134-1145.

Harkness, A. M. B., Long, B. C., Bermbach, N., Patterson, K., Jordan, S., & Kahn, H. (2005).

Talking about work stress: Discourse analysis and implications for stress interventions.

Work & Stress, 19(2), 121-136.

Hart, P. M. (1994). Teacher quality of work life: Integrating work experiences, psychological

distress and morale. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 67(2), 109-

132.

Hart, P. M., Wearing, A. J., & Headey, B. (1995). Police stress and well-being: Integrating

personality, coping and daily work experiences. Journal of Occupational and

Organizational Psychology, 68(2), 133-156.

Heaney, C. A., Israel, B. A., Schurman, S. J., Baker, E. A., House, J. S., & Hugentobler, M. (1993).

Industrial relations, worksite stress reduction, and employee well-being: A participatory

action research investigation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14(5), 495-510.

Heaphy, E. D., & Dutton, J. E. (2008). Positive social interactions and the human Body at work:

Linking organizations and physiology. The Academy of Management Review, 33(1), 137-

162.

Page 75: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 75

Hellerstedt, W. L., & Jeffery, R. W. (1997). The association of job strain and health behaviours in

men and women. International Journal of Epidemiology, 26(3), 575-83.

Hemenover, S. H. (2003). Individual differences in rate of affect change: studies in affective

chronometry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 121-31.

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280-1300.

Highley-Marchington, J. C., Cooper, C. L., & Britain, G. (1998). An Assessment of Employee

Assistance and Workplace Counselling Programmes in British Organisations. Sudbury:

HSE Books.

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources. A new attempt at conceptualizing stress.

American Psychologist, 44(3), 513-24.

Houston, D., & Allt, S. K. (1997). Psychological distress and error making among junior house

officers. British Journal of Health Psychology, 2, 141-152.

HSE (Health and Safety Executive). (2008). Work Related Stress. London: Health and Safety

Executive.

Hui, M. K., Au, K., & Fock, H. (2004). Empowerment effects across cultures. Journal of

International Business Studies, 35(1), 46-61.

Hyman, R. B. (1993). Evaluation of an intervention for staff in a long-term care facility using a

retrospective pretest design. Evaluation and the Health Professions, 16(2), 212-224.

Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenbeck, J. R. (1991). The structure of work: Job design and roles. In M.

Dunnette & L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd

ed., Vol. 2, pp. 165-207). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Page 76: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 76

Israel, B. A., Baker, E. A., Goldenhar, L. M., Heaney, C. A., & Schurman, S. J. (1996).

Occupational stress, safety, and health: conceptual framework and principles for effective

prevention interventions. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1(3), 261-86.

Jackson, S. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1985). A meta-analysis and conceptual critique of research on role

ambiguity and role conflict in work settings. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 36(1), 16-78.

Jex, S. M., & Beehr, T. A. (1991). Emerging theoretical and methodological issues in the study of

work-related stress.

Jimmieson, N. L., & Terry, D. J. (1999). The moderating role of task characteristics in determining

responses to a stressful work simulation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20(5), 709-

736.

Johnson, J. V., Hall, E. M., & Theorell, T. (1989). Combined effects of job strain and social

isolation on cardiovascular disease morbidity and mortality in a random sample of the

Swedish male working population. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and Health,

15(4), 271-9.

Jones, F., Conner, M., McMillan, B., & Ferguson, E. (2007). Impact of daily mood, work hours,

and iso-strain variables on self-reported health behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology,

92(6), 1731-1740.

de Jonge, J., & Dormann, C. (2003). The DISC model: Demand-induced strain compensation

mechanisms in job stress. In M. F. Dollard, H. R. Winefield, & H. R. Winefield (Eds.),

Occupational stress in the service professions (pp. 43-74). London: Taylor & Francis.

de Jonge, J., & Dormann, C. (2006). Stressors, resources, and strain at work: a longitudinal test of

the triple-match principle. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(6), 1359-74.

Page 77: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 77

Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of self-esteem,

neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core

construct?.

Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., & Durham, C. C. (1997). The Dispositional Causes of Job Satisfaction:

A Core-Evaluations Approach. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19, 151-188.

Kahn, R. L., & Byosiere, P. (1992). Stress in organizations, In: Dunnette, MD and Hough, LM

(Eds) Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 3. In M. D. Dunnette &

L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 571-

650). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Kahn, R. L., Wolfe, D. M., Quinn, R. P., Snoek, J. D., & Rosenthal, R. A. (1964). Occupational

stress: Studies in Role Conflict and Ambiguity. New York: Wiley.

Kalimo, R., & Toppinen, S. (1999). Finland: Organizational well-being. Ten years of research and

development in a forest industry corporation. In M. Kompier & C. L. Cooper (Eds.),

Preventing Stress, Improving Productivity. European Case Studies in the Workplace (pp.

52-85). London: Routledge.

Karasek, R. (1992). Stress prevention through work reorganization: A summary of 19 international

case studies. In V. Di Martino (Ed.), (Vol. 11). Geneva.

Karasek, R., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy Work: Stress, Productivity, and the Reconstruction of

Working Life. Basic Books.

Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job Demands, Job Decision Latitude, and Mental Strain: Implications for

Job Redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24(2), 285-308.

Karasek, R. A. (1989). Control in the workplace and its health related aspects. In S. L. Sauter, J. J.

Hurrell, & Cooper (Eds.), Job control and work health (pp. 129-159). New York: Wiley.

Page 78: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 78

Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychology of organizations (revised edition.). New

York: Wiley.

Kawakami, N., Araki, S., Kawashima, M., Masumoto, T., & Hayashi, T. (1997). Effects of work-

related stress reduction on depressive symptoms among Japanese blue-collar workers.

Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 23(1), 54-59.

Kinicki, A. J., & Latack, J. C. (1990). Explication of the construct of coping with involuntary job

loss. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 36(3), 339-360.

Kirkcaldy, B. D., Trimpop, R., & Cooper, C. L. (1997). Working hours, job stress, work

satisfaction, and accident rates among medical practitioners and allied personnel.

International Journal of Stress Management, 4(2), 79-87.

Kjellberg, A., & Wadman, C. (2007). The role of the affective stress response as a mediator of the

effect of psychosocial risk factors on musculoskeletal complaints—Part 1: Assembly

workers. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 37(4), 367-374.

Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1978). The reciprocal effects of the substantive complexity of work

and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal assessment. American Journal of Sociology,

84(1), 24-52.

Kouvonen, A., Kivimaki, M., Cox, S. J., Cox, T., & Vahtera, J. (2005). Relationship between work

stress and body mass index among 45,810 female and male employees. Psychosomatic

medicine, 67(4), 577-583.

Kushnir, T., & Malkinson, R. (1993). A rational-emotive group intervention for preventing and

coping with stress among safety officers. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-

Behavior Therapy, 11(4), 195-206.

Page 79: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 79

Lallukka, T., Sarlio-Lähteenkorva, S., Roos, E., Laaksonen, M., Rahkonen, O., & Lahelma, E.

(2004). Working conditions and health behaviours among employed women and men: the

Helsinki Health Study. Preventive Medicine, 38(1), 48-56.

Lance, C. E., Lautenschlager, G. J., Sloan, C. E., & Varca, P. E. (1989). A comparison between

bottom-up, top-down, and bidirectional models of relationships between global and life facet

satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 57(3), 601-624.

de Lange, A. H., Taris, T. W., Kompier, M. A., Houtman, I. L., & Bongers, P. M. (2003). " The

very best of the millennium": longitudinal research and the demand-control-(support)

model. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8(4), 282-305.

Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer.

Lee, R., & Ashforth, B. (1996). A meta-analytic examination of the correlates of the three

dimensions of job burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(2), 123-133.

Lees, S., & Ellis, N. (1990). The design of a stress-management programme for nursing personnel.

Journal of Advanced Nursing, 15(8), 946-961.

Leiter, M. P. (1993). Burnout as a developmental process: Consideration of models. In W. B.

Schaufeli, C. Maslach, & Marek (Eds.), Professional burnout: Recent developments in

theory and research (pp. 237-250). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

LePine, J. A., Podsakoff, N. P., & LePine, M. A. (2005). A meta-analytic test of the challenge

stressor-hindrance stressor framework: An explanation for inconsistent relationships among

stressors and performance. The Academy of Management Journal, 48(5), 764-775.

Page 80: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 80

van der Linden, D., Taris, T. W., Beckers, D. G. J., & Kindt, K. B. (2007). Reinforcement

sensitivity theory and occupational health: BAS and BIS on the job. Personality and

Individual Differences, 42(6), 1127-1138.

Locke, E. A. (1991). Goal theory vs. control theory: Contrasting approaches to understanding work

motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 15(1), 9-28.

Locke, E. A. (1994). The Emperor is naked. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 43(3),

367-370.

Long, B. C., & Flood, K. R. (1993). Coping with work stress: psychological benefits of exercise.

Work and Stress, 7(2), 119-209.

Lourijsen, E., Houtman, I., Kompier, M., & Grundemann, R. (1999). The Netherlands: a hospital,

healthy working for health. In M. Kompier & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), .

Maslach, C. (1998). A multidimensional theory of burnout. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of

Organizational Stress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of

Occupational Behavior, 2(2), 99-113.

Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. (2008). Early predictors of job burnout and engagement.

Matrajt, M. (1992). Using ergonomic analysis and group discussion to identify and prevent stress in

managers and assembly-line workers: A Mexican case study. Conditions of Work Digest, 11,

189-189.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping effectiveness in an adult

sample. Journal of Personality, 54(2), 385-404.

McCue, J. D., & Sachs, C. L. (1991). A stress management workshop improves residents' coping

skills. Archives of Internal Medicine, 151(11), 2273-2277.

Page 81: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 81

McDonough, P. (2000). Job insecurity and health. International Journal of Health Services, 30(3),

453-476.

McGrath, J. E. (1976). Stress and behavior in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of

Industrial and Organizational Psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1351–1395). Chicago: Rand

McNally.

McGrath, J. E., & Beehr, T. A. (1990). Time and the stress process: Some temporal issues in the

conceptualization and measurement of stress. Stress Medicine, 6(2), 93-104.

Meier, L. L., Semmer, N. K., Elfering, A., & Jacobshagen, N. (2008). The double meaning of

control: Three-way interactions between internal resources, job control, and stressors at

work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 13(3), 244-58.

Meijman, T. F., & Mulder, G. (1998). Psychological aspects ol workload. In P. J. D. Drenth &

Thierry (Eds.), (Vol. 2, pp. 5-33). Hove, England: Psychology Press.

Mikkelsen, A., & Gundersen, M. (2003). The effect of a participatory organizational intervention on

work environment, job stress, and subjective health complaints. International Journal of

Stress Management, 10(2), 91-110.

Miller, J. G. (1965). Living systems: Basic concepts. Behavioral Science, 10(3), 193-237.

Morrison, D., Payne, R. L., & Wall, T. D. (2003). Is job a viable unit of analysis? A multilevel

analysis of demand-control-support models. Journal of, 8(3), 209.

Munz, D. C., Kohler, J. M., & Greenberg, C. I. (2001). Effectiveness of a comprehensive worksite

stress management program: Combining organizational and individual interventions.

International Journal of Stress Management, 8(1), 49-62.

Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does

self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247-259.

Page 82: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 82

Murphy, L. R. (1985). Individual coping strategies. In C. L. Cooper & M. J. Smith (Eds.), Job

Stress and Blue Collar Work. Chichester: Wiley.

Neufeld, R. W. J. (1999). Dynamic differentials of stress and coping. Psychological Review, 106(2),

385-397.

Newman, J. E., & Beehr, T. A. (1979). Personal and organizational strategies for handling job

stress: A review of research and opinion. Personnel Psychology, 32(1), 1-43.

Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: a meta-analysis. Journal

of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087.

Ohira, H., Isowa, T., Nomura, M., Ichikawa, N., Kimura, K., Miyakoshi, M., et al. (2008). Imaging

brain and immune association accompanying cognitive appraisal of an acute stressor.

Neuroimage, 39(1), 500-514.

Ortqvist, D., & Wincent, J. (2006). Prominent consequences of role stress: A meta-analytic review.

International Journal of Stress Management, 13(4), 399-422.

Parker, S. K. (1998). Enhancing role breadth self-efficacy: the roles of job enrichment and other

organizational interventions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(6), 835-52.

Parker, S. K., & Sprigg, C. A. (1999). Minimizing strain and maximizing learning: the role of job

demands, job control, and proactive personality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(6), 925-

39.

Parker, S. K., Turner, N., & Griffin, M. A. (2003). Designing healthy work. In D. A. Hofmann &

Tetrick (Eds.), Health and Safety in Organizations: A Multilevel Perspective (pp. 91-130).

San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Parker, S. K., Williams, H. M., & Turner, N. (2006). Modeling the antecedents of proactive

behavior at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(3), 636-52.

Page 83: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 83

Peters, K. K., & Carlson, J. G. (1999). Worksite stress management with high-risk maintenance

workers: a controlled study. International Journal of Stress Management, 6(1), 21-44.

Podsakoff, N. P., LePine, J. A., & LePine, M. A. (2007). Differential Challenge Stressor-Hindrance

Stressor Relationships With Job Attitudes, Turnover Intentions, Turnover, and Withdrawal

Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(2), 438-454.

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases

in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879-903.

Poelmans, S., Compernolle, T., De Neve, H., Buelens, M., & Rombouts, J. (1999). Belgium: A

pharmaceutical company. In M. Kompier & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Preventing Stress,

Improving Productivity: European Case Studies in the Workplace (pp. 121–48). London:

Routledge.

Powell, K. E., Thompson, P. D., Caspersen, C. J., & Kendrick, J. S. (1987). Physical activity and

the incidence of coronary heart disease. Annual Review of Public Health, 8(1), 253-287.

Probst, T. M., & Brubaker, T. L. (2001). The effects of job insecurity on employee safety outcomes:

Cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,

6(2), 139-159.

Rector, N. A., & Roger, D. (1996). Cognitive style and well-being: A prospective examination.

Personality and Individual Differences, 21(5), 663-674.

Reynolds, S. (2000). Interventions: what works, what doesn't? Occupational Medicine, 50(5), 315-

319.

Reynolds, S., Taylor, E., & Shapiro, D. (1993). Session impact and outcome in stress management

training. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 3(4), 325-337.

Page 84: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 84

Richardson, K. M., & Rothstein, H. R. (2008). Effects of occupational stress management

intervention programs: A meta-analysis. Journal of occupational health psychology, 13(1),

69-93.

de Rijk, A. E., Le Blanc, P. M., Schaufeli, W. B., & De Jonge, J. (1998). Active coping and need

for control as moderators of the job demand-control model: effects on burnout. Journal of

Occupational and Organisational Psychology, 71(1), 1-18.

Rodin, J., & Salovey, P. (1989). Health psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 40(1), 533-579.

Roe, R. A. (2008). Time in applied psychology: The study of ``what happens'' rather than``what is''.

European Psychology, 13(1), 37-52.

Rutledge, T., Stucky, E., Dollarhide, A., Shively, M., Jain, S., Wolfson, T., et al. (2009). A real-

time assessment of work stress in physicians and nurses. Health Psychology, 28(2), 194-

200.

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 719-727.

Salanova, M., Peiró, J. M., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2002). Self-efficacy specificity and burnout among

information technology workers: An extension of the job demand-control model.

Sauter, S. H., Murphy, L. R., & Hurrell, J. J. (1990). Proposed national strategy for the prevention

of psychological disorders in the workplace. American Psychologist, 45, 1146–1158.

Schaubroeck, J., & Merritt, D. E. (1997). Divergent effects of job control on coping with work

stressors: The key role of self-efficacy. Academy of Management Journal, 40(3), 738-754.

Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and their relationship with

burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(3),

293-315.

Page 85: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 85

Schaufeli, W. B., Taris, T. W., & van Rhenen, W. (2008). Workaholism, burnout, and work

engagement: Three of a kind or rhree different kinds of employee well-being? Applied

Psychology: An International Review, 57(2), 173-203.

Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2007). What to do? The effects of discrepancies, incentives, and

time on dynamic goal prioritization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(4), 928-941.

Schönpflug, W. (1983). Coping efficiency and situational demands. . In G. R. J. Hockey (Ed.),

Stress and fatigue in human performance (pp. 299-330). Chichester: Wiley.

Segerstrom, S. C. (2007). Stress, energy, and immunity: An ecological view. Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 16(6), 326-330.

Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. (2004). Psychological stress and the human immune system: a

meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin, 601-630.

Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction.

American Psychologist, 55(1), 5-14.

Shapiro, D. A., Cheesman, M., & Wall, T. D. (1993). Secondary Prevention–Review of Counselling

and EAPs. In Promoting Mental Health Policies in the Workplace (pp. 86-102). London:

HMSO.

Sheppard, W. D., Staggers, F. J., & John, L. (1997). The effects of a stress management program in

a high security government agency. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 10(4), 341-350.

Siegrist, J. (1996). Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 1(1), 27-41.

Sonnentag, S. (2003). Recovery, work engagement, and proactive behavior: A new look at the

interface between nonwork and work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 518-528.

Page 86: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 86

Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2006). Endocrinological processes associated with job stress:

Catecholamine and cortisol responses to acute and chronic stressors. In P. L. Perrewe & D.

C. Ganster (Eds.), Employee Health, Coping and Methodologies (pp. 1-59). Amsterdam,

NL: Elsevier.

Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The Recovery Experience Questionnaire: development and

validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 12(3), 204-21.

Sonnentag, S., & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological detachment from work during off-job time: The

role of job stressors, job involvement, and recovery-related self-efficacy. European Journal

of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(2), 197-217.

Sonnentag, S., & Zijlstra, F. R. (2006). Job characteristics and off-job activities as predictors of

need for recovery, well-being, and fatigue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(2), 330-50.

Sparks, K., & Cooper, C. L. (1999). Occupational differences in the work-strain relationship:

Towards the use of situation-specific models. Journal of Occupational and Organizational

Psychology, 72(2), 219-229.

Sparks, K., Faragher, B., & Cooper, C. L. (2001). Well-being and occupational health in the 21st

century workplace. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 74(4), 489-

509.

Spector, P. E. (1998). A control theory of the job stress process. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of

organizational stress (pp. 153-169). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sprigg, C. A., Stride, C. B., Wall, T. D., Holman, D. J., & Smith, P. R. (2007). Work

characteristics, musculoskeletal disorders, and the mediating role of psychological strain: A

study of call center employees. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(5), 1456-66.

Page 87: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 87

Starrin, B., Larsson, G., Brenner, S. O., Levi, L., & Petterson, I. L. (1990). Structural changes, ill

health, and mortality in Sweden, 1963-1983: a macroaggregated study. International

Journal of Health Services, 20(1), 27-42.

Steffy, B. D., Jones, J. W., & Noe, A. W. (1990). The impact of health habits and life-style on the

stressor-strain relationship: An evaluation of three industries.

Stewart, W., & Barling, J. (1996). Daily work stress, mood and interpersonal job performance: A

mediational model. Work and stress, 10(4), 336-351.

Sutherland, V. J., & Cooper, C. L. (1991). Personality, stress and accident involvement in the

offshore oil and gas industry. Personality and Individual Differences, 12(2), 195-204.

Taris, T. W., Schaufeli, W. B., & Verhoeven, L. C. (2005). Workaholism in the Netherlands:

Measurement and implications for job strain and work-nonwork conflict. Applied

Psychology: An International Review, 54(1), 37-60.

Terra, N. (1995). The prevention of job stress by redesigning jobs and implementing selfregulating

teams. In L. R. Murphy, J. J. Hurrell, S. L. Sauter, & G. P. Keita (Eds.), Job Stress

Interventions (pp. 235-263). Washington, DC: APA.

Theorell, T., Emdad, R., Arnetz, B., & Weingarten, A. M. (2001). Employee effects of an

educational program for managers at an insurance company. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63,

724-733.

Thompson, B., Kirk-Brown, A., & Brown, D. (2001). Women police: The impact of work stress on

family members. In P. A. Hancock & P. A. Desmond (Eds.), Stress, Workload and Fatigue:

Theory, Research, and Practice (pp. 200–210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Page 88: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 88

Toivanen, H., Helin, P., & Hänninen, O. (1993). Impact of regular relaxation training and

psychosocial working factors on neck-shoulder tension and absenteeism in hospital cleaners.

Journal of Occupational Medicine, 35(11), 1123-1130.

Toivanen, H., Länsimies, E., Jokela, V., & Hänninen, O. (1993). Impact of regular relaxation

training on the cardiac autonomic nervous system of hospital cleaners and bank employees.

Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 19(5), 319-325.

Totterdell, P. (2005). Work schedules. In Handbook of work stress (pp. 53-81). Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage.

Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and hitting runs: Mood linkage and subjective performance

in professional sport teams. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(6), 848-859.

Trimpop, R., Kirkcaldy, B., Athanasou, J., & Cooper, C. (2000). Individual differences in working

hours, work perceptions and accident rates in veterinary surgeries. Work and Stress, 14(2),

181-188.

Troup, C., & Dewe, P. (2002). Exploring the nature of control and its role in the appraisal of

workplace stress. Work & Stress, 16(4), 335-355.

Tsai, S. L., & Crockett, M. S. (1993). Effects of relaxation training, combining imagery, and

meditation on the stress level of Chinese nurses working in modern hospitals in Taiwan.

Issues Mental Health Nursing, 14(1), 51-66.

Tsai, W. C., Chen, C. C., & Liu, H. L. (2007). Test of a model linking employee positive moods

and task performance. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(6), 1570-1583.

Tsutsumi, A., Kayaba, K., Yoshimura, M., Sawada, M., Ishikawa, S., Sakai, K., et al. (2003).

Association between job characteristics and health behaviors in Japanese rural workers.

International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10(2), 125-142.

Page 89: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 89

Van Doornen, L. J. P., & De Geus, E. J. C. (1993). Stress, physical activity and coronary heart

disease. Work and Stress, 7(2), 121-139.

Van Vegchel, N., De Jonge, J., & Landsbergis, P. A. (2005). Occupational stress in (inter) action:

the interplay between job demands and job resources. Journal of Organizational Behavior,

26(5), 535-60.

van Veldhoven, M., Taris, T. W., de Jonge, J., & Broersen, S. (2005). The relationship between

work characteristics and employee health and well-being: How much complexity do we

really need? International Journal of Stress Management, 12(1), 3-28.

Viswesvaran, C., Sanchez, J. I., & Fisher, J. (1999). The Role of Social Support in the Process of

Work Stress: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54(2), 314-334.

Wallace, J. C., Edwards, B. D., Arnold, T., Frazier, M. L., & Finch, D. M. (2009). Work stressors,

role-based performance, and the moderating influence of organizational support. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 94(1), 254-262.

Walter, F., & Bruch, H. (2008). The positive group affect spiral: A dynamic model of the

emergence of positive affective similarity in work groups. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 29(2), 239-261.

Warr, P. (1994). A conceptual framework for the study of work and mental health. Work & Stress,

8(2), 84-97.

Warr, P. (2005). Work, well-being, and mental health. In J. Barling, E. K. Kelloway, & M. R. Frone

(Eds.), Handbook of Work Stress (pp. 547-573). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Watson, D., & Hubbard, B. (1996). Adaptational style and dispositional structure: Coping in the

context of the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 64(4), 737-774.

Page 90: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 90

Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of

affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 820-838.

Whatmore, L., Cartwright, S., Cooper, C., Kompier, M. A., & Cooper, C. L. (1999). United

Kingdom: evaluation of a stress management programme in the public sector. In Preventing

Stress, Improving Productivity: European Case Studies in the Workplace (pp. 149-174).

London: Routledge.

Wiholm, C., Arnetz, B., & Berg, M. (2000). The impact of stress management on computer-related

skin problems. Stress Medicine, 16(5), 279-285.

Zapf, D., Dormann, C., & Frese, M. (1996). Longitudinal studies in organizational stress research:

A review of the literature with reference to methodological issues. Journal of Occupational

Health Psychology, 1(2), 145-69.

Zellars, K. L., Perrewe, P. L., & Hochwarter, W. A. (2000). Burnout in health care: The role of the

five factors of personality. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(8), 1570-1598.

Page 91: Stress and Well Being

Stress and well-being 91

Goalprocesses

Responseprocesses

EnvironmentTaskRole

SocialPhysical

PersonPersonality

ValuesSkills

Abilities

OutcomesHealth

Well-beingPerformance

Active Reactive

Approach Avoid

AppraisalProcesses

Resources Demands

Short-term dynamics

Longer-term dynamics

Figure 1. Integrating framework for different time cycles