testing the sport drug control model of factors ... · threat appraisal (threat to health; threat...

266
______________________________________________________________________ TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS INFLUENCING PERFORMANCE-ENHANCING SUBSTANCES USE Geoffrey Jalleh, MPH, B.Com(Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of The University of Western Australia School of Population Health 2013 ______________________________________________________________________

Upload: others

Post on 15-Aug-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

1

______________________________________________________________________

TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL

MODEL OF FACTORS INFLUENCING

PERFORMANCE-ENHANCING

SUBSTANCES USE

Geoffrey Jalleh, MPH, B.Com(Hons)

This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

of

The University of Western Australia

School of Population Health

2013

______________________________________________________________________

Page 2: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

i

DECLARATION

_______________________________________________________________________

To the best of my knowledge and belief this thesis contains no material previously

published by any other person except where due acknowledgement has been made.

This thesis contains no material which has been accepted for the award of any other

degree or diploma in any university.

Signature:

Date: 26 November 2012

Page 3: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

ii

ABSTRACT

_______________________________________________________________________

This study empirically examines the Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002)

via survey data of a large sample of elite Australian athletes. The model consists of six

components believed to predict an athlete’s attitude (and intentions) towards

performance-enhancing substances use: (1) threat appraisal; (2) benefit appraisal; (3)

personal morality; (4) legitimacy; (5) personality; and (6) reference group opinion. In

addition, two ‘market’ factors, the affordability and availability of drugs, believed to

facilitate or inhibit the translation of attitudes and intentions into behaviour are included

in the model. To date the sports literature shows no systematic national or international

single source research covering the views of athletes on the comprehensive list of

factors in the Sport Drug Control model.

A cross-sectional nationwide mail survey of 1,237 elite Australian athletes was

conducted. Prior to the mail-out, the survey instrument measuring all of the constructs

in the Sport Drug Control model was pre-tested among athletes in focus group

discussions and a small scale survey. A structural equation modelling approach was

employed to test the Sport Drug Control model. The findings provided empirical

validation of the theoretical model; the test model accounted for 81% of the variance in

attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use and 13% of the variance in

doping behaviour.

When considering the six model components together, morality (personal moral stance

on performance-enhancing substances use), reference group opinion (perceived moral

stance of reference group on performance-enhancing substances use) and legitimacy

Page 4: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

iii

(perceptions of the testing and appeals processes) evidenced significant relationships

with attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use. That is, weaker personal

and perceived referent groups’ moral stance against performance-enhancing substances

use and perceptions of the testing and appeals processes as unfair and inequitable were

associated with more favourable attitude towards performance-enhancing substances

use. Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards

for performing well; perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on

performance), and personality (risk taking propensity) showed non-significant

associations with attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use.

Attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use was positively and

significantly associated with doping behaviour. That is, a more favourable attitude

towards performance-enhancing substances use was associated with actual use of these

substances. This finding provides further evidence that attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use is a central component for understanding performance-

enhancing substances use in sport.

The findings of this study have important and practical implications for the content of

anti-doping education programs and research. Current anti-doping education programs

focus on building awareness and knowledge of banned performance-enhancing

substances, reporting and testing requirements, and penalties for non-compliance. For

example, in addition to these ‘educational’ components, anti-doping prevention could

incorporate discussion on moral issues in relation to performance-enhancing substances

use. Kohlberg’s sequence for moral thinking could be applied to foster a moral stance

against performance-enhancing substances use. Bandura’s moral disengagement

mechanisms can be applied in reverse to counter the psychological processes athletes

Page 5: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

iv

may use to disengage from a moral stance against performance-enhancing substances

use. The moral emotions experienced if caught using performance-enhancing substances

– guilt, shame, embarrassment – are another area for discussion as anticipation of these

moral emotions for actions that violate a personal moral norm serve as the regulators of

self-censure. The pedagogical strategies could be based on Blatt and Kohlberg’s

teaching method for fostering moral competencies and learnings from the Konstanz

Method of Dilemma Discussion. Given that personal morality is a significant factor in

understanding performance-enhancing substances use, research examining and applying

mechanisms to morally engage athletes in relation to performance-enhancing substances

use warrants further investigation.

The findings from this study also highlight the need for anti-doping organisations to

maintain perceptions of legitimacy by ensuring that their drug testing regime and

appeals processes are fair and just. Increasing the perceived legitimacy of anti-doping

organisations in conducting drug testing will lead to more likely compliance with anti-

doping regulations. This is a new dimension in understanding performance-enhancing

substances use.

Overall, the Sport Drug Control model appears useful for understanding influences on

performance-enhancing substances use. Future studies could survey athletes

representing a broader range of competition levels and involving cross-cultural research

to test the model’s applicability to other population of athletes. Longitudinal study

designs are required to validate these cross-sectional findings and make assertions about

cause and effect relationships. The findings from this study will better inform theory

development, practical application in anti-doping interventions, and future research.

Page 6: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

_______________________________________________________________________

DECLARATION .............................................................................................................. i

ABSTRACT ..................................................................................................................... ii

TABLE OF CONTENTS ................................................................................................ v

INDEX OF TABLES ..................................................................................................... xi

INDEX OF FIGURES ................................................................................................. xiv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .......................................................................................... xv

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION .................................................................................. 1

1.1 The Importance of Sport ....................................................................................... 1

1.2 Performance-Enhancing Substances Use in Sport ................................................ 3

1.3 The World Anti-Doping Code .............................................................................. 6

1.4 Prevalence of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use ....................................... 7

1.4.1 Prevalence of adverse analytical findings ................................................. 8

1.4.2 Perceived prevalence of banned performance-enhancing

substances use ............................................................................................ 9

1.4.3 Self-reported use of banned performance-enhancing substances ............ 11

1.4.4 Summary.................................................................................................. 16

1.5 Current Anti-Doping Programs ........................................................................... 16

1.6 Theoretical Models of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use ....................... 18

1.6.1 The Drugs in Sport Deterrence model ..................................................... 18

1.6.2 The Sport Drug Control model ................................................................ 19

1.7 Research Objectives of this Study ...................................................................... 23

1.8 Structure of this Thesis........................................................................................ 24

Page 7: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

vi

CHAPTER 2: THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL ....................................... 26

2.1 Threat Appraisal .................................................................................................. 27

2.1.1 Appraisal of threat of enforcement .......................................................... 27

2.1.2 Appraisal of threat of ill-health effects of performance-enhancing

substances use .......................................................................................... 37

2.2 Benefit Appraisal ................................................................................................ 40

2.3 Personal Morality ................................................................................................ 43

2.4 Reference Group Opinion ................................................................................... 44

2.5 Legitimacy........................................................................................................... 46

2.6 Personality ........................................................................................................... 48

2.7 Model Summary in Predicting Likelihood of Performance-Enhancing

Substances Use .................................................................................................... 50

2.8 Market Factors .................................................................................................... 52

2.9 Attitudes and Intentions Towards Performance-Enhancing Substances Use ..... 54

2.10 Recent Research Findings in the Psychosocial Doping in Sports Literature ...... 57

2.10.1 Applications of the Theory of Planned Behaviour .................................. 57

2.10.2 Moral disengagement .............................................................................. 61

2.10.3 Perceptual deterrence theory ................................................................... 67

2.10.4 Personality variables ............................................................................... 68

2.11 An Opportunistic Examination of the Sport Drug Control Model ...................... 70

Page 8: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

vii

CHAPTER 3: RESEARCH METHODOLOGY ....................................................... 72

3.1 Participants and Procedure .................................................................................. 72

3.2 Pilot Survey ......................................................................................................... 74

3.3 The Questionnaire Items ..................................................................................... 74

3.3.1 Threat appraisal ....................................................................................... 75

3.3.2 Benefit appraisal ...................................................................................... 76

3.3.3 Personal morality ..................................................................................... 77

3.3.4 Reference group opinion ......................................................................... 78

3.3.5 Legitimacy ............................................................................................... 79

3.3.6 Personality ............................................................................................... 81

3.3.7 Availability and affordability .................................................................. 82

3.3.8 Attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use ....................... 83

3.3.9 Behaviour: use of banned performance-enhancing substances or

methods.................................................................................................... 84

3.3.10 Sporting background and sample characteristics ................................... 85

3.4 Response Rate ..................................................................................................... 85

3.5 Statistical Analysis Overview ............................................................................. 86

3.5.1 Data screening ......................................................................................... 86

3.5.2 Analytical procedures for testing the model ............................................ 88

3.6 Concluding Comment ......................................................................................... 91

Page 9: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

viii

CHAPTER 4: SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS AND DESCRIPTIVE DATA ..... 92

4.1 Sample Characteristics ........................................................................................ 92

4.2 Prevalence of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use ..................................... 96

4.2.1 Self-reported use of a banned performance-enhancing substance .......... 97

4.2.2 Self-reported use of specific types of banned performance-

enhancing substances and methods ......................................................... 98

4.2.3 Tested positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance .......... 100

4.3 Attitude Towards Banned Performance-Enhancing Substances Use................ 101

4.4 Threat Appraisal ................................................................................................ 103

4.4.1 Appraisal of threat of enforcement ........................................................ 103

4.4.2 Appraisal of threat of ill-health effects of performance-enhancing

substances use ........................................................................................ 106

4.5 Benefit Appraisal .............................................................................................. 110

4.5.1 Perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on sport

performance ........................................................................................... 110

4.5.2 Likelihood and desirability of potential positive outcomes for

performing well in sport ........................................................................ 112

4.6 Perceived Morality of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use ...................... 116

4.6.1 Moral judgment towards performance-enhancing substances use ........ 116

4.6.2 Emotions experienced if caught using performance-enhancing

substances .............................................................................................. 117

4.7 Perceived Reference Groups’ Moral Stance on Banned Performance-

Enhancing Substances Use ................................................................................ 119

Page 10: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

ix

4.8 Legitimacy......................................................................................................... 123

4.8.1 Distributive justice................................................................................. 123

4.8.2 Procedural justice .................................................................................. 127

4.8.3 Interactional justice ............................................................................... 128

4.9 Personality ......................................................................................................... 130

4.10 Market Factors .................................................................................................. 135

4.10.1 Perceived availability of performance-enhancing substances............... 135

4.10.2 Perceived affordability of performance-enhancing substances ............. 137

4.11 Summary of Findings ........................................................................................ 139

CHAPTER 5: CONSTRUCT DEVELOPMENT .................................................... 143

5.1 Threat Appraisal ................................................................................................ 143

5.2 Benefit Appraisal .............................................................................................. 146

5.3 Personal Morality .............................................................................................. 147

5.4 Reference Group Opinion ................................................................................. 149

5.5 Legitimacy......................................................................................................... 150

5.6 Personality ......................................................................................................... 152

5.7 Availability of Performance-Enhancing Substances ........................................ 155

5.8 Affordability of Performance-Enhancing Substances ...................................... 156

5.9 Attitude Towards Performance-Enhancing Substances Use ............................. 157

5.10 Performance-Enhancing Substances Use Behaviour ........................................ 158

5.11 Summary of the Constructs ............................................................................... 158

Page 11: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

x

CHAPTER 6: RESULTS OF THE STRUCTURAL EQUATION

MODELLING ANALYSES ....................................................................................... 162

CHAPTER 7: DISCUSSION ..................................................................................... 166

7.1 Testing the Sport Drug Control Model ............................................................. 167

7.1.1 Predictors of performance-enhancing substances use ........................... 168

7.1.2 Predictors of attitude towards performance-enhancing substances

use .......................................................................................................... 168

7.2 Implications for Anti-Doping Education Programs .......................................... 173

7.2.1 Fostering a moral stance against performance-enhancing

substances use ........................................................................................ 173

7.2.2 Enhancing adherence to a moral stance against performance-

enhancing substances use ...................................................................... 177

7.2.3 Maintaining perceptions of the legitimacy of the testing and

appeals processes ................................................................................... 179

7.3 Limitations and Future Research ...................................................................... 180

CHAPTER 8: CONCLUSIONS................................................................................. 184

8.1 Concluding Comment on Findings ................................................................... 188

REFERENCES ............................................................................................................ 187

APPENDICES ............................................................................................................. 234

Appendix 1: The Questionnaire .................................................................................... 235

Appendix 2: The Covering Letter ................................................................................. 248

Appendix 3: The Reminder Letter ................................................................................ 249

Page 12: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

xi

INDEX OF TABLES

_______________________________________________________________________

Table 2.1: Highest and lowest predicted likelihood of performance-enhancing

substances use based on athletes’ scores on the six components in the

Sport Drug Control model ......................................................................... 52

Table 4.1: Athletes’ highest level of competition ....................................................... 92

Table 4.2: Main sports in which athletes were involved ............................................. 93

Table 4.3: Sample demographics ................................................................................ 94

Table 4.4: Sporting background and achievements .................................................... 96

Table 4.5: Proportion of athletes who considered or have used a banned

performance-enhancing substance ............................................................. 97

Table 4.6: Proportion of athletes who used a banned performance-enhancing

substance in the last 12 months.................................................................. 99

Table 4.7: Proportion of athletes who ‘ever used’ a banned performance-

enhancing substance ................................................................................. 99

Table 4.8: Proportion of athletes who have ever been drug tested ........................... 100

Table 4.9: Amount of consideration athletes would give to an offer of a banned

performance-enhancing substance ........................................................... 101

Table 4.10: Rating of necessity to use banned performance-enhancing substances

to perform at the very highest levels ........................................................ 102

Table 4.11: Perceived likelihood of being tested in and out of competition .............. 104

Table 4.12: Perceived likelihood of evading detection if using performance-

enhancing substances in and out of competition ..................................... 105

Table 4.13: Perceived severity of the sanctions for testing positive to a

performance-enhancing substance ........................................................... 106

Page 13: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

xii

Table 4.14: Perceived health risk of ‘once or twice ever’ use of six banned

performance-enhancing substances ......................................................... 107

Table 4.15: Perceived health risk of regular use of six banned performance-

enhancing substances ............................................................................... 109

Table 4.16: Perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on

sport performance .................................................................................... 111

Table 4.17: Perceived positive outcomes for performing well in sports .................... 114

Table 4.18: Desirability of the positive outcomes for performing well in sports ....... 115

Table 4.19: Personal moral stance on banned performance-enhancing

substances use .......................................................................................... 116

Table 4.20: Emotions experienced if caught using performance-enhancing

substances ................................................................................................ 118

Table 4.21: Perceived moral stance on banned performance-enhancing

substances use among people involved in the athlete’s life ..................... 120

Table 4.22: Perceived moral stance on banned performance-enhancing

substances use among people involved in sports in general .................... 122

Table 4.23: Perceived fairness of the Australian Sports Drug Agency in

treating all athletes equally ...................................................................... 124

Table 4.24: Perceived security of the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s drug

testing procedures in Australia ................................................................ 125

Table 4.25: Perceived accuracy of the current drug tests ........................................... 125

Table 4.26: Perceived fairness of the appeals process ................................................ 128

Table 4.27: Drug testing experiences among athletes who had been drug tested ....... 129

Table 4.28: Personality characteristics ........................................................................ 131

Table 4.29: Reactions to a positive event ................................................................... 133

Table 4.30: Reactions to a negative event ................................................................... 134

Page 14: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

xiii

Table 4.31: Perceived availability of six performance-enhancing substances ............ 136

Table 4.32: Perceived affordability of six performance-enhancing substances .......... 138

Table 5.1: Principal components and loadings on threat to health: short-term use .. 144

Table 5.2: Principal components and loadings on threat to health: regular use ........ 144

Table 5.3: Principal components and loadings on benefits of performance-

enhancing substances use on performance .............................................. 147

Table 5.4: Principal components and loadings of emotional items .......................... 148

Table 5.5: Principal components and loadings on reference group opinion ............. 150

Table 5.6: Principal components and loadings of legitimacy items: distributive

and procedural justice .............................................................................. 152

Table 5.7: Principal components and loadings of personality items......................... 153

Table 5.8: Principal components and loadings of optimism items ........................... 155

Table 5.9: Principal components and loadings on availability ................................. 156

Table 5.10: Principal components and loadings on affordability ............................... 157

Table 5.11: Descriptive statistics and internal reliability estimates for the

variables measuring the constructs in the Sport Drug Control model ..... 159

Page 15: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

xiv

INDEX OF FIGURES

_______________________________________________________________________

Figure 1.1: The Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002) .............................. 21

Figure 6.1: Overview of results of structural equation analysis of the Sport

Drug Control model ................................................................................. 164

Figure 6.2: Overview of results of structural equation analysis with affordability

allowed to covary with availability .......................................................... 165

Page 16: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

xv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

_______________________________________________________________________

Professor Rob Donovan, my supervisor, has been instrumental in the completion of my

thesis. His expertise in behavioural research is renowned both nationally and

internationally. I am privileged and immensely grateful to have received his advice and

guidance throughout this research study. For many years he has been my mentor, and

more importantly, a trusted friend. I will treasure his many words of encouragement and

advice, not only in relation to my thesis, but also, to life in general.

I would like to thank Professor Billie Giles-Corti, my coordinating supervisor, for

encouraging me to complete my thesis. Special thanks are extended to Doctor Chad Lin

and Doctor Daniel Gucciardi for their advice on multivariate statistical analyses.

This study was funded by an Australian Research Council grant. I am grateful to the

Australian Sports Drug Agency (now known as the Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority), the Australian Institutes and Academies of Sports, Basketball Australia, the

Australian Football League, the National Rugby League and the Australian Rugby

Union for their assistance in distributing the survey. In addition, I would like to thank

all the athletes who participated in the group meetings and survey.

Thanks go to my golfing mates – Roger Follows, John Klime, Chris Lovatsis, Peter

Matrakis and others – for enjoying with me many years of laughter and taking my mind

off my thesis. The latter, some might say, was done too well.

Page 17: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

xvi

Thank you to Sr Aquinas, Michael Goh, Peter Howat and all those whose support and

encouragement have helped me to complete this thesis.

Special thanks to Jacintha Hee whom I love dearly. Thank you for your patience and

efforts in encouraging me to complete my thesis prior to our wedding later this year.

Finally, I am indebted to my parents and sisters for their continual love, support and

encouragement. They have always been there for me, and for that, I am eternally

thankful.

Page 18: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

1

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION

1.1 The Importance of Sport

There is considerable interest in sport in Australia and internationally. Many social,

health and economic benefits are derived from sport. Several studies have found that

sport participation is positively associated with mental health (e.g., psychological

wellbeing, self-esteem) (Donaldson & Ronan 2006; Fox 1999; Pedersen & Seidman

2004; Saxena et al. 2005; Steptoe & Butler 1996; White, Duda & Keller 1998) and

physical health (Kemper et al. 1990; Rae-Grant et al. 1989; Schumaker, Linwood &

Wood 1986; Steiner et al. 2000). Sport provides young people with positive alternatives

and can also help protect young people from peer and other societal pressures to smoke

and take drugs (Jessor, Chase & Donovan 1980; Escobedo et al. 1993; Kulig, Brener &

McManus 2003; Pate et al. 1996, 2000; Rainey et al. 1996; Verschuur 1987; Winnail et

al. 1997).

In general, sport participation involves substantial amounts of physical activity. There is

increasing epidemiological evidence of the health benefits of regular physical activity,

and a dose response relationship with greater benefits derived from increased intensity

and duration of physical activity (Bauman & Owen 1999; US Department of Health and

Human Services 1996; Lissner et al. 1996; Morris et al. 1990; Oguma et al. 2002; Pate

et al. 1995; Powell et al. 1987; Shephard 1995; World Cancer Research Fund/American

Institute for Cancer Research 2007).

Besides direct health benefits, there are many other positive outcomes of sports

participation by young people including improved academic achievement (Marsh &

Page 19: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

2

Kleitman 2003; Schumaker, Linwood & Wood 1986), decreased delinquency and

school dropout (Chung 2000; Schafer 1969; Yin & Moore 2004), reduced deviant

behaviours and arrests (Buhrmann & Bratton 1977; Cohen et al. 2007; Segrave &

Hastad 1984), decreased suicidal behaviour (Brown & Blanton 2002; Miller et al. 2002)

and increased leadership qualities (Sharpe, Brown & Crider 1995). At the same time,

there have been reports of misconduct in some sports being associated with alcohol and

violence (Dietze, Fitzgerald & Jenkinson 2008; Dimeo 2006; Nattiv & Puffer 1991;

Nelson & Wechsler 2001; O’Brien et al. 2012).

There are many positive effects on the economy derived from sport. According to the

2006 Australian census, 75,155 people were employed in a sport or physical recreation

occupation (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2009). The growth of this sector of the

economy is reflected by the fact that this figure is 23% higher than the corresponding

figure in the 2001 census. [The increase for all occupations over the same period was

lower at 10%.] (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2009). Furthermore, hosting major

sporting events has a significant positive economic impact on Australia. For example,

the 2000 Sydney Olympic Games had direct impacts such as the construction of venues

and other facilities prior to the Games, the sale of television broadcast rights during the

Games, and the influx of athletes and tourists to participate in or watch the Games, and

indirect impacts such as the international tourism resulting from media coverage of

Sydney and Australia during the Games (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2000). The

value of the 2000 Sydney Olympic Games on Australia’s Gross Domestic Product

(GDP) was estimated at $6.5 billion (Madden 2002). An economic impact study of the

2003 Rugby World Cup held in Australia estimated that this event generated $494

million in revenues to the Australian government and created more than 4,000 jobs. The

Page 20: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

3

value of the Cup on Australia’s GDP was estimated at $289 million (Bohlmann & van

Heerden 2005).

1.2 Performance-Enhancing Substances Use in Sport

There has been, and continues to be, widespread international concern about athletes’

use of performance-enhancing substances (British Medical Association 2002; Hunt &

Dimeo 2012; Kamber & Mullis 2010), with athletes themselves perceiving that there is

a problem. For example, in Yesalis and Bahrke’s (1995) survey of 155 United States

Olympians who participated in the 1992 Winter Games in France, 80% of the athletes

classified steroid use among Olympic competitors as a ‘very’ or ‘somewhat’ serious

problem. Only 5% of these athletes thought it was not a problem. In a 2005 survey of

874 athletes in the United Kingdom, 40% considered there to be, to some extent, a

doping issue in their sport (UK Sport 2006).

The issue of performance-enhancing substances use in sport is highlighted by media

coverage of a number of high profile athletes caught taking such substances. The most

recent case involves Lance Armstrong, an American cyclist who won the Tour de

France seven times in consecutive years from 1999 to 2005. In August 2012, he was

banned for life after declaring that he would not contest the five charges that the United

States Anti-Doping Agency filed against him: (1) use and/or attempted use of banned

substances and/or methods including erythropoietin (EPO; a blood boosting hormone),

blood transfusions, testosterone, corticosteroids and masking agents; (2) possession of

banned substances and/or methods including EPO, blood transfusions and related

equipment (such as needles, blood bags, storage containers and other transfusion

Page 21: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

4

equipment and blood parameters measuring devices), testosterone, corticosteroids and

masking agents; (3) trafficking of EPO, testosterone, and corticosteroids; (4)

administration and/or attempted administration to others of EPO, testosterone, and

cortisone; and (5) assisting, encouraging, aiding, abetting, covering up and other

complicity involving one or more anti-doping rule violations and/or attempted anti-

doping rule violations (United States Anti-Doping Agency 2012). In 2006, Marion

Jones a former American track and field athlete tested positive to EPO. In 2007, she

admitted taking steroids prior to the 2000 Sydney Olympic Games in which she won

three gold and two bronze medals. The United States Anti-Doping Agency imposed a 2-

year period of ineligibility and disqualification of all her competitive results from

September 2000 (United States Anti-Doping Agency 2007). The United States Anti-

Doping Agency’s (2013) published list of athletes that have received a sanction for a

doping violation since 2001 highlights the diversity of sports implicated in doping.

High profile athletes are role models for young people not only for achievement in

sports, but life in general (Catlin & Murray 1996; Stamm et al. 2008), and have been

‘actively’ promoted as role models for young people. For example, the ARMtour

(Athletes As Role Models; National Aboriginal Sporting Chance Academy 2013)

program brings high profile Australian athletes to remote Territory communities to

encourage Indigenous youth to adopt healthy lifestyles. Similarly, the Role Models for

the Future program – an initiative of Beyondblue, Athlete Development Australia and

the Bounce Back Foundation – trains elite Australian athletes from a number of sports

(e.g., football, basketball, netball) to promote messages about health and lifestyle to

young people. Hence, elite athletes’ use of performance-enhancing substances not only

sends a message to young athletes, but also to all young people about drugs in general,

Page 22: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

5

including recreational drugs. According to social learning theory (Bandura 1977), which

focuses on learning that occurs within a social environment, high profile athletes can be

viewed as modelling behaviours to young people. In 2003, the former Australian

cricketer Shane Warne’s drug case with connotations of drug use to improve body

image, had the potential to accelerate the emerging trend of young males using steroids

not to improve athletic performance or competitive body building, but for aesthetic

reasons (Baker, Graham & Davies 2006; Benson 2002; DrugScope 2006; DuRant,

Escobedo & Heath 1995; Egan 2002; Parkinson & Evans 2006; Wright, Grogan &

Hunter 2001).

Sporting organisations such as the Australian Academies and Institutes of Sport have

been instrumental in developing international sport champions. According to an

Australian Bureau of Statistics survey in 2005-06, 66% of Australians aged 15 years

and over (i.e., 10.5 million people) participated in sports and physical recreation at least

once during the 12 months prior to the interview (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2007).

However, drug use has the potential to undermine people’s interest in and intent in

participating in sport.

Research shows that parents are socialising agents and primary motivators in children’s

sports participation (Brustad 1993; Gadsdon 2001; Hendry et al. 1993; Jambor 1999;

Lewko & Greendorfer 1988; Smoll, Magill & Ash 1988; Wuerth, Lee & Alfermann

2004). It is through parental example and encouragement that children learn how to play

and to participate in sporting activities. Parents are unlikely to encourage participation

in sports known to have drug problems. This belief is also held by sporting officials, and

is reflected in the following comment by Gian Franco Kasper, President of the

Page 23: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

6

International Ski Federation, Switzerland: “How can we convince parents to let their

children practice a sport if they know that their child’s health will sooner or later be

destroyed by doping?” (World Anti-Doping Agency 2008). Performance-enhancing

substances use has significant actual and potential harmful health effects including

death (Dimeo 2007; Hausmann, Hammer & Betz 1998; Houlihan 2002; Yesalis &

Bahrke 2002).

1.3 The World Anti-Doping Code

In response to the perception that the International Olympic Committee failed to support

and operationalise an effective anti-doping system (e.g., the drug scandal involving the

East German sports system), the 1998 Tour de France drug scandal and following the

proposal of the First World Conference on Doping in Sport, the World Anti-Doping

Agency was established in 1999 as an international independent organisation “to

promote, coordinate and monitor the fight against doping in sport in all its forms”

(Dimeo, Hunt & Horbury 2011; Dimeo & Taylor in press; Hanstad, Smith &

Waddington 2008; Houlihan 2001; Kamber 2011; World Anti-Doping Agency 2010).

The World Anti-Doping Agency is responsible for monitoring the World Anti-Doping

Code that was first adopted in 2003 and became effective in 2004. The Code provides a

framework for anti-doping policies across all sports and countries. Government

signatories to the UNESCO International Convention against Doping in Sport (ratified

in October 2005; UNESCO 2005) are legally bound to comply with the regulations of

the World Anti-Doping Agency and the World Anti-Doping Code (Advisory Council on

the Misuse of Drugs 2010). The Code stipulates that each country is expected to establish

Page 24: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

7

its own national anti-doping organisation. In Australia, the Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority (formerly known as the Australian Sports Drug Agency) was established for

this purpose. A revised Code was approved by the World Anti-Doping Agency in

November 2007 and implemented on 1st January 2009.

The Code contains the list of banned performance-enhancing substances and methods.

Substances and methods are placed on the banned list if they meet two of the following

three criteria, or if they have the potential to mask the use of such substances or

methods: (1) medical or other scientific evidence, pharmacological effect or experience

that the substance or method, alone or in combination with other substances or methods,

has the potential to enhance or enhances sport performance; (2) medical or other

scientific evidence, pharmacological effect or experience that the use of the substance or

method represents an actual or potential health risk to the athlete; or (3) the World Anti-

Doping Agency’s determination that the use of the substance or method violates the

spirit of sport (World Anti-Doping Agency 2009, p. 32-33). The banned list is updated

on an annual basis, and signatories to the Code are obliged to distribute the list to its

members and constituents.

1.4 Prevalence of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

The prevalence of performance-enhancing substances use in sport has been assessed in

three main ways: (1) prevalence of adverse analytical findings; (2) perceived prevalence

of banned performance-enhancing substances use; and (3) self-reported use of banned

performance-enhancing substances.

Page 25: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

8

1.4.1 Prevalence of adverse analytical findings

The World Anti-Doping Code defines an adverse analytical finding as “a report from a

laboratory or other WADA-approved entity that, consistent with the International

Standard for Laboratories and related technical documents, identifies in a sample the

presence of a prohibited substance or its metabolites or markers (including elevated

quantities of endogenous substances) or evidence of the use of a prohibited method”

(World Anti-Doping Agency 2009, p. 126), and defines an atypical finding as “a report

from a laboratory or other WADA-approved entity which requires further investigation

as provided by the International Standard for Laboratories or related technical

documents prior to the determination of an adverse analytical finding” (World Anti-

Doping Agency 2009, p. 128).

From 2003 to 2011, the proportion of samples analysed by WADA-approved anti-

doping laboratories that resulted in adverse analytical findings and atypical findings

ranged between 1.62% and 2.13% (World Anti-Doping Agency 2011a). From 1993 to

2003, the corresponding proportions by the International Olympic Committee’s

accredited laboratories ranged between 1.36% and 1.98% (Mottram 2005).

In each year, adverse analytical findings are found in a variety of sports in numerous

countries. For example, in 2011, 91% (n = 32) of the sports recognised by the

International Olympic Committee reported adverse analytical findings.

Page 26: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

9

1.4.2 Perceived prevalence of banned performance-enhancing substances use

In some studies, athletes, coaches and other people involved in sports were asked to

estimate performance-enhancing substances use among athletes.

In Scarpino et al.’s (1990) study, 1,015 Italian athletes and 216 doctors, coaches and

managers estimated the frequency of use of performance-enhancing substances among

elite athletes at the national and international level. Among athletes, estimated use was

highest for anabolic steroids (42%; frequent use: 16%; occasional use: 26%), followed

by amphetamines (38%; frequent use: 11%; occasional use: 27%), blood doping (32%;

frequent use: 7%; occasional use: 25%), and beta-blockers (8%; frequent use: 2%;

occasional use: 6%). Among doctors, coaches and managers, the figures were similar:

anabolic steroids (46%; frequent use: 20%; occasional use: 26%); amphetamines (23%;

frequent use: 6%; occasional use: 17%); blood doping (24%; frequent use: 7%;

occasional use: 17%); and beta-blockers (16%; frequent use: 3%; occasional use: 13%).

Two other studies (Laure & Reinsberger 1995; Ohaeri et al. 1993) conducted prior to

2000 had a more specific focus in terms of the type of sport and performance-enhancing

substances used. In Laure and Reinsberger’s (1995) survey of 102 elite French

endurance walkers, 41% had heard of other endurance walkers using performance-

enhancing substances. In Ohaeri et al.’s (1993) survey of 250 Nigerian athletes (mean

age: 24.6 years), 15% claimed they knew athletes who had used anabolic steroids to

enhance performance.

In Waddington et al.’s (2005) study, among a sample of 706 professional United

Kingdom footballers, 6% indicated that they personally knew current or former players

Page 27: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

10

who used performance-enhancing substances, and 2% indicated that they knew players

at their current club who used such substances.

In Alaranta et al.’s (2006) study, among a sample of 446 elite athletes in Finland (mean

age: 23.0 years), 30% reported that they knew an athlete who had used performance-

enhancing substances. The proportions for specific substances were: stimulants: 17%;

anabolic steroids: 10%; blood doping: 6%; peptide hormones: 6%; oral glucocorticoids:

4%; sedatives: 3%; diuretics: 2%; beta-blockers: 1%; and narcotic analgesics: 0.5%.

Analysis of the data by type of sports revealed that the proportion was highest for

athletes in speed and power sports (42%), followed by endurance sports (37%), team

sports (22%), and demanding motor skills sports (18%). There were some variations in

reports of use of these substances by type of sports: one third of the athletes in speed

and power sports personally knew someone using stimulants compared with less than

15% for each of the other types of sport; and 18% of the endurance athletes knew

someone using blood doping compared with less than 5% for each of the other types of

sport.

In Moran et al.’s (2008) survey of 375 elite athletes (mean age: 23.8 years) from 16

different countries (mainly Ireland: 69% of the total sample), 25% of athletes claimed to

personally know athletes who had used, or were currently using banned substances.

The reliability of these data on perceptions of performance-enhancing substances use

has received criticism for a number of reasons including self-reporting bias of doping

for fear of social and legalistic sanctions, and reliance on anecdotal evidence of doping

by other athletes (Dimeo & Taylor in press; Mottram 2011; Petróczi et al. 2011).

Furthermore, the data may be biased by recent media reports of cases of performance-

Page 28: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

11

enhancing substances use, and the actual number of athletes perceived to be using

performance-enhancing substances cannot be ascertained as respondents may well be

referring to the same athlete. Nevertheless, these data reflect the salience of the issue of

performance-enhancing substances use, and there is some evidence that athletes are

more likely to intend to use performance-enhancing substances in the future if they

believe that other athletes in their sport are engaging in performance-enhancing

substances use (see Kondric et al. 2011; Zenic et al. 2010)

1.4.3 Self-reported use of banned performance-enhancing substances

There are a number of studies that presented data on self-reported performance-

enhancing substances use among athletes. The data are categorised here according to:

(1) athletes in high school or lower; (2) college athletes; and (3) adult athletes and elite

athletes that do not fall into the first two categories. Studies that investigated self-

reported performance-enhancing substances among adolescents in general or that do not

present the data for athletes are not discussed here (e.g., Gradidge, Coopoo &

Constantinou 2011; Mattila et al. 2009; Nilsson et al. 2005; Rees, Zarco & Lewis 2008;

Thorlton et al. 2012).

Athletes in high school or lower:

Among a sample of 1,553 pre-high school youth sports participants aged 10 to 14 years

in 34 states in the United States of America, 0.7% reported current or previous usage of

anabolic steroids (Wroble, Gray & Rodrigo 2002).

Among high school athletes, self-reported personal use of banned performance-

enhancing substances was 3.1% among a sample of 300 athletes in Italy (Lucidi et al.

Page 29: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

12

2004), 4.0% among a sample of 6,402 athletes in France (Laure & Binsinger 2005), and

2.5% among a sample of 241 athletes in the United States (Dodge & Jaccard 2008). In

the samples of Italian and French athletes, reported use of banned performance-

enhancing substances was higher among males than females: 3.8% vs 1.8%; and 5.6%

vs 2.6% respectively.

In Wanjek et al.’s (2010) study of recreational athletes (i.e., those who engage in, on

average, seven hours of sports per week) and competitive athletes (i.e., those who

engage in, on average, 20 hours of sports per week) in the state of Thuringia (Germany),

reported use of a number of banned performance-enhancing substances was low for

both types of athletes: stimulants (2.3% and 1.2%); anabolic androgenic steroids (0.9%

and 0.4%); growth hormones (0.6% and 0.2%); and erythropoietin (0.2%, 0%),

respectively.

In Gradidge, Coopoo & Constantinou’s (2011) survey of 100 male high school athletes

aged 15 to 18 years in Johannesburg (South Africa), the proportions who reported

having used growth hormone and anabolic androgenic steroids were 5% and 4%,

respectively.

In Striegel, Ulrich and Simon’s (2010) survey of 480 elite German athletes aged 15 to

18 years (median age: 16 years), 6.8% of athletes reported having ‘ever used doping

substances’ when questioned via a randomised response technique (an anonymous

indirect interview technique).

Page 30: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

13

College athletes:

In the National Collegiate Athletic Association’s (NCAA) survey of 20,474 athletes in

2009, reported performance-enhancing substances use in the last 12 months was 3.7%

for amphetamines, 0.9% for ephedrine, and 0.6% for anabolic steroids (Bracken 2012).

The proportions for amphetamines and anabolic steroids were lower than in the last

NCAA survey conducted in 2005: 1.4% and 1.3% respectively. The author reported that

ephedrine usage was not included in the 2005 survey in a way that was comparable with

the 2009 survey. Green et al. (2001) reported that in the 1996 NCAA survey (N =

13,914 athletes), amphetamines usage was 3.1% which was higher than in 2005. In

1996, anabolic steroids usage was 1.1%. In Spence and Gauvin’s (1996) study, 0.9% of

the 754 Canadian university athletes surveyed reported use of anabolic steroids.

Adult athletes:

In Laure’s (1997) review of self-reported use studies in adult athletes between 1980 and

1996, performance-enhancing substances use ranged between 5% and 15%.

The National Football League (NFL) in the United States first tested for anabolic

steroids in 1988, and detected its use in 6% of professional football players (Cowart

1988). Among 2,552 NFL retired professional football players who completed a self-

reported general health questionnaire between 2001 and 2003, 9% of the total sample

reported the use of steroids during their professional career. The highest reported

prevalence was among those who played primarily during the 1980s: 1940s: 0%; 1950s:

1%; 1960s: 5%; 1970s: 10%; 1980s: 20%; and 1990 to 2003: 13% (Horn, Gregory &

Guskiewicz 2009). In Ama et al.’s (2003) study of 314 elite African footballers (mean

age: 20.4%), 5% reported using cocaine which was a banned substance.

Page 31: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

14

In Özdemir et al.’s (2005) survey of 433 elite Turkish athletes, 14.5% reported using

doping and/or performance-enhancing substances. No information was provided as to

the distinction between these two categories nor the proportion for each category.

In Tsorbatzoudis et al.’s (2009) survey of 1,040 elite Greek athletes from nine different

sports (football, basketball, volleyball, handball, athletics, swimming, shooting,

taekwondo, and rowing) (mean age: 22.9 years), 8.2% reported past use of performance-

enhancing substances (systematic use: 1.5%; occasional use: 3.0%; use only once in the

past: 3.7%). Similar results were found in a survey of 1,075 elite Greek athletes: 9.9%

reported past use of performance-enhancing substances (systematic use: 2.0%;

occasional use: 3.6%; use only once in the past: 4.3% (Lazuras et al. 2010).

In Uvacsek et al.’s (2011) survey of 82 Hungarian athletes, 14.6% reported using

banned performance-enhancing substances. It is noteworthy that the sample was

recruited by the authors’ personal and professional contacts with athletes.

In two studies (Somerville & Lewis 2005; Moran et al. 2008), elite athletes were asked

whether they had ever inadvertently used banned performance-enhancing substances.

Among 74 elite British athletes (mean age: 27 years) who participated in athletics,

cycling, rowing and sailing, 5% reported that they had accidentally used a performance-

enhancing substance in the past (Somerville & Lewis 2005). In Moran et al.’s (2008)

survey of 375 elite athletes, 3% reported that they had inadvertently used banned

substances in the past. In the same survey, 2% of athletes reported that they had

knowingly used banned performance-enhancing substances. The authors did not clarify

whether there was any overlap between athletes who reported inadvertent versus

intentional use of banned substances.

Page 32: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

15

The validity of self-reported performance-enhancing substances use may be adversely

impacted by confidentiality issues, which is highlighted by two other studies (Alaranta

et al. 2006; Ohaeri et al. 1993). In Alaranta et al.’s (2006) study, none of the 446 elite

Finnish athletes surveyed (mean age: 23.0 years) reported ever using banned substances.

Given that the questionnaire was conducted during national team camps, athletes’

perception of the confidentiality of the survey may have been compromised. Similarly,

confidentiality may have been an issue in Ohaeri et al.’s (1993) survey of 250 Nigerian

athletes (mean age: 24.6 years) as the questionnaire was distributed and returned

through the athletes’ team captains. In that survey, 1.2% of athletes reported having

used anabolic steroids. Self-reported use of banned performance-enhancing substances

is clearly susceptible to under-reporting of use. Compromising confidentiality in the

survey administration context would further exacerbate this limitation.

Lentillion-Kaestner & Ohl (2011) found that among a sample of 1,920 French Swiss

school athletes, self-reported use of performance-enhancing substances differed widely

between 1.3% and 39.2% depending on the definition of doping and the types of

questions used (e.g., open versus closed ended questions). This finding is not surprising

as previous research has demonstrated that people’s responses can be readily

manipulated or influenced by different question formats (e.g., McAuliffe et al. 2007;

Waller, McCaffery and Wardle 2004; King 1994), introductory statements or message

content (e.g., Donovan and Jalleh, 1999, 2000; Jalleh and Donovan 2001), response

categories or order (e.g., Sturgis, Allum and Smith 2008; Donovan 1998; Kronsnick

1999), data collection modes (e.g., Donovan et al. 1997; Tourangeau and Smith 1996),

and so on. Hence, as suggested by Lentillion-Kaestner & Ohl (2011), the use of multiple

question formats including measuring use of specific substances is necessary to give a

Page 33: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

16

more reliable prevalence of doping. [In this study, this approach was adopted in

assessing athletes’ use of performance-enhancing substances.]

1.4.4 Summary

The statistics on adverse analytical findings clearly demonstrate performance-enhancing

substances use in a variety of sports. Levels of perceived prevalence and self-reported

personal use of performance-enhancing substances varies widely, but are substantially

higher than the figures for adverse analytical findings. This reflects the difficulty in

testing for and detecting use of performance-enhancing substances. [Section 2.1

provides a discussion on this topic.]

1.5 Current Anti-Doping Programs

To date, only a small number of studies have been conducted to explore the

effectiveness of intervention programs for preventing athlete performance-enhancing

substances use. The two prevention programs that have been evaluated are the

Adolescents Training and Learning to Avoid Steroids (ATLAS) (Goldberg et al. 1996,

2000), and Athletes Targeting Healthy Exercise and Nutrition Alternatives (ATHENA)

(Elliott et al. 2004, 2006) programs.

The ATLAS program aims to reduce intentions and actual use of anabolic steroids and

other substances (e.g., alcohol, illicit drugs), and to promote healthy nutrition and

exercise behaviours among male high school athletes. The 10-week program consists of

three components: (1) classroom-based education; (2) weight-room skill training; and

(3) an informational session for parents that focuses on the program goals and content.

Page 34: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

17

The program focuses on reducing anabolic steroid use by presenting nutrition and

strength training programs as alternatives to anabolic steroid use.

Goldberg et al.’s (2000) pre (N = 3,207), post (intervention group: N = 1,145; control

group: N = 1,371) and 1-year follow-up (intervention group: N = 591; control group: N

= 700) surveys of male high school football athletes in the United States showed that in

the post-survey, intentions to use and actual use of anabolic steroids were significantly

lower among the intervention than control group. In the 1-year follow-up survey,

intentions to use and actual use of anabolic steroids remained lower among the

intervention than control group, but the difference was significant for intentions to use

only. Further analysis of this same dataset found that athletes with a higher degree of

intent to use anabolic steroids before the program were more likely to decrease their

intentions to use anabolic steroids after the intervention than those with lower intentions

to use anabolic steroids (Fritz et al. 2005). The effectiveness of the ATLAS program in

reducing actual use of anabolic steroids cannot be ascertained from these data as the

numbers of new anabolic steroids users (cumulative from baseline) in the control and

intervention groups were small: post-survey: 18 and 7 new users respectively; and 1-

year follow-up survey: 19 and 9 new users respectively.

The ATHENA program aims to reduce disordered eating and body-shaping substances

use among female high school athletes. The program’s eight weekly 45-minute sessions

cover sport nutrition, exercise training, substance use and other unhealthy behaviours’

effects on sport performance, media images of females, and depression prevention.

Elliot et al.’s (2004) pre (N = 928) and post (intervention group: N = 337; control group:

N = 331) intervention surveys of female high school athletes in team sports in the

Page 35: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

18

United States showed that athletes in the intervention group reported less new use of

athletic-enhancing and body-shaping substances, including anabolic steroids. However,

the data specifically for anabolic steroids were not presented in this paper nor in later

papers on the outcomes of the ATHENA program (Elliot et al. 2006, 2008).

Notwithstanding other positive outcomes of the ATLAS and ATHENA programs, there

is insufficient evidence of these programs’ effectiveness in reducing performance-

enhancing substances use. More importantly, these programs do not provide a

theoretical framework for understanding or influencing performance-enhancing

substances use.

1.6 Theoretical Models of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

Backhouse et al.’s (2007) comprehensive review of the drugs in sport literature

concluded that a significant limitation of the body of knowledge at that time was the

limited application and empirical validation of doping models in sport. The review

identified only two conceptual models that attempted to provide a framework for

understanding performance-enhancing substances use: the Sport Drug Control model

(Donovan et al. 2002) and the Drugs in Sport Deterrence model (Strelan & Boeckmann

2003).

1.6.1 The Drugs in Sport Deterrence model

The Drugs in Sport Deterrence model (Strelan & Boeckmann 2003) is based on

deterrence theory which is an approach to understanding compliance with the law (see

Paternoster 1987). The model consists of three components: (1) the deterrence or costs

Page 36: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

19

of a decision to use performance-enhancing substances which are categorised in terms

of legal sanctions (i.e., fines and suspensions), social sanctions (i.e., disapproval by

team mates, criticism by important others, material loss), self-imposed sanctions (i.e.,

guilt, reduced self-esteem) and health concerns (i.e., negative side-effects of using

performance-enhancing substances); (2) the benefits associated with using performance-

enhancing substances which are categorised in terms of material benefits (e.g., prize

money, sponsorship, endorsement, contracts), social benefits (e.g., prestige,

acknowledgement by important others) and internalised benefits (e.g., satisfaction of

high achievement); and (3) situational factors that may impact on the perceived cost-

benefit analysis of using performance-enhancing substances. The potential mediating

situational factors listed in the model are: perceptions of the prevalence of performance-

enhancing substances use; experience with punishment and punishment avoidance;

perceptions of one’s competitiveness; professional status; perceptions of the legitimacy

of the regulating authorities in enforcing anti-doping rules; and the performance-

enhancing properties and side-effects of the substance.

The model postulates that the deterrence or costs associated with performance-

enhancing substances use are weighed up against the benefits of using such substances,

and this cost-benefit analysis is influenced by situational factors. There are no published

data examining the utility of this model.

1.6.2 The Sport Drug Control model

The Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002) was the first published theoretical

model of factors influencing performance-enhancing substances use. The model was

developed from three behavioural science frameworks: social cognition models, threat

Page 37: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

20

(or fear) appeals, and instrumental and normative approaches. The model consists of six

components believed to predict an athlete’s attitudes and intentions towards

performance-enhancing substances use: (1) threat appraisal; (2) benefit appraisal; (3)

personal morality; (4) reference group opinion; (5) legitimacy; and (6) personality. In

addition, two ‘market’ factors, the affordability and availability of performance-

enhancing substances, believed to facilitate or inhibit the translation of attitudes and

intentions into behaviour were included in the model (see Figure 1.1). The Sport Drug

Control model incorporates many of the components of the Drugs in Sport Deterrence

model and is far more comprehensive. Chapter 2 provides a detailed description of this

model.

Donovan (2009) later placed the Sport Drug Control model in two broader contexts: the

overall sociocultural context of a ready acceptance of new technologies that save time

and effort, prolong life, prevent suffering and enhance body image and cognitive

functioning; and a sport culture that has become medicalised and commercialised.

However, the focus in this thesis is on the constructs of the model shown in Figure 1.1.

Besides an opportunistic examination of some of the constructs in the Sport Drug

Control model that were included in a larger questionnaire that was concerned with

personality profiling of elite athletes and their susceptibility to performance-enhancing

substances use (Gucciardi, Jalleh & Donovan 2011), there is no published empirical

examination of the model. [Section 2.11 presents the results of this published paper.]

Page 38: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

21

Figure 1.1: The Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002)

THREAT APPRAISAL

[DETERRENCE]

BENEFIT APPRAISAL

[INCENTIVE]

PERSONAL

MORALITY

LEGITIMACY

ATTITUDES/

INTENTIONS

RE PEDs

BEHAVIOUR

COMPLIANCE /

NON COMPLIANCE

REFERENCE GROUP

OPINION

PERSONALITY /

SELF ESTEEM

OPTIMISM

AFFORDABILITY

AVAILABILITY

The Sport Drug Control model theorised that multiple factors influence performance-

enhancing substances use. Backhouse et al.’s (2007) review of the sports literature

found few empirical studies investigating the various factors presented in the model,

with most of these studies examining one (or only a few) of these factors, and hence,

not accounting for other factors or the inter-relationship between multiple factors.

Since Backhouse et al.’s (2007) review, two other conceptual models that attempt to

provide a framework for understanding performance-enhancing substances are Petróczi

and Aidman’s (2008) life-cycle model of performance enhancement and Stewart and

Smith’s (2008) model of drug use in sport.

Page 39: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

22

Petróczi and Aidman’s (2008) life-cycle model of performance enhancement posits that

in the course of their career, athletes constantly set goals and make choices regarding

the way these goals can be achieved. Opportunities for behaviour change, including

performance-enhancing substances use, are presented throughout the cycle of ‘choice –

goal commitment – execution – feedback on goal attainment – goal

evaluation/adjustment’. The model is based on expectancy theory, hence athletes’

motivation to engage in performance-enhancing substances use is assumed to be

influenced by the desire to attain expected positive outcomes, and at the same time,

controlled by the expected undesirable outcomes from use of performance-enhancing

substances. The model focuses on vulnerability and inhibiting factors across the stages

of athlete development. These factors are categorised as individual differences

(psychological variables such as dispositional risk taking and sensation seeking, attitude

towards peers, authority and fair play, self-esteem, confidence and integrity, cognitive

ability, beliefs about doping efficacy, independence and vulnerability to peer pressure)

and systematic factors (e.g., motivational climate, fairness and other attributes of the

testing procedures and enforcement sanctions). Both of these factors are moderated by

situational factors (e.g., peer interactions, salience of role models and significant others,

availability of performance enhancement alternatives). There is no published empirical

testing of this model, the difficulty of which is recognised by the authors: “Considering

the complexity and reiterative nature of the model, empirical testing of the model as a

whole is not feasible” (Petróczi & Aidman 2008, p. 7).

Similar to Donovan’s (2009) expansion, Stewart and Smith’s (2008) model of drug use

in sport combines the micro orientation of individual athlete intentions with a macro

orientation on sporting context and culture. The authors argue that decisions made by

athletes are not always rational or bound by clear intentionality. Hence contextual

Page 40: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

23

factors may affect athletes’ values, beliefs, and decision making. The authors identified

three contextual factors: globalisation and commercialisation of sport; sporting culture;

and perceptions of masculinity and identity. There is no published empirical

examination of the model, however the authors and colleagues (Smith et al. 2010)

explored contextual factors influencing athletes’ attitudes towards drugs in sport via 11

narrative-based case histories. Eight of the participants were male and three were

female. Six of the participants completed at the elite level, while the others played in

community leagues and competitions. Results indicated that athletes’ attitudes towards

drugs in sport were influenced by a sporting culture that lends itself to drug use through

its emphasis on performance at all costs or the use of substances as a social lubricant. In

addition, the more frequent the early experiences of serious competition, the more likely

athletes were to hold favourable attitudes towards both licit and illicit performance-

enhancing substances. The main commercial pressures impacting on participants’

attitudes to substance use were professionalisation, sponsorship, material reward and

fame.

The lack of empirical evidence to support or refute any of these conceptual models

leaves a significant gap in the literature in understanding influences on performance-

enhancing substances use.

1.7 Research Objectives of this Study

This study will empirically test the Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002).

The research objectives are:

Page 41: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

24

1. To assess via structural equation modelling, the extent to which the variables in the

model together predict attitude towards and use of performance-enhancing

substances; and

2. To assess the relative importance of the various concepts in the model. Specifically,

to determine: (a) the relative influence of threat appraisal, benefit appraisal,

personal morality, reference group opinion, legitimacy, and personality on attitude

towards the use of performance-enhancing substances; and (b) the influence of

attitude towards use of performance-enhancing substances and affordability and

availability of performance-enhancing substances on the use of such substances.

Confirmation and quantification of the model have the potential to provide direct input

to anti-doping organisations’ programs for the prevention of doping in sport.

1.8 Structure of this Thesis

This thesis consists of eight chapters. Chapter 2 describes the Sport Drug Control model

and provides a literature review of the concepts in the model. Chapter 3 provides the

methodological details of this research study. The chapter starts with the ontological and

epistemological stances adopted. Descriptive results, including the sample

characteristics, are presented in Chapter 4. Chapter 5 describes the development of each

of the constructs for use in testing the Sport Drug Control model. Chapter 6 presents the

results of the structural equation modelling analyses in testing the model, and details the

influence of each of the constructs in the model on predicting attitude towards and

actual use of performance-enhancing substances. Chapter 7 provides a discussion of the

results in relation to the literature and the implications for the content of anti-doping

Page 42: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

25

education programs and future research. The limitations of this study are also discussed.

The final chapter summarises the major findings of this study.

Page 43: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

26

CHAPTER 2: THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL

The Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002) was based on a review of public

health interventions in general and the drugs in sports literature. The model includes six

psychosocial inputs to an athlete’s attitudes and intentions towards performance-

enhancing substances use. One of these relates to personality factors (e.g., risk taking

propensity, self-esteem, optimism), while the other five correspond to elements from

various attitude and behaviour change models: threat appraisal (perceived negative

outcomes of using a banned performance-enhancing substance, including the likelihood

of being caught, the severity of the penalties, and ill-health effects); benefit appraisal

(perceived positive outcomes of using a banned performance-enhancing substance,

including social and financial rewards); personal morality (moral judgment towards

using a banned performance-enhancing substance); reference group opinions

(perceptions about relevant others’ attitude towards using a banned performance-

enhancing substance); and legitimacy (beliefs about whether laws governing

performance-enhancing substances use are justified and applied fairly and equitably

across all athletes). The model includes two ‘market’ factors, affordability and

availability of performance-enhancing substances, that may facilitate or inhibit use of

these substances.

The theoretical foundations supporting these eight major inputs in the model are

described below. Donovan et al. (2002) provided an important clarification of the

model, that is, the model is dependent on athletes’ perceptions with respect to these

inputs which may or may not reflect reality. Hence, to deter athletes from using

performance-enhancing substances, anti-doping organisations may seek to influence

athletes’ perceptions of these inputs. Donovan et al. (2002) provided the example that

Page 44: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

27

the deterrence effect of drug testing is enhanced by athletes’ overestimating their

likelihood of being tested out of competition. Conversely, athletes’ underestimating this

likelihood would lessen the deterrence effect of drug testing. Hence, where available,

empirical evidence of athletes’ perceptions in relation to each of these inputs in the

model are also presented. It is noteworthy that a number of the studies referred to in

support of the constructs in the model were published after the Donovan et al. (2002)

paper, and confirm the relevance and importance of these constructs in understanding

performance-enhancing substances use.

2.1 Threat Appraisal

Donovan et al. (2002) posit that threat appraisal consists of two appraisals: (1) the

negative consequences of being caught (enforcement); and (2) the possible ill-health

effects of performance-enhancing substances use.

2.1.1 Appraisal of threat of enforcement

Threats relating to enforcement (i.e., detection and sanctions) have been and continue to

be the main approach of anti-doping efforts. This is reflected in the monetary spend by

anti-doping organisations on enforcement. For example, in 2008-09, the Australian

Sports Anti-Doping Authority spent $9.6 million on its detection and enforcement

program (i.e., funds related to drug testing and prosecution of anti-doping rule

violations), while $4.4 million was spent on its education program (Australian Sports

Anti-Doping Authority 2009).

Page 45: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

28

The threat appraisal component of the Protection Motivation Theory (Rogers 1983)

provides a useful framework for conceptualising the appraisal of threat of enforcement

of the anti-doping rules stipulated in the World Anti-Doping Code. The theory

postulates that a threat is appraised on two factors: the perceived severity of the threat if

it occurs, and the perceived likelihood of the threat occurring. Hence, the appraisal of

threat of enforcement is appraised on the perceived severity of the consequences of

detection of banned performance-enhancing substances use and the perceived likelihood

of detection of banned performance-enhancing substances use. This theoretical

framework implies that increasing the perceived likelihood of detection and the

perceived severity of the negative consequences of detection will deter athletes from

using banned performance-enhancing substances.

According to Donovan et al. (2002), the relationship between these factors is

multiplicative, and that both the perceived severity of the consequences of detection and

the perceived likelihood of detection must achieve a certain threshold to have any

deterrence effect. That is, compliance behaviour will not occur if: (a) the sanctions for

using performance-enhancing substances are severe but there is no drug testing program

in place; and (b) conversely, where athletes are drug tested on a regular basis but there

are no or only minimal sanctions for the detection of banned performance-enhancing

substances.

Donovan et al. (2002), proposed five measures of enforcement threat appraisal: (1)

perceived severity of the consequences of detection; (2) likelihood of being tested; (3)

predictability of drug testing; (4) likelihood of detecting performance-enhancing

substances; and (5) likelihood of successfully appealing a positive test.

Page 46: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

29

Perceived severity of the consequences of detection:

The direct consequence of an anti-doping violation is the sanction imposed. Under the

World Anti-Doping Code, the sanction is two years ineligibility for a first anti-doping

violation and lifetime ineligibility for a second anti-doping violation (World Anti-

Doping Agency 2009).

Indirect consequences of an anti-doping violation include current and future financial

loss, social ostracism and personal achievement loss. The extent to which these factors

inhibit use of banned substances and methods would vary according to an athlete’s

motives for participation in sports (discussed later in Section 2.2: ‘Benefit Appraisal’).

There is a paucity of published research on athlete’s perception of the severity of these

sanctions. Among a sample of 706 professional footballers in the United Kingdom, a

majority of footballers felt that the sanction for using performance-enhancing

substances was ‘about right’ (59%). However, one in four footballers (25%) felt it was

‘not severe enough’. Only 3% of footballers felt that the sanction was ‘too severe’

(Waddington et al. 2005). Jalleh and Donovan (2007) investigated elite Australian

athletes’ beliefs about the appropriateness of the sanctions for a first anti-doping rule

violation for intentional use of a banned performance-enhancing substance (sample size:

368 athletes). For a first offence, 33% of athletes nominated two years ineligibility and

40% nominated a longer ineligibility period (lifetime ineligibility: 24%). These data

suggest that elite Australian athletes are largely intolerant of a first doping offence and

that two years ineligibility for a first offence may well be perceived to be too lenient. In

a more recent study of a sample of 974 elite Australian athletes, 63% of athletes

(strongly) agreed that the punishment of being caught using banned substances is of

Page 47: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

30

appropriate severity, and 23% (strongly) agreed that the punishment should be more

severe (Dunn et al. 2010).

Likelihood of being tested:

The number and frequency of athletes subjected to drug testing are constrained by the

financial costs associated with collecting and analysing samples, such as sample

collection personnel, improving detection technology and establishing testing

laboratories. From 2006-07 to 2008-09, the average costs per year of the Australian

Sports Anti-Doping Authority’s detection program (i.e., intelligence gathering, targeted

testing and investigation) was $7.4 million, and their enforcement program (i.e.,

managing and presenting cases of possible anti-doping rule violations to the Court of

Arbitration for Sport, other sporting tribunals, and the Administrative Appeals Tribunal)

was $1.7 million (Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2008, 2009). Given that

21,088 drug tests were conducted during this time period, the average cost of a drug test

in relation to detection and enforcement was $1,288.

In 2005, a survey of 874 athletes in the United Kingdom found that 52% had been tested

in competition and 41% had been tested out of competition (UK Sport 2006). Among

athletes who had undertaken a drug test, a substantial minority of athletes reported that

they had not been tested in the last 18 months (not tested in competition: 28%; not

tested out of competition: 22%). In 2000, a survey of 101 elite German athletes found

that over 60% felt that the current frequency of drug testing was ‘not often enough’, and

only 21% felt it was at the ‘right’ level (Striegel, Vollkommer & Dickhuth 2002).

The World Anti-Doping Code states that drug testing should take place at both national

and international events. The Code stipulates that the organisation responsible for

Page 48: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

31

coordinating the collection of doping control samples is the ruling body for the

international event (e.g., the International Olympic Committee for the Olympic Games,

the International Federation for a World Championship event), and at a national event,

the national anti-doping organisation of that country. Currently, there are 33 laboratories

worldwide that are accredited by the World Anti-Doping Agency to conduct doping

control sample analyses (World Anti-Doping Agency 2012).

Over the years, the number of athletes drug tested has substantially increased. This is

reflected in the number of samples analysed by the World Anti-Doping Agency

accredited laboratories which has increased from 89,166 in 1993 (Mottram 2005) to

243,193 in 2011 (World Anti-Doping Agency 2011a).

However, there is some evidence that many elite athletes still perceive their likelihood

of being tested as ‘unlikely’. In 2005, among 706 professional footballers in the United

Kingdom, less than half of the sample felt that they were likely to be drug tested in the

next 12 months (40%; ‘certain’: 2%; ‘likely’: 38%) (Waddington et al. 2005). This

belief may well be based on their actual experience of drug testing with 35% of

footballers reporting not been drug tested in the preceding two years. Of the total

sample, 33% were drug tested only once in this time period (tested twice: 18%; tested

three or more times: 14%). Approximately three in four footballers felt that drug testing

was ‘certainly’ or ‘probably’ a deterrent to use of performance-enhancing substances

(73%). However a substantial minority of footballers felt that drug testing was

‘certainly’ or ‘probably’ not a deterrent (23%). Similarly, in Dunn et al. (2010) study,

76% of elite Australian athletes (strongly) agreed that testing is an effective deterrent to

drug use.

Page 49: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

32

Predictability of drug testing:

Predictability of drug testing depends on two factors: (1) whether or not the athlete has

advance notice; and (2) in and out of competition testing.

The predictability of drug testing was substantially reduced with the introduction of no

advance notice testing. Previous advance-warning drug testing policy was considered to

be ineffective as it provided athletes with the opportunity to withdraw from competition.

For example, the withdrawal of a large number of athletes from the 1983 Pan American

Games was attributed to the announcement that gas chromatography-mass spectrometry

would be used to test for anabolic steroids at the Games (Bowers 1998; Houlihan 2002).

The World Anti-Doping Code states that national anti-doping organisations are

responsible for establishing a national ‘Registered Testing Pool’ (see Australian Sports

Anti-Doping Authority 2013 for the criteria used in selecting athletes) to collect,

monitor and manage athletes’ whereabouts information. Athletes are obliged to provide

information as to where they will be at all times. This whereabouts information enables

the location of athletes for no advance notice testing. In recent years, the Australian

Sports Anti-Doping Authority has conducted virtually all of their drug testing with no

advance notice (2007-08 to 2010-11: over 99% in each time period; 2006-07: 97%;

2005-06: 84%; Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2006, 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010,

2011).

The predictability of drug testing was also reduced with the introduction of out of

competition testing. In 1995, a survey of 874 United Kingdom athletes found that 41%

had been tested out of competition (tested in competition: 52%) (Sports Council 1996).

From 1998-99 to 2008-09, 63% of all drug tests conducted by the Australian Sports

Page 50: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

33

Anti-Doping Authority occurred out of competition (ranged between 52% and 72% in

each year) (Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2008, 2009).

The World Anti-Doping Agency manages an out of competition testing program that

targets areas of the world with minimal or insufficient anti-doping programs. In 2011,

this out of competition testing program completed 742 urine and 215 blood tests on

athletes from 71 different countries (World Anti-Doping Agency 2011b).

Given the implementation of no advance notice out of competition drug testing and the

capability to test athletes anytime and anywhere, it could be argued that the

predictability of drug testing is low for most athletes.

Likelihood of detecting performance-enhancing substances:

The likelihood of a test detecting a substance may vary according to the

pharmacological properties of that substance. Water soluble anabolic-androgenic

steroids taken in tablet form are usually expelled from the body within weeks, whereas

oil-based anabolic-androgenic steroids take months to completely clear the body (Voy

& Deeter 1991). Stimulants, beta-blockers, narcotics and diuretics must be taken within

a few hours of competition to have a physiological effect. The use of these substances

can be detected in a urine test taken up to 48 hours after use.

Prior to out of competition testing, it was much easier for athletes to avoid detection of

banned performance-enhancing substances use. For example, given that some steroids

are eliminated by the body within a few weeks, athletes were able to time their use of

steroids to avoid detection during competition.

Page 51: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

34

The Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority and national anti-doping organisations

have the capability to store urine and blood samples for up to eight years. In the event of

advances in detection technology, these samples may be subjected to further testing.

This approach is in response to the time lapse between the time when anti-doping

organisations are aware of a previously unknown performance-enhancing substance and

the subsequent time required to develop reliable and practical detection techniques to

detect the substance. For example, in 2003, the United States Anti-Doping Agency

became aware of tetrahydrogestrinone (THG) from an anonymous informant. A number

of elite athletes were later found to be using this undetectable steroid prior to its

discovery by the United States Anti-Doping Agency (Knight 2003).

Anti-doping organisations are continually confronted with the challenge of detecting

new performance-enhancing substances. Detection technology needs to be constantly

improving to detect new designer steroids that are chemically modified to avoid

detection. This difficulty in detecting banned performance-enhancing substances is

reflected in a survey of 15 members of the United States powerlifting team, in which

67% reported use of anabolic steroids, and 33% claimed to have overcome the

International Olympic Committee’s doping control procedures (Curry & Wagman

1999).

For substances such as testosterone, the testing procedures have to overcome the

difficulty of differentiating between naturally occurring testosterone and exogenous

testosterone. Similarly, erythropoietin (EPO) is very difficult to detect as it occurs

naturally within the body and it has a short half-life of a few hours, while its effects on

red blood cell counts last for over a month. To overcome this, the test does not directly

Page 52: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

35

detect erythropoietin use; rather, limits are set for red blood cell indices. Readings

above the acceptable limits are deemed to constitute erythropoietin use.

Anti-doping organisations are now confronted with the potential of genetic

manipulation (or ‘gene doping’) (Lippi & Guidi 2004; Schneider & Friedmann 2006;

Unal & Ozer Unal 2004). It is generally believed that gene doping would be very

difficult to detect as it seeks to produce recombinant proteins within human cells rather

than introducing the recombinant proteins into the body. Currently, no tests are

available to detect gene doping (Azzazy, Mansour & Christenson 2005; DeFrancesco

2004; Unal & Ozer Unal 2004).

Drug testing programs can lead to a substantial decline in use of performance-enhancing

substances that are detectable (Beckett 1983; Beckett & Cowan 1979; Donald 1983;

Williams 1994). For example, Donald (1983) presented data which showed a substantial

decrease in the percentage of positive doping tests among Belgium cyclists from 25% in

1965 to 20% in 1966 to 8% in 1967 to 4% in 1976. In a survey of 197 varsity sport

athletes, 56% of the total sample believed that drug testing discourages drug use by

athletes (Schneider & Morris 1993). Overall, the threat of detection via drug testing acts

as a deterrent for performance-enhancing substances that are currently detectable or

may be in the near future.

Likelihood of successfully appealing a positive test:

Athletes have the right to appeal to the Administrative Appeal Tribunal for a review of

an anti-doping rule violation decision and the right to a hearing before the Court of

Arbitration for Sport.

Page 53: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

36

Prior to the use of the gas chromatography and mass spectrometry in the analytical test

of samples in 1983, the inaccuracy of the detection technology for some substances

(e.g., anabolic-androgenic steroids) resulted in a number of false positive (i.e., detection

of the substance when it is not present) and false negative (i.e., failure to detect the

substance) results. Athletes challenged positive test results on the basis of the

unreliability of the detection technology, and also, on the haphazard collection process

(Voy & Deeter 1991). The technology of testing is continually improving (Kammerer

2001; Silver 2001). For example, Reichel (2011) provides a review of the recent

developments in testing for erythropoietin. In addition, regulations have been

strengthened to ensure the sample collection process is stringent. The Australian Sports

Anti-Doping Authority’s sample collection program is conducted in accordance with

the International Standard for Testing, the World Anti-Doping Code, and the National

Anti-Doping scheme (Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2009).

The World Anti-Doping Code specifies that the period of ineligibility from competing

for an anti-doping violation may be eliminated or reduced if an individual is able to

establish to the satisfaction of the hearing panel that the banned performance-enhancing

substance was not intended to enhance sport performance or mask the use of banned

performance-enhancing substances. However, these rules do not apply to certain classes

of banned substances and methods specified in the World Anti-Doping Code’s

Prohibited List (i.e., banned substances in sections: S1, S2.1 to S2.5, S.4.4, S6.a; banned

methods in sections: M1, M2 and M3). The individual’s degree of fault is the criterion

considered in assessing the elimination or reduction of the period of ineligibility (World

Anti-Doping Agency 2009).

Page 54: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

37

Over the years, athletes have argued that their anti-doping violation was inadvertent

(Somerville & Lewis 2005). For example, Samantha Riley, an Australian swimmer,

successfully claimed inadvertent ingestion of a banned narcotic analgesic,

dextropropoxyphene, contained in a headache tablet she had taken at the 1995 World

Short Course Championships in Rio de Janiero. Riley claimed that her coach gave her

the tablet (Magdalinski 2001). Examples of high profile Australian athletes whose

appeals were rejected include: Shane Warne, an Australian cricketer who tested positive

to a diuretic in 2003, claimed that his mother gave him one Moduretic tablet which he

took to improve his appearance; Nathan Baggaley, an Australian kayaker who tested

positive to the anabolic steroids stanozolol and methandienone in 2005, claimed he was

unaware that his brother mixed steroids in the juice he drank; Carol Gaudie, an

Australian netballer who tested positive to testosterone in 2002, claimed an unknown

person spiked her drink in a night club; Dean Capobianco, an Australian sprinter who

tested positive to the anabolic steroid stanozolol in 1996, claimed the steroid was due to

eating contaminated meat.

In a recent high profile case, Alberto Contador, a Spanish cyclist who tested positive to

Clenbuterol successfully claimed to the Spanish Cycling Federation that his positive test

was the result of food contamination. However, the decision was later overturned by the

Court of Arbitration for Sport.

2.1.2 Appraisal of threat of ill-health effects of performance-enhancing

substances use

As with the appraisal of threat of enforcement, the threat appraisal component of the

Protection Motivation Theory (Rogers 1983) can be applied to ill-health effects of

Page 55: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

38

banned performance-enhancing substances use. Threats relating to ill-health effects can

be appraised on the perceived severity of the ill-health effects and the perceived

likelihood of the ill-health effects occurring.

Donovan et al. (2002) suggested four measures of appraisal: (1) perceived likelihood of

ill-health effects; (2) perceived imminence of ill-health effects; (3) perceived

reversibility of ill-health effects; and (4) perceived severity of ill-health effects.

Following perceived ill-health effects of related substance use in general (e.g., Amonini

2001; Health Department of Western Australia 1998; Nucci, Guerra & Lee 1991), this

study measured health appraisal via two measures: perceived severity of harm from one

or twice ever use and regular use.

Despite the lack of human studies on the biomedical side-effects of banned

performance-enhancing substances, it is generally believed that the use of banned

performance-enhancing substances poses health risks (see Peters, Schulz & Michna

2002 for a review of the health risks). Notwithstanding evidence of deaths in sport

attributed to doping that were unfounded (e.g., Dimeo 2007; López 2011; Møller 2005),

there have been a number of deaths resulting from doping in sport (see Hausmann,

Hammer & Betz 1998). In the early 1960s, anabolic steroids and amphetamines were

reported to have led to several deaths from cardiovascular disease and various cancers

(Houlihan 2002; Yesalis & Bahrke 2002). Pärssinen & Seppälä (2002) reported that the

risk of mortality is 4.6 times greater among chronic steroid users than non-users. In

addition, athletes who use new designer substances and gene doping are exposing

themselves to potentially adverse health risks (Unal & Ozer Unal 2004).

Page 56: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

39

Athletes’ perceptions of the ill-health effects of banned performance-enhancing

substances are influenced by a number of sources including sports doctors, competitors,

team mates and the media. Medical practitioners who publicly minimise the ill-health

effects of banned performance-enhancing substances (Boudreau & Konzak 1991) and

who actively supply these drugs (e.g., 61% of a sample of French athletes obtained

banned performance-enhancing substances from their doctor; Laure 1997), thereby

lessen the perceived ill-health effects of banned performance-enhancing substances use.

However, most athletes do believe that banned performance-enhancing substances have

adverse health effects. In a survey of 446 elite Finnish athletes, 74% reported that the

use of banned substances was dangerous to health. Only 3% of these athletes believed

that banned substances may be used ‘completely’ or ‘nearly’ without any adverse health

effects (Alaranta et al. 2006). In a survey of 6,402 French high school athletes, 93%

believed that the use of banned performance-enhancing substances was ‘always’

dangerous to health (Laure & Binsinger 2005).

Studies have found that users are less aware of or less willing to acknowledge the health

risks associated with use of banned performance-enhancing substances. Melia, Pipe and

Greenberg (1996) reported that among a sample of 16,119 Canadian students in the

sixth grade and above, greater proportions of steroid users than non-users believed that

steroids were not harmful (30% vs 6%). In Laure et al.’s (2004) survey of 1,459 high

school athletes in France, the proportion who believed that doping was not always

hazardous to health was 49% among those who had used banned performance-

enhancing substances compared to 6% among non-users.

Page 57: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

40

2.2 Benefit Appraisal

The benefit appraisal construct was based on the Health Belief Model (Becker 1974;

Rosenstock 1977). According to the Health Belief Model, the likelihood of adopting a

behaviour to prevent a health problem is based on an individual’s: (a) perceived

susceptibility to that particular health problem; (b) perceived severity of the health

problem; (c) perceived benefits of averting the health problem; and (d) the costs or

constraints of undertaking the behaviour. In addition, a trigger is required to initiate

action.

Donovan et al. (2002) suggested that the Health Belief Model concepts can be applied

in reverse where the behaviour is not a threat of a health problem but a ‘promise’ of a

desired level of sport performance. That is, the likelihood of using banned performance-

enhancing substances will increase if: (a) athletes believe that they are unable to achieve

at their desired level of performance without using a performance-enhancing substance;

(b) if achieving at their desired level of performance has considerable rewards; and (c) if

they believe that performance-enhancing substances use has little or no undue adverse

health effects or expense (i.e., price of the performance-enhancing substance; costs in

terms of time and effort in obtaining and using the performance-enhancing substance).

An example of a trigger for action to use performance-enhancing substances is an

athlete being informed that a competitor in a forthcoming event is using such substances

(Donovan et al. 2002).

Donovan et al. (2002) emphasised two measures: (1) the perceived extent to which use

of a performance-enhancing substance would lead to success in sport performance; and

(2) the extent to which various rewards were available for success in their sport.

Page 58: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

41

Perceived performance-enhancing effects of banned substances and methods:

The perceived effectiveness of different categories of banned substances and methods

would vary according to the desired enhancement sought in a particular sport. For

example, blood doping and erythropoietin (EPO) enhances oxygen capacity which is

beneficial for endurance sports; the muscle development properties of anabolic steroids

and human growth hormone (hGH) are beneficial for power sports; the ability of

diuretics to reduce body weight is beneficial for weight classification sports; and

stimulants have a wider appeal across different types of sports due to their ability to

increase alertness and reduce or delay fatigue, which facilitates increased intensity and

duration of training. In addition, some banned performance-enhancing substances have

an indirect rather than a direct impact on enhancing sport performance. For example,

one of the benefits sought from diuretics is that it masks the use of some other banned

performance-enhancing substances such as anabolic steroids.

In a survey of 446 elite Finnish athletes, 90% believed that banned substances or

methods can enhance sport performance (Alaranta et al. 2006). However, there were

wide variations in perceived performance-enhancing effects of different categories of

substances and methods. Seventy-three percent of athletes believed that anabolic

steroids have performance-enhancing effects; other categories of banned substances or

methods that were perceived to be performance-enhancing by a majority of athletes

were: blood doping (60%); peptide hormones (59%); and stimulants (57%). The

proportions were substantially lower for beta-blockers (22%), diuretics (16%), oral

glucocorticoids (16%), narcotic analgesics (14%) and sedatives (9%). In comparison to

this sample of elite athletes, the perceived efficacy of performance-enhancing

substances varies among school athletes. In a survey of 1,459 high school athletes in

France, 68% believed “doping agents really improve performance in sports” (Laure et

Page 59: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

42

al. 2004, p. 134), but among 1,553 pre-high school athletes aged 10 to 14 years in 34

states in the United States, less than 45% believed that using anabolic steroids would

improve performance in their sport (Wroble, Gray & Rodrigo 2002). It is noteworthy

that in the latter study, 12% of the total sample reported that they had not heard of

anabolic steroids. Hence, the lower perceived efficacy of performance-enhancing

substances among school athletes may be due in part to lower levels of knowledge

about these substances in general.

Perceived rewards of performing well in sport:

There are financial (e.g., prize money, sponsorship deals), psychological (e.g.,

achievement satisfaction) and social rewards (e.g., social prestige, public recognition)

for performing well in sports. The appeal of these different rewards will differ between

athletes. Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan 1985) provides a conceptual

framework for understanding athletes’ motivations in striving for sporting success. The

theory distinguishes between intrinsic and extrinsic motivations. In a sporting context,

intrinsic motivation refers to the inherent rewards derived from participation in the sport

itself (e.g., fun, enjoyment, mastering skills), whereas extrinsic motivation refers to

seeking rewards (or avoiding punishment) external to sport participation (e.g., financial

rewards, public recognition). Intrinsic motivation is associated with increased effort,

persistence and satisfaction among individuals engaging in tasks in an achievement

context (Deci, Koestner & Ryan 1999; Mugford, Mugford & Donnelly 1999;

Nicholson, Wynd & Cohen 1993). In contrast, extrinsic motivation is associated with

obtaining external rewards.

Vallerand and Losier’s (1999) review of studies on self-determination provided support

for the notion that athletes who are intrinsically motivated are more likely than those

Page 60: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

43

extrinsically motivated to display higher levels of sportspersonship (i.e., be more likely

to show respect for others and less likely to cheat). In a survey of 1,201 elite Canadian

athletes (mean age: 16.3 years), extrinsically motivated athletes were more likely than

intrinsically motivated athletes to use banned performance-enhancing substances

(Donahue et al. 2006).

2.3 Personal Morality

Personal morality is generally referred to as an individual’s judgement of right versus

wrong and what one ‘ought’ to do (Mischel & Mischel 1976; Sjoberg & Winroth 1986).

As pointed out by Donovan et al. (2002), the subjective norm component of Fishbein’s

(1967) Theory of Reasoned Action originally included personal (or moral) normative

beliefs, but were later omitted from the model (Fishbein & Ajzen 1975). Personal

normative beliefs reflect an individual’s internalised moral rules about what he or she

should do in a given situation (Parker, Manstead & Stradling 1995).

There is evidence that the more an individual believes a behaviour is wrong (i.e.,

personal normative beliefs), the less likely the individual is to engage in that behaviour

(Foglia 1997; Nagin & Pogarsky 2001; Paternoster 1987). Often it is internalised norms

that are the most important explanation for behaviour (Paternoster 1989; Tittle 1980).

Moral norms have been found to be predictive of behaviours such as donating blood

(Pomazel & Jaccard 1976), recycling (Vining & Ebreo 1992), using condoms (Godin &

Kok 1996), committing driving violations (Parker, Manstead & Stradling 1995), and

excessive alcohol use (Amonini & Donovan 2006).

Page 61: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

44

At the time of the Donovan et al. (2002) paper and to date, little is known about

athletes’ perceptions of the morality of performance-enhancing substances use and the

values on which such perceptions are based. Donovan et al. (2002) believed that some

of these values will be related to sporting values (e.g., cooperation, respect for others,

respect for rules, compassion and honesty), while others will be related to an

individual’s general upbringing and life experiences. Donovan et al. (2002) suggested

that a values-based anti-doping stance would be one of the most effective protective

factors inhibiting performance-enhancing substances use; that is, to develop attitudes

and values that lead an athlete to view not using performance-enhancing substances as

the right thing to do, regardless of the incentives or deterrents associated with

performance-enhancing substances use.

Emotions such as shame, guilt and embarrassment are likely to result from violating a

moral personal norm, whereas positive feelings of pride and self-approval are likely to

result from doing the right thing (Tangney, Stuewig & Mashek 2007). According to

Donovan et al. (2002), if an athlete believes that taking banned performance-enhancing

substances is wrong from a moral personal norm perspective, the use of such substances

– regardless of getting caught – would result in feelings of shame and guilt. From a

social norm perspective, getting caught taking banned performance-enhancing

substances would also result in feelings of embarrassment.

2.4 Reference Group Opinion

The subjective norm component of the Theory of Reasoned Action model (Fishbein &

Ajzen 1975) and later adaptations of the model (e.g., Theory of Planned Behaviour,

Ajen 1985; Theory of Trying, Bagozzi & Warshaw 1990), provides the basis for

Page 62: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

45

Donovan et al.’s (2002) reference group opinion construct. Subjective norm refers to the

perceived social position with respect to a particular behaviour. It is theorised that

beliefs about what relevant others think about a particular behaviour influences

intentions to perform that behaviour, which in turn, influences actual behaviour. Hence,

in a sporting context, Donovan et al. (2002) theorised that if athletes perceive their

reference groups to be positive towards use of banned performance-enhancing

substances they will have more positive attitudes and intentions to use such substances.

According to the Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002), the reference groups

for athletes may include their primary contact groups (e.g., team mates and other

athletes, family, coaches, trainers, nutritionists, physiotherapists, sports psychologists),

sporting administrators (sporting federations at the state, national and international

levels), corporations (e.g., pharmaceutical companies) and institutions (e.g., the media,

government, medical profession). Donovan et al. (2002) suggested that the relative

influence of these reference groups would vary by individual, type of sport and sporting

level.

For most athletes, coaches play an integral role in their sporting lives. However few

studies have investigated coaches’ attitude towards performance-enhancing substances.

In Laure, Thouvenin and Lecerf’s (2001) study, a sample of 260 professional coaches in

France was presented with a number of statements about doping and asked to rate the

extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement. Among these coaches,

there was a belief that using performance-enhancing substances is necessary to perform

well: “Most records have been broken due to doping”: 70% agreement; “Most of the

great champions resort to doping”: 51% agreement; “An athlete who declines doping

has little chance of succeeding”: 30% agreement.

Page 63: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

46

2.5 Legitimacy

The legitimacy of anti-doping organisations refers to the extent to which they are duly

constituted and have valid authority to enforce anti-doping regulations. It is generally

believed that the stronger an organisation’s legitimacy, the more likely people will

comply with that organisation’s rules and regulations (Tyler 1990). The anti-doping

effort is led by the World Anti-Doping Agency with the World Anti-Doping Code

harmonising the rules and regulations in sport (Houlihan 2004). The UNESCO

International Convention against Doping in Sport (UNESCO 2005) provides the legal

imperative for governments to comply with the World Anti-Doping Code. The

Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority is a government statutory authority with the

responsibility for implementing the Code in Australia. Donovan et al. (2002)

hypothesised that low perceived legitimacy of anti-doping organisations would

reinforce continued performance-enhancing substances use among users and facilitate

use among athletes susceptible to doping.

Tyler’s (1990) conceptualisation of the influence of justice on the legitimacy of legal

authorities provides a framework for understanding athletes’ perception of the

legitimacy of anti-doping organisations in undertaking drug testing. According to Tyler

(1990), an authority’s legitimacy is influenced by three dimensions of justice: (1)

distributive justice – the fairness of the outcomes of a system; (2) procedural justice –

the fairness of the processes (e.g., accuracy or consistency of the procedures); and (3)

interactional justice – the fairness of the interpersonal treatment during implementation

of the procedures (e.g., empathy and honesty of the personnel administering the

procedures) (Bies & Moag 1986; Gilliland 1993). Hence, the legitimacy of anti-doping

organisations is based on establishing a fair and just drug testing regime (distributive

Page 64: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

47

justice), with fair and just processes in collecting, analysing and storing of samples for

testing and any subsequent appeals of an anti-doping rule violation in tribunals and the

Court of Arbitration for Sport (procedural justice), and fair and just treatment of athletes

by personnel administering the drug collection procedure (interactional justice).

Donovan et al. (2002) theorised that if athletes perceive an anti-doping organisation’s

drug testing regime to be fair and just on these dimensions of justice, then the

legitimacy of the anti-doping organisation in conducting drug testing is likely to be

enhanced, and compliance with anti-doping regulations more likely.

The World Anti-Doping Code and the international standards specified in the Code set

the technical and operational aspects of drug testing in sport. Anti-doping organisations

enforce anti-doping regulations via their detection and enforcement programs. This

involves drug testing of athletes and prosecution of anti-doping rule violations. There

are stringent controls on the security of the testing procedures (i.e., collection, handling

and storage of samples) and the integrity of the test laboratories in terms of accuracy in

detecting banned performance-enhancing substances. Laboratories require annual

accreditation from the World Anti-Doping Agency to analysis drug samples. The Code

specifies stringent testing and appeal procedures. These regulations facilitate the

equitable treatment of all athletes throughout the process from sample collection to any

subsequent hearing and sanctions. However, the practice of targeting elite athletes for

drug testing, particularly those who perform well (e.g., medal winners), could be

perceived as inequitable treatment of athletes.

In a survey of 874 elite United Kingdom athletes, 66% trusted the testing process, while

8% did not (UK Sport 2006). Athletes who had undertaken a drug test were

substantially more likely to indicate that they trusted the process than those who had

Page 65: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

48

never had a test: 72% vs 54% respectively. [Thirty-two percent of those who had never

been tested provided a neutral response.] Among athletes who had been tested, 90%

were satisfied that the test had been carried out fairly and accurately. Only 3% were

dissatisfied with the testing process.

2.6 Personality

Donovan et al. (2002) suggested several personality factors that may influence attitudes

and intentions towards performance-enhancing substances use. These included self-

esteem, optimism, inner-outer directedness, risk taking propensity and self-efficacy.

Self-esteem:

Self-esteem refers to an individual’s judgement of their personal value or self-worth

(Coopersmith 1967; Rosenberg 1979). At the time of developing the Sport Drug Control

model, low self-esteem had been related to other substance use (Carvajal et al. 1998;

Stein, Newcomb & Bentler 1987), and Blouin and Goldfield’s (1995) survey of a

sample of 43 bodybuilders in Canada found that self-esteem was significantly lower

among users of anabolic steroids than among non-users.

Optimism:

Seligman (1991) categorises individuals as optimists or pessimists based on their

psychological reactions to events. Reactions to both positive and negative events are

assessed on three dimensions: (1) Internality-Externality: attributing the event to either

oneself (internal factors) or other people (or something else) (external factors); (2)

Stability-Instability: perceiving the event as within one’s control (stable factors) or out

of one’s control (unstable factors); and (3) Globality-Specificity: attributing the event to

Page 66: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

49

permanent factors (global factors) or temporary factors (specific factors). Optimistic

athletes are predicted to attribute success to internal sources, and to broad and

generalised conditions (e.g., the goal that I kicked in this match was due to my ball

kicking skills and I can rely on my ball kicking skills in future matches), whereas

pessimistic athletes are predicted to attribute success to external factors, and to specific

and temporary conditions (e.g., the goal that I kicked in this match was due to luck and I

may not be as lucky in future matches).

In Seligman’s (1991) study of elite athletes, previous poor performances affected

pessimistic athletes more than optimistic athletes. Optimistic athletes attributed failure

to external factors, and to specific and temporary conditions (e.g., the weather), whereas

pessimistic athletes attributed failure to internal sources, and to broad and generalised

conditions (e.g., lack of will power; lack of skills in general).

Given that optimism may be related to high self-esteem and pessimism to lower self-

esteem, Donovan et al. (2002) hypothesised that athletes who attribute their poor

performance to internal sources could be more predisposed to use of banned

performance-enhancing substances than those attributing poor performance to transient

external sources.

Inner-outer directedness:

Reisman (1950) theorised that inner-directeds are motivated by internalised values,

whereas outer-directeds are motivated by external social rewards. Donovan et al. (2002)

hypothesised that since outer-directed athletes would be more motivated by social and

financial rewards of performing well in sport, they would be more susceptible to banned

performance-enhancing substances use.

Page 67: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

50

Risk taking propensity:

Risk taking can be viewed as the voluntary engagement in behaviours that are

associated with some probability of undesirable results (Boyer 2006). Based on other

areas of drug use (Weber & Roehl 1998), Donovan et al. (2002) hypothesised that risk-

takers would be more susceptible to banned performance-enhancing substances use.

Self-efficacy:

Based on Elliot and Goldberg’s (1996) study in which adolescent athletes who were

susceptible to banned performance-enhancing substances use found it more difficult

than nonsusceptibles to reject an offer of these substances, Donovan et al. (2002)

proposed that self-efficacy is another personality factor that could influence banned

performance-enhancing substances use. They noted that in Elliot and Goldberg’s (1996)

study, other personal factors found to be related to athletes’ potential steroid use were

dissatisfaction with body image, hostility, impulsivity and a ‘win at all costs’ attitude.

2.7 Model Summary in Predicting Likelihood of Performance-

Enhancing Substances Use

Donovan et al. (2002) posit that the predicted likelihood of performance-enhancing

substances use is based on athletes’ ‘scores’ on the six components in the model. Table

2.1 summaries ratings on the six components in the model for highest and lowest

predicted likelihood of performance-enhancing substances use.

The likelihood of performance-enhancing substances use is predicted to be highest

when:

Page 68: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

51

– Threat appraisal is low (e.g., low perceived likelihood of drug test occurring both in

and out of competition; low perceived likelihood of banned performance-enhancing

substances being detected; high perceived likelihood of successfully appealing a

positive test; low perceived severity of consequences of positive test; low perceived

likelihood of ill-health effects);

– Benefit appraisal is high (e.g., significant enhancement of sport performance from

performance-enhancing substances use; high financial or social rewards for

performing well in sports);

– Weak personal moral stance supportive of performance-enhancing substances use;

– Perceived legitimacy of the laws and anti-doping organisations is low (e.g., the

drug testing and appeals processes are viewed as unfair and unjust);

– Relevant reference groups are positive towards performance-enhancing substances

use; and

– High vulnerability on personality factors (e.g., low self-esteem, risk taker,

pessimist).

Conversely the likelihood of performance-enhancing substances is predicted to be

lowest when:

– Threat appraisal is high (e.g., high perceived likelihood of drug test occurring both

in and out of competition; high perceived likelihood of banned performance-

enhancing substances being detected; low perceived likelihood of successfully

appealing a positive test; high perceived severity of consequences of positive test;

high perceived likelihood of ill-health effects);

– Benefit appraisal is low (e.g., insignificant enhancement of sport performance from

performance-enhancing substances use; low financial or social rewards for

performing well in sports);

Page 69: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

52

– Strong personal moral stance against performance-enhancing substances use;

– Perceived legitimacy of the laws and anti-doping organisations is high (e.g., the

drug testing and appeals processes are viewed as fair and just);

– Relevant reference groups are against performance-enhancing substances use; and

– Low vulnerability on personality factors (e.g., high self-esteem, risk avoider,

optimist).

Table 2.1: Highest and lowest predicted likelihood of performance-enhancing

substances use based on athletes’ scores on the six components

in the Sport Drug Control model

Component Highest likelihood of use Lowest likelihood of use

Threat appraisal Low High

Benefit appraisal High Low

Personal morality Supportive of performance-

enhancing substances use

Against performance-

enhancing substances use

Legitimacy Low High

Reference group

opinion

Positive to performance-

enhancing substances use

Against performance-

enhancing substances use

Personality High vulnerability Low vulnerability

2.8 Market Factors

Availability and affordability are key factors influencing use of related substances and

consumer products in general (Anderson 2006; Casswell & Thamarangsi 2009;

Chaloupka, Grossman & Saffer 2002; Chaloupka et al. 2000; Popova et al. 2009). In the

Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002), availability and affordability of

banned performance-enhancing substances were considered facilitators or inhibitors in

the translation of attitudes and intentions to uptake of performance-enhancing

Page 70: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

53

substances. Donovan et al. (2002) theorised that when such substances were cheaper

and readily available, individuals with a positive disposition towards banned

performance-enhancing substances use would be more likely to act on this disposition.

There are limited studies investigating athletes’ knowledge (and perceptions) of the

availability and affordability of performance-enhancing substances. In Scarpino et al.’s

(1990) survey of 1,015 Italian high school athletes, 74% felt that access to banned

substances was not difficult (‘very easy’: 35%; ‘not very difficult’: 39%). In a survey of

873 high school football players in the United States, 49% reported that they could

obtain anabolic steroids if they so desired (Stilger & Yesalis 1999).

Donovan et al. (2002) suggested that young athletes are unlikely to be able to afford

performance-enhancing substances, but their coaches and team doctors may have the

means. There is considerable evidence that these members of the athlete’s entourage

and their team mates are sources of supply of performance-enhancing substances. In the

National Collegiate Athletic Association’s (NCAA) survey of 13,914 athletes in 1996,

physicians were the main source of supply of anabolic steroids: team physician: 5.7%;

and other physicians: 32.1% (Green et al. 2001). In Stilger and Yesalis’s (1999) study,

among high school football players who provided a source for obtaining anabolic

steroids (n = 76), 30% mentioned ‘other athletes/health club’, 29% ‘team mate/friend’,

25% ‘physician’, and 16% ‘coach’. In Laure et al.’s (2004) survey of 1,459 high school

athletes in France, among those who reported use of performance-enhancing substances

(3.9% of the total sample), the most frequently nominated source of supply was friends

(26%). The other main sources nominated were doctors/pharmacists (19%), the black

market (14%), coaches (12%), and parents (7%). In a survey of 446 elite Finnish

athletes, 15% reported that they had been offered banned substances: 21% of athletes in

Page 71: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

54

speed and power events, 14% of athletes in team sports; 14% of athletes in motor skills

demanding events, and 10% of athletes in endurance events. The substance reported to

be offered the most was stimulants (7% of the total sample), followed by anabolic

steroids (4%) and sedatives (3%) (Alaranta et al. 2006). Among a sample of 260

professional coaches in France, the main perceived sources of performance-enhancing

substances for athletes were team members (72%), coaches (69%), physicians (64%),

and pharmacists (41%) (Laure, Thouvenin & Lecerf 2001).

2.9 Attitudes and Intentions Towards Performance-Enhancing

Substances Use

The Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002) proposes that the six components

discussed above (threat appraisal, benefit appraisal, personal morality, reference group

opinion, personality, and legitimacy) are major inputs to an athlete’s attitudes and

intentions towards performance-enhancing substances use. Donovan et al. (2002) did

not specify how attitudes and intentions could be measured; rather, attitudes would be

derived from the six components. Guidance on the operationalisation and measurement

of attitudes and intentions was limited to reference to the Theory of Reasoned Action

(Fishbein & Ajzen 1975) and later adaptions of it (e.g., Theory of Planned Behaviour,

Ajzen 1985; Theory of Trying, Bagozzi & Warshaw 1990). These models of behaviour

change and other social cognition models (Conner & Norman 2005; Godin 1994)

assume an individual’s beliefs about a behaviour will determine the individual’s

attitudes toward the behaviour, which in turn, predicts an individual’s intentions with

respect to that behaviour. These intentions, subject to environmental facilitators and

inhibitors, predict how the individual would actually act with respect to that behaviour.

Kraus’s (1995) meta-analysis of 88 attitude-behaviour studies revealed that attitudes

Page 72: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

55

significantly and substantially predicted future behaviour (mean r = .38). Further, the

methodological factors associated with high attitude-behaviour correlations included

self-report measures of behaviour and the use of non-students as subjects.

In this study, to better reflect that Donovan et al.’s (2002) attitude is towards the use of

performance-enhancing substances – not towards performance-enhancing substances

per se – attitude was conceptualised in terms of (a) the perceived necessity of using

performance-enhancing substances and (b) the amount of consideration given to an

offer of using performance-enhancing substances.

Conceptualising attitude towards banned performance-enhancing substances in terms of

the perceived necessity of performance-enhancing substances use is supported by the

work of Petróczi and colleagues (Petróczi 2002; Petróczi & Aidman 2009) in

developing and validating a scale to measure attitude towards performance-enhancing

substances use. Three items in their Performance Enhancement Attitude Scale (PEAS)

refer to justifications for the necessity of performance-enhancing substances use

(“Doping is not cheating since everyone does it”, “Athletes often lose time due to

injuries and drugs can help to make up the lost time”, and “Doping is an unavoidable

part of the competitive sport”), and one of the items is a direct measure of perceived

necessity of performance-enhancing substances use: “Doping is necessary to be

competitive” (Petróczi & Aidman 2009).

There is evidence that a substantial minority of athletes believes that it is necessary to

use performance-enhancing substances to perform at the highest level and to compete

against athletes who use such substances. In Laure and Binsinger’s (2005) survey of

6,402 adolescent athletes in France, 21% believed that to “refuse doping means losing

Page 73: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

56

all chances of becoming a great champion”. In Stilger and Yesalis’s (1999) survey of

873 high school football players in the United States, among those who reported that

they had never used anabolic steroids (94%), 22% reported that they would use anabolic

steroids if they knew their opponents were using them.

Conceptualising attitude in terms of consideration given to using performance-

enhancing substances is supported by the consumer and health decision making

literature, where, for most high involvement decisions (i.e., high personal relevance), it

is arguably more realistic – and valid – to measure intention to perform some

intermediate behaviour such as seeking more information (e.g., calling a Helpline) about

a product or behaviour rather than intention to purchase or adopt the behaviour after

being given limited information about that product or behaviour (Donovan & Jalleh

2000; Rossiter & Percy 1988). For example, Donovan & Jalleh (2000) presented

mothers with information about an immunisation for infants that had potential negative

side-effects. Rather than asking their intention to have their child immunised (a very

high involvement decision), mothers were asked their likelihood of seeking more

information about the immunisation and its side-effects – a more realistic request given

the nature of the decision. It is assumed that this reflects a ‘tentative positive attitude’

towards the behaviour which would then be confirmed or otherwise after more

information is obtained (Rossiter & Percy 1998).

In a sporting context, Gucciardi, Jalleh and Donovan (2010) posit that any level of

consideration to use performance-enhancing substances reflects a positive attitude

towards, and renders an athlete susceptible to, performance-enhancing substances use.

In 2007-08, when elite Australian athletes (N = 710) were presented with a scenario of

an offer of a banned performance-enhancing substance, 28% would give the offer some

Page 74: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

57

consideration (Jalleh & Donovan 2007, 2008a, 2008b). Other studies have shown that

athletes would consider using performance-enhancing substances if it would: help them

get to the National Hockey League (33% of 122 collegiate hockey players in the United

States; Bents, Tokish & Goldberg 2004); and guarantee them selection for their national

side in the World Cup (5% of 706 professional football players in the United Kingdom;

Waddington et al. 2005). In Laure et al.’s (2004) survey of 1,459 high school athletes in

France, 5% reported that they would use performance-enhancing substances if they

were offered them by their best friend.

The operationalisation of attitude is described in Chapter 3.

2.10 Recent Research Findings in the Psychosocial Doping in Sports

Literature

Since the Donovan et al. (2002) paper, there have been a number of theoretically

grounded research studies into performance-enhancing substances use in sport that

provide support for the importance of a number of the constructs in the Sport Drug

Control model.

2.10.1 Applications of the Theory of Planned Behaviour

The Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen 1985, 1991) has been applied to a wide range

of risk behaviours. The theory posits that behaviour is determined by intention to

perform that behaviour, with the stronger the intention to engage in a given behaviour,

the more likely that behaviour is to occur. The strength of the intention is determined by

three factors: (1) attitude towards the behaviour; (2) subjective norms (i.e., perceptions

Page 75: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

58

of the extent to which important others would want them to perform the behaviour); and

(3) perceived behavioural control (i.e., perceptions of their ability to perform the

behaviour).

Armitage and Conner’s (2001) review of 185 research studies found that the Theory of

Planned Behaviour accounted for 27% and 39% of the variance in behaviour and

intention respectively. This theory has been used as a framework in more recent

research studies investigating intentions to use and actual use of performance-enhancing

substances (Lucidi et al. 2004, 2008; Petróczi 2007; Tsorbatzoudis et al. 2009;

Wiefferink et al. 2008).

Wiefferink et al.’s (2008) study of 144 gym users in the Netherlands who practiced

bodybuilding, fitness, powerlifting or combat sports was based on the Theory of

Planned Behaviour and Social Cognitive Theory. According to the latter theory,

behaviour is a function of intention, while intention is a function of attitudes, social

influences and self-efficacy (Bandura 1986). The main predictors of intention to use

performance-enhancing substances were personal norms, beliefs about improved

performance outcomes and social influences (i.e., perceived use of performance-

enhancing substances by their reference groups).

Lucidi et al. (2004) investigated whether the Theory of Planned Behaviour could predict

intentions to use performance-enhancing substances in adolescents. A moral

disengagement measure and past use of nutritional supplements (e.g., creatine, amino

acids) were incorporated into the model. Moral disengagement is the psychological

process of persuading oneself that transgressions of one’s own moral standards are

actually morally permissible (Bandura 1990, 1999; Bandura et al. 1996; Shu, Gino &

Page 76: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

59

Bazerman 2011). As noted by Lucidi et al. (2004), previous studies had found an

association between moral disengagement and other health risk and antisocial

behaviours such as aggression, bullying and alcohol consumption (e.g., Bandura et al.

2001; Barnes et al. 1999). Data from a sample of 952 Italian high school students (mean

age: 16 years) showed that the variables in the Theory of Planned Behaviour were

significant predictors of behavioural intentions to use performance-enhancing

substances. Specifically, attitudes were the strongest predictor of intentions to use

performance-enhancing substances, subjective norms showed a moderate effect, and

perceived behavioural control had a weaker effect. Moral disengagement and past use of

supplements also significantly influenced behavioural intentions to use performance-

enhancing substances. These findings were found to be invariant across gender and type

of involvement in sport (i.e., ‘sedentary’: participating in less than two hours of sport

per week; ‘recreational’: participating in two to four hours of sport per week;

‘competitive’: participating in more than four hours of sport per week or took part in

competitions at least once a month).

Lucidi et al.’s (2008) study re-examined the predictive utility of the constructs in the

Theory of Planned Behaviour and moral disengagement on intentions to use

performance-enhancing substances, with the inclusion of a social cognition construct

(i.e., self-regulatory efficacy). Longitudinal data from a sample of 1,232 Italian high

school students (mean age: 16 years) found that favourable attitude towards use of

performance-enhancing substances, their belief that significant others would approve if

they were to use performance-enhancing substances (subjective norms), and moral

disengagement all contributed to greater intentions to use performance-enhancing

substances. Self-regulatory efficacy also was a significant predictor of intention to use

performance-enhancing substances; that is, confidence in their ability to resist pressure

Page 77: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

60

from their social surroundings contributed to weaker intentions. Perceived behavioural

control did not have a significant effect on intention to use, and, as mentioned above,

only had a weak effect in Lucidi et al.’s (2004) study. The longitudinal data showed that

stronger intentions to use performance-enhancing substances influenced use of these

substances and moral disengagement had significant direct effects on later use of these

substances.

Zelli, Mallia and Lucidi (2010) extended Lucidi et al.’s (2008) study by investigating

whether interpersonal appraisals of eight hypothetical scenarios in which someone

offered or advised them to use performance-enhancing substances would predict

intentions to use performance-enhancing substances and actual use of such substances.

Of the initial sample of 1,022 Italian high school students who participated in the first

assessment, 86% provided data in a second assessment four to five months after the first

assessment. Overall, the more students personally justified use of performance-

enhancing substances (i.e., moral disengagement), had more positive attitude towards

use, thought that significant others would approve of their use, and felt less confident in

their capacity to resist social pressure to use, the more they expressed the intention to

use these substances. In turn, adolescents’ intentions significantly predicted use of

performance-enhancing substances measured three months later. In relation to

interpersonal appraisals of the hypothetical scenarios of performance-enhancing

substances use, students who were more likely to attribute a positive meaning to

someone’s motives for soliciting performance-enhancing substances use were also those

who held stronger positive attitudes toward performance-enhancing substances use and

expressed stronger intentions to use performance-enhancing substances in the near

future.

Page 78: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

61

Both Tsorbatzoudis et al.’s (2009) study of 1,040 elite Greek athletes who participated

in nine different sports (football, basketball, volleyball, handball, athletics, swimming,

shooting, taekwondo, and rowing) (mean age: 22.9 years) and Goulet et al.’s (2010)

study of 3,573 Canadian athletes from a variety of sports (mean age: 15.5 years) provide

further support for the important role that the Theory of Planned Behaviour variables

play in understanding the influences on intentions to engage in performance-enhancing

substances use. Both studies showed that all of the Theory of Planned Behaviour

variables were significant predictors of intentions to use performance-enhancing

substances. These variables predicted up to 60% of the variance in intentions to use

performance-enhancing substances in the Tsorbatzoudis et al. (2009) study, and 39% in

the Goulet et al. (2010) study.

These studies provide empirical support for the Theory of Planned Behaviour variables

in predicting intentions to use and actual use of performance-enhancing substances.

2.10.2 Moral disengagement

In recent years, the concept of moral disengagement has received more attention by

researchers in the sports domain. As noted above, Lucidi and colleagues (Lucidi et al.

2004, 2008; Zelli, Mallia & Lucidi 2010) investigated the concept of moral

disengagement in their research on behavioural intentions to use performance-

enhancing substances. Research in this area is based mainly on the earlier works of

Bandura (1986, 1991) that provide a framework for understanding the psychological

mechanisms by which moral standards translate into behaviours.

Page 79: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

62

According to social cognitive theory (Bandura 1986), transgressive behaviour is

regulated by anticipating two major sources of sanctions: social sanctions (i.e., engaging

in the behaviour will result in social disapproval and other adverse consequences); and

internalised self-sanctions (i.e., engaging in the behaviour will result in self-loathing

and diminishment of self-worth). Moral conduct is motivated and regulated mainly by

self-reactive influences. Bandura (1991) outlined eight psychological processes by

which self-sanctions can be disengaged from behaving according to personal moral

standards (i.e., moral disengagement). These eight psychological processes can be

grouped into four categories: (1) reconstruing the transgressive behaviour; (2) obscuring

causal agency; (3) disregarding or misrepresenting the consequences of the

transgressive behaviour on others; and (4) blaming and devaluing the victim. A brief

description of these psychological processes and examples of moral disengagement in a

sporting context are provided here:

Category 1. Reconstruing the transgressive behaviour – Cognitive reconstrual of the

transgressive behaviour so that it is perceived as not immoral (i.e., moral justification,

euphemistic labelling, and advantageous comparison):

1. Moral justification refers to perceiving the behaviour as personally and socially

acceptable by cognitive reconstruing the behaviour as having a moral purpose. The

behaviour is then engaged in on a moral imperative; for example: “An opponent

hurts a teammate. The referee doesnʼt punish him as he should . . . well, Iʼll kill the

opponent during the next play” (Long et al. 2006, p. 342). In this example, the

intentional aggression towards an opponent who hurts a team mate serves the moral

purpose of defending the team mate;

Page 80: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

63

2. Euphemistic labelling refers to using language to give the behaviour a sanitised

label or even a respectable status. A behaviour may be viewed in different ways

depending on the label given to it; for example: referring to aggressive behaviour as

“letting off steam” (Boardley & Kavussanu 2007) and cheating as “cunning”

(Traclet et al. 2011);

3. Advantageous comparison refers to evaluating the behaviour as trivial or even

benevolent by favourably comparing it with other transgressive behaviours; for

example: “It is true that one cheats quite a lot in Taekwondo, but it is nothing

compared to stealing or killing somebody!” (Corrion et al. 2009, p. 397).

Category 2. Obscuring causal agency – Minimising or distorting the relationship

between the transgressive behaviour and its consequences on others (i.e., displacement

and diffusion of responsibility):

4. Displacement of responsibility refers to justifying the behaviour on the basis of

social pressure from the directives of others, usually someone with legitimate

authority (e.g., coach, referee, parents). The behaviour is not viewed as resulting

from a personal decision; for example: “The coach shows us how to cheat” and

“Professional players pretend, lie and cheat, so we play like that too” (Traclet et al.

2011, p. 148);

5. Diffusion of responsibility refers to attributing responsibility for a behaviour not

just on oneself, but on others too, hence minimising or excusing oneself of the

consequences of the behaviour on others. For example: “In sport, if your coach tells

you, “you have to do that!” you have to, you have no choice. Otherwise, just leave,

thereʼs no point in you staying. Sometimes, you have to tackle to hurt, thatʼs what

you are told to do” (Long et al. 2006, p. 341). In this example, the responsibility for

Page 81: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

64

tackling an opponent with the intention to hurt the player is attributed to pressure

from the coach.

Category 3. Disregarding or misrepresenting the consequences of the transgressive

behaviour on others (i.e., distortion of consequences):

6. Distortion of consequences is illustrated in the following example: “In this match,

which was physical, it ended at the hospital. In fact XX injured his ankle when I got

back the ball in an aggressive way . . . I pushed him violently on his back but the

referee did not catch me. But if you get obsessed about it, you can’t play anymore .

. . it’s no concern of mine” (Corrion et al. 2009, p. 397).

Category 4. Blaming and devaluing the victim (i.e., dehumanisation and attribution of

blame):

7. Dehumanisation refers to portraying the victim of the behaviour as inhuman; for

example: “That girl was somebody awful, a real monster that we ran into all the

time…and that we also used to insult time to time” (Corrion et al. 2009, p. 398); and

8. Attribution of blame refers to perceiving the behaviour as forced by circumstances

(e.g., responding to actions of the victim) rather than as a personal decision; for

example: “I could see that she was a fighter . . . and I wasn’t going to let her push

me around, because she started breaking the rules, grabbing on . . . So, I fought

just like her” (Corrion et al. 2009, p. 398).

Qualitative studies have documented evidence of moral disengagement in sports

(Corrion et al. 2009; Long et al. 2006; Traclet et al. 2011). In Long et al.’s (2006) study

of 10 male elite athletes in France (mean age: 16.5 years), reasons for transgressive

behaviours towards the referee (i.e., cheating, strategic fouls) and opponents (i.e., verbal

Page 82: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

65

intimidation, physical aggression) revealed evidence of moral disengagement. Similarly,

in Corrion et al.’s (2009) study of 24 elite athletes in basketball and taekwondo in

France (mean age: 23.8 years), all eight of Bandura’s (1991) moral disengagement

mechanisms were evident in athletes’ discussion of their transgressive behaviours.

These behaviours included using the sport rules to one’s advantage, including

concealing and instigating fouls, verbal intimidation of referee or opponent and physical

aggression against an opponent. The frequently adopted moral disengagement

mechanisms were displacement and diffusion of responsibility, attribution of blame,

minimising or ignoring consequences, and euphemistic labelling.

In Traclet et al.’s (2011) qualitative study of 30 French male soccer players (mean age:

19.2 years), each player was shown video clips of four of their antisocial behaviours on

the field and discussed the reasons for engaging in those behaviours. For each

participant, the four categories of behaviours were: cheating (i.e., not respecting

distance or running down the clock); instrumental aggression (i.e., aggressive tackling

or holding); hostile aggression against an opponent (i.e., hitting or kicking); and hostile

aggression against the referee (i.e., pushing or beating). The more frequent moral

disengagement mechanisms used by players to justify their antisocial behaviours were

displacement of responsibility (e.g., referees should have responded to the foul) and

moral justification. Also, cheating and instrumental aggression elicited more

displacement of responsibility than hostile behaviours.

Boardley and Kavussanu (2007), in developing and validating a 32-item scale to

measure in a sporting context the eight moral disengagement mechanisms proposed by

Bandura (1991), collected data from a sample of 1,605 athletes in England who

participated in five team sports (i.e., soccer, netball, hockey, rugby, basketball). [An 8-

Page 83: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

66

item short version of the scale was later developed (Boardley & Kavussanu 2008).]

These data revealed that moral disengagement was positively associated with antisocial

behaviour (e.g., trying to injure opponents and breaking the rules of the game) and

negatively associated with prosocial behaviour (e.g., helping injured opponents and

congratulating opponents for good play).

Boardley and Kavussanu (2009) examined whether the effects of perceived coaching

character-building competency (i.e., instilling an attitude of moral character, fair play,

and respect for others; Myers et al. 2006) on prosocial and antisocial sport behaviours

are mediated by moral disengagement. The sample consisted of 379 hockey and netball

players (mean age: 22.2 years) in England. Players who perceived their coach as

competent in character-building were less likely to morally disengage. These players

were less likely to behave antisocially towards opponents and team mates and more

likely to act prosocially towards their opponents. As noted by the authors, these findings

suggest that the coach may be an influential figure in deterring the use of moral

disengagement in sports.

Boardley and Kavussanu (2010) investigated whether moral disengagement mediates

the effects of ego orientation (i.e., tendency to use normative criteria to evaluate

competence) and perceived value of toughness (i.e., importance attached to dominating

others to gain acceptance and status) on antisocial behaviour toward opponents and

team mates. Data from 307 male football players’ in England (mean age: 21.4 years)

showed that perceived value of toughness and ego orientation had positive effects on

both types of antisocial behaviour, which were mediated by moral disengagement. That

is, higher levels of moral disengagement in athletes who were ego oriented and who

Page 84: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

67

perceived that their team mates respect those who were seen as being tough were

associated with more frequent antisocial behaviour.

Based on Bandura et al.’s (2001, 2003) work, d’Arripe-Longueville et al.’s (2010) study

investigated moral disengagement in examining a structural model of the self-regulatory

mechanisms governing the acceptability and likelihood of cheating in a sporting context

among a sample of 804 French adolescents (mean age: 17.2 years). Adolescents were

presented with five scenarios describing situations in team sports in which athletes

might be tempted to cheat. Moral disengagement was found to significantly mediate the

effects of negative affective self-regulatory efficacy (i.e., perceived efficacy to regulate

negative affect) on the acceptability and likelihood of cheating, and on prosocial

behaviour.

Overall, these studies provide evidence that moral disengagement promotes antisocial

behaviour, and to a lesser extent, leads to less frequent prosocial behaviour (Boardley &

Kavussanu 2007, 2008, 2009; d’Arripe-Longueville et al. 2010). However, moral

disengagement has not been investigated in terms of actual use of performance-

enhancing substances.

2.10.3 Perceptual deterrence theory

There are no published data examining the utility of Strelan and Boeckmann’s (2003)

Drugs in Sport Deterrence model. However, Strelan and Boeckmann (2006) applied

principles of perceptual deterrence theory to hypothetical decisions to use a

performance-enhancing substance (i.e., human growth hormone (hGH)) among a

sample of 116 elite Australian footballers and soccer players. Perceptual deterrence

Page 85: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

68

theory is concerned with perceptions of the certainty and severity of the punishment of a

crime. It is theorised that the more certain and severe the perceived punishment, the less

likely the crime will occur. After reading the scenario, athletes indicated the probability

that they would use hGH in that scenario and rated the certainty and severity of legal,

social, moral, and health threats. The strongest influences on athletes’ hypothetical

decisions to use hGH were their personal moral beliefs and health concerns.

2.10.4 Personality variables

There have been some studies investigating the impact of personality variables on

performance-enhancing substances use in sport. These personality variables included:

– Sport motivation: Donahue et al.’s (2006) study tested a motivational model of

performance-enhancing substances use in a sample of 1,201 elite Canadian athletes.

Intrinsically motivated athletes were more likely to have internalised

sportspersonship orientations, and in turn were less likely to use performance-

enhancing substances than their extrinsically motivated counterparts;

– Achievement goals: Sas-Nowosielski and Swiatkowska’s (2008) study of 830

athletes in Poland found that ego orientation (i.e., outperforming others) was

positively related to, and task orientation (i.e., mastery of the sport) was negatively

related to favourable attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use. The

most favourable attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use was held by

high ego and low task oriented athletes, while high task and low ego oriented athletes

held the least favourable attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use;

and

Page 86: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

69

– Self-esteem: In a prospective cohort study of 3,564 French pre-adolescent athletes

followed for a 4-year period, low self-esteem was found to be a contributing factor to

use of performance-enhancing substances (Laure & Binsinger 2007).

In Tsorbatzoudis et al.’s (2009) study examining the effect of psychosocial variables on

intention to engage in performance-enhancing substances use, three personality

variables were included in the study (i.e., motivational regulations, achievement goals,

and sportspersonship orientations). Data were obtained from 1,040 elite Greek athletes

who participated in nine different sports (football, basketball, volleyball, handball,

athletics, swimming, shooting, taekwondo, and rowing) (mean age: 22.9 years). Cluster

analysis revealed three achievement goal groups (mastery, approach oriented and high

achievers), three self-determination groups (high motivated, low motivated and

amotivated), and two sportspersonship groups (high and low sportspersonship). Mastery

oriented and high motivated athletes had lesser intention to engage in performance-

enhancing substances use compared to high achievers and amotivated athletes. No

significant differences were revealed between the sportspersonship groups.

As evident in the studies above, personality variables’ influence on susceptibility to

performance-enhancing substances use had been studied only in isolation or with one or

two other variables. Given this gap in the literature, Donovan, Jalleh and Gucciardi

(2009) investigated how athletes differing on susceptibility to performance-enhancing

substances use differed on 22 personality subscales representing 10 broad personality

variables. Fourteen of these personality subscales significantly differentiated between

three susceptibility clusters (high, medium and low susceptibility) identified using

cluster analysis. The personality variables that most differentiated between the high,

moderate and low susceptible athletes were self-presentation concerns, public athletic

Page 87: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

70

identity, moral decision making, concerns over mistakes and fear of failure. These

scales can be used to profile athletes susceptible to performance-enhancing substances

use.

2.11 An Opportunistic Examination of the Sport Drug Control Model

Gucciardi, Jalleh and Donovan (2011) presented findings from an opportunistic

examination of some of the constructs in the Sport Drug Control model. Data were from

a survey of 643 elite Australian athletes for the purpose of personality profiling of elite

athletes and their susceptibility to performance-enhancing substances use. Items in the

questionnaire were identified that related to the following concepts in the model: threat

appraisal (i.e., perceived likelihood of detection out of competition and while

competing, and successfully appealing a positive drug test); personality (i.e., self-

esteem); legitimacy (i.e., perceived seriousness and effectiveness of the Australian

Sports Anti-Doping Authority in preventing performance-enhancing substances use;

perceived security of the drug testing procedures in Australia); morality (i.e., cheating

behaviour); benefit appraisal (i.e., perceived necessity for athletes to use performance-

enhancing substances to perform at the very highest levels); and reference group

opinion (i.e., relevant others’ perceptions of them if they were caught using

performance-enhancing substances).

Structural equation modelling revealed that the hypothesised model accounted for 30%

of the variance in attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use. Morality,

benefit appraisal and threat appraisal evidenced the strongest relationships with attitude

towards performance-enhancing substances use. Self-esteem, perceptions of legitimacy

Page 88: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

71

and reference group opinions showed small non-significant associations with attitude

towards performance-enhancing substances use.

Despite the fact that the questionnaire items were not constructed to specifically

measure the constructs, these findings provided preliminary support for the model and

its usefulness in understanding influences on attitude towards performance-enhancing

substances use.

There are no other published data examining the utility of the Sport Drug Control model

(Donovan et al. 2002).

Page 89: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

72

CHAPTER 3: RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

In this thesis, the ontological position adopted is that the world is considered as being

basically a function of human thought, analysis and perception, hence a positive

epistemology would be the most appropriate basis for this research study. It is believed

that human thoughts and behaviours are measureable and quantifiable. The

epistemological assumption is that the objectives of this study in testing the tenets of the

Sport Drug Control model via the collection of quantitative empirical data is a

satisfactory means for generating new knowledge in understanding influences on

performance-enhancing substances use. Adopting a positive epistemology supports the

use of survey methods and quantitative analysis of survey data.

A cross-sectional nationwide mail survey of elite Australian athletes was conducted in

2004. This research study was approved by the Curtin University Human Research

Ethics Committee. No deception or misinformation was involved in this study. The data

were collected anonymously. Participants were provided with an information sheet that

described the aims of the project and the nature of their involvement in the project, and

were informed that participation in the project is voluntary.

3.1 Participants and Procedure

The Australian Sports Drug Agency (now known as the Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority), the five Australian state academies and institutes of sport, and four national

professional sporting organisations (Basketball Australia, Australian Football League,

National Rugby League, Australian Rugby Union) were approached to distribute the

Page 90: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

73

survey to athletes registered on their databases. Only two Australian state academies

and institutes of sport declined the invitation to take part in the study.

To preserve the confidentiality of the athletes’ contact details, the Australian Sports

Drug Agency undertook the mail-out for athletes on their own database and those on the

Australian state academies and institutes of sport and Basketball Australia’s databases.

The Australian Football League, National Rugby League and Australian Rugby Union

undertook the mail-out for athletes on their national competition databases.

Athletes were mailed-out a package containing the questionnaire, a Curtin University

covering letter, a covering letter from their respective sporting organisation encouraging

athletes to participate in the survey, and a Curtin University-addressed reply-paid

envelope. The Curtin University covering letter invited athletes to participate in a

“survey of elite athletes’ attitudes and opinions on sport issues”. Athletes were

informed that the study was being conducted by researchers at Curtin University and to

return the completed survey to Curtin University for processing. Athletes were informed

that their responses were confidential, that they could not be identified from the

questionnaire, and that only group data would be reported. In addition, the letter

stressed the importance that all views with respect to all of the issues covered in the

questionnaire were allowed – and expected – to be expressed.

The mail survey was distributed between February and May 2004. A reminder letter

was sent approximately two weeks after the initial mail-out. The questionnaire

contained a number of items unrelated to this study. The questionnaire items pertaining

to this study, and the covering and reminder letters are attached in Appendices 1, 2 and

3 respectively.

Page 91: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

74

3.2 Pilot Survey

Prior to the mail-out, the survey instrument was pre-tested to assess athletes’

understanding of the questionnaire items, to ensure that the wording was appropriate

and unambiguous, and to determine the amount of time required to complete the survey.

Two group meetings were held with a total of 18 athletes from the Western Australian

Institute of Sports (WAIS).

The time taken to complete the questionnaire ranged between 18 and 38 minutes.

Overall, the question items were found to be easy to read and understand. Following

minor modifications indicated by the groups to some wording, the questionnaire was

piloted with 200 athletes on the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s database. The pilot

revealed no significant problems with the questionnaire, hence these data were included

in the final sample.

3.3 The Questionnaire Items

Questionnaire items were developed to measure all of the constructs in the Sport Drug

Control model. Each of the items used to measure the eight model factors (personal

morality, threat appraisal, benefit appraisal, legitimacy, reference group opinion,

personality characteristics; availability and affordability of performance-enhancing

substances) and past and current performance-enhancing substances use, and attitude

towards performance-enhancing substances use are presented here.

Page 92: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

75

3.3.1 Threat appraisal

Threat appraisal was measured in terms of threats relating to enforcement and threats

relating to ill-health effects of performance-enhancing substances use.

Appraisal of threats relating to enforcement:

The perceived likelihood of being tested in and out of competition was measured by the

item: “How likely is it that athletes at your level would be drug tested at least once a

year: (a) out of competition; and (b) in competition?” The response categories were:

very likely, quite likely, a little likely, not likely, not at all likely.

The perceived likelihood of evading detection if using doping in and out of competition

was measured by the item: “From what you know or have heard, if you were to take

banned performance-enhancing substances (out of competition)/(while competing), how

likely do you think that you could get away with it if you really tried to?” The response

categories were: very likely, quite likely, a little likely, not likely, not at all likely.

The perceived severity of the sanctions for testing positive was measured by the item:

“From what you know or have heard, are the penalties for a positive drug test in your

sport severe or lenient?” The response categories were: very severe, fairly severe, fairly

lenient, very lenient.

Appraisal of threats relating to ill-health effects of performance-enhancing

substances use:

The perceived harm to health in using different types of banned performance-enhancing

substances ‘once or twice ever’ and ‘regularly’ were assessed separately using similar

Page 93: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

76

measures employed in studies of drug use (e.g., Amonini 2001; Health Department of

Western Australia 1998; Nucci, Guerra & Lee 1991):

– Once or twice: “How much harm to your health do you think would be caused by

using each of the following substances once or twice ever: anabolic steroids; beta-

blockers; designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG); erythropoietin (EPO)

and other similar substances; human growth hormones (hGH); and diuretics?”; and

– Regularly: “How much harm to your health do you think would be caused by using

each of the following substances regularly: anabolic steroids; beta-blockers;

designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG); erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances; human growth hormones (hGH); and diuretics?”

The response categories were: no harm, a little harm, some harm, a lot of harm, don’t

know.

3.3.2 Benefit appraisal

Benefit appraisal was measured in terms of the perceived impact of performance-

enhancing substances use on sport performance, and the perceived likelihood and

desirability of potential positive outcomes of success in sport.

The impact of performance-enhancing substances use on performance was measured by

the item: “If you were to use the following, how likely is it that would improve your

performance in your sport: anabolic steroids; beta-blockers; designer steroids like

tetrahydrogestrinone (THG); erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances; and

human growth hormones (hGH)?” The response categories were: definitely would not,

probably would not, might or might not, probably would, definitely would, don’t know.

Page 94: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

77

The likelihood of several potential positive outcomes of success in their sport was

measured by: “What outcomes does your sport offer you if you perform well: national

celebrity status; lucrative sponsorship deals; personal best achievements; opportunities

for remaining in the sport as coach, trainer or administrator; future financial security;

and international celebrity status?” The response categories were: a lot, a little, not at

all.

The desirability of each of the above potential positive outcomes of success in their

sport was measured by: “How much would you like these outcomes for performing well

in your sport: national celebrity status; lucrative sponsorship deals; personal best

achievements; opportunities for remaining in the sport as coach, trainer or

administrator; future financial security; and international celebrity status?” The

response categories were: a lot, a little, not at all.

3.3.3 Personal morality

The measures of personal morality in terms of moral judgement towards performance-

enhancing substances use and moral affect of being caught using performance-

enhancing substances were adapted from Amonini (2001).

Moral judgement towards performance-enhancing substances use was measured by:

“Regardless of whether you believe performance-enhancing substances should be

banned or allowed, which of the following statements best describes your personal

feelings about deliberately using banned performance-enhancing substances: (1) I

believe deliberately using banned performance-enhancing substances to improve

performance is morally wrong under any circumstances; (2) I believe deliberately using

Page 95: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

78

banned performance-enhancing substances to improve performance is OK under some

circumstances, but wrong under others; (3) I believe deliberately using banned

performance-enhancing substances to improve performance is OK under any

circumstances?”

Moral affect (i.e., emotional responses of guilt, shame, embarrassment) of being caught

using performance-enhancing substances was measured by: “If you were caught using

banned performance-enhancing substances, to what extent would you experience the

following feelings: ashamed; embarrassed; guilty?” The response categories ranged

from 1 ‘not at all’ to 5 ‘to a great extent’.

Five other emotions that could potentially be experienced if caught using performance-

enhancing substances were included on the list: angry; resentful; devastated; unlucky;

and relieved.

3.3.4 Reference group opinion

Perceptions about relevant others’ involved in the athlete’s life moral judgment towards

the use of banned performance-enhancing substances were measured using the same

question format as for personal morality: “Here is a list of people that may have an

involvement in your life. Regardless of whether you believe performance-enhancing

substances should be banned or allowed, which of the following statements do you think

best describes their personal feelings about deliberately using banned performance-

enhancing substances: your coach/trainer; your team mates/training partner; your

family/partner; your close friends; your sports doctor; your sports psychologist; and

Page 96: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

79

your lawyer?” The response categories were the same as for personal morality, except

that a ‘not applicable’ response was permitted.

Moral judgment of performance-enhancing substances use among people involved in

sports in general was measured by: “What about the following people’s feelings about

using banned performance-enhancing substances: most other athletes; most spectators;

most of the general public; sports lawyers in general; and sports journalists in

general?” The response categories were the same as for personal morality.

3.3.5 Legitimacy

Perceptions of legitimacy of the testing and appeals processes were measured on Tyler’s

(1990) three dimensions of justice: (1) distributive justice (i.e., perceived fairness of the

drug testing process); (2) procedural justice (i.e., perceived fairness of the appeals

process); and (3) interactional justice (i.e., interpersonal interactions with personnel

administering the drug collection procedure).

Distributive justice:

Distributive justice was measured by three items:

1. Equitable treatment of all athletes by the enforcement agency: “How fair is the

Australian Sports Drug Agency in terms of treating all athletes equally?” The

response categories were: very fair, fair, unfair, very unfair;

2. Security of the testing procedures: “How secure is the Australian Sports Drug

Agency’s drug testing procedures in Australia? (That is, in taking of samples and

care of samples).” The response categories were: very secure, quite secure, not

really secure, not at all secure; and

Page 97: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

80

3. Accuracy of the current drug tests: “How accurate do you feel the current drug tests

are in terms of being able to correctly identify these substances: anabolic steroids;

beta-blockers; designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG); erythropoietin

(EPO) and other similar substances; human growth hormones (hGH); and

diuretics?” The response categories for each were: very accurate, quite accurate, a

little accurate, not accurate, not at all accurate, don’t know.

Procedural justice:

Procedural justice was measured by the following three items:

1. Level of satisfaction in receiving a fair hearing in appealing a positive test: “How

satisfied are you that athletes who appeal a positive test in Australia will be given a

fair hearing?” The response categories were: very satisfied, somewhat satisfied,

somewhat dissatisfied, very dissatisfied;

2. Level of satisfaction in receiving a fair hearing prior to decision on imposing

sanctions: “How satisfied are you that athletes who test positive in your sport will

be given a fair hearing before a decision is made about applying a penalty?” The

response categories were: very satisfied, somewhat satisfied, somewhat dissatisfied,

very dissatisfied; and

3. Level of satisfaction in receiving a fair hearing in the Court of Arbitration in Sport:

“How satisfied are you that athletes who appeal a positive test before the Court of

Arbitration in Sport, will be given a fair hearing?” The response categories were:

very satisfied, somewhat satisfied, somewhat dissatisfied, very dissatisfied.

Page 98: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

81

Interactional justice:

Athletes who have been drug tested were asked: “How would you describe the conduct

of the testing personnel: (a) Courteous or Rude or Neither; (b) Helpful or Unhelpful or

Neither; (c) Friendly or Unfriendly or Neither; (d) Sensitive or Insensitive or Neither?”

3.3.6 Personality

Personality was measured using items adapted from Amonini (2001) and Weber and

Roehl (1988): risk taking propensity (‘I am the kind of person who avoids risk’; ‘I am

the kind of person who enjoys risk’); self-esteem (‘Most people my age are better liked

than me’; ‘I like my physical appearance’); assertiveness (‘Usually, if I have something

to say, I say it’); temperament (‘I tend to be aggressive’; ‘I am even tempered’); self-

control (‘I have self-control’); and determination (‘I am ready to win at all costs’).

Respondents were presented with these items and asked: “Here are a number of

statements that may or may not describe you. For each statement, please indicate

whether you agree or disagree, or have mixed feelings with it?” Response categories

were: strongly agree, agree, no feelings/mixed feelings, disagree, strongly disagree.

In relation to optimism, the Seligman Attributional Style Questionnaire (SASQ)

(Seligman 1991) was adapted to measure respondents’ reactions to a positive and a

negative event on three dimensions: internality-externality; stability-instability; and

globality-specificity (Schulman, Castellon & Seligman 1989; Seligman 1991).

Respondents’ reactions to a positive event (i.e., “When you are performing well”) was

assessed on three bipolar 7-point scales: (1) ‘It is mainly due to other people or

circumstances’ – ‘It is mainly due to me’; (2) ‘This is the only thing I’m good at’ – ‘I

Page 99: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

82

can do anything’; and (3) ‘That it won’t happen again’ – ‘That it will happen again’.

Then respondents reactions to a negative event (i.e., “When something goes wrong”)

were assessed on three bipolar 7-point scales: (1) ‘It is mainly due to other people or

circumstances’ – ‘It is mainly due to me’; (2) ‘That everything will go wrong’ – ‘That it

won’t affect other aspects of my life’; and (3) ‘That it won’t happen again’ – ‘That it

will happen again’.

3.3.7 Availability and affordability

The availability and affordability of performance-enhancing substances were assessed

on scales similar to those used by Amonini (2001), Eggert and Herting (1993), and

Newcomb and Felix-Ortiz (1992).

Perceived availability of performance-enhancing substances: “How easy or difficult

would it be to get each of the following types of substances: anabolic steroids; beta-

blockers; designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG); erythropoietin (EPO) and

other similar substances; human growth hormones (hGH); and diuretics?” The

response categories were: probably impossible, very hard, fairly hard, fairly easy, very

easy, don’t know.

Perceived affordability of performance-enhancing substances: “How expensive would it

be for you personally to buy each of the following types of substances: anabolic

steroids; beta-blockers; designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG);

erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances; human growth hormones (hGH);

and diuretics?” The response categories were: very cheap, quite cheap, neither, quite

expensive, very expensive, don’t know.

Page 100: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

83

3.3.8 Attitude towards banned performance-enhancing substances use

Donovan et al.’s (2002) ‘attitudes/intentions’ towards performance-enhancing

substances use was operationalised in terms of perceived necessity to use performance-

enhancing substances to perform at the highest levels (following the work of Petróczi

2002), and susceptibility to performance-enhancing substances use (via a hypothetical

scenario format as in Bamberger & Yaeger 1997; Corbin et al. 1994; Goldman & Klatz

1992).

Perceived necessity to use performance-enhancing substances to perform at the highest

levels was measured by asking athletes: “In your sport, how necessary do you believe it

is for athletes to use banned performance-enhancing substances at least at some time,

to perform at the very highest levels?” The response categories were: definitely have to

use performance-enhancing substances at some time; probably have to use performance-

enhancing substances at some time; might or might not have to use performance-

enhancing substances at some time; probably don’t have to use performance-enhancing

substances at some time; definitely don’t have to use performance-enhancing substances

at some time.

Susceptibility to performance-enhancing substances use was determined by presenting

athletes with the following scenario: “If you were offered a banned performance-

enhancing substance under medical supervision at low or no financial cost and the

banned performance-enhancing substance could make a significant difference to your

performance and was currently not detectable”, and asking: “How much consideration

would you give to the offer?” The response categories were: a lot of consideration, some

consideration, a little consideration, none at all.

Page 101: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

84

3.3.9 Behaviour: use of banned performance-enhancing substances or methods

Prevalence of use of banned performance-enhancing substances and methods was

assessed via three measures:

1. Self-reported use of banned performance-enhancing substances in general: “Which

one of the following most applies to you:

– I have never considered using a banned performance-enhancing substance;

– At one stage I thought briefly about using a banned performance-enhancing

substance;

– At one stage I thought quite a bit about using a banned performance-enhancing

substance;

– I still think occasionally about using a banned performance-enhancing

substance because other athletes are using them;

– I briefly used a banned performance-enhancing substance in the past but no

longer do so;

– I occasionally use a banned performance-enhancing substance now for specific

purposes;

– I regularly try or use banned performance-enhancing substances?”;

2. Self-reported past and current use of specific banned performance-enhancing

substances and methods: “In the last 12 months, have you used any of the following,

for whatever reason: anabolic steroids; beta blockers; designer steroids like

tetrahydrogestrinone (THG); erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances;

human growth hormones (hGH); diuretics; doping methods; and alphabodies?”

The response categories were: have never used, did not use in the last 12 months, 1

to 2 times, 3 to 5 times, 6 to 10 times, more than 10 times; and

Page 102: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

85

3. Tested positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance: “Have you ever

tested positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance?” Response

categories were ‘yes’ or ‘no’.

3.3.10 Sporting background and sample characteristics

The questionnaire also collected data on athletes’ sporting background (e.g., main sport

involved in, highest level competed at, national and international titles held) and socio-

demographic information (e.g., age, gender, education level, annual income).

Appendix 1 contains the questionnaire items used to measure all of the constructs in the

Sport Drug Control model.

3.4 Response Rate

By 30 November 2004, of the 4,103 surveys mailed-out by the Australian Sports Drug

Agency, 1,174 completed questionnaires were returned, 196 were ‘returned-to-sender’

and 53 apologies were received for non-completion. Hence, a 30% response rate was

achieved.

Of the 979 surveys that were sent to Australian Football League (AFL; N = 429),

National Rugby League (NRL; N = 420) and Australian Rugby Union (ARU; N = 130)

national clubs, 83 completed questionnaires were returned (AFL: n = 42; NRL: n = 33;

ARU: n = 8). Given that the actual number of athletes in these three football codes

receiving the survey is unknown, conservative estimates of the response rates are 10%

for AFL, 8% for NRL and 6% for ARU.

Page 103: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

86

In total, 1,257 completed questionnaires were returned. A conservative estimate of the

overall response rate is 26%. Given that the targeted population was elite athletes, the

response rate achieved is good when compared with the market research industry

average response rate for mail surveys of the general population of approximately 25%.

Furthermore, this response rate is similar to other large mail surveys of elite athletes in

Australia (30%, Jalleh & Donovan 2007; 33%, Jalleh & Donovan 2008a) and the United

Kingdom (29%, UK Sport 2006). In a substantially smaller mail survey of elite United

Kingdom athletes in four sports (athletics, cycling, rowing, sailing), a somewhat higher

response rate was achieved (38%, Somerville & Lewis 2005).

In relation to football codes, a mail survey of National Football League players achieved

a low response rate as in this survey (7.5%, N = 120, Yesalis, Kopstein & Bahrke 2001).

However a mail survey of professional footballers in the United Kingdom achieved a

higher response rate (25%, N = 706, Waddington et al. 2005). The low response rate in

the football codes may be due to lack of interest among athletes or the survey wasn’t

fully distributed to all athletes on the national competition databases.

3.5 Statistical Analysis Overview

3.5.1 Data screening

Statistical analysis was performed using PASW Statistics 17 (PASW Statistics 2009)

and Amos 17 (Arbuckle 2009). Data were screened for accuracy of data entry, missing

values, outliers and normality prior to construct development for use in structural

equation modelling.

Page 104: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

87

The data were first examined for accuracy of data entry. All variables were found to

contain legitimate numerical codes or values. A non-significant Little’s chi-square

statistics indicated that missing data were not missing completely at random. This was

expected given that multiple items were used to measure some components of each of

the constructs in the model. For example, under the threat appraisal construct, the risk to

health of regular use of performance-enhancing substances was measured via six items,

(i.e., the six banned performance-enhancing substances). For each of these multiple item

questions, a small proportion of respondents did not answer any items (3% or less),

which may have influenced the results of the missing value analysis. However, only a

small proportion of missing data were found for each questionnaire item, and no

systematic pattern to the missing data was identified. With the exception of two of

twelve items measuring reference group opinions towards banned performance-

enhancing substances use (missing values: 7.6% and 6.0%), the missing values ranged

between 1.2% and 4.6% (mean: 2.6%) for all items measuring the constructs in the

model. Missing values were replaced using the expectation maximisation method in the

missing value analysis (MVA) in PASW Statistics 17 (Arbuckle 1996). Of the total

sample, 1.6% of cases were excluded due to non responses to all of the items measuring

the dependent variable (i.e., self-reported use of banned performance-enhancing

substances).

Mahalanobis distance was computed to check for multivariate outliers. Given that only

a minimal number of cases was found (n = 16), these cases were retained.

Transformation or score alteration of these multivariate outliers were not performed as

such manipulations may be ineffective as it is the combination of scores on two or more

variables that is aberrant rather than the score on a particular variable (Tabachnick &

Fidell 2001).

Page 105: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

88

An assessment of normality was conducted. The multivariate kurtosis statistics (i.e.,

Mardia’s coefficient) was 58.76 which suggested significant multivariate non-normality

in the data. This outcome does not affect estimates of regression coefficients, but can

lead to less accurate standard errors and significance tests (Byrne 2001; Nevitt &

Hancock 2001; West et al. 1995). Maximum likelihood estimation was used in the

structural equation modeling, which assumes that the data are continuous and

multivariate normally distributed. Given that the assumption of multivariate normality

was violated, assessment of the structural equation model fit was evaluated using the

Bollen-Stine bootstrap p-value instead of the chi-square statistics (Bollen & Stine 1992;

Byrne 2001). Bootstrapping is a technique that allows more accurate estimation of

standard errors, confidence intervals, and significance for non-normal data by

repeatedly resampling from the total sample to estimate the distribution of the

population (Mooney & Duvall 1993; Tabachnick & Fidell 2001). The number of

bootstrap samples was set at 1,000.

In total, 1,237 cases were used in the main analyses.

3.5.2 Analytical procedures for testing the model

Exploratory factor analysis procedures:

Exploratory factor analyses were conducted in the construct development phase to

assess the factor structure of components of the constructs in the model. The criteria for

determining adequacy of the data for factor analysis were a Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin

measure of sampling adequacy of 0.6 and above and a Bartlett’s test of sphericity

significant at p < 0.001 (Tabachnick & Fidell 2001).

Page 106: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

89

Since the purpose of this exploratory analysis was to identify the underlying dimensions

and reduce the number of variables to a smaller set of factors for subsequent analyses,

principle axis factoring was used with varimax rotation. The factor loading for each

item was considered in determining items to include in the analysis. The higher the

loading the higher the correlation with the factor. A loading of ±0.30 was considered to

meet the minimum level, a loading of ±0.40 was considered more important, and a

loading of ±0.50 or greater was considered practically significant. Items with similar

loadings on more than one factor were deleted (Hair et al. 1998).

Cronbach’s alpha reliability estimates were calculated to assess the internal consistency

of the multivariate scales (Nunnally 1978). Unidimensional items with factor loadings

above 0.4 and eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were retained (Hair et al. 1998). Coefficient

alphas were computed each time items were deleted (Choi et al. 2004; Flynn & Pearcy

2001). The criterion validity of each item was assessed by the item-to-total correlation

of each measure. Items with a low correlation with the total score of less than 0.6 were

deleted.

Structural equation modelling procedures:

The Sport Drug Control model was evaluated by assessing the model’s goodness-of-fit

with the data and by examining parameter estimates to determine the relative

contribution of the different paths contained in the predictive model.

The structural model for testing can be summarised as follows:

1. Attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use is influenced by personal

morality (moral judgement; moral emotions), reference group opinions (moral

judgement of relevant others), benefit appraisal (performance-enhancing substances

Page 107: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

90

use on improving performance; rewards for performing well in sport), threat

appraisal (threat to health from performance-enhancing substances use; threat of

detection), personality (risk taking propensity) and legitimacy (testing and appeals

processes);

2. Attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use, in turn, impacts upon

performance-enhancing substances use (i.e., behaviour); and

3. Performance-enhancing substances use (behaviour) is influenced by the

affordability and availability of these substances.

Model fit was examined using the chi-square statistics and several other indices for

goodness-of-fit: root-mean-square residual (RMR); root-mean-square error of

approximation (RMSEA); goodness-of-fit index (GFI); adjusted goodness-of-fit index

(AGFI); comparative fit index (CFI); Tucker-Lewis index (TLI); and incremental fit

index (IFI) (Hu & Bentler 1999; Marsh, Kit-Tai & Zhonglin 2004; Quintana & Maxwell

1999). The RMSEA 90% confidence intervals are also provided to assist in interpreting

these point estimates (MacCallum & Austin 2000). An absolute fit index (e.g., RMSEA)

assesses how well a model reproduces the sample data without comparison to a

reference model, whereas an incremental fit index (e.g., CFI, TLI, and IFI) compares

the specified model to a baseline null model (Hu & Bentler 1999). In comparison with

the chi-square statistics, these indices are reported as less sensitive to sample size

differences and to the number of indicators per latent variable increase (Landis, Beal &

Tesluk 2000; Schreiber et al. 2006). For CFI, TLI, IFI, GFI and AGFI values of 0.90

and 0.95 reflect acceptable and excellent fit to the data respectively. For the RMSEA

and RMR, values of 0.05 or less indicate a good fit, and between 0.05 and 0.08 a

moderate fit (Browne & Cudeck 1993; Byrne 2001; Hu & Bentler 1999; Schreiber et al.

Page 108: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

91

2006). Taken together, these indices provide a more conservative and comprehensive

evaluation of model fit than any single index alone.

Procedure for fixing the measurement properties of the single indicator constructs:

For each of the single indicator constructs in the structural equation model (i.e.,

personality, reference group opinion, availability and affordability of performance-

enhancing substances), Munck’s (1979) formula was used to calculate both the

regression coefficients and measurement error variances. These values were used for the

single indicator constructs in the structural equation model.

3.6 Concluding Comment

This chapter described the methodology of this research study. Details of the study

design, sampling method, data collection procedure, survey instrument and statistical

analysis procedures were provided. Chapter four presents the sample characteristics and

descriptive data for the variables in the model.

Page 109: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

92

CHAPTER 4: SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS AND

DESCRIPTIVE DATA

4.1 Sample Characteristics

Table 4.1 shows the highest level in which respondents had competed. Of the total

sample, 10.8% have participated in the Olympics/Paralympics, 46.3% at World

Championship events, 37.1% at the national level and 5.0% at the state or lower levels.

Table 4.1: Athletes’ highest level of competition

N=1,237

%

Olympic/Paralympics games 10.8

World championship events 46.3

National competition 37.1

State competition 4.3

Regional competition 0.6

City district competition 0.1

No response 0.7

Table 4.2 shows the main type of sports in which athletes were involved. The main type

of sport was ‘team sports’ (e.g., soccer, football, netball) (41.1%), followed by ‘water

sports’ (e.g., surfing, rowing, sailing) (12.2%).

Athletes were represented from a large number of sports with less than 10% of the total

sample represented by any one sport. The main sports participated in were athletics

(8.4%), swimming (7.8%), hockey (7.0%), rowing (6.3%), soccer (5.7%), basketball

(4.7%), netball (4.1%), cycling (3.5%), softball (3.3%), Australian Rules Football

(3.3%), weight lifting/power lifting (3.1%), and cricket (2.7%).

Page 110: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

93

Table 4.2: Main sports in which athletes were involved

N=1,237

%

Team sports 41.1

Water sports 12.2

Athletics 7.9

Swimming 8.2

Winter sports 2.7

Cycling 3.4

Weightlifting 3.1

Martial arts 2.5

Other sports 17.9

No response 1.1

Table 4.3 shows the socio-demographic characteristics of respondents surveyed

according to highest level of competition: Olympics/World championships level

(‘Olympics/World’); national level (‘National’); and state or lower levels

(‘State/Regional’). [In Tables 4.3 to 4.32, the number of athletes in these competition

levels combined does not equate to the number of athletes in the ‘total sample’ column

due to 0.7% of athletes not reporting their highest level of competition.]

Males and females were approximately equally represented in both the

‘Olympics/World’ and ‘National’ levels (males: 46.4% and 50.8% respectively). There

was a somewhat higher proportion of males in the ‘State/Regional’ level (58.1%).

The age distributions reflected an older sample for ‘Olympics/World’ athletes than

‘National’ and ‘State/Regional’ athletes, with substantially more athletes aged 26 years

and over (35.4% vs 13.1% and 1.6%, p = .000 for both comparisons).

Page 111: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

94

Table 4.3: Sample demographics

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample*

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

Characteristics % % % %

Gender:

Males 46.4 50.8 58.1 48.7

Females 51.5 47.9 40.3 49.5

No response 2.1 1.3 1.6 1.8

Age group:

13 to 17 years 9.6 33.1 56.5 20.8

18 to 25 years 50.6 50.1 40.3 50.0

26 to 35 years 25.2 10.9 1.6 18.7

36+ years 10.2 2.2 0.0 6.6

No response 4.4 3.7 1.6 4.0

Highest education level:

Year 10 or less 6.8 15.9 33.9 11.6

Years 11 to 12 19.9 32.2 35.5 25.2

Some/currently enrolled in TAFE 8.8 6.7 6.4 7.9

Some/currently enrolled in university 29.7 27.4 14.5 28.0

Completed TAFE 7.2 6.1 6.5 6.9

Completed university 25.3 10.7 1.6 18.6

No response 2.3 0.9 1.6 1.7

Income derived from sport:

No income at all from sport 38.0 44.7 66.1 42.0

Occasional income as an ‘extra’ 38.3 32.0 14.5 34.8

Regular income but less than half of

total income

7.4

3.7

4.8

5.8

About half of income from sport 2.5 2.2 0.0 2.3

More than half of income from sport,

but not all

3.4

1.5

1.6

2.6

All or almost all of income from sport 8.1 13.5 3.2 9.8

No response 2.3 2.4 9.7 2.7

Total annual income:

Less than $10,000 42.1 60.6 67.7 50.4

$10,000 to $49,999 37.6 21.4 11.3 30.2

$50,000 to $99,999 10.5 5.9 1.6 8.3

$100,000 or more 4.7 5.7 4.8 5.0

No response 5.1 6.5 14.5 6.1

* Note: The number of athletes in the competition levels combined does not equate to

the number of athletes in the ‘total sample’ column due to 0.7% of athletes not

reporting their highest level of competition.

Page 112: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

95

The level of education increases with higher competition levels, with the proportion

having greater than secondary education increasing from 29.0% for ‘State/Regional’

athletes to 50.9% for ‘National’ athletes to 71.0% for ‘Olympics/World’ athletes. This

reflects the age distribution of the athletes in each competition level.

A substantial minority of both ‘Olympics/World’ and ‘National’ athletes derive no

income from sport (38.0% and 44.7% respectively). A further 38.3% of

‘Olympics/World’ athletes and 32.0% of ‘National’ athletes derive occasional income

from sport. A majority of ‘State/Regional’ athletes derive no income from sport

(66.1%). The vast majority of athletes’ total annual income is less than $50,000

(‘Olympics/World’: 79.7%; ‘National’ 82.0%; ‘State/Regional’: 79.0%).

Table 4.4 shows the sporting background and achievements of respondents according to

highest level of competition. A small proportion of athletes competed in events for

athletes with a disability (‘Olympics/World’: 12.6%; ‘National’: 3.7%;

‘State/Regional’: 3.2%). In all subgroups, the vast majority of athletes had competed in

their sport for five years/seasons or more (‘Olympics/World’: 86.1%; ‘National’:

83.0%; ‘State/Regional’: 75.8%). Substantially greater proportions of

‘Olympics/World’ athletes than ‘National’ and ‘State/Regional’ athletes held national or

international title(s) (77.8% vs 42.3% and 3.2%, p = .000 for both comparisons).

Overall, the sample consisted of primarily elite Australian athletes aged 13 to 61 years

(mean age: 23.1 years; standard deviation: 7.8 years) from a variety of sports, with

approximately equal representation of males and females.

Page 113: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

96

Table 4.4: Sporting background and achievements

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Competed in events for athletes

with a disability:

Yes 12.6 3.7 3.2 8.7

No 78.8 91.7 95.2 84.6

No response 8.6 4.6 1.6 6.7

Years competed in main sport:

Less than 1 year (or season) 0.3 0.9 8.1 1.0

1 or 2 years (or seasons) 1.7 2.0 1.6 1.8

More than 2 but less than 5 years

(or seasons)

11.9

13.9

14.5

12.7

5 or more years (or seasons) 86.1 83.0 75.8 84.2

No response 0.0 0.2 0.0 0.3

Highest level of title held:

International 34.8 5.9 1.6 38.2

National 43.0 36.4 1.6 22.2

None 21.2 57.1 96.8 38.4

No response 1.0 0.7 0.0 1.2

4.2 Prevalence of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

Prevalence of performance-enhancing substances use was assessed via three measures:

(1) self-reported use of a banned performance-enhancing substance; (2) self-reported

use of specific types of banned performance-enhancing substances and methods; and (3)

self-reported positive test for a banned performance-enhancing substance.

Page 114: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

97

4.2.1 Self-reported use of a banned performance-enhancing substance

Respondents were presented with the statements in Table 4.5 and asked: “Which one of

the following most applies to you?” In each subgroup, the vast majority of respondents

reported that they had never considered using a banned performance-enhancing

substance (ranged between 86.4% and 95.2%). Of the total sample, 8.0% of respondents

considered doing so at some time, and 1.3% had used a banned performance-enhancing

substance (‘briefly’: 0.9%; ‘occasionally’: 0.2%; ‘regularly’: 0.2%).

Table 4.5: Proportion of athletes who considered or have used

a banned performance-enhancing substance

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

I have never considered using a

banned performance-enhancing

substance

86.4 92.4 95.2 89.2

At one stage I thought briefly about

using a banned performance-

enhancing substance

6.4 3.9 3.2 5.3

At one stage I thought quite a bit

about using a banned performance-

enhancing substance

0.8 1.5 0.0 1.1

I still think occasionally about using

a banned performance-enhancing

substance because other athletes are

using them

2.3 0.9 0.0 1.6

I briefly used a banned performance-

enhancing substance in the past but

no longer do so

1.3 0.2 1.6 0.9

I occasionally use a banned

performance-enhancing substance

now for specific purposes

0.1 0.4 0.0 0.2

I regularly try or use banned

performance-enhancing substances

0.3 0.0 0.0 0.2

No response 2.4 0.7 0.0 1.6

Page 115: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

98

4.2.2 Self-reported use of specific types of banned performance-enhancing

substances and methods

Athletes were presented with a list of eight banned performance-enhancing substances

and methods and asked: “In the last 12 months, have you used any of the following, for

whatever reason?” The response categories were: have never used, did not use in the

last 12 months, 1 to 2 times, 3 to 5 times, 6 to 10 times, more than 10 times.

Usage of each substance or method was classified into two categories: (1) ‘used in the

last 12 months’ (i.e., reported use of the substance or method at least ‘1 to 2 times’); and

(2) ‘ever used’ (i.e., reported use of the substance or method at least ‘1 to 2 times’ or

‘did not use in the last 12 months’). The data on ‘used in the last 12 months’ and ‘ever

used’ for the banned performance-enhancing substances and methods are shown in

Tables 4.6 and 4.7 respectively.

Of the total sample, 6.9% reported using a banned performance-enhancing substance or

method (used in the last 12 months: 3.4%). Diuretics was the most frequently used

banned performance-enhancing substance (ever used: 4.6%; used in the last 12 months:

2.3%).

Page 116: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

99

Table 4.6: Proportion of athletes who used a banned performance-enhancing

substance in the last 12 months

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% (n) % (n) % (n) % (n)

Diuretics 3.2 (23) 1.1 (5) 0.0 (0) 2.3 (28)

Anabolic steroids 0.7 (5) 0.9 (4) 0.0 (0) 0.7 (9)

Beta-blockers 0.3 (2) 0.2 (1) 0.0 (0) 0.2 (3)

Human growth hormones (hGH) 0.3 (2) 0.0 (0) 0.0 (0) 0.2 (2)

Doping methods 0.4 (3) 0.0 (0) 0.0 (0) 0.2 (3)

Alphabodies 0.0 (0) 0.2 (1) 0.0 (0) 0.1 (1)

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)

0.0 (0)

0.2 (1)

0.0 (0)

0.1 (1)

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances

0.1 (1)

0.0 (0)

0.0 (0)

0.1 (1)

Any of the above banned

performance-enhancing

substances or methods 4.2 (30) 2.6 (12) 0.0 (0) 3.4 (42)

Table 4.7: Proportion of athletes who ‘ever used’ a banned

performance-enhancing substance

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% (n) % (n) % (n) % (n)

Diuretics 6.5 (46) 2.4 (11) 0.0 (0) 4.6 (57)

Anabolic steroids 1.6 (11) 2.0 (9) 0.0 (0) 1.6 (20)

Beta-blockers 1.1 (8) 0.9 (4) 0.0 (0) 1.0 (12)

Human growth hormones (hGH) 0.7 (5) 0.2 (1) 0.0 (0) 0.5 (6)

Doping methods 0.6 (4) 0.0 (0) 0.0 (0) 0.3 (4)

Alphabodies 0.3 (2) 0.2 (1) 0.0 (0) 0.2 (3)

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)

0.0 (0)

0.2 (1)

0.0 (0)

0.1 (1)

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances

0.1 (1)

0.0 (0)

0.0 (0)

0.1 (1)

Any of the above banned

performance-enhancing

substances or methods

8.5 (60)

5.4 (25)

0.0 (0)

6.9 (85)

Page 117: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

100

4.2.3 Tested positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance

Athletes were asked: “Have you ever been drug tested?”, and if so, “Have you ever

tested positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance?” The results are shown

in Table 4.8.

Of the total sample, 61.4% of athletes reported that they had been drug tested. The

proportion who reported being drug tested increased with higher levels of competition:

78.5% in ‘Olympic/World’ athletes; 39.9% in ‘National’ athletes; and 25.8% in

‘State/Regional’ athletes. This is consistent with anti-doping organisations’ practice of

targeting mainly elite athletes in their testing program.

Among the athletes who were drug tested, 1.1% (n = 8) reported ever testing positive

for a banned performance-enhancing substance.

Table 4.8: Proportion of athletes who have ever been drug tested

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Ever been drug tested:

Yes 78.5 39.9 25.8 61.4

No 21.5 60.1 74.2 38.6

Ever tested positive for

a banned performance-

enhancing substance:

N=555

N=183

N=16

N=759

Yes 0.9 1.1 6.3 1.1

No 97.7 96.2 87.5 97.1

No response 1.4 2.7 6.3 1.8

Page 118: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

101

4.3 Attitude Towards Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

Attitude towards banned performance-enhancing substances use was assessed via two

measures: (1) amount of consideration athletes would give to an offer of a banned

performance-enhancing substances; and (2) necessity to engage in banned performance-

enhancing substances use to perform at the highest levels.

Athletes were presented with a scenario of an offer of a banned performance-enhancing

substance and asked how much consideration they would give to the offer. The response

categories and results are shown in Table 4.9. In each competition level, a majority of

athletes would give the offer no consideration at all (ranged between 51.6% and 61.3%).

A small proportion of athletes in all competition levels would give the offer ‘a lot’ of

consideration (ranged between 6.5% and 8.1%).

Table 4.9: Amount of consideration athletes would give to an offer

of a banned performance-enhancing substance

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

A lot of consideration 8.1 7.6 6.5 7.9

Some consideration 8.2 9.2 6.5 8.4

A little consideration 21.9 30.7 24.2 25.1

None at all 59.3 51.6 61.3 56.7

No response 2.5 0.9 1.6 1.9

Perceived necessity to engage in performance-enhancing substances use to perform at

the highest levels was assessed by asking athletes: “In your sport, how necessary do you

believe it is for athletes to use banned performance-enhancing substances at least at

Page 119: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

102

some time, to perform at the very highest levels?” The response categories and results

are shown in Table 4.10.

At all competition levels, the vast majority of respondents responded ‘definitely’ don’t

have to use banned performance-enhancing substances (ranged between 65.8% and

75.8%). Only a small proportion of respondents felt that it is necessary to do so, with

the proportion greater at higher competition levels (‘definitely/probably’ have to use:

‘Olympics/World’: 7.7%; ‘National’: 3.0%; ‘State/Regional’: 0.0%).

Table 4.10: Rating of necessity to use banned performance-enhancing

substances to perform at the very highest levels

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Definitely have to use 3.0 1.5 0.0 2.3

Probably have to use 4.7 1.5 0.0 3.3

Might or might not have to use 9.5 6.1 8.1 8.1

Probably don’t have to use 14.7 12.9 11.3 13.8

Definitely don’t have to use 65.8 74.1 75.8 69.2

No response 2.4 3.9 4.8 3.3

Perceived necessity to use banned performance-enhancing substances was significantly

and positively correlated with amount of consideration given to the offer of a banned

performance-enhancing substance (r = .273, p = .000). The proportion of ‘definitely’

don’t have to use banned performance-enhancing substances response decreased with

greater consideration given to the offer (none at all: 77.0%; a little: 66.9%; some:

54.8%; a lot: 36.7%).

Page 120: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

103

4.4 Threat Appraisal

Threat appraisal was measured in terms of threats relating to enforcement and threats

relating to ill-health effects of performance-enhancing substances use.

4.4.1 Appraisal of threat of enforcement

The appraisal of threat of enforcement was measured in terms of: (1) perceived

likelihood of being drug tested at least once a year (in competition)/(out of

competition); (2) perceived likelihood of getting away with taking banned performance-

enhancing substances (in competition)/(out of competition); and (3) perceived severity

of the penalties for a positive drug test.

Perceived likelihood of being tested in and out of competition was assessed by asking

athletes: “How likely is it that athletes at your level would be drug tested at least once a

year: (a) out of competition; and (b) in competition?” The response categories and

results are shown in Table 4.11.

The vast majority of ‘Olympics/World’ athletes felt that it is likely athletes at their level

would be drug tested out of competition at least once a year (87.3%). The proportion

was 68.6% in ‘National’ athletes and 46.8% in ‘State/Regional’ athletes. This is

consistent with the drug testing regime of anti-doping organisations in which the

number and frequency of athletes tested increases at higher levels of competition.

In each competition level, the perceived likelihood of being tested in competition was

higher than out of competition: ‘Olympics/World’: 94.0% vs 87.3%; ‘National’: 88.0%

Page 121: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

104

vs 68.6%; and ‘State/Regional’: 67.7% vs 46.8%. It is clear from these data that

perceived likelihood of being tested both in and out of competition increases with

higher levels of competition.

Table 4.11: Perceived likelihood of being tested in and out of competition

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Out of competition:

Very likely 46.0 22.7 9.7 35.3

Quite likely 24.6 22.2 14.5 23.2

A little likely 16.7 23.7 22.6 19.7

Not likely 7.6 23.3 30.6 14.7

Not at all likely 4.0 7.4 22.6 6.1

No response 1.0 0.7 0.0 0.9

In competition:

Very likely 52.2 36.8 25.8 45.0

Quite likely 28.6 29.6 25.8 28.9

A little likely 13.2 21.6 16.1 16.5

Not likely 4.1 10.5 22.6 7.4

Not at all likely 0.8 0.9 9.7 1.3

No response 1.1 0.7 0.0 0.9

Perceived likelihood of evading detection if using performance-enhancing substances in

and out of competition was assessed by asking athletes: “From what you know or have

heard, if you were to take banned performance-enhancing substances (out of

competition)/(while competing), how likely do you think that you could get away with it

if you really tried to?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.12.

For out of competition, the responses were mixed with 32.1% of the total sample

responding ‘very/quite’ likely to getting away with performance-enhancing substances

use, 31.9% ‘a little’ likely, and 34.6% ‘not (at all)’ likely. For in competition, the

Page 122: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

105

distribution of responses was skewed towards not likely to get away with performance-

enhancing substances use: ‘very/quite’ likely: 15.7% of the total sample; ‘a little’ likely:

22.7%; ‘not (at all)’ likely: 60.5%. For both in and out of competition, there were no

significant differences in responses according to level of competition.

Table 4.12: Perceived likelihood of evading detection if using performance-

enhancing substances in and out of competition

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Out of competition:

Very likely 7.4 8.1 12.9 7.8

Quite likely 23.6 24.8 22.6 24.3

A little likely 31.4 33.8 25.8 31.9

Not likely 26.6 24.8 25.8 25.9

Not at all likely 9.3 7.2 12.9 8.7

No response 1.7 1.3 0.0 1.5

In competition:

Very likely 2.4 2.8 4.8 2.8

Quite likely 12.4 13.7 11.3 12.9

A little likely 21.6 25.5 12.9 22.7

Not likely 37.9 35.7 45.2 37.3

Not at all likely 24.5 20.9 25.8 23.2

No response 1.1 1.3 0.0 1.1

Perceived severity of the sanctions for testing positive was assessed by asking athletes:

“From what you know or have heard, are the penalties for a positive drug test in your

sport severe or lenient?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.13.

In each competition level, the vast majority of athletes felt that the penalties for a

positive drug test in their sport were ‘very’ or ‘fairly’ severe (ranged between 78.5%

Page 123: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

106

and 88.7%). The proportion of athletes who nominated ‘very’ lenient was 2.0% or less

in each competition level.

Table 4.13: Perceived severity of the sanctions for testing positive to a

performance-enhancing substance

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Very severe 23.3 21.8 33.9 23.4

Fairly severe 55.2 59.0 54.8 56.5

Fairly lenient 15.8 12.6 6.5 14.2

Very lenient 2.0 2.0 1.6 1.9

No response 3.7 4.6 3.2 4.0

4.4.2 Appraisal of threat of ill-health effects of performance-enhancing

substances use

Athletes were presented with the performance-enhancing substances listed in Table 4.14

and asked: “How much harm to your health do you think would be caused by using each

of the following substances (once or twice ever)/(regularly)?” The response categories

and results for ‘once or twice ever’ and ‘regular’ use are shown in Tables 4.14 and 4.15

respectively.

For health risk of ‘once or twice ever’ use, ‘anabolic steroids’ was perceived to be the

most harmful of the six performance-enhancing substances (‘a lot of harm’: 39.0% of

the total sample), followed by ‘human growth hormones (hGH)’ (36.1%), ‘designer

steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)’ (34.9%), ‘erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances’ (31.9%), and ‘beta-blockers’ (22.6%). For ‘diuretics’, the proportion

was substantially lower at 13.8%.

Page 124: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

107

For each of the six performance-enhancing substances, the proportion who rated it as ‘a

little harm’ or ‘no harm’ was greater at higher levels of competition (e.g., ‘diuretics’:

14.5% in ‘State/Regional’ athletes vs 34.2% in ‘National’ athletes vs 44.8% in

‘Olympics/World’ athletes). Other than ‘anabolic steroids’ (11.5%), a substantial

minority of athletes responded ‘don’t know’ (ranged between 22.2% and 37.6%).

Table 4.14: Perceived health risk of ‘once or twice ever’ use of six banned

performance-enhancing substances

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Anabolic steroids:

No harm 6.6 2.8 0.0 4.9

A little harm 14.1 12.4 14.5 13.5

Some harm 29.3 28.8 35.5 29.4

A lot of harm 38.9 40.1 33.9 39.0

Don’t know 8.9 15.3 11.3 11.5

No response 2.1 0.7 4.8 1.7

Human growth hormones (hGH):

No harm 6.1 3.5 0.0 4.9

A little harm 11.6 8.9 8.1 10.4

Some harm 23.2 23.1 38.7 24.1

A lot of harm 35.4 37.7 32.3 36.1

Don’t know 21.9 26.4 17.7 23.2

No response 1.8 0.4 3.2 1.4

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG):

No harm 4.4 1.1 0.0 3.0

A little harm 10.9 8.5 4.8 9.7

Some harm 21.4 22.4 27.4 22.2

A lot of harm 35.1 34.2 40.3 34.9

Don’t know 26.2 33.1 24.2 28.5

No response 2.1 0.7 3.2 1.6

Page 125: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

108

Table 4.14: Perceived health risk of ‘once or twice ever’ use of six banned

performance-enhancing substances (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances:

No harm 6.4 2.2 0.0 4.6

A little harm 11.2 10.0 4.8 10.3

Some harm 21.9 19.2 29.0 21.3

A lot of harm 32.0 32.0 32.3 31.9

Don’t know 26.4 35.3 30.6 29.9

No response 2.1 1.3 3.2 1.9

Beta-blockers:

No harm 8.1 3.5 0.0 5.9

A little harm 10.9 9.6 11.3 10.4

Some harm 20.7 21.8 24.2 21.2

A lot of harm 21.9 23.5 25.8 22.6

Don’t know 35.9 40.1 35.5 37.6

No response 2.5 1.5 3.2 2.3

Diuretics:

No harm 22.6 14.6 3.2 18.7

A little harm 22.2 19.6 11.3 20.7

Some harm 23.1 22.9 27.4 23.1

A lot of harm 12.3 14.6 24.2 13.8

Don’t know 18.0 27.7 30.6 22.2

No response 1.8 0.7 3.2 1.5

Table 4.15 shows that relative to the health risk of ‘once or twice ever’ use, the

proportion of ‘a lot of harm’ from ‘regular’ use is more than trebled for ‘diuretics’ and

approximately doubled for each of the other substances. With the exception of

‘diuretics’ (9.2%), the proportion of athletes who responded ‘a little harm’ or ‘no harm’

was 2.5% or less for each substance.

Page 126: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

109

Table 4.15: Perceived health risk of regular use of six banned

performance-enhancing substances

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Anabolic steroids:

No harm 0.4 0.0 0.0 0.2

A little harm 0.6 1.1 0.0 0.7

Some harm 6.2 5.4 4.8 5.9

A lot of harm 85.9 80.0 77.4 83.2

Don’t know 5.5 13.1 16.1 8.9

No response 1.4 0.4 1.6 1.1

Human growth hormones (hGH):

No harm 0.8 0.2 0.0 0.6

A little harm 2.5 1.1 0.0 1.9

Some harm 6.8 7.2 19.4 7.6

A lot of harm 72.8 68.4 61.3 70.7

Don’t know 15.3 22.4 16.1 17.9

No response 1.7 0.7 3.2 1.4

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG):

No harm 0.1 0.0 0.0 0.1

A little harm 0.8 0.2 0.0 0.6

Some harm 5.7 4.8 3.2 5.2

A lot of harm 73.1 65.1 71.0 70.2

Don’t know 18.7 28.8 22.6 22.6

No response 1.6 1.1 3.2 1.5

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances:

No harm 0.6 0.2 0.0 0.4

A little harm 2.0 0.4 0.0 1.3

Some harm 7.2 5.7 8.1 6.6

A lot of harm 69.0 61.0 59.7 65.7

Don’t know 19.5 31.8 27.4 24.4

No response 1.7 0.9 4.8 1.5

Page 127: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

110

Table 4.15: Perceived health risk of regular use of six banned

performance-enhancing substances (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Beta-blockers:

No harm 1.0 0.4 0.0 0.7

A little harm 1.6 0.9 0.0 1.2

Some harm 8.6 9.8 6.5 9.0

A lot of harm 58.0 52.5 56.5 55.7

Don’t know 28.7 35.5 33.9 31.7

No response 2.1 0.9 3.2 1.7

Diuretics:

No harm 2.0 0.7 0.0 1.4

A little harm 9.5 6.3 0.0 7.8

Some harm 19.9 18.7 21.0 19.5

A lot of harm 51.9 47.1 51.6 50.3

Don’t know 15.1 26.4 24.2 19.7

No response 1.6 0.9 3.2 1.4

4.5 Benefit Appraisal

Benefit appraisal was measured in terms of the perceived impact of performance-

enhancing substances use on sport performance, and the perceived likelihood and

desirability of potential positive outcomes of success in sport.

4.5.1 Perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on sport

performance

Impact of performance-enhancing substances use on sport performance was measured

by presenting athletes with the substances listed in Table 4.16, and asking them: “If you

were to use the following, how likely is it that would improve your performance in your

sport?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.16.

Page 128: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

111

Of the five substances, ‘anabolic steroids’ was perceived to be the most beneficial to

sport performance (‘definitely/probably would’: 57.9%), followed by ‘human growth

hormones (hGH)’ (47.7%), ‘designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)’

(41.4%), and ‘erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances’ (33.1%). The

proportion for ‘beta-blockers’ was substantially lower (13.4%), with a substantial

minority of athletes responding ‘don’t know’ (40.5%). With the exception of ‘anabolic

steroids’ (10.2%), the proportion of ‘don’t know’ responses was substantial for each of

the other substances (ranged between 22.9% and 32.7%).

Table 4.16: Perceived impact of performance-enhancing

substances on sport performance

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Anabolic steroids:

Definitely would not 13.0 11.8 21.0 12.9

Probably would 7.6 4.8 6.5 6.5

Might or might not 9.9 11.1 6.5 10.1

Probably would 24.6 27.2 21.0 25.2

Definitely would 33.4 32.0 27.4 32.7

Don’t know 9.1 11.5 9.7 10.2

No response 2.4 1.5 8.1 2.3

Human growth hormones (hGH):

Definitely would not 12.3 10.0 14.5 11.5

Probably would 5.8 4.4 4.8 5.2

Might or might not 7.6 13.3 19.4 10.3

Probably would 24.5 24.2 16.1 23.8

Definitely would 24.2 24.0 17.7 23.9

Don’t know 22.5 22.9 22.6 22.9

No response 3.1 1.3 4.8 2.5

Page 129: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

112

Table 4.16: Perceived impact of performance-enhancing

substances on sport performance (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG):

Definitely would not 11.3 10.9 16.1 11.3

Probably would 5.4 3.7 8.1 4.9

Might or might not 9.5 10.2 11.3 9.8

Probably would 18.8 21.8 16.1 19.7

Definitely would 22.9 20.5 14.5 21.7

Don’t know 28.9 31.2 27.4 29.7

No response 3.3 1.7 6.5 2.8

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances:

Definitely would not 12.7 12.6 14.5 12.7

Probably would 10.2 5.2 8.1 8.2

Might or might not 9.1 11.1 14.5 10.1

Probably would 15.0 16.3 3.2 14.8

Definitely would 20.7 15.5 11.3 18.3

Don’t know 28.9 36.8 41.9 32.7

No response 3.5 2.4 6.5 3.2

Beta-blockers:

Definitely would not 23.1 17.0 27.4 20.9

Probably would 11.0 7.8 6.5 9.5

Might or might not 11.5 13.5 4.8 11.8

Probably would 8.2 8.7 4.8 8.2

Definitely would 5.7 4.6 4.8 5.2

Don’t know 36.5 45.1 43.5 40.5

No response 4.1 3.3 8.1 4.0

4.5.2 Likelihood and desirability of potential positive outcomes for performing

well in sport

Athletes were presented with the list of potential positive outcomes for performing well

in their sport and asked: “What outcomes does your sport offer you if you perform

well?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.17.

Page 130: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

113

The main perceived outcome for performing well in sports was ‘personal best

achievements’ (‘a lot’: 64.5% to 68.0% in each competition level), followed by

‘opportunities for remaining in the sport as coach, trainer or administrator’ (39.7%). The

proportions were substantially lower for outcomes related to celebrity status and

financial wealth/security (ranged between 9.9% and 14.4%). With the exception of

‘personal best achievements’, the perceived likelihood of each of the potential outcomes

is lower with higher levels of competition.

The desirability of these potential positive outcomes for performing well in sport was

measured by asking athletes: “How much would you like these outcomes for performing

well in your sport?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.18.

‘Personal best achievements’ was the most desired outcome (‘a lot’: 82.3% to 88.1% in

each competition level). Financial outcomes were desired by the majority of athletes:

‘future financial security’: 71.0% to 78.5%; and ‘lucrative sponsorship deals’: 66.7% to

71.7%. Approximately half of the total sample desired ‘opportunities for remaining in

the sport as coach, trainer or administrator’ (51.8%). The desirability of national and

international celebrity status were substantially lower at 39.2% and 33.8% respectively.

The desirability of celebrity status and opportunities for remaining in the sport

decreased with higher levels of competition.

Page 131: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

114

Table 4.17: Perceived positive outcomes for performing well in sports

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Personal best achievements:

A lot 68.0 66.2 64.5 67.3

A little 24.2 28.1 29.0 25.8

Not at all 5.7 3.7 3.2 4.8

No response 2.1 2.0 3.2 2.1

Opportunities for remaining in

the sport as coach, trainer or

administrator:

A lot 34.5 45.3 56.5 39.7

A little 53.2 46.2 27.4 49.2

Not at all 10.5 5.2 11.3 8.6

No response 1.8 3.3 4.8 2.5

Lucrative sponsorship deals:

A lot 11.0 15.7 40.3 14.4

A little 29.0 46.2 38.7 35.9

Not at all 58.1 35.9 17.7 47.7

No response 1.8 2.2 3.2 2.0

National celebrity status:

A lot 9.1 18.5 25.8 13.6

A little 40.6 45.1 58.1 43.1

Not at all 48.5 34.0 12.9 41.2

No response 1.8 2.4 3.2 2.1

Future financial security:

A lot 9.2 16.8 30.6 13.3

A little 18.7 26.8 27.4 22.2

Not at all 70.6 54.5 37.1 62.7

No response 1.6 2.0 4.8 1.9

International celebrity status:

A lot 8.2 10.9 21.0 9.9

A little 28.0 33.8 38.7 30.7

Not at all 62.0 53.2 37.1 57.3

No response 1.8 2.2 3.2 2.0

Page 132: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

115

Table 4.18: Desirability of the positive outcomes for performing well in sports

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Personal best achievements:

A lot 88.1 86.3 82.3 87.1

A little 8.1 9.8 12.9 9.0

Not at all 1.6 1.3 1.6 1.5

No response 2.3 2.6 3.2 2.4

Opportunities for remaining in

the sport as coach, trainer or

administrator:

A lot 48.1 54.2 72.6 51.8

A little 40.9 36.8 22.6 38.3

Not at all 8.6 6.3 1.6 7.4

No response 2.4 2.6 3.2 2.5

Lucrative sponsorship deals:

A lot 71.7 66.7 69.4 69.7

A little 23.6 29.2 24.2 25.8

Not at all 3.1 1.7 3.2 2.6

No response 1.6 2.4 3.2 1.9

National celebrity status:

A lot 36.6 40.7 58.1 39.2

A little 50.6 47.7 32.3 48.7

Not at all 11.0 9.2 6.5 10.0

No response 1.7 2.4 3.2 2.0

Future financial security:

A lot 78.5 78.0 71.0 77.8

A little 17.0 18.5 19.4 17.8

Not at all 2.5 1.3 6.5 2.3

No response 2.0 2.2 3.2 2.1

International celebrity status:

A lot 30.8 36.4 48.4 33.8

A little 44.3 41.0 37.1 42.8

Not at all 22.9 20.0 11.3 21.1

No response 2.0 2.6 3.2 2.3

Page 133: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

116

4.6 Perceived Morality of Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

The measures of personal morality were in terms of moral judgement towards

performance-enhancing substances use and moral affect if caught using performance-

enhancing substances.

4.6.1 Moral judgment towards performance-enhancing substances use

Athletes were presented with the three statements in Table 4.19 and asked: “Regardless

of whether you believe performance-enhancing substances should be banned or

allowed, which of the following statements best describes your personal feelings about

deliberately using banned performance-enhancing substances?”

Table 4.19: Personal moral stance on banned

performance-enhancing substances use

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

I believe deliberately using banned

performance-enhancing substances to

improve performance is morally

wrong under any circumstances

89.8 87.8 90.3 89.1

I believe deliberately using banned

performance-enhancing substances to

improve performance is morally OK

under some circumstances, but wrong

under others

8.9 10.7 8.1 9.5

I believe deliberately using banned

performance-enhancing substances to

improve performance is morally OK

under any circumstances

0.4 0.9 0.0 0.6

No response 0.8 0.7 1.6 0.9

Page 134: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

117

Table 4.19 shows that in each competition level, the vast majority of athletes felt that

using banned performance-enhancing substances to improve performance is morally

wrong (ranged between 89.8% and 90.3%). Of the total sample, 0.6% of athletes felt

that performance-enhancing substances use is morally ok under any circumstances.

4.6.2 Emotions experienced if caught using performance-enhancing substances

Athletes were presented with the emotions listed in Table 4.20 and asked: “If you were

caught using banned performance-enhancing substances, to what extent would you

experience the following feelings?” The response categories and results are shown in

Table 4.20.

In each competition level, the three moral emotions had the highest ‘to a great extent’

responses (i.e., rating 5): ‘ashamed’ (82.3% to 84.3%); ‘embarrassed’ (82.0% to

83.4%); and ‘guilty’ (75.8% to 76.9%). The other emotion that was nominated by a

majority of athletes was ‘devastated’ (‘to a great extent’: 72.7% to 80.6%). Responses

were mixed for ‘angry’, ‘resentful’ and ‘unlucky’. For both ‘angry’ and ‘resentful’, the

distributions of responses were skewed towards ‘to a great extent’, while responses for

‘unlucky’ were skewed towards ‘not at all’. Athletes were not inclined to feel ‘relieved’

if caught (‘not at all’: 56.4% to 58.6%).

Page 135: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

118

Table 4.20: Emotions experienced if caught using performance-enhancing substances

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Ashamed:

To a great extent: 5 83.0 84.3 82.3 83.4

4 10.5 11.1 16.1 11.0

3 3.0 2.6 0.0 2.7

2 1.1 0.2 1.6 0.8

Not at all: 1 1.1 0.9 0.0 1.0

No response 1.3 0.9 0.0 1.1

Embarrassed:

To a great extent: 5 82.0 83.4 82.3 82.5

4 11.7 10.2 12.9 11.3

3 3.3 3.7 3.2 3.4

2 0.7 0.9 1.6 0.8

Not at all: 1 0.8 0.4 0.0 0.7

No response 1.4 1.3 0.0 1.3

Guilty:

To a great extent: 5 76.1 76.9 75.8 76.4

4 12.9 15.5 16.1 14.0

3 5.1 4.8 6.5 5.0

2 1.7 1.3 0.0 1.5

Not at all: 1 2.7 0.4 1.6 1.9

No response 1.6 1.1 0.0 1.3

Devastated:

To a great extent: 5 72.7 77.1 80.6 74.7

4 11.7 11.3 6.5 11.3

3 8.2 5.2 4.8 7.0

2 2.5 3.1 1.6 2.7

Not at all: 1 3.3 1.5 4.8 2.7

No response 1.6 1.7 1.6 1.6

Angry:

To a great extent: 5 31.5 35.3 43.5 33.6

4 17.7 23.5 22.6 20.2

3 22.9 22.4 19.4 22.4

2 12.0 9.8 3.2 10.8

Not at all: 1 13.6 7.0 9.7 10.9

No response 2.3 2.0 1.6 2.1

Page 136: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

119

Table 4.20: Emotions experienced if caught using performance-

enhancing substances (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Resentful:

To a great extent: 5 26.2 33.8 37.1 29.6

4 11.5 19.2 16.1 14.6

3 23.1 18.1 21.0 21.0

2 16.5 11.8 6.5 14.2

Not at all: 1 19.5 13.7 16.1 17.2

No response 3.3 3.5 3.2 3.3

Unlucky:

To a great extent: 5 13.9 14.2 9.7 13.7

4 8.9 11.8 6.5 10.0

3 27.0 26.1 24.2 26.4

2 21.5 21.6 24.2 21.7

Not at all: 1 26.4 24.4 35.5 26.2

No response 2.3 2.0 0.0 2.0

Relieved:

To a great extent: 5 5.5 7.6 9.7 6.5

4 2.3 4.1 0.0 2.8

3 14.4 13.1 21.0 14.3

2 16.3 16.6 12.9 16.3

Not at all: 1 58.6 56.4 56.5 57.5

No response 3.0 2.2 0.0 2.5

4.7 Perceived Reference Groups’ Moral Stance on Banned

Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

Athletes’ perceptions of their reference groups’ moral judgment towards banned

performance-enhancing substances use were measured using the same question format

as for personal morality. The results for people involved in the athlete’s life and people

involved in sports in general are shown separately in Tables 4.21 and 4.22. For people

involved in the athlete’s life, the response categories included ‘not applicable’.

Page 137: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

120

Tables 4.21 shows that the vast majority of athletes in each competition level perceived

that people involved in their lives would consider performance-enhancing substances

use ‘morally wrong under any circumstances’: ‘family/partner’ (90.7% to 94.9%);

‘coach/trainer’ (86.9% to 94.9%); ‘close friends’ (82.4% to 84.2%); ‘team

mates/training partner’ (79.3% to 89.5%); ‘sports doctor’ (86.5% to 90.4%); ‘sports

psychologist’ (89.3% to 93.8%); and ‘lawyer’ (84.4% to 91.7%). [These proportions

exclude the ‘not applicable’ responses.]

Table 4.21: Perceived moral stance on banned performance-enhancing substances

use among people involved in the athlete’s life

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Family/partner:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 88.3 87.4 90.3 88.0

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

5.5 4.8 1.6 5.0

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.6 0.9 1.6 0.7

Not applicable 3.0 3.7 4.8 3.4

No response 2.7 3.3 1.6 2.8

Coach/trainer:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 83.3 87.4 90.3 85.2

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

8.8 5.0 1.6 7.0

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.8 0.9 1.6 0.9

Not applicable 4.1 3.7 4.8 4.0

No response 3.0 3.1 1.6 2.9

Close friends:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 79.1 78.4 77.4 78.7

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

12.3 12.2 9.7 12.1

Morally OK under any circumstances 1.3 1.1 1.6 1.2

Not applicable 5.0 4.8 8.1 5.1

No response 2.4 3.5 3.2 2.8

Page 138: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

121

Table 4.21: Perceived moral stance on banned performance-enhancing substances

use among people involved in the athlete’s life (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Team mates/training partner:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 76.8 79.5 82.3 78.1

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

16.0 13.3 6.5 14.5

Morally OK under any circumstances 1.6 0.9 1.6 1.3

Not applicable 3.1 3.3 8.1 3.5

No response 2.5 3.1 1.6 2.7

Sports doctor:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 75.2 80.6 75.8 77.1

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

8.3 6.5 4.8 7.4

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.3 0.7 1.6 0.5

Not applicable 13.0 8.3 16.1 11.6

No response 3.1 3.9 1.6 3.3

Sports psychologist:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 70.0 72.8 72.6 71.1

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

3.3 3.9 1.6 3.4

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.0 0.7 1.6 0.3

Not applicable 23.2 18.5 22.6 21.6

No response 3.5 4.1 1.6 3.6

Your lawyer:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 41.4 47.5 53.2 44.3

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

3.0 3.1 3.2 3.0

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.4 0.7 0.0 0.5

Not applicable 50.9 44.2 41.9 48.0

No response 4.2 4.6 1.6 4.2

Tables 4.22 shows that the vast majority of athletes in each competition level perceived

that people involved in sports in general would consider performance-enhancing

substances use ‘morally wrong under any circumstances’: ‘most spectators’ (74.2% to

81.3%); ‘most of the general public’ (71.0% to 80.9%); ‘sports journalists in general’

Page 139: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

122

(77.4% to 80.2%); ‘most other athletes’ (70.0% to 76.5%); and ‘sports lawyers in

general’ (66.5% to 74.2%). These proportions were somewhat lower than for people

involved in the athlete’s life.

For each of the reference groups, 1.8% of athletes or less perceived the group to view

performance-enhancing substances use as ‘morally OK under any circumstances’.

Table 4.22: Perceived moral stance on banned performance-enhancing substances

use among people involved in sports in general

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Most spectators:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 81.3 80.8 74.2 80.7

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

14.3 13.7 21.0 14.5

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.6 1.1 0.0 0.7

No response 3.8 4.4 4.8 4.1

Most of the general public:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 80.9 78.2 71.0 79.3

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

15.1 16.8 24.2 16.2

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.3 0.9 0.0 0.5

No response 3.7 4.1 4.8 4.0

Sports journalists in general:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 77.4 80.2 77.4 78.4

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

16.1 13.3 12.9 14.9

Morally OK under any circumstances 0.8 1.5 3.2 1.2

No response 5.7 5.0 6.5 5.5

Page 140: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

123

Table 4.22: Perceived moral stance on banned performance-enhancing substances

use among people involved in sports in general (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Most other athletes:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 70.0 76.5 74.2 72.8

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

24.8 18.1 19.4 21.8

Morally OK under any circumstances 1.4 1.3 1.6 1.4

No response 3.8 4.1 4.8 4.0

Sports lawyers in general:

Morally wrong under any circumstances 66.5 71.5 74.2 68.8

Morally OK under some circumstances,

but wrong under others

24.2 20.5 14.5 22.2

Morally OK under any circumstances 1.8 1.3 4.8 1.8

No response 7.5 6.8 6.5 7.2

4.8 Legitimacy

Perceptions of legitimacy in terms of the testing and appeals processes were measured

on three dimensions of justice: (1) distributive justice; (2) procedural justice; and (3)

interactional justice.

4.8.1 Distributive justice

Distributive justice (i.e., perceived fairness of the drug testing process) was measured in

terms of: (1) equitable treatment of all athletes by the enforcement agency: “How fair is

the Australian Sports Drug Agency in terms of treating all athletes equally?”; (2)

security of the testing procedures: “How secure is the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s

drug testing procedures in Australia? (That is, in taking of samples and care of

Page 141: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

124

samples)”; and (3) accuracy of the current drug tests: “How accurate do you feel the

current drug tests are in terms of being able to correctly identify these substances:

anabolic steroids; beta-blockers; designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG);

erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances; human growth hormones (hGH);

and diuretics?” The response categories and results are shown in Tables 4.23 to 4.25

respectively.

Table 4.23 shows that the vast majority of athletes believe that the Australian Sports

Drug Agency is fair in treating all athletes equally (89.9% of the total sample; ‘very’

fair: 41.0%).

Table 4.23: Perceived fairness of the Australian Sports Drug Agency

in treating all athletes equally

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Very fair 43.0 38.3 38.7 41.0

Fair 45.8 52.7 54.8 48.9

Unfair 6.5 4.8 3.2 5.7

Very unfair 2.3 1.3 0.0 1.8

No response 2.4 2.8 3.2 2.6

There was an almost universal perception that the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s

drug testing procedures in Australia are secure (95.3% of the total sample; ‘very’

secure: 51.7%) (Table 4.24).

Page 142: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

125

Table 4.24: Perceived security of the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s

drug testing procedures in Australia

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Very secure 56.4 46.8 35.5 51.7

Quite secure 39.9 46.6 59.7 43.6

Not really secure 1.1 3.1 1.6 1.9

Not at all secure 0.1 0.0 0.0 0.1

No response 2.4 3.5 3.2 2.8

Table 4.25 shows that for each of the six banned performance-enhancing substances, a

substantial minority of athletes ‘don’t know’ whether or not the drug test is accurate in

correctly identifying the substance: ‘anabolic steroids’ (31.2%); ‘human growth

hormones (hGH)’ (45.7%); ‘designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)

(46.2%)’; ‘erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances’ (45.7%); ‘beta-blockers’

(47.5%); and ‘diuretics’ (39.0%). For each substance, 7.6% of athletes or less believed

that the drug test is inaccurate.

Table 4.25: Perceived accuracy of the current drug tests

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Anabolic steroids:

Very accurate 35.4 35.1 40.3 35.3

Quite accurate 28.0 24.6 35.5 27.1

A little accurate 4.1 3.1 0.0 3.6

Not accurate 1.6 0.7 0.0 1.1

Not at all accurate 0.4 0.2 0.0 0.3

Don’t know 29.3 35.3 19.4 31.2

No response 1.3 1.1 4.8 1.4

Page 143: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

126

Table 4.25: Perceived accuracy of the current drug tests (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Human growth hormones (hGH):

Very accurate 19.9 20.9 19.4 20.2

Quite accurate 18.8 18.7 32.3 19.5

A little accurate 6.5 5.0 1.6 5.7

Not accurate 3.7 2.0 1.6 2.9

Not at all accurate 6.4 2.6 1.6 4.7

Don’t know 43.4 49.7 38.7 45.7

No response 1.3 1.1 4.8 1.4

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG):

Very accurate 18.7 20.5 19.4 19.3

Quite accurate 22.3 17.2 30.6 20.8

A little accurate 8.2 6.3 6.5 7.4

Not accurate 2.7 2.2 0.0 2.3

Not at all accurate 3.5 1.3 0.0 2.5

Don’t know 43.3 51.4 38.7 46.2

No response 1.3 1.1 4.8 1.4

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances:

Very accurate 19.8 19.0 21.0 19.5

Quite accurate 18.8 19.0 27.4 19.2

A little accurate 11.3 5.4 3.2 8.7

Not accurate 4.2 2.6 3.2 3.6

Not at all accurate 2.7 0.9 0.0 1.9

Don’t know 41.9 51.9 40.3 45.7

No response 1.3 1.3 4.8 1.5

Beta-blockers:

Very accurate 25.7 25.5 22.6 25.4

Quite accurate 22.1 19.2 24.2 21.0

A little accurate 4.1 2.6 4.8 3.6

Not accurate 0.6 0.9 0.0 0.6

Not at all accurate 0.3 0.2 0.0 0.2

Don’t know 45.5 50.3 43.5 47.5

No response 1.7 1.3 4.8 1.7

Page 144: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

127

Table 4.25: Perceived accuracy of the current drug tests (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Diuretics:

Very accurate 29.7 27.2 24.2 28.4

Quite accurate 26.6 22.4 32.3 25.2

A little accurate 5.5 3.7 3.2 4.8

Not accurate 0.7 1.1 1.6 0.9

Not at all accurate 0.3 0.4 0.0 0.3

Don’t know 35.9 44.0 33.9 39.0

No response 1.3 1.1 4.8 1.4

4.8.2 Procedural justice

Procedural justice (i.e., perceived fairness of the appeals process) was measured in

terms of: (1) level of satisfaction in receiving a fair hearing in appealing a positive test:

“How satisfied are you that athletes who appeal a positive test in Australia will be given

a fair hearing?”; (2) level of satisfaction in receiving a fair hearing prior to decision on

imposing sanctions: “How satisfied are you that athletes who test positive in your sport

will be given a fair hearing before a decision is made about applying a penalty?”; and

(3) level of satisfaction in receiving a fair hearing in the Court of Arbitration in Sport:

“How satisfied are you that athletes who appeal a positive test before the Court of

Arbitration in Sport, will be given a fair hearing?” The response categories and results

are shown in Table 4.26.

For each of the three appeal scenarios, the levels of satisfaction of a fair hearing were

high: 87.8% to 91.5% of the total sample (‘very’ satisfied: 22.6% to 25.3%). The

distributions of responses for all three measures were very similar by level of

competition.

Page 145: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

128

Table 4.26: Perceived fairness of the appeals process

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Fair hearing in appealing

a positive test:

Very satisfied 22.5 22.9 22.6 22.6

Somewhat satisfied 67.0 66.0 69.4 66.9

Somewhat dissatisfied 6.8 7.8 4.8 7.1

Very dissatisfied 1.7 2.2 1.6 1.9

No response 2.0 1.1 1.6 1.6

Fair hearing before a

decision is made about

applying a penalty:

Very satisfied 25.3 24.2 32.3 25.3

Somewhat satisfied 62.1 64.1 56.5 62.5

Somewhat dissatisfied 8.3 7.6 8.1 8.0

Very dissatisfied 2.1 3.1 0.0 2.4

No response 2.1 1.1 3.2 1.8

Fair hearing in the Court

of Arbitration in Sport

Very satisfied 25.3 23.5 22.6 24.5

Somewhat satisfied 66.8 67.3 67.7 67.0

Somewhat dissatisfied 3.0 5.7 6.5 4.1

Very dissatisfied 1.4 1.5 0.0 1.4

No response 3.5 2.0 3.2 3.0

4.8.3 Interactional justice

Interactional justice was measured in terms of interpersonal interactions with drug

testing personnel. Athletes who had been tested were asked: “Did you find the

experience of being tested traumatic or upsetting in any way?”, and “How would you

describe the conduct of the testing personnel: (a) courteous or rude or neither; (b)

helpful or unhelpful or neither; (c) friendly or unfriendly or neither; (d) sensitive or

insensitive or neither?” These results are shown in Table 4.27.

Page 146: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

129

Table 4.27: Drug testing experiences among athletes who had been drug tested

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=555 N=183 N=16 N=759

% % % %

Testing process was traumatic or

upsetting:

Yes – very much 1.6 4.9 0.0 2.4

Yes – somewhat 13.7 15.3 6.3 13.8

No 83.1 77.0 81.3 81.7

No response 1.6 2.7 12.5 2.1

Conduct of the testing personnel:

Friendly 87.6 84.7 81.3 86.8

Unfriendly 1.3 2.7 0.0 1.6

Neither 9.4 8.7 12.5 9.2

No response 1.8 3.8 6.3 2.4

Courteous 85.9 80.9 62.5 84.2

Rude 0.9 3.8 0.0 1.6

Neither 11.4 12.6 31.3 12.1

No response 1.8 2.7 6.3 2.1

Helpful 84.7 81.4 75.0 83.8

Unhelpful 2.2 3.3 0.0 2.4

Neither 11.4 11.5 18.8 11.5

No response 1.8 3.8 6.3 2.4

Sensitive 62.5 62.8 37.5 62.2

Insensitive 2.7 2.2 6.3 2.6

Neither 33.0 30.6 50.0 32.7

No response 1.8 4.4 6.3 2.5

The majority of athletes did not find the experience traumatic or upsetting (81.7% of the

total sample). A small but substantial proportion of ‘Olympics/World’ and ‘National’

athletes found the experience traumatic or upsetting: 15.3% and 20.2% respectively. A

high proportion of athletes felt that the testing personnel were friendly (86.8%),

courteous (84.2%) and helpful (83.8%). The proportion of athletes who felt that the

testing personnel were sensitive was substantially lower at 62.2%, mainly due to 32.7%

Page 147: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

130

responding ‘neither’ sensitive or insensitive. Less than 3% of athletes found the testing

personnel unfriendly, rude, unhelpful or insensitive.

4.9 Personality

Athletes were presented with the personality items in Table 4.28, and asked to rate the

extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement. The response categories

and results are shown in Table 4.28.

Most athletes believed that they have self-control (85.9% agreement), are assertive

(‘Usually, if I have something to say, I say it’: 66.6% agreement), and have high self-

esteem with respect to their likeability and appearance (‘Most people my age are better

liked than me’: 72.8% disagreement; ‘I like my physical appearance’: 63.0%

agreement).

Athletes were far more likely to be risk takers than risk avoiders; that is, athletes were more

likely to agree that ‘I am the kind of person who enjoys risk’ than disagree: 52.1% vs

17.6%; and to disagree that ‘I am the kind of person who avoids risk’ than agree: 55.8%

vs 18.6%.

The results for temperament were mixed; that is, athletes were far more likely to agree than

disagree that ‘I am even tempered’ (61.6% vs 13.3%), but they were more likely to agree

than disagree that ‘I tend to be aggressive’ (45.8% vs 29.5%).

The responses were mixed for the statement ‘I am ready to win at all costs’, with 33.1%

agreement and 37.6% disagreement. The proportion who disagreed with this statement

Page 148: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

131

increased with higher levels of competition from 21.0% in ‘State/Regional’ to 30.7% in

‘National’ to 43.7% in ‘Olympics/World’ athletes. The distributions of responses for all

the other personality measures were very similar by level of competition.

Table 4.28: Personality characteristics

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

I have self-control:

Strongly agree 29.8 24.4 30.6 27.8

Agree 56.3 60.3 61.3 58.1

No feelings/mixed feelings 9.3 10.0 1.6 9.2

Disagree 2.4 3.3 0.0 2.6

Strongly disagree 0.3 0.7 0.0 0.4

No response 1.8 1.3 6.5 1.9

Usually, if I have something to say,

I say it:

Strongly agree 19.7 16.6 6.5 17.9

Agree 47.9 49.5 51.6 48.7

No feelings/mixed feelings 18.8 20.5 17.7 19.3

Disagree 11.2 11.1 17.7 11.4

Strongly disagree 0.6 0.7 1.6 0.6

No response 1.8 1.7 4.8 1.9

Most people my age are better liked

than me:

Strongly agree 1.3 0.9 0.0 1.1

Agree 5.8 3.7 3.2 4.9

No feelings/mixed feelings 17.7 18.5 35.5 19.0

Disagree 50.5 56.6 48.4 52.7

Strongly disagree 22.8 18.1 6.5 20.1

No response 2.0 2.2 6.5 2.3

I like my physical appearance:

Strongly agree 13.3 10.0 14.5 12.1

Agree 49.2 54.5 41.9 50.9

No feelings/mixed feelings 24.6 24.0 27.4 24.4

Disagree 9.5 7.6 9.7 8.8

Strongly disagree 1.6 2.4 1.6 1.9

No response 1.8 1.5 4.8 1.9

Page 149: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

132

Table 4.28: Personality characteristics (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

I am the kind of person who enjoys

risk:

Strongly agree 9.8 8.1 9.7 9.2

Agree 42.6 43.8 43.5 42.9

No feelings/mixed feelings 26.6 32.0 19.4 28.3

Disagree 17.1 13.7 21.0 16.1

Strongly disagree 1.8 0.9 1.6 1.5

No response 2.1 1.5 4.8 2.0

I am the kind of person who avoids

risk:

Strongly agree 2.4 1.5 0.0 1.9

Agree 17.8 15.3 12.9 16.7

No feelings/mixed feelings 21.4 27.5 25.8 23.8

Disagree 46.4 44.9 41.9 45.8

Strongly disagree 10.3 9.4 12.9 10.0

No response 1.7 1.5 6.5 1.9

I am even tempered:

Strongly agree 12.6 11.3 17.7 12.3

Agree 48.7 51.9 37.1 49.3

No feelings/mixed feelings 22.1 24.0 29.0 23.0

Disagree 13.3 9.8 9.7 12.0

Strongly disagree 1.1 1.7 0.0 1.3

No response 2.3 1.3 6.5 2.1

I tend to be aggressive:

Strongly agree 8.9 10.9 8.1 9.7

Agree 36.4 35.9 35.5 36.1

No feelings/mixed feelings 22.2 24.2 22.6 22.9

Disagree 23.5 22.7 21.0 23.0

Strongly disagree 7.5 5.0 6.5 6.5

No response 1.6 1.3 6.5 1.7

I am ready to ‘win at all costs’:

Strongly agree 6.1 9.6 17.7 7.9

Agree 23.6 27.0 24.2 25.2

No feelings/mixed feelings 24.0 31.4 32.3 27.0

Disagree 33.8 23.5 14.5 29.0

Strongly disagree 9.9 7.2 6.5 8.6

No response 2.5 1.3 4.8 2.2

Page 150: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

133

In relation to optimism, athletes were asked their reactions to a positive event (i.e.,

when they are performing well) and a negative event (i.e., when something goes wrong)

on the response scales in Tables 4.29 and 4.30 respectively. The results are shown in the

respective tables.

Table 4.29: Reactions to a positive event

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

When you are performing well,

do you tell yourself ....

It is mainly due to other people

or circumstances: 1

0.1

1.1

0.0

0.5

2 1.0 1.3 3.2 1.3

3 3.5 4.1 8.1 4.0

4 10.7 14.2 19.4 12.4

5 20.9 30.1 29.0 24.7

6 36.8 32.5 25.8 34.4

It is mainly due to me: 7 25.0 15.5 9.7 20.8

No response 1.8 1.3 4.8 1.8

This is the only thing I’m good at: 1 0.4 0.9 1.6 0.7

2 2.0 2.2 3.2 2.1

3 4.1 3.7 4.8 4.0

4 9.8 17.6 25.8 13.7

5 26.7 29.6 21.0 27.5

6 34.9 31.8 27.4 33.2

I can do anything: 7 20.2 12.9 11.3 17.0

No response 1.8 1.3 4.8 1.8

That it won’t happen again: 1 1.1 0.2 1.6 0.8

2 1.6 0.9 0.0 1.2

3 2.3 2.6 1.6 2.3

4 6.6 7.6 16.1 7.5

5 13.3 19.4 12.9 15.6

6 33.1 36.8 40.3 34.8

That it will happen again: 7 40.2 30.9 22.6 35.8

No response 1.8 1.5 4.8 1.9

Page 151: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

134

Table 4.30: Reactions to a negative event

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

When something goes wrong,

do you tell yourself ....

It is mainly due to other people

or circumstances: 1

0.1

0.4

0.0

0.2

2 1.4 0.4 1.6 1.1

3 3.4 4.6 4.8 3.9

4 19.9 23.3 30.6 21.7

5 26.4 32.7 22.6 28.6

6 28.6 23.3 32.3 26.8

It is mainly due to me: 7 18.2 13.5 3.2 15.8

No response 1.8 1.7 4.8 1.9

That everything will go wrong: 1 0.4 0.7 0.0 0.5

2 1.7 1.7 3.2 1.8

3 6.2 6.8 4.8 6.3

4 14.1 20.5 17.7 16.7

5 29.3 31.2 24.2 29.6

6 30.1 25.5 29.0 28.5

That it won’t affect other aspects

of my life: 7

16.1

11.3

16.1

14.4

No response 2.0 2.4 4.8 2.3

That it won’t happen again: 1 8.9 6.5 4.8 7.8

2 20.7 17.0 17.7 19.3

3 19.2 19.4 22.6 19.4

4 30.4 31.6 27.4 30.7

5 12.4 16.6 16.1 14.1

6 4.2 4.6 6.5 4.5

That it will happen again: 7 1.7 2.2 0.0 1.8

No response 2.4 2.2 4.8 2.4

It is clear that the vast majority of athletes were optimistic in their reactions to a positive

event, with the responses clearly skewed towards the ‘It is mainly due to me’, ‘I can do

anything’ and ‘That it will happen again’ response categories (mean ratings: 5.5, 5.4

and 5.9 respectively).

Page 152: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

135

Respondents’ reactions to a negative event were mixed with respect to categorising

athletes as optimists or pessimists. Athletes took responsibility for the event, with

responses skewed towards the ‘It is mainly due to me’ response category (mean rating:

5.2), which is considered to be a pessimistic reaction according to Seligman’s (1991)

optimism framework. However, there was evidence of optimism with responses skewed

towards ‘That it won’t affect other aspects of my of my life’ and ‘That it won’t happen

again’ (mean ratings: 5.2 and 3.5 respectively).

4.10 Market Factors

The two market factors assessed were the availability and affordability of performance-

enhancing substances.

4.10.1 Perceived availability of performance-enhancing substances

Athletes were presented with the performance-enhancing substances listed in Table 4.31

and asked: “How easy or difficult would it be to get each of the following types of

substances?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.31.

The substance perceived to be the most accessible was ‘diuretics’ (i.e., ‘very/fairly’ easy

to obtain: 33.3% vs 4.7% to 13.8% for each of the other substances). For ‘diuretics’,

47.3% of athletes responded ‘don’t know’. For each of the other substances, a majority

of athletes responded ‘don’t know’: ‘anabolic steroids’ (50.8%); ‘human growth

hormones (hGH)’ (55.5%); ‘designer steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)’

(57.0%); ‘erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances’ (57.1%); and ‘beta-

blockers’ (61.8%).

Page 153: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

136

Table 4.31: Perceived availability of six performance-enhancing substances

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Anabolic steroids:

Probably impossible 7.5 5.2 12.9 7.0

Very hard 11.0 14.6 17.7 12.7

Fairly hard 14.9 12.2 16.1 13.9

Fairly easy 10.7 10.2 8.1 10.4

Very easy 3.5 3.5 1.6 3.4

Don’t know 49.5 53.6 43.5 50.8

No response 2.8 0.7 0.0 1.9

Human growth hormones (hGH):

Probably impossible 12.4 9.4 24.2 11.8

Very hard 15.1 14.8 16.1 15.1

Fairly hard 8.2 9.2 1.6 8.2

Fairly easy 3.5 6.5 9.7 5.0

Very easy 2.3 1.7 1.6 2.0

Don’t know 55.4 56.9 46.8 55.5

No response 3.0 1.5 0.0 2.3

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG):

Probably impossible 13.7 9.4 22.6 12.4

Very hard 15.1 17.2 12.9 15.8

Fairly hard 7.6 9.2 3.2 8.0

Fairly easy 2.7 3.3 6.5 3.2

Very easy 1.4 1.7 1.6 1.5

Don’t know 56.6 58.2 53.2 57.0

No response 2.8 1.1 0.0 2.0

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances:

Probably impossible 13.3 8.9 22.6 12.0

Very hard 15.7 17.6 16.1 16.5

Fairly hard 7.5 8.1 1.6 7.4

Fairly easy 2.5 4.1 4.8 3.3

Very easy 1.6 1.5 1.6 1.5

Don’t know 56.6 58.4 53.2 57.1

No response 2.8 1.3 0.0 2.1

Page 154: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

137

Table 4.31: Perceived availability of six performance-enhancing substances (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Beta-blockers:

Probably impossible 7.1 5.7 11.3 6.8

Very hard 8.8 11.1 21.0 10.2

Fairly hard 9.8 9.6 3.2 9.4

Fairly easy 6.6 8.1 6.5 7.1

Very easy 2.7 2.4 1.6 2.5

Don’t know 62.0 62.1 56.5 61.8

No response 3.1 1.1 0.0 2.2

Diuretics:

Probably impossible 3.1 3.3 9.7 3.6

Very hard 4.8 7.8 12.9 6.3

Fairly hard 7.4 8.1 6.5 7.5

Fairly easy 19.9 16.8 11.3 18.4

Very easy 17.4 12.2 6.5 14.9

Don’t know 44.6 50.5 53.2 47.3

No response 2.8 1.3 0.0 2.1

4.10.2 Perceived affordability of performance-enhancing substances

Athletes were presented with the performance-enhancing substances listed in Table 4.32

and asked: “How expensive would it be for you personally to buy each of the following

types of substances?” The response categories and results are shown in Table 4.32.

The substance perceived to be the most affordable was ‘diuretics’ (i.e., ‘very/quite’

cheap: 13.2% vs 1.8% or less for each of the other substances). For each substance, the

proportion of ‘don’t know’ responses was higher than for accessibility: ‘anabolic

steroids’ (58.6%); ‘human growth hormones (hGH)’ (63.1%); ‘designer steroids like

tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)’ (63.9%); ‘erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar

substances’ (64.3%); ‘beta-blockers’ (69.8%); and ‘diuretics’ (58.9%).

Page 155: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

138

Table 4.32: Perceived affordability of six performance-enhancing substances

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Anabolic steroids:

Very cheap 0.3 0.2 0.0 0.2

Quite cheap 0.6 0.9 0.0 0.6

Neither 1.4 2.0 1.6 1.6

Quite expensive 10.7 11.8 14.5 11.3

Very expensive 25.2 25.3 32.3 25.6

Don’t know 59.0 59.0 51.6 58.6

No response 2.8 0.9 0.0 1.9

Human growth hormones (hGH):

Very cheap 0.3 0.4 0.0 0.3

Quite cheap 0.1 0.2 1.6 0.2

Neither 0.7 1.3 0.0 0.9

Quite expensive 5.5 5.4 9.7 5.7

Very expensive 27.3 27.2 37.1 27.8

Don’t know 63.4 64.3 51.6 63.1

No response 2.7 1.1 0.0 1.9

Designer steroids like

Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG):

Very cheap 0.3 0.4 0.0 0.3

Quite cheap 0.1 0.2 0.0 0.2

Neither 0.4 0.9 0.0 0.6

Quite expensive 5.2 5.2 8.1 5.4

Very expensive 28.0 27.0 27.4 27.6

Don’t know 63.2 65.1 64.5 63.9

No response 2.7 1.1 0.0 1.9

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other

similar substances:

Very cheap 0.4 0.4 0.0 0.4

Quite cheap 0.4 0.9 0.0 0.6

Neither 0.4 0.7 0.0 0.5

Quite expensive 5.2 5.4 6.5 5.4

Very expensive 27.4 25.3 32.3 26.9

Don’t know 63.4 66.2 61.3 64.3

No response 2.7 1.1 0.0 1.9

Page 156: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

139

Table 4.32: Perceived affordability of six performance-enhancing substances (Cont’d)

Olympics/

World

National

State/

Regional

Total

sample

N=707 N=459 N=62 N=1,237

% % % %

Beta-blockers:

Very cheap 0.4 0.4 0.0 0.4

Quite cheap 1.6 1.1 1.6 1.4

Neither 2.4 2.0 1.6 2.2

Quite expensive 8.1 8.9 14.5 8.6

Very expensive 16.3 14.8 11.3 15.4

Don’t know 68.3 71.7 71.0 69.8

No response 3.0 1.1 0.0 2.1

Diuretics:

Very cheap 3.5 2.2 3.2 3.0

Quite cheap 14.0 5.7 1.6 10.2

Neither 6.9 5.4 4.8 6.3

Quite expensive 6.8 10.0 12.9 8.4

Very expensive 10.5 12.0 14.5 11.2

Don’t know 55.3 63.6 62.9 58.9

No response 3.0 1.1 0.0 2.1

4.11 Summary of Findings

In this sample of mainly elite Australian athletes, 6.9% reported use of banned

performance-enhancing substances. Among those who were drug tested, 1.1% reported

testing positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance.

A majority of athletes would not give an offer of a banned performance-enhancing

substance any consideration at all (56.7% of the total sample) and felt that they

‘definitely’ don’t have to use banned performance-enhancing substances to perform at

the very highest levels (69.2% of the total sample).

Page 157: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

140

There was an almost universal perception that the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s

drug testing procedures in Australia are secure (95.3% of the total sample; ‘very’

secure: 51.7%). Athletes who had been drug tested felt that the testing personnel were

friendly (86.8%), courteous (85.2%) and helpful (83.8%). The proportion of athletes

who felt that the testing personnel were sensitive was substantially lower at 62.2%,

mainly due to 32.7% responding ‘neither’ sensitive or insensitive. Less than 3% of

athletes found the testing personnel unfriendly, rude, unhelpful or insensitive.

For each of the six substances, appraisal of the threat of ill-health effects of regular use

was high, with over 50% rating the health risk in the ‘top box’ (i.e., ‘a lot of harm’). Of

these substances, excluding ‘diuretics’, ‘anabolic steroids’ was perceived to be the most

beneficial in enhancing sport performance (‘definitely/probably would’: 57.9%),

followed by ‘human growth hormones (hGH)’ (47.7%) and ‘designer steroids like

tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)’ (41.4%).

Appraisal of the threat of enforcement is high. Consistent with the drug testing regime

of anti-doping organisations, the perceived likelihood of being tested in and out of

competition increases with higher levels of competition; for example, the proportion of

athletes who felt that they would be tested in competition increases from 67.7% in

‘State/Regional’ to 88.0% in ‘National’ to 94.0% in ‘Olympics/World’ athletes.

Athletes felt that they were likely to get caught if they were to use banned performance-

enhancing substances in competition (‘not (at all)’ likely to get away with it: 60.5%).

However, the responses were mixed for out of competition, with 32.1% of the total

sample responding ‘very/quite’ likely to getting away with it, 31.9% ‘a little’ likely, and

34.6% ‘not (at all)’ likely. The vast majority of athletes felt that the penalties for a

positive drug test in their sport were ‘very/fairly’ severe (79.9% of the total sample).

Page 158: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

141

Less than 8% of athletes believed that the drug test for each substance is inaccurate in

identifying the substance, with a substantial minority of athletes responding ‘don’t

know’ (ranged between 31.2% and 47.5%).

The main perceived outcome for performing well in sports was ‘personal best

achievements’ (offer ‘a lot’: 67.3% of the total sample), followed by ‘opportunities for

remaining in the sport as coach, trainer or administrator’ (39.7%). The proportions were

substantially lower for outcomes related to celebrity status and financial wealth/security

(ranged between 9.9% and 14.4%). With the exception of ‘personal best achievements’,

the perceived likelihood of each of the potential outcomes was lower with higher levels

of competition.

‘Personal best achievements’ was the most desired outcome (desire ‘a lot’: 87.1% of the

total sample). Financial outcomes were desired by the majority of athletes: ‘future

financial security’: 77.8%; and ‘lucrative sponsorship deals’: 69.7%. Approximately

half of the total sample desired ‘opportunities for remaining in the sport as coach,

trainer or administrator’ (51.8%). The desirability of national and international celebrity

status were substantially lower at 39.2% and 33.8% respectively.

The vast majority of athletes believe deliberately using banned performance-enhancing

substances to improve performance is morally wrong under any circumstances (89.1%

of the total sample), and believed that people involved in their lives hold this same

belief: ‘family/partner’ (91.1%); ‘coach/trainer’ (88.8%); ‘close friends’ (83.0%); ‘team

mates/training partner’ (80.9%); ‘sports doctor’ (87.3%); ‘sports psychologist’ (90.6%);

and ‘lawyer’ (85.2%).

Page 159: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

142

With respect to the six banned performance-enhancing substances presented to athletes,

‘diuretics’ was perceived to be the most accessible (‘very/fairly easy’ to obtain: 33.3%

vs 4.7% to 13.8%) and affordable (‘very/quite’ cheap: 13.2% vs 0.5% to 1.8%). A

substantial proportion of athletes reported that they do not know whether these

substances were accessible or affordable: 47.3% to 61.8% and 58.6% to 69.8%

respectively.

A majority of the athletes believed that they have self-control (85.9% agreement), are

assertive (‘Usually, if I have something to say, I say it’: 66.6% agreement), and have

high self-esteem with respect to their likeability and appearance (‘Most people my age

are better liked than me’: 72.8% disagreement; ‘I like my physical appearance’: 63.0%

agreement). Athletes were far more likely to be risk takers than risk avoiders; that is,

athletes were more likely to agree that ‘I am the kind of person who enjoys risk’ than

disagree: 52.1% vs 17.6%; and to disagree that ‘I am the kind of person who avoids

risk’ than agree: 55.8% vs 18.6%.

This chapter presented data on the sample characteristics and descriptive data on the

variables in the model. The next chapter presents the development of each of the

constructs for use in testing the Sport Drug Control model.

Page 160: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

143

CHAPTER 5: CONSTRUCT DEVELOPMENT

This chapter describes the factor analytic procedures used to select items to represent

each construct in later analyses. Assessments of the reliability of the scales are also

presented.

5.1 Threat Appraisal

Appraisal of threat of ill-health effects:

For the items measuring the perceived health risk of using each of the six banned

performance-enhancing substances assessed in this study, ‘don’t know’ responses were

recoded in a neutral position between ‘a little harm’ and ‘some harm’, and the responses

to the items were reverse scored. Hence, the response categories ranged from 1 (a lot of

harm) to 5 (no harm).

‘Threat to health: short-term use’:

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the health risk of ‘once or twice ever’ use

of the six banned performance-enhancing substances. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

=

5052.11, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO

= 0.89) indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable. One factor emerged with an

eigenvalue of greater than one (eigenvalue = 4.16) and accounted for 69% of the total

variance in the data. All six substances loaded significantly onto one factor only with

loadings of 0.68 and above (see Table 5.1). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.91.

Hence, one factor score was computed for each respondent by averaging responses to

the six substances. This composite variable ranged from 1 (a lot of harm) to 5 (no

harm).

Page 161: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

144

Table 5.1: Principal components and loadings on threat to health: short-term use

Component

1

Factor 1 (Threat to health: short-term use):

Anabolic steroids* .845

Beta blockers* .805

Designer steroids like Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)* .901

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances* .887

Human growth hormones (hGH)* .858

Diuretics* .678

* Responses were reverse scored.

‘Threat to health: regular use’:

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the health risk of ‘regular use’ of the six

banned performance-enhancing substances. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

= 3753.68,

p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.87)

indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable. One factor emerged with an

eigenvalue of greater than one (eigenvalue = 3.75) and accounted for 63% of the total

variance in the data. All six substances loaded significantly onto one factor only with

loadings of 0.69 and above (see Table 5.2). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.87.

Hence, one factor score was computed for each respondent by averaging responses to

the six substances. This composite variable ranged from 1 (a lot of harm) to 5 (no

harm).

Table 5.2: Principal components and loadings on threat to health: regular use

Component

1

Factor 1 (Threat to health: regular use):

Anabolic steroids* .725

Beta blockers* .760

Designer steroids like Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)* .870

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances* .849

Human growth hormones (hGH)* .834

Diuretics* .690

* Responses were reverse scored.

Page 162: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

145

Appraisal of threat of enforcement:

A deterrence variable was computed for in competition and out of competition,

separately, by manipulating responses to the following three measures: (1) perceived

likelihood of being drug tested at least once a year (in competition)/(out of

competition); (2) perceived likelihood of getting away with taking banned performance-

enhancing substances (in competition)/(out of competition); and (3) perceived severity

of penalties for a positive drug test.

High threat of detection was defined as ‘very/quite likely’ being drug tested at least

once a year (in competition)/(out of competition) and ‘a little likely/not (at all) likely’ of

getting away with taking banned performance-enhancing substances (in

competition)/(out of competition) and the penalties for a positive drug test seen as

‘very/fairly severe’.

Low threat of detection was defined as ‘a little likely/not (at all) likely’ of being drug

tested at least once a year (in competition)/(out of competition) and ‘very/quite likely’

of getting away with taking banned performance-enhancing substances (in

competition)/(out of competition) and the penalties for a positive drug test seen as

‘very/fairly lenient’.

All other combinations of responses to the three questions were grouped together in a

‘neutral’ category. Hence, the computed deterrence variable constituted a 3-point scale:

1 (high threat of detection); 2 (neutral); and 3 (low threat of detection).

Page 163: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

146

Modelling of threat appraisal:

Threat appraisal was modelled as a second-order factor, with two first order factors

being appraisal of threat of ill-health effects and threat of enforcement.

5.2 Benefit Appraisal

Rewards for performing well in sport:

A composite score was calculated by multiplying the perceived likelihood of each of the

outcomes for performing well in sport (i.e., national celebrity status; lucrative

sponsorship deals; personal best achievements; opportunities for remaining in the sport

as coach, trainer or administrator; future financial security; and international celebrity

status) by the rated desirability of the outcome. These scores were recoded into a 5-

point scale, where higher numbers represented greater rewards for performing well in

sport.

Perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on performance:

For the items measuring the perceived impact on performance of the five banned

performance-enhancing substances shown in Table 5.3, ‘don’t know’ responses were

recoded into the ‘might or might not’ category. Hence the response categories ranged

from 1 (definitely would not improve performance) to 5 (definitely would improve

performance).

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the five banned performance-enhancing

substances. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

= 3704.98, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-

Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.84) indicated that the

correlation matrix was factorable. One factor emerged with an eigenvalue of greater

Page 164: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

147

than one (eigenvalue = 3.32) and accounted for 66% of the total variance in the data. All

five substances loaded significantly onto one factor only with loadings of 0.50 and

above (see Table 5.3). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.87. Hence one factor score

was computed for each athlete by averaging responses to the five substances. This

composite variable ranged from 1 (definitely would not improve performance) to 5

(definitely would improve performance).

Table 5.3: Principal components and loadings on benefits of

performance-enhancing substances use on performance

Component

1

Factor 1 (Benefits appraisal):

Anabolic steroids .873

Beta blockers .500

Designer steroids like Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG) .921

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances .808

Human growth hormones (hGH) .896

5.3 Personal Morality

Moral judgment:

Overall personal moral judgement of banned performance-enhancing substances use

was represented by a single item measure. Response categories were: 1 – I believe

deliberately using performance-enhancing substances to improve performance is

morally wrong under any circumstances; 2 – I believe deliberately using banned

performance-enhancing substances to improve performance is OK under some

circumstances, but wrong under others; and 3 – I believe deliberately using banned

performance-enhancing substances to improve performance is OK under any

circumstances.

Page 165: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

148

Moral emotions:

Table 5.4 shows the eight emotions potentially experienced if caught using

performance-enhancing substances. The responses to all of these emotions were reverse

scored, with higher numbers representing lesser experience of the emotion.

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on these eight emotions. Bartlett’s Test of

Sphericity (x2

= 2304.68, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling

adequacy (KMO = 0.70) indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable. Three

factors emerged with an eigenvalue of one or more and accounted for 66% of the total

variance in the data (Factor 1: eigenvalue = 2.55, 32% of variance; Factor 2, eigenvalue

= 1.72, 22%; Factor 3: eigenvalue = 1.04, 13%) (see Table 5.4).

Table 5.4: Principal components and loadings of emotional items

Component

1 2 3

Factor 1 (Moral emotions):

Ashamed* .878 .052 -.048

Embarrassed* .853 .056 -.011

Guilty* .800 -.003 .081

Factor 2 (Anger):

Angry* .067 .859 .052

Resentful* .016 .760 .218

Factor 3:

Relieved* .093 -.029 .881

Unlucky* -.131 .348 .603

Other item:

Devastated* .497 .472 -.188

* Responses were reverse scored.

Page 166: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

149

The majority of emotions loaded significantly onto one factor only, with rotated

loadings of 0.60 and above. There was one exception: ‘Devastated’ had absolute

loadings of 0.497 on Factor 1 and 0.472 on Factor 2. Hence, this emotion was removed

to improve interpretation.

The eigenvalue associated with the third factor was just above one. Factor 3 was

characterised by two emotions: ‘relieved’ and ‘unlucky’. Since this factor had a low

Cronbach’s alpha of 0.38 it was not included in further analyses.

The two factor solution accounted for 54% of the total variance in the data. Factor 1

(moral emotions) was characterised by three emotions: ‘ashamed’, ‘embarrassed’ and

‘guilty’. Factor 2 (anger) was characterised by two emotions: ‘angry’ and ‘resentful’.

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.80 for ‘moral emotions’ and 0.66 for ‘anger’. The

reliability statistic for ‘anger’ was slightly below the criterion of 0.70 for adequate

internal reliability (Kline 1986). Given that ‘moral emotions’ fit with the personal

morality construct and ‘anger’ does not, only one factor score was computed for each

athlete by averaging responses to the three emotions associated with ‘moral emotions’.

This composite variable ranged from 1 (moral emotions experienced to a great extent)

to 5 (moral emotions not experienced at all).

5.4 Reference Group Opinion

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the responses to the five reference groups

shown in Table 5.5. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

= 1455.40, p < 0.001) and the

Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.74) indicated that the

correlation matrix was factorable. One factor emerged with an eigenvalue of greater

Page 167: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

150

than one (eigenvalue = 2.527) and accounted for 51% of the total variance in the data.

All five groups loaded significantly onto one factor only with loadings of 0.58 and

above (see Table 5.5). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.74. Hence, one factor score

was computed for each respondent by averaging responses to the five groups. This

composite variable (‘Reference group’) ranged from 1 (performance-enhancing

substances use is morally wrong under any circumstances) to 3 (performance-enhancing

substances use is morally OK under any circumstances).

Table 5.5: Principal components and loadings

on reference group opinion

Component

1

Factor 1 (Reference group):

Most other athletes .577

Most spectators .775

Most of the general public .746

Sports lawyers in general .742

Sports journalists in general .698

5.5 Legitimacy

Interactional justice was measured in terms of interpersonal interactions with drug

testing personnel. However, a substantial minority of respondents reported that they had

never been drug tested (39%). Given that complete data are required for structural

equation modelling, these items were not included in the modelling to avoid

substantially reducing the sample available for analysis.

Page 168: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

151

One component of procedural justice is the perceived accuracy of the drug tests in

correctly identifying banned performance-enhancing substances. For each of the six

banned performance-enhancing substances presented to athletes, a substantial minority

responded ‘don’t know’: ‘beta-blockers’ (47.5%); ‘human growth hormones (hGH)’

(45.7%); ‘erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances’ (45.7%); ‘designer

steroids like tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)’ (46.2%)’; ‘diuretics’ (39.0%); and ‘anabolic

steroids’ (31.2%). Hence, the perceived accuracy items were not included in the

structural equation modelling.

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the five items measuring facets of

distributive and procedural justice shown in Table 5.6. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

=

2400.69, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO

= 0.74) indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable. Two factors emerged with

an eigenvalue of one or more and accounted for 77% of the total variance in the data

(Factor 1: eigenvalue = 2.71, 54% of variance; Factor 2, eigenvalue = 1.13, 23%) (see

Table 5.6).

Factor 1 (Appeals process) was characterised by 3 items: ‘fair hearing on positive test’;

‘fair hearing in Court of Arbitration’; and ‘fair hearing before decision on penalty’.

Factor 2 (Testing process) was characterised by 2 items: ‘security of testing procedure’

and ‘equitable treatment of athletes’. Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.87 for ‘appeals

process’ and 0.60 for ‘testing process’. The reliability statistic for ‘testing process’ was

slightly below the criterion of 0.70 for adequate internal reliability (Kline 1986).

Legitimacy was modelled as a second-order factor, with two first order factors being

appeals and testing processes. Both of these composite variables were 4-point scales,

Page 169: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

152

where lower numbers represented greater perceived legitimacy of the appeals and

testing processes.

Table 5.6: Principal components and loadings of legitimacy items:

distributive and procedural justice

Component

1 2

Factor 1 (Appeals process):

Fair hearing on positive test .911 .149

Fair hearing in Court of Arbitration .884 .153

Fair hearing before decision on penalty .852 .156

Factor 2 (Testing process):

Security of testing procedure .120 .844

Equitable treatment of athletes .170 .822

5.6 Personality

Table 5.7 shows the nine items measuring personality. The responses with an asterisk

were reverse scored. Exploratory factor analysis was performed on these nine items.

Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

= 1485.16, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin

measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.61) indicated that the correlation matrix was

factorable. Three factors emerged with an eigenvalue of one or more and accounted for

55% of the total variance in the data (Factor 1: eigenvalue = 2.08, 23% of variance;

Factor 2, eigenvalue = 1.71, 19%; Factor 3: eigenvalue = 1.15, 13%) (see Table 5.7).

The majority of items loaded significantly onto one factor only, with rotated loadings of

0.60 and above. There was one exception: ‘Most people my age are better liked than me’

had absolute loadings of 0.419 on one factor. However, including this item on that

Page 170: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

153

factor slightly reduced the Cronbach’s alpha for that factor. Hence, this item was

removed to improve interpretation.

Table 5.7: Principal components and loadings of personality items

Component

1 2 3

Factor 1 (Self-control):

I have self-control .751 .031 .058

I am even tempered .665 .054 .258

I like my physical appearance .623 -.041 -.327

Factor 2 (Risk taking propensity):

I am the kind of person who avoids risk .007 .901 .037

I am the kind of person who enjoys risk* .007 .833 .220

Factor 3 (Assertiveness):

I tend to be aggressive* .276 .149 .723

I am ready to ‘win at all costs’* .031 .011 .706

Usually, if I have something to say, I say it* -.163 .124 .601

Other item:

Most people my age are better liked than me* .419 -.344 -.015

* Responses were reverse scored.

Factor 1 (self-control) was characterised by 3 items: ‘I have self-control’; ‘I am even

tempered’; and ‘I like my physical appearance’. Factor 2 (risk taking propensity) was

characterised by 2 items: ‘I am the kind of person who avoids risk’; and ‘I am the kind of

person who enjoys risk’. Factor 3 (assertiveness) was characterised by 3 items: ‘I tend to

be aggressive’; ‘I am ready to win at all costs’; and ‘Usually, if I have something to say, I

say it’. Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.49 for ‘self-control’, 0.75 for ‘risk taking

propensity’ and 0.51 for ‘assertiveness’. The reliability statistics for ‘self-control’ and

‘assertiveness’ were substantially below the criterion of 0.70 for demonstrating

adequate internal reliability (Kline 1986). Hence, only one factor score was computed

Page 171: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

154

for each athlete by averaging responses to the two items associated with ‘risk taking

propensity’. This composite variable ranged from 1 (risk adverse) to 5 (risk seeking).

Optimism:

Table 5.8 shows the six items measuring optimism. The responses with an asterisk were

reverse scored. Exploratory factor analysis was performed on these six items. Bartlett’s

Test of Sphericity (x2

= 639.31, p < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of

sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.68) indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable.

Two factors emerged with an eigenvalue of one or more and accounted for 51% of the

total variance in the data (Factor 1: eigenvalue = 1.96, 33% of variance; Factor 2,

eigenvalue = 1.09, 18%).

All six items loaded significantly onto one factor only with loadings of 0.61 and above

(see Table 5.8). Factor 1 (Responses to positive event) was characterised by four items:

three relating to responses to a positive event (i.e., ‘internal’, ‘global’ and ‘stable’) and

one relating to responses to a negative event (i.e., ‘global’). Given that the item

‘response to a negative event: global’ was not congruent to theorising on responses to a

positive event, this item was removed to improve interpretation. Factor 2 (Responses to

negative event) was characterised by two items relating to responses to a negative event

(i.e., ‘internal’ and ‘stable’).

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.60 for ‘Responses to positive event’ and 0.10 for

‘Responses to negative event’. The reliability statistic for ‘Responses to negative event’

was substantially below the criterion of 0.70 for demonstrating adequate internal

reliability (Kline 1986). The reliability statistic for ‘Responses to positive event’ was

slightly below this criterion. Averaging responses to the three ‘Responses to negative

Page 172: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

155

event’ items revealed that there was little variation in the data with 89% of response

values ranging between 1 to 3 on the 7-point scale (response values 5 to 7: 2%). Hence,

these optimism items were not included in further analyses.

Table 5.8: Principal components and loadings of optimism items

Component

1 2

Factor 1 (Responses to positive event):

Response to positive event: Internal* .660 -.316

Response to positive event: Global* .696 -.270

Response to positive event: Stable* .658 .065

Response to negative event: Global* .612 .173

Factor 2 (Responses to negative event):

Response to negative event: Internal -.323 .677

Response to negative event: Stable .323 .725

* Responses were reverse scored.

5.7 Availability of Performance-Enhancing Substances

For the items measuring availability of the six banned performance-enhancing

substances shown in Table 5.9, the response categories ‘probably impossible’ and ‘very

hard’ to buy were combined, and ‘don’t know’ responses were recoded in a neutral

position between ‘fairly hard’ and ‘fairly easy’ to buy. Hence, the response categories

ranged from 1 (probably impossible/very hard to buy) to 5 (very easy to buy).

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the availability of the six banned

performance-enhancing substances. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

= 6362.08, p <

0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.85)

indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable. One factor emerged with an

eigenvalue of greater than one (eigenvalue = 4.13) and accounted for 69% of the total

Page 173: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

156

variance in the data. All six substances loaded significantly onto one factor only with

loadings of 0.46 and above (see Table 5.9). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.90 for

‘availability’. Hence, one factor score was computed for each athlete by averaging

responses to the six substances. This composite variable ranged from 1 (very hard to

buy) to 5 (very easy to buy).

Table 5.9: Principal components and loadings on availability

Component

1

Factor 1 (Availability):

Anabolic steroids .867

Beta blockers .821

Designer steroids like Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG) .921

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances .911

Human growth hormones (hGH) .905

Diuretics .458

5.8 Affordability of Performance-Enhancing Substances

For the items measuring affordability of the six banned performance-enhancing

substances shown in Table 5.10, ‘don’t know’ responses were recoded into the ‘neither

cheap nor expensive’ category and the responses to the items were reverse scored.

Hence, the response categories ranged from 1 (very expensive) to 5 (very cheap).

Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the affordability of the six banned

performance-enhancing substances. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (x2

= 7816.49, p <

0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.89)

indicated that the correlation matrix was factorable. One factor emerged with an

eigenvalue of greater than one (eigenvalue = 4.47) and accounted for 74% of the total

variance in the data. All six substances loaded significantly onto one factor only with

Page 174: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

157

loadings of 0.60 and above (see Table 5.10). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.93.

Hence, one factor score was computed for each athlete by averaging responses to the six

substances. This composite variable ranged from 1 (very expensive to buy) to 5 (very

cheap to buy).

Table 5.10: Principal components and loadings on affordability

Component

1

Factor 1 (Affordability):

Anabolic steroids* .893

Beta blockers* .835

Designer steroids like Tetrahydrogestrinone (THG)* .939

Erythropoietin (EPO) and other similar substances* .938

Human growth hormones (hGH)* .929

Diuretics* .597

* Responses were reverse scored.

5.9 Attitude Towards Performance-Enhancing Substances Use

Two single item measures were used to represent attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use: (1) the amount of consideration the athlete would give to an

offer of a banned performance-enhancing substance – reverse scored – with the

response categories 1 (none at all) to 4 (a lot of consideration); and (2) the perceived

necessity for athletes to use banned performance-enhancing substances – reverse scored

– with the response categories 1 (definitely don’t have to use performance-enhancing

substances at some time) to 4 (definitely have to use performance-enhancing substances

at some time).

Page 175: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

158

5.10 Performance-Enhancing Substances Use Behaviour

A behaviour variable was computed by manipulating responses to the following three

measures: (1) self-reported use of banned performance-enhancing substances in general;

(2) self-reported past and current use of specific banned performance-enhancing

substances; and (3) tested positive for a banned performance-enhancing substance.

Substance use was defined as self-reported use of performance-enhancing substances

‘briefly’, ‘occasionally’ or ‘regularly’ or self-reported past or current use of specific

performance-enhancing substances or methods or self-reported positive test for a

performance-enhancing substance. The computed behaviour variable was therefore a

dichotomous scale: 0 (self-reported never use of a performance-enhancing substance),

and 1 (self-reported ever use of a performance-enhancing substance).

5.11 Summary of the Constructs

Table 5.11 presents the descriptive statistics (means and 95% confidence intervals;

standard deviations) and reliability estimates (Cronbach’s alpha) for all the variables

measuring each of the constructs in the Donovan et al. (2002) model. These variables

are used in the structural equation modelling analyses to test the model. The results of

which are presented in the next chapter.

Page 176: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

159

Table 5.11: Descriptive statistics and internal reliability estimates for the variables measuring

the constructs in the Sport Drug Control model (N = 1,237)

Variables

Mean

(95% CI)

Standard

deviation

Cronbach’s

alpha

Response scales

Use of banned performance-enhancing

substances

.08

(.06-.10)

.27 --- (0) Self-reported never use of a performance-

enhancing substance to (1) self-reported ever use

of a performance-enhancing substance

Attitude towards performance-enhancing

substances use

Need to use banned performance-enhancing

substances to perform at the very highest level

1.52

(1.47-1.57)

.94 --- (1) Definitely don’t have to use performance-

enhancing substances at some time to

(4) definitely have to use performance-enhancing

substances at some time

Consideration of an offer to use performance-

enhancing substances

1.68

(1.63-1.73)

.93 --- (1) None at all to (4) a lot of consideration

Affordability of performance-enhancing

substances

2.47

(2.43-2.51)

.79 .93 (1) Very hard to buy to (5) very easy to buy

Availability of performance-enhancing

substances

2.64

(2.59-2.69)

.81 .90 (1) Probably impossible/very hard to buy to (5)

very easy to buy

Benefit appraisal

Impact of performance-enhancing substances on

performance

3.23

(3.17-3.29)

1.04 .87 (1) Definitely would not improve performance to

(5) definitely would improve performance

Rewards for performing well 3.79

(3.75-3.83)

.76 --- (1) Little rewards to (5) a lot of rewards

Page 177: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

160

Table 5.11: Descriptive statistics and internal reliability estimates for the variables measuring

the constructs in the Sport Drug Control model (N = 1,237) (Cont’d)

Variables

Mean

(95% CI)

Standard

deviation

Cronbach’s

alpha

Response scales

Threat appraisal

Threat to health:

Use of performance-enhancing substances once or

twice

2.34

(2.28-2.40)

1.03 .91 (1) A lot of harm to (5) no harm

Regular use of performance-enhancing substances 1.55

(1.51-1.59)

.75 .87 (1) A lot of harm to (5) no harm

Appraisal of threat of enforcement:

Deterrence in competition 1.44

(1.41-1.47)

.53 --- (1) High threat of detection to (3) low threat of

detection

Deterrence out of competition 1.63

(1.60-1.66)

.56 --- (1) High threat of detection to (3) low threat of

detection

Personal morality

Moral judgment on performance-enhancing

substances use

1.11

(1.09-1.13)

.32 --- (1) Performance-enhancing substances use is

morally wrong under any circumstances to (3)

performance-enhancing substances use is morally

OK under any circumstances

Moral emotions 1.06

(1.04-1.08)

.29 .80 (1) Moral emotions experienced to a great extent

to (5) moral emotions not experienced at all

Page 178: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

161

Table 5.11: Descriptive statistics and internal reliability estimates for the variables measuring

the constructs in the Sport Drug Control model (N = 1,237) (Cont’d)

Variables

Mean

(95% CI)

Standard

deviation

Cronbach’s

alpha

Response scales

Reference group moral judgment on

performance-enhancing substances use

1.16

(1.14-1.18)

.38 .74 (1) Performance-enhancing substances use is

morally wrong under any circumstances to (3)

performance-enhancing substances use is morally

OK under any circumstances

Legitimacy

Testing process:

Security of testing procedure 1.48

(1.45-1.51)

.54 --- (1) Very secure to (4) not at all secure

Equitable treatment of athletes 1.68

(1.64-1.72)

.66 --- (1) Very fair to (4) very unfair

Appeals process:

Fair hearing for positive test appeal 1.88

(1.84-1.92)

.60 --- (1) Very satisfied to (4) very dissatisfied

Fair hearing before decision on sanctions 1.88

(1.85-1.91)

.64 --- (1) Very satisfied to (4) very dissatisfied

Fair hearing in Court of Arbitration 1.82

(1.79-1.85)

.56 --- (1) Very satisfied to (4) very dissatisfied

Personality

Risk taking propensity 3.60

(3.55-3.65)

.85 .75 (1) Risk adverse to (5) risk seeking

Page 179: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

162

CHAPTER 6: RESULTS OF THE STRUCTURAL

EQUATION MODELLING ANALYSES

Overall, the model shown in Figure 6.1 is an acceptable fit of the data. Although a

statistically significant Bollen-Stine index of fit was observed (x2 = 653.6, df = 162,

p < .001), all other indices of fit were satisfactory. The RMSEA was .050 with 90%

confidence intervals of .046 and .054; RMR was .03; GFI was .95; AGFI was .93;

CFI was .88; IFI was .88; and TLI was .86. The square multiple correlation indicated

that 81% and 13% of the variance in attitude towards and use of performance-

enhancing substances, respectively, were explained by this model.

Examination of modification indices and standardised residuals suggested that

affordability be allowed to covary with availability. Given that affordability and

availability of other substances use are commonly interrelated in terms of

consumption (Anderson 2006; Chaloupka et al. 2000), and were theorised by

Donovan et al. (2002) to be interrelated in terms of performance-enhancing

substances use, the model was re-analysed with that change. The refitted model

provided a good fit to the data. A significant Bollen-Stine index of fit was observed

(x2 = 564.0, df = 161, p < .001). All other indices of fit were satisfactory. The

RMSEA was .045 with 90% confidence intervals of .041 and .049; RMR was .03;

GFI was .96; AGFI was .94; CFI was .90; IFI was .90; and TLI was .89. The square

multiple correlations remained at 81% and 13% of the variance in attitude towards

and use of performance-enhancing substances respectively.

Page 180: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

163

The standardised parameter estimates shown in Figure 6.2 indicate a significant and

strong relationship between attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use

and personal morality (0.64), a significant and moderate relationship with legitimacy

(0.25), and a significant but lesser relationship with reference group opinion (0.19).

The moderate relationship between attitude towards performance-enhancing

substances use and benefit appraisal (0.40) approached significance (p = .091). In

turn, performance-enhancing substances use was significantly and moderately

associated with attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use (0.36). All

other relationships were non-significant (p > .05).

The standardised parameter estimates of the paths are similar to and interpreted in the

same way as regression weights (Nunnally & Bernstein 1994). For example, the

standardised coefficient for personal morality was 0.64, which means that

susceptibility to performance-enhancing substances use will change by a factor of

0.64 for every unit change in personal morality, while holding all other constructs

constant. Given that the coefficient is a positive number, changes in these two

constructs occur in the same direction.

These results support the hypothesised model and are discussed in the next chapter,

together with their implications for anti-doping education programs and future

research.

Page 181: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

164

Figure 6.1: Overview of results of structural equation analysis of the Sport Drug Control model

Note: Standardised estimates shown. Error terms not shown. Significance testing: * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001

Page 182: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

165

Figure 6.2: Overview of results of structural equation analysis with affordability allowed to covary with availability

Note: Standardised estimates shown. Error terms not shown. Significance testing: * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001

Page 183: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

166

CHAPTER 7: DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to empirically test the Sport Drug Control model

(Donovan et al. 2002) via structural equation modelling. A cross-sectional nationwide

mail survey of elite Australian athletes was conducted. The sample consisted of 1,237

athletes (mean age: 23.1 years) from a variety of sports. The survey data permitted

testing of the model as it included a sufficient number of athletes who reported use of

banned performance-enhancing substances.

Among athletes who were drug tested, 1.1% reported testing positive for a banned

performance-enhancing substance. This is in line with the proportion of samples

analysed by the International Olympic Committee and World Anti-Doping Agency

accredited anti-doping laboratories that resulted in adverse analytical findings and

atypical findings from 1993 to 2011 (ranged between 1.3% and 2.1%; Mottram 2005;

World Anti-Doping Agency 2011a). Self-reported use of banned performance-

enhancing substances was 6.9%. This is within the range of self-reported use studies in

adult athletes between 1980 and 1996 (5% to 15%; Laure 1997), and similar to a recent

survey of 1,040 elite Greek athletes (8%; Tsorbatzoudis et al. 2009).

As in previous studies of attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use, the

data suggest that a substantial minority of athletes are susceptible to performance-

enhancing substances use. In this study, 41.4% of athletes indicated they would give

some consideration to an offer of a banned performance-enhancing substance. This

finding is consistent with other studies that examined athletes’ responses to an offer of a

performance-enhancing substance in hypothetical scenarios (e.g., Bamberger & Yaeger

Page 184: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

167

1997; Bents, Tokish & Goldberg 2004; Corbin et al. 1994; Goldman & Klatz 1992;

Waddington et al. 2005).

In this study, when athletes were asked whether they believed it necessary to use banned

performance-enhancing substances at least at some time to perform at the very highest

levels, 30.8% of athletes in this study did not respond definitively that athletes

‘definitely’ don’t have to use banned performance-enhancing substances. Other studies

have also found that a substantial minority of athletes perceive it necessary to engage in

performance-enhancing substances use. For example, in Laure and Binsinger’s (2005)

study, 21% of athletes believed that ‘to refuse doping means losing all chances of

becoming a great champion’. In Stilger and Yesalis’s (1999) study, 22% of high school

football players reported that they would use anabolic steroids ‘if they knew their

opponents were using them’.

7.1 Testing the Sport Drug Control Model

The model tested hypothesised that six psychosocial variables (threat appraisal, benefit

appraisal, personal morality, reference group opinion, legitimacy, personality) were

predictors of attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use, and that attitude

towards performance-enhancing substances use, together with affordability and

availability of performance-enhancing substances, would predict performance-

enhancing substances use. Results from structural equation modelling supported the

hypothesised model and revealed that some, but not all of these relationships were

significant.

Page 185: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

168

7.1.1 Predictors of performance-enhancing substances use

A favourable attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use was associated

with actual use of performance-enhancing substances. This finding provides support for

past research that reported associations between attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances and actual use of these substances among United States male

college athletes (Petróczi 2007) and elite Australia athletes (Gucciardi, Jalleh &

Donovan 2011), as well as intentions to use these substances among Italian high school

students (Lucidi et al. 2004, 2008) and gym users in the Netherlands (Wiefferink et al.

2008).

The hypothesised relationships between availability and affordability of performance-

enhancing substances and the use of these substances were non-significant. However,

this result was impacted by a substantial proportion of athletes reporting that they did

not know whether the six performance-enhancing substances were accessible or

affordable: 47.3% to 61.8% and 58.6% to 69.8% for each substance respectively.

Contrary to this study, performance-enhancing substances were perceived to be easily

accessible in two studies conducted prior to the year 2000 (i.e., Scarpino et al. 1990;

Stilger & Yesalis 1999). However, in both of these studies, the sample consisted of high

school athletes only. Hence, the findings are not directly comparable to this study.

7.1.2 Predictors of attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use

When considering the six hypothesised psychosocial variables together, personal

morality revealed the strongest association with attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use. That is, athletes with weaker moral stance against

Page 186: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

169

performance-enhancing substances use had a more favourable attitude towards use of

these substances.

Donovan et al. (2002) appears to have been the first published paper to highlight

personal morality as an important component for understanding performance-enhancing

substances use in sport. In this study, personal morality was conceptualised and

measured in terms of moral judgement towards performance-enhancing substances use

(i.e., judgement of the behaviour as morally right or wrong), and moral emotions

experienced if caught using performance-enhancing substances. Since the Donovan et

al. (2002) paper, there have been no published studies on athletes’ moral judgement

towards performance-enhancing substances use and only the Strelan and Boeckmann

(2006) study investigated moral emotions. In that study, the moral emotion of guilt

anticipated from use of human growth hormone (hGH) was the strongest influence on

Australian footballers and soccer players’ hypothetical decision not to use that

substance. The Strelan and Boeckmann’s (2006) study and this study provide support

for the importance of moral emotions in influencing performance-enhancing substances

use.

In a sporting context, morality has been associated with the concept of cheating, with

performance-enhancing substances use viewed by athletes as the most serious form of

cheating (Moran et al. 2004). In Donovan, Jalleh and Gucciardi’s (2009) study,

acceptance of cheating as measured by the Attitudes to Moral Decision-Making in

Youth Sport Questionnaire (Lee, Whitehead & Ntoumanis 2007) was significant in

differentiating athletes according to low, moderate and high susceptibility to

performance-enhancing substances use. In Gucciardi, Jalleh and Donovan’s (2011)

study, of the constructs examined, morality – measured in terms of cheating – evidenced

Page 187: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

170

the strongest relationship with attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use.

A few studies have found that moral disengagement is significantly associated with

intentions to use performance-enhancing substances (Lucidi et al. 2004, 2008; Zelli,

Mallia & Lucidi 2010). These studies provide further support to the importance of

morality in understanding performance-enhancing substances use.

There was a significant relationship between perceived reference group opinions on the

morality of performance-enhancing substances use and attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use. The direction of the association was the same as for personal

morality, but the strength of the association was less. There are no published studies on

the moral stance on performance-enhancing substances use of athletes’ reference

groups. In this study, the vast majority of athletes perceived that people involved in

their lives (i.e., family/partner, coach/trainer, close friends, team mates/training partner,

sports doctor, sports psychologist, and lawyer) would consider performance-enhancing

substances use as morally wrong (‘morally wrong under any circumstances’: over 78%

for each reference group).

There was a significant moderate relationship between legitimacy and attitude towards

performance-enhancing substances use. The components of legitimacy tested in the

model were in relation to distributive justice (i.e., perceived fairness of the drug testing

process) and procedural justice (i.e., perceived fairness of the appeals process). The data

supported Donovan et al.’s (2002) theorising that if athletes perceive an anti-doping

organisation’s drug enforcement regime to be fair and just, then the legitimacy of the

anti-doping organisation in conducting drug testing and prosecution is likely to be

enhanced and compliance with anti-doping regulations is more likely. This is a new and

promising dimension in understanding performance-enhancing substances use.

Page 188: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

171

The moderate relationships between attitude towards performance-enhancing substances

use and both threat and benefit appraisals did not reach significance. This is contrary to

the findings in Gucciardi, Jalleh and Donovan’s (2011) study in which there was a

significant moderate relationship between attitude towards performance-enhancing

substances use and benefit appraisal, and a significant but small relationship with threat

appraisal. The inconsistencies in findings may well be due to variations in measurement

of these two constructs between the two studies. The opportunistic nature of the

Gucciardi, Jalleh and Donovan’s (2011) study meant that some of the indicators were

not designed to specifically measure the constructs of interest. In this study, threat

appraisal was conceptualised and measured in terms of threats relating to both ill-health

effects of performance-enhancing substances use and enforcement, while benefit

appraisal was measured in terms of both rewards for performing well in sport and

perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on performance. In Gucciardi,

Jalleh and Donovan’s (2011) study, threat and benefit appraisals were measured only in

terms of the latter measure for each construct (i.e., appraisal of threat of enforcement,

and perceived impact of performance-enhancing substances on performance).

In this study, personality was not a significant predictor of attitude towards

performance-enhancing substances use. This finding is consistent with Gucciardi, Jalleh

and Donovan’s (2011) study. It is noteworthy that both studies were limited to a single

measure of personality: risk taking propensity (this study) and self-esteem (Gucciardi,

Jalleh & Donovan 2011). In line with this study, Donovan, Jalleh and Gucciardi (2009)

found that risk taking propensity (measured by the Risk Propensity Scale; Meertens &

Lion 2008) did not significantly differentiate athletes with respect to susceptibility to

performance-enhancing substances use.

Page 189: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

172

Since the Donovan et al. (2002) paper and data collection for this study, a number of

personality variables have been linked to performance-enhancing substances use via a

sound theoretical basis and for which there are rigorously tested measurement scales

(for a review see Donovan, Jalleh & Gucciardi 2009). For example, in Donovan, Jalleh

& Gucciardi’s (2009) study, the personality variables that most differentiated between

high, moderate and low susceptible athletes were self-presentation concerns, public

athletic identity, moral decision making, concerns over mistakes and fear of failure.

Including these scales in future studies would provide a more comprehensive measure

of the personality construct.

The model accounted for a substantial proportion of the variance in attitude towards

performance-enhancing substances use (81%). Despite this study’s finding that

performance-enhancing substances use was significantly and moderately associated

with attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use, the model accounted for

only 13% of the variance in use of performance-enhancing substances. [The other two

hypothesised predictors of performance-enhancing substances use – affordability and

availability of performance-enhancing substances – were non-significant]. Kraus’s

(1995) meta-analysis of 88 attitude-behaviour studies revealed that attitudes

significantly and substantially predicted future behaviour. However the reported mean

correlation of 0.38 can be considered as a low-moderate correlation. These data

highlight the complexity of the attitude-behaviour relationship and predicting behaviour

per se. Notwithstanding attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use, there

are a multitude of environmental and situational factors that may facilitate or inhibit

performance-enhancing substances use.

Page 190: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

173

The findings of this study are now discussed in terms of practical implications for the

content of anti-doping education programs and directions for future research.

7.2 Implications for Anti-Doping Education Programs

Current anti-doping education programs focus on building awareness and knowledge of

banned performance-enhancing substances, reporting and testing requirements, and

penalties for non-compliance. Such programs may include sporting values to assist

athletes in resisting inclinations or invitations to use banned performance-enhancing

substances (Lippi et al. 2008). However, this ‘education’ approach ignores psychosocial

variables increasingly being found to be related to attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use. The results of this study have a number of implications for

anti-doping programs, and particularly with respect to two largely ignored areas:

morality; and legitimacy. This study’s findings suggest that in addition to educational

components, anti-doping prevention may benefit from attention to morality and

legitimacy issues that influence attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use.

How that could be done is elaborated below.

7.2.1 Fostering a moral stance against performance-enhancing substances use

Anti-doping education programs could incorporate discussion on moral issues in

relation to performance-enhancing substances use to foster a moral stance against use of

such substances. Kolhberg’s (1976) stages of moral development provide a framework

for understanding athletes’ level of moral reasoning in relation to performance-

enhancing substances use. Kolhberg posits six stages of moral development that are

classified into three levels: (1) preconventional morality; (2) conventional morality; and

Page 191: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

174

(3) postconventional morality. The tenets of each moral development stage and the level

of moral reasoning of athletes towards performance-enhancing substances use at each

stage are outlined here.

Level 1. Preconventional morality:

Stage 1. Obedience and punishment orientation: This moral development stage mainly

occurs in young children. Morality is viewed as something external to the individual.

The concern is with what authorities permit and punish, with unquestioning obedience

to the directives of an authority. At stage 1, athletes’ compliance with anti-doping

organisations’ bans on performance-enhancing substances use can be viewed as due to

obedience to the rules to avoid punishment (i.e., sanctions) for non-compliance.

Stage 2. Individualism and exchange: This moral development stage also mainly occurs

in children. They recognise that the authority’s view is not the only view, and that

individuals have different interests and viewpoints. Athletes at stage 2 recognise that

there are different viewpoints on anti-doping organisations’ bans on performance-

enhancing substances use. Their compliance with the ban is based on self-interests, that

is, to avoid the negative consequences of being caught using performance-enhancing

substances.

Level 2. Conventional morality:

Stage 3. Good interpersonal relationships: This moral development stage mainly occurs

in children entering their teenage years. At this stage, good behaviour means having

good motives and being concerned with the feelings of other individuals. There is a

belief that individuals should live up to the expectations of the family and community

Page 192: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

175

and behave accordingly. At stage 3, athletes’ compliance with bans on performance-

enhancing substances use can be viewed as due to adhering to social expectations.

Stage 4. Maintaining the social order: At this stage, moral decisions are made from the

perspective of society as a whole. The emphasis is on obeying laws, respecting

authority, and performing duties so that the social order is maintained. At stage 4,

athletes’ compliance with bans on performance-enhancing substances use is based on

feelings of obligation to respect the rules and authority.

Level 3. Postconventional morality:

Stage 5. Social contract and individual rights: At this stage, consideration is given to the

rights and values that a society ought to uphold. The concern is with protecting

individual rights and settling disputes through democratic processes. There is a

commitment to the social contract and to changing laws through democratic agreements.

Only when individual rights are clearly at risk does violating the law seem justified. At

stage 5, athletes’ compliance with the ban on performance-enhancing substances use is

based on being consistent with values that are deemed to be important in sport and to

society. The ban protects the rights of individuals to compete on equal terms.

Stage 6: Universal principles: At this stage, there is a recognition that democratic

processes do not always result in outcomes that are just. Laws are valid only if they are

grounded in justice for everyone, and a commitment to justice implies an obligation to

disobey unjust laws. At stage 6, athletes’ compliance with the ban on performance-

enhancing substances use is based on principles of fairness and justice, and taking into

consideration the impact of non-compliance on others.

Page 193: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

176

Kohlberg’s sequence for moral thinking can be applied to foster a moral stance against

performance-enhancing substances use. It is postulated that these stages emerge from

one’s own thinking about moral problems (Kohlberg 1976). Therefore, moving through

these stages is dependent on stimulating mental processes such as via social discussions,

interactions and experiences. Blatt and Kohlberg’s (1975) teaching method of moral

dilemma discussion for fostering moral competencies – founded in the philosophy of

education and in psychological and educational research – has been shown to have a

substantial effect size (r = 0.40) and to be highly effective in various age groups from

children aged 10 years to adults (Lind 2006). The teaching method involves presenting

and discussing a hypothetical or factual value dilemma story in a group setting and

inducing cognitive conflict. The facilitator attempts to present or encourage arguments

that are one stage higher than those being discussed by the group participants.

Another teaching method of moral education is the Konstanz Method of Dilemma

Discussion which is based on Blatt and Kohlberg’s (1975) method of dilemma

discussion. One of the main differences between these two methods is that in the

Konstanz Method of Dilemma Discussion the facilitator refrains from using his or her

authority to impose his or her aims and pace of learning onto the participants, but rather,

engages in a free discourse with the participants. In Blatt and Kohlberg’s method, the

technique of presenting arguments one stage higher portrays the facilitator as having the

‘right’ answer and it is the task of participants to come up with that answer (Berkowitz

1981; Berkowitz, Gibbs & Broughton 1980).

There are two main learnings from the Konstanz Method of Dilemma Discussion (Lind

2006):

Page 194: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

177

1. Mutual respect and free moral discourse: In the discussion session, everyone is to

be respected and treated equally regardless of their power or status. To facilitate

free moral deliberation and discourse on performance-enhancing substances use,

the facilitator must not use his or her authority to impose his or her own opinions

on performance-enhancing substances use; and

2. Maintain high levels of attention and learning motivation: In moral dilemma

discussions, learning is most conducive in situations in which the level of attention

of participants is maintained as high as possible throughout the session. To

maintain attention and learning motivation at an optimal level, Lind (2006)

suggests that the discussion alternates between cycles of challenge and support

approximately every ten minutes. It is believed that through challenges participants

are emotional and attentive, and eager to solve a problem or to ease bad feelings.

However, it is cautioned that challenges must not persist for too long or get too

strong as raising emotions too high is thought to prevent learning. In the phases of

support, participants are given time for their emotions to subside.

7.2.2 Enhancing adherence to a moral stance against performance-enhancing

substances use

Given that moral disengagement has been found to be significantly associated with

intentions to use performance-enhancing substances (Lucidi et al. 2004, 2008; Zelli,

Mallia & Lucidi 2010), it is essential that anti-doping education programs not only

foster a moral stance against performance-enhancing substances use, but also attempt to

reinforce adherence to that stance. Bandura (1991) outlines eight psychological

processes allowing an individual to disengage from behaving consistent with their

personal moral standards. These moral disengagement mechanisms can be applied in

Page 195: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

178

reverse to activate self-regulation of personal moral standards, and therefore adherence

to a moral stance against use of performance-enhancing substances. Findings from this

study suggest the following six psychological processes could be used to counter

disengagement from a moral stance against performance-enhancing substances use:

1. Immoral justification (contrary to moral justification): Using performance-

enhancing substances is personally and socially unacceptable. Hence, it cannot be

viewed in the service of a moral purpose;

2. Immoral labelling (contrary to euphemistic labelling): Using language to accentuate

the moral wrongness of performance-enhancing substances use; that is, referring to

performance-enhancing substances as ‘immoral’, ‘wrong’, ‘cheating’, ‘stealing a

medal’, and ‘robbing other athletes of financial and social rewards’;

3. Disadvantageous comparison (contrary to advantageous comparison): Comparing

use of performance-enhancing substances to transgressive behaviours that are

abhorrent and not tolerated in everyday life; for example, the gravity of

performance-enhancing substances use in a sporting context can be increased by

comparing it to the act of violent assault in everyday life;

4. Personalisation of responsibility for use of performance-enhancing substances

(contrary to displacement of responsibility and attribution of blame): Attributing

responsibility for own use of performance-enhancing substances as ultimately a

personal decision;

5. Personalisation of responsibility for the negative consequences of performance-

enhancing substances use on others (contrary to diffusion of responsibility):

Attributing personal responsibility for the negative consequences on others (e.g.,

depriving other athletes of the financial and social rewards for performing well in

sports) resulting from own use of performance-enhancing substances. The

consequences on others is a direct result of a personal decision to use performance-

Page 196: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

179

enhancing substances, hence responsibility cannot be attributed to the actions of

other people; and

6. Humanisation of other athletes (contrary to dehumanisation): Valuing the human

qualities of other athletes. Performance-enhancing substances provide unfair

advantage in competing with other athletes. They are humans too.

Bandura (1991) theorised that the anticipation of moral emotions (i.e., guilt, shame,

embarrassment) for actions that violate a personal moral norm serves as a regulator of

self-censure. However, moral disengagement may reduce or negate experience of moral

emotions. This emphasises the importance of enhancing moral engagement in anti-

doping education programs and the need to highlight the moral emotions likely to be

experienced if caught using performance-enhancing substances.

7.2.3 Maintaining perceptions of the legitimacy of the testing and appeals

processes

In this study, the vast majority of athletes believed that the Australian Sports Drug

Agency is fair in treating all athletes equally (90% of the total sample), and there is an

almost universal perception that the Australian Sports Drug Agency’s drug testing

procedures in Australia are secure (96% of the total sample). These very favourable

attitudes towards the testing regime are consistent with the views of elite United

Kingdom athletes: 90% of those who had been drug tested were satisfied that the test

had been carried out fairly and accurately (UK Sport 2006). Furthermore, in 2010-11,

there were no successful challenges on procedural grounds for non-compliance with the

Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority’s legislation and the World Anti-Doping

Agency Code (Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2011). These data are a

Page 197: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

180

testament to the World Anti-Doping Agency’s stringent procedures for collecting and

analysing samples.

The legitimacy of anti-doping organisations is based on establishing a fair and just drug

testing regime (distributive justice), with fair and just processes in collecting, analysing

and storing of samples for testing and in any subsequent appeals of an anti-doping rule

violation in tribunals and the Court of Arbitration for Sport (procedural justice), and fair

and just treatment of athletes by personnel administering the drug collection procedure

(interactional justice). The data from this study support the need for anti-doping

organisations to maintain perceptions of legitimacy by ensuring that their drug testing

regime and appeals processes are fair and just. This increases the perceived legitimacy

of anti-doping organisation in conducting drug testing, and consequently, compliance

with anti-doping regulations is more likely. As exemplified in the recent case involving

Lance Armstrong, anti-doping organisations must respond decisively to accusations of

unfair treatment of athletes in pursuing anti-rule violations.

7.3 Limitations and Future Research

This study collected empirical data that supported Donovan et al.’s (2002) Sport Drug

Control model. However it is important to discuss the limitations of this research not

just to interpret the findings in this study, but also because many of these limitations

provide directions for future research in order to confirm (and extend) the current

findings.

The data analysis technique employed in this study (i.e., structural equation modelling)

allows evaluation of the hypothesised causal pathways. However, this study is limited

Page 198: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

181

by its cross-sectional design which does not allow causal inferences about the direction

of the various hypothesised relationships in the model. Furthermore, despite the fact that

the tested model was developed based on a grounded theory approach, other untested

models (causal pathways) also may fit the data. Longitudinal study designs are required

to validate these cross-sectional findings and make assertions about cause and effect

relationships.

This study is based on self-report data which are susceptible to response bias,

particularly under-reporting of socially undesirable attitudes and behaviours. Research

in related areas indicates that reports of illicit drug and alcohol use may be prone to

social desirability effects (see Tourangeau & Yan 2007 for a review). Given the

sensitive nature of performance-enhancing substances use (legal and social

consequences), the reliability of information provided by athletes on attitude towards

performance-enhancing substances may be compromised by socially desirable

responding. Also, the validity of athletes’ self-reported performance-enhancing

substances use cannot be verified. In Petróczi’s (2007) study of 199 male college

athletes in the United States investigating the relationship between athletes’ attitudes,

sport orientation and doping behaviour, a social desirability scale (Crowne & Marlowe

1960) was included in the survey instrument. The data showed a statistically significant

but weak relationship between social desirability and doping attitudes. More recently,

Gucciardi, Jalleh and Donovan (2010) examined the influence of social desirability on

the relationship between attitudes to doping and doping susceptibility in a sample of

elite Australian athletes. Social desirability was found to partially mediate the

association between doping attitudes and doping susceptibility, and the data provided

strong support for the presence of a moderating effect of social desirability. Hence,

these findings highlight the importance of controlling for social desirability when

Page 199: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

182

obtaining athletes’ self-reported attitude towards doping and their susceptibility for

engaging in doping in sport. In future studies, a social desirability scale (e.g., Social

Desirability Scale–17; Stöeber 2001) could be included to measure the tendency to

provide socially desirable responses. In this study, to minimise such reporting bias and

maximise the validity of the data collected, athletes were informed that the survey was

conducted by Curtin University and not the Australian Sports Drug Agency or their

sporting body, participation was voluntary, and the invitation letter stressed the

importance that “all views with respect to all of the issues covered in the questionnaire

are allowed – and expected – to be expressed”. Furthermore, athletes were assured of

anonymity and confidentiality of their responses and Curtin University reply-paid

envelopes were provided.

The sample consisted of and represented the responses of athletes in Australia, mainly

at the elite level. Hence, the results may not be generalisable to athlete samples from

other countries and at other levels of competition. Future studies could survey athletes

representing a broader range of competition levels and involving cross-cultural research

to test the model’s applicability and the relative importance and relevance of the various

constructs in other populations of athletes.

Since the time of data collection for this study, there have been published scales

measuring some of the concepts in the Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002),

mainly in relation to attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use

(Performance Enhancement Attitude Scale; Petróczi & Aidman 2009) and personality

(see Donovan, Jalleh & Gucciardi 2009 for a review). Future studies may consider the

use of these scales to provide a more comprehensive measure of these constructs.

However, consideration must be given to the additional length of the survey instrument

Page 200: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

183

as the Performance Enhancement Attitude Scale consists of 17-items and the Donovan,

Jalleh and Gucciardi’s (2009) study found that a number of personality variables

differentiated athletes with respect to susceptibility to performance-enhancing

substances use.

Research studies on moral disengagement in a sporting context have focussed mainly on

its impact on antisocial and prosocial behaviours and attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use, but none have investigated its association with the use of

performance-enhancing substances. Given that personal morality is a significant factor

in understanding performance-enhancing substances use, research examining and

applying mechanisms to morally engage athletes in relation to performance-enhancing

substances use warrants further investigation.

Despite these limitations, this study provides insights to understanding the factors that

influence attitude towards and actual use of performance-enhancing substances, and a

theoretical foundation for future research on performance-enhancing substances use.

Page 201: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

184

CHAPTER 8: CONCLUSIONS

The two research objectives of this study were achieved. Objective 1 was to assess the

overall fit of the Sport Drug Control model (Donovan et al. 2002). Data on the

constructs in the model were collected via a cross-sectional nationwide mail survey of

1,237 elite Australian athletes. Results from structural equation analyses revealed that

the model was an acceptable fit of the data.

Objective 2 was to assess the relative importance of the various concepts in the model.

Specifically, the objective was to determine: (a) the influence of threat appraisal, benefit

appraisal, personal morality, reference group opinion, legitimacy and personality on

attitude towards the use of performance-enhancing substances; and (b) the influence of

attitude towards use of performance-enhancing substances and affordability and

availability of performance-enhancing substances on the use of such substances.

First, the data indicated a strong relationship between attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use and personal morality, a moderate relationship with

legitimacy and a lesser relationship with reference group opinion. The influence of

threat appraisal, benefit appraisal and personality on attitude towards performance-

enhancing substances use was not significant.

Second, performance-enhancing substances use was moderately associated with attitude

towards performance-enhancing substances use. The influence of affordability and

availability of performance-enhancing substances on the use of such substances was not

significant.

Page 202: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

185

These findings suggest that anti-doping education programs that include only education

and information on banned performance-enhancing substances, reporting and testing

requirements, and penalties for non-compliance – the focus of most anti-doping

education programs – will have little impact on susceptible athletes’ propensity to use

performance-enhancing substances. Based on this study’s findings, anti-doping

education programs would be more effective in influencing attitude towards and actual

use of performance-enhancing substances by including components discussing moral

decision making behaviour in a sporting context, moral affect resulting from being

caught using performance-enhancing substances, the legitimacy base of the anti-doping

organisation, the stringency of drug testing procedures, and the equitable and fair

treatment of all athletes in the drug testing program and appeals process for anti-doping

rule violations.

8.1 Concluding Comment on Findings

Previous studies have focused largely on individual or small subsets of risk factors that

influence attitude towards and actual use of performance-enhancing substances, with

only a small number of studies providing a theoretical research framework and none

examining the multitude of factors proposed in the Sport Drug Control model (Donovan

et al. 2002). This study contributes to the literature by testing the Sport Drug Control

model which incorporates a broad range of psychosocial variables and market factors

known or hypothesised to influence performance-enhancing substances use, and in

obtaining single source data on these variables.

The major findings of this thesis were that morality (i.e., personal moral stance and

perceived reference group moral stance on performance-enhancing substances use) and

Page 203: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

186

perceived legitimacy (i.e., in relation to the testing and appeals processes) are

significant predictors of attitude towards performance-enhancing substances use. In

turn, attitude was a significant predictor of actual use of these substances. Both morality

and legitimacy have received little attention in the sporting literature and are avenues

for future anti-doping education programs and future research to increase understanding

of performance-enhancing substances use.

Anti-doping organisations are relatively good at testing for and detecting drug use.

However, it is important to continually find ways to improve, particularly in prevention.

The results of this research project can provide direct input to anti-doping organisations’

strategic planning in the areas other than deterrence that influence attitudes and

behaviours in relation to performance-enhancing substances.

A positive epistemology underpinned the methodological approach adopted in this

study which allowed the empirical examination of the Sport Drug Control model.

Following the epistemological assumption that knowledge is objective, verifiable and

replicable, further studies testing the Sport Drug Control model are necessary to support

or refute the findings of this study.

Page 204: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

187

REFERENCES

1. Advisory Council on the Misuse of Drugs 2010, Consideration of the anabolic

steroids, Advisory Council on the Misuse of Drugs, London.

2. Ajzen, I 1985, ‘From intentions to actions: a theory of planned behavior’ in

Action-control: from cognition to behavior, eds J Kuhl & J Beckman, Springer,

Heidelberg, pp. 11-39.

3. Ajzen, I 1991, ‘The theory of planned behaviour: some unresolved issues’,

Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, vol. 50, no. 2, pp.

179-211.

4. Alaranta, A, Alaranta, H, Holmila, J, Palmu, P, Pietila, K & Helenius, I 2006,

‘Self-reported attitudes of elite athletes towards doping: differences between type

of sport’, International Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 27, no. 10, pp. 842-846.

5. Ama, PFM, Betnga, B, Ama Moor, VJ & Kamga, JP 2003, ‘Football and doping:

study of African amateur footballers’, British Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 37,

no. 4, pp. 307-310.

6. Amonini, C 2001, The relative influence of morality and legitimacy and other

determinants on youth alcohol, tobacco and marijuana use. Ph.D thesis,

University of Western Australia.

Page 205: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

188

7. Amonini, C & Donovan, RJ 2006, ‘Perceptions of morality and adolescent use of

alcohol, tobacco and marijuana’, Health Education Research, vol. 21, no. 2, pp.

276-286.

8. Anderson, P 2006, ‘Global use of alcohol, drugs and tobacco’, Drug and Alcohol

Review, vol. 25, no. 6, pp. 489-502.

9. Arbuckle, JL 1996, ‘Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete

data’ in Advanced structural equation modeling, eds GA Marcoulides & RE

Schumacker, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New Jersey, pp. 243-277.

10. Arbuckle, JL 2009, Amos 17.0 user’s guide, SPSS Inc, Chicago.

11. Armitage, CJ & Conner, M 2001, ‘Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: a

meta-analytic review’, British Journal of Social Psychology, vol. 40, no. 4, pp.

471-499.

12. d’Arripe-Longueville, F, Corrion, K, Scoffier, S, Roussel, P & Chalabaev, A

2010, ‘Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing judgments of the

acceptability and likelihood of sport cheating’, Journal of Sport & Exercise

Psychology, vol. 32, no. 5, pp. 595-618.

13. Australian Bureau of Statistics 2000, Special article: the Sydney Olympic Games,

Australian Bureau of Statistics, Canberra, Australia.

Page 206: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

189

14. Australian Bureau of Statistics 2007, Participation in sports and physical

recreation, Australia, 2005-06, Australian Bureau of Statistics, Canberra,

Australia.

15. Australian Bureau of Statistics 2009, Sports and physical recreation: a statistical

overview, Australia, Australian Bureau of Statistics, Canberra, Australia.

16. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2006, Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority 2005:06 annual report, Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

17. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2007, Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority 2006:07 annual report, Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

18. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2008, Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority 2007:08 annual report, Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

19. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2009, Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority 2008:09 annual report, Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

20. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2010, Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority 2009:10 annual report, Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

21. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2011, Australian Sports Anti-Doping

Authority 2010:11 annual report, Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

Page 207: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

190

22. Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority 2013, Testing pools. Available from:

<http://www.asada.gov.au/testing/testing_pools.html>. [8 April 2013].

23. Azzazy, HM, Mansour, MM & Christenson, RH 2005, ‘Doping in the

recombinant era: strategies and counterstrategies’, Clinical Biochemistry, vol. 38,

no. 11, pp. 959-965.

24. Backhouse, S, McKenna, J, Robinson, S & Atkin, A 2007, Attitudes, behaviours,

knowledge and education – drugs in sport: past, present and future. Report to the

World Anti-Doping Agency, Carnegie Research Institute, Leeds Metropolitan

University, Canada.

25. Bagozzi, RP & Warshaw, PR 1990, ‘Trying to consume’, Journal of Consumer

Research, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 127-140.

26. Baker, JS, Graham, MR & Davies, B 2006, ‘Steroid and prescription medicine

abuse in the health and fitness community: a regional study’, European Journal of

Internal Medicine, vol. 17, no. 7, pp. 479-484.

27. Bamberger, M & Yaeger, D 1997, ‘Over the edge’, Sports Illustrated, vol. 86, no.

15, pp. 60-70.

28. Bandura, A 1977, Social learning theory, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New

Jersey.

Page 208: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

191

29. Bandura, A 1986, Social foundations of thought and actions: a social cognitive

theory, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.

30. Bandura, A 1990, ‘Selective activation and disengagement of moral control’,

Journal of Social Issues, vol. 46, no. 1, pp. 27-46.

31. Bandura, A 1991, ‘Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action’ in

Handbook of moral behavior and development, eds WM Kurtines & JL Gewirtz,

Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 45-103.

32. Bandura, A 1999, ‘Moral disengagement in the preparation of inhumanities’,

Personal and Social Psychology Review, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 193-209.

33. Bandura, A, Barbaranelli, C, Caprara, G & Pastorelli, C 1996, ‘Mechanisms of

moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency’, Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, vol. 71, no. 2, pp. 364-374.

34. Bandura, A, Caprara, GV, Barbaranelli, C, Gerbino, M & Pastorelli, C 2003,

‘Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse spheres of psychosocial

functioning’, Child Development, vol. 74, no. 3, pp. 769-782.

35. Bandura, A, Caprara, GV, Barbaranelli, C, Pastorelli, C & Regalia, C 2001,

‘Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behaviors’,

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 80, no. 1, pp. 125-135.

Page 209: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

192

36. Bandura, A, Caprara, GV, Barbaranelli, C, Pastorelli, C & Regalia, C 2001,

‘Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior’,

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 80, no. 1, pp. 125-135.

37. Barnes, GM, Welte, JW, Hoffman, JH & Dintcheff, BA 1999, ‘Gambling and

alcohol use among youth: influences of demographic, socialization, and individual

factors’, Addictive Behaviors, vol. 24, no. 6, pp. 749-767.

38. Bauman, A & Owen, N 1999, ‘Physical activity of adult Australians:

epidemiological evidence and potential strategies for health gain’, Journal of

Science and Medicine in Sport, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 30-41.

39. Becker, MH 1974, ‘The Health Belief Model and personal health behavior’,

Health Education Monograph, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 324-473.

40. Beckett, AH 1983, ‘Sports injuries: drugs in sport’, British Journal of Hospital

Medicine, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 221-223.

41. Beckett, AH & Cowan, DA 1979, ‘Misuse of drugs in sport’, British Journal of

Sports Medicine, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 185-194.

42. Benson, E 2002, ‘More male than male’, Monitor on Psychology, vol. 33, no. 9,

pp. 49-51.

Page 210: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

193

43. Bents, RT, Tokish, JM & Goldberg, L 2004, ‘Ephedrine, pseudoephedrine, and

amphetamine prevalence in college hockey players’, The Physician and

Sportsmedicine, vol. 32, no. 9, pp. 30-34.

44. Berkowitz, MW 1981, ‘A critical appraisal of the ‘plus-one’ convention in moral

education’, Phi Delta Kappa, vol. 62, no. 7, pp. 488-489.

45. Berkowitz, MW, Gibbs, J & Broughton, J 1980, ‘The relation of moral judgment

stage disparity to developmental effects of peer dialogues’, Merrill-Palmer

Quarterly, vol. 26, no. 4, pp. 341-357.

46. Bies, RJ & Moag, JS 1986, ‘Interactional justice: communication criteria of

fairness’ in Research on Negotiation in Organisations, volume 1, eds RJ Lewicki,

BH Sheppard & MH Bazerman, JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, pp. 43-55.

47. Blatt, M & Kohlberg, L 1975, ‘The effect of classroom moral discussion upon

children’s level of moral judgment’, Journal of Moral Education, vol. 4, no. 2, pp.

129-161.

48. Blouin, AG & Goldfield, GS 1995, ‘Body image and steroid use in male

bodybuilders’, International Journal of Eating Disorders, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 159-

165.

49. Boardley, ID & Kavussanu, M 2007, ‘Development and validation of the moral

disengagement in sport scale’, Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, vol. 29,

no. 5, pp. 608-628.

Page 211: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

194

50. Boardley, ID & Kavussanu, M 2008, ‘The moral disengagement in sport scale–

short’, Journal of Sports Sciences, vol. 26, no. 14, pp. 1507-1517.

51. Boardley, ID & Kavussanu, M 2009, ‘The influence of social variables and moral

disengagement on prosocial and antisocial behaviours in field hockey and

netball’, Journal of Sports Sciences, vol. 27, no. 8, pp. 843-854.

52. Boardley, ID & Kavussanu, M 2010, ‘Effects of goal orientation and perceived

value of toughness on antisocial behavior in soccer: the mediating role of moral

disengagement’, Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 176-

192.

53. Bohlmann, HR & van Heerden, JH 2005, The impact of hosting a major sport

event on the South African economy, Department of Economics, University of

Pretoria, South Africa.

54. Bollen, KA & Stine, RA 1992, ‘Bootstrapping goodness-of-fit measures in

structural equation models’, Sociological Methods Research, vol. 21, no. 2, pp.

205-229.

55. Boudreau, F & Konzak, B 1991, ‘Ben Johnson and the use of steroids in sport:

sociological and ethical considerations’, Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences, vol.

16, no. 2, pp. 88-98.

56. Bowers, LD 1998, ‘Athletic drug testing’, Clinical Sports Medicine, vol. 17, no. 2,

pp. 299-318.

Page 212: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

195

57. Boyer, TW 2006, ‘The development of risk-taking: a multi-perspective review’,

Developmental Review, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 291-345.

58. Bracken, NM 2012, National study of substance use trends among NCAA college

student-athletes, National Collegiate Athletic Association, Indianapolis, Indiana.

59. British Medical Association 2002, Drugs in sport: the pressure to perform, BMJ

Books, London.

60. Brown, DR & Blanton, CJ 2002, ‘Physical activity, sports participation, and

suicidal behavior among college students’, Medicine and Science in Sports and

Exercise, vol. 34, no. 7, pp. 1087-1096.

61. Browne, MW & Cudeck, R 1993, ‘Alternative ways of assessing model fit’ in

Testing structural equation models, eds KA Bollen & JS Long, Sage, Newbury,

CA, pp. 132-162.

62. Brustad, RJ 1993, ‘Who will go out and play? Parental and psychological

influences on children’s attraction to physical activity’, Pediatric Exercise

Science, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 210-223.

63. Buhrmann, HG & Bratton, RD 1977, ‘Athletic participation and status of Alberta

high school girls’, International Review of Sport Sociology, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 57-

69.

Page 213: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

196

64. Byrne, BM 2001, Structural equation modelling with AMOS: basic concepts,

applications, and programming, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New Jersey.

65. Carvajal, SC, Clair, SD, Nash, SG & Evans, RI 1998, ‘Relating optimism, hope,

and self-esteem to social influences in deterring substance use in adolescents’,

Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 443-465.

66. Casswell, S & Thamarangsi, T 2009, ‘Reducing harm from alcohol: call to

action’, The Lancet, vol. 373, no. 9682, pp. 2247-2257.

67. Catlin, DH & Murray, TH 1996, ‘Performance-enhancing drugs, fair competition,

and Olympic sport’, Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 276, no. 3,

pp. 231-237.

68. Chaloupka, FJ, Grossman, M & Saffer, H 2002, ‘The effects of price on alcohol

consumption and alcohol-related problems’, Alcohol Research and Health, vol.

26, no. 1, pp. 22-34.

69. Chaloupka, FJ, Hu, T, Warner, KE, Jacobs, R & Yurekli, A 2000, ‘The

taxation of tobacco products’ in Tobacco control in developing countries, eds

P Jha & F Chaloupka, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 237-272.

70. Choi, KS, Cho, WH, Lee, S, Lee, H & Kim, C 2004, ‘The relationships among

quality, value, satisfaction and behavioral intention in health care provider choice:

a South Korean study’, Journal of Business Research, vol. 57, no. 8, pp. 913-921.

Page 214: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

197

71. Chung, AM 2000, Working for children and families: safe and smart after-school

programs, Department of Justice, Washington, DC.

72. Cohen, D, Taylor, S, Zonta, M, Vestal, K & Schuster, M 2007, ‘Availability of

high school extracurricular sports programs and high-risk behaviors’, Journal of

School Health, vol. 77, no. 2, pp. 80-86.

73. Conner, M & Norman, P 2005, ‘Predicting health behaviour: a social cognition

approach’ in Predicting health behaviour, eds M Conner & P Norman, Open

University Press, Maidenhead, Berkshire, England, pp. 1-27.

74. Coopersmith, S 1967, The antecedents of self-esteem, W.H. Freeman, San

Francisco.

75. Corbin, CB, Feyrer-Melk, SA, Phelps, C & Lewis, L 1994, ‘Anabolic steroids: a

study of high school athletes’, Pediatric Exercise Science, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 149-

158.

76. Corrion, K, Long, T, Smith, AL & d’Arripe-Longueville, F 2009, ‘“It’s not my

fault; it’s not serious”: athlete accounts of moral disengagement in competitive

sport’, The Sport Psychologist, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 388-404.

77. Cowart, VS 1988, ‘Accord on drug testing, sanctions sought before 1992

Olympics in Europe’, Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 988, no.

260, pp. 3397-3398.

Page 215: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

198

78. Crowne, DP & Marlowe, D 1960, ‘A new scale of social desirability independent

of psychopathology’, Journal of Consulting Psychology, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 349-

354.

79. Curry, LA & Wagman, DF 1999, ‘Qualitative description of the prevalence and

use of anabolic androgenic steroids by United States powerlifters’, Perceptual

Motor Skills, vol. 88, no. 1, pp. 224-233.

80. Deci, EL, Koestner, R & Ryan, RM 1999, ‘A meta-analytic review of experiments

examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation’, Psychological

Bulletin, vol. 125, no. 6, pp. 627-668.

81. Deci, EL & Ryan, RM 1985, ‘The general causality orientations scale: self-

determination in personality’, Journal of Research in Personality, vol. 19, no. 2,

pp. 109-134.

82. DeFrancesco, L 2004, ‘The faking of champions’, Nature Biotechnology, vol. 22,

no. 9, pp. 1069-1071.

83. Dietze, PM, Fitzgerald, JL & Jenkinson, RA 2008, ‘Drinking by professional

Australian Football League (AFL) players: prevalence and correlates of risk’,

Medical Journal of Australia, vol. 189, no. 9, pp. 479-483.

84. Dimeo, P 2006, Drugs, alcohol and sport, Routledge, London.

Page 216: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

199

85. Dimeo, P 2007, A history of drug use in sport, 1876–1976: beyond good and evil,

Routledge, London.

86. Dimeo, P, Hunt, TM & Horbury, R 2011, ‘The individual and the state: a social

historical analysis of the East German ‘doping system’’, Sport in History, vol. 31,

no. 2, pp. 218-237.

87. Dimeo, P & Taylor, J in press, ‘Monitoring drug use in sport: the contrast between

official statistics and other evidence’, Drugs: Education, Prevention, Policy,

viewed 19 November 2012, Informa Healthcare.

88. Dodge, T & Jaccard, JJ 2008, ‘Is abstinence and alternative? Predicting adolescent

athletes’ intention to use performance enhancing substances’, Journal of Health

Psychology, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 703-711.

89. Donahue, EG, Miquelon, P, Valois, P, Goulet, C, Buist, A & Vallerand, RJ 2006,

‘A motivational model of performance-enhancing substance use in elite athletes’,

Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 511-520.

90. Donald, K 1983, The doping game, Boolarong Publication, Spring Hill, Brisbane.

91. Donaldson, S & Ronan, K 2006, ‘The effects of sports participation on young

adolescents’ emotional well-being’, Adolescence, vol. 41, no. 162, pp. 369-389.

92. Donovan, RJ 1998, ‘Increased support for Aboriginal reconciliation: fact or

artefact?’, Australasian Journal of Marketing Research, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 13-25.

Page 217: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

200

93. Donovan, RJ 2009, ‘Towards an understanding of factors influencing athletes’

attitudes towards performance enhancing technologies: implications for ethics

education’ in Performance-enhancing technologies in sports: ethical, conceptual

and scientific issues, eds TH Murray, KJ Maschke & AA Wasunna, Johns

Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp. 283-317.

94. Donovan, RJ, Egger, G, Kapernick, V & Mendoza, J 2002, ‘A conceptual

framework for achieving performance-enhancing drug compliance in sport’,

Sports Medicine, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 269-284.

95. Donovan, RJ, Holman, CDJ, Corti, B & Jalleh, G 1997, ‘Face to face household

interviews versus telephone interviews for health surveys’, Australian and New

Zealand Journal of Public Health, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 134-140.

96. Donovan, RJ & Jalleh, G 1999, ‘Positive versus negatively framed product

attributes: the influence of involvement’, Psychology & Marketing, vol. 28, no. 4,

pp. 215-234.

97. Donovan, RJ & Jalleh, G 2000, ‘Positive versus negative framing of a

hypothetical infant immunisation: the influence of involvement’, Health

Education and Behaviour, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 82-95.

98. Donovan, RJ, Jalleh, G & Gucciardi, DF 2009, Psychological profile of at-risk

athletes, Report to the Department of Health and Ageing, Centre for Behavioural

Research in Cancer Control, Curtin University, Perth.

Page 218: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

201

99. DrugScope 2006, New DrugScope survey reveals scale of mainstream steroid

misuse. Available from: <http://www.drugscope.org.uk>. [20 February 2007].

100. DuRant, RH, Escobedo, LG & Heath, GW 1995, ‘Anabolic-steroid use, strength

training, and multiple drug use among adolescents in the United States’,

Pediatrics, vol. 96, no. 1, pp. 23-28.

101. Egan, T 2002, ‘Body-conscious boys adopt athletes’ taste for steroids’, The New

York Times 22 November, p. A1.

102. Eggert, LL & Herting, JR 1993, ‘Drug involvement among potential dropouts and

“typical” youth’, Journal of Drug Education, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 31-55.

103. Elliot, DL & Goldberg, L 1996, ‘Intervention and prevention of steroid use in

adolescents’, The American Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 24, no. 6 (suppl), pp.

S46-47.

104. Elliot, DL, Goldberg, L, Moe, EL, DeFrancesco, CA, Durham, MB & Hix-Small,

H 2004, ‘Preventing substance use and disordered eating: initial outcomes of the

ATHENA (Athletes Targeting Healthy Exercise & Nutrition Alternatives)

program’, Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, vol. 158, no. 11, pp.

1043-1049.

Page 219: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

202

105. Elliot, DL, Goldberg, L, Moe, EL, DeFrancesco, CA, Durham, MB, McGinnis, W

& Lockwood, C 2008, ‘Long-term outcomes of the ATHENA (Athletes Targeting

Healthy Exercise & Nutrition Alternatives) program for female high school

athletes’, Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, vol. 52, no. 2, pp. 73-92.

106. Elliot, DL, Moe, EL, Goldberg, L, DeFrancesco, CA, Durham, MB & Hix-Small,

H 2006, ‘Definition and outcome of a curriculum to prevent disordered eating and

body-shaping drug use’, Journal of School Health, vol. 76, no. 2, pp. 67-73.

107. Escobedo, LG, Marcus, SE, Holtzman, D & Giovino, GA 1993, ‘Sports

participation, age at smoking initiation, and the risk of smoking among US high

school students’, Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 269, no. 11,

pp. 1391-1395.

108. Fishbein, M 1967, ‘Attitude and the prediction of behavior’ in Readings in

attitude theory and measurement, ed M Fishbein, Wiley, New York, pp. 477-492.

109. Fishbein, M & Ajzen, I 1975, Belief, attitude, intention and behaviour: an

introduction to theory and research, Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA.

110. Flynn, LR & Pearcy, D 2001, ‘Four subtle sins in scale development: some

suggestions for strengthening the current paradigm’, International Journal of

Market Research, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 409-423.

Page 220: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

203

111. Foglia, WD 1997, ‘Perceptual deterrence and the mediating effect of internalized

norms among inner-city teenagers’, Journal of Research in Crime and

Delinquency, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 414-442.

112. Fox, KR 1999, ‘The influence of physical activity on mental well-being’, Public

Health Nutrition, vol. 2, no. 3a (suppl), pp. 411-418.

113. Fritz, MS, MacKinnon, DP, Williams, J, Goldberg, L, Moe, EL & Elliot, Dl 2005,

‘Analysis of baseline by treatment interactions in a drug prevention and health

promotion program for high school male athletes’, Addictive Behaviours, vol. 30,

no. 5, pp. 1001-1005.

114. Gadsdon, S 2001, Psychology and sport, Heinemann, Oxford.

115. Gilliland, SW 1993, ‘The perceived fairness of selection systems: an

organisational justice perspective’, The Academy of Management Review, vol. 18,

no. 4, pp. 694-734.

116. Godin, G 1994, ‘Social-cognitive models’ in Advance in exercise adherence, ed

RK Dishman, Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL, pp. 113-136.

117. Godin, G & Kok, G 1996, ‘The theory of planned behaviour: a review of its

applications to health-related behaviors’, American Journal of Health Promotion,

vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 87-98.

Page 221: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

204

118. Godin, G, Maticka-Tyndale, E, Adrien, A, Manson-Singer, S, Willms, D &

Cappon, P 1996, ‘Cross-cultural testing of three social cognitive theories: an

application to condom use’, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, vol. 26, no. 17,

pp. 1556-1586.

119. Goldberg, L, Elliot, D, Clarke, GN, MacKinnon, DP, Zoref, L, Moe, E, Green, C

& Wolf, SL 1996, ‘The Adolescents Training and Learning to Avoid Steroids

(ATLAS) prevention programme: background and results of a model

intervention’, Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, vol. 150, no. 7, pp.

713-721.

120. Goldberg, L, MacKinnon, DP, Elliot, DL, Moe, E, Clarke, GN & Cheong, L 2000,

‘The Adolescents Training and Learning to Avoid Steroids (ATLAS) program:

preventing drug use and promoting health behaviours’, Archives of Pediatric and

Adolescent Medicine, vol. 154, no. 4, pp. 332-338.

121. Goldman, B & Klatz, R 1992, Death in the locker room, Elite Sports Medicine

Publications, Chicago, IL.

122. Goulet, C, Valois, P, Buist, A & Cote´, M 2010, ‘Predictors of the use of

performance-enhancing substances by young athletes’, Clinical Journal of Sport

Medicine, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 243-248.

Page 222: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

205

123. Gradidge, P, Coopoo, Y & Constantinou, D 2011, ‘Prevalence of performance-

enhancing substance use by Johannesburg male adolescents involved in

competitive high school sports’, Archives of Exercise in Health and Disease, vol.

2, no. 2, pp. 114-119.

124. Green, GA, Uryasz, FD, Petr, TA & Bray, CD 2001, ‘NCAA study of substance

use and abuse habits of college student-athletes’, Clinical Journal of Sport

Medicine, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 51-56.

125. Gucciardi, DF, Jalleh, G & Donovan, RJ 2010, ‘Does social desirability influence

the relationship between doping attitudes and doping susceptibility in athletes?’,

Psychology of Sport and Exercise, vol. 11, no. 6, pp. 479-486.

126. Gucciardi, DF, Jalleh, G & Donovan, RJ 2011, ‘An examination of the Sport Drug

Control Model with elite Australian athletes’, Journal of Science and Medicine in

Sport, vol. 14, no. 6, pp. 469-476.

127. Hair, JF, Anderson, RE, Tatham, RL & Black, WC 1998, Multivariate data

analysis, 5th edn, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.

128. Hanstad, DV, Smith, A & Waddington, I 2008, ‘The establishment of the World

Anti-Doping Agency: a study of the management of organizational change and

unplanned outcomes’, International Review for the Sociology of Sport, vol. 43, no.

3, pp. 227-249.

Page 223: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

206

129. Hausmann, R, Hammer, S & Betz, P 1998, ‘Performance-enhancing drugs

(doping agents) and sudden death: a case report and review of the literature’,

International Journal of Legal Medicine, vol. 111, no. 5, pp. 261-264.

130. Health Department of Western Australia 1998. Licit and illicit drug use among 12

to 17 year old Western Australian school students in 1996: A summary report for

Western Australian Schools, Health Department of Western Australia, Perth.

131. Hendry, LB, Shucksmith, J, Love, JG & Glendinning, A 1993, Young people’s

leisure and lifestyles, Routledge, London.

132. Horn, S, Gregory, P & Guskiewicz, KM 2009, ‘Self-reported anabolic-androgenic

steroids use and musculoskeletal injuries: findings from the center for the study of

retired athletes health survey of retired NFL players’, American Journal of

Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, vol. 88, no. 3, pp. 192-200.

133. Houlihan, BMJ 2001, ‘The World Anti-Doping Agency: prospects for success’ in

Drugs and doping in sport: socio-legal perspectives, ed J O’Leary, Cavendish,

London, pp. 125-145.

134. Houlihan, BMJ 2002, Dying to win: doping in sport and the development of anti-

doping policy, 2nd edn, Council of Europe Publishing, Strasbourg, Germany.

135. Houlihan, BMJ 2004, ‘Harmonising anti-doping policy: the role of the World

Anti-Doping Agency’ in Doping and Public Policy, eds J Hoberman & V Moller,

University Press of Southern Denmark, Odense, pp. 19-30.

Page 224: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

207

136. Hu, LT & Bentler, PM 1999, ‘Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance

structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives’, Structural

Equation Modeling, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 1-55.

137. Hunt, TM & Dimeo, P 2012, ‘History of IOC anti-doping at the Olympics, 1960-

1976’ in Perfect bodies: sports, medicine and immortality, ed V Lo, British

Museum Press, London, pp. 91-96.

138. Jalleh, G & Donovan, RJ 2001, ‘Beware of product labels!’, Journal of Research

for Consumers, vol. 2. Available from: <http://www.jrconsumers.com>. [18

September 2005].

139. Jalleh, G & Donovan, RJ 2007, Survey of Australian elite athletes’ attitudes

towards and beliefs about sport issues, Report to the Australian Sports Anti-

Doping Authority, Social Marketing Research Unit, School of Marketing, Curtin

University, Perth.

140. Jalleh, G & Donovan, RJ 2008a, Survey of Australian elite athletes’ attitudes

towards and beliefs about sport issues: November 2007 survey, Report to the

Australian Sports Anti-Doping Authority, Social Marketing Research Unit,

School of Marketing, Curtin University, Perth.

141. Jalleh, G & Donovan, RJ 2008b, Survey of Australian elite athletes’ attitudes

towards and beliefs about sport issues: May 2008 survey, Report to the Australian

Sports Anti-Doping Authority, Social Marketing Research Unit, School of

Marketing, Curtin University, Perth.

Page 225: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

208

142. Jambor, EA 1999, ‘Parents as children’s socializing agents in youth soccer’,

Journal of Sport Behavior, vol. 22, no. 33, pp. 350-359.

143. Jessor, R, Chase, JA & Donovan, JE 1980, ‘Psychosocial correlates of marijuana

use and problem drinking in a national sample of adolescents’, American Journal

of Public Health, vol. 70, no. 6, pp. 604-613.

144. Kamber, M 2011, ‘Development of the role of national anti-doping organisations

in the fight against doping: from past to future’, Forensic Science International,

vol. 213, no. 1, pp. 3-9.

145. Kamber, M & Mullis, PE 2010, ‘The worldwide fight against doping: from the

beginning to the World Anti-Doping Agency’, Endocrinology and metabolism

clinics of North America, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 1-9.

146. Kammerer, RC 2001, ‘What is doping and how is it detected?’ in Doping in elite

sport: the politics of drugs in the Olympic movement, eds W Wilson & E Derse,

Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL, pp. 3-28.

147. Kemper, HCG, Snel, J, Verschuur, R. & Essen, LS 1990, ‘Tracking of health and

risk indicators of cardiovascular diseases from teenager to adult: Amsterdam

growth and health study’, Preventive Medicine, vol. 19, no. 6, pp. 642-655.

148. Kline, P 1986, A handbook of test construction: introduction to psychometric

design, Methuen, New York.

Page 226: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

209

149. Knight, J 2003, ‘Drugs bust reveals athletes’ secret steroid’, Nature, vol. 425, no.

6960, pp. 752.

150. Kohlberg, L 1976, ‘The cognitive-developmental approach to moral education’ in

Moral education: it comes with the territory, eds D Purpel & K Ryan, McCutchen

Publishing, Berkley, CA, pp. 176-195.

151. Kondric, M, Sekulic, D, Petroczi, A, Ostojic, L, Rodek, J & Ostojic, Z 2011, ‘Is

there a danger for myopia in anti-doping education? Comparative analysis of

substance use and misuse in Olympic racket sports calls for a broader approach’,

Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 27.

152. Kraus, SJ 1995, ‘Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: a meta-analysis of the

empirical literature’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 21, no. 1,

pp. 58-75.

153. Kronsnick, JA 1999, ‘Survey research’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 50, pp.

537-567.

154. Kulig, K, Brener, N & McManus, T 2003, ‘Sexual activity and substance use

among adolescents by category of physical activity plus team sports

participation’, Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, vol. 157, no. 9, pp.

905-912.

Page 227: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

210

155. Landis, RS, Beal, DJ & Tesluk, PE 2000, ‘A comparison of approaches to

forming composite measures in structural equation models’, Organization

Research Methods, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 186-207.

156. Laure, P 1997, ‘Epidemiologic approach of doping in sport’, Journal of Sports

Medicine and Physical Fitness, vol. 37, no. 3, pp. 218-224.

157. Laure, P & Binsinger, C 2005, ‘Adolescent athletes and the demand and supply of

drugs to improve their performance’, Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, vol.

4, no. 3, pp. 272-277.

158. Laure, P & Binsinger, C 2007, ‘Doping prevalence among preadolescent athletes:

a 4-year follow-up’, British Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 41, no. 10, pp. 660-

663.

159. Laure, P, Lecerf, T, Friser, A & Binsinger, C 2004, ‘Drugs, recreational drug use

and attitudes towards doping of high school athletes’, International Journal of

Sports Medicine, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 133-138.

160. Laure, P & Reinsberger, H 1995, ‘Doping and high-level endurance walkers:

knowledge and representation of a prohibited practice’, Journal of Sports

Medicine and Physical Fitness, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 228-231.

161. Laure, P, Thouvenin, F & Lecerf, T 2001, ‘Attitudes of coaches toward doping’,

Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 132-136.

Page 228: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

211

162. Lazuras, L, Barkoukis, V, Rodafinos, A & Tzorbatzoudis, H 2010, ‘Predictors of

doping intentions in elite-level athletes: a social cognition approach’, Journal of

Sport & Exercise Psychology, vol. 32, no. 5, pp. 694-710.

163. Lee, MJ, Whitehead, J & Ntoumanis, N 2007, ‘Development of the Attitudes to

Moral Decision-making in Youth Sport Questionnaire (AMDMYS)’, Psychology

of Sport and Exercise, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 369-392.

164. Lentillon-Kaestner, V & Ohl, F 2011, ‘Can we measure accurately the prevalence

of doping?’, Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, vol. 21, no.

6, pp. 132-142.

165. Lewko, JH & Greendorfer, SL 1988, ‘Family influence in socialization of children

and adolescents’ in Children in sport, 3rd edn, eds FL Smoll, RA Magill & MJ

Ash, Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL, pp. 288-300.

166. Lind, G 2006, ‘Effective moral education: the Konstanz Method of Dilemma

Discussion’, Hellenic Journal of Psychology, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 189-196.

167. Lippi, G, Banfi, G, Franchini, M & Guidi, GC 2008, ‘New strategies for doping

control’, Journal of Sports Sciences, vol. 26, no. 5, pp. 441-445.

168. Lippi, G & Guidi, GC 2004, ‘Gene manipulation and improvement of athletic

performances: new strategies in blood doping’, British Journal of Sports

Medicine, vol. 38, no. 5, pp. 641.

Page 229: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

212

169. Lissner, L, Bengtsson, C, Björkelund, C & Wedel, H 1996, ‘Physical activity

levels and changes in relation to longevity: a prospective study of Swedish

women’, American Journal of Epidemiology, vol. 143, no. 1, pp. 54-62.

170. Long, T, Pantaléon, N, Bruant, G & d’Arripe-Longueville, F 2006, ‘A qualitative

study of moral reasoning of young elite athletes’, The Sport Psychologist, vol. 20,

no. 3, pp. 330-347.

171. López, B 2011, ‘The invention of a ‘drug of mass destruction’: deconstructing the

EPO myth’, Sport in History, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 84-109.

172. Lucidi, FC, Grano, C, Leone, L, Lombardo, C & Pesce, C 2004, ‘Determinants of

the intention to use doping substances: an empirical contribution in a sample of

Italian adolescents’, International Journal of Sport Psychology, vol. 35, no. 2, pp.

133-148.

173. Lucidi, FC, Zelli, A, Mallia, L, Grano, C, Russo, PM & Violani, C 2008, ‘The

social-cognitive mechanism regulating adolescents’ use of doping substances’,

Journal of Sports Sciences, vol. 26, no. 5, pp. 447-456.

174. MacCallum, RC & Austin, JT 2000, ‘Applications of structural equation

modelling in psychological research’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 51, pp.

201-226.

175. Madden, JR 2002, ‘The economic consequences of the Sydney Olympics: the

CREA/Arthur Andersen study’, Current Issues in Tourism, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 7-20.

Page 230: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

213

176. Magdalinski, T 2001, ‘Drugs inside sport: the rehabilitation of Samantha Riley’,

Sporting Traditions, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 17-32.

177. Marsh, HW, Kit-Tai, H & Zhonglin, W 2004, ‘In search of golden rules: comment

on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and

dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings’, Structural

Equation Modeling, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 320-341.

178. Marsh, HW & Kleitman, S 2003, ‘School athletic participation: mostly gain with

little pain’, Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 205-228.

179. Mattila, VM, Parkkari, J, Laakso, L, Pihlajamäki, H & Rimpelä, A 2010, ‘Use of

dietary supplements and anabolic-androgenic steroids among Finnish adolescents

in 1991–2005’, European Journal of Public Health, vol., 20, no. 3, pp. 306-311.

180. McAuliffe, TL, DiFranceisco, W & Reed, BR 2007, ‘Effects of question format

and collection mode on the accuracy of retrospective surveys of health risk

behavior: a comparison with daily sexual activity diaries’, Health Psychology,

vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 60-67.

181. McCaffery, K, Wardle, J & Waller, J 2003, ‘Knowledge, attitudes and behavioral

intentions in relation to early detection of colorectal cancer in the United

Kingdom’, Preventive Medicine, vol. 36, no. 5, pp. 525-535.

182. Melia, P, Pipe, A & Greenberg, L 1996, ‘The use of anabolic androgenic steroids

by Canadian students’, Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 9-14.

Page 231: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

214

183. Meertens, RM & Lion, R 2008, ‘Measuring an individual’s tendency to take risks:

the risk propensity scale’, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, vol. 38, no. 6,

pp. 1506-1520.

184. Miller, KE, Barnes, GM, Sabo, D, Melnick, MJ & Farrell, MP 2002, ‘A

comparison of health risk behaviour in adolescent users of anabolic androgenic

steroids, by gender and athlete status’, Sociology of Sport Journal, vol. 19, no. 4,

pp. 385-402.

185. Mischel, W & Mischel, HN 1976, ‘A cognitive social learning approach to

morality and self regulation’ in Moral development and behaviour: theory,

research, and social issues, ed T Lickona, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New

York, pp. 84-107.

186. Møller, V 2005, ‘Knud Enemark Jensen’s death during the 1960 Rome Olympics:

a search for truth?’, Sport in History, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 452-471.

187. Mooney, CZ & Duvall, RD 1993, Bootstrapping: a nonparametric approach to

statistical inference, 3rd edn, Sage Publications, Newbury Park, CA.

188. Moran, A, Guerin, S, Kirby, K & MacIntyre, T 2008, The development and

validation of a doping attitudes and behaviour scale, Report to the World Anti-

Doping Agency & The Irish Sports Council, University College Dublin &

University of Ulster, Ireland.

Page 232: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

215

189. Moran, A, Guerin, S, MacIntyre, T & McCaffrey, N 2004, Why do athletes cheat?

An investigation of Irish athletes’ understanding of, and attitudes to cheating

behaviour (including doping) in sport, Report to the Irish Sports Council, Dublin,

Ireland.

190. Morris JN, Clayton, DG, Everitt, MG, Semmence, AM & Burgess, EH 1990,

‘Exercise in leisure time: coronary attack and death rates’, British Heart Journal,

vol. 63, no. 6, pp. 325-334.

191. Mottram, DR 2005, ‘Prevalence of drug misuse in sport’ in Drugs in sport, 4th

edn, ed DR Mottram, Routledge, London, pp. 357-380.

192. Mottram, DR 2011, Drugs in sport, 5th edn, Routledge, New York.

193. Mugford, S, Mugford, J & Donnelly, D 1999, Social research project: athletes’

motivations for using or not using performance-enhancing drugs, Report to the

Australian Sports Drug Agency, Canberra, ACT.

194. Munck, IME 1979, ‘Model building in comparative education’, International

Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement Monograph Studies,

no. 10, Almqvist & Wiksell International, Stockholm, Sweden.

195. Myers, ND, Feltz, DL, Maier, KS, Wolfe, EW & Reckase, MD 2006, ‘Athletes’

evaluations of their head coach’s coaching competency’, Research Quarterly for

Exercise and Sport, vol. 77, no. 1, pp. 111-121.

Page 233: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

216

196. Nagin, DS & Pogarsky, G 2001, ‘Integrating celerity, impulsivity, and extralegal

sanction threats into a model of general deterrence: theory and evidence’,

Criminology, vol. 39, no. 4, pp. 865-892.

197. National Aboriginal Sporting Chance Academy 2013, ARMtour (Athletes As Role

Models). Available from: <http://www.nasca.com.au/index.php/what-we-

do/armtour>. [8 April 2013].

198. Nattiv, A & Puffer, JC 1991, ‘Lifestyles and health risks of collegiate athletes’,

Journal of Family Practice, vol. 33, no. 6, pp. 585-590.

199. Nelson, TF & Wechsler, H 2001, ‘Alcohol and college athletes’, Medicine and

Science in Sports and Exercise, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 43-47.

200. Nevitt, J & Hancock, GR 2001, ‘Performance of bootstrapping approaches to

model test statistics and parameter standard error estimation in structural equation

modelling’, Structural Equation Modelling, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 353-377.

201. Newcomb, MD & Felix-Ortiz, M 1992 ‘Multiple protective and risk factors for

drug use and abuse: cross-sectional and prospective findings’, Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 63, no. 2, pp. 280-296.

202. Nicholson, N, Wynd, T & Cohen, P 1993, ‘Drugs in sport: performance before

health’, Health Promotion Journal of Australia, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 20-22.

Page 234: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

217

203. Nilsson, S, Spak, F, Marklund, B, Baigi, A & Allebeck, P 2005, ‘Attitudes and

Behaviours with Regards to Androgenic Anabolic Steroids among Male

Adolescents in a County of Sweden’, Substance Use and Misuse, vol. 40, no. 1,

pp. 1-12.

204. Nucci, L, Guerra, N & Lee, J 1991 ‘Adolescent judgments of the personal,

prudential, and normative aspects of drug usage’, Developmental Psychology, vol.

27, no. 5, pp. 841-848.

205. Nunnally, JC 1978, Psychometric theory, 2nd edn, McGraw-Hill, New York.

206. Nunnally, JC & Bernstein, IH 1994, Psychometric theory, 3rd edn, McGraw-Hill,

New York.

207. O’Brien, KS, Kolt, GS, Martens, MP, Ruffman, T, Miller, PG & Lynott, D 2012,

‘Alcohol-related aggression and antisocial behaviour in sportspeople/athletes’,

Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 292-297.

208. Oguma, Y, Sesso, HD, Paffenbarger, RS & Lee, IM 2002, ‘Physical activity and

all cause mortality in women: a review of the evidence’, British Journal of Sports

Medicine, vol. 36, no. 3, pp. 162-172.

209. Ohaeri, JU, Ikpeme, E, Ikwuagwu, PU, Zamani, A & Odejide, OA 1993, ‘Use and

awareness of effects of anabolic steroids and psychoactive substances among a

cohort of Nigerian professional sports men and women’, Human

Psychopharmacology, vol. 8, no. 6, pp. 429-432.

Page 235: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

218

210. Özdemir, L, Nur, N, Bagcivan, I, Bulut, O, Sumer, H & Tezeren, G 2005,

‘Doping and performance-enhancing drug use in athletes living in Sivas, Mid-

Anatolia: a brief report’, Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, vol. 4, no. 3, pp.

248-252.

211. Parker, D, Manstead, ASR & Stradling, SG 1995, ‘Extending the theory of

planned behaviour: the role of personal norm, British Journal of Social

Psychology, vol. 34, no. 2, pp. 127-137.

212. Parkinson, AB & Evans, NA 2006, ‘Anabolic androgenic steroids: a survey of

500 users’, Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 644-

651.

213. Pärssinen, M & Seppälä, T 2002, ‘Steroid use and long-term health risks in former

athletes’, Sports Medicine, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 83-94.

214. PASW Statistics 2009, PASW statistics version 17.0, SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL.

215. Pate, RR, Heath, GW, Dowda, M & Trost, SG 1996, ‘Associations between

physical activity and other health behaviors in a representative sample of US

adolescents’, American Journal of Public Health, vol. 86, no. 11, pp. 1577-1581.

Page 236: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

219

216. Pate, RR, Pratt, M, Blair, SN, Haskell, WL, Macera, CA, Bouchard, C, Buchner,

D, Ettinger, W, Heath, GW, King, AC, Kriska, A, Leon, AS, Marcus, BH, Morris,

J, Paffenbarger, RS, Patrick, K, Pollock, ML, Rippe, JM, Sallis, J & Wilmore, JH

1995, ‘Physical activity and public health: a recommendation from the Centers for

Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine’,

Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 273, no. 5, 402-407.

217. Pate, RR, Trost, SG, Levin, S & Dowda, M 2000, ‘Sports participation and health-

related behaviors among US youth’, Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent

Medicine, vol. 154, no. 9, pp. 904-911.

218. Paternoster, R 1987, ‘The deterrent effect of the perceived certainty and severity

of punishment: a review of the evidence and issues’, Justice Quarterly, vol. 4, no.

2, pp. 173-217.

219. Paternoster, R 1989, ‘Decisions to participate in and desist from four types of

common delinquency: deterrence and the rational choice perspective’, Law and

Society Review, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 7-40.

220. Pedersen, S & Seidman, E 2004, ‘Team sports achievement and self esteem

development among urban adolescent girls’, Psychology of Women Quarterly,

vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 412-422.

221. Peters, C, Schulz, T & Michna, H 2002, Biomedical side effects of doping: project

of the European Union, Sport und Buch Strauss, Köln, Cologne.

Page 237: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

220

222. Petróczi, A 2002, Exploring the doping dilemma in elite sport: can athletes’

attitudes be responsible for doping?, University of Northern Colorado, USA.

223. Petróczi, A 2007, ‘Attitudes and doping: a structural equation analysis of the

relationship between athletes’ attitudes, sport orientation and doping behaviour’,

BMC Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, vol. 2, no. 34, pp. 34-

48.

224. Petróczi, A & Aidman, E 2008, ‘Psychological drivers in doping: the life-cycle

model of performance enhancement’, Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention,

and Policy 2008, vol. 3, no. 7, pp. 1-12.

225. Petróczi, A & Aidman, EV 2009, ‘Measuring explicit attitude toward doping:

review of the psychometric properties of the Performance Enhancement Attitude

Scale’, Psychology of Sport and Exercise, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 390-396.

226. Petróczi, A, Uvacsek, M, Deshmukh, N, Shah, I, Nepusz, T, Aidman, EV, Tóth,

M, Barker, J & Naughton, DP 2011, ‘Incongruence in doping related attitudes,

beliefs and opinions in the context of discordant behavioural data: in which

measure do we trust?’, PLoS One, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. e18804.

227. Pomazel, R & Jaccard, J 1976, ‘An informational approach to altruistic

behaviour’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 33, no. 3, pp. 317-

326.

Page 238: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

221

228. Popova, S, Giesbrecht, N, Bekmuradov, D & Patra, J 2009, ‘Hours and days of

sale and density of alcohol outlets: impacts on alcohol consumption and damage:

a systematic review’, Alcohol & Alcoholism, vol. 44, no. 5, pp. 500-516.

229. Powell, KE, Thompson, PD, Caspersen, CJ & Kendrick, JS 1987, ‘Physical

activity and the incidence of coronary heart disease’, Annual Review of Public

Health, vol. 8, pp. 253-287.

230. Quintana, SM & Maxwell, SE 1999, ‘Implications of recent developments in

structural equation modeling for counseling psychology’, Counseling

Psychologist, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 485-527.

231. Rae-Grant, N, Thomas, BH, Offord, DR & Boyle, MH 1989, ‘Risk, protective

factors, and the prevalence of behavioural and emotional disorders in children and

adolescents’, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 262-268.

232. Rainey, CJ, McKeown, RE, Sargent, RG & Valois, RF 1996, ‘Patterns of tobacco

and alcohol use among sedentary, exercising, nonathletic, and athletic youth’,

Journal of School Health, vol. 66, no. 1, pp. 27-32.

233. Rees, R, Zarco, E & Lewis, D 2008, ‘The steroids/sports supplements connection:

pragmatism and sensation-seeking in the attitudes and behavior of JHS and HS

students on Long Island’, Journal of Drug Education, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 329-349.

Page 239: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

222

234. Reichel, C 2011, ‘Recent developments in doping testing for erythropoietin’,

Analytical and Bioanalytical Chemistry, vol. 401, no. 2, pp. 463-481.

235. Reisman, D 1950, The lonely crowd, Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.

236. Rogers, RW 1983, ‘Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and

attitude change: a revised theory of protection motivation’ in Social psychology: a

sourcebook, eds JR Cacioppo & RE Petty, Guildford Press, New York, pp. 153-

176.

237. Rosenberg, M 1979, Conceiving the self, Basic Books, New York.

238. Rosenstock, IM 1977, ‘What research in motivation suggests for public health’,

American Journal of Public Health, vol. 50, no. 3, pp. 295-302.

239. Rossiter, JR & Percy, L 1998, Advertising communications & promotion

management, McGraw-Hill, New York.

240. Sas-Nowosielski, K & Swiatkowska, L 2008, ‘Goal orientations and attitudes

towards doping’, International Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 29, no. 7, pp.

607-612.

241. Saxena, S, Ommeren, MV, Tang, KC & Armstrong, TP 2005, ‘Mental health

benefits of physical activity’, Journal of Mental Health, vol. 14, no. 5, pp. 445-

441.

Page 240: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

223

242. Scarpino, V, Arrigo, A, Benzi, G, Garattini, S, La Vecchia, C, Bernadi, LR,

Silvestrni, G & Tuccimei, G 1990, ‘Evaluation of prevalence of doping among

Italian athletes’, Lancet, vol. 336, no. 8722, pp. 1048-1050.

243. Schafer, WE 1969, ‘Some sources and consequences of interscholastic athletics:

the case of participation and delinquency’, International Review for the Sociology

of Sport, vol. 4, no. 1, pp. 63-79.

244. Schneider, AJ & Friedmann, T 2006, Gene doping in sports: the science and

ethics of genetically modified athletes, Elsevier, London.

245. Schneider, D & Morris, J 1993, ‘College athletes and drug testing: attitudes and

behaviours by gender and sport’, Journal of Athletic Training, vol. 28, no. 2, pp.

146-150.

246. Schreiber, JB, Stage, FK, King, J, Nora, A & Barlow, EA 2006, ‘Reporting

structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: a review’,

Journal of Education Research, vol. 99, no. 6, pp. 323-337.

247. Schulman, P, Castellon, C & Seligman, MEP 1989, ‘Assessing explanatory style:

the content analysis of verbatim explanations and the Attributional Style

Questionnaire’, Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 505-512.

248. Schumaker, JF, Linwood, S & Wood, J 1986, ‘Self-concept, academic

achievement, and athletic participation’, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 62, no.

2, pp. 387-390.

Page 241: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

224

249. Segrave, JO & Hastad, DN 1984, ‘Interscholastic athletic participation and

delinquent behavior: an empirical assessment of relevant variables’, Sociology of

Sport Journal, vol. 1, no. 2, pp. 117-137.

250. Seligman, MEP 1991, Learned optimism, Knopf, New York.

251. Sharpe, T, Brown, M & Crider, K 1995, ‘The effects of a sportsmanship

curriculum intervention on generalized positive social behavior of urban

elementary school students’, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, vol. 28, no. 4,

pp. 401-416.

252. Shephard, RJ 1995, ‘Physical activity, health, and well-being at different life

stages’, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, vol. 66, no. 4, pp. 298-302.

253. Shu, LL, Gino, F & Bazerman, MH 2011, ‘Dishonest deed, clear conscience:

when cheating leads to moral disengagement and motivated forgetting’,

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 37, no. 3, pp. 330-349.

254. Silver, MD 2001, ‘Use of ergogenic aids by athletes’, Journal of the American

Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 61-70.

255. Sjoberg, L & Winroth, E 1986, ‘Risk, moral value of actions and mood’,

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 191-208.

256. Smoll, FL, Magill, RA & Ash, MJ 1988, Children in sport, 3rd edn, Human

Kinetics, Champaign, IL.

Page 242: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

225

257. Somerville, SJ & Lewis, M 2005, ‘Accidental breaches of the doping regulations

in sport: is there a need to improve the education of sportspeople?’, British

Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 39, no. 8, pp. 512-516.

258. Spence, JC & Gauvin, L 1996, ‘Drug and alcohol use by Canadian university

athletes: a national survey’, Journal of Drug Education, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 275-

287.

259. Sports Council 1996, Doping control in the UK: a survey of the views of elite

competitors 1995, Sports Council, London.

260. Stamm, H, Lamprecht, M, Kamber, M, Marti, B & Mahler, N 2008, ‘The public

perception of doping in sport in Switzerland, 1995-2004’, Journal of Sports

Sciences, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 235-242.

261. Stein, JA, Newcomb, MD & Bentler, PM 1987, ‘Personality and drug use:

reciprocal effects across four years’, Personality and Individual Differences, vol.

8, no. 3, pp. 419-430.

262. Steiner, H, McQuivey, RW, Pavelski, R, Pitts, T & Kraemer, H 2000,

‘Adolescents and sports: risk or benefit?’, Clinical Pediatrics, vol. 39, no. 3, pp.

161-166.

263. Steptoe, A & Butler, N 1996, ‘Sports participation and emotional wellbeing in

adolescents’, The Lancet, vol. 347, no. 9018, pp. 1789-1792.

Page 243: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

226

264. Stewart, B & Smith, ACT 2008, ‘Drug use in sport: implications for public

policy’, Journal of Sport & Social Issues, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 278-298.

265. Stilger, VG & Yesalis, CE 1999, ‘Anabolic-androgenic steroid use among high

school football players’, Journal of Community Health, vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 131-

145.

266. Stöeber, J 2001, ‘The Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17): convergent validity,

discriminant validity, and relationship with age’, European Journal of

Psychological Assessment, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 222-232.

267. Strelan, P & Boeckmann, RJ 2003, ‘A new model for understanding performance-

enhancing drug use by elite athletes’, Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, vol.

15, no. 2, pp. 176-183.

268. Strelan, P & Boeckmann, RJ 2006, ‘Why drug testing in elite sport does not work:

perceptual deterrence theory and the role of personal moral beliefs’, Journal of

Applied Social Psychology, vol. 36, no. 12, pp. 2909-2934.

269. Striegel H, Ulrich R & Simon P 2010, ‘Randomized response estimates for doping

and illicit drug use in elite athletes’, Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 106, no.

2-3, pp. 230-232.

270. Striegel, H, Vollkommer, G & Dickhuth, HH 2002, ‘Combating drug use in

competitive sport: an analysis from the athletes’ perspective’, Journal of Sports

Medicine and Physical Fitness, vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 354-359.

Page 244: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

227

271. Sturgis, P, Allum, N & Smith, P 2008, ‘An experiment on the measurement of

political knowledge in surveys’, Public Opinion Quarterly, vol. 72, no. 1, pp. 90-

102.

272. Tabachnick, BG & Fidell, LS 2001, Using multivariate statistics, 4th edn, Allyn

and Bacon, Boston, MA.

273. Tangney, JP, Stuewig, J & Mashek, DJ 2007, ‘Moral emotions and moral

behaviour’, Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 58, pp. 345-372.

274. Thorlton, J, McElmurry, B, Park, C & Hughes, T 2012, ‘Adolescent performance

enhancing substance use: regional differences across the USA’, Journal of

Addictions Nursing, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 97-111.

275. Tittle, CR 1980, Sanctions and social deviance: the question of deterrence,

Praeger, New York.

276. Tourangeau, R & Smith, TW 1996, ‘Asking sensitive questions: the impact of

data collection mode, question format, and question context’, Public Opinion

Quarterly, vol. 60, no. 2, pp. 275-304.

277. Tourangeau, R & Yan, T 2007, ‘Sensitive questions in surveys’, Psychological

Bulletin, vol. 133, no. 5, pp. 859-883.

Page 245: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

228

278. Traclet, A, Romand, P, Moret, O & Kavussanu, M 2011, ‘Antisocial behavior in

soccer: a qualitative study of moral disengagement’, International Journal of

Sport and Exercise Psychology, vol. 9, no. 2, pp. 143-155.

279. Tsorbatzoudis, H, Rodafinos, A, Spiliopoulou, H, Barkoukis, V & Lazuras, L

2009, Determinants of doping intentions in sport (Project DIDIS), Aristotle

University of Thessaloniki, Greece.

280. Tyler, TR 1990, Why people obey the law, Yale University Press, New Haven.

281. UK Sport 2006, 2005 drug free sport survey, UK Sport, London.

282. Unal, M & Ozer Unal, D 2004, ‘Gene doping in sports’, Sports Medicine, vol. 34,

no. 6, pp. 357-362.

283. UNESCO 2005, International convention against doping in sport, UNESCO,

Paris, France.

284. United States Anti-Doping Agency 2007, Marion accepts sanction for doping

violation; hands over Olympic medals. Available from: <http://www.usada.org/

files/active/resources/press_releases/Press%20Release%20-%20JONES%20-

%20October%202007.pdf>. [8 April 2013].

Page 246: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

229

285. United States Anti-Doping Agency 2012, Lance Armstrong receives lifetime ban

and disqualification of competitive results for doping violations stemming from

his involvement in the United States postal service pro-cycling team doping

conspiracy. Available from: <http://www.usada.org/media/sanction-armstrong

8242012>. [24 August 2012].

286. United States Anti-Doping Agency 2013, Sanctions. Available from:

<http://www.usada.org/sanctions>. [8 April 2013].

287. Uvacsek, M, Nepusz, T, Naughton, DP, Mazanov, J, Ránky, MZ & Petróczi, A

2011, ‘Self-admitted behavior and perceived use of performance-enhancing vs

psychoactive drugs among competitive athletes’, Scandinavian Journal of

Medicine & Science in Sports, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 224-234.

288. US Department of Health and Human Services 1996, Physical activity and health:

a report of the Surgeon General, Jones and Bartlett Publishers, Sudbury, MA.

289. Vallerand, RJ & Losier, GF 1999, ‘An integrative analysis of intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation in sport’, Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, vol. 11, no. 1,

pp. 142-169.

290. Verschuur, R 1987, Daily physical activity and health, Uitgeverij de Vrieseborch,

Netherlands.

Page 247: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

230

291. Vining, J & Ebreo, A 1992, ‘Predicting recycling behaviour from global and

specific environmental attitudes and changes in recycling opportunities’, Journal

of Applied Social Psychology, vol. 22, no. 20, pp. 1580-1607.

292. Voy, R & Deeter, KD 1991, Drugs, sport and politics, Human Kinetics,

Champaign, IL.

293. Waddington, I, Malcolm, D, Roderick, M & Naik, R 2005, ‘Drug use in English

professional football’, British Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 39, no. 4, pp. e18.

294. Weber, K & Roehl, WS 1998, ‘An analysis of consumer‘s’ risk approach, risk

behaviour and risk perception of selected leisure/tourist activities’. Paper

presented at the Symposium on the Consumer Psychology of Travel, University of

Hawaii, Hawaii.

295. West, SG, Finch, JF & Curran, PJ 1995, ‘Structural equation models with

nonnormal variables: problems and remedies’ in Structural equation modeling:

concepts, issues, and applications, ed RH Hoyle, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp.

56-75.

296. White, S, Duda, J & Keller, M 1998, ‘The relationship between goal orientation

and perceived purposes of sport among youth sport participants’, Journal of Sport

Behaviour, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 474-484.

Page 248: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

231

297. Wiefferink, CH, Detmar, SB, Coumans, B, Vogels, T & Paulussen, TGW 2008,

‘Social psychological determinants of the use of performance-enhancing drugs by

gym users’, Health Education Research, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 70-80.

298. Williams, MH 1994, ‘The use of nutritional ergogenic aids in sports: is it an

ethical issue?’, International Journal of Sport Nutrition, vol. 4, no. 2, pp. 120-131.

299. Winnail, SD, Valois, RF, Dowda, M, McKeown, RE, Saunders, RP & Pate, RR

1997, ‘Athletics and substance use among public high school students in a

southern state’, American Journal of Health Studies, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 187-194.

300. World Anti-Doping Agency 2008, Play True Magazine. Available from:

<http://playtrue.wada-ama.org/testimonials/gfk_t.html>. [8 July 2008].

301. World Anti-Doping Agency 2009, World Anti-Doping Code 2009, World Anti-

Doping Agency, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

302. World Anti-Doping Agency 2010, About WADA. Available from:

<http://www.wada-ama.org/en/About-WADA/>. [8 September 2012].

303. World Anti-Doping Agency 2011a, 2011 Laboratory testing figures reported by

accredited laboratories: overview of results. Available from: <http://www.wada-

ama.org >. [8 August 2012].

304. World Anti-Doping Agency 2011b, WADA 2011 out of competition testing

statistics. Available from: <http://www.wada-ama.org >. [8 August 2012].

Page 249: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

232

305. World Anti-Doping Agency 2012, Accredited laboratories for doping control

analysis. Available from: <http://www.wada-ama.org >. [8 August 2012].

306. World Cancer Research Fund/American Institute for Cancer Research 2007,

Food, nutrition, physical activity, and the prevention of cancer: a global

perspective, American Institute for Cancer Research, Washington DC, USA.

307. Wright, S, Grogan, S. & Hunter, G 2001, ‘Body-builders’ attitudes towards

steroid use’, Drugs: Education Prevention and Policy, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 91-95.

308. Wroble, RR, Gray, M & Rodrigo, JA 2002, ‘Anabolic steroids and pre-adolescent

athletes: prevalence, knowledge, and attitudes’, The Sport Journal, vol. 5, no. 3,

pp. unpaginated.

309. Wuerth, S, Lee, MJ & Alfermann, D 2004, ‘Parental involvement and athletes’

career in youth sport’, Psychology of Sport and Exercise, vol. 5., no. 1, pp. 21-33.

310. Yesalis, CE 1993, Anabolic steroids in sport and exercise, Human Kinetics,

Champaign, IL.

311. Yesalis, CE & Bahrke, MS 1995, ‘Anabolic-androgenic steroids: current issues’,

Sports Medicine, vol. 19, no. 5, pp. 326-340.

312. Yesalis, CE & Bahrke, MS 2002, ‘Anabolic-androgenic steroids and related

substances’, Current Sports Medicine Reports, vol. 1, no. 4, pp. 246-252.

Page 250: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

233

313. Yesalis, CE, Kopstein, AN & Bahrke, MS 2001, ‘Difficulties in estimating the

prevalence of drug use among athletes’ in Doping in elite sport: the politics of

drugs in the Olympic movement, eds W Wilson & E Derse, Human Kinetics,

Champaign, IL, pp. 43-62.

314. Yin, Z & Moore, JB 2004, ‘Re-examining the role of interscholastic sport

participation in education’, Psychological Reports, vol. 94, no. 3c, pp. 1447-1454.

315. Zelli, A, Mallia, L & Lucidi, F 2010, ‘The contribution of interpersonal

appraisals to a social-cognitive analysis of adolescents’ doping use’, Psychology

of Sport and Exercise, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 304-311.

316. Zenic, N, Peric, M, Zubcevic, NG, Ostojic, Z & Ostojic, L 2010, ‘Comparative

analysis of substance use in ballet, dance sport, and synchronized swimming:

results of a longitudinal study’, Medical Problems of Performing Artists, vol. 25,

no. 2, pp. 75-81.

Page 251: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

234

APPENDICES

Page 252: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

235

Appendix 1: The Questionnaire

Page 253: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

236

Page 254: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

237

Page 255: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

238

Page 256: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

239

Page 257: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

240

Page 258: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

241

Page 259: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

242

Page 260: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

243

Page 261: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

244

Page 262: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

245

Page 263: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

246

Page 264: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

247

Page 265: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

248

Appendix 2: The Covering Letter

Dear Athlete,

I am an associate director of the Centre for Behavioural Research in Cancer Control at

Curtin University, and a PhD student at the University of Western Australia. My PhD

research investigates elite athletes’ philosophies on sports and their attitudes towards

performance-enhancing technologies and substances.

For this project, performance-enhancing technologies and substances are considered

amoral. That is, these technologies and substances are not viewed as either right or

wrong. While some athletes may think these are wrong. However, in the same way that

some people think marijuana should be legalised, other athletes may think that certain

performance-enhancing technologies and substances that are banned should be

permitted.

The survey was developed and tested with the assistance of a number of elite athletes.

Now the survey is being sent to a large sample of elite athletes, like yourself,

nationally. The data collected are for my PhD research, and it is important that I hear

the views of as many elite athletes as possible.

You have been randomly selected from a number of lists of Australian athletes.

Participation in this survey is voluntary. No question is compulsory. The survey will

take you about 35 minutes to complete. You do not need to record your name on the

survey or any other materials. After the data have been entered, the actual surveys will

be destroyed.

The information you provide is confidential. I am interested in your personal opinions

and not what other people may or may not think. Your answers will be assessed in

aggregate form. The data will be presented as a group and not individually. Data

analysis will involve regression analyses and structural equation modelling. The

purpose is to look at overall distributions of responses and analyse patterns.

The project is being funded by a grant from the Australian Research Council to Curtin

University of Technology and the University of Queensland. Your participation in this

project is very much appreciated. After completing the survey, please return it to

Curtin University of Technology in the enclosed reply paid envelope as soon as

possible, preferably within a week. If you would like more information about this

project please contact Geoffrey Jalleh on (08)92663789 or email

([email protected]).

Sincerely

Geoffrey Jalleh

Page 266: TESTING THE SPORT DRUG CONTROL MODEL OF FACTORS ... · Threat appraisal (threat to health; threat of enforcement), benefit appraisal (rewards for performing well; perceived impact

249

Appendix 3: The Reminder Letter

Dear Athlete,

About two weeks ago I sent you a questionnaire seeking your philosophies on sports

and attitudes towards performance-enhancing technologies and substances. Your name

was randomly selected from a number of lists of Australian athletes.

If you have already completed and returned the questionnaire, thank you very much,

and I apologise for this reminder.

If you have not yet completed the questionnaire, please do so and return it in the reply

paid envelope, preferably within a week. The information you provide is confidential.

You do not need to record your name on the questionnaire or any other materials. After

the data have been entered, the actual surveys will be destroyed.

The data collected are for my PhD research, and it is important that I hear the views of

as many elite athletes as possible. If you would like more information about this project

please contact me on (08)92663789 or email ([email protected]).

Many thanks for your participation.

Sincerely

Geoffrey Jalleh