the cognitive status of morphological products · • a number of differences between compounds and...

30
COMPOUNDS vs. PHRASES The cognitive status of morphological products Katja Böer, Sven Kotowski & Holden Härtl Universität Kassel 15 th International Morphology Meeting Vienna, 9-12 February, 2012 1

Upload: others

Post on 11-Nov-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

COMPOUNDS vs. PHRASESThe cognitive status of morphological

products

Katja Böer, Sven Kotowski & Holden HärtlUniversität Kassel

15th International Morphology MeetingVienna, 9-12 February, 2012

1

Page 2: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

THE MORPHOLOGY-SYNTAX INTERFACECOMPOUND or PHRASE?

• Are morphology and syntax two distinct modules in the human language faculty?

• Classical testbed: Constructions located at the interface of the two modules

Compound Phrase

blackbird black bird

2

Page 3: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Contents

1 Semantic and grammatical differences between compounds and phrases

2 Experimental inquiry into the cognitive status of compounds: 3 studies

3 Conclusion

3

Page 4: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Why does a language need compounds?

• Compounds are often said to have a name-giving function, to be representative of a category or to refer to a lexicalized concept

• Phrases on the other hand are often argued to have a primarily descriptive function

green house greenhouseblack board blackboardfree mason Freemason

4

Page 5: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Why does a language need compounds?

Yet, such contrasts are far from universal:

• Many compounds are not established namesterror dad Nacktprotest [lit. ‘naked_protest’]

• Many phrases are established namered cabbage grüner Tee [‘green tea’]

• Are compounds nevertheless possibly prime suggestions for lexicalization, cf. Lipka (1977); Motsch (2004)?

Wutbürger [lit. ‘anger_citizen’]

5

Page 6: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Grammatical differences

• Compounds are not accessible to syntactic operations, i.e., they display lexical integrity:

a. Kate drives an Audii, whichi recently has had engine failure.

a'. *Kate is an Audii driver, whichi recently has had engine failure.

b. a drunk driver and a melancholic oneb’. *a truck driver and an Audi one

cf. Booij (2009); Giegerich (2006)

6

Page 7: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Semantic differences

• Compounds seem to be more restrictive in terms of interpretability:

a. Max is a sweet tálker. PHRASE Max is sweet as well as a talker. (intersective reading) Max is someone who talks sweetly (i.e., in order to

achieve a certain goal). (non-intersective)

b. Max is a swéet talker. COMPOUND allows for the non-intersective reading only: Max is

someone who talks sweetly.

cf. Bücking (2009); Schlücker & Hünning (2009); Schäfer (2010); Giegerich (2009)

7

Page 8: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Semantic differences

• Compounds often designate kinds, while phrases do not seem to trigger a kind reading:

a. ??A bottle of beer is green in Germany.

b. A beer bottle is green in Germany.

8

Page 9: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Semantic differences

• Unlike phrases, novel compounds are often marked and therefore compatible with sogenannte-contexts:

a. Dies ist ein sogenannter Tiefkamm.[‘this is a so-called deep_comb’]

b. ?? Dies ist ein sogenannter tiefer Kamm.[‘this is a so-called deep comb’]

cf. Bücking (2009); Carlson (1977); Krifka et al. (1995); Schlücker & Hünning (2009)

9

Page 10: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Why does a language need compounds?

• A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic

However,

• Underlying problem of the compound-phrase distinction:

Investigations into the distinction rely on the a prioriassumption that this very distinction exists

Thus, arguments are assailable on grounds of an inherent circularity (cf. Haspelmath 2011)

cf. Jacobs (2010)

10

Page 11: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Investigations into cognitive differences

• Are these differences also mirrored in the respective cognitive treatment of the two constructions?

Morphology as an economic way of producing and memorizing complex linguistic structures, see Härtl (2011); Wunderlich (2008)

Williams syndrome: selective impairment for lexical computation/access with grammatical computation left intact, see Clahsen & Almazan (2000)

Aphasic data: selective impairment for syntactic phrases like strange fever with compound retrieval left intact, see Mondini et al. (2002)

11

Page 12: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Investigations into cognitive differences

• Experiment 1:Are novel compounds memorized differently than corresponding phrases?

• Experiments 2 / 3:Do compounds behave differently from phrases as regards discourse salience?

12

Page 13: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Experiment 1: Memorization study

13

LEARNING PHASE: Subjects were asked to memorize unknown picture labels

eine Flachsäge

eine Kurzsäge ein breiter Kamm

ein tiefer Kamm

eine Kurzsäge

Compound

ein breiter Kamm

Phrase

RECALL PHASE: Subjects were asked to decide whether the presented picture labels were correct or false

RESPONSE VARIABLE: Reaction time needed to decide The procedure was repeated three times for each subject on days 1, 4, and 8

Page 14: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

14

Results: main effects

• Learned items were decided faster (p < .001)

• Phrases were decided faster (p < .01)

• Subjects decided faster over time (p < .001)

900

950

1000

1050

1100

U L

RT

RT

0

500

1000

1500

D1 D2 D3

RT

RT

980

1000

1020

1040

1060

C P

RT

RT

Page 15: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

15

ITEM TYPE X DAY interaction (not significant)

• Neither item type is memorized better over time (p < .26)

860

910

960

1010

1060

1110

Session 1 Session 2 Session 3

CompoundsPhrasesRT

Page 16: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

16

LEARNED X ITEM TYPE interaction (p < .09)

We find a stronger memorization effect for novel compounds

• Subjects took longer to decide unlearned compounds than unlearned phrases(p < .001)

• This discrepancy disappeared for learned compounds (p < .67)

Page 17: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Experiment 2: Questionnaire Study

• Do novel compounds behave differently in terms of discourse salience than phrases?

Testbed: Sentences which contain verbs of implicit causality

e.g. like, love and hate

17

Page 18: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Verbs of implicit causality

• Verbs of implicit causality create a strong bias for pronouns to be resolved as a stimulus (cf. Härtl 2001, Brown & Fish, 1983)

(1) EXPERIENCER-STIMULUS verbs (E-S verbs)

Frieda(EXP) likes Peter(STIM) because he / ?she is friendly.

(2) STIMULUS-EXPERIENCER verbs (S-E verbs)

Linda (STIM), delights Hans (EXP) because she / ?he is very friendly.

18

Page 19: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Questionnaire Study (pilot)

FACTORS: STIM as compound vs. phraseEXP-STIM and STIM-EXP verbs

Sentence completion task:(1) a. Die flache Säge begeistert Christoph, weil [sie/er]

b. Die Flachsäge begeistert Christoph, weil [sie/er]

a. The flat saw enthuses Christoph, because [it/he]b. The flat_saw enthuses Christoph, because [it/ he]

HYP: Novel compounds produce an even stronger bias towards the STIMULUS explication.

19

Page 20: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Results

020406080

100120140160180

COMPOUNDPHRASE

EXP STIM

Total 177 314

Compound 80 168

Phrase 97 146

STIM vs. EXP (p < .0001)COMPOUND vs. PHRASE ( p < .09)

20

Page 21: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

• Experiments 1 and 2 show that novel compounds behave differently from phrases in that

they show a stronger memorization effect than the corresponding phrases

they noticeably raise discourse salience in comparison to phrases

However:Are these in fact structural effects or are they rather down to the novelty of the compounds (i.e., their semantic intransparency in comparison to phrases)?

Intermediate conclusion

21

Page 22: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Experiment 3: Self-paced-reading

Der A+N mag Maria weil PRONOUN…

• A+N: compound (marked) compound (unmarked) phrase (marked) phrase (unmarked)

A prior questionnaire study verified that native speakers of German could not assign meanings to the unmarked A+N-constructions.

22

Page 23: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Design

The deep_teacher pities Kim, because he has a good heart. Does the deep_teacher have a good heart?

• Sentences were self-paced-read word by word• Reaction times were measured for each word• After reading the sentences a comprehension question was asked• Due to German orthography there was a clear-cut distinction

between compounds (one word) and phrases (two) throughout• The last slide contained 1-3 words

23

Page 24: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Results

• For the main measuring point – the pronoun position – no significant reading time differences between any of the four conditions could be observed

This was the case for overall-analyses as well as for analyses comparing individual subject performance

• This lead us to conduct statistical post-hoc analyses for the A+N-position

24

Page 25: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Results: A+N-position

• As expected, marked compounds and marked phrases were read slower than umarked compounds and unmarked phrases (p < .001)

• Marked compounds were read slower than marked phrases (p < .001)

• Reading times for unmarked compounds and unmarked phrases were equal (p < .99)

• Marked phrases took longer than unmarked ones (p < .05)

25

Page 26: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Results: A+N-position II

26

700

800

900

1000

1100

1200

Compound Phrase

unmarkedmarked

rt ms

Page 27: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Conclusion

• We conducted three experiments to investigate the cognitive status of A+N-phrases as well as A+N-compounds

• Results imply processing and reading differencesbetween the two constructions

• Novel compounds show a more pronounced memorization effect as opposed to phrases and enhance discourse salience

• Furthermore, novel compounds are read the slowest among our four different test categories

27

Page 28: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Discussion

• The cause for these differences is unclear:

first, it may be down to transparency effects second, structural differences may be at the bottom of

these discrepancies third, general reading differences between complex and

simplex words may cause the observed effects (cf. Inhoffet al., 1996)

• One aim of future research is to disentangle structural and semantic differences of phrases and novel compounds

28

Page 29: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

Thank You!

ReferencesBooij, Geert (2009) Lexical integrity as a formal universal: a constructionist view. In: Scalise, Sergio; Elisabetta Magni, and Antonietta Bisetto (eds.) Universals of Language Today. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media, 83‐100.

Brown, Roger & Deborah Fish (1983) The psychological causality implicit in language. Cognition, 14, 237‐273.

Bücking, Sebastian (2009) German nominal compounds as underspecified names for kinds. Linguistische Berichte Sonderheft, 17, 253‐281.

Carlson, Greg (1977) A unified analysis of the English bare plural. Linguistics and Philosophy, 1‐3, 413‐458.

Clahsen, Harald & Mayella Almazan (2001) Compounding and inflection in language impairment: evidence from Williams Syndrome (and SLI), Lingua, 111, 729‐757.

Egg, Markus (2006) Anti‐Ikonizität an der Syntax‐Semantik‐Schnittstelle. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 25‐1, 1‐38.

Giegerich, Heinz (2006) Attribution in English and the distinction between phrases and compounds. In: Rösel, Peter (ed.) Englisch in Zeit und Raum ‐ English in Time andSpace: Forschungsbericht für Klaus Faiss, Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag. 

Giegerich, Heinz (2009) Compounding and Lexicalism. In: Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Stekauer (eds.) Handbook of Compounding. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 178‐200.

Härtl, Holden (2001) CAUSE und CHANGE. Thematische Relationen und Ereignisstrukturen in Konzeptualisierung und Grammatikalisierung (= studia grammatica 50), Berlin: Akademie.

Härtl, Holden & Peter Schöpperle (2011) Morphological vs. syntactic composition ‐ the case of nominal compounds. Talk at the AG 3 "Wort oder Phrase?" of the 33rd DGfSconference, Universität Göttingen.

Inhoff, Albrecht; Deborah Briihl & Jill Schwartz (1996) Compound word effects differ in reading, on‐line naming, and delayed naming tasks. Memory & Cognition 24 (4), 466‐476. 

Jacobs, Joachim (2010). Grammatik ohne Wörter? In : Engelberg, S./Holler, A./Proost, K. (eds.) Sprachliches Wissen zwischen Lexikon und Grammatik. Institut für Deutsche Sprache, Jahrbuch 2010

Jespersen, Otto (1942) A Modern English Grammar. Part VI, Morphology. Copenhagen: Ejnar Munksgaard. 

29

Page 30: The cognitive status of morphological products · • A number of differences between compounds and phrases can be attested – grammatical as well as semantic However, • Underlying

30

Krifka, Manfred; Francis. J. Pelletier; Greg. N. Carlson; Alice ter Meulen; Gennaro Chierchia & Godehard Link (1995) Genericity: an introduction. In Greg N. Carlson & Francis J. Pelletier (eds.), The Generic Book. Chicago/London: University of Chicago Press, 1‐124. 

Lipka, Leonhard (1977) Lexikalisierung, Idiomatisierung und Hypostasierung als Probleme einer synchronen Wortbildungslehre. In: Brekle, Herbert & Dieter Kastovsky (eds.) Perspektiven der Wortbildungsforschung, Bonn: Bouvier, 155‐164. 

Mondini, Sara; Gonia Jarema; Claudio Luzzatti; Cristina Burani, and Carlo Semenzai (2002) Why is red cross different from yellow cross? A neuropsychological study of noun‐adjective agreement within Italian compounds. Brain and Language, 81, 621‐634.

Motsch, Wolfgang (2004) Deutsche Wortbildung in Grundzügen. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. 

Lipka, Leonhard (1977) Lexikalisierung, Idiomatisierung und Hypostasierung als Probleme einer synchronen Wortbildungslehre. In: Brekle, Herbert & Dieter Kastovsky (eds.) Perspektiven der Wortbildungsforschung, Bonn: Bouvier, 155‐164. 

Mondini, Sara; Gonia Jarema; Claudio Luzzatti; Cristina Burani, and Carlo Semenzai (2002) Why is red cross different from yellow cross? A neuropsychological study of noun‐adjective agreement within Italian compounds. Brain and Language, 81, 621‐634.

Motsch, Wolfgang (2004) Deutsche Wortbildung in Grundzügen. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. 

Olsen, Susan (2000) Composition. In: Booij, G. et al. (eds.) Morphologie / Morphology, 897‐916, Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Plag, Ingo; Gero Kunter; Sabine Lappe, and Maria Braun (2008) The role of semantics, argument structure, and lexicalization in compound stress assignment in English. Language, 84‐4, 760‐794.

Wunderlich, Dieter (2008) Spekulationen zum Anfang von Sprache. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 27‐2, 229‐266.

Schäfer, Martin (2010) Prä‐ und postnominale Modifikation im Englischen und das Sitationsargument. Talk the 10th workshop Ereignissemantik, Universität Tübingen.

Schlücker, Barbara & Matthias Hüning (2009) Compounds and phrases. A functional comparison between German A+N compounds and corresponding phtrases, Rivista di Linguistica, 21‐1, 209‐234.