the dimensionality of color vision in carriers of...

19
The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of anomalous trichromacy Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK Gabriele Jordan Departments of Medicine and Genome Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, USA Samir S. Deeb Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Jenny M. Bosten Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK J. D. Mollon Some 12% of women are carriers of the mild, X-linked forms of color vision deciencies called anomalous trichromacy.Owing to random X chromosome inactivation, their retinae must contain four classes of cone rather than the normal three; and it has previously been speculated that these female carriers might be tetrachromatic, capable of discriminating spectral stimuli that are indistinguishable to the normal trichromat. However, the existing evidence is sparse and inconclusive. Here, we address the question using (a) a forced-choice version of the Rayleigh test, (b) a test using multidimensional scaling to reveal directly the dimensionality of the participantscolor space, and (c) molecular genetic analyses to estimate the X-linked cone peak sensitivities of a selected sample of strong candidates for tetrachromacy. Our results suggest that most carriers of color anomaly do not exhibit four-dimensional color vision, and so we believe that anomalous trichromacy is unlikely to be maintained by an advantage to the carriers in discriminating colors. However, 1 of 24 obligate carriers of deuteranomaly exhibited tetrachromatic behavior on all our tests; this participant has three well-separated cone photopigments in the long- wave spectral region in addition to her short-wave cone. We assess the likelihood that behavioral tetrachromacy exists in the human population. Keywords: color vision, psychophysics, human, tetrachromacy, anomalous trichromacy Citation: Jordan, G., Deeb, S. S., Bosten, J. M., & Mollon, J. D. (2010). The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of anomalous trichromacy. Journal of Vision, 10(8):12, 119, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/12, doi:10.1167/10.8.12. Introduction Normal human color vision is mediated by three classes of retinal cone, which have peak sensitivities in the short- wave (S), middle-wave (M), and long-wave (L) regions of the spectrum, but 6% of men exhibit a mild deficiency of color vision called “anomalous trichromacy,” in which either the M- or the L-cone sensitivity is thought to be displaced in its spectral position (DeMarco, Pokorny, & Smith, 1992; Hayashi, Motulsky, & Deeb, 1999; Mollon, 1997; Nathans, Piantanida, Eddy, Shows, & Hogness, 1986; Rayleigh, 1881). In the most common case of “deuteranomaly,” affecting 5% of men, the M photopig- ment is replaced by one closer to L, and in “protanomaly,” affecting 1% of men, the L photopigment is replaced by one closer to M. Intriguingly, the mother (or daughter) of an anomalous trichromat is a candidate for tetrachromacy: on one of her X chromosomes, such a heterozygote has the genes for the normal L- and M-cone photopigments, but on her other she will have, say, a gene for the normal L photopigment and a gene for the spectrally shifted hybrid photopigment (LV ) that her anomalous son inherits (see Figures 1A and 1B). Owing to random X chromosome inactivation, only one or other of her two X chromosomes will be expressed in any given cone cell in her retina (Lyon, 1961; Teplitz, 1965). Thus, she will have a retinal mosaic containing four types of cone (see Figure 1C). But does her visual system have enough plasticity to take advantage of the input from an extra class of cone? Would not a special neural apparatus need to evolve before color vision became four-dimensional? Two animal models suggest that no additional neural mechanism may be necessary. First, although the males of nearly all New World primate species are dichromatic, having an autosomal S-photopigment gene and a single, polymorphic, X-linked photopigment gene, two thirds of the females, heterozygous at the latter locus, become functional trichromats (Mollon, Bowmaker, & Jacobs, 1984). Second, and perhaps more remarkably, some heterozygous knock-in mice expressing the human L photopigment as well as the native mouse M photopigment become trichromatic, exhibiting wavelength Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 119 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/12 1 doi: 10.1167/10.8.12 Received January 12, 2010; published July 20, 2010 ISSN 1534-7362 * ARVO

Upload: truongdat

Post on 18-May-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

The dimensionality of color vision in carriersof anomalous trichromacy

Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University,Newcastle upon Tyne, UKGabriele Jordan

Departments of Medicine and Genome Sciences,University of Washington, Seattle, USASamir S. Deeb

Department of Experimental Psychology, University ofCambridge, Cambridge, UKJenny M. Bosten

Department of Experimental Psychology, University ofCambridge, Cambridge, UKJ. D. Mollon

Some 12% of women are carriers of the mild, X-linked forms of color vision de!ciencies called “anomalous trichromacy.”Owing to random X chromosome inactivation, their retinae must contain four classes of cone rather than the normal three;and it has previously been speculated that these female carriers might be tetrachromatic, capable of discriminating spectralstimuli that are indistinguishable to the normal trichromat. However, the existing evidence is sparse and inconclusive. Here,we address the question using (a) a forced-choice version of the Rayleigh test, (b) a test using multidimensional scaling toreveal directly the dimensionality of the participants’ color space, and (c) molecular genetic analyses to estimate the X-linkedcone peak sensitivities of a selected sample of strong candidates for tetrachromacy. Our results suggest that most carriers ofcolor anomaly do not exhibit four-dimensional color vision, and so we believe that anomalous trichromacy is unlikely to bemaintained by an advantage to the carriers in discriminating colors. However, 1 of 24 obligate carriers of deuteranomalyexhibited tetrachromatic behavior on all our tests; this participant has three well-separated cone photopigments in the long-wave spectral region in addition to her short-wave cone. We assess the likelihood that behavioral tetrachromacy exists in thehuman population.

Keywords: color vision, psychophysics, human, tetrachromacy, anomalous trichromacyCitation: Jordan, G., Deeb, S. S., Bosten, J. M., & Mollon, J. D. (2010). The dimensionality of color vision in carriers ofanomalous trichromacy. Journal of Vision, 10(8):12, 1–19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/12, doi:10.1167/10.8.12.

Introduction

Normal human color vision is mediated by three classesof retinal cone, which have peak sensitivities in the short-wave (S), middle-wave (M), and long-wave (L) regions ofthe spectrum, but 6% of men exhibit a mild deficiencyof color vision called “anomalous trichromacy,” in whicheither the M- or the L-cone sensitivity is thought to bedisplaced in its spectral position (DeMarco, Pokorny, &Smith, 1992; Hayashi, Motulsky, & Deeb, 1999; Mollon,1997; Nathans, Piantanida, Eddy, Shows, & Hogness,1986; Rayleigh, 1881). In the most common case of“deuteranomaly,” affecting 5% of men, the M photopig-ment is replaced by one closer to L, and in “protanomaly,”affecting 1% of men, the L photopigment is replaced byone closer to M. Intriguingly, the mother (or daughter) ofan anomalous trichromat is a candidate for tetrachromacy:on one of her X chromosomes, such a heterozygote hasthe genes for the normal L- and M-cone photopigments,but on her other she will have, say, a gene for the normal

L photopigment and a gene for the spectrally shiftedhybrid photopigment (LV) that her anomalous son inherits(see Figures 1A and 1B). Owing to random X chromosomeinactivation, only one or other of her two X chromosomeswill be expressed in any given cone cell in her retina(Lyon, 1961; Teplitz, 1965). Thus, she will have a retinalmosaic containing four types of cone (see Figure 1C).But does her visual system have enough plasticity to

take advantage of the input from an extra class of cone?Would not a special neural apparatus need to evolvebefore color vision became four-dimensional? Two animalmodels suggest that no additional neural mechanism maybe necessary. First, although the males of nearly all NewWorld primate species are dichromatic, having an autosomalS-photopigment gene and a single, polymorphic, X-linkedphotopigment gene, two thirds of the females, heterozygousat the latter locus, become functional trichromats (Mollon,Bowmaker, & Jacobs, 1984). Second, and perhaps moreremarkably, some heterozygous knock-in mice expressingthe human L photopigment as well as the native mouse Mphotopigment become trichromatic, exhibiting wavelength

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/8/12 1

doi: 10 .1167 /10 .8 .12 Received January 12, 2010; published July 20, 2010 ISSN 1534-7362 * ARVO

Page 2: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

discrimination in the red-green region of the spectrum(Jacobs, Williams, Cahill, & Nathans, 2007).In the case of heterozygous women, as in that of New

World monkeys, the neural basis for any enhanced colordiscrimination is likely to be the midget class of retinalganglion cells (Mollon et al., 1984). In the fovea, suchganglion cells draw their center input from a single cone(Dacey, 1999). If the surround input is drawn indiscrim-inately from all (non-S) cones (Lennie, Haake, &Williams, 1991), then the ganglion cell will automaticallybecome chromatically specific (see Figure 1C). We thenneed only to suppose that the cortex, by Hebbian learning(Linsker, 1990), is able to segregate the signals thatoriginate in the L, M, and “anomalous” cones of thepotential tetrachromat (Jordan & Mollon, 1993; Mollon etal., 1984). However, in modeling work, Wachtler, Doi, Lee,and Sejnowski (2007) have suggested that there is insuffi-cient information in natural scenes to allow unsupervisedlearning of signals specific to a fourth cone type.It was De Vries (1948, p. 380) who first suggested that

carriers of anomalous trichromacy should be tetrachro-matic, but the existing evidence supports only weaktetrachromacy (Jordan & Mollon, 1993; Nagy, MacLeod,Heyneman, & Eisner, 1981). In weak tetrachromacy, asdefined by Jordan and Mollon (1993), there are four types

of cone but only three post-receptoral signals. Such subjectsmay reveal themselves by a failure of Grassmann’s ThirdLaw of Color Mixing (MacLeod, 1985). The evidence forstrong tetrachromacyVwhere four independent cone sig-nals are available for cortical transformationsVis stillelusive.In the present study, we first obtained classical Rayleigh

matches for a population of obligate carriers of anomaloustrichromacy and their sons. We then used two testsspecifically designed to detect behavioral tetrachromacyin the carriers. The first was a discrimination test of lightsin the Rayleigh region. The second was a multidimen-sional scaling test specially developed to reveal the extradimension of color space expected to exist in the carriersof deuteranomaly. Finally, we used molecular geneticanalyses to identify the 1max values of our carriers’ middle-to long-wave photopigments.

Rayleigh matches

In selecting participants, our strategy was first toidentify boys who are anomalous trichromats. We pre-dicted that the most plausible candidates for strong

Figure 1. Two stochastic events in succession determine the distribution of cones in the retina of a carrier of anomalous trichromacy. (A)Random X<chromosome inactivation determines whether the maternal (Xm) or the paternal X chromosome (Xp) is expressed within agiven cell. The hybrid gene on Xp is indicated by a green-orange band. (B) The binding of an upstream locus control region (LCR) to thepromoter region of the !rst or second opsin gene determines which photopigment is expressed in the cell. There is a bias in favor of the!rst gene in the array. The more saturated bands in the diagram represent exons. TEX28 is an interposed non-opsin gene. (C) Thehypothetical distribution of four cone types in the heterozygous retina: blue, short-wave; green, middle-wave; red, long-wave and orange,hybrid. Superposed on the array is the receptive !eld of a midget ganglion cell that draws its center input from a single hybrid cone and anopposed surround input from all M, L and LVcones.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 2

Page 3: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

tetrachromacy would be the mothers of those boys whohad small matching ranges on a clinical anomaloscope: itis thought that precision in color matching indicates goodspectral separation of the underlying photopigments (Regan,Reffin, & Mollon, 1994; Shevell, He, Kainz, Neitz, &Neitz, 1998).

Methods

The participants were 7 obligate carriers of protanomaly(cPa), 24 obligate carriers of deuteranomaly (cDa), 12female controls (fCo), and 10 male controls (mCo). Theaverage ages of the four groups were 52.7 (41–66), 52.3(38–68), 47.3 (38–76), and 47.7 (37–65) years, respec-tively. The means did not differ significantly (F(3, 49) =1.4, p = 0.25). In the case of carriers, we tested at least oneanomalous offspring, usually sons but in the case of onecarrier, three deuteranomalous daughters. Initially, color-deficient children were recruited either as a result of atelevision feature about the research or through a localschool. The mothers of selected cases were invited to takepart in the study. Control participants were recruitedthrough the university staff network or were friends of themothers. Ethical permission was given by the researchcommittee of the North East Strategic Health Authority(ref 04/Q0905/62). Genetic analysis on the DNA of theparticipants was approved by the institutional reviewboard: Human Subjects Committee of the University ofWashington.Rayleigh match mid-points and matching ranges were

established using the Oculus anomaloscope (OptikgerateGmbH, Wetzlar). For a range of red/green ratios set by theexaminer, participants were asked to equate the brightnessof the mixture and monochromatic fields and were thenasked whether the fields matched in hue. For each ratio,the participants rated the quality of the match on a 5-pointscale, where 5 indicated a perfect match. Only ratiosscoring 5 were used to derive the matching range.Participants used their preferred eye.

Results and discussion

In Figure 2, we show Rayleigh match mid-points andmatching ranges for deutan and protan carriers, for theircolor-deficient offspring, and for the male and femalecontrols. The carriers and the controls are ordered, withineach group, according to their match mid-points. Relatedoffspring are shown against each carrier. Colored symbolsidentify participants who took part in the multidimen-sional scaling test described below. The red symbolsrepresent those participants who had more than onedimension on the test, whereas those with blue symbolshad only one dimension on the test. For details, see below.

From the data of Figure 2, several interesting pointsarise:

1. For both match mid-points and ranges, one-wayanalyses of variance were performed for the fourgroups: male and female controls and protan anddeutan carriers. There were no significant differencesbetween groups for either mid-points (F(3, 48) =0.51, p = 0.67) or matching ranges (F(3, 48) = 1.01,p = 0.39). However, there were a few deutan carrierswith unusually low match mid-points, an observationsimilar to that made by Waaler (1927).

2. There was no significant correlation between thematch mid-points of protan carriers and those of theirsons (r = j0.15, p = 0.73) nor between the mid-points of deutan carriers and those of their sons (r =j0.28, p = 0.18).

3. Similarly, there were no significant correlationsbetween the matching ranges of carriers and theirsons (for protans r = j0.59, p = 0.15; for deutans r =0.18, p = 0.39).

4. One carrier of deuteranomaly (cDa29) did not acceptany match on the Oculus anomaloscope. Her sons’matches are shown at the very bottom of Figure 2.

Figure 2. Rayleigh match-midpoints and ranges for all carriers andtheir color-de!cient offspring as well as female and male controlparticipants. For explanation see text.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 3

Page 4: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

5. Within the group of deuteranomalous offspring, therewas a small but significant correlation betweenmatch mid-points and matching ranges (r = 0.37,p = 0.034).

Rayleigh discriminations

It is provocative when carriers of color anomalies refuseRayleigh matches as in the case of cDa29 in the presentstudy and in the case of eight carriers of anomaly in thelarge-field Rayleigh match study of Jordan and Mollon(1993). However, the conventional Rayleigh match is acriterion-dependent task, and the dimensionality of colorvision is more securely established by a forced-choicediscrimination task than by subjective matching (Cornsweet,1970): Rather than asking whether two stimuli look alike,one ought to establish whether the potential tetrachromatcan objectively discriminate them.In the present test, the task was a three-alternative,

temporal forced choice, essentially a performance versionof the Rayleigh match. On each trial, two of the threepresentations were of a monochromatic orange light andthe third was of a mixture of a red and a green light; theparticipant was asked to indicate the odd one out. Acrosstrials, the computer sampled the space defined by (a) thered/green ratio and (b) the luminance of the monochro-matic field. Within this space, first explicitly defined byFranceschetti (1928), the normal observer is dichromaticsince the S cones are very insensitive.

Methods

Participants were 18 deutan and 7 protan carriers,10 male and 12 female controls. All had participated inthe preceding measurements of Rayleigh matches.Discrimination of light stimuli in the Franceschetti

space was measured using a purpose-built Maxwellian-view colorimeter. Light from a single tungsten-halogensource was drawn by custom-made fiber-optic light guidesto the inputs of three monochromators (Jobin-Yvon Triax180). Each fiber bundle was formed into a linear array andwas imaged onto the input slit of its respective mono-chromator. This optical coupling minimized stray light andmaximized throughput. The full bandwidths of the mono-chromators were set to be 3.5 nm at half height. The outputof each monochromator was collimated and refocused on ahigh-speed shutter (Uniblitz). Placed close to this focus wasa neutral-density wedge (1 log unit), mounted on a servomotor. All three beams were combined with cube beamsplitters and then refocused on a piece of ground glassmounted on a 2-mm aperture. The latter served as asecondary source, and it was this source, relayed by two

lenses, that was focused on the participant’s pupil. A singlecircular field stop in this final common channel defined the2-deg disc seen by the participant.In order to minimize any cues arising from chromatic

aberration at the edges of the mixture field, an ancillaryCRT monitor was used to display a mask of dynamicluminance and chromatic noise. An image of the monitorscreen was formed in the same plane as the field stop thatdefined the primary stimulus disc. Thus, the participantsaw the annular mask superposed on the edges of thestimulus. To prevent the participant using auditory cuesfrom the shutters, he or she wore headphones over whichwhite noise was played.On each trial, three stimuli were presented in rapid

succession, two of them being a monochromatic orangelight (590 nm) and the third a mixture of a red (670 nm)and a green (546 nm) light (see Figure 3A). Each stimulusfield was presented for 150 ms and the inter-stimulusinterval was 300 ms. The luminances of the red andgreen primaries were equal (60 td). The position of themixture field in the trial sequence was randomized, andthe participant was asked to indicate its position bypushbuttons.In random order, the computer sampled a two-dimen-

sional stimulus grid of 90 cells defined by (a) 10 ratios ofthe 670- and 546-nm components of the mixture field and(b) 9 luminances of the monochromatic 590-nm field. Inan initial practice run, only stimuli from the outer cornersof this sampling space were used. A further practice blockof 90 trials gave participants experience of the entirestimulus array, each trial sampling a different combinationof red-green ratio and luminance of the monochromaticstandard. Thereafter, there were three experimental blocksof 90 trials each; thus, three independent estimates weremade of accuracy and speed for each cell.For a normal trichromat, we expected there to be a

combination of the red/green ratio (R670 / (R670 + G546))and of the luminance of the monochromatic orange field(I590) at which the participant would fail to discriminateand would exhibit long response times. These values of(R670 / (R670 + G546)) and I590 would correspond to theclassical Rayleigh match but are based on performancerather than on subjective judgment. In contrast, atetrachromat should be able to discriminate at all com-binations of (R670 / (R670 + G546)) and I590.

Results and discussion

Since we could not assume a particular Rayleigh matchin advance, our algorithm searched, in random sequence, amatrix of combinations of (R670 / (R670 + G546)) and I590that enclosed the full range of possibilities. The upperpanel of Figure 3B shows, for a typical male control, thedistribution of response times within this matrix, and thelower panel shows the distribution of errors. It can be seenthat the region of stimulus combinations that yields errors

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 4

Page 5: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

is also a region of long response times. Figure 3C summarizesthe performance of two carrier groups and two normalcontrol groups. Response times (upper panel) and errorrates (lower panel), averaged across different values ofI590, are shown as a function of (R670 / (R670 + G546)). Allgroups exhibit a maximum at an intermediate value of(R670 / (R670 + G546)), and the positions of the peaks aresimilar for the two independent performance measures. AnANOVA revealed that there is no difference betweengroups in the means of errors made for different positionsof R / (R + G) (F(3, 43) = 1.67, p 9 0.19). Some of theresponse times differ significantly between groups, but thisdifference is not restricted to any specific region of our gridand we therefore conclude that it is caused by individualdifferences in response times unrelated to color vision.However, one participant cDa29 (represented by open

circles) behaves as if she has access to an additional conesignal: she made no errors at any value of (R670 / (R670 +

G546)) (see Figure 3C, lower panel) and her responsetimes not only are faster than those of other participants(M = 1.221 s from onset of first stimulus) but are roughlyeven across the stimulus space (see Figure 3C, upperpanel). The same accuracy and similar response timeswere obtained on a retest after two months. This participanthas two deuteranomalous sons and one normal son and thusis necessarily heterozygous. Recall that this carrier did notaccept any match on the Oculus anomaloscope. She readthe Ishihara plates without error.Perhaps the most important result of our Rayleigh

discrimination test is that most carriers exhibit a region ofthe Franceschetti space in which their discrimination isimpaired in the same way as is found for color-normalcontrols. Thus, by this test, the great majority of carriersshow no sign of tetrachromacy.cDa29 is the single exception. If we accept for themoment

that she is behaviorally tetrachromatic, her behavioral

Figure 3. Rayleigh discrimination test. (A) Spatial arrangement of stimulus !eld (upper panel) and sequence of stimuli in one trial.Superposed on the edge of the !eld was an annulus of dynamic spatio-chromatic noise, which served to mask any chromatic aberrationthat could provide an additional cue to the discrimination. The duration of each stimulus !eld was 150 ms. The dark inter-stimulus intervalwas 300 ms. The participant’s task was to identify which of the three successive !elds was a red-green mixture. (B) Distribution ofresponse times (upper panel) and errors (lower panel) in the I590, (R670 / (R670 + G546)), matrix for one typical normal male observer. Thered/green ratios are expressed as percentage of luminance contributed by the 670-nm primary in the red/green mixture. (C) Meanresponse times (upper panel) and number of errors (lower panel) for different participant groups as a function of the red-green ratio of themixed !eld. Filled circles, 17 carriers of deuteranomaly (cDa); !lled squares, 7 carriers of protanomaly (cPa); open triangles, 10 malecontrols (mCo); !lled triangles, 12 female controls (fCo). Error bars denote +1 standard deviation of the means. Response times weremeasured from the onset of the !rst of the three stimuli. The horizontal, dotted line corresponds to the offset of the third stimulus !eld. Theopen circles represent a single carrier of deuteranomaly, cDa29, who exhibits the fastest response times with no clear peak, and makesno errors at any ratio. Her data are not included in the cDa group.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 5

Page 6: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

tetrachromacy could arise in one of two ways. Firstly, shemight enjoy “strong tetrachromacy”: in any one small regionof her visual field, she may have access to both normal anddeuteranomalous color signals, and her color space maybe truly four-dimensional. Secondly, her retina may be arelatively coarse mosaic of normal and deuteranomalouspatches, and in any one patch she may not have concurrentaccess to both the normal and the deuteranomalous signals.In this case, when presented with red-green ratios near thenormal match, she might rely on signals from thedeuteranomalous patches in her retina; and when presentedwith ratios near the deuteranomalous match, she might relyon signals from normal patches. Subjectively, the stimulusfields might appear blotchy to the observer. We did askcDa29 whether the 2-deg field appeared patchy to her. Herresponse was negative.However, we have to ask whether other factors could

have contributed to cDa29’s flawless discriminationperformance on the Rayleigh test. (1) We discount theeffects of chromatic aberrations, as we took great care tomask any possible cues at the edges of the mixture field.The experimenter spent some time at the beginning ofeach session to help align the participants’ eyes inMaxwellian view. No participant reported residual edgeeffects. (2) We cannot entirely exclude detection by eitherS-cones or rod signals, but their effects must be negligiblesince S-cone sensitivity is minimal at 546 nm, and theluminance of the stimuli was within the photopic range. Ifthere had been any significant stimulation of these othertwo receptor systems, then these signals should have beenavailable to other carriers and normal controls as well.(3) A final possibility is that cDa29, unlike other observers,is able to exploit the spatial gradient in the density ofmacular pigment across the central fovea, but absorption bythe macular pigment is in fact negligible at the wavelengthof our shortest (546 nm) primary (Bone, Landrum, &Cairns, 1992).

Dimensionality of color spacemeasured by multidimensionalscaling

Our second test used multidimensional scaling (MDS)(Kruskal, 1964; Shepard, 1962) to reveal the extradimension of a tetrachromat’s color space. The input toan MDS program is a matrix of judged similarities of allpossible pairs of a set of stimuli; its output is a map ofthese stimuli that minimizes the differences between theinput proximities and the corresponding proximities in thederived space.We used an MDS test that we have specifically

designed to reveal a deuteranomalous dimension of colorspace: the test contains pairs of stimuli that are near

metameric for the normal observer but are calculated to bedistinct for the deuteranomalous (Bosten, Robinson,Jordan, & Mollon, 2005).For this test, we selected a subset of our deuteranom-

alous carriers on the basis of the anomaloscope matchingranges of their sons. We reasoned that tetrachromacy wasmost likely to be manifest in those carriers whose sonshave a small matching range and thus may have a largespectral separation between their L and LV pigments(Regan et al., 1994).

Methods

Participants were 9 carriers of deuteranomaly, 10 oftheir male, deuteranomalous sons and one normal son.These participants are identified by colored symbols inFigure 2. We chose deuteranomalous carriers whoseanomalous sons had matching ranges on the Oculusanomaloscope extending from 0 (in the case of one sonof cDa29) to 4.3 Nagel units. We also tested 6 female and10 male controls, some of whom were newly recruited forthis test. No protan carriers were tested since the MDS testwas designed to detect the unique deuteranomalousdimension.Stimuli were 15 colored discs subtending 4.5- at a

typical viewing distance. Two stimulus sets were used:one painted with mixtures of cobalt blue and cadmiumyellow single-pigment acrylics (X) and one painted withmixtures of yellow oxide and ultramarine blue (Y).In selecting suitable subsets of X and Y discs, we

modeled the photon catches of individual cone types whenexposed to light from a given stimulus under theexperimental illuminant (a broadband amber). Using aspectroradiometer (PhotoResearch 650) at the position ofthe observer’s eye, we measured the spectral powerdistribution of each disc (E(1)) and multiplied it in turnby the spectral sensitivities of S, M, L, and anomalous (LV)cones (8(S)(1), 8(M)(1), 8(L)(1), and 8(LV)(1)). We usedcone sensitivities as estimated by DeMarco et al. (1992)and Smith and Pokorny (1975). We normalized all thecone sensitivities since it is not known how the scaling ofthe anomalous photopigment relates to that of the normal.Integrating each product between 400 and 700 nm gavethe photon catches in the four classes of cone. Thechromatic signals available to a normal and to adeuteranomalous observer could then be modeled as ratiosof cone activations, e.g., (X8(M)(1).E(1)%1)/(X8(L)(1).E(1)%1). Further information on this test can be found inBosten et al. (2005).Importantly, we included pairs of samples with spectral

power distributions that we had calculated to be distin-guishable to a hypothetical deuteranomalous observer(DeMarco et al., 1992), but to be indistinguishablemetamers for a normal. We plotted the positions of ourstimuli in a color space similar to that of MacLeod andBoynton (1979), but where the cone sensitivities are

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 6

Page 7: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 7

Page 8: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

normalized. The X and Y subsets of stimuli fall alonga single line in this color space (see Figure 4A, lowerpanel), but they are separated in an analogous spaceconstructed for the hypothetical deuteranomalous observer(see Figure 4A, upper panel). We have previously shownthat, in practice, the X and Y stimulus subsets are readilyseparated by many deuteranomalous participants but notby normals (Bosten et al., 2005). We here ask whethercarriers of deuteranomaly can make the discriminationsmade by their anomalous sons.Participants rated the dissimilarity of all possible pairs

of stimulus discs on a scale from 0 to 10. Non-metricMDS was used to reconstruct a subjective space from thematrix of dissimilarity judgments for each observer. Weused non-metric, rather than metric, MDS since it is safestto treat the dissimilarity ratings as an ordinal scale: theverbal ratings given by the participants cannot be assumedto be linearly related to the internal signal on which theyare based (Kruskal, 1964).

Results and discussion

Panel A of Figure 4 shows the theoretical MDSsolutions expected for a deuteranomalous observer (top)and for a normal observer (bottom). The deuteranomalousobserver is assumed to rely on the chromatic dimensionsS / (L + LV) and L / (L + LV). The modeled data show aclear separation between the X and Y pairs described inMethods above. The normal observer is assumed to relyon the chromatic dimensions S / (L + M) and L / (L + M),and in the modeled data the X and Y subsets fall on asingle line.

In discussing the experimental results from the separategroups of participants, we take as a quantitative measurethe Spearman rank correlation between the empiricalordering of the chips and their ordering on the modeleddimensions. We give below the correlations and signifi-cance values without correction for multiple correlations:a more conservative estimate would mean that fewercarriers would show a two-dimensional solution.Of the 16 control subjects (6 female, 10 male), none

gave a two-dimensional solution. Fourteen gave a one-dimensional solution that correlated with S / (L + M) (n =13, 0.632 e rs e 0.857, p G 0.011) or L / (L + M) (n = 1,rs = 0.679, p = 0.005) and 2 gave a 0-dimensional solutionwhere no dimensions correlated with any dimension mod-eled and were assumed to represent noise.Of the 10 deuteranomalous sons tested, 6 gave two-

dimensional solutions (see Figure 4, upper panels of B, C,E, G and J) where one dimension correlated with L / (L +LV) (n = 6, 0.579 e rs e 0.882, p G 0.016) and the otherwith S / (L + LV) (n = 6, 0.604 e rs e 0.850, p G 0.017) and4 gave one-dimensional solutions that correlated with S /(L + LV) (n = 2, 0.782 e rs e 0.918, p G 0.001) or L / (L +LV) (n = 2, 0.621 e rs e 0.775, p G 0.013). The sons whogave two-dimensional solutions are identified in Figure 2by the red symbols.Of the nine carriers of deuteranomaly tested, four gave

two-dimensional solutions (see Figure 4, lower panels ofB, C, E and J), where the first axis correlated with S / (L +M) (n = 4, 0.714 e rs e 0.807, p G 0.001) and where thesecond correlated with L / (L + LV) (n = 4, 0.643 e rs e0.825, p G 0.01). The remainder gave one-dimensionalsolutions that correlated with S / (L + M) (n = 5, 0.696 ers e 0.889, p G 0.004). Correlations for individual carriersare shown in Table 1.In Figure 4B (upper panels), we show the MDS

solutions for the two deuteranomalous sons of participantcDa29. Both solutions are clearly two-dimensional andresemble closely the theoretical deuteranomalous space (seeFigure 4A, upper panel). The first dimension correlateswith the theoretical anomalous signal L / (L + LV) [Da29-1:rs = 0.711, p = 0.003; Da29-2: rs = 0.729, p = 0.002],whereas their second dimension correlates with S / (L + LV)[Da29-1: rs = 0.85, p G 0.001; Da29-2: rs = 0.825, p G0.001]. The lower left panel of Figure 4B shows the MDSsolution for cDa29’s normal son, N29. As expected, hedoes not separate the X and Y subsets. His first dimensioncorrelates with the two normal ratios S / (L + M) [rs =0.818, p G 0.001] and L / (L + M) [rs = 0.911, p G 0.001],but his second dimension does not correlate significantlywith L / (L + LV) [rs = 0.07, p = 0.79] or with any othermodeled dimension and probably represents only noise.Of special interest is the MDS solution for the mother,

cDa29 (see Figure 4B, lower right panel). Although hersolution is rotated relative to those of her anomalous sons,she clearly separates the X and Y stimulus subsets. Herfirst dimension correlates, as expected, with the coneratios of the color normal space S / (L + M) [rs = 0.807,

Figure 4. Multidimensional scaling experiment. (A) Chromaticitiesof 15 colored discs modeled in a color space for a standardnormal observer (lower panel) and an analogous space for ahypothetical deuteranomalous observer (upper panel). The X(blue circles) and Y (red circles) stimulus subsets are separatedon the deuteranomalous dimension L / (L + LV) but form a singlearray and are intermingled on the normal dimensions L / (L + M)and S / (L + M). (They are also intermingled on the normal anddeuteranomalous luminance dimensions L + M and L + LV). (B)Multidimensional scaling solutions for the two deuteranomaloussons of cDa29 (top), for her normal son (bottom left) and forcDa29 herself (bottom right). Her normal son does not separatethe two subsets of stimuli that are clearly separated by hisdeuteranomalous brothers. His solution is horseshoe shaped andhis data might be adequately described by a one-dimensionalsolution (Rodieck, 1977). cDa29 separates the X and Y subsets,but the second dimension of her solution corresponds to the !rstdimension of the solutions for her deuteranomalous sons, thusleading to a rotation of her space. (C–J) MDS solutions for allother heterozygous mother and son pairs tested. Results fordeuteranomalous sons are always represented in the upperpanel. Correlations with color dimensions are given in Table 1.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 8

Page 9: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

p G 0.001] and L / (L + M) [rs = 0.932, p G 0.001].However, her second dimension correlates with the firstdimension of her anomalous sons [Da29-1: rs = 0.771, p G0.001; Da29-2: rs = 0.65, p = 0.009] and also with thetheoretical deuteranomalous dimension L / (L + LV) [rs =0.825, p G 0.001]. Thus, this heterozygote is capable both

of the discriminations made by her anomalous sons and ofthose made by her normal son.How unusual is cDa29’s ability to discriminate along the

“anomalous” axis? Recall that none of 16 control partic-ipants separated the X and Y stimulus subsets or exhibited adimension that correlated significantly with L / (L + LV).

Table 1. Data for nine carriers of deuteranomaly and their sons. The !rst three horizontal sections show the carriers’ genotypesand corresponding peak sensitivities according to both Asenjo et al. (1994) and Merbs and Nathans (1992a). Also shown are theanomaloscope mid-points and matching ranges for both sons and mothers and their respective color dimensions on the MDS test. TheDNA for cDa35 and cDa49 degraded before analyses and therefore data are unavailable.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 9

Page 10: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

Besides cDa29, three further carriers (cDa14, cDa20, andcDa50; all with sons who did exhibit two-dimensionalMDS solutions) had a dimensionValways the second onein the two-dimensional MDS solutionVthat correlatedsignificantly with the theoretical anomalous axis [0.643 ers e 0.711, p G 0.01]. However, these carriers differedfrom cDa29 in that they exhibited a localized region oferrors on our Rayleigh discrimination test.

Molecular genetic analysis

Saliva samples were obtained from all carriers and theirmale offspring, and the X chromosome opsin arrays weresequenced.

Methods

DNA from saliva samples was prepared using theOraGene DNA Purification Kit and protocol. Analysis ofthe sequence of opsin genes was performed accordingto the procedure described by Oda, Ueyama, Nishida,Tanabe, and Yamade (2003). Briefly, the first genes andthe downstream genes were independently amplified bylong-range polymerase chain reaction (PCR). The PCRproducts were used as templates to separately amplify theexons. The secondary PCR products were sequenceddirectly by Dye Terminator Cycle Sequencing Kit and aPRISM 310 Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems. FosterCity CA, USA). The primers and conditions used toamplify the exons are given by Deeb, Hayashi, Windericks,and Yamaguchi (2000).

Results

In Table 1, all nine carriers who participated in the MDStest are listed. The first section shows their genotypes, thesecond and third sections show the inferred 1max values oftheir M, LV, and two L-cone photopigment variants. Twosets of values are given: one is taken from Merbs andNathans (1992a) the other from Asenjo, Rim, and Oprian(1994). For a discussion on the use of the two sets, pleasesee the Appendix A. Also shown in the table are the mid-points and ranges on the anomaloscope for sons andmothers as well as their performance on the MDS test.The inferred separation of the two pigments of the deutansons ranges from 12 nm to as little as 4 nm, even thoughall these sons were selected for having restricted matchingranges on the anomaloscope.Figure 5 shows the inferred gene arrays of cDa29 and

her three sons. The opsin arrays of her normal (N29) andanomalous sons (Da29-1 and Da29-2) suggest that sheherself has a hybrid gene in which exons 1–4 exhibit thenormal M sequence and exons 5–6 exhibit the normalL sequence and that, in addition to this hybrid gene, shehas normal L and M pigment genes. These inferencesfrom her sons’ X chromosomes were confirmed by directsequencing of her own DNA.Table 2 shows the full amino acid sequences for the first

and second genes of cDa29’s normal and deuteranomaloussons. We note one curiosity in the sequence of the firstgene of the deuteranomalous sons: the sequence is that ofa wild-type long-wave gene (Nathans et al., 1986) exceptthat at site 153, leucine is replaced by methionine. Thelatter is characteristic of the wild-type middle-wavesequence, but it is not thought to affect the 1max of thephotopigment (Asenjo et al., 1994).

General discussion

Carriers of deuteranomaly constitute about 10% of allwomen (Pokorny, Smith, Verriest, & Pinckers, 1979), butmost carriers in our sample showed little evidence of asalient tetrachromatic signal. Our one candidate for strongtetrachromacy is cDa29. Is there one and only one criticalfactor determining her phenotype? One important factorought to be the spectral separation of her photopigments.Her hybrid (LV) gene draws exons 1–4 from the normal Msequence and exons 5–6 from the normal L sequence (seeFigure 5 and Table 2). In addition, she has normal M and Lgenes. From the in vitro experiments of Merbs and Nathans(1992a), we should expect the corresponding M, LV, andL photopigments to peak at approximately 530, 545, and557 nm, giving a large spectral separation of 12 nmbetween LV and L. (The alternative in vitro estimates ofAsenjo et al. (1994) give different 1max values of 532, 551and 563 nm for the M, LV and L photopigments, but a

Figure 5. Opsin gene arrays in the candidate tetrachromat cDa29and her three sons. The two deuteranomalous sons (Da29-1 andDa29-2) exhibit a long-wave gene that encodes an opsin withnormal peak sensitivity (but see text for further discussion) and ahybrid gene in which exons 1–4 correspond to the M sequenceand exons 5–6 correspond to the L sequence. The third, middle-wave gene in the array is thought not to be expressed. Forinferred peak sensitivities of the corresponding photopigments,please see Table 1.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 10

Page 11: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 11

Page 12: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 12

Page 13: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 13

Page 14: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

similar separation of 12 nm between LV and L). In anunselected population, carriers with such optimal spacingmay be uncommon. We note that the carrier with thesecond strongest correlation with the anomalous dimen-sion (L / (L + LV) in the MDS test (cDa50) has the samelarge inferred separation between her pigments. However, alarge separation of 1max does not seem to be sufficient for

exceptional performance on our tests. For example cDa15has an inferred spectral separation of 11 nm yet does notshow unusual performance on the Rayleigh discriminationtest or the MDS test. Conversely, cDa20 has an inferredpigment separation of only 4 nm, but she shows a signif-icant correlation with the anomalous dimension (see Table 1)and her son’s anomaloscope matching range is small.

Table 2. Amino acid sequences for the !rst and second genes of N29, Da29-1, and Da29-2.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 14

Page 15: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

In addition to spectral separation, two other factors mustplay an important role. The first is the relative number ofdifferent types of cone and the weightings given to themby the midget ganglion cells. Cone ratios vary consid-erably between individuals, and there is some evidence innormals and heterozygotes that color discrimination ispoorer when there are disproportionate numbers of L andM cones (Gunther & Dobkins, 2002; Hood, Mollon,Purves, & Jordan, 2006). Deutan carriers specifically areexpected to have a greater imbalance of L to M cones (infavor of L cones) and their chromatic discrimination alongthe L / (L + M) axis in MacLeod Boynton space has beenshown to be less precise compared to protan carriers andnormal controls (Hood et al., 2006). However, Hood et al.(2006) emphasize that this relatively poorer performanceon a single dimension in color space does not rule out theexistence of a separate channel that may give weight tothe deuteranomalous signal and they speculate that thissignal may be “most salient in those heterozygotes whohave few M cones and for whom the normal L/M signal isweak” (p. 2898). It is of interest here that cDa29 was oneof the participants in this earlier study and, although L/Mcone ratios were not measured directly, there is a measureof her relative sensitivity to the red and green phosphorsused as stimuli in this study (for details, see Hood et al.,2006). cDa29’s performance on their luminance matchingtest is shown in Figure 6 together with the group data ofHood et al., and it suggests that her cone mosaic isdominated by cones sensitive to long wavelengths: sheneeded less of the red phosphor to equate luminance whenthe phosphors were of equal objective luminance for theJudd1951 standard observer (Smith & Pokorny, 1996).

Finally, the optical density of the photopigments shouldbe considered as a possible factor in color discrimination.If a photopigment is present at a low density, itsabsorption spectrum becomes narrower, owing to theabsence of self-screening (Brindley, 1953; Knowles &Dartnall, 1977). It has already been suggested thatanomalous trichromats may gain relatively good discrim-ination by comparing the photon catches in two pigmentsthat have similar 1max values but different optical densities(He & Shevell, 1995; Neitz, Neitz, He, & Shevell, 1999;Thomas & Mollon, 2004). Similarly, Elsner, Waldvogel,and Burns (1989) found two deuteranomalous observerswhose behavior on a color matching task could only beexplained by assuming an increased optical density inaddition to shifts in their photopigment 1max values. Inthis context, we note the substitution of methionine forleucine at site 153 of the long-wave pigment of Da29-1and Da29-2 (see Table 2): it is conceivable that thissubstitution alters the effective optical density of the mole-cule without changing its 1max.Given individual variation in spectral separation, cone

ratios, and optical densities, we might expect carriers todisplay varying degrees of behavioral tetrachromacy,according to the relative amplifications of the normaland the anomalous chromatic signals reaching the cortex.In most cases, the “anomalous” signal may be masked bya much stronger L/M signal or may be too weak duringontogeny to recruit an independent subset of post-receptoral channels (Wachtler et al., 2007).Deuteranomalous color vision affects about 5% of men

in Caucasian populations and it is of interest to ask whythis anomalous phenotype is maintained. In the case ofplatyrrhine primates, it is plausible that polymorphic colorvision is maintained by a heterozygous advantage to thefemales, who gain an additional dimension of colordiscrimination (Mollon et al., 1984). It has sometimesbeen supposed that a visual advantage to the hetero-zygotesVe.g., in judging from complexions the fitnessof a potential partner or the health of a childVmightmaintain tetrachromacy in our species (Mollon & Jordan,1988). However, the rarity of behavioral tetrachromacysuggests that the advantage, if it is to do with color vision,may be to the sons themselves (Bosten et al., 2005).

Conclusions

In summary, among our sample of participants, we havefound only one person, a carrier of deuteranomaly, whosatisfies the criteria for behavioral tetrachromacy on allour tests. Firstly, she could not make a match on theclinical anomaloscope. Secondly, when asked to discrim-inate a red/green mixture from a monochromatic orange,there was no combination of R670 / (R670 + G546) and

Figure 6. Relative sensitivity to red and green phosphors used byHood et al. (2006) for two groups of carriers and two controlgroups. Added as a single participant (red circle) is cDa29, whoserelative sensitivity indicates a retinal mosaic that is dominated bycones sensitive to long wavelengths. Error bars are T1 standarddeviations.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 15

Page 16: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

luminance where her responses became either slow orinaccurate. Thirdly, her perceptual color space exhibitedthe L / (L + LV) dimension, which she shares with herdeuteranomalous sons. However, while molecular analysisof this participant reveals that she has three well-spacedphotopigments in the long-wave spectral region, it isquestionable whether this in itself is sufficient to conferstrong tetrachromacy.

Appendix A

Modeling MDS performance for our carrierpopulation

Within our sample of carriers, our molecular geneticresults show that there are several alleles for theanomalous pigment. This individual variability causesdifferences in the peak sensitivity of the photopigmentmolecule. Although we do not know how other factors

(i.e. optical density, lens and macular pigment) might varyin our sample, it is reasonable to expect that individualvariation in the peak sensitivity of anomalous and normalphotopigments will affect performance on our MDS test.The test was constructed (Bosten et al., 2005) so that pairsof X and Y chips would be metameric for the Smith andPokorny (1975) normal observer and would be clearlydistinguishable by the anomalous observer of DeMarcoet al. (1992). For other observers, the two sets of stimuliwould be expected to depart from metamerism to a greateror lesser degree. In this Appendix, we assess how well adeuteranomalous signal would separate the X and Y chipsin the case of cDa29 and other carriers in our sample.From each genotyped carrier’s opsin genes, we sought

to estimate the peak sensitivity of the M, L, and LVopsins.There are several strategies by which this can be achieved.Full spectral sensitivities are available from Sharpe et al.(1998), obtained by psychophysical methods from deuter-anopes with known hybrid genotypes. But since ourcandidate tetrachromat’s (cDa29) genotype was not partof their sample, we decided against using these spectralsensitivities.Instead we simulated cone fundamentals for each of our

carriers using the following method: We first used Lamb’s(1995) formula for simulating cone fundamentals of agiven peak sensitivity:

S 1! " # 1

exp a Aj 1max1

! "$ exp b Bj 1max

1

! "$ exp c Cj 1max

1

! "$ D

# $

!A1"

where S(1) is the sensitivity of the simulated photopig-ment as a function of wavelength, a = 70, b = 28.5, andc = j14.1, A = 0.880, B = 0.924, and C = 1.104. We thenaccounted for self-screening (Brindley, 1953). Since it isnot known how anomalous photopigments vary in opticaldensity (see Discussion), we assumed the same value of0.5 for all photopigments: S(1) = 1–10(j0.5*s(1)).

Figure A2. Chromaticities of 15 X and Y stimulus discs modeled in cDa29’s perceptual color space.

Figure A1. Simulated cone fundamentals for cDa29.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 16

Page 17: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

We accounted for macular pigment using data providedby Bone et al. (1992) and for lens using data provided byvan Norren and Vos (1974).Two data sets are available for estimating photopigment

peak sensitivities from known genotypes. Merbs andNathans (1992a, 1992b) have estimated the absorptionspectra of anomalous and normal photopigments insuspension by photopigment bleaching absorption spec-troscopy. Asenjo et al. (1994) have estimated peaksensitivities for various L–M hybrid pigments by a similarmethod. These two data sets disagree markedly. Forexample, Merbs and Nathans give the peak sensitivity ofthe L photopigment with serine at site 180 as 556.7 nm,while Asenjo et al. give it as 563 nm. We tried both datasets in our model but chose the data given by Merbs andNathans because the reconstructed M and L peaksensitivities corresponded to those of the Smith–Pokornycone fundamentals (Smith & Pokorny, 1975).We also simulated a spectral sensitivity curve for the S-

cones, assuming that the peak sensitivity of the S photo-pigment (without correction for pre-receptoral filteringand self-screening) was 426.3 nm (Merbs & Nathans,1992b). Again, our choice was based on the goodcorrespondence to the Smith–Pokorny cone fundamentals(Smith & Pokorny, 1975) used to construct the MDS test.An example of our simulated cone fundamentals (forsubject cDa29) is given in Figure A1.Using cone fundamentals modeled for each carrier, we

plotted the positions of our MDS stimuli in a normalchromaticity diagram with the axes L / (L + M) and S /(L + M) and in an analogous anomalous chromaticitydiagram with the axes L / (L + LV) and S / (L + LV). Ourcarriers should have access to the signals represented inboth chromaticity diagrams. An example of these recon-structed chromaticity diagrams for subject cDa29 is shownin Figure A2.The X and Y stimulus subsets are separated along L /

(L + LV) dimensions of all the chromaticity diagrams mod-eled for our carriers, but not along the normal L / (L + M)dimensions. This is to be expected if the modeledfundamentals approximate the DeMarco et al. (1992) andSmith and Pokorny (1975) fundamentals from which thetest was designed. Our modeling predicts that of all ourcarriers, those with cDa29’s genotype should have themost salient L / (L + LV) dimension on our MDS test, sincethis genotype leads to the greatest separation between theX and Y stimulus subsets in our model.The same model can be used to address the question of

whether a woman who is not a carrier for anomaloustrichromacy, but who is heterozygous for the serine/alanine polymorphism at site 180 (Winderickx et al.,1992) might show tetrachromacy on our MDS test. Wefound that for such an observer, the X and Y stimulussubsets are indeed separated along the axis Lser / (Lser +Lala), but the size of the difference is less than half of thatpresent along the L / (L + LV) axis for cDa29’s genotype.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Leverhulme Trustgrant F/00125/K (G.J., J.D.M. & S.S.D.), the WellcomeTrust JIF grant 058711/Z/99/Z/LA/ra/ad (G.J.), and theNational Institutes of Health grant EY008395 (S.S.D.).We thank N. Atkinson, A. Whitmore, L. Purves, and L. Fufor experimental assistance and W. Smith for programmingthe dynamic mask.

Commercial relationships: none.Corresponding author: Gabriele Jordan.Email: [email protected]: Newcastle University, Institute of Neuroscience,Henry Wellcome Building, Framlington Place, Newcastleupon Tyne, NE2 4HH, UK.

References

Asenjo, A. B., Rim, J., & Oprian, D. D. (1994). Moleculardeterminants of human red/green color discrimina-tion. Neuron, 12, 1131–1138.

Bone, R., Landrum, J., & Cains, A. (1992). Opticaldensity spectra of the macular pigment in vivo andvitro. Vision Research, 32, 105–110.

Bosten, J. M., Robinson, J. D., Jordan, G., & Mollon, J. D.(2005). Multidimensional scaling reveals a colordimension unique to ‘color-deficient’ observers. Cur-rent Biology, 15, R950–R952.

Brindley, G. S. (1953). The effects of colour vision onadaptation to very bright lights. The Journal ofPhysiology, 122, 332–350.

Cornsweet, T. N. (1970). Visual Perception. New York:Academic Press.

Dacey, D. M. (1999). Primate retina: Cell types, circuitsand color opponency. Progress in Retinal EyeResearch, 18, 737–763.

Deeb, S. S., Hayashi, T., Windericks, J., & Yamaguchi, T.(2000). Molecular analysis of human red/green visualpigment gene locus: Relationship to color vision.Methods in Enzymology, 316, 651–670.

DeMarco, P., Pokorny, J., & Smith, V. C. (1992). Full-spectrum cone sensitivity functions for X-chromosome-linked anomalous trichromats. Journal of the OpticalSociety of America A, 9, 1465–1476.

De Vries, H. (1948). The fundamental response curves ofnormal and abnormal dichromatic and trichromaticeyes. Physica, 14, 367–380.

Elsner, A. E., Waldvogel, J., & Burns, S. A. (Eds.) (1989).High illuminance color matching in anomalous

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 17

Page 18: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

trichromacy (vol. IX). Dordrecht: Kluwer AcademicPublishers.

Franceschetti, A. (1928). Die Bedeutung der Einstellungs-breite am Anomaloskop fur die Diagnose dereinzelnen Typen der Farbensinnstorungen, nebstBemerkungen uber ihren Vererbungsmodus.Schweizerische Medizinische Wochenschrift, 52,1273–1279.

Gunther, K. L., & Dobkins, K. R. (2002). Individualdifferences in chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitiv-ity are constrained by the relative number of L- versusM-cones in the eye. Vision Research, 42, 1367–1378.

Hayashi, T., Motulsky, A. G., & Deeb, S. S. (1999).Position of a ‘green-red’ hybrid gene in the visualpigment array determines colour-vision phenotype.Nature Genetics, 22, 90–93.

He, J. C., & Shevell, S. K. (1995). Variation in colormatching and discrimination among deuteranomaloustrichromats: Theoretical implications of smalldifferences in photopigments. Vision Research, 35,2579–2588.

Hood, S. M., Mollon, J. D., Purves, L., & Jordan, G.(2006). Color discrimination in carriers of colordeficiency. Vision Research, 46, 2894–2900.

Jacobs, G. H., Williams, G. A., Cahill, H., & Nathans, J.(2007). Emergence of novel color vision in miceengineered to express a human cone photopigment.Science, 315, 1723–1725.

Jordan, G., & Mollon, J. D. (1993). A study of womenheterozygous for color deficiencies. Vision Research,33, 1495–1508.

Knowles, A., & Dartnall, H. J. A. (1977). The eye (vol. 2B).New York: Academic Press.

Kruskal, J. B. (1964). Multidimensional scaling byoptimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis.Psychometrika, 29, 1–27.

Lamb, T. (1995). Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities:Common shape in the long-wavelength region. VisionResearch, 35, 3083–3091.

Lennie, P., Haake, P. W., & Williams, D. R. (1991). Thedesign of chromatically opponent receptive fields. InM. S. Landy & J. A. Movshon (Eds.), Computationalmodels of visual processing (pp. 71–82). Cambridge,MA: MIT Press.

Linsker, R. (1990). Perceptual neural organization: Someapproaches based on network models and informationtheory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 257–281.

Lyon, M. F. (1961). Gene action in the X-chromosome ofthe mouse (Mus musculus L.). Nature, 190, 372–373.

MacLeod, D. I. A. (1985). Receptoral constraints oncolour appearance. In D. Ottoson & S. Zeki (Eds.),

Central and peripheral mechanisms of colour vision(pp. 103–116). London: MacMillan.

MacLeod, D. I. A., & Boynton, R. M. (1979). Chroma-ticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli ofequal luminance. Journal of the Optical Society ofAmerica, 69, 1183–1186.

Merbs, S. L., & Nathans, J. (1992a). Absorption spectra ofthe hybrid pigments responsible for anomalous colorvision. Science, 258, 464–466.

Merbs, S. L., & Nathans, J. (1992b). Absorption spectra ofhuman cone pigments. Nature, 356, 433–435.

Mollon, J. D. (1997). ‘Iaus dreyerley Arten vonMembranen oder Molekulen’: George Palmer’s leg-acy. In C. R. Cavonius (Ed.), Colour vision deficien-cies: XIII (pp. 3–20). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Mollon, J. D., Bowmaker, J. K., & Jacobs, G. H. (1984).Variations of colour vision in a New World primatecan be explained by polymorphism of retinal photo-pigments. Proceedings of the Royal Society ofLondon B, 222, 373–399.

Mollon, J. D., & Jordan, G. (1988). Eine evolutionareInterpretation des menschlichen Farbensehens. DieFarbe, 35/36, 139–170.

Nagy, A. L., MacLeod, D. I., Heyneman, N. E., &Eisner, A. (1981). Four cone pigments in womenheterozygous for color deficiency. Journal of theOptical Society of America, 71, 719–722.

Nathans, J., Piantanida, T. P., Eddy, R. L., Shows, T. B.,& Hogness, D. S. (1986). Molecular genetics ofinherited variation in human color vision. Science,232, 203–232.

Neitz, J., Neitz, M., He, J. C. A., & Shevell, S. K. (1999).Trichromatic color vision with only two spectrallydistinct photopigments. Nature Neuroscience, 2,884–888.

Oda, S., Ueyama, H., Nishida, Y., Tanabe, S., & Yamade, S.(2003). Analysis of L-cone/M-cone visual pigment genearrays in females by long-range PCR. Vision Research,43, 489–495.

Pokorny, J., Smith, V. C., Verriest, G., & Pinckers, A. J.L. G. (1979). Congenital and acquired color visiondefects. New York: Grune & Stratton.

Rayleigh, L. (1881). Experiments on colour. Nature, 25,64–66.

Regan, B. C., Reffin, J. P., & Mollon, J. D. (1994).Luminance noise and the rapid determination ofdiscrimination ellipses in colour deficiency. VisionResearch, 34, 1279–1299.

Rodieck, R. W. (1977). Metric of color borders. Science,197, 1195–1196.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 18

Page 19: The dimensionality of color vision in carriers of ...vision.psychol.cam.ac.uk/jdmollon/papers/JordanDeebBostenMollonOn... · The dimensionality of color vision in ... a forced-choice

Sharpe, L. T., Stockman, A., Jagle, H., Knau, H.,Klaussen, G., Reitner, A., et al. (1998). Red, green,and red-green hybrid pigments in the human retina:Correlations between deduced protein sequences andpsychophysically measured spectral sensitivities.Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 10053–10069.

Shepard, R. N. (1962). The analysis of proximities:Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distancefunction. Psychometrika, 27, 219–246.

Shevell, S., He, J. C., Kainz, P., Neitz, J., & Neitz, M.(1998). Relating color discrimination to photopig-ment genes in deutan observers. Vision Research, 38,3371–3376.

Smith, V. C., & Pokorny, J. (1975). Spectral sensitivityof the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and500 nm. Vision Research, 15, 161–171.

Smith, V. C., & Pokorny, J. (1996). The design and use ofa cone chromaticity space: A tutorial. Color Researchand Application, 21, 375–383.

Teplitz, R. L. (1965). Sex chromatin of cone cells ofhuman retina. Science, 150, 1828–1829.

Thomas, P. B., & Mollon, J. D. (2004). Modelling theRayleigh match. Visual Neuroscience, 21, 477–482.

van Norren, D., & Vos, J. (1974). Spectral transmissionof the human ocular media. Vision Research, 14,1237–1244.

Waaler, G. H. M. (1927). Uber Erblichkeitsverhaltnisseder verschiedenen Arten von angeborener Rotgrun-blindheit. Zeitschrift fur induktive Abstammungs- undVererbungslehre, 45, 279–333.

Wachtler, T., Doi, E., Lee, T.-W., & Sejnowski, T. J.(2007). Cone selectivity derived from the responsesof the retinal cone mosaic to natural scenes. Journalof Vision, 7(8):6, 1–14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/7/8/6, doi:10.1167/7.8.6. [PubMed][Article]

Winderickx, J., Lindsey, D. T., Sanocki, E., Teller, D. Y.,Motulsky, A. G., & Deeb, S. S. (1992). A Ser/Alapolymorphism in the red photopigment underliesvariation in colour matching among colour-normalindividuals. Nature, 356, 431–433.

Journal of Vision (2010) 10(8):12, 1–19 Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon 19