taste, food intake and obesity

6
© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218 213 Taste, food intake and obesity incorporated throughout the article and summarized at the end. Taste perception and obesity Sweet, salty, and umami tastes are usually well accepted by humans and contribute to the palatability of foods, that is, perception of these basic tastes in foods tends to promote their intake. Bitter and sour tastes are usually unacceptable to humans especially at high concentrations, and therefore contribute to the reduced palatability of foods. Sensitivity to basic tastes does not seem to differ between lean and obese humans, however, increases in bitter and sour sensitivity and decreases in sweet and salt sensitivity are reported in the morbidly obese after gastric bypass surgery (4,5). Mixtures of sugar, a source of sweetness, and fat are considered especially palatable by obese humans (6,7,8), however, Cox et al. (9) suggest that overall, lean and obese humans do not differ in the selection of foods with perceived tastes and hedonic attributes. The lack of reported differences in taste perceptions and food selections between lean and obese humans seems counter-intuitive as good taste is usually the number one reason given for eating a food (10), and a decrease in good taste is a reason given for stopping food intake (11). It is possible that individual genetic and/or physiological obesity reviews Department of Psychiatry, Division of Substance Abuse, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons; and, New York Obesity Research Center, St. Luke’s/Roosevelt Hospital Center, New York, NY, USA Received 15 February 2001; revised 11 April 2001; accepted 18 April 2001 Address reprint requests to: Jennifer Nasser, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Unit 66, 1051 Riverside Drive, New York, NY 10032, USA E-mail: [email protected] J. Nasser PhD Abstract Research in human eating behaviour prior to 1990 has shown that taste impacts the palatability and selection of food for intake; sensory-specific satiety; satiation; and thermic effect of food. Research in the last decade has added information to these areas; expanded the field to comparisons of the impact of ‘wanting’ vs. ‘liking’ food on intake, and provided insight into the relationship of food intake and brain functioning through new imaging techniques. This article will review literature from the last decade on research in the area of taste and its impact on food intake. Emphasis will be placed on differences seen between lean and obese humans and how these may contribute to the development of human obesity. Suggestions for future research directions will also be discussed. Keywords: Food intake, obesity, palatability, taste. obesity reviews (2001) 2, 213–218 Introduction Consumers tend to use the word taste to refer to all oral sensory sensations associated with food in the mouth, namely, taste, retronasal olfaction and chemosensation (1). In general, taste refers to four basic oral perceptions: sweet, salty, sour, and bitter, with some also including the taste of starch and umami (taste of monosodium glutamate) in this category (2). Since most human taste and food-intake studies are performed without the use of nose clips, they really measure flavor, which is a composite of taste per- ception and retronasal olfaction. In this article addressing taste and obesity, no distinction will be made between taste and flavor. Contributions of chemosensation to food intake will not be addressed in this article. Evidence in humans suggests that taste contributes to the selection of foods to be eaten and to palatability, defined by Yeomans (3) as ‘the hedonic evaluation of orosensory food cues under standardized conditions’. Taste also contributes to the development of sensory-specific satiety (SSS) (64,65), satiation (68,69), thermic effect of food (75,76,78,79), and the ‘reward value’ of food (81). This review will examine literature from the last 10 years with particular emphasis on the differences between lean and obese humans and how the differences may contribute to obesity. Suggestions for future research directions will be

Upload: j-nasser

Post on 06-Jul-2016

238 views

Category:

Documents


25 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Taste, food intake and obesity

© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218 213

Taste, food intake and obesity

incorporated throughout the article and summarized at theend.

Taste perception and obesity

Sweet, salty, and umami tastes are usually well accepted byhumans and contribute to the palatability of foods, that is,perception of these basic tastes in foods tends to promotetheir intake. Bitter and sour tastes are usually unacceptableto humans especially at high concentrations, and thereforecontribute to the reduced palatability of foods. Sensitivityto basic tastes does not seem to differ between lean and obese humans, however, increases in bitter and soursensitivity and decreases in sweet and salt sensitivity arereported in the morbidly obese after gastric bypass surgery(4,5). Mixtures of sugar, a source of sweetness, and fat areconsidered especially palatable by obese humans (6,7,8),however, Cox et al. (9) suggest that overall, lean and obesehumans do not differ in the selection of foods with perceived tastes and hedonic attributes.

The lack of reported differences in taste perceptions and food selections between lean and obese humans seemscounter-intuitive as good taste is usually the number onereason given for eating a food (10), and a decrease in goodtaste is a reason given for stopping food intake (11). It is possible that individual genetic and/or physiological

obesity reviews

Department of Psychiatry, Division of

Substance Abuse, Columbia University

College of Physicians and Surgeons; and,

New York Obesity Research Center, St.

Luke’s/Roosevelt Hospital Center, New York,

NY, USA

Received 15 February 2001; revised 11

April 2001; accepted 18 April 2001

Address reprint requests to: Jennifer Nasser,

New York State Psychiatric Institute, Unit 66,

1051 Riverside Drive, New York, NY 10032,

USA

E-mail: [email protected]

J. Nasser PhD

AbstractResearch in human eating behaviour prior to 1990 has shown that taste impactsthe palatability and selection of food for intake; sensory-specific satiety; satiation;and thermic effect of food. Research in the last decade has added information tothese areas; expanded the field to comparisons of the impact of ‘wanting’ vs.‘liking’ food on intake, and provided insight into the relationship of food intakeand brain functioning through new imaging techniques. This article will reviewliterature from the last decade on research in the area of taste and its impact onfood intake. Emphasis will be placed on differences seen between lean and obesehumans and how these may contribute to the development of human obesity. Suggestions for future research directions will also be discussed.

Keywords: Food intake, obesity, palatability, taste.

obesity reviews (2001) 2, 213–218

Introduction

Consumers tend to use the word taste to refer to all oralsensory sensations associated with food in the mouth,namely, taste, retronasal olfaction and chemosensation (1).In general, taste refers to four basic oral perceptions: sweet,salty, sour, and bitter, with some also including the taste ofstarch and umami (taste of monosodium glutamate) in thiscategory (2). Since most human taste and food-intakestudies are performed without the use of nose clips, theyreally measure flavor, which is a composite of taste per-ception and retronasal olfaction. In this article addressingtaste and obesity, no distinction will be made between tasteand flavor. Contributions of chemosensation to food intakewill not be addressed in this article.

Evidence in humans suggests that taste contributes to theselection of foods to be eaten and to palatability, definedby Yeomans (3) as ‘the hedonic evaluation of orosensoryfood cues under standardized conditions’. Taste also contributes to the development of sensory-specific satiety(SSS) (64,65), satiation (68,69), thermic effect of food(75,76,78,79), and the ‘reward value’ of food (81). Thisreview will examine literature from the last 10 years withparticular emphasis on the differences between lean andobese humans and how the differences may contribute toobesity. Suggestions for future research directions will be

Page 2: Taste, food intake and obesity

differences within the lean and obese phenotypes confounddetection of differences in taste perceptions between thesegroups. For example, we know that lean individuals withobese parents are at greater risk of developing obesity thanthose with lean parents; however, the contribution of famil-ial obesity history to taste responses and food choice inhumans has only been reported in a few studies (12,13). Therelationship of the genetic ability to detect the bitter taste ofPROP(6-propylthiouracil) with the intensity of sweet andfat perception, relative preferences for sweet and fatty foodsand body mass index has recently been reviewed (14,15).Inclusion of PROP taster status as a grouping variable instudies examining the impact of taste on obesity may revealadditional differences between the lean and the obese.

Physiological impacts on taste

Among the obese, the distribution of body fat is associatedwith variations in physiology such as hyperinsulinaemia(16), hyperleptinaemia and leptin resistance (17,18). Theimpact of these physiological differences on responses totaste stimuli and food intake needs further study given thatboth insulin and leptin are involved in the hypothalamiccontrol of food intake (19). Obesity is associated with thehyperinsulinaemia that precedes Type 2 diabetes (20), andaltered taste responses have been reported in Type 2 dia-betics (21,22). Using lean humans, Rodin and co-workers(23) independently manipulated plasma insulin and glucoselevels, and reported that the acute hyperinsulinaemiaresulted in increased palatability of sweet solutions. How-ever, the impact of chronic hyperinsulinaemia on tasteresponses in the obese, as well as the time course for thedevelopment of altered taste responses in relation to thedevelopment of Type 2 diabetes in the obese have not beenreported.

Leptin, which can decrease food intake (19), suppressesoral responses to sweet stimuli when administered periph-erally to lean mice, but not to obese, diabetic, leptin recep-tor defective db/db mice (24). Taste cells in the lean miceexpress the leptin receptor, but no report has been made ofleptin receptors in human taste cells. There are conflictingreports of the relationship of leptin to perceptions of foodpleasantness (i.e. a contributing factor to the palatabilityresponse) in obese humans. Karhunen and co-workers(25,26) found that high leptin levels in obese women wereassociated with decreased responsiveness to food stimuli,reduced dietary energy and fat intakes. Raynaud and co-workers (27), however, report that fasting plasma levels ofleptin in humans, even when corrected for body mass index(BMI), are positively correlated with palatability ratings ofa 75-g carbohydrate-load breakfast. The positive correla-tion of plasma leptin with carbohydrate palatability sug-gests a potential role for leptin resistance in the reportedtaste preference for sugar/fat mixtures seen in obese women

214 Taste, food intake and obesity J. Nasser obesity reviews

© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218

and weight cyclers (28). Further studies are needed to deter-mine the impact of leptin on taste sensitivity and its relationship to food intake in humans.

Evaluation of taste perception

An additional confounding factor in evaluating humantaste perceptions and its impact on food intake and obesitymay be the use of visual analogue scales (VAS) to compareratings of perceptions between individuals and groups.While VAS have been reported to have reproducibility(29,30) for rating appetite sensations, Bartoshuk (31) sug-gests that these instruments are prone to insensitivity andceiling effects associated with placement of verbal descrip-tors along the measurement line, and variation in usage ofthe verbal descriptors among individuals (for example, seeYeomans and Symes (32) for variations in the use of palata-bility). This could limit their usefulness in making inter-group comparisons and possibly obscure true differencesbetween individuals and groups of individuals as to theimpact of taste on food intake and obesity.

One approach for addressing this concern may be the use of the Labelled Magnitude Scale (LMS) (33). In a side-by-side comparison of VAS with LMS, Lucchina and co-workers (34) demonstrated increased sensitivity andelimination of a ceiling effect in the evaluation of sweet-ness intensity and PROP taste sensitivity. Recent advancesin instrumental electromyographic (EMG) recordings re-ported by Hu and co-workers (35,36) as well as work byRousmans and co-workers (37) utilizing variations in auto-nomic nervous system parameters to evaluate pleasantnessand palatability of various taste solutions suggest possiblemethods for calibrating individual VAS ratings of hedonicperceptions against continuous, objective instrumentaloutput. This type of calibration may allow for more sensi-tive interindividual and intergroup comparisons of tasteperceptions.

Taste and palatability

Smith (38) states that taste provides a feed-forward signalfor promoting food intake. The palatability response ofhumans to the taste of individual foods and whole mealsaccounts for a large with-in individual variation in foodintake but only a small variation between subjects (39).(For detailed reviews of the effects of taste and palatabil-ity on food intake see 3,40,41,42,43).

Palatability, a measure of the pleasantness of food, isrelated to the intake response towards a food, and is corre-lated with the energy density (kJ g -1) of a food. High energy-dense foods (>6.0kJg -1) are more palatable than low energydense foods (<4 kJ g -1) (40), and promote their own inges-tion, where low energy-dense foods promote satiety. Foodswith a high fat content or high percent of sugar/fat mixtures

Page 3: Taste, food intake and obesity

tend to be high in energy density and are preferred by theobese (44–47). Mixtures of fat with salt are commonlyfound in high-fat, high energy-dense snacks and fast foods,which currently contribute 11% of total dietary fat in theAmerican diet (45,48). Salt contributes to the palatability ofhigh-fat foods by suppressing bitterness (49), which candevelop during cooking from the oxidation of fatty acidscontained in the fat (50). Since humans eat a constantvolume of food (51), the higher the energy density of thefood, the greater the caloric intake. This is one way in whichtaste may contribute to the development of obesity due toenergy intake in excess of energy expenditure over a pro-longed period of time. (For a review of efforts to dissociateenergy density from palatability see 52).

In addition to providing a feed-forward signal for foodintake, taste in animals can also determine the amounteaten. Numerous laboratory studies, reviewed by Sclafani(53), have reported the development of obesity in rats due to over-consumption of a palatable, cafeteria-style diet.Recent papers have also described the effect of taste to limitfood intake independent of postingestive satiety signals.Pass and Foley (54) and Stapley and co-workers (55) reportthat the taste of bitter secondary plant metabolites causes a dose-dependent reduction in plant intake in Australianbrush tail possums. In addition, orosensory detection of freefatty acids by rats (56–58), described as bitter by humans(50), is reported to reduce the amount of fat eaten by carbohydrate-preferring, but not fat-preferring, rats (59).

Such a fatty acid-sensitive, orosensory based inhibitorysystem for fat intake has not yet been described in humans,possibly due to the fact that fatty acids are removed fromprocessed fat sources(e.g. vegetable oils used in taste andfood intake studies) prior to being offered for sale (forhealth and safety reasons, as well as product stability concerns). However, correlation of decreased consumeracceptance of products, such as milk, home-ground wheatflour, and frozen fish, containing milligram quantities of freefatty acids has been reported (60–62). Preliminary work by Nasser and co-workers (63) suggests that detection ofmicrogram levels of free fatty acids in a high fat food ispositively correlated to PROP Taster status. Such a lowdetection level for fatty acids would provide a 1000-foldgradient over which to demonstrate dose-dependent intakereduction before consumer product rejection. Furtherstudies are needed to determine the contribution of orosen-sory, fatty acid-based signals to the control of dietary fatintake in humans.

Taste and sensory specific satiety

Brain imaging in non-human primates has shown that thereare sets of neurones in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortexthat respond to tastes of food only when the animal ishungry (64). The response of these neurones decreases to

almost zero when the animal has eaten to satiety and is spe-cific for the food eaten. This observed event is referred to assensory-specific satiety (SSS), a decreased desire to consumemore of a food just eaten while still desiring more food. It isobserved in human eating behaviour and is directly relatedto the sensory properties of foods (65–67). SSS contributesto satiation, that is, the termination of a meal (68,69).

Of interest is whether there are differences in SSSbetween the lean and obese, or among various ethnicgroups where obesity is more prevalent. Evans and Foltin (70) report preliminary results suggesting that obesewomen have reduced SSS both to food eaten and food just tasted, compared to lean women. Schiffman and co-workers (71) report that overweight African-Americanwomen, compared to overweight European Americanwomen, show an elevated and sustained desire for sweettasting foods in a SSS, oral habituation test procedure.They suggest that since sweet tasting foods can have highenergy density when mixed with fat, this propensity forsweet taste may well contribute to development of obesityin this ethnic group.

Additional support for reduced SSS in the obese is foundin Epstein and co-workers (72). In this study obese womenwere found to have a significantly slower decline in saliva-tion due to repeated food cues. Reduced decline in saliva-tion is also consistent with a difference in reinforcing valueof food between the lean and obese. In rats, SSS has been associated with changes in dopamine efflux in thenucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex of the brain, sitesassociated with the reward circuitry of the brain (73).Recently positron emission tomography (PET), a techniquethat measures changes in brain blood flow or neurotrans-mitter receptor binding by detecting positrons (positivelycharged particles emitted by injected radio-labelled sub-stances), has been used to image brain dopamine receptorsin obese and lean humans (74). Wang and co-workers (74)report that the obese have significantly fewer dopaminereceptors than the lean, and suggest two possibilities forthis phenomena, namely, down regulation of receptors dueto chronic high dopamine levels from overeating, OR, over-eating to produce increased dopamine release, which wouldcompensate for fewer receptors and under activation ofreward circuits. In either case, reduced sensitivity to SSSsignals in humans could be another way in which altered tasteresponses promote excessive energy intake and contributeto the development or maintenance of obesity in humans.

Taste, thermic effect of food and satiation

Thermic effect of food (TEF) refers to the increase in energyexpenditure associated with food intake. It is dependent on palatable, sensory stimulation (75) and is reduced inobese compared to lean humans (75–77). This reduction isrelated to impaired insulin sensitivity seen in obesity (77).

obesity reviews Taste, food intake and obesity J. Nasser 215

© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218

Page 4: Taste, food intake and obesity

TEF is associated with the satiating power of foods (78,79).Westerterp-Plantenga and co-workers (80) suggest that thisfunction of TEF is also reduced in the obese, which wouldagain promote over-consumption of energy and foster thedevelopment of obesity.

Taste, food liking, and food wanting

The perceived ‘liking’ of a food, which Berridge (81) suggests corresponds closely with palatability, is under thecontrol of brain opioid and serotonin pathways (81,82),while ‘wanting’ food (or appetite) (81), is modulated bydopamine pathways (81,83). The impact of ‘liking’ on foodintake in animals and humans has recently been reviewedby Sclafani (84) and Mela (85), and will not be coveredhere. It should be noted that the opioid and serotonin path-ways can modulate meso-limbic dopaminergic activity(83), suggesting a link between ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ foodand a possible basis for ‘food craving’ (86). However,Rogers and Smit (87) suggest that craving is dependent onpsychological rather than neurophysiological processes.

The association of taste with ‘wanting’ food and foodreward (positive reinforcement) is related to the release ofdopamine in the meso-limbic circuitry of the brain (88).Dopamine release due to food stimuli (i.e. taste) is phasic,and contingent on stimulus novelty (89). Habituation tothe stimulus develops over time (83), leading to decreasedreinforcement as is seen in development of SSS during ameal (90). Dopamine release also has a role in associativelearning (91) whereby sensory stimulus reward is associ-ated with the metabolic consequences of energy intake.Saelens and Epstein (92) report that food is more reinforc-ing in the obese than the lean, and differences in function-ing of the dopaminergic pathway between the lean andobese are beginning to be revealed (74). Further work isneeded to determine the impact of differences in dopamin-ergic pathways on taste perceptions, reinforcing value offood and the development and maintenance of obesity.

Summary

Taste contributes to food palatability, which promotesintake, and to thermic effect of a meal and SSS, whichpromote termination of intake. Reduced sensitivity to thesesensory-based termination signals, coupled with possibleincreased incentive salience of food stimuli and decreasedactivation of food-reward circuits, may contribute to theover-consumption of energy that leads to obesity.

Suggestions for future research

This summary of suggestions for future research was generated from discussions with experienced taste andeating behaviour researchers (see acknowledgements) and

216 Taste, food intake and obesity J. Nasser obesity reviews

© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218

is offered for additional consideration and discussion:determination of the impact of leptin level on taste per-ceptions and food intake, identification of an orosensorybased fat intake inhibition system, answers to questionssuch as: how different does a food have to taste to get amore rapid SSS effect? and can we titrate taste perceptionsso as to maximize food reward value and minimize energyintake and identification of genetic differences that modu-late the potency of foods as unconditioned stimuli in thedevelopment of conditioned sensory preferences?

Acknowledgements

The author wishes to thank Drs Linda Bartoshuk (YaleUniversity), Adam Drewnowski (University of Washing-ton), Allan Geliebter (Columbia University), Harry Kissileff(Columbia University), David Mela (Unilever, the Nether-lands), Barbara Rolls (Pennsylvania State University) andAnthony Sclafani (City University of New York) for shar-ing their expertise and suggestions for future research.

References

1. Duffy VB, Bartoshuk LM. Food acceptance and genetic varia-tion in taste. J Am Diet Assoc 2000; 100: 647–655.2. Scott TR, Giza BK. Issues of gustatory neural coding: wherethey stand today. Physiol Behav 2000; 69: 65–70.3. Yeomans M. Taste, palatability, and the control of appetite.Proc Nutr Soc 1998; 57: 609–615.4. Scrugg DM, Buffington C, Cowan GS Jr. Taste acuity of themorbidly obese before and after gastric bypass surgery. Obes Surg1994; 4: 24–28.5. Burge JC, Schaumburg JZ, Di Choban PSS, Ilvestro RA,Flancbaum L. Changes in patients’ taste acuity after Roux-en-Ygastric bypass for clinically severe obesity. J Am Diet Assoc 1995;95: 666–670.6. Drewnowski A. Kurth,C, Holden-Wiltse, J, Saari, J. Food pref-erences in human obesity: carbohydrates versus fats. Appetite1992; 18: 207–221.7. Gibney M, Sigman-Grant M, Stanton JL, Keast DR. Con-sumption of sugars. Am J Clin Nutr 1995; 62: 1785–1945.8. Mela DJ. Sacchetti, DA. Sensory preferences for fats: relation-ship with diet and body composition. Am J Clin Nutr 1991; 53:908–915.9. Cox DN, Perry L, Moore PB, Vallis L, Mela DJ. Sensory andhedonic associations with macronutrient and energy intakes oflean and obese consumers. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 1999;23: 403–410.10. Glanz K, Basil M, Maibach E, Goldberg J, Snyder D. WhyAmericans eat what they do: Taste, nutrition, cost, convenience,and weight control concerns as influences on food consumption.J Am Diet Assoc 1998; 98: 1118–1126.11. Zylan KD. Gender differences in the reasons given for mealtermination. Appetite 1996; 26: 37–44.12. Drewnowski A, Kurth CL, Rahaim JE. Taste preference inhuman obesity: environmental and familial factors. Am J ClinNutr 1991; 54: 635–641.13. Heitmann BL, Lissner L, Sorensen TI, Bengtsson C. Dietaryfat intake and weight gain in women genetically predisposed forobesity. Am J Clin Nutr 1995; 61: 1213–1217.

Page 5: Taste, food intake and obesity

14. Tepper BJ. 6-n-Propylthiouracil: a genetic marker for taste,with implications for food preference and dietary habits. Am JHum Genet 1998; 63: 1271–1276.15. Prutkin J, Duffy VB, Etter L, Fast K, Gardner E, Lucchina LA,Snyder DJ, Tie K, Weiffenbach J, Bartoshuk LM. Genetic varia-tion and inferences about perceived taste intensity in mice andmen. Physiol Behav 2000; 69: 161–173.16. Pi-Sunyer FX. Obesity: criteria and classification. Proc NutrSoc 2000; 59: 505–509.17. Harris RB. Leptin – much more than a satiety signal. Ann RevNutr 2000; 20: 45–75.18. Clement K. Leptin and the genetics of obesity. Acta PaediatrSuppl 1999; 88: 51–57.19. McMinn JE, Baskin DG, Schwartz MW. Neuroendocrinemechanisms regulating food intake and body weight. Obes Rev2000; 1: 37–46.20. Astrup A, Finer N. Redefining Type 2 diabetes: ‘Diabesity’ or‘Obesity Dependent Diabetes Mellitus’? Obes Rev 2000; 1: 57–59.21. Laitnen JH, Tuorila HM, Uusitupa MI. Changes in hedonicresponses to sweet and fat in recently diagnosed non-insulin-dependent diabetic patients during diet therapy. Eur J Clin Nutr1991; 45: 393–400.22. Perros P, MacFarlane TW, Counsell C, Frier BM. Altered tastesensation in newly diagnosed NIDDM. Diabetes Care 1996; 19:768–770.23. Rodin J, Wack J, Ferrannini E, DeFronzo RA. Effect of insulinand glucose on feeding behavior. Metabolism 1985; 34: 826–831.24. Kawai K, Sugimoto K, Nakashima K, Miura H, Ninomiya Y.Leptin as a modulator of sweet taste sensitivities in mice. Proc NatAcad Sci (USA) 2000; 97: 11044–11049.25. Karhunen LJ, Lappalainen RI, Tammela L, Turpeinen AK.Subjective and physiological cephalic phase responses to food inobese binge-eating women. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21: 321–328.26. Karhunen LJ, Lappalainen RI, Haffner SM, Valve RH, TuorilaH, Miettinen H, Uusitupa MI. Serum leptin, food intake and pref-erences for sugar and fat in obese women. Int J Obes Relat MetabDisord 1998; 22: 819–821.27. Raynaud E, Brun JF, Perez-Martin A, Sagnes C, Boularan A,Fedou C, Mercier J. Serum leptin is associated with the perceptionof palatability during a standardized high-carbohydrate breakfasttest. Clin Sci 1999; 96: 343–348.28. Drewnowski A, Holden-Wiltse J. Taste responses and foodpreferences in obese women: effects of weight cycling. Int J ObesRelat Metab Disord 1992; 16: 639–648.29. Raben A, Tagliabue A, Astrup A. The reproducibility of sub-jective appetite scores. Br J Nutr 1995; 73: 517–530.30. Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A. Reproducibility,power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment ofappetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes RelatMetab Disord 2000; 24: 38–48.31. Bartoshuk LM. Comparing sensory experiences across indi-viduals: recent psychophysical advances illuminate genetic varia-tion in taste perception. Chem Senses 2000; 25: 447–460.32. Yeomans MR, Symes T. Individual differences in the use ofpleasantness and palatability ratings. Appetite 1999; 32: 383–394.33. Green BG, Shaffer GS, Gilmore MM. Derivation and eva-luation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties. Chem Senses 1993; 18: 683–702.34. Lucchina LA, Curtis OF, Putnam P, Drewnowski A, PrutkinJM, Bartoshuk LM. Psychophysical measurements of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) taste perception. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998;855: 816–819.35. Hu S, Player KA, Mcchesney KA, Dalistan MD, Tyner CA,Scozzafava JE. Facial EMG as an indicator of palatability inhumans. Physiol Behav 1999; 68: 31–35.

36. Hu S, Luo YJ, Hui L. Preliminary study of associationsbetween objective parameters of facial electromyography and subjective estimates of taste palatability. Perceptual Motor Skills2000; 91: 741–747.37. Rousmans S, Robin O, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E. Auto-nomic nervous system responses associated with primary tastes.Chem Senses 2000; 25: 709–718.38. Smith GP. The controls of eating: a shift from nutritionalhomeostasis to behavioral neuroscience. Nutrition 2000; 16:814–820.39. De Castro JM. Eating behavior: Lessons from the real worldof humans. Nutrition 2000; 16: 800–813.40. Drewnowski A. Energy density, palatability, and satiety:Implications for weight control. Nutrition Rev 1998; 56: 347–353.41. French SJ. The effects of specific nutrients on the regulationof feeding behavior in human subjects. Proc Nutr Soc 1999; 58:533–539.42. Mela DJ. Eating behavior, food preferences and dietary intakein relation to obesity and body-weight status. Proc Nutr Soc 1996;55: 803–816.43. Zandstra EH, De Graaf C, Mela DJ, Van Staveren WA. Short-and long-term effects of changes in pleasantness on food intake.Appetite 2000; 34: 253–260.44. Astrup A, Buemann B, Western P, Toubro S. Raben,A, Christensen, NJ. Obesity as an adaptation to a high-fat di et: evidence from a cross-sectional study. Am J Clin Nutr 1994; 59: 350–355.45. Warwick ZS, Schiffman SS. Sensory evaluation of fat-sucroseand fat-salt mixtures: relationship to age and weight status.Physiol Behav 1990; 48: 633–636.46. Blundell JE, Stubbs RJ. High and low carbohydrate and fatintakes: limits imposed by appetite and palatability and their impli-cations for energy balance. Eur J Clin Nutr 1999; 53: S148–S165.47. Laurent-Jaccard A, De Matteis L, Hofstetter JR, Schutz Y. Arefood preferences affected by body mass index, age, sex or tobacco?Schweiz Med Wochenschr 1994; 124: 2039–2041.48. Popkin BM, Siega-Riz AM, Haines PS, Jahns L. Where’s thefat? Trends in U.S. diets. Prev Med, 1965–96; 2001: 245–254.49. Breslin PA, Beauchamp GK. Suppression of bitterness bysodium: variation among bitter taste stimuli. Chem Senses 1995;20: 609–623.50. Grosch W, Laskawy G. Contribution of linoleic acid to thebitter taste of poppy seeds (Papaver somniferum). Z LebensmUnters Forsch 1984; 178: 257–259.51. Rolls BJ, Bell EA. Intake of fat and carbohydrate: role ofenergy density. Eur J Clin Nutr 1999; 53: S166–S173.52. Drewnowski A. Intense Sweeteners and energy density offoods: implications for weight control. Eur J Clin Nutr 1999; 53:757–763.53. Sclafani A. Psychobiology of food preferences. Int J ObesRelat Metab Disord 2001; in press.54. Pass GJ, Foley WJ. Plant secondary metabolites as mammalianfeeding deterrents: separating the effects of the taste of salicin fromits post-ingestive consequences in the common brushtail possum.J Comp Physiol B 2000; 170: 185–192.55. Stapley J, Foley WJ, Cunningham R, Eschler B. How well cancommon brushtail possums regulate their intake of Eucalyptustoxins? J Comp Physiol B 2000; 170: 211–218.56. Gilbertson TA, Fontenot DT, Liu L, Zhang H, Monroe WT.Fatty acid modulation of K+ channels in taste receptor cells: gustatory cues for dietary fat. Am J Physiol 1997; 272:C1203–C1210.57. Gilbertson TA, Liu L. York, DA, Bray, GA. Dietary fat pref-erences are inversely correlated with peripheral gustatory fatty acidsensitivity. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998; 855: 165–168.

obesity reviews Taste, food intake and obesity J. Nasser 217

© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218

Page 6: Taste, food intake and obesity

58. Smith JC, Fisher EM, Maleszewski V, McClain B. Orosensoryfactors in the ingestion of corn oil/sucrose mixtures by the rat.Physiol Behav 2000; 69: 135–146.59. Bray GA. Afferent signals regulating food intake. Proc NutrSoc 2000; 59: 373–384.60. Bodyfelt WF, Tobias J, Trout G (eds). The Sensory Evaluationof Dairy Products. Van Norstrand Reinhold: New York, 1998: pp 534–537.61. Hansen L, Rose MS. Sensory acceptability is inversely relatedto development of fat rancidity in bread made from stored flour.J Am Diet Assoc 1996; 96: 792–793.62. Refsgaard HH, Brockhoff PM, Jensen B. Free polyunsaturatedfatty acids cause taste deterioration of salmon during frozenstorage. J Agric Food Chem 2000; 48: 3280–3285.63. Nasser JA, Chou CJ, Kissileff HR, Boozer CN, Pi-Sunyer FX.PROP taster status and oral fatty acid perception. Eating Behav-iors 2001; in press.64. Rolls ET. Taste and olfactory processing in the brain and itsrelation to the control of eating. Crit Rev Neurobiol 1997; 11:263–287.65. Rolls BJ. Sensory specific satiety and variety in the meal. In:Meiselman HI (ed). Dimensions of the Meal. Aspen Publishers:Gaithersburg, 2000, pp110.66. Johnson J, Vickers Z. Factors influencing sensory-specificsatiety. Appetite 1992; 19: 15–31.67. Guinard JX, Brun P. Sensory-specific satiety: comparison oftaste and texture effects. Appetite 1998; 31: 141–157.68. Raynor HA, Epstein LH. Effects of sensory stimulation andpost-ingestive consequences on satiation. Physiol Behav 2000; 70:465–470.69. De Graaf C, De Jong LS, Lambers AC. Palatability affects satiation but not satiety. Physiol Behav 1999; 66: 681–688.70. Evans SM, Foltin RW. Menstrual cycle changes in food ‘crav-ings’ and sensory-specific satiety in lean and obese women. ObesRes 1999; 7: 68S: O202 (Abstract).71. Schiffman SS, Graham BG, Sattely-Miller EA, Person-DancyM. Elevated and sustained desire for sweet taste in African-Americans: a potential factor in the development of obesity. Nutrition 2000; 16: 886–893.72. Epstein LH, Paluch R, Coleman KJ. Differences in salivationto repeated food cues in obese and nonobese women. PsychosomMed 1996; 58: 160–164.73. Ahn S, Phillips AG. Dopaminergic correlates of sensory-specific satiety in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accum-bens of the rat. J Neurosci 1999; 19: RC29.74. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Logan J, Pappas NR, Wong CT, ZhuW, Netusil N, Fowler JS. Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet2001; 357: 354–357.75. Brondel L, Fricker J, Fantino M. Postprandial thermogenesisand alimentary sensory stimulation in human subjects. Int J ObesRelat Metab Disord 1999; 23: 34–40.

218 Taste, food intake and obesity J. Nasser obesity reviews

© 2001 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 2, 213–218

76. Hashkes PJ, Gartside PS, Blondheim SH. Effect of foodpalatability on early (cephalic) phase of diet-induced thermogene-sis in nonobese and obese man. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord1997; 21: 608–613.77. Segal KR, Albu J, Chun A, Edano A, Legaspi B, Pi-Sunyer FX.Independent effects of obesity and insulin resistance on post-prandial thermogenesis in men. J Clin Invest 1992; 89: 824–833.78. LeBlanc J, Soucy J. Interactions between postprandial thermogenesis, sensory stimulation of feeding and hunger. Am JPhysiol 1996; 271: R936–R940.79. Crovetti R, Porrini M, Santangelo A, Testolin G. The influ-ence of thermic effect of food on satiety. Eur J Clin Nutr 1998;52: 482–488.80. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Rolland V, Wilson SAJ, WesterterpKR. Satiety related to 24 h diet-induced thermogenesis during highprotein/carbohydrate vs high fat diets measured in a respirationchamber. Eur J Clin Nutr 1999; 53: 495–502.81. Berridge KC. Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting andliking. Neurosci Biobehav Revs 1995; 20: 1–25.82. Drewnowski A, Krahn DD, Demitrack MA, Nairn K, GosnellBA. Taste responses and preferences for sweet high-fat foods: evidence for opioid involvement. Physiol Behav 1992; 51: 371–379.83. Spanagel R, Weiss F. The dopamine hypothesis of reward: pastand current status. Trends Neurosci 1999; 22: 521–527.84. Sclafani A. Basic Mechanisms of food preference and liking.Proc Nutr Soc 1995; 54: 419–427.85. Mela DJ. Why do we like what we like? J Sci Food Agric 2000;81: 10–16.86. Mercer ME, Holder MD. Food cravings, endogenous opioidpeptides, and food intake: a review. Appetite 1997; 29: 325–352.87. Rogers PJ, Smit HJ. Food craving and food ‘addiction’: a crit-ical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective.Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2000; 66: 3–14.88. Martel P, Fantino M. Influence of the amount of food ingestedon mesolimbic dopaminergic system activity: a microdialysis study.Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1996; 55: 297–302.89. Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Modulation of feeding-induced acti-vation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by appetitive stimuliand its relation to motivational state. Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11:4389–4397.90. Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. CerebralCortex 2000; 10: 284–294.91. Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Differential influence of associativeand nonassociative learning mechanisms on the responsiveness ofprefrontal and accumbal dopamine transmission to food stimuli inrats fed ad libitum. J Neurosci 1997; 17: 851–861.92. Saelens BE, Epstein LH. Reinforcing value of food in obeseand non-obese women. Appetite 1996; 27: 41–50.