the effect of mental training on precision tasks in tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving...

201
The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis and Soccer - A Study on Educational Technology Dissertation zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Doktors der Sozialwissenschaften (Dr.rer.soc.) vorgelegt von Khaled Hegazy Universität Konstanz Geisteswissenschaftliche Sektion Fachbereich Geschichte und Soziologie / Sportwissenschaft Konstanz 2012 1. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Alexander Woll 2. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Markus Gruber

Upload: others

Post on 19-Mar-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis and Soccer

- A Study on Educational Technology

Dissertation zur Erlangung des

akademischen Grades eines

Doktors der Sozialwissenschaften (Dr.rer.soc.)

vorgelegt von

Khaled Hegazy

Universität Konstanz

Geisteswissenschaftliche Sektion

Fachbereich Geschichte und Soziologie / Sportwissenschaft

Konstanz 2012

1. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Alexander Woll 2. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Markus Gruber

Page 2: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

2

Abstract

The aim of the study was to test specifically whether the precision of tennis

serves and, similarly, penalty soccer kicks as representative closed skills can

be improved through mental training. Further research questions illuminate

which potential influencing variables play the biggest role in determining the

efficacy of the mental training intervention in this study. These variables

include imagery ability; commitment; the level of somatic fear, anxiety, and

confidence; ability to concentrate; sex, and age. The study used experimental

design with a pre- and post- test and a control group. Data were collected

from a sample of 60 (38 male, 22 female) amateur athletes with a high skill

level in tennis, ages 12 to 35 in Germany and 54 (male) soccer athletes

playing in the third league, ages 14 to 22 in Germany. All subjects were

registered with the state tennis and soccer federations of Baden Württemberg,

Germany. The subjects for tennis and soccer each completed the

questionnaire and were divided into two groups: The intervention group (IG)

received both technical practice and mental training units. The control group

(CG) received only technical practice (Normal training) for the same time

period. Analysis of covariance showed a highly significant improvement for

the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty

kicking the effect of mental training on precision was only marginally

significant (p=.059). Findings suggest that self-confidence seems to have an

impact on improving performance when the athlete has more control over his

movements, such as in closed skills. Furthermore, this effect may result from

more advanced players having already achieved high serving or kicking

Page 3: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

3

speeds and so training focuses on accuracy; while younger players focus

more on speed. Moreover, the interaction effect between time and sex is not

significant, but shows a tendency, that the improvement in the women’s

precision was higher than that of the men. However, there is no significant

correlation among imagery ability; commitment; the level of somatic fear;

anxiety and the ability to concentrate.

Page 4: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

4

Acknowledgements

First of all, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to Professor

Herbert Haag who helped me to begin my PhD studies at Konstanz

University, under the supervision of Professor Alexander Woll. Thank you for

encouraging and teaching me from the beginning, the art of research

methodology for sport, and thank you for also being a great friend.

I would like to express my sincere gratitude to my supervisor Professor

Alexander Woll for all his help. Thank you for taking me on as a PhD student

in your program although the duration of my scholarship was very limited, but

you always encouraged me to continue searching, and sent me to many

conferences to set me on the right path. My professor, I am always learning

from you how I can always be a positive role model, thank you very much for

all your effort, it is greatly appreciated.

I would like to extend a very special thank you to Dr. Darko Jekauc for

all of his support and for assisting me in completing this thesis in a timely

manner, and thank you for letting me to participate in most of your lectures

(Sport psychology, statistics, tennis training for advanced/ beginners), your

support is greatly appreciated, as well as for your close friendship.

I would like also to extend warm thanks to the teaching staff, especially

Dr. Filip Mess for all his efforts in facilitating my study, including

transportation, financial support and cameras for my research. Many thanks to

Dr. Hagen Wäsche for lending me the wireless recorder to record my

interviews with the players in my pilot study, and for all his important advice.

Page 5: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

5

Many thanks also to Dr. Nadine Mewes who gave me her time and comments

on my statistics.

I would also like to express my appreciation to Professor Markus

Gruber for his effort reading my dissertation.

I would also like to express many thanks to Jonathan Bass from the

USA, the student in the department of economics in the University of

Konstanz for his correction for my dissertation language.

Finally, I am incredibly grateful for the wonderful support and constant

encouragement from my family. To my Mum and Dad, I could not have asked

for two greater role models in life, I have learnt a great deal about the

meaning of life from the both of you. Thank you. Above all, I thank my wife

Adina Muresan for her love and support through a very trying and busy time,

and for supporting me in all aspects of my life.

Page 6: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

6

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract.......................................................................................................... 2

Acknowledgements....................................................................................... 4

List of Figures................................................................................................ 8

List of Tables................................................................................................ 10

1. Introduction.............................................................................................. 14

1.1 Purpose of the Study..................................................................... 15

1.2 Thesis Outline............................................................................... 16

2. Theoretical Foundations of Mental Training......................................... 18

2.1 What is Imagery?.......................................................................... 18

2.2 Effectiveness of Imagery............................................................... 26

2.3 How Imagery Works?.................................................................... 37

2.4 The Use of Imagery....................................................................... 43

2.5 Mental Training Studies in Tennis……………………….......……. 54

2.6 Mental training Studies in Soccer……………….......…………….. 63

3. Representative Closed Motor Skills in Tennis and Soccer.................. 68

3.1 Tennis Serve……………………………………………….......…….. 68

3.2 Soccer Penalty Kick……………………………….…………......….. 78

4. Methods.................................................................................................... 86

4.1 Pilot Study………………………........……………………………….. 86

4.1.1 Tennis……………………………………………........…….. 86

4.1.2 Soccer………………………………………….......……….. 86

4.2 Hypotheses………………………………………………….......…… 88

Page 7: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

7

4.3 Design of the Study……………………………………….......…….. 89

4.4 Participants………………………………………………….......……. 90

4.5 Measurements……………………………………………….......…... 91

4.5.1 Tennis Precision Test (TPT)…………………….......……. 91

4.5.2 Penalty Precision Test (PPT)…………………….......…… 92

4.5.3 Questionnaire…………………………………….…........... 93

4.6 Implementation…………………………………………….......….... 101

4.7 Data evaluation………………………………………………........... 104

5. Results.................................................................................................... 105

5.1 Tennis Precision Test (TPT) and Penalty Precision Test

(PPT)……………………………………………………………...........… 105

5.2 The Imagery Ability………………………………………….......…. 109

5.3 The Commitment…………………………………………….......…. 112

5.4 Competition Fear Inventory Assessment (Wettkampf Angst

Inventar-State: WAI-S)………………………………………….......….. 115

5.5 The Concentration Test…………………………………........……. 117

5.6 Demographical and Personal Data……………………........……. 119

5.6.1 The Sex………………………………………………......... 119

5.6.2 The Age………………………………………………........ 122

6. Discussion…………………………………………………………….......… 125

7. Conclusion…………………………………………………………….......… 140

8. Zusammenfassung................................................................................ 142

References.................................................................................................. 144

Appendices................................................................................................. 180

Page 8: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

8

List of Figures

Figure 1. Summary of research support for the effectiveness of

imagery.......................................................................................................... 27

Figure 2. Experimental study with a control group, a ‘one-within+one-

between’ design............................................................................................. 89

Figure 3. An experimental study with a control group, effect of the

intervention can not be ‘biased’ by any natural development........................ 90

Figure 4. Target areas for the tennis precision test and the imagination

exercise………………………………………………………………………........ 92

Figure 5. Target areas for the penalty precision test and the imagination

exercise.......................................................................................................... 93

Figure 6. Example of editing and distribution of the items…………............... 94

Figure 7. Some questions which test imagery ability are presented….......... 95

Figure 8. Questions about the commitment of the subjects to the mental

training………………………………………………………………………........... 97

Figure 9. WAI-S, used in the questionnaire to measure somatic fear, anxiety

(unease) and confidence……………………………………..……..................... 98

Figure 10. Demographical and personal data of the subjects……….......… 100

Figure 11. The last page in the questionnaire………………………….......... 101

Figure 12. Results of Both Groups in the Pre-Test and Post-Test (TPT) in

Tennis…………………………………………………………………….......……107

Page 9: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

9

Figure 13. Results of Both Groups in the Pre-Test and Post-Test (PPT) in

Soccer........................................................................................................... 109

Figure 14. Improvement in tennis serve precision dependant on

sex................................................................................................................ 121

Figure 15. The effect of sex on mental training and precision

test……………………………………………………………………………........ 121

Page 10: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

10

List of Tables

Table 1. Results of the Intervention and Control Groups in Tennis Serve

Precision Test in Pre- and Post-Test (Descriptive Statistics)…................... 105

Table 2. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts in Tennis…………….............. 106

Table 3. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects in Tennis……………….......… 106

Table 4. Results of the Intervention and Control Group in Soccer Penalty Kick

Precision Test in Pre- and Post-Test (Descriptive statistics)....................... 107

Table 5. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts in Soccer............................... 108

Table 6. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects in Soccer................................ 108

Table 7. Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability of the Intervention and Control

Groups in Tennis.......................................................................................... 109

Table 8. Correlation Between Visual Imagery Ability, Number of Hits in the

TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group

in Tennis...................................................................................................... 110

Table 9. Correlation Between Visual Imagery Ability, Number of Hits in the

TPT Pre-test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in

Tennis.......................................................................................................... 110

Table 10. Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability in Intervention and Control

Groups in Soccer......................................................................................... 111

Table 11. Correlation Between Visual Imagery Ability, Number of Kicks in the

PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group

in Soccer...................................................................................................... 111

Page 11: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

11

Table 12. Correlation Between Visual Imagery Ability, Number of Kicks in the

PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in

Soccer.......................................................................................................... 112

Table 13. Comparison of Both Groups with Respect to Commitment in

Tennis.......................................................................................................... 112

Table 14. Comparison of Both Groups with Respect to Commitment in

Soccer.......................................................................................................... 113

Table 15. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-

Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in

Tennis.......................................................................................................... 113

Table 16. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-

Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control group in

Tennis.......................................................................................................... 114

Table 17. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Kicks in the PPT

Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in

Soccer.......................................................................................................... 114

Table 18. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Kicks in the PPT

Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in

Soccer.......................................................................................................... 115

Table 19. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of

Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the

Intervention Group in Tennis........................................................................ 115

Page 12: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

12

Table 20. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of

Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control

Group in Tennis........................................................................................... 116

Table 21. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of

Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the

Intervention Group in Soccer....................................................................... 116

Table 22. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of

Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the

Control Group in Soccer.............................................................................. 117

Table 23. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value with the Number of Hits in the

TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group

in Tennis...................................................................................................... 117

Table 24. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value With the Number of Hits in the

TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in

Tennis.......................................................................................................... 118

Table 25. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value with the Number of Kicks in the

PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group

in Soccer...................................................................................................... 118

Table 26. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value with the Number of Kicks in the

PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in

Soccer.......................................................................................................... 119

Table 27. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts in tennis............................... 119

Table 28. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects in tennis............................... 120

Table 29. Data of the intervention group in tennis....................................... 122

Page 13: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

13

Table 30. Data of the control group in tennis.............................................. 122

Table 31. Correlation between Age with the Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-

Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for all Subjects in

Tennis.......................................................................................................... 123

Table 32. Data of the intervention group in soccer...................................... 124

Table 33. Data of the control group in soccer.............................................. 124

Table 34. Correlation between Age with the Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-

Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for all Subjects in

Soccer.......................................................................................................... 124

Page 14: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

14

1. Introduction

The development and acquisition of psychological skills to enhance

performance within a sport context has been well documented (Fletcher &

Hanton, 2001; Parker & Lovell, 2009). One psychological skill that has

attracted considerable interest is mental imagery, with its use being reliably

reported among elite athletes by a number of researchers (Munroe et al.,

2000; Parker & Lovell, 2009). Mental training techniques have been largely

used by psychologists, coaches and athletes as a tool for improving learning

and performance in sports (Hall & Fishburne, 2010). Coaches have indicated

that they used imagery more than any other mental training technique, and

felt that imagery was the most useful technique they used with their athletes

(Bloom et al., 1997; Hall & Rodgers, 1989).

Humans are capable of imitating the motor actions of others, because

the mind “photographs” the motor skill and uses it as foundation of the

performance. Athletes use imagery for many different reasons, including skill

learning and practice, strategy development and rehearsal, competition

preparation, including familiarization with venues and mental warm-ups,

mental skill development and refinement. Imagery has also been shown to

help athletes cope with various sport stressors or obstacles, such as injuries,

heavy training, and distractions (Morris et al., 2005; White & Hardy, 1998).

Research has shown that preparatory imagery, or using imagery

immediately before a performance, can improve performance in strength

tasks and muscular endurance tasks (Vealey & Greenleaf, 2006). Research

has also concluded that mental training programs increase the importance

Page 15: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

15

that athletes place on using mental training techniques and strategies, as well

as their intentions to use these techniques and strategies (Brewer &

Shillinglaw, 1992; Gould et al, 1990; Grove et al., 1999; Vealey, 2007).

Furthermore, improvement in the following sport skills has been

documented through mental practice: tennis serving, volleyball serving,

soccer placekicking, basketball shooting, golf putting, figure skating,

swimming starts, dart throwing, alpine skiing, trampoline skills, diving,

competitive running, karate skills, field hockey performance, dance and rock

climbing (Vealey & Greenleaf, 2006). Proving this statement, Coelho et al.

(2007) examined the effect of imagery in the performance of open and closed

skills in tennis, and showed a significant improvement in the closed skills.

Specifically, it has been suggested that closed skills (e.g., serve in

tennis and penalty kick in soccer) allow time for the athlete to prepare and to

execute when ready (McLean & Richardson, 1994). Since closed skills tend to

be more affected by imagery, because they are completely under the athlete’s

control, the athlete can use imagery to visualize them precisely (Singer, 2000;

Coelho et al., 2007, 2008).

1.1 Purpose of the study

The present study seeks to test specifically whether the precision of

tennis serves, and similarly, soccer penalty kicks can be improved through

mental training. Tennis serves and soccer penalty kicks usually involve

closed-skills because the environment is relatively unchanging. The focus of

the current study investigates whether athletes in closed-skill sports utilize

imagery differently. Further research questions illuminate which potential

Page 16: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

16

influencing variables play the biggest role in determining the efficacy of the

mental training intervention of this study. Variables including imagery ability;

commitment; the level of somatic, anxiety, and confidence; ability to

concentrate; sex; and age were investigated to study the possible efficacy of a

four-week mental training intervention compared with a control group which

received only technical practice without mental training for the same time

period.

1.2 Thesis Outline

The next chapter explains the theoretical foundations of mental

training. These include the definitions of imagery, a closer description of the

mechanisms of action and the variables influencing the efficacy of the mental

training. In addition this chapter explains the use of imagery, not only in theory

but also in practice. Furthermore, a specific overview of mental training

studies in tennis and soccer is given in this chapter.

The established knowledge about tennis and soccer is reviewed in the

third chapter. This includes the representative closed motor skills (tennis

serve- soccer penalty kick) to be studied.

The Methods chapter, describes the pilot studies in tennis and soccer,

explains the hypotheses of the study as well as the design of the study. The

participants of the study are described, the descriptions of measurements

including the tennis precision test (TPT) and the penalty precision test (PPT)

as well as the questionnaire are explained, the implementation of the study

from the beginning until end and the data evaluation are also included.

Page 17: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

17

In chapter five the results of the study are presented. The statistics

were calculated for each of the two games (tennis serve and penalty kick in

soccer) in terms of the results of the precision test, the imagery ability, the

commitment, competition fear inventory assessment “Wettkampf Angst

Inventar-State: WAI-S”, the concentration test and the demographical,

personal data ”the sex, the age”.

Chapter six contains the discussion of the results with respect to the

hypotheses. The results are discussed and included in the context of the

relevant scientific literature. This chapter also identifies future research

directions. A critical issue of this study is also explained at the end of this

chapter.

Finally, the results of this study point to some theoretical and practical

conclusions in chapter seven. The summary this dissertation is discussed in

the German language in chapter eight and followed by references and the

appendices.

Page 18: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

18

2. Theoretical Foundations of Mental Training

2.1 What is Imagery?

Imagery is one of the most widely-researched topics in sport

psychology (Smith & Wright, 2008; Wakefield & Smith, 2009). All athletes

have the potential to increase their imagery abilities through systematic

procedure (Evans et al., 2004; Orlick & Partington, 1988; Rodgers et al.,

1991).

Jacobson (1930) was the first to show by using electromyography that

subjects showed muscle contractions when simply imaging a simple

movement of arm flexion. Later on, these findings were replicated and

confirmed by other studies (e.g., .Bird, 1984; Jody & Harris, 1990).

The first study of mental practice effects on motor skills took place in

1934 (Vandall et al., 1934). Since that time, a plethora of research has been

conducted in this area (Vealey & Greenleaf, 2006).

Williams (1995) improved the definition proposed by (Lang et al., 1980)

who had defined Imagery as a mental technique which sets mind and body to

respond correctly to a desired movement. White and Hardy (1998) explained

that imagery is a volitional experience in which an individual creates, or

recreates, a particular physical skill or situation through use of one or more of

the senses. Humans are capable of imitating the motor actions of others,

because the mind “photographs” the motor skill and uses it as foundation to

the performance. The imagery is based in the memory that we internally

experiment to reconstruct external events in our minds.

Page 19: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

19

The definition of imagery proposed by Lang and others (1980) was

improved by Williams (1994, p. 148) who stated “imagery is a mental

technique that programs the mind and body to respond optimally. It is the use

of all senses to recreate or create an experience in the mind.” He explained

that through imagery actions of others can be imitated because the mind

takes a picture of the skill which serve as a blueprint for performance. Imagery

is based on memory and is experienced internally by reconstructing external

events mentally. Williams also proposed that imagery is useful to re-create

one’s own performance after competition to evaluate performance and identify

strengths and weaknesses.

Mental practice, according to Corbin, is the “repetition of a task, without

observable movement, with the specific intent of learning” (1972, p. 94).

Mental practice may not use imagery at all; it could include non-image-based

strategies such as verbal rehearsal or self-talk. For the purposes of this book,

a distinction originally outlined by Murphy and Jowdy (1992) and discussed

again in Murphy and Martin (2002) provides a critical differentiating

framework. It defines imagery as a mental process and mental practice as a

non-physical rehearsal or practice technique used by athletes. Aligned with

this distinction is the definition of mental rehearsal as “the employment of

imagery to mentally practice an act” (Hardy et al., 1996, p. 28). The emphasis

here is also on mental rehearsal as a technique rather than imagery as a

process.

A number of years ago, Suinn (1976a, 1983, 1993) used the term

“imagery” in relation to his procedure known as visuomotor behaviour

Page 20: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

20

rehearsal (VMBR) The imagery of visuomotor behaviour rehearsal apparently

is more than sheer imagination. It is a well-controled copy of experience, a

sort of body-thinking similar to the powerful illusion of certain dreams at night.

Perhaps the major difference between such dreams and VMBR is that

imagery rehearsal is subject of conscious control, It is instructive to note that

Suinn (with his development of VMBR) was the first psychologist who

provided on-site services to Olympic athletes at the 1976 Olympic Games.

This technique really involves the use of relaxation prior imagery (including

imagery of the skill itself and imagery practice incorporating the context under

which the skill will be performed), and because relaxation has been

recommended prior to performing imagery to enhance its effectiveness, it is

really an imagery intervention. Suinn cited several case studies which

indicated that the use of VMBR can improve performance in sports such as

football (Titley, 1976), basketball (Lane, 1980), tennis (Noel, 1980) and skiing

(Suinn, 1976). Combined with the other more recent case study results, there

is a growing body of intervention evidence to support the effectiveness of

imagery in enhancing performance (Weinberg, 2008).

Studies have shown that almost all elite athletes intentionally employ

imagery. Further, most sport psychologists systematically aplly imagery in

their work with sport performers (e.g., De Francesco & Burke 1997; Gould et

al., 1989; Martin et al., 1999; Rushall & Lippman, 1998).

One mental rehearsal technique is imagery, which according to Lang et

al. (1980) “is a process that involves systematically rehearsing some behavior

imaging a specific motor skill.” It is also known as muscular memory.

Page 21: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

21

Furthermore, imagery is defined as the process which involves

systematic practice of a motor behaviour, by imagination of a specific motor

skill, also known as muscular memory, and one variable that has been shown

to influence participation, motivation, and performance in the physical activity

domains of motor learning, exercise and sport, is mental imagery (Hall, 2001).

Imagery has been defined also as “the ability to represent perceptual

states in the absence of the appropriate sensory input (Kosslyn et al., 2002, p.

342).”

Comprehensive reviews of the mental training literature have

demonstrated that Imagery is a method of mental practice efficient to the

enhancement of sports performance and motor learning (Woolfolk et al.,

1985; Paivio, 1985; Shaw & Goodfellow, 1997; Gammage et al., 2000; Taylor

& Shaw, 2002; Coelho et al., 2007; Greenspan & Feltz, 1989; Meyers et al.,

1996; Vealey, 1994).

Thus, when the movement is complex and requires more than one

focus of attention, the imagery used must follow an adequate protocol in order

to reach its goals and specificity.

Another important element related to mental skills training is how much

athletes and coaches use mental training strategies and techniques.

Research indicates that successful elite athletes (Durand-Bush & Salmela,

2002; Gould, Eklund, et al., 1993; Gould, Finch, et al., 1993) and coaches

(Bloom et al., 1997) use mental training techniques and strategies to help

them achieve success in sport.

Page 22: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

22

However, Heishman and Bunker (1989) found that although 81% of

elite athletes from various countries rated mental preparation as very

important, only 44% made frequent use of mental preparation strategies and

techniques. In addition, athletes tend to use mental training techniques more

in competition than in practice (Frey et al., 2003). Overall, this research

indicates that athletes believe in the efficacy of mental training, but most fail to

use it systematically as part of their physical training regiment (Vealey, 2007).

Imagery conducted for sports performance is referred to as sport

imagery, but can be used interchangeably with the boarder term, mental

imagery (Taylor & Wilson, 2005). Several other terms including mental

practice, mental rehearsal and visualization have also been used to refer to

various components of mental imagery in sport (Morris et al., 2005; Taylor &

Wilson, 2005; Weinberg & Gould, 2007).

The concept of mental imagery is used in many different domains and

contexts (cognitive psychology, sport psychology, education,

psychopedagogy, sciences, etc.) to indicate a representation process of static

or dynamic objects and past, present or future events, in the absence of

external stimuli (Chevalier, 1995; Decety, 1989; Denis, 1979, 1989, 1991;

Finke, 1989; Khaled, 2004; Richardson, 1999).

Furthermore, it has been suggested that in using mental imagery, “we

can be aware of ‘seeing’ an image, feeling imaginary movements, or

experiencing imaginary of smell, taste or sounds without experiencing the real

thing” (White & Hardy, 1998,p. 389). Simons (2000) presented an excellent

analysis of the process of using imagery as an psychological-skills training

Page 23: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

23

technique. The formation relevant to his definition of imagery emanated from

his conclusions about the manner in which athletes process imagery. Simons

described the process as follows: Imagery is intriguing for its close relationship

to perception and action. It is such a rich memory system, matching the

complexity of information presented by the environment and contained in the

execution of motor skills. Images bind personal thoughts and emotions to

experience, and they have qualities far beyond simple stimulus/response

propositions…Imagery can be creative, allowing one to experience attitudes

and actions mentally in ways that have not yet been encountered in real

performance.(Morris et al., 2005, p. 92).

Coaches often encourage athletes to use imagery to help facilitate the

improvement and learning of skills (Hall & Rodgers, 1989). As well, imagery is

often a central component in the mental training programs developed and

implemented by sport psychologists (e.g., Daw & Burton, 1994). Given the

recognized value and widespread use of imagery in sport, it is not surprising

that researchers have been interested in investigating why imagery works and

how athletes can use it effectively.

Sport imagery can be defined as using all senses to re-create or create

a sport experience in the mind with the goal of enhancing sport performance

during training and competition (Morris et al., 2005; Taylor & Wilson, 2005;

Weinberg & Gould, 2007). It was explained clearly that your brain recalls and

reconstructs pieces of information stored in your memory to build a

meaningful image. It is means that most athletes can recall previous

experiences in great vividness and detail through imagery because according

Page 24: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

24

to Hall (2001) said most imagery research has concerned the effect of the

cognitive rehearsal of sport skills on subsequent performance. This can be

explaining by this phenomenon; a softball player may recall what it feels like

hit or contact the pitched ball. Athletes can also create images of events yet to

occur by piercing together bits of information already stored in their memories.

There has been increased attention, interest, and research given to the

role of imagery in physical activity, particularly sport. In the sports arena, the

value of mental imagery as a performance enhancing technique is well

recognized by athletes, coaches, and sport psychologists (Hall, 1995).

Despite this, there has been limited research specifically focused on the use

of mental imagery in physical education (e.g., Anderson, 1997; Goudas &

Giannoudis, 2008). However, a great deal of research on mental imagery has

been conducted in the motor learning (Hall et al., 1992; Holmes & Collins,

2001) and sport psychology (In Morris et al., 2005).

Athletes report using imagery for both motivational (imagining winning

a medal) and cognitive functions (e.g., imagining strategies; Hall et al., 1998).

Imagery, in the context of sport may be considered as the voluntary or

involuntary creation. Or recreation of an experience generated from memorial

information involving quasisensorial, quasiperceptional, and quasi-affective

characteristics which may occur in the absence of the real stimulus

antecedents normally associated with the actual experience, and which may

have physiological and psychological effects on the imager, modified version

of (Morris et al., 2005). Furthermore, (Watt et al., 2002) defined imagery use

as the manner in which people imagine themselves in ways that can lead to

Page 25: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

25

learning and developing skills and can facilitate performance of those skills. It

is normally assessed in terms of its cognitive and motivational attributes

(Fauzee et al., 2009).

Murphy and Martin (2002) provided a descriptive outline of imagery use

in relation to their overview of Martin et al. (1999) applied model of imagery

use in sport. They proposed that imagery use is an athlete’s “use of imagery”

to achieve a variety of cognitive, behavioural, and affective changes. Another

important feature of this model is that it centeres on imagery content (i.e.,

what the athlete images) as a key determinant of these changes” (p. 418).

Like “imagery ability”, “imagery use” is a term that has been developed in

relation to its measurement; consequently definitions will highlight those

characteristics that the measures of the attribute evaluate.

Both athletes and exercisers use imagery to aid in their performances

(Haunsenblas et al., 1999). But not all athletes are able to verbally describe

exactly how they use imagery, but some can. Clearly, imagery has been

useful for great athletes. This fact was supported by Anderson (2000), the

imagery system can be used to help person meet some personal or

performance goal, but it most effective when it is used for a specific purpose.

(Fauzee et al., 2009).

Imagery use not only for replaying but also can be creating new

experiences. Or in the other meaning, imagery is a product of your memory

system. It meanings that athletes need to be able to manipulate the content of

their imagery to create images that do what they want to do. Without strong

imagery control, athletes especially those low in self-confident my find

Page 26: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

26

themselves repeating mistakes in their imagery. Some example of weak

imagery is a softball player might see herself making critical error, or a runner

might experience an overwhelming sense of fatigue in the last leg of a race.

Such negative images are counterproductive, serving only to hurt

performance (Beilock et al., 2001; Short et al., 2002).

Imagery is a part of sport psychology skill (mental skill), where it effect

to athletes to success in their tournament or game. In addition, many athletes

and coaches today recognize the power of imagery in sport performance. In

fact, athletes from most sport attribute at least part of their success to their

use of imagery. This report was supported Murphy and Martin (2002), which

showed that imagery has a strong relationship between imagery ability and

sport performance. Over the past several decades there has been a growing

body of research which shows the positive effects of mental training on sports

performance, an overview of this research as it relates to this study is

presented in the following section.

2.2 Effectiveness of Imagery

Mental imagery use has been researched quite extensively over the

past 50 years in the areas of motor learning and sport psychology (Hall &

Fishburne, 2010).

Of 235 Canadian athletes who participated in the 1984 Olympic

Games, 99% reported using imagery (Orlick & Partington, 1988). These

athletes estimated that during training they engaged in systematic imagery at

least once a day, 4 days per week, for about 12 minutes each time. At the

Page 27: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

27

Olympic site, some reported engaging in imagery for 2 to 3 hours in

preparation for their events (Vealey, 2007).

In fact, with increases in imagery ability enhancing the effectiveness of

imagery training (Isaac, 1992).

Furthermore, Vealey and Greenleaf (2006) proposed research

evidence supporting the effectiveness of imagery as a mental training tool is

divided into three areas. As shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Summary of research support for the effectiveness of imagery (Vealey & Greenleaf, 2006, p. 311).

First, imagery has been shown to enhance sport performance and

learning. Second, imagery has been shown to enhance thoughts and

emotions in athletes that are critical to athletes’ performance. Third, research

shows that successful athletes use imagery more extensively and

systematically than less successful athletes (Vealey & Greenleaf, 2006).

Studies showed also the effect of mental imagery on the enhancement

of athletic performance (Feltz & Landers, 1983; Jones & Stuth, 1997; Martin

Evidence that Imagery Works

Enhances Performance and Learning

Mental practice of skill over

time

Preparatory imagery for competition

Part of multimodal mental

training program

Enhances Thoughts and Emotions

↑ Self-confidence

↑ Motivation

↑Attention Control

↓ Anxiety

Successful Athletes Use It!

Successful athletes use

imagery more extensively

and more systematically

than less successful

athletes

Page 28: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

28

et al., 1999), learning of basketball tactical strategies (Guillot et al., 2009)

motor performance and skills (Morris et al., 2005; Papadelis et al., 2007; Wei

& Luo, 2009). According to those literature reviews, imagery as an element of

sport psychology skill or mental training tool can be useful on the success of

athletes and prolonging physical performance, especially during competition

(Sadeghi et al., 2010). Mental rehearsal, a practical psychological tool to

improve performance, has been widely used by athletes and coaches.

Through mental rehearsal interventions, athletes may speed learning,

increase performance, change attitudes, overcome competitive stress, and

identify problems they may be having in a particular performance situation.

There are also Specific types of imagery that were effective in changing

athletes’ perceptions of anxiety from harmful and negative to facilitative and

challenging (Evans et al., 2004; Hale & Whitehouse, 1998; Nideffer, 1981;

Page et al., 1999).

Furthermore, imagery has been shown to be effective in enhancing

self-confidence (Callow et al., 2001; Evans et al., 2004; Garza & Feltz, 1998;

Hale & Whitehouse, 1998; McKenzie & Howe, 1997; Short et al., 2002),

motivation (Martin & Hall, 1995), the ability to maintain concentration in sport

tasks (Calmels et al., 2004; Farrow & Kemp, 2003), and visual search abilities

(Jordet, 2005) of athletes during competition (Vealey, 2007, P.296).

In the domain of motor skills and sport psychology, mental imagery has

become an important component of a strategically organized learning

experience. Several studies have demonstrated that the use of mental

imagery, combined with physical practice, contributes to the optimization of

Page 29: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

29

motor performance ( Grouios, 1992b; Lesley, & Gretchen, 1997; Martin & Hall,

1995; Overby et al., 1997-1998; Screws & Surburg, 1997; Taktek, 2000;

Wrisberg, & Anshel, 1989).

The theoretical basis of mental imagery is frequently debated. In fact,

when behaviourism was the prominent psychological paradigm, imagery was

rarely researched (Kosslyn, 1994). The very nature of mental imagery is

inconsistent with theories of behaviourism, as mental images are not

demonstrated in any observable behaviour (Tversky, 2000).

Therefore, the process of representation is reliant on the generation of

mental images that are based on collected information (Decety, & Mick, 1989;

Fortin, & Rousseau, 1989).

It has been well documented in the literature that mental imagery

enhances athletes' performance (Driskell et al., 1994; Hall, 2001; Paivio,

1985). This is reflected in the frequent use of imagery reported by athletes

(Barr & Hall, 1992; Hall et al., 1990) and the inclusion of imagery in mental

skills training programs implemented by sport psychologists (e.g., Gould et al,

1990; Kendall et al., 1990). It is not surprising that imagery has been

described as the “central pillar of applied sport psychology” (Perry & Morris,

1995, p. 339).

Mental imagery is an internal and subjective experience because it

relies primarily on the cognitive activity of the performer (Kosslyn et al, 1995;

Richardson, 1967a, 1967b, 1977; Richardson, 1991).

Cognitive general Imagery refers particularly to imagery strategies

related to a competitive event. For example, Chris Evert Lloyd reported

Page 30: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

30

imagery rehearsal of strategies tuned to the style of a specific tennis

opponent, that means, cognitive specific Imagery refers to imagery directed at

improving particular perceptual-motor skills (Hall, et al., 2007).

Since Mahoney and Avener’s (1977) classic study, researchers and

practitioners have been interested in the effects of internal and external

imagery. Internal imagery requires an approximation of the real-life

phenomenology such that the person actually imagines being inside his or her

body and experiences those sensations which might be expected in the actual

situation. In external imagery, a person views himself or herself from the

perspective of an external observer. Such an experience is not directly

observable but can be made more concrete by means of a subjective report

and/or drawing (Fortin & Rousseau, 1989; Piaget, & Inhelder, 1966). Often a

combination of internal and external imagery information is used to build

symbolic-verbal based interventions of mental training, which use written

descriptions of a task (Mayer & Hermann, 2009; Weinberg & Gould, 2011). In

addition to the coding and recall of information related to perceived objects,

mental imagery also allows for the anticipation of future events (Decety &

Mick, 1989; piage & Inhelder, 1966). It represents a constructive mental

process that is largely related to perception and retains a specific autonomous

cognitive status. In fact, mental imagery activity produces perceptions that are

relatively less intense, precise, detailed, and stable (Denis, 1979). Thus,

mental imagery seems to be an imitation of perceptual and sensory

experiences; a conscious activity distinct from a dream; an activity which may

manifest itself in the absence of external stimuli; and an activity where the

Page 31: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

31

effects are different from the ones emerging from sensory or perceptual

experiences (Murphy, 1994). However, Hardy (1997) argues that imagery is

beneficial, to the extent that it adds useful information to the movement

experience. Because internal and external imagery probably provide different

information about a movement experience, practitioners are advised to

combine perspectives (which evidently is often spontaneously done already).

Although it should be noted that imagery is effective for all types of tasks but,

some tasks were more affected than others. The original meta-analysis by

(Feltz & Landers, 1983; Hinshaw, 1991) reported that mentally practicing had

a more positive effect on subsequent performance of a motor skill than not

practising at all. There has also been research that has shown that combining

physical and mental practice is more beneficial to performance of a motor skill

than just physical practice on its own (Blair et al., 1993).

More recent meta-analyses (e.g., Driskell et al., 1994; Hinshaw, 1991)

have supported the conclusion that for tasks with a great deal of cognitive

components the best uses of imagery involve a complete sensory experience

that engages the senses of taste, smell, and hearing, as well as those of sight

and feel. That is why the fourth and fifth perspectives are generally most

effective (Karageorghis & Terry, 2011). For example, a tennis player using

imagery to recreate the experience of serving should imagine the feeling of

stretching up and priming his dominant arm, the sound of the ball as he

strikes it, the smells in the air, and the taste of sweat, in addition to seeing

himself perform the action.

Page 32: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

32

Another related task dimension that has been studied, is the open-

closed continuum. This research is complicated by the fact that the

relationship of imagery to task type might be mediated by the internal-external

dimension of imagery. Specifically, it has been suggested that closed skills

(e.g., golf, bowling and archery, or elements in sports, such as the serve in

tennis, the foul shot in basketball, or the serve in volleyball) allow time for the

athlete to prepare and to execute when ready, would benefit most from

internal imagery whereas open skills (e.g., boxing, batter hitting a baseball,

returning a serve in tennis or volleyball, and a hockey goalkeeper stopping a

puck) might benefit most from external imagery (McLean & Richardson,

1994). Since closed skills should be more affected by imagery because they

are completely under the athlete’s control, the athlete can use imagery to

visualize them precisely. Open skills are more variable and reactive to specific

game situations, so an athlete is not able to image them as precisely (Singer,

2000; Coelho,et al., 2007, 2008).

In addition to, research by Morris and his colleagues (summarized in

Morris et al., 2005), argues that although there is some evidence to support

the above contention (internal-closed, external-open), more definitive

research is necessary to more clearly demonstrate the relationship of type of

imagery and type of task. Finally, although not directly related to performance,

it has been found that both novice and elite athletes use different types of

imagery in open versus closed sports; although the effectiveness of such

types of imagery needs further exploration (Arvinen-Barroe et al., 2007).

Page 33: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

33

Research has clearly found that positive imagery leads to enhanced

performance, whereas negative imagery leads to decreased performance. For

example, researchers (e.g.,Malouff et al., 2008) found that positive imagery

and using self-instructions just before serving in tennis leads to greater

serving accuracy. Researchers (e.g., Taylor & Shaw, 2002; Woolfolk et al.,

1985) also found that the positive images enhanced performance, as well as

the execution of a golf-putting task, whereas, negative imagery produced a

decrease in performance and execution. Athletes commonly use positive

performance imagery and self- instruction as part of their routine just before

an athletic performance in the hopes of performing as well as possible

(Cumming et al., 2006; Defrancesco & Burke, 1997; Neck & Manz, 1992). In

addition, Beilock and others (2001) found that suppressive imagery (where

the performer was told to imagine “don’t miss”) also produced decreases in

performance. This research supports the notion that when a person is asked

“not” to do something, the mind often cannot forget the “not” and simply sees

the miss or other bad performance. Furthermore, (although not shown

experimentally), Hall (2001) has noted that elite athletes typically image

positive outcomes because they do not want to have negative images in their

minds when performing.

Feltz and Landers (1983) concluded that imagery could be effective for

both beginners and highly skilled performers, although there was a lot of

variability found across studies. Some studies have reported greater effects

for highly skilled performers, whereas others show a larger effect for lower

skilled performers (e.g., Bohan et al., 1989). This relationship is further

Page 34: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

34

complicated by the notion that the content of the imagery needs to match the

stage of development of the skill. So, imagery that would be too complicated

for a novice (due to their lack of knowledge of the finer aspects of the skill)

might be appropriate for a highly skilled performer. Along these lines, Woolfolk

et al., (1985) found that low-ability performers had a decrease in performance

after using imagery whereas high ability performers had an increase in

performance. For example, If novice athletes do not have accurate schemas

of skills, then their mental imagery may consist of the rehearsal of “bad

habits,” or the wrong set of skills (Hall, 2001; Noel, 1980). Once a high level of

expertise and skill has been established, visualization may serve as a conduit

for the transfer of skills from practice to competition (Driskell et al., 1994;

Gallucci, 2008; Gould & Damarjian, 1996).

It was hypothesized that this was due to the fact that low-ability

performers used incorrect imagery, and in doing so reinforced errors.

Furthermore, the vast majority of habits formed by human beings are acquired

through observing and imitating others (Bandura’s, 1977 a,b), so that one of

the techniques used in sport psychology to enhance imagery rehearsal and

athletic performance is video modelling, which involves the observation of a

model or expert performer executing a specific skill successfully (Perry &

Morris, 1995; Morris et al., 2005) Additionally, research has shown that

observational learning is enhanced when the model being viewed is similar to

the observer in age, gender, and competence in the activity, because similar

models may enhance self-efficacy and performance (McCullagh & Weiss

2001; Ram & McCullagh 2003; Schunk et al., 1987).

Page 35: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

35

The two key patterns discussed in the context of practice speed are

slow-motion and real-time imagery. Originally, it was felt that slow motion

imagery would be more effective because it should allow athletes time to

isolate weak elements of their performance and eventually correct faulty

components. This, then, should result in improved performance. However,

researchers (Holmes & Collins, 2001) have suggested that rehearsing actions

or movements in slow motion may elicit inappropriate neural patterns that are

different from those created during physical performance, or real time

imagery, and thus lead to errors in actual execution. Furthermore, they argue

that matching movement tempo and consistency of relative timing for both

mental and physical practice conditions should eventually lead to improved

preparation and execution of both imagery and the equivalent motor

performance. The current consensus in sport psychology is that athletes

should rehearse imagery of performance using a real time rhythm and tempo

whenever possible (e.g., Moran, 2004; Weinberg & Gould, 2007; Weinberg,

2008). In fact, The idea of video modelling is to ensure that the ideal or near-

ideal performance becomes the image that the athlete uses in the mental

rehearsal (Morris et al., 2005).

The combination of visual and kinesthetic imagery would be more

effective than either type alone because it activates both neural pathways and

adds to the information from imagery (Hardy & Callow, 1999). Thus the most

effective imagery might be that which utilizes internal and external imagery

with visual and kinesthetic imagery (Morris et al., 2005).

Page 36: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

36

It is hypothesized that imagery using all senses would be a closer

representation of the actual situation and therefore it should help improve

performance (Munroe et al., 2000; Hausenblas et al., 1999).

Thus, there is a general agreement that to make imagery most

effective, performers should include as many senses as possible, Related to

this notion that the imagery should “mirror” the actual performance

environment as much as possible (e.g., Moran, 2004; Vealey & Greenleaf,

2005; Weinberg & Gould, 2007; Weinberg, 2008).

Therefore, mental rehearsal is an efficient way of maintaining intensity

levels and attentional focus during delays in the action of athletic contests

(Moran, 1996), because mental simulations may promote confidence and

evoke anxiety, but by mentally representing realistic and successful mastery

in these situations, anxiety can be diminished (Callow et al., 2001; Evans et

al., 2004; Mamassis & Doganis, 2004; Moritz, 1996; Short et al., 2002; Page

et al., 1999; Taylor et al., 1998).

In the course of forming mental simulations, people can anticipate

problems and “think through” their solutions (Driskell et al., 1994; Gould &

Damarjian, 1996). Mental imagery may also divert attention from questions

about the likelihood of successful performance and direct attention to the

behavioural elements that will result in successful performance (Bandura,

1997; Camels et al., 2004). A closer description of the mechanisms of action

and the variable influencing the efficacy of the mental training is given in the

next part.

Page 37: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

37

2.3 How Imagery Works?

Imagery of things we have done can lead to imaginations of future

events (Munzert, 2001), many theories have been proposed to explain how

imagery affects thoughts, feelings, and behaviour. In sport, the focus has

been on explaining how imagery can enhance learning and performance

(Morris, 2010 in Hanrahan & Andersen, 2010), and during the past two

decades we have begun to understand much about how imagery works and

how best to apply it to enhance performance. In fact, every human percepts

different and creates his own reality (Foerster & Ollrogge, 1993).

Of particular interest in recent years have been the findings from the

field of neuroscience, where it has been well established that imagery and

physical performance share some common neural mechanisms, a

phenomenon termed ‘functional equivalence’ (Jeannerod, 1997 in Wakefield

& Smith, 2009).

How can just thinking about jumping over the high bar, hitting a perfect

tennis serve, healing an injured arm, or sinking a golf putt actually help

athletes accomplish these things? We can generate information from memory

that is essentially the same as an actual experience; consequently, imaging

events can have an effect on our nervous system similar to that of the real, or

actual experience (Weinberg & Gould, 2011).

In fact, the ability to generate high muscular strength within short time

periods is of functional importance not only as a basic quality in many sports

disciplines but also for active stabilization of joints (Gruber & Gollhofer, 2004,

p. 98).

Page 38: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

38

In 1894, the psychoneuromuscular theory originated with Carpenter (as

cited in Hale, 1982) proposed the ideo-motor principle of imagery. According

to this principle, imagery facilitates the learning of motor skills because of the

nature of the neuromuscular activity patterns activated during imaging., which

postulated that any thoughts predominant in the mind would thereby express

themselves in the muscles. This theory has since been labelled the

neuromuscular feedback theory, which posits that vivid and well-controlled

images stimulate explicit muscles, specifically those muscles that would be

activated during the imaged task (Kossert & Chandler, 2007).

The first scientific support of this phenomenon came from Start and

Richardson (1964) and (1967a) were the first researchers to develop a

psychoneuromuscular explanation of mental preparation, based on early

psychophysiological studies such as those by Edmund Jacobson (1930a, b, c,

d, 1931a, b, c), Shaw (1938, 1940), and Allers and Scheminsky (1926), but

others have proposed similar explanations (Corbin, 1972; Schmidt & Lee,

1999). These psychoneuromuscular theories propose that the efficacy of

imagery rehearsal of a motor task is due to feedback that results from the

minute muscle innervations (identical in pattern to those that occur during

actual execution) that occur when an individual imagines performing a motor

skill. This feedback enables adjustments in motor behaviour (Morris et al.,

2005), Jacobson (1931) also argued that imagery triggers low-level

innervation of the muscles involved in performance, strengthening those

specific brain-to-muscle connections through feedback similar to that

experienced when the task is performed physically. Jacobson also reported

Page 39: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

39

that the imagined movement of bending the arm created small muscular

contractions in the flexor muscles of the arm (Morris, 2010; Hanrahan &

Andersen, 2010). It has been established in research with downhill skiers,

Suinn (1972, 1976 in Weinberg & Gould, 2011) monitored the electrical

activity in the skiers’ leg muscles as they imagined skiing the course; results

showed that the muscular activity changed during the skiers’ imaginings.

Muscle activity was highest when the skiers were imagining themselves skiing

rough sections in the course, which would actually require greater muscle

activity. By another meaning, When the player vividly imagine performing a

movement, he uses neural pathways similar to those you use in actual

performance of the movement (Kosslyn et al., 2001; Kossert & Chandler,

2007). Furthermore, it has been shown that an increase in force development

is closely related to improvements in neural drive of the trained muscles,

especially in a dynamic, explosive type of strength training (Hakkinen et al.,

1985; Gruber & Gollhofer, 2004).

For example, example of trying to perfect your tennis serve. The goal is

to make your swing for the serve as fluid and natural as possible. To

accomplish this, before you serve each time, imagine the whole serve, from

beginning to end, including seeing the ball go into the target zone, trying to

automate it (i.e., groove your serve swing). In effect, you are strengthening

the neural pathways that control the muscles related to your serve swing

(Malouff et al., 2008; Weinberg & Gould, 2007).

Another concern about the psychoneuromuscular theory is that

research seems to indicate that cognitive processing rather than

Page 40: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

40

neuromuscular feedback is a more likely explanation for the efficacy of mental

preparation and imagery. For instance, research (e.g., Ryan & Simons 1981,

1983), as well as reviews of mental preparation and imagery literature (e.g.,

Driskell et al., 1994; Feltz & Landers 1983; Feltz et al., 1988), suggest that

cognitive rather than strength tasks benefit most from imagery (Morris et al.,

2005).

Athlete performance has been improved to a greater degree through

imagery that emphasizes productive responses, as opposed to imagery that

focuses just on stimulus characteristics of the situation (Smith & Collins,

2004; Smith et al., 2001; Vealey, 2007).

Furthermore, the benefits of mental imagery that occurs immediately

prior to performances may also be due to psychoneuromuscular “priming” or

activation of the muscular groups that are responsible for the actual

performance (Vealey & Greenleaf, 2006; Vealey & Walter, 1993). The

empirical support for the psychoneuromuscular theory is uneven and limited

(Moran, 1996; Murphy & Martin, 2002).

Symbolic learning theory (Sackett, 1934) focuses on cognition. Sackett

specifically claimed that imagery involves rehearsal of the symbolic aspects of

tasks, such as sequences of actions (Hanrahan & Andersen, 2010). Also he

argued that imagery can help individuals understand their movements. His

symbolic learning theory suggests that imagery may function as a coding

system to help people understand and acquire movement patterns. That is,

one way individuals learn skills is by becoming familiar with what needs to be

done to successfully perform them. When an individual creates a motor

Page 41: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

41

program in the central nervous system, a mental blueprint is formed for

successfully completing the movement. For example, in a doubles match in

tennis, if a player knows how her partner will move on a certain shot, she will

be able to better plan her own course. This will help the athlete plan her

movement patterns (Weinberg & Gould, 2007, 2011).

Sackett also demonstrated that mental rehearsal improved

performance on a finger maze (a largely cognitive task). Other research (e.g.,

Minas 1980; Morrisett 1956; Ryan & Simons 1981, 1983; Wrisberg &

Ragsdale, 1979 in Morris et al., 2005) has supported symbolic learning theory

by showing that mental rehearsal facilitated cognitive more than motor

performance, although most sport skills have both motor and cognitive

components; imagery can be effective to an extent, therefore, in helping

players with a variety of skills. Johnson (1982) further demonstrated that

imagining movements biased later performance when the participant

completed a visual-interference task. Johnson’s findings support the symbolic

learning theory because he had hypothesized that images of movements

would be primarily visual (Denis, 1985; Morris et al., 2005).

Indeed, the link between mental imagery and human motor

performance is clearly inextricable and the central precondition for the

existence of imaginations, is the ability of our memory to store and represent

perceptions with the possibility to go back to them actively, so that, the

bioinformational theory postulates that mental images comprise propositional

representations stored in the long-term memory (Lang, 1979; Chandler &

Kossert, 2007).

Page 42: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

42

Bioinformational theory has been a popular cognitive theoretical

explanation for how imagery enhances sport performance, due to its intuitive

appeal and pragmatic implications for using imagery to create “mental

blueprints for perfect responses” (Vealey, 2005, 2007) .

According to Lang, images contain two main classes of propositions:

stimulus and response. Stimulus propositions describe the content of the

scene to be imagined. For example, a tennis player at a major competition

might imagine the match he is going to play, his serves, and the people sitting

around the tennis court, Response propositions, on the other hand, are

statements that describe the imagery’s response to the particular scenario,

and they are designed to produce psychological activity. For example, having

a tennis player feel that he will play now the first serve, that makes him move

his racket and his body including his ritual for the ball, he might feel also a

little tension in his muscles preparing for the serve, all those things are

response propositions.

Imagery scripts should contain both stimulus and response

propositions, which are more likely to create a vivid image than a stimulus

propositions alone (Weinberg & Gould, 2007, 2011). Studies that have tried to

link performance enhancement to stimulus and response propositions have

found equivocal results. Ziegler (1987) could not find any significant difference

in basketball free-throw shooting between groups using active (stimulus and

response propositions) and passive (stimulus propositions) imagery. Kremer

and Pressing (1998) found greater improvement in pistol-shooting

performance for the group using stimulus-only propositions when compared to

Page 43: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

43

a group using stimulus-and-response propositions (Morris et al., 2005).

Understanding these theories of imagery one can see how mental training has

been used, some of the uses of mental training are enumerated in the next

section.

2.4 The Use of Imagery

The use of imagery for enhancement of sport performance has been of

central interest to applied sport psychologists (Hall et al., 1998).

Overall, more successful elite athletes use imagery more extensively

and more systematically and have better imagery skill than less successful

athletes (Calmels et al., 2003; Cumming et al., 1990; Salmon et al., 1994;

Vealey, 2007)

Munroe et al. (2000) proposed four fundamental questions for imagery

use in sport: Where is imagery used? When is imagery used? Why is imagery

used? and What is being imagined? They referred to these four questions as

the “four W’s of imagery use”. These fundamental questions were designed to

help describe imagery use in sport, and provide the foundation for imagery

interventions (Hall & Fishburne, 2010). With respect to “where” imagery is

used, the two most common places for athletes to employ imagery are in

conjunction with practice and competition (Hall, 2001). This applies to athletes

of all levels and ages (Munroe-Chandler et al., 2007), and for coaches as well

(Thelwell et al., 2008). However, studies have also found that athletes and

coaches report using imagery outside of practice and competition (Salmon et

al., 1994; Thelwell et al., 2008). Salmon and colleagues found that athletes

report the use of imagery at work, home, and school, and Thelwell and

Page 44: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

44

colleagues reported similar results (e.g., at home, in the office, outside) when

examining where coaches utilize imagery.

Using imagination constructively involves much more than idle

daydreaming. Only when he can use it skilfully and systematically to help

create, or revive the ultimate performance he will benefit. Imagination

channelled in this disciplined way is referred to as visualization or imagery.

Although it may sound a bit complicated, in reality, imagery is a simple conce

pt with very basic applications. Most of us use imagery every day, but

in an unstructured or sporadic way (Karageorghis & Terry, 2011). Many

more athletes and exercisers have begun using imagery not only to help their

performances, but also to make their experiences in sport and exercise

settings more enjoyable (Weinberg & Gould, 2007). Players can make use of

imagery in a number of ways. He can remember experiences and learn from

them. He can recall good experiences and best performances and practice

recreating the feeling (Andersen, 2000). Mental training is also used to a

greater degree by international-calibre athletes as compared to national-

calibre athletes (Calmels et al., 2003). International-calibre athletes used a

wider range and more elaborate and complex mental strategies and

techniques than national-calibre athletes (Vealey, 2007).

The ways visualisation can be used to benefit sport performance and

skill development are limited only by the player’s imagination, that means

“Individually”. In addition to, he may be fairly confident that he can distinguish

between events that happen in reality and those that he imagines. This

distinction, however, is actually much less obvious than he might think. The

Page 45: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

45

messages sent in the form of neural impulses to and from the brain during

imagined action closely resemble those sent during physical action.

Visualization establishes a mental blueprint for sport skills and tactical ploys.

Thus, WYSIWYG (i.e., What You See Is What You Get) is a popular acronym

used by sport psychologists (Karageorghis & Terry, 2011). Imagery does have

a long history in psychology and in the physical and mental health fields

(Graham, 1995). Wundt (1896), using the technique of introspection to study

the mind, considered imagery one of the functions of mind open to his

methods of investigation.

Coaches and athletes have become increasingly aware of the

importance of mental training in sporting activities. Mental imagery is used by

athletes for a number of reasons, but its two primary purposes seem to be to

prime the athlete for peak performance, and to enhance skill learning (Hall et

al., 1990) Paivio (1985) contends that imagery serves two functions: a

cognitive function and a motivational function. While some recent research

has attempted to examine both of these functions of imagery (Barr & Hall,

1992; Hall, Rodgers & Barr, 1990; Hall, Toews & Rodgers, 1990), most

research, especially those studies comprising the mental practice literature,

has been directed towards examining cognitive function (see Feltz et al.,

1988).

Imagery is an excellent tool for motor skill development. Imagery can

facilitate the understanding of motor patterns because the perceptual

experiences of action can be retained by imagery in much of their complexity.

The key is to develop a close match between images and perceptual-motor

Page 46: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

46

elements of performance. Athletes then can use imagery to recall past

experiences accurately and to mentally rehearse their best performances.

They can also analyse images as feedback from performance attempts, and

they can create images of desired actions to serve as intentions for future

performance (Andersen, 2000).

Coaches often encourage athletes to use imagery to help facilitate the

improvement and learning of skills (Hall & Rodgers, 1989). As well, imagery is

often a central component in the mental training programs developed and

implemented by sport psychologists (e.g. Daw & Burton, 1994). Given the

recognized value and widespread use of imagery in sport, it is not surprising

that researchers have been interested in investigating why imagery works and

how athletes can use it effectively. Sport, however, is only one form of

physical activity. It is conceivable that imagery may be used in other forms of

physical activity, in particular exercise (Hausenblas et al., 1999).

Imagery therefore has the effect of “priming” the appropriate muscles

for subsequent physical action, and this clearly has potential benefits for the

performance of many sport skills. Physical skills may be improved even when

they are only practised in the mind (Karageorghis & Terry, 2011).

Athletes can use imagery in many ways to improve both physical and

psychological skills. One of the most common uses of imagery is mental

practice for skill learning and practice a particular sport skill, in fact using

imagery in this way allows athletes to learn and maintain technical skills for

their sport, athletes also can use imagery to detect and correct errors in their

routine, motion, or movement pattern for example, swimming the backstroke-

Page 47: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

47

all in their minds. A player can practice skills to fine-tune them, or he can

pinpoint weaknesses and visualize correcting them. Imagery can be also used

to practice and learn new strategies or review alternative tactics for either

team or individual sports, because they all play with different styles (Martens,

2004; Morris et al., 2005; Weinberg & Gould, 2011).

Athletes can also use imagery to get themselves ready to perform their

best before a particular competition, for example, this preparation could take

the form of imagining the arena where the athlete will perform, or an athlete

could image his performance routine. That is, imagery can be applied to

performance and competition, not just practice (Morris et al., 2005).

Additionally, mental practice can never replace physical practice, but they are

very effective when combined. Imagery can also be used in situations where

athletes cannot practice physically, such as during travel or when they are

injured (Weinberg & Gould, 2011; Ievleva & Orlick, 1991).

To use imagery effectively (Gould et al., 2000 in Gill & Williams, 2008)

have suggested some strategies: Practice imagery regularly, and develop it

through training, and continued practice. Because imagery is not a substitute

for physical practice, but a skill to enhance practice and performance. In

addition, imagery combined with relaxation is more effective than imagery

alone, Use imagery not only in theory but also in practice as well as

competition, use videotapes or audiotapes to enhance imagery skills, use

triggers or cues to facilitate imagery quality. For example, words, phrases, or

objects may aid concentration, most imagery should match real time and

speed, Slowing down or speeding up may be useful in imagery training. Use

Page 48: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

48

imagery logs. Logs can help people monitor progress, remember cues, or

stay with a training program. Any training must be adjusted for the individual

and the situation, because people are vary in their imagery skills and styles.

So that most sports and exercise psychologists advise individual

assessments, evaluation, and modifications with cognitive skill training.

Assessment can increase awareness of skills and help the consultant work

with the person to develop appropriate training strategies. Training strategies

typically follow three steps. First, sensory awareness training helps people

become more aware of all sensations (sounds, smells, feelings) and use them

in imagery. Second, vividness training might start with simple exercises, such

as imagining a childhood bedroom, to develop clearer, more vivid images and

with controllability training, participants learn to regulate images, such as

speeding up and slowing down while imagining running. Vealey and

Greenleaf (2006) provide several excellent examples of exercises, as well as

guidelines for imagery training and use. Once imagery skills have been

developed, they can be used for many purposes, including practising physical

skills or strategies, correcting errors, practising emotional control, and

developing confidence.

Moreover, imagery will only bring benefits to the athlete if he or she

knows how, when and where to use it. Therefore, understanding the use of

different models of imagery is necessary (Coelho et al., 2008).

For examples, when Smith and Holmes (2004) designed an imagery

training program to improve golf putting, performance was enhanced more by

mental practice using audiotapes and videotapes than by mental practice

Page 49: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

49

using written scripts that were read by the golfers. The interpretation of this

finding was that imagery training using the audio- and videotapes engaged

more functionally equivalent neural processes related to the actual execution

of putting as compared to written scripts.

Imagery is a technique that is incorporated into many different mental

training strategies and models. These include the applied model of imagery

use in sport (Martin et al., 1999; Paivio, 1985), the PETTLEP (physical,

environmental, task, timing, learning, emotion, perspective) model (Holmes &

Collins, 2001) provides useful guidance, but even with its seven elements, it

does not cover all considerations, the three-level model of sport imagery

(Murphy & Martin, 2002), and the sport imagery ability model (Watt et al.,

2004). Specific mental training strategies incorporating imagery include

visuomotor behaviour rehearsal (Suinn, 1984), the Five-Step Strategy (Singer,

1988).

Hausenblas et al. (1999) suggested a multidimensional model. They

named this the model Exercise Model, which has three distinct primary

reasons: energy, appearance and technique. Energy imagery refers to mental

images associated to the feeling of increase energy and decrease stress.

Appearance imagery relates to imagery of a graceful corporal appearance,

healthy and well-conditioned. Technique imagery refers to movement

synesthesia, it is the corporal position during diverse phases of the

movement. Energy and appearance are linked to motivation, whilst technique

imagery works through cognitive function.

Page 50: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

50

A study developed by Taylor and Shaw (2002) confirms this

assumption. They added one extra element to be researched, the precision of

performance. They conducted a study to determine the effects of positive or

negative imagery in “putting” (last shot) performance in golf. The sample of

this study was composed by golfers with different levels of ability. They

performed the “putting” technique using three different types of Imagery

intervention: (a) Imagery of positive result; (b) Imagery of negative result and

(c) Control group of no imagery. These three conditions were performed in a

competition situation. The results showed that imagery of negative result was

harmful to performance. The imagery of positive result did not present

significance. They reported there is evidence which suggests that imagery of

result operates through a mechanism of self-confidence. The imagery of

negative result negatively influenced the self-confidence and performance of

the athletes. They concluded that athletes should avoid the visualization of

negative images, since this strongly harms self-confidence and as well

performance.

Other similar studies conducted by Woolfolk et al. (1985)

demonstrated an improvement in performance precision by using imagery of

positive result technique. They investigated what effect an imagery

intervention would have on the precision of the motor action during “putting” in

golf. In this study, the data was collected out of a sample of 30 university

students, who were randomly divided into three groups: (a) Imagery of

positive result; (b) Imagery of negative result and (c) Control group. In group

(a) The subjects had to imagine the golf ball falling inside the hole making a

Page 51: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

51

“put”, while in group (b) The subjects had to visualize the ball not falling in the

hole, losing the “put”. The results of this study demonstrated significant

improvement of performance of group (a), when compared to control group

(c), and also with (b). Group (a) showed an improvement in performance,

while the control group demonstrated the worst performance and group (b)

presented deteriorated performance.

Many researches proved the existence of an association between the

content of imagery and self-confidence and anxiety levels in elite athletes

(Vealey, 1986; Moritz et al., 1996; Vadocz et al., 1997; Callow & Hardy, 2001;

Callow & Waters, 2005). Furthermore, several personal characteristics have

been shown to influence the use of mental training by athletes. Obviously,

self-motivation is a big factor in predicting adherence to mental training (Bull,

1991), and type of motivation influences use of mental training as well.

Harwood et al. (2004) found that high task/ moderate ego-oriented athlete in

terms of achievement goal orientations used more imagery, goal setting, and

positive self-talk as compared to low task/ high ego- and moderate task/ low

ego-oriented athletes (Vealey, 2007).

For example, Callow and Walters (2005) examined the cause-effect

relationship between an intervention of synesthesic imagery and self-

confidence in five professional jockeys. Synesthesic imagery, according to the

authors is characterized by effort, special sensation and consciousness of

body position during the movement. The results confirmed the hypotheses

that synesthesic imagery would increase self-confidence in sport, improving

then the performance.

Page 52: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

52

Vealey (1986) identified a significant correlation between self-

confidence and performance in sports. The results of his study showed that

athletes who had high levels of self-confidence were the ones who

demonstrated the best performance when compared to other athletes with low

self-confidence.

Brouziyne and Molinaro (2005) investigated the improvement of the

precision of teeing off in golf, through mental training with 23 beginners, who

were divided into three groups. In a seven week intervention one group

combined mental training with physical practice of the approach shot. The

second group received one training per week of physical practice only, and a

third group engaged in various sporting activities instead of either mental or

physical practice of the stroke. Analysis showed that the beginners' approach

shot performance improved most in the group combining physical practice

and mental imagery, when compared with the group just physically practising

the approach shot. It seems mental training can be used effectively to improve

performance even with beginners.

Some researchers suggest that many factors affect the mental

practice, such as the movement synesthesia, the duration of training sections,

the ability and experience of the athlete (Barton, 1996; Scales, 1995; Epstein,

1980).

Barton (1996) developed a study to determine if a method of imagery

intervention could help performance improvement in rappelling, while taking

into consideration gender, age, height, weight and performance. The sample

was composed by 47 males and females. They were divided into

Page 53: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

53

experimental and control groups. A test of technical level was applied pre and

post treatment. The experimental group participated in 100 minutes of mental

practice of imagery during a 10 days period. The obtained results did not

show any significance interactions between males and females. The

difference was also not significant in performance between groups

(experimental and control). The results did not present significance in the

correlation between age, height, weight, gender and improvement in

performance variables.

Sport psychology has generally moved from the laboratory to the field

over the past 20 years, but imagery research has remained mostly laboratory

or case study oriented. This is, in part, due to the difficulty of conducting

experimental imagery in the field (Weinberg, 2008). Therefore, the majority of

research attention should be directed towards investigating imagery use in the

field. Particularly of interest is understanding how, when, and why imagery

affects an athletes’ performance, as well as its effects on the thoughts,

emotions, and motivations that accompany this performance (Morris et al.,

2005). Also, research efforts should begin to examine how effective imagery

is for athletes when packaged in different ways using specific strategies or

models of intervention (Vealey, 2007).

Furthermore, few studies have been conducted on the practical

efficacy of psychological techniques to improve tennis and soccer

performance, because the efficacy of mental training is dependent on several

factors. An overview of such influencing variables is outlined in the next

chapter.

Page 54: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

54

2.5 Mental Training Studies In Tennis

Particularly in professional tennis, psychological strategies are well-

established and important tools for improving performance. According to a

survey by DeFranesco and Burke (1997), professional tennis players use

strategies such as mental training, preparatory routines, relaxation training,

goal setting, and systematically structured self-talk to aid in their performance.

In addition, DeFranesco and Burke (1997) report that higher-ranked players

attribute a significantly larger part of their performance success to

psychological variables, therefore, the use of these methods should be made

a priority both before and during the competition (Mayer & Hermann, 2009).

Surburg (1968) investigated the effectiveness of mental training in

combination with various forms of instruction on the execution of forehand

strokes in tennis. In all experiment groups: mental training in combination with

audio instruction; audiovisual instruction; and visual instruction, performance

improvements in forehand strokes, in comparison to the control group, could

be observed. The most effective program in this study proved to be mental

training in combination with audio instruction. In particular, mental training was

studied in connection with the learning elements of the technique.

DeFrancesco and Burke (1992) in their study, Performance

Enhancement Strategies Used in a Professional Tennis Tournament,

attempted to identify which types of performance enhancement strategies

were employed most frequently by 115 professional tennis players who

competed in the 1992 Lipton Tennis Tournament. The results of a 13-item

questionnaire revealed that the most common strategies used by the tennis

Page 55: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

55

players were imagery/ visualization, use of a consistent preparatory routine for

every pre-service or pre-service return (i.e., mental preparation), relaxation,

goal-setting and self-talk. The players felt that their motivation to compete,

maintain concentration throughout the match, and self-confidence affected

their performances considerably.

Morans’ (1995) proposed study to determine how effectively

psychological techniques were used to enhance performance in tennis was

based on the views of some international tennis coaches. The study, carried

out by Moran in 1995, involved interviewing thirty full time tennis coaches that

train professional players. All of the participants had taken part in a workshop

in Dubai in 1991. They originated from thirteen different countries, were on

average 37.63 years old and give training an average of 22.58 hours a week.

For the study, twenty seven questions were formulated to provide an overview

of the different mental techniques that were used in training. In the study the

following sections are interrogated: How important mental abilities were for

success in tennis competitions, or rather, to what content did they influence

the success? How can those abilities be trained? How important are those

abilities in tennis and what are the main observable differences in the different

techniques?

The results were divided in four parts. The first part outlined the

importance of being receptive to mental training in tennis. Ninety percent of

the coaches felt that mental abilities were important for success in tennis

competitions and gave mental training on a weekly basis. Almost all of the

coaches believed that mental abilities could be developed with training. The

Page 56: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

56

coaches felt that the problem of under developed mental abilities was due to a

lack of time spent training and in the realization of the theoretical knowledge.

The second part dealt with special mental abilities. Different mental abilities

have to be rated by the coaches with the help of a 7-stage Likert-scale (1=not

important at all; 7= very important). For validation, motivation, concentration

and self-confidence were those most often named. The efficiency of special

psychological techniques was the third part. The coaches had to rate how

important the use of certain psychological techniques was for them. Out of

thirteen items listed, the coaches most frequently identified the soliloquy,

imagination of the next hit and imagination of the technique, which all

correspond with visualization. The fourth, and final, part of the results dealt

with the question of what the coaches need, or rather, what would help them

most. The answer, it turns out, is that that the coaches want some form of

instruction to aid them in designing and executing a mental training program.

In Morans’ study three weak points can be identified. The first weak

point is that the answers only represented the opinion of the coaches since

there was no other form of evaluation in used at the time. The second

weakness is the small number of spot tests. The questions were answered by

only thirty coaches and, therefore, cannot be generalized. Third, the

participants were not chosen at random. All of the coaches had taken part in a

work shop in Dubai and represent just a small percent of all coaches.

In summary the study comes to the conclusion that the coaches of

professionals are completely aware of the importance of mental factors in

Page 57: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

57

training and, therefore, desire more development, knowledge and studies on

mental preparation (Müller, 2011)

Féry and Morizot (2000) examined what effect mental training could

have on learning how to serve in tennis. Their study included two conditions:

one focused only on the observation of a model and the other on the

observation of the model followed by mental training involving the movement.

In this context, it seemed that mental training played a crucial role as the

effect of a model in the learning of a tennis serve was greater when the

movement was additionally added to the training.

Immenroth et al. (2003) studied mental training in connection with the

improvement of tennis serves in 10-year-old children. The children

participating had all had approximately three years of experience playing

tennis. The training program was carried out over four weeks, in 15-minute

training sessions held two times per week. The children were divided into

three training groups: the first group received mental training in a child-friendly

form according to the ‘Stepwise’ model of Eberspächer (2001); the second

group received practical training in serving; and the third, a control group,

received practical training for their forehand. As expected, it was

demonstrated that both the mental training group and the practical training

group had improved in comparison to the control group; the mental training

group had only slightly different results from the practical training group. This

result indicates that for technique learning, actual practice time can, to some

degree, be replaced by mental training sessions, without a resulting loss in

quality of the technique acquisition. Also notable is that mental training alone

Page 58: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

58

can lead to improved technique. This is holds true for technique acquisition in

children and adolescents (Mayer & Hermann, 2009). In the authors opinion,

that by starting mental training as a youth mental training as an adult will be

easier and more effective.

Shaw and Goodfellow (1997) proposed another model, based on

imagery, which they called “Imagery of Result”. It is a kind of imagery focuses

on what happens after the completion of a motor action. They suggest that

imagery of result can be positive or negative. The image of a positive result

can influence the posterior imagery of a service in tennis, while the image of a

negative result can prejudice it. Beilcock et al. ( 2001) reinforce this premise,

emphasizing that results after having positive or negative images are

positively correlated with the outcome , e.g. positive images result in positive

outcomes while negative images result in negative outcomes. Therefore, from

the point of view of the author it would be better to eliminate negative imagery

and instead focus on only positive imagery before executing a movement.

Defrancesco and Burke (1997) stated that tennis is a sport which

requires precise movements and is relatively long in duration. Therefore,

improving and maintaining a high level of self-confidence is the primary goal

of an imagery intervention. In fact, an intervention using imagery, should be

directed to the improvement of self-confidence in a continuous manner. This

point of view is shared by Mamassis and Doganis (2004). They developed a

study with the purpose of indentifying the effects of an intervention, using

imagery, to decrease pre-competition anxiety and increase self-confidence,

thereby, the performance of tennis athletes. The results indicated that the

Page 59: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

59

intervention period led to a decrease of anxiety and an increase of self-

confidence after the imagery intervention period.

Similarly, Ryska (1998) studied the use of various cognitive strategies

and their contribution to the reduction of cognitive and somatic anxiety as well

as an increase in self-confidence before an official competition. He found that

approximately thirty procent of athletes utilize mental skills such as relaxation

training, mental training, attention control, positive self-talk, and goal setting

which correlate to lower cognitive and somatic anxiety and higher self-

confidence.

Another similar conclusion was reached by Mamassis and Doganis

(2004). They found evidence supporting the positive effect of a mental training

program on perceived competition anxiety. The mental training Program

(MTP) includes five different psychological skills (Goal setting, positive

thinking and self-talk, concentration and routines, arousal regulation

techniques, and imagery). There was a total of nine participants, elite junior

tennis players from the northwest part of Greece, that were divided into two

groups. A control group, tennis practice only, had four players. Tennis

practice plus Mental training program, used as a intervention (experimental)

group had five players. The results indicated an increase in the direction

dimension of somatic anxiety, cognitive anxiety and self-confidence for the

intervention group in the post-test. Moreover, the intensity of self-confidence

and the overall tennis performance was greater for all participants of the

intervention group after the MTP.

Page 60: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

60

Gould et al. (1999) proposed a survey involving 153 junior tennis

coaches in order to determine their opinions about the importance of mental

skills training, the specific mental skills they teach, strategies they use to

teach mental skills, and recommendations for making mental skills training

more effective. The results identified some of the mental skills thought to be

most difficult to teach including reframing pressure, crisis management, self-

confidence, and emotional control. Roadblocks to mental skills training

included a lack of time, lack of player interest, difficulty evaluating mental

skills training success, and a lack of models or examples of coaches actually

teaching mental skills. Coaches also indicated a need for practical mental

skills training exercises that could be taught in 10-15 minutes, strategies for

better engaging players in mental skills training, and the need for mental skills

training videos to use with players. Differences in the coaches’ opinions were

compared between the more and less experienced and sport psychology

trained and untrained coaches.

Scales (1995) proposed a study With the purpose of determining the

level of influence of mental training (Imagery) in service performance in tennis

for white and black people, a study was developed involving thirty five tennis

players from American universities. The groups were divided into high and low

skills, according to results in the University American League. The subjects of

the experimental group were submitted to a relaxation program and a positive

Imagery intervention with the exhibition of a movie showing the desired

technique in each training section. The subjects of the control group were

exposed to a placebo video containing nature images and sounds. After

Page 61: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

61

seven days of practice the subjects of the experimental group were recorded

in a game situation. The results of the test group did not reveal any significant

differences in the performance level when compared to the control group.

Jekauc and Woll (2006) designed a study about the tennis serve, The

purpose of this study is to investigat whether the visualization of a positive

result before the actual execution of a serve has a positive effect on the

precision of the serve or not. The test exercise was to land the serve in a

target area (80 x 40 cm) positioned on the right side, near the center line, of

the opponents’ court. The study included 26 tournament players (10 female

and 16 male) between the ages of 15 and 28, with a median age of 22.3, with

skill levels ranging from regional and first division. All players had, in the

previous year, played tennis at least once a week. Recruiting for the study

took place in two clubs of the Baden Tennis Association. Participation in the

experiment was voluntary. The results of this experiment seem to support the

hypothesis that the visualization of a positive result before the actual

execution of a serve has a positive effect on the precision of the serve. The

effect size in this case was ETA2 = .16, which indicates an increase in the hit

rate of about 7.6 percentage points.

Coelho et al. (2007) studied the effect of imagery in the tennis service

stroke (closed skill) and service receiving stroke (open skill). Forty eight male

tennis players ranging in age from 16 to 18 years were divided into two

groups; a control group that received only technical practice and an

experimental group which received both imagery and technical practice. The

results of this study suggested that visualizing a positive outcome may be

Page 62: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

62

more powerful in improving performance of closed skill movements than open

skill movements.

Coelho et al. (2008) also proposed a study to determine whether an

imagery training intervention program could affect the performance of junior

Brazilian tennis players in two distinct situations: (1) serving without precision

and (2) serving at a target with precision. Forty six junior male tennis players

with ages ranging from 14 to 16 years were, as in previous studies, divided

into two groups; a control group receiving only technical practice and an

experimental group that was given imagery training as well as technical

practice. The results demonstrated an effect (p< 0.05) between experimental

and control groups in the situation “service without precision”. However, the

difference in the situation of “service with precision” was not significant

(p>0.5). These results demonstrate the effectiveness of imagery training for

building self confidence to improve performance.

However, Malouff et al. (2008) evaluated the effects of pre-competition

positive imagery and self-instructions on serving accuracy in a tennis serving

competition. One hundred and fifteen adult tennis players were randomly

assigned to one of three conditions; positive imagery about serving; self-

instructions relating to serving and a control group in which no mental training

techniques were used. The results demonstrated that the participants in both

the imagery and the self-instruction conditions served significantly more

accurately than those in the control condition.

Those studies (Arvinen-Barrow et al., 2007; Coelho et al., 2007, 2008;

Jekauc & Woll, 2005, 2006; Jekauc, 2006; Malouff et al., 2008) aimed to

Page 63: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

63

determine whether the effect of an intervention using imagery resulted in an

increase in self-confidence, thereby improving performance and precision of

the serve in tennis as a closed motor skill. The results of those experiments

seem to support the hypothesis that the visualization of a positive result

before the actual execution of a serve has a positive effect on the precision of

the serve. This mental technique of imagining positive results before carrying

out a movement, seems to function successfully not only in golf (Woolfolk et

al., 1985) and darts (Powell, 1973), but also in tennis. Practising psychologists

such as Loehr (1991) have long known from their own practical experience

that this mental technique can give one an advantage in tennis.

2.6 Mental Training Studies in Soccer

The study of Crossman (1992) about the effect of three methods on the

learning of in the soccer penalty kick tested 30 varsity level players with the

Crossman Penalty Kick Test. In a three-group design (internal imagery, slow-

motion mental practice, and no practice) during eight days the subjects were

tested before, during and at the end of the intervention. As a result, internal

imagery and slow-motion mental practice showed a performance increasing

effect. Furthermore mental practice was more effective in the first days of the

intervention (Kiefer, 2011)

Blair et al. (1993) were interested in studying both novice and

experienced soccer players. They investigated the effectiveness of mental

training in performance in the form, both of accuracy and speed in a sport-

specific typical skills course. The course participants were tested on,

consisted of a combination of dribbling with obstacles, running without the

Page 64: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

64

ball, and passing, and finally shots at a defined target area. Mistakes in each

task were punished with time penalties. The experimental group mentally

trained for the course; the control group did not train for the course at all.

The mental training was carried out over the course of 6 weeks in 15-

minute training sessions 2 times per week. Three different types of mental

training were utilized: Observer perspective and visual modality, internal

perspective and visual modality, and internal perspective and kinesthetic

modality.

While no significant effects in terms of accuracy of performance could

be found, both beginners and experienced players in the experimental group

improved with regard to speed in comparison to the control group. Blair et al.

(1993) emphasized that mental training in soccer-specific tasks is effective,

and that mental training can be used at every level of ability (Mayer &

Hermann, 2009).

Franks and Hanvey (1997) have recently focused on the penalty kick

as a critical element in deciding the outcome of a game. Its importance has

been obvious in England, extending from World Cup '90 to World Cup '98.

There can be little doubt that the penalty kick has been an important

determining factor in deciding recent major football competitions. Therefore,

such research is both timely and relevant. There are two branches to this

research. First, is the examination of the penalty kick from the goalkeeper's

perspective. Can we provide the goalkeeper with information that will enable

him to predict the placement of the kick? Furthermore, can we train the

goalkeeper to use the appropriate response cues provided by the kicker.

Page 65: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

65

Second, is the examination of the penalty shoot-out. Is there an optimal

ordering of the selected group of penalty takers (Franks et al., 1999).

Frank and Hanvey (1997) also examined the following visual cues

provided by the penalty taker for their predictive utility – the starting position,

the angle of approach to the ball, the forward or backward lean of the torso,

the placement of the non-kicking foot just prior to contact, and the point of

contact on the ball of the kicking foot. The only visual cue found to be both

reliable and time efficient was the placement of the non-kicking foot, which

pointed in the direction that the ball would travel on more than 80% of the

penalty kicks. From the penalty kicks taken in the 1996 European

Championships, the placement of the non-kicking foot predicted the direction

of the penalty-kick in excess of 85%. Furthermore they found that, detection of

the placement of the non-kicking foot was estimated to allow time of 150-200

ms up to the instant of ball contact. These results indicated that a training

program aimed at improving a goalkeeper’s decision-making capability within

realistic time constraints might yield good dividends for the goalkeeper.

The penalty kick represents a decision problem with major incentives,

making it intriguing to determine whether expert players – who are

encouraged through hefty financial rewards to do their best during the few

seconds of a penalty kick – deviate from rational decision making. Numerous

studies have examined cognitive processes such as anticipation, cue

utilization, and response time of goalkeepers during penalty kicks (e.g., Morris

& Burwitz, 1989; McMorris et al., 1993; Williams & Burwitz, 1993; McMorris et

al., 1995; McMorris & Colenso, 1996; Savelsbergh et al., 2002, 2005).

Page 66: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

66

However, the question of whether goalkeepers and kickers act optimally or

reveal biased decision making is still an under-researched topic. Accordingly,

Bar-Eli et al. (2007) investigated this problem and found that goalkeepers

jump to their right or left in 94% of the penalty kicks, although given the

distribution of kicks, it is optimal for goalkeeper to stay in the center more than

6% of the time. Bar-Eli and others (2009), although not directly addressing the

issue of biased decision making, found that the most difficult penalty kicks to

stop are the ones that reach the upper third part of the goal, but nevertheless

only 13% of penalty kicks reach this area (P. 99).

Thelwell et al. (2006) examined how effective a mental training

program tailored for midfielders can be on position-specific performance. The

mental training program consisted of relaxation training (progressive muscle

relaxation), training in performance-conducive self-talk, and mental training in

which basic skills (pass reception, pass-making, and tackling in a variety of

situations) were developed and mentally trained. Additionally, critical

situations were also taken into account, e.g., actions after a bad pass, etc.

The study was carried out with 5 (performance-class) midfielders as subjects.

At least minor improvements in all position-specific performance variables

were observed. While four players showed clear improvements, the

performance of one of the players improved only in terms of the consistency

of the observed performance. The Results are, elite athletes in soccer make

more use of mental training than non-elite athletes, the use of mental training

for a soccer-specific skills course showed no significant effects in terms of

accuracy. Speed, on the other hand, improved for both inexperienced and

Page 67: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

67

experienced players, and mental training can optimize position-specific

performance in soccer (Mayer & Hermann, 2009).

Some recent research has suggested that penalty kickers in major

international tournaments are particularly susceptible to choking under

pressure (e.g. Jordet et al. ( 2007). Specifically, under conditions where the

negative effect of a shot is of key importance (i.e., shots where a miss

instantly produces a loss), players respond with avoidance behaviors, such as

looking away or speeding up their preparation, and they perform considerably

worse than on neutral and positive valence shots (Jordet & Hartman, 2008).

Thus, when these performers’ egos are severely threatened, they seem to

engage in self-regulatory behaviors that backfire and reduce performance.

Choking under pressure is defined as performing worse than expected

in situations with a high degree of perceived importance (Blaumeister, 1984;

Beilock & Gray, 2007).

The results of the study showed that players with high current status

performed worse and seemed to engage more in escapist self-regulatory

behaviors than players with future high status. Some of these performance

drops may be accounted for by misdirected self-regulation (particularly low

response time), but only small multivariate effects were found (Jordet, 2009).

This chapter gives an overview of several studies. Nevertheless, there is less

research about the effects of mental training on soccer penalties without goal

keeper. So that this work seeks to clarify this area of research.

Page 68: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

68

3. Representative Closed Motor Skills in Tennis and Soccer

3.1 Tennis Serve

Tennis belongs to the so-called “return games.” Two players (or

doubles teams) face each other on a playing court divided by a net. The goal

is to aim one’s shot at the opponent’s side of the court so that it cannot be

returned. The basic strokes are the serve, forehand, and backhand, all of

which can be utilized in a number of variants, such as smashes and volleys

(Mayer & Hermann, 2009).

From the beginner to the professional levels in tennis, efficient and

effective on-court movement is key to success. Movements can be classified

as baseline to net, close range, wide-ball and recovery, and sprint, with the

split step and the ready position the omnipresent, preparatory movement

sequences. Research has identified the mechanical characteristics of some of

these common movement strategies, while factors such as the game style,

morphology and sex of the player, court surface, type of ball and shoe wear

help to determine the specific movement strategies a player employs (Reid &

Crespo, 2003). Previous research (Elliot et al, 1989, 1990; Muller, 1989;

Schönborn, 1984) indicates that the most important factors affecting sports

results in tennis performance are coordination, agility, dynamic power and

speed, tennis technique, footwork and a group of mental features. It is also

well established that agility and footwork as well as speed and power are

important components of the performance levels amongst tennis players

(Elliot et al., 1989, 1990; Krolak, 1990; Muller, 1989; Schönborn, 1984, 1993;

Unierzyski, 1996). There are many publications available (Higger, 1993, 2002;

Page 69: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

69

Pluim 1999; Snelleman, 1993; Unierzyski et al., 1998; van Aken, 1998) which

describe training methods for improving these abilities. Based on this

information many coaches focus on developing major abilities affecting

performance during the training process from the beginning of a player’s

career (Unierzyski, 2004, p. 55). In modern tennis, all fast strokes must be

precisely coordinated if they are to be carried out successfully. The basis of

excellent action velocity is a high level of coordination (the interaction of the

central nervous system and the skeletal system) and coordination speed

(Grosser & Schönborn, 2007, p. 13). Tennis players need to work at

improving a variety of strokes and types of movements. They must be able to

perform these strokes and movements from different positions on the court, in

different play situations, at different speeds and with minimal risk of injury

(Elliot & Reid, 2003).

Speed in sport, in general, means the ability to achieve the highest

possible reaction and movement speeds under certain given conditions, by

means of the cognitive process, will-power and the functionality of the neuro-

muscular system (Grosser & Schönborn, 2007).

The greatest danger for a beginner in tennis is learning a stroke in an

incorrect way. Once the body learns a movement pattern it is hard to unlearn

it. Erasing an existing muscle memory and replacing it with a new one can be

a frustrating and painstaking process.

The basis of tennis play is technique. Without a well mastered

technique, all efforts of a coach in furthering the development of the player are

questionable. For example, during speed training, the quality of the technical

Page 70: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

70

performance of individual forms of movement is considered as being of

paramount importance. Through the use of wrong technical processes,

“wrong” patterns are stored in the brain (e.g. a poor running and throwing

technique) that can later severely limit the optimal development of speed

(Grosser & Schönborn, 2002, p. 56).

Technique is a factor that can be seen in connection with movement.

This is especially relevant in modern technique training in tennis (Schönborn,

2000). Furthermore, tennis stroke and movement production has long been of

interest to players, coaches and researchers. The very first description and

images of tennis strokes appeared in literature published during the 14th and

15th centuries. Since then, virtually every book or video devoted to tennis

technique and trainging has detailed the technical characteristics that

constitute tennis strokes and movements (Elliot & Reid, 2003, p. 11).

“Technique learning and technique training must, from the beginning, be

characterised by considerable variety and high complexity” (Lehnrtz, 1996).

Tennis playing necessitates the use of movements that have a high level of

complexity. There are relatively few sports which are so demanding in this

area. Not only the complexity of different types of movements in the stroke,

but also the complexity, interconnection and interdependence of the different

conditioning, co-ordination, psychological and functional factors during the

learning and training process are of the utmost importance for achieving

optimal levels of learning and training success.

Technique can, therefore, be optimally developed if it is seen as part

and parcel of a whole system. This means, however, that they system must

Page 71: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

71

first be developed. The human body forms a unit, and can only function

optimally as a unit; otherwise it does not function at all or does so

unsuccessfully being limited by existing deficiencies and weaknesses. This

topic will be further explored later in the text. But in the end, the concentration

of the athlete should be focused completely on the speed of the movement

and only “incidentally” on the technique itself (Grosser & Schönborn, 2007).

The quality of technique has a decisive influence on performance in

tennis, as technique is the signature of the player by which he transfers all his

conditioning, mental abilities and tactics to the ball, and, through it, on to the

opponent. This is all the more reason why a great deal of attention should to

be given to its optimal development (Schönborn, 2000, p. 13).

Furthermore, an excellent technique and well-defined coordination

abilities allow speed to develop on its own. Seen in this way, speed, co-

ordination ability and technique work as a “unit” in tennis (Grosser &

Schönborn, 2007).

Before diagnosing any stroke a coach must acknowledge that flaws in

technique may not be mechanically based. They may be caused by

psychological factors (e.g. stress, anxiety), poor tactical decisions (e.g.

position on court does not permit the selected shot to be hit effectively) or a

lack of fitness (e.g., poor foot movement in positioning for the stroke) (Elliot &

Alderson, 2003).

In tennis training methodology, the systematization of the entire

development and training process, the direct connection between technique,

tactics and load distribution within conditioning training are still considered

Page 72: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

72

three main weak points. It must be understood that a normal training session

can include several different or a unity of co-ordination, conditioning,

technique, tactics, and mental, physical and social environments (Crespo et

al., 2002; Schönborn, 2000). However, successful tennis play is inextricably

linked to the effectiveness and efficiency of a player’s on-court movement and

strokes. Therefore, proper conditioning leads to enhanced performance and

prevents injury (Elliot & Crespo, 2003).

Furthermore speed, strength, basic endurance, and optimal muscular

stretchability have a positive effect on speed resulting in top performance

development in tennis (Grosser & Schönborn, 2007).

Any meaningful discussion on variations in tennis stroke technique

requires knowledge of sport biomechanics. Biomechanics is a field of study

that focuses on understanding motion and the causes of motion in living

things (Knudson, 2006).

The game of tennis has dramatically changed over the last fifteen years

in that it is now a game dominated by the serve. Increases in the speed of the

serve has meant that aces and one or two shot rallies are now becoming

commonplace. It is now accepted on the professional circuit that it is difficult

to compete at the top level without a fast serve (Higgins & Lees, 1995).

Aggressive game style is also dominate in modern tennis. Almost all

professional players use every opportunity to open the court as soon as

possible (usually with a serve or return) and put their opponents into a

defensive position. Players who try “just to hit over the net” can barely

compete at a serious level. Despite similarities between all players it is known

Page 73: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

73

that is different players win the most important tournaments played on fast

(e.g. grass) and slow (e.g. red clay) surfaces (Unierzyski & Wieczorek, 2004,

p. 169). Winning a tennis match is impossible without a mastery of serve and

return, both technically and tactically, so that, special attention has been given

to researching the service and return (Bollettieri, 1995; Hedelund &

Rasmusen, 1997; Schönborn, 1999; Kleinoder, 2001); considered as the most

important strokes in a modern game in tennis.

The serve is both the easiest and the most difficult shot in tennis. It can

be said to be the easiest because the opponent has no influence whatsoever

on it, except perhaps in a psychological sense. However, it is also very

difficult because the target is limited and hidden behind the net. Also there are

more things happening at once during a serve than in the other strokes in

tennis (Schuyler, 1993, p. 69).

The service stroke puts the ball in play at each point of a game and is,

therefore, considered to be the most important stroke in tennis (Emery &

Hopper, 2001; DTB, 2001; Jekauc & Woll, 2006; Knudson, 2006; Moran &

Marshall, 2009; Pittman, 1989; Roetert & Groppel, 2001). Furthermore, the

service stroke has also been recognised as an important part of Grand Slam

tennis (Donoghue & Ballantyne, 2004). A tennis player must acquire it in order

to have an effective attack. In addition, the serve is the only stroke over which

a player has full control. The two chances allowed on the serve make it

unique and are a luxury not permitted in other sports.

The serve is an effective offensive weapon because the ball can be hit

with a tremendous velocity, thus reducing the oppositions ’ reaction time and

Page 74: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

74

consequently their ability to return the ball. Variations of the service action can

also cause the ball to spin which affects the trajectory of the ball after the

bounce (Behamonde & Knudson, 2003; Emery & Hopper, 2001; Pittman,

1989).

The object of the serve is to put the ball in play so that it will be difficult

for the opponent to make a strong return or, in fact, to make a return at all.

Serving to an opponents' weak side or moving an opponent out of position are

common examples of the effect the serve can have on an opponents’ ability to

return a serve and thus on winning or losing a point (Pittman, 1989, p. 57).

Expert performance in many racket sports is characterised by athletes

reacting extremely quickly to key events such as the direction of an

opponent’s shot. For example, in serve and return, the first serve is a

dominant factor in the game because the serve is largely responsible for

determining the speed of the point (Coe, 2000; Jackson & Gudgeon, 2004).

The importance of speed in tennis includes; the player to be able to

react as fast as possible to surprise actions by an opponent, being able to

give the ball as much release velocity as possible in many different strokes

(e.g. in the service and the smash) and lastly combining speed with technical-

tactical and conditioning abilities to be able to play the whole match as fast

and as effectively as possible (Grosser & Schönborn, 2007).

The power in a tennis serve can dominate the game at the elite level

and should be drawn from multiple sources; ritual, an upward drive of the

legs, a rotation of the shoulders (loading) and snap the wrist - all occurring in

one smooth sequence. It would also appear that higher loading is an

Page 75: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

75

important factor in producing higher service velocity. There is, however, no

rule written that accurately explains to a player how to coordinate the body

movements for the serve with the toss of the ball. The must explore it for

himself during practice. Physical preparation is therefore essential for players

who wish to develop a powerful serve (Clark, 1991; Elliot & Yue, 2003;

Knudson, 2006; Schippers & Lange, 2006).

Traditionally, serves are classified as either first or second serves. First

serves attain higher-speeds (flat) and second serves are slower but with

greater spin (slice). However, this terminology is problematic for a couple of

reasons. First, all serves are hit with spin, so a serve hit will no spin is not

possible, and secondly, a “flat” serve is a misnomer. A flat serve is just a

serve hit to maximize ball speed and, therefore, has minimal spin. Tennis

players should think of all serves as having a mix of ball speed and spin that

they select based on match conditions. This complication also affects the

biomechanical research on the serve because players hit serves with a

combination of spins in competition and experimental settings. Second, the

increased power and accuracy capable with modern racquets has begun to

change the traditional serving strategy. Examination of match statistics has

shown that some players may benefit from faster first and second serves

(Knudson, 2006, p. 45).

An important principle of tennis is the positioning of a server - three feet

or less from the center mark is recommended when serving on the right side.

Serving from that location facilitates service to the receiver’s backhand and

promotes proper court coverage in defending against the opponent’s return.

Page 76: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

76

On the left side, a position about 3 to 5 feet from the center mark

enhances success in serving to the receiver’s backhand while also providing

the server a good forehand position for service returns.

The developing player should habitually assume these service

positions because this practice readily promotes consistency and develops

skill in serving placement (Collins & Hodges, 1985; DTB. Bd. 1, 1995)

The philosophy of intelligent serve tactics is to open the point with an

advantage. For the technically complete player, this should usually be

possible, provided he plans the play out and uses a little creativity.

For example, “The height that the ball should be “pushed” should be

approximately the top of the racket if a player is to hit a relatively stationary

ball” (Elliot, 1995; DTB. Bd. 2, 1996; Schuyler, 1993).

Speed of the serve is determined not only by how hard the ball is struck

but also by the amount of spin that is imparted to the ball, with a high service

speed being of paramount importance. The hard flat serve gives the receiver

less time to react but is more difficult to place in the opponent’s service court

than the slice or twist serve. Hitting the side of the ball furthest from the player

spins the ball around it’s vertical axis and produces a slice serve. The slice

motion takes some speed off the ball and allows it to more easily clear the net

than the flat serve. Slice serves from a right-handed server to a right-handed

receiver cause the ball to curve and also bounce toward the forehand side of

the court. Thus, placement of this serve to the receiver’s backhand side

causes the ball to bounce in his direction, forcing the execution of a rather

difficult return. On the other hand, slice serves should be placed deep in the

Page 77: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

77

service court for maximum effectiveness, but even shallow slices are effective

if they possess adequate spin. The receiver must move to the far side of the

court to return a slice serve and is thereby not in a good position to rush the

net or return to the baseline (Collins & Hodges, 1985; DTB. Bd. 1, 1995;

Moran & Marshall, 2009).

Striking the ball on the side nearest the server constitutes a twist serve

and produces the reverse effect to the slice serve. In this situation, a right-

hander’s serve to the forehand side of a right-handed opponent tends to veer

toward him/her, while the same serve to the backhand side forces the receiver

to move laterally for the return (Collins & Hodges, 1985, p. 67)

There are some common serve tactics that players can utilize (e.g.

Serve to Weakness). If an opponent has a poor backhand; you serve to it in

order to elicit a weak return. However, a weakness will improve if it constantly

gets used, therefore another tactic is to attack an opponents’ stronger side as

well (Serve to Strength). Doing so keeps an opponent off balance, and in

certain situations, a wide serve to the forehand in the deuce court, for

example, you are assured to hit the next shot at the players’ weakness, while

at the same time forcing them to run for the ball. The Serve at the Opponent’s

Body tactic is also known as “jamming” the receiver, because the player has

to get out of his or her own way before returning the ball. Hit “Change-ups”

involves varying the speed and pace of your serves, while being careful to use

the same motions. With the Change your Serving Position on the Baseline

technique a player should occasionally move a little farther from the service

center mark. This changes the angles possible for the players’ serve and

Page 78: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

78

requires a position shift from the opponent. Lastly it is important that a player

hit at least two different serves. Although a player may find one type of serve

to be the most comfortable they still must mix in another type. If a player can

hit all three of the service variations so much the better (DTB. Bd. 1, 1995;

Schuyler, 1993, p. 169).

A final note on the serve: When the player is playing an important

match, focus only on making his racket strings drive up through the ball

(Clark, 1991).

3.2 Soccer Penalty Kick

Everybody involved with the game of association soccer realizes that

training is a necessary part of preparing for competition. Playing soccer itself

is only one part of the preparation. There is the requirement to be fit to play, to

work on correcting physical deficiencies and enhance individual strengths

(Reilly, 2007).

The ability to anticipate the opponents’ intentions and to make

appropriate decisions based on this information are essential components of

skilled performance in soccer at youth and adult levels (Stratton et al., 2004).

Having a visual demonstration of a technique or a tactical maneuver is

an important element of coaching – but it doesn’t have to come from the

coach. An important element of “the game is the teacher” philosophy, that is,

players will also learn from other players. The two go hand in hand; emulating

is the practical application of what they see other players do on the field of

play (Stokell, 2002, p. 43).

Page 79: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

79

The ability to determine what an opponent is likely to do before he

actually does it is referred to as anticipation. The process underlying

anticipation skills are likely to be perceptual and cognitive in nature. Over the

last 20 years or so scientists have been able to answer many of the mysteries

in this field (Stratton et al., 2004).

The main purpose of training is to improve human capabilities in all

their manifestations. These capabilities are characterized in physical,

psychomotor and psychological attributes. The highest level for example in

fitness assessments, comprises limits to human performance, and training

programs must therefore be designed to raise these functional limits (Reilly,

2007).

Soccer is a team sport in which the ball may be played with the foot

and head. The only exception is the goalkeeper, who may also use his hands

within his own penalty area. The 11 players on a team seek to hit the ball into

the opponent’s goal and to prevent the opposing side from doing the same.

The basic requirements for a successful soccer team are a combination of

physical fitness, individual talents and technique, and last but not least

effective teamwork (Mayer & Hermann, 2009).

There is also widespread acceptance amongst coaches that at the elite

level players are more often differentiated by these ‘mental’ or psychological

factors rather than by physical or physiological characteristics. (William &

Reilly, 2000).

Another common perception amongst coaches is that the ability to

‘read the game’ and to make appropriate strategic and tactical decisions are

Page 80: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

80

largely innate or, at best, far too difficult to improve through practice and

instruction (Stratton et al., 2004).

Salmon et al. (1994) examined the ways in which soccer players utilize

various levels of mental training. Soccer players use mental training more

often in competition than in training sessions and in both cases, more before

than after playing. Elite athletes make more frequent use of mental training

than non-elite athletes.

The penalty kick is often a critical determinant of success in soccer.

While the odds for success favors the penalty taker instead of the goalkeeper,

the outcome of a penalty kick remains uncertain (McGarry & Franks, 2005).

Penalty kicks were introduced in soccer in 1891 – for the express

purpose of giving a player in the 18 – yard box another opportunity to try for a

goal after an attempt was denied by a major foul.

Penalty kick shootouts were first introduced in a major competition in

the 1976 European Championship. Czechoslovakia had been leading West

Germany 2-0 in the Final until the West Germans equalized in the last minute.

There were no goals scored in the overtime. the Finals committee had agree

to host the first penalty kick tiebreaker, which West Germany subsequently

lost (Witzig, 2006).

Taking a penalty kick in a shoot-out is a discrete motor skill in which

there is a recognizable beginning and end. It is, however, a complex

movement which requires a high level “motor” aspect. The two general control

strategies for taking a penalty kick, presented by Kuhn (1988), are closed and

Page 81: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

81

open loop control, referring to the control process for responding to, and

ignoring, the goalkeeper’s movements, respectively (Bowtell, 2009).

The soccer “penalty kick” in the knock-out phase of major international

tournaments is one of the most dramatic events in international soccer. The

outcome of these kicks is typically attributed to such factors as psychology

(e.g. coping with stress), skill (e.g. kicking technique), physiology (e.g.

overcoming the fatigue of 120 min play), and chance (e.g. in what direction

the goalkeeper moves) (Jordet et al., 2007).

Penalty kicks create a simple but interesting real-life decision-making

task under uncertainty. The topic of judgment and decision making under

uncertainty has attracted the attention of sport/exercise psychologists (Bar-Eli

& Raab, 2006). But, skilled soccer players are able to use information from

opponents’ postural orientation to anticipate their intentions. For example,

through the use of a variety of experimental techniques such as film

occlusion, questionnaires and eye movement recording, researchers have

shown that skilled soccer goalkeepers are able to use information arising from

a penalty taker’s run-up and body shape immediately prior to foot-ball contact

to anticipate the destination of the penalty kick successfully (Williams et al,

1999; Stratton et al., 2004).

After analyzing penalty kicks from the FIFA World Cup Finals between

1982 and 1994, Franks et al. (1999) reported that it takes about 600 ms from

the instant of foot contact for the ball to cross the goal line, whereas it takes in

the region of 500-700 ms for the goalkeeper to complete the movement in an

attempt to stop the shot. For the optimal goalkeeping performance the

Page 82: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

82

decision to initiate movement (including the direction of the dive) must

therefore be made some time before the ball contact (McGarry & Franks,

2005). And also, a player taking a penalty in competition must be clear on

which strategy will give the greatest chance of scoring. For an average-paced

shot, 32 m•s-1, it takes just 344 ms for the ball to reach the goal-line (Morya

et al., 2005). Therefore, goalkeepers must dive prior to the player making

contact with the ball to have the greatest probability of saving the shot. Some

players use a strategy in which they aim to take advantage of this by watching

the keeper’s movement and adjust their kicking motion to shoot into the empty

side of the goal (Bowtell et al., 2009).

The nature of the penalty kick necessitates a decision by the

goalkeeper in real-time based on the detection of visual cues. Laboratory-

based studies have reported 100 ms to be the lower limit for reacting correctly

to these visual cues. Thus, the goalkeeper must detect and react to visual

cues provided by the penalty taker approximately 100 ms before the ball is

contacted if he is to stop the shot. The visual cues that are provided outside of

this window occur to early or too late to make a decision. The goalkeeper

should not react too late of course given the constraints of reaction time and

movement time noted earlier (McGarry & Franks, 2005).

The consensus in the literature is that the most important visual cues

or clues include the orientation of the penalty taker’s non-kicking foot, which

tends to point in the same direction as the ball’s intended destination, the

angle of the hips prior to ball contact, which tend to slope away from the

goalkeeper when the ball is played to his right side, assuming a right-footed

Page 83: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

83

penalty taker, and the arc of the kicking leg as it approaches the ball (see

Franks and Hanvey, 1997; Savelsbergh et al., 2002 in Stratton et al., 2004, p.

140).

According to Palacios-Huerta (2003), the time it takes the ball to get

from the penalty mark to the goal is about 0.3 s; this would imply that because

of the short distance between the ball and the goal, and because of the high

speed at which the ball is kicked, the goalkeeper usually cannot afford to wait

until he sees clearly in what direction the ball is kicked (Bar-Eli et al., 2009).

Therefore, the goalkeeper should react too early for two reasons. The first

reason is that the goalkeeper will not benefit from subsequent (more reliable)

visual cues provided by the penalty taker.

The increase in penalty-kick speeds, from times to the goal of 600 ms

in the 1980's to 300 ms in 2005 shows goalkeepers must increasingly decide

on their course of action before the ball is kicked. In the opinion of American

soccer player Jonathan Bass (2012) this results in an increasing advantage

for the penalty-kicker, but results necessarily in an arms-race where faster

and more subtle goalkeepers are required to mitigate the advantage of

increasingly high velocity kickers. Those penalty-kickers are then pushed to

increase their penalty-kick velocities in order to maintain their advantage.

For example, Frank et al. (1999) reported goalkeepers to be about 46%

correct in their direction of movement when they reacted too early. (In the

1982-94 World Cup Finals, the movement of the goalkeeper before ball

contact was against the rules but was allowed in practice nonetheless).

Excluding any decision to not move, the reported 46% accuracy in prediction

Page 84: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

84

of shot direction is consistent with that of 50% that would be expected from

chance (i.e. guessing). The second reason for the goalkeeper not to react to

early cues is that the goalkeeper will provide the penalty taker with late visual

cues before the instant of ball contact (McGarry & Franks, 2005, p. 272).

Given all that, the goalkeeper has to make a decision whether to jump

to one side or to stay in the center, and this decision is made at about the

same time or before that the kicker chooses where to direct the kick. The

goalkeeper has to choose whether to jump to the right or to the left, or to stay

in the center, in order to minimize the risk of a goal being scored, under

conditions of uncertainty regarding the direction and height of the ball (Bar-Eli

et al., 2009). That means, the decision-making abilities of the goalkeeper

within the time constraints mandated by the penalty kick will be improved if

reliable visual cues provided by the penalty taker can be detected in sufficient

time (Frank et al., 1999; McGarry & Franks, 2005).

The goalkeeper may use his knowledge of the directional distribution of

the penalty kicks in general, the past behavior of the kicker, and cues that

might be obtained from the kicker’s behavior such as approach ot the ball, to

help him decide to which side to jump, if at all. Similarly, the kicker has to

decide how to take the shot, facing uncertainty regarding what the goalkeeper

will do (Bar-Eli et al., 2009, p. 99).

Previous studies have shown that right-footed players are more

accurate shooting to the left, and vice versa for left footed players, thus giving

rise to the terms natural side and unnatural side for the more and less

accurate sides respectively (Chiappori et al, 2002; Palacios-Huerta, 2003). So

Page 85: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

85

it is proposed that a player should be better able to successfully adjust, from

shooting from the unnatural side to shooting towards the natural side, in less

time than the reverse (Bowtell et al., 2009).

Furthermore, McGarry and Franks (2000) analyzed all line-up

combinations using computer simulations to try to answer the question of

which penalty kick should the best penalty taker be assigned – the first

penalty kick in order (hopefully) to get the team off to a good start, the fifth

penalty kick, which might be considered as the most important should the

shoot-out reach that far, or the third penalty kick , which is more important

than the first penalty kick while, at the same time, guarantees that the best

penalty taker will take a kick in the shoot-out? The analyses indicated that the

order of 5-4-3-2-1 represents the best line-up with which to contest a penalty

shoot-out-, that is, the fifth best penalty taker should take the first penalty kick,

the fourth best penalty taker the second penalty kick and so on. These results

hold for a certain set of initial conditions. The best strategy following a tie after

five kicks each (i.e ‘sudden death’) is to assign the next best penalty taker to

the next penalty kick. This is to say that the sixth best penalty taker should be

assigned to the sixth penalty kick, the seventh best penalty taker to the

seventh penalty kick and so on. This sequence applies for both teams

regardless of which team wins the coin toss and hence takes the first penalty

kick (McGarry & Franks, 2005).

Page 86: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

86

4. Methods

This chapter explains the hypotheses of the study and describes the

pilot study as well as the research design. In further steps the selected

variables of the study and their measurement are stated and the statistical

procedures which were used to analyse the data in regard to the hypotheses

are described.

4.1 Pilot Study

4.1.1 Tennis

The study takes a critical look at the potential effects of mental training

on performance of tennis players. Thirteen professional tennis players were

interviewed to determine their opinions on the importance of mental skills

training, and the specific mental skills they use before, during, and after a

tennis match. Three professional tennis coaches were also interviewed to

determine the specific mental skills they teach and the strategies they use to

teach mental skills. This study was conducted at the Davidoff Swiss Indoors

Tournament in Basel in November 2009. Only one player said that he didn’t

use mental training; the rest of the sample said that mental training is a very

individual process and that the most important mental training skills were:

concentration, positive imagery, self-confidence, and relaxation. Furthermore,

the respondents felt that tennis players should focus on the process rather

than the outcome.

4.1.2 Soccer

The pilot study was conducted in July 2010 with student athletes at the

University of Konstanz. A small representative sample was selected as the

Page 87: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

87

experimental group, in this case four student athletes in the age between 20

and 22. They completed a three-week training program with four sessions

including previous pre- and followed post-test. They also carried out the

mental training and relaxation in the first three sessions. Thereafter they had

were tasked with reading a workbook daily during these three weeks. Besides

the quantitative methods, also qualitative methods were used to find mistakes

or opportunities to optimize in the procedure of the study. Interviews of the

athletes investigated their experiences during the intervenetion and their

impression of the relaxation and the workbook.

To separate the target area to be hit, a cord was tested for suitability. It

was tested whether this line is seen from the penalty spot and whether is are

robust enough to resist wind and rain. It was also tested to see how they

could be fixed on the ground (with tent peg or brick). Due to statistical reasons

the size of the target area was chosen well, almost half (46%) of the shots of

the athletes hit the target area in the pre-test, which implies that also possible

high deviations in the precision could be measured in the post-test.

Furthermore the workbook has been seen as helpful and understandable.

However, it was noted that the relaxation period should be longer than it was

in the pilot study to access the necessary calm and concentration. The

position of the camera, which records the players during the first training

seemed to be best placed in the pitch circle just outside of the eighteen-yard

area. The exact location depended on the run-up of the player. The use of

video-analyses was also mentioned as positive, especially, when the camera

was focused simultaneously on the player and the goal (Kiefer, 2011).

Page 88: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

88

4.2 Hypotheses

The study is based on the following hypotheses:

1- The intervention group will improve their performance in precision

measuring tests between the pre- and post- tests, more than the

control group in tennis and soccer.

2- The age of the subjects has an influence on both the results in the

precision pre-test and the effectiveness of the mental training

intervention in tennis and soccer.

3- The imagery ability of the subjects has an influence on both the results

in the precision pre-test and the effectiveness of the mental training

intervention in tennis and soccer.

4- The commitment of the subjects has an influence on both the results in

the precision pre-test and the effectiveness of the mental training

intervention in tennis and soccer.

5- The level of somatic fear, anxiety (unease) and confidence of the

subjects has an influence on both the results in the precision pre-test

and the effectiveness of the mental training intervention in tennis and

soccer.

6- The ability of the subjects to concentrate has an influence on both the

results in the precision pre-test and the effectiveness of the mental

training intervention in tennis and soccer.

In addition to these hypotheses, I will test sex differences with regard to

the effectiveness of mental training for the tennis samples in an

exploratory manner.

Page 89: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

89

4.3 Design of the study

The design for the current study is “one-within + one-between” design

(experimental study with a control group) (Figure 2) (Haag et al., 2004).

Figure 2. Experimental study with a control group, a ‘one-within+one-between’ design.

The study investigates the effects of mental training as applied to two

closed-skill activities. At the beginning of the study, the subjects are divided

randomly into two groups. After this selection, the experimental or intervention

group received both technical practice and mental training units. The control

group received only technical practice (Normal training) for the same time

period. The real effect of the training program can be evaluated by comparing

the change in the experimental-group with the change in the control-group

(Figure 3) (Haag et al., 2004).

Study

population

intervention

intervention period

X = measurement

time

Page 90: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

90

Figure 3. An experimental study with a control group, effect of the intervention can not be ‘biased’ by any natural development.

4.4 Participants

Tennis

A total of 60 (38 male, 22 female) amateur athletes with a high skill

level in tennis, ages 12 to 35 in Germany were selected as subjects for the

study. The classification of the clubs in intervention group included (Tennis

Club Konstanz, Tennis Club Markdorf, Tennis Club Sportcenter in Konstanz,

Tennis Club Radolfzell).

The classification of the clubs in the control group included (Tennis

Club Fellbach, Tennis Club Readlingen, Tennis Club Bad Buchau, Tennis

Club Bad Saulgau). The tennis study was carried out between November

2010 and April 2011.

Participants which were randomly assigned to a no-imagery control

group (CG: n= 31) received only technical practice for the same time period,

arbitrary value

intervention group

control group

intervention

period time

Page 91: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

91

the intervention group (IG: n= 29) received both technical practice and mental

training units.

Soccer

A total of 54 (male) soccer athletes playing in the third league, ages 14

to 22 in Germany were chosen to participate in the study. The intervention

group practiced at the soccer field Waldheim in Konstanz, and the control

group practiced at the soccer field in Wollmatingen). The soccer study was

conducted between October and December 2010.

Participants were randomly assigned to a no-imagery control group

(CG: n= 21) which received only technical practice for this time period. The

intervention group (IG: n= 33) received both technical practice and mental

training units.

The mental training units for both tennis and soccer included imagery

instructions on the court or field using video analysis, as well as an imagery

workbook for daily practice at home. The duration of intervention was four

weeks.

4.5 Measurements

4.5.1 Tennis Precision Test (TPT)

The TPT consisted of 20 first serves on a standard tennis court based

on the guidance of the DTB (Deutscher Tennis Bund). Due to the time of year

when the study took place indoor granulate courts were chosen. The subjects

were asked to take their serve position on the base line and serve alternately

in the target areas advantage and deuce (2 rounds). The target areas were

2m long by 1m wide (as illustrated in Figure 4). The target area for the deuce

Page 92: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

92

side was placed next to the middle line of the serve field. The same is true for

the advantage side where the middle line is seen as the axis of reflection. The

rectangular field was marked with small cones. This way the target area was

visible from the baseline. As soon as a player hit a cone it was repositioned

before the next serve. There were no temporal restrictions.

Figure 4. Target areas for the tennis precision test and the imagination exercise.

4.5.2 Penalty Precision Test (PPT)

The PPT included 20 shots from the penalty spot, with no time

restriction, on a specially prepared standard soccer goal which conforms to

FIFA guidelines. On this goal, target areas are marked with a cord which is

hung at a distance of 70cm exactly parallel to the goal posts (as illustrated in

Figure 5).

Page 93: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

93

Figure 5. Target areas for the penalty precision test and the imagination exercise (Sketch of Kiefer, 2011).

The kick was considered a hit if it passed between the rope and the

goal post, and considered not hit if the ball passed either outside of the goal

or in the area between the ropes. Each subject took four series of five shots

on goal, for a total of 20 shots, from the penalty spot, and each shot was

measured as a hit or not a hit. The hits of each player were summed up for

the data analysis. If the shot touched the cord and moved it out of its position,

it had to be brought back into position before the next shot and the shot was

counted as a hit.

4.5.3 Questionnaire

The questionnaire of the study consists of single elements explained in

the following:

The Questionnaire starts with the FAIR (Frankfurter Aufmerksamkeits-

Inventar). The FAIR measures the inter-individual difference in attention and

concentration of the subjects. It is suitable for 9-72 year olds which means it is

applicable for the population. The test consists of 640 items (2 test sheets

with 320 items each, and 3min time allowed for each) that have to be judged

Page 94: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

94

individually. Two kinds of items have to be marked (circle inside circle with

three dots in its center; square inside circle with two dots in its center). The

items are distributed stochasticly, in lines, each line is to be marked with a

pen, line by line, as illustrated in Figure 6.

Figure 6. Example of editing and distribution of the items (Moosbrugger & Goldhammer, 2010).

There are two test forms. Form A differentiates better in the upper

range of performance and B is more applicable in the lower range of

performance. For the intervention on hand form A was chosen. FAIR was

used as an individual test of the subjects, not as a group test. The

performance value (L) was calculated as a representation of the speed, and

one quality oriented value (Q) was calculated representing the absence of

mistakes. The multiplication of L and Q led to a value K (extent of

continuously sustaining concentration). The execution, evaluation and

interpretation of the test is impartial and normalized. Validity of content as well

as of the construction has been proven. The split-half-reliabilities are for the

test values L and K are between α=.90 and α=.91. The parallel-test reliabilities

(within one week) are between r=.81 and r=.83. The re-test-reliability gives

values between rtt=.85 and rtt=.91. The pure test duration is 6 minutes

(Moosbrugger & Oehlschlägel, 1996; Moosbrugger & Goldhammer, 2010).

The “Movement Imagery Questionnaire” (MIQ) by Hall and Pongrac

(1983) is the next section of the questionnaire. αvisual=.87-.89 (Cronbach) and

Page 95: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

95

αkinesthetic=.86-.91 point to good reliability. The test is not temporally limited and

is recommended for young and strong people. Morris et al. (2005) prove a

moderate to high validity for the MIQ correlation to the ability of the target

group. The German version of the MIQ that is used in this intervention can be

found in Mayer and Hermann (2009, p. 219-228). In the study, questions 1 to

4 are applied. They contain exercises for arms, legs and the whole body. The

remaining 14 questions are not included due to time constraints. Each

movement exercise has to be executed physically and then mentally.

Depending on how difficult it was to imagine the movement, a number

between (1 = “easy to imagine” to 8 = “difficult to imagine”) was marked. Two

questions refer to the visual (the first and the third) and to the kinesthetic

(second and fourth) imagery ability. The totals for the exercises are then

summed up. A low sum (2 at least) indicates a high imagery ability and a high

sum (16 at maximum) indicates a low imagery ability. The test for imagery

ability lasts about seven minutes. As mentioned in Figure 7.

Assume the starting position described on the right and do the exercise. Then read the instructions on the mental exercise.

Starting position

Clench your dominating fist (the one you write with) and bring this hand to your shoulder (for example right hand to right shoulder) so that your elbow points directly forward. Exercise

Stretch out your arm such that your hand leaves your shoulder and that it is points directly out in front of you, being outstretched. Your fist continues to be clenched. Slowly do this movement. Mental Exercise Take the start position (as described above). Try to imagine the movement from above as clearly as possible, but do not execute this movement physically.

Now rate this mental exercise from easy to imagine up to hard to imagine. Please mark the field that represents your opinion best.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

Easy hard to imagine

Page 96: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

96

Assume the starting position described on the right and do the exercise. Then read the instructions on the mental exercise.

Starting position

Stand upright, your feet shoulders width apart and keep your arms at your sides. Exercise

Lift your right knee as high as possible, so that you are standing on your left leg, with your right leg crooked. Now lower your right leg, so that you are standing on both of your legs again. Do this movement slowly. Mental Exercise

Take the start position (as described above). Try to feel the how you performed the previous movement (without really doing the movement).

Now rate this mental exercise from easy to imagine up to hard to imagine. Please mark the field that represents your opinion best.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

Easy to hard imagine

Assume the starting position described on the right and do the exercise. Then read the instructions on the mental exercise.

Starting position

Stand upright, your feet shoulders width apart and keep your arms at your sides. Exercise

Squat down then jump straight upward as high as possible. Stretch your arms into the sky high above your head. Land with your feet a shoulder width apart and lower your arms again. Mental Exercise

Take the start position (as described above). Try to imagine the movement from above as clearly as possible.

Now rate this mental exercise from easy to imagine up to hard to imagine. Please mark the field that represents your opinion best.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

Easy to hard imagine

Assume the starting position described on the right and do the exercise. Then read the instructions on the mental exercise.

Starting position

Stand upright, your feet shoulders width apart and keep your arms at your sides. Exercise Jump and spin left in the air such that you land back in the starting position. In other words do a 360° rotation. Mental Exercise Take on your starting position. Try to feel how you did the previous movement (without really doing the move).

Now rate this mental exercise from easy to imagine up to hard to imagine. Please mark the field that represents your opinion best.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

Easy to hard imagine

Figure 7. Some questions which test imagery ability are presented.

Page 97: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

97

In the next questionnaire the commitment. The Psychologist for

motivation and social studies Prof.Dr.Gollwitzer (University of Konstanz)

referred to an article by Hollenbeck et al. (1989). The following questions were

chosen Figure 8.

Please specify how much you agree with the following statements

I want to pass the mental training as well as possible.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

correct not

It would be a pity if I could not pass the mental training as well as possible.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

correct not

I feel myself to be connected to the mental training.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

correct not

I believe that the mental training will improve my performance.

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

correct not

Figure 8. Questions about the commitment of the subjects to the mental training.

The identification of the subjects commitment was measured via an

ordinal scale from 1 (high commitment) to 8 (no commitment). A low sum (4

at least) indicates that the subject identifies with the study and believes in the

efficacy of the mental training. A high sum (32 at maximum) corresponds to

the opposite. The validity was empirically tested by Hollenbeck et al. (1989).

The Cronbach α=.71 means a satisfying reliability of the test.

The next questionnaire is the Wettkampf-Angst-Inventar State (WAI-S)

is a short questionnaire and measures the values corresponding to the

subjects’ emotional state (somatic fear, anxiety (unease) and confidence).

This test (key data included) can be found in Brand et al. (2009) as well as on

Page 98: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

98

the homepage of the “Bundesinstitut für Sportwissenschaften” (BiSp)1. WAI-S

is a psychometric validated short questionnaire composed of 12 items, (BiSP,

2011b) including the Competitive Anxiety Inventory (CSAI-2) by Martens et al.

(1990). The reliability of the Cronbach α can be seen as good. For the

somatic fear value the reliability is given with αsom. fear = .81. For the unease

value with αunease = .79 and for the confidence value with αconfidence = .82

(BISP, 2011a). In figure 9, the 12 items of the WAI-S used in the study are

shown, the ranking of the ordinal scale is from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). Also

the description of the task can be seen in the top of the figure. All included

statements refer to the feelings at the moment of which four subscales are

assigned to somatic fear (2,4,7,10), anxiety (Unease) (1,6,9,11) and

confidence (3,5,8,12) as shown in Figure 9.

Now, at this moment… 1. not at all 2. a little bit 3. pretty 4. a lot …I believe to perform not as good as I could

□1 □2 □3 □4

…my heart is throbbing

□1 □2 □3 □4

.. I am convinced I am able to realize my full performance

□1 □2 □3 □4

… I have an upset stomach

□1 □2 □3 □4

…I feel confident because success is in sight

□1 □2 □3 □4

…I am concerned about people being disappointed by my performance

□1 □2 □3 □4

…I feel shivery

□1 □2 □3 □4

… I am sure to pass the □1 □2 □3 □4

1

http://www.bisp.de/cln_090/nn_15924/SharedDocs/Publikationen/SpoPsy/DE/Fragebogen/

WAI__S,templateId=raw,property=publicationFile.pdf/WAI_S.pdf

Page 99: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

99

precision test

…I am concerned about being unable to deal with the pressure

□1 □2 □3 □4

.. I feel a strong tension in my body

□1 □2 □3 □4

…I am concerned that I will perform badly

□1 □2 □3 □4

… I am confident that I will master the challenge

□1 □2 □3 □4

Figure 9. WAI-S, used in the questionnaire to measure somatic fear, anxiety (unease) and confidence.

Somatic fear describes the experience of corporal noticeable signs of

fear, like heart palpitation, wet hands, or feeling queasy. Anxiety or cognitive

unease measures the experience of self-doubts, specific concerns or negative

expectations of the athlete before a competition. Confidence deals with the

perception of the athlete, to go in a competition self-confident and well

prepared and to see this as a challenge rather than a threat.

For the evaluation the sum-scores for each component are calculated.

In Figure 9, the WAI-S is pictured.

The questionnaire ends with demographical and performance data and

questions on the fitness and mental training experience level Figure 10.

Page 100: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

100

What is your sex? □ Male □ Female

How old are you? Years

How tall are you? Cm

In what League/Class does you play in?

□ Bundesliga (“Federal League”) □ Regionalliga (“Regional League”) □ Südwestliga/Badenliga □ Oberliga (“First League”) □ 1. Bezirksliga □ 2. Bezirksliga □ Bezirksklasse 1 □ Bezirksklasse 2 □ (1.Kreisliga) □ (2. Kreisliga and lower) □ no team player

How many times did you practice in the last week? (in days)

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7

About how many times did you practice weekly during the last three months? (in days)

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7

Do you have any previous experience with mental training?

□1 □2 □3 □4 □5 □6 □7 □8

True Not

Figure 10. Demographical and personal data of the subjects.

The questions on training frequency in the last week and within the last

three months should be answered with number of practice days , from 1 =

“one day” to 7 = “seven days.”

The questions about height are based on the metric scale. And the last

question concerning the previous experience with mental training was

measured with one item, namely: “Do you have previous experience with

mental training?” The subjects could answer from 1 = “true” to 8 = “not true.” A

low sum indicates more experiences with mental training, and a high sum

indicates to the opposite.

Page 101: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

101

At the end of the study the last page in the questionnaire (Figure 11)

includes the results of precision pre- and post- test and the numbers of

attendances during the four weeks.

The results of precision pre- test: The results of precision post- test: The numbers of attendances the four Weeks:

Figure 11. The last page in the questionnaire.

4.6 Implementation

At the beginning of the study the subjects of the study were split into

intervention and control groups. The tennis study was carried out from

November 2010 until April 2011. Whereas, the soccer study was conducted

between October and December 2010. The intervention group in both tennis

and soccer received both technical practice and mental training units. This

included positive imagery instruction on the court using video analysis on a

weekly basis, as well as an imagery workbook (see Appendix) which included

several imagery and concentration tasks, energy level and imagery ability

exercises as well as sensory tasks for daily practice at home. The exercises in

this workbook were based on the work of Kluge (2008).

In total, four sessions were held. The duration of the intervention

group's first session in tennis / soccer was 90 minutes, the duration of the

second, third, and the fourth sessions was 60 minutes in tennis, whereas, in

/ / / /

/ / / /

Page 102: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

102

soccer they were 90 minutes. The subjects were informed about the purpose

of the study, and they were assured that their data would remain confidential.

The first session established contact: the athletes were informed about

the content of the study and were encouraged to participate. Participants filled

out the questionnaire described above, which took about 15 to 20 minutes.

After a short warm-up period, they practiced serving in tennis, penalty kicking

in soccer. Then they watched practice videos to analyze their personal

serving or kicking motion with the help of video analysis using SimiVidBack

1.5 (by Simi Reality Motion Systems). This was followed by the

breathing/relaxation exercise with mental training using the self-instruction

and the Positive visualization (Internal/External) of the ball’s trajectory, which

lasted about ten minutes (see Appendix). Thereafter, the athletes were asked

to integrate this mental training into their respective per-performance rituals.

The subjects were then given a precision test to determine how accurately

they hit a target (20 serves (TPT)/penalty kicks (PPT). Pre-test). Finally, there

was a short debriefing session. At the end of the first session the athletes

were given a workbook entitled “Mental Training” (see Appendix ). They were

asked to complete exercises at home during the period of the intervention with

the help of these workbooks. The exercises were divided into blocks: in the

first week, the first block consisted of three exercises on sensory perception;

in the second week, the second block had three exercises on the regulation of

the subject's energy level; and in the third week, the third block presented four

exercises on visualization of movement. The workbook exercises were

voluntary, but the athletes were motivated to complete them, since this was a

Page 103: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

103

way to work on mental training that could eventually be beneficial and

strengthen the long-term effects of the intervention sessions.

The second session began after a short warm-up period with the

breathing relaxation exercise and mental training (including visualization of

the ball’s trajectory), lasting about ten minutes. Afterward, as in the first

session, the athletes were asked to incorporate the visualization techniques

into their performance rituals, and were shown practice videos. After the

practical exercises, a debriefing session followed. The third session was

identical to the second.

The fourth session began as the second and third had, but after the

practical exercises another precision test was carried out, as in the first

session (20 serves (TPT)/penalty kicks (PPT). Post-test). Afterward, there

was a debriefing, and the athletes were thanked for their participation.

The control group in both tennis and soccer received only technical

practice (Normal training) for the same time period, without mental training or

practice videos. Additionally, the tennis group received a control group

workbook which included articles with photos about tennis specific training,

some of which referred to sport psychology from the “German tennis

magazine” (e.g. Von den Besten lernen- Hoch hinaus- Raumgewinn).

Similarly, in soccer the control group received a control group

workbook which included articles with photos about soccer specific training,

some of which partially referred to sport psychology from the “German soccer

magazine” (e.g. “fussballtraining” magazines 5/2009- 4/2010 published by

Philippka-Sportverlag.

Page 104: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

104

In total, four sessions were held. The duration of the first control group

session in tennis/soccer was 90 minutes, the duration of the second, the third,

and the fourth sessions was 60 minutes in tennis and soccer.

4.7 Data evaluation

The data was evaluated with SPSS Windows, version 17.0

(24.02.2010). Due to the hypothesis formulation the following methods were

chosen for data evaluation:

Hypothesis one: ANOVA with repeated measurement was used for the

investigation of the interaction of time and groups as measured by the change

in tennis serve and soccer penalty precision. The results of the pre-test and

the post-test are the dependent variables. The groups (intersubjective factor)

and the time (intrasubjective factor) are the independent variables.

Hypotheses 2-6: These investigate the effectiveness of the mental

training intervention and its influence on imagery ability. The commitment, the

level of somatic fear, anxiety, and confidence, ability to concentrate, and age

were investigated in regard to possible effects on the efficacy of a four-week

mental training intervention with a control group which received only technical

practice without mental training for the same time period. The analysis of the

correlation between the named factors on the difference between the pre- and

post-test results of the intervention group subjects was used in terms of both

(tennis serve and soccer penalty) groups. In addition to these hypotheses, I

will test sex differences with regard to the effectiveness of mental training for

the tennis subjects in an exploratory manner.

Page 105: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

105

5. Results

In this chapter the results of the study are presented, discussed and

included in the context of the relevant scientific literature. The statistics were

calculated for each of the two games (tennis serve and penalty kick in

soccer).

5.1 Tennis Precision Test (TPT) and Penalty Precision Test (PPT)

In tennis, the results of ANOVA with repeated measurements show a

significant interaction effect between time and group in TPT (F1, 55 = 22.5;

ETA2 = .29). The mean for the hit ratio of the intervention group increased

from pre-test (Mpre= 6.3; SDpre= 3.0 ) to post-test (Mpost = 10.4; SDpost= 2.9)

while the mean of the control group (Mpre= 4.0; SDpre= 2.6; Mpost= 5.0; SDpost=

2.8 did not change significantly (Table 1).

Table 1. Results of the Intervention and Control Groups in Tennis Serve Precision Test in Pre- and Post-Test (Descriptive Statistics).

TPT Group Mean SD N

Result TPT - pre-test

Intervention group 6.30 2.99 31

Control group 4.00 2.58 29

Total 5.25 3.03 60

Result TPT- post-test

Intervention group 10.40 2.86 28

Control group 5.04 2.85 29

Total 7.70 3.91 57

N= number of subjects SD=standard deviation

The results of ANOVA with repeated measurements (Table 2) shown

that the effect of time is significant and explains 54% of the within-subjects

variance in the precision test on the tennis serve.

The interaction effect between time and group is also significant, which

explains 29% of the within-subjects variance of precision test in the tennis

serve. This means that both groups developed differently over time.

Page 106: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

106

The improvement in the precision of the serve in the experimental

group was significantly higher than the improvement in the control group. This

indicates that the intervention consisting of mental training significantly

improved the precision of the serve (Table 2).

Table 2. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts in Tennis

Effects Type III Sum of Squares

Df Mean

Square F Sig.

Partial Eta Squared

Time 170.520 1 170.520 64.529 .000 0.540

Time * Group 59.608 1 59.608 22.557 .000 0.291

Error(time) 145.339 55 2.643

Looking at the Between-Subjects Variance (Table 3), only group

membership had a significant effect on precision. The intervention group

showed higher precision rates than the control group (Figure 12). The effect of

the intervention explains 37% of the between-subjects variance.

Table 3. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects in Tennis

Effects Type III Sum of

Squares Df

Mean Square

F Sig. Partial Eta Squared

Intercept 4800.888 1 4800.888 361.577 .000 0.868

Group 436.010 1 436.010 32.838 .000 0.374

Error 730.270 55 13.278

Page 107: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

107

Figure 12. Results of Both Groups in the Pre-Test and Post-Test (TPT) in Tennis.

Whereas, in soccer, the results of the PPT of the intervention and

control groups in pre- and post-tests can be seen in (table 4). The intervention

group improve their averaged kicking numbers from the pre-test to the post-

test (IGpre= 8.8, IGpost= 9.6) However, the control group did not improve their

average kicking numbers from the pre-test to the post-test (CGpre= 9.0,

CGpost= 8.24).

Table 4. Results of the Intervention and Control Group in Soccer Penalty Kick Precision Test

in Pre- and Post-Test (Descriptive statistics).

PPT Group Mean SD N

Result PPT - pre-test

Intervention group 8.80 2.30 33

Control group 9.00 2.24 21

Total 8.90 2.24 54

Result PPT- post-test

Intervention group 9.60 2.45 33

Control group 8.24 3.10 21

Total 9.10 2.80 54

The results of ANOVA with repeated measurements (table 5) show

that the effect of time is not significant in the precision test in soccer. Nor, the

Page 108: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

108

interaction effect between time and group is significant, but it shows a

tendency (.09 above significance level) This means, that the intervention

group had an improvement in the number of kicks, the control group,

however, shows a decline.

Table 5. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts in Soccer.

Effects Type III Sum of

Squares Df

Mean Square

F Sig. Partial

Eta Squared

Time .020 1 .020 .005 .946 .000

Time * Group 16.020 1 16.020 3.713 .059 .067

Error(Time) 224.359 52 4.315

Looking at the Between-Subjects Variance (Table 6) shows that the

intervention group increased their precision test results, while the control

group shows lower values in the PPT post-test than in the PPT pre-test. There

are outliers in both groups (Figure 13).

Table 6. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects in Soccer.

Effects Type III Sum of

Squares Df

Mean Square

F Sig. Partial Eta Squared

Intercept 8119.179 1 8119.179 996.756 .000 .950

Group 7.697 1 7.697 .945 .336 .018

Error 423.571 52 8.146

Page 109: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

109

Figure 13. Results of Both Groups in the Pre-Test and Post-Test (PPT) in Soccer.

5.2 The Imagery Ability

In tennis, the intervention group shows a higher mean in the visual

imagery ability, the control group, however, shows a higher mean in the

kinesthetic imagery ability (Table 7) as measured with the MIQ before the

intervention.

Table 7. Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability of the Intervention and Control Groups in Tennis

Imagery ability

Group N Mean SD

Visual imagery

Intervention group

31 4.4 1.5

Control group 29 4.2 2.0

Kinesthetic imagery

Intervention group

31 4.5 1.7

Control group 29 5.1 3.0

The intervention group as well as the control group show a weakly

inverse correlation between the imagery ability and the results in the TPT pre-

Page 110: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

110

test. As shown in the tables (8, 9) there is no significant correlation of both

imagery ability values. In the intervention group (Table 8), the correlation

coefficient of the visual imagery is (P= .21). The correlation coefficient of the

kinesthetic imagery ability is (P= .45). Furthermore, in the control group (Table

9), the correlation coefficient with the visual imagery is (P= .45). The

correlation coefficient of the kinesthetic imagery ability is (P= .26).

Furthermore, the imagery ability in tennis did not have a significant

influence on the effectiveness of the mental training. A correlation analysis

was applied to the test subjects and no significant correlation of visual and

kinesthetic imagery ability with the difference of the TPT pre- and post-test

results could be shown in the intervention and control groups. (Tables 8, 9).

Table 8. Correlation Between Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability, Number of Hits in the TPT Pre- Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Tennis (N=31)

Intervention group Visual

imagery Kinesthetic

imagery

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.23 -.14

Sig. (2-tailed) .21 .45

N 31 31

Difference

Pearson Correlation .08 .04

Sig. (2-tailed) .69 .83

N 28 28

Table 9. Correlation Between Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability, Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-

test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Tennis (N=29)

Control group Visual

imagery Kinesthetic

imagery

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.15 -.22

Sig. (2-tailed) .45 .26

N 29 29

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.28 -.18

Sig. (2-tailed) .14 .33

N 29 29

Page 111: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

111

However, in soccer, the intervention group shows a lower mean in the

visual imagery ability, similarly, the control group, shows a lower mean in the

kinesthetic imagery ability (Table 10) as measured with the MIQ before the

intervention.

Table 10. Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability in Intervention and Control Groups in Soccer

Imagery Ability Group N Mean SD

Visual imagery

Intervention 33 4.06 1.75

Control 22 5.27 2.81

Kinesthetic imagery Intervention 33 5.00 2.47

Control 22 4.68 2.34

Similarly, in soccer, the intervention group as well as the control group

shows a weak inverse correlation between the imagery ability and the results

in the PPT pre-test. As shown in the tables (11, 12) there is no significant

correlation of both imagery ability values. In the intervention group (Table 11),

the correlation coefficient of the visual imagery is (P= .14) The correlation

coefficient of the kinesthetic imagery ability is (P= .69). Furthermore, in the

control group (Table 12), the correlation coefficient of the visual imagery is

(P= .78) The correlation coefficient of the kinesthetic imagery ability is (P=

.43).

Table 11. Correlation Between Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability, Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Soccer

Intervention group Visual

imagery Kinethetic imagery

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .26 .07

Sig. (2-tailed) .14 .69

N 33 33

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.08 .17

Sig. (2-tailed) .64 .35

N 33 33

Page 112: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

112

Table 12. Correlation Between Visual and Kinesthetic Imagery Ability, Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-

Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Soccer

Control group Visual imagery Kinethetic imagery

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.06 -.18

Sig. (2-tailed) .78 .43

N 22 22

Difference

Pearson Correlation .46*

.13

Sig. (2-tailed) .04 .59

N 21 21

Similarly, in intervention group, the imagery ability in soccer did not

have a significant influence on the effectiveness of the mental training. A

correlation analysis was applied to the test subjects. No significant correlation

of visual (P= .64) and kinesthetic (P= .35) imagery ability with the difference of

the PPT pre- and post- test results could be shown (Table 11). But in the

control group, there was significant correlation of visual imagery ability with

the difference of the PPT pre- and post- test (P= .04) (Table 12). However,

there is no significant correlation which could be shown for kinesthetic

imagery with the difference of the PPT pre- and post- test (P= .59).

5.3 The Commitment

The commitment of the all subjects was high on average for both tennis

and soccer. The mean result of all subjects in terms of commitment was 11.30

for tennis (table 13) , and for soccer was 10.54 (Table 14 ).

Table 13. Comparison of Both Groups with Respect to Commitment in Tennis

Group N Mean SD

Intervention 31 8.7 3.5

Control 29 14 5.5

Page 113: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

113

The commitment of the intervention group was higher than the

commitment of the control group for both tennis and soccer subjects (Tables

13, 14).

Table 14. Comparison of Both Groups with Respect to Commitment in Soccer

Group N Mean SD

Intervention 33 9.30 5.77

Control 22 11.77 4.90

In the intervention group in tennis, the commitment did not have a

significant influence on both the results of the TPT pre-test and the

effectiveness of the mental training. A correlation analysis was applied to test

for the effect of the subjects' commitment. The correlation coefficient of the

commitment for the intervention group (Pre-test) was (P= .53) and also no

significant correlation of the commitment with the difference of the TPT pre-

and post- test results could be shown (P= .59) (Table 15).

Table 15. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Tennis

However, in the control group in tennis, there was significant correlation

between the commitment and the results of the TPT pre-test (P= .01) (Table

16). Furthermore, there is no significant correlation of the commitment with

the difference of the TPT pre- and post- test results that could be shown (P=

.42) (Table 16).

Intervention group Commitment

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.12

Sig. (2-tailed) .53

N 31

Difference

Pearson Correlation .11

Sig. (2-tailed) .59

N 28

Page 114: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

114

Table 16. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control group in Tennis

Control group Commitment

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.49**

Sig. (2-tailed) .01

N 29

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.15

Sig. (2-tailed) .42

N 29

In the intervention group in soccer, the commitment did not have a

significant influence on either the results of the PPT pre-test or the

effectiveness of the mental training. A correlation analysis was applied to test

the effects of the subjects' commitment. The correlation coefficient of the

commitment for the intervention group (Pre-test) was (P= .35) and also no

significant correlation of commitment with the difference of the PPT pre- and

post- test results could be shown (P= .98) (Table 17).

Table 17. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Soccer

Intervention group Commitment

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.17

Sig. (2-tailed) .35

N 33

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.01

Sig. (2-tailed) .98

N 33

Similarly, in the control group in soccer, The commitment did not have

a significant influence on either the results of the PPT pre-test or the

effectiveness of the mental training. The correlation coefficient of the

commitment for the control group (Pre-test) was (P= .33) and also no

significant correlation of the commitment with the difference of the PPT pre-

and post- test results could be shown (P= .78) (Table 18).

Page 115: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

115

Table 18. Correlation between Commitment, Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Soccer

Control group Commitment

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.22

Sig. (2-tailed) .33

N 22

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.07

Sig. (2-tailed) .78

N 21

5.4 Competition Fear Inventory Assessment (Wettkampf Angst Inventar-

State: WAI-S)

In tennis, the analysis of the items of somatic fear and anxiety

(unease) of the intervention group does not show significant correlation with

the difference in precision between TPT pre- to post-test. However, the values

of confidence correlate significantly (p≤ .05) with the difference in precision

pre-and post-tests (Table 19).

Table 19. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Tennis

Intervention group Somatic

fear Anxiety Confidence

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .02 -.13 .02

Sig. (2-tailed) .94 .50 .91

N 31 31 31

Difference

Pearson Correlation .02 .25 -.38*

Sig. (2-tailed) .90 .20 .05

N 28 28 28

Whereas, in the control group in tennis, the analysis of the items of the

somatic fear, anxiety (unease) and confidence does not show significant

correlation with the increase of precision between TPT pre- to post-test (Table

20).

Page 116: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

116

Table 20. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Tennis

Control group Somatic

fear Anxiety Confidence

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.12 -.21 -.05

Sig. (2-tailed) .55 .28 .78

N 29 29 29

Difference

Pearson Correlation .17 .35 -.20

Sig. (2-tailed) .39 .06 .29

N 29 29 29

In soccer, the analysis of the items of the somatic fear, anxiety

(unease) and confidence of the intervention group does not show significant

correlation with the increase in precision between PPT pre- to post-tests.

However, the confidence values correlate significantly (p= .03) with the results

of the PPT pre-test (Table 21).

Table 21. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Soccer

Intervention group Somatic

fear Anxiety Confidence

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.05 .14 .38*

Sig. (2-tailed) .78 .44 .03

N 33 33 33

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.06 -.13 -.04

Sig. (2-tailed) .72 .49 .84

N 33 33 33

Whereas, in the control group in soccer, the analysis of the items of the

somatic fear, anxiety (unease) and confidence does not show significant

correlation with the increase in precision between PPT pre- to post-tests

(Table 22).

Page 117: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

117

Table 22. Correlations between the Values from WAI-S with the Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Soccer

Control group Somatic

fear Anxiety Confidence

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.14 -.15 .19

Sig. (2-tailed) .55 .52 .41

N 22 22 22

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.09 .30 -.34

Sig. (2-tailed) .71 .19 .13

N 21 21 21

5.5 The Concentration Test

Tables 23, 24, 25 and 26 show that the ability to concentrate, which is

intended to be measured by the FAIR with the values L (referring to the

speed), Q (referring to the absence of mistakes) and K (extent of continuously

sustained concentration), is positive, but not significantly correlated with both

TPT and PPT.

In tennis, the analysis of the items L (referring to the speed), Q

(referring to the absence of mistakes) and K (extent of continuously sustained

concentration) of the intervention group and control group does not show

significant correlation with the increase in precision between TPT pre- to post-

tests (Tables 23, 24).

Table 23. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value with the Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Tennis

Intervention group L Q K

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .30 .09 .28

Sig. (2-tailed) .10 .65 .13

N 31 31 31

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.03 .21 .02

Sig. (2-tailed) .86 .29 .91

N 28 28 28

Page 118: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

118

Table 24. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value With the Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Tennis

Control group L Q K

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .18 .27 .20

Sig. (2-tailed) .35 .15 .31

N 29 29 29

Difference

Pearson Correlation -.03 .05 .01

Sig. (2-tailed) .90 .80 .97

N 29 29 29

Similarly, in soccer, the analysis of the items L (referring to the speed),

Q (referring to the absence of mistakes) and K (extent of continuously

sustained concentration) of the intervention group and control group does not

show significant correlation with the increase in precision between PPT pre- to

post-tests (Tables 25, 26).

Table 25. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value with the Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Intervention Group in Soccer

Intervention group L Q K

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .14 .14 .13

Sig. (2-tailed) .47 .48 .49

N 29 29 29

Difference

Pearson Correlation .21 -.03 .22

Sig. (2-tailed) .29 .89 .25

N 29 29 29

Page 119: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

119

Table 26. Correlation of the L, Q and K Value with the Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for the Control Group in Soccer

Control group L Q K

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .10 .10 .11

Sig. (2-tailed) .67 .65 .64

N 22 22 22

Difference

Pearson Correlation .16 .02 .14

Sig. (2-tailed) .50 .94 .54

N 21 21 21

5.6 Demographical and Personal Data

5.6.1The sex

In tennis, the study population (N=60) consisted of 22 women and 38

males between the ages of 12 and 35 (mean value M=19,7 and SD=4,7). The

subjects play in the district league, regional league and national league.

Table 27. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts in tennis

Effects Type III Sum

of Squares Df

Mean

Square F Sig.

Partial Eta

Squared

Time 180.664 1 180.664 73.732 .000 .582

Time * Group 67.297 1 67.297 27.465 .000 .341

Time * sex 9.029 1 9.029 3.685 .060 .065

Time * Group * sex 6.906 1 6.906 2.818 .099 .050

Error(time) 129.866 53 2.450

The results of ANOVA (Table 27) shown that the effect of time is

significant and explains 58% of the within-subjects variance in the precision

test on the tennis serve.

The interaction effect between time and group is also significant, which

explains 34% of the within-subjects variance of the precision test in the tennis

serve. This means that both groups developed differently over time.

Page 120: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

120

The improvement in the precision of the serve in the intervention group

was significantly higher than the improvement in the control group. This

indicates that the intervention consisting of mental training significantly

improved the precision of the serve.

The interaction effect between time and sex is not significant, but

shows a tendency (10% above significance level), that the improvement in the

women’s precision was higher than that of the men. The interaction effect

between time and sex explains 6.5% of the within-subject variance on the

precision test in the tennis serve. The interaction effects among time, group,

and sex is not significant (P=.099), however, it also shows the tendency that

women improved more through the mental training than men. This threefold

interaction effect among time, group, and sex explains 5.0% of the within-

subjects variance on the precision test in the tennis serve.

Table 28. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects in tennis

Source Type III Sum

of Squares Df

Mean

Square F Sig.

Partial Eta

Squared

Intercept 4450.820 1 4450.820 343.728 .000 .866

Group 476.854 1 476.854 36.826 .000 .410

Sex .823 1 .823 .064 .802 .001

Group * Sex 42.771 1 42.771 3.303 .075 .059

Looking at the Between-Subjects Variance (Table 28), only the group

had a significant effect on precision. The intervention group showed higher

precision rates than the control group. The effect of the intervention explains

41% of the between-subjects variance, however, sex in the group results,

does not have a significant effect on precision.

Page 121: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

121

The interaction between group and sex is not significant but shows a

tendency that women in the intervention group improved more than the men

in the intervention group. These results suggest that sex does not have a

direct effect, but rather, moderates the effects of the intervention consisting of

mental training, as shown in (Tables 27, 28). The variance between the

difference in pre- and post-test results in tennis serve precision showed a

greater level of improvement among the women in the study, when compared

to the improvement among the men.

Figure 14. Improvement in tennis serve precision dependant on sex

Figure 15. The effect of sex on mental training and precision test.

Sex

Mental training Precision test

Page 122: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

122

Whereas, in soccer, all subjects are male and have experience in

playing in the over-regional league (Association of Junior Leagues or higher).

Therefore, we do not know the effects of sex on the effectiveness of the

intervention in soccer.

5.6.2 The age

In tennis, the subjects in the control group (CG) are on average

younger (Age. CG=18.7) than the subjects in the intervention group (Age.

IG=20.5). Therefore, the experience with mental training2 for the intervention

group was greater than the control group (IG= 5.7) (CG= 7.30). Tables (28,

29). When having a look at the training time per week as well as the average

training frequency in the last three months, the CG has a higher arithmetic

mean. The mean value of the variable body height (Size in cm) is 2nearly

identical in both groups. Tables (29, 30).

Table 29. Data of the intervention group in tennis (N=31)

Age in years

Size in cm Training

Frequency (in days)

Average training frequency in the

last three months (in days)

Experiences with mental

training

Mean 20.5 174.9 1.9 2.1 5.7

SD 5.7 6.1 1.2 1 2.4

Minimum 12 127 1 1 1

Maximum 35 193 6 5 8

Table 30. Data of the control group in tennis (N=29)

Age in years

Size in cm Training

Frequency (in days)

Average training frequency in the

last three months (in

days)

Experiences with Mental

Training

Mean 18.7 176 2.2 2.4 7.3

SD 3.2 11.9 1.7 1.7 2

Minimum 16 127 1 1 1

Maximum 34 193 7 7 8

2 “ As measured on an inverse scale, where more experience translates to a lower score.”

Page 123: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

123

Furthermore, the data was analysed with SPSS Windows, version

17.0.msi which revealed no significant correlation of the TPT pre-test and

post-test results with socio-demographic variables, training frequency and

experience with mental training. (Table 31) showed a non-significant

correlation between age and TPT pre-test results (level of significance p=.67)

as well as between age and the difference of the TPT pre- and post-test

(p=.27).

Table 31. Correlation between Age with the Number of Hits in the TPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for all Subjects in Tennis

All tennis subjects Age in years

Result TPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation .06

Sig. (2-tailed) .67

N 60

Difference

Pearson Correlation .15

Sig. (2-tailed) .27

N 57

Whereas, in soccer, the subjects in the control group (CG) are on

average older (Age. CG=17.60) than the subjects in the intervention group

(Age. IG=16.34). However, the experience with mental training for the

intervention group was a little bit bigger than the control group (IG= 6.25)

(CG= 6.91). Tables (32, 33). When having a look at the training time per

week, as well as the average training frequency in the last three months, the

IG has a higher arithmetic mean. The mean value of the variable, body height

(Size in cm) is nearly identical in both groups. Tables (31, 32).

Page 124: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

124

Table 32. Data of the intervention group in soccer (N=33)

Age in years

Size in cm Training

frequency (in days)

Average training frequency in the

last three months (in days)

Experiences with mental

training

Mean 16.34 178.69 3.06 4.00 6.25

SD 1.31 7.17 1.41 1.44 2.36

Minimum 14 164 1 2 1

Maximum 20 192 7 9 8

Table 33. Data of the control group in soccer (N=22)

Age in years

Size in cm Training

frequency (in days)

Average training frequency in the

last three months (in days)

Experiences with mental

training

Mean 17.60 178.90 2.61 1.93 6.91

SD 2.30 6.84 0.78 1.08 1.59

Minimum 14 170 1 0.5 3

Maximum 22 193 4 4 8

N 20 20 23 23 23

Furthermore, there is no significant correlation of the PPT pre-test and

post-test results with socio-demographic variables, training frequency and

experience with mental training. (Table 34) showed there is no significant

correlation between age and PPT pre-test results (p= .31).

However, there is a highly significant correlation between age and the

difference of the PPT pre- and post-tests.

Table 34. Correlation between Age with the Number of Kicks in the PPT Pre-Test, and Difference of Pre- and Post-Test for all Subjects in Soccer

All soccer subjects Age in years

Result PPT - pre-test

Pearson Correlation -.14

Sig. (2-tailed) .31

N 52

Difference

Pearson Correlation .43**

Sig. (2-tailed) .00

N 51

**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Page 125: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

125

6. Discussion

This chapter contains the discussion of the results with respect to the

hypotheses. Also possible exogenous variables and an investigation of the

study’s results are discussed. Furthermore, the design of the study is

analyzed and possible ideas for improvement are proposed. As a last step the

practical relevance of mental imagery for closed motor skills is illustrated.

The first purpose of the present study was to investigate the influence

of mental training, specifically (self-instruction and positive imagery) technique

on the improvement of the precision of the first serve in tennis and penalty

kick in soccer (closed motor skills).

Since closed skills should be more affected by imagery, because they

are completely under the athlete’s control. Therefore, the athlete can use

imagery to visualize them precisely (Coelho et al., 2007).

As predicted, in tennis, The results of ANOVA with repeated

measurements show a significant interaction effect between time and group in

TPT (F1, 55 = 22.5; ETA2 = .29). The mean for the hit ratio of the intervention

group increased from pre-test (Mpre= 6.3; SDpre= 3.0 ) to post-test (Mpost =

10.4; SDpost= 2.9) while the mean of the control group (Mpre= 4.0; SDpre= 2.6;

Mpost= 5.0; SDpost= 2.8) did not change significantly.

This improvement of precision, in the intervention group in comparison

to the control group, demonstrates the effectiveness of positive imagery on

closed motor skills in tennis. The effect of the intervention can be seen as

large (Cohens d= 1.00). The intervention group improved their performance in

the precision task by 4 hits in 20 trials, whereas, the control group improved

Page 126: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

126

only by one hit. This result means an increasing the percentage of the first

serve from 32% to 52%. This leads to an increase the percentage of

successful first serves. Since a hard serve is difficult to return which

decreases the percentage of the double faults. Furthermore, Eta2

time*groups=.29, which explained that, 29 % of the improvement of the precision

is due to the intervention.

Such a big explained variance (29 %) could be explained by

(Eberspächer, 2001; Immenroth et al., 2008; Morris et al., 2005; Mayer et al.,

2009; Weinberg & Gould, 2007). In this intervention the workbook was

connected with the imagination of different sensory modalities. Also the

intervention group used video analysis3 the postulate indicates an enhanced

efficacy of mental training when used with video analysis.

Specifically, it has been established before, that using positive imagery

and self-instruction just before serving would lead to greater serving accuracy

(Malouff et al., 2008).

The control group enhanced their precision in the tennis precision test

(TPT) post-test as well, via only practical training. Furthermore, as previously

mentioned the intervention group improved more on average from the first

(pre-) to the second measurement (post-test) than the control group. Previous

studies support the hypothesis that the visualization of a positive result before

the actual execution of a serve has a positive effect on the precision of the

serve in tennis ( Jekauc & Woll, 2006; Malouff et al., 2008).

3 One of the techniques used in sport psychology to enhance imagery rehearsal and athletic

performance is video modeling (Morris et al., 2005).

Page 127: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

127

As well as, Velentzas et al. (2011) supported the same result. That the

imagery group served significantly more accurately with significantly

increased ball velocity for volleyball serves. While, Coelho et al. (2007)

studied the effect of imagery in the tennis service stroke (closed skill) and

service receiving stroke (open skill) The results suggested that imaging a

positive outcome may be more powerful in improving performance of closed

skill movements than of open skill movements in tennis.

The explained variance of the present study in terms of the precision

test can be explained by the performance of the mental training (self-

instruction and positive visualization). Possible reasons that the mean was

lower and did not increase for the control group could be due to a lack of

access to the mental training in the field, the mental training workbook and the

lack of video analysis.

However, in soccer, The intervention group improve their average

kicking numbers from the pre-test to the post-test (IGpre= 8.8, IGpost= 9.6)

However, the control group did not improve their average kicking numbers

from the pre-test to the post-test, but showed a decline (CGpre= 9.0, CGpost=

8.24). The interaction of time and groups, as measured by the penalty

precision test (PPT) in the pre- and post-test could not be proven significantly

(p=.059) but it was marginally significant and it showed a tendency (.09 above

significance level). Therefore, a strong improvement of the mean of the

intervention group concerning the PPT results of the pre- and post-test, along

with the simultaneous deterioration of the control group results, between PPT

pre- and post-test, indicates a positive effect of the mental training

Page 128: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

128

intervention, which was combined with practical training, in contrast to just

practical penalty kick training.

It is noteworthy, that positive imagery can thus work as a tool to

activate, optimize, and even reorganize mental self-representations,

especially if these representations incorporate cognitive as well as behavioral

aspects, like self-talk or breathing control (Thelwell et al., 2006; Velentzas et

al., 2011).

The results of the present study partially corroborate those found in

previous studies (Beilcock et al., 2001; Cerin et al., 2000), which claim that

not all performance situations could be improved by imagery, only those that

do not require precision.

As well, Coelho et al. (2008) also proposed a study to determine

whether an imagery training intervention program could affect the

performance of junior Brazilian tennis players in two distinct situations: (1)

serving without precision and (2) serving at a target with precision. The results

demonstrated an effect (p< 0.05) between experimental and control groups in

the situation “service without precision”. However, the difference in the

situation of “service with precision” was not significant (p>0.5). In tennis and

soccer the use of video analysis in just the intervention group may have

directly or indirectly influenced the results.

The results confirm the fact that, self-instruction and positive

visualization are more appropriately integrated into training routines and

game strategy for athletes who already possess basic skills in their respective

Page 129: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

129

sport (Velentzas et al., 2011), such as the tennis and soccer players in the

current study.

Specifically, the level of significance for TPT in tennis was higher than

PPT in soccer, that might be due to the difference of the closed motor skill

characteristics for both tennis and soccer. For example, in tennis, each point

must start with a serve, while in soccer, the match may not have a single

penalty kick. This means, the frequency of serving in tennis during the match

is much higher than penalty kicking in soccer.

The second hypotheses (The age) might be also the reason why the TPT was

higher than PPT in terms of the level of significance, the average mean age

for tennis subjects was 20 years old, whereas, for soccer it was 17.

Furthermore, the level of experience with mental training for tennis subjects

(M= 5.7) was also higher than in soccer (M= 6.3). These findings have been

confirmed by Mulder et al. (2004); Guillot et al. (2005) that mental training

may be efficient only when athletes are trained in imagery for movements they

had already practiced. According to their results, the learning of a new

movement might not be possible through mental training, because no

representation can be made in the absence of any earlier physical execution.

Hall et al. (1990) also reported greater imagery use among higher-level

athletes than lower-level athletes. Perhaps, as Vealey and Greenleaf (1998)

suggested, this is because elite athletes have a clearer image of what an

accurate skill should look like. Other researchers suggested that the greater

use of imagery by elite performers reflects a greater dedication to their sport

(Hall, 2001 in Arvinen-Barrow et al., 2007). Moreover, Malouff et al. (2008)

Page 130: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

130

have suggested that in tennis, this difference may have resulted from older

players being in the habit of trying harder for accuracy, while younger players

might usually focus more on speed or spin. Furthermore, Researchers have

shown that the best age for high performance in tennis is between 22 and 28

(Shwarzer, 2007). Thus, to achieve a top 100 ranking in the professional list, it

takes more than ten years of training and five years of high performance

competition (Seibold, 2011, p. 82).

The third purpose of this study was to analyze the influence of the

subject’s imagery ability on both the results of the TPT, PPT pre-test and the

effectiveness of the mental training intervention in tennis and soccer by using

MIQ, this test is recommended by Mayer and Hermann (2009) for measuring

imagery ability. The MIQ includes movement exercises, each one has to be

executed physically and then mentally. Depending on how difficult it was to

generate the imagination a number (between 1 = “easy to imagine” and 8 =

“difficult to imagine”) was marked. Two questions refer to the visual and

kinesthetic imagery ability.

The numerical answers of the imagery ability exercises are summed

instead of being averaged. A low sum (2 at least) indicates a high imagery

ability and a high sum (16 at maximum) indicates a low imagery ability. In

tennis, (IG) showed a higher mean in the visual imagery ability (M= 4.4 vs. M=

4.2), (CG) however, showed a higher mean in the kinesthetic imagery ability

(M= 5.1 vs. M= 4.5).

However, in soccer, the intervention group showed a lower mean in the

visual imagery ability (M= 4.1 vs. M= 5.3), similarly, the control group, showed

Page 131: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

131

a lower mean in the kinesthetic imagery ability (M= 4.7 vs. M= 5.0). In tennis,

(IG) as well as (CG) showed a weak inverse correlation between both imagery

ability values and the results in the TPT pre-test. (IG): the correlation

coefficients of the visual and kinesthetic imagery are (P. visual= .21)

(P.kinesthetic= .45). (CG): (P. visual= .45) (P. kinesthetic= .26).

Similarly, in soccer, (IG) as well as (CG) showed also a weak inverse

correlation between both imagery ability values and the results in the PPT

pre-test. (IG): (P. visual= .14) (P.kinesthetic= .69). (CG): (P. visual= .78) (P.

kinesthetic= .43). Furthermore, in tennis, there is no significant correlation of

visual and kinesthetic imagery ability with the difference of the TPT pre- and

post-test results that could be shown for either group. (IG): (P. visual= .69) (P.

kinesthetic= .83). (CG): (P. visual= .14) (P. kinesthetic= .33).

Similarly, in soccer, there is also no significant correlation of visual and

kinesthetic imagery ability with the difference of the PPT pre- and post-test

results that could be shown in (IG): (P. visual= .64) (P. kinesthetic= .35).

However, in (CG) there is a significant correlation of visual imagery ability with

the difference of the PPT pre- and post- test (P= .04). However, there is no

significant correlation for kinesthetic imagery (P= .59).

The analysis showed that, except for the visual imagery ability of the

control group in soccer, imagery ability is not significantly correlated with; the

TPT and PPT pre-test; and the effectiveness of mental training intervention in

tennis and soccer. Surprisingly, a weak negative relationship between the

imagery ability in the questionnaire and the results of both TPT and PPT test

was found. The literature has been searched to find studies that would

Page 132: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

132

support this result, but no studies on imagery ability without relation to mental

training were to be found. Probably the imagery ability is useful only when in

connection to instructed mental training, whereas in the absence of active

mental training, it is unusable.

For example, it has been established, that kinesthetic modeling has

better outcomes for the tennis serve than visual modeling because of the use

of spatial anchor points which are mainly defined with respect to the body, rely

heavily on kinesthetic cues. In the study thirty-two participants were assigned

to kinesthetic or visual modeling with or without mental practice. The results

showed the advantages of kinesthetic representation over visual

representation in terms of speed scores and form performances but only

when the participant had the opportunity to practice the model mentally. The

results support the theory that the motor system can program closed skills

more easily when one can efficiently represent the kinesthetic image of the

skill in question (Fery & Morizot, 2000).

On the other hand, Jones et al. (1997) defined such sports as

kinaesthetic sports and state that these types of sports are characteristically

played by athletes that use kinaesthetic imagery. The existing research

suggests that kinaesthetic imagers have been found to use more imagery in

general than athletes who use more visually based imagery (Arvinen-Barrow

et al., 2007).

Furthermore, there has been a large amount of imagery research

conducted in both the fields of sport psychology and motor learning (Hall &

Fishburne, 2010). Morris et al. (2005, page 39) named a certain level of

Page 133: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

133

imagery ability as a precondition for the efficiency of the mental training. This

means, the more an athlete activates the links by imagery rehearsal the

stronger they become.

Another reason for the negative relationship could be the distraction of

some players. There were a lot of factors that could have deflected the

player’s imagery ability. Most of the players came directly from work or were

studying for exams. Mental imagination is a challenge and it can be that the

subjects did not want to or could not face this challenge at the end of a hard

working day. But these theories have neither been analyzed in the

questionnaire nor statistically studied. They are just speculations.

Furthermore, the practice for the study in soccer was outdoor during the

winter season (October and December) and especially in November and

December cold, rain and snow affected the study procedure and possibly the

results. It has been reported (Morris et al., 2005), that the imagery ability of a

movement or action is not the only dominant factor in the execution of the

mental training, commitment also plays an important role too.

Therefore, the fourth purpose of this study was to analyse the influence

of commitment or motivation on both the result of the TPT, PPT pre-test and

the effectiveness of the mental training intervention in tennis and soccer. The

subjects were asked to agree or disagree with the following statements which

were chosen to as proxies for their level of commitment to mental training ( I

want to pass the mental training as well as possible- It would be a pity if I

could not pass the mental training as well as possible- I feel myself to be

Page 134: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

134

connected to the mental training- I believe that the mental training will improve

my performance).

The identification of the subjects’ commitment was measured via an

ordinal scale from 1 (high commitment) to 8 (no commitment). A low sum (4 at

least) indicates that the subject identifies with the study and believes in the

efficacy of the mental training. A high sum (32 at maximum) corresponds to

the opposite.

The commitment of the intervention group was higher than the

commitment of the control group for both tennis and soccer subjects: Tennis

(M. IG= 8.7; M. CG= 14); Soccer (M. IG= 9.30; M. CG= 11.77).

In (IG) in tennis, The commitment did not have a significant influence

on both the results of the TPT pre-test (P= .53) and the efficiency of the

mental training (P= .59).

Similarly, (IG) in soccer, the commitment did not have a significant

influence on both the results of the PPT pre-test (P= .35) and the efficiency of

the mental training (P= .98).

It has been suggested that the use of motivational imagery may be

more prevalent immediately before a competition (Barrow et al., 2007). Since

the current study didn’t include competition, but only precision test by serve or

kick alternately in the target area, perhaps some of the subjects didn’t create

as high a level of motivation, because they didn’t feel there is a real

competition. Therefore, future researchers need to be aware of the potential

impact of the time differential between questionnaire distribution and

upcoming competitions.

Page 135: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

135

However, for the (CG) in tennis, there is a highly significant correlation

between the commitment and the results of the TPT pre-test. Since the

subjects have been selected randomly, it is possible that a few of them had

already positive experiences with using mental training. But there is no

significant correlation of the commitment with the difference of the TPT pre-

and post- test results could be shown (P= .42).

Whereas, the commitment of the (CG) in soccer did not have a

significant influence on both the results of the PPT pre-test (P= .33) and the

efficiency of the mental training (P= .78).

The fifth purpose of this study was to analyse the influence the

emotional state as measured by the Wettkampf-Angst-Inventar State (WAI-S)

on both the results of the TPT, PPT pre-test and the effectiveness of the

mental training intervention in tennis and soccer. (WAI-S) is short

questionnaire and measures the value of emotional state (somatic fear,

anxiety (unease) and confidence) on both the result of TPT, PPT pre-test and

the effectiveness of the mental training intervention in tennis and soccer.

In tennis, the analysis of the items of somatic fear and anxiety (unease)

of the (IG) does not show significant correlation with the increase in precision

between TPT pre- to post-test. However, the confidence values correlate

significantly (p≤.05) with the difference of the TPT pre- and post- test.

Whereas, in soccer, the analysis of the items of the somatic fear,

anxiety (unease) and confidence of the (IG) does not show significant

correlation with the increase in precision between PPT pre- to post-test.

Page 136: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

136

However, the values of confidence correlate significantly (p= .03) with the only

results of the PPT pre-test.

The interpretation for that has been supported by Coelho et al. (2007,

2008), that somatic fear and anxiety seem more evident in open skill than

close skill movements. Therefore, in the current study, there is no significant

correlation for both somatic fear and anxiety. However, self-confidence seems

to affect performance when the players have control over movement, as in

closed skills. Therefore, in the current study, self-confidence correlated

significantly.

Whereas, both the (CG) in tennis and soccer didn’t show any

significant correlation for any the items of somatic fear, anxiety (unease) and

confidence with the increase of precision between pre- to post-test. The

interpretation for this result is that there was no practice of mental training, but

only normal practical training. Or perhaps, that the questionnaire WAI-S did

not represent the real emotions of the subjects, which means, the real state of

the subjects was likely not as self-confident as they pretended to be.

The sixth purpose of this study was to analyse the influence of the

concentration ability on both the result of the TPT, PPT pre-test and the

effectiveness of the mental training intervention in tennis and soccer. The first

test in the questionnaire is the FAIR-Test (Frankfurter Aufmerksamkeits-

Inventar) measuring inter-individual differences in concentration and attention,

which includes the items L (referring to the speed), Q (referring to the

absence of mistakes) and K (the extent of continuously sustained

concentration) of the intervention group and control group with the increase of

Page 137: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

137

precision between pre- to post-test for both TPT & PPT. But it didn’t show any

significant correlation for all groups. The subjects in tennis play in the district

league, regional league and national league. Moreover, the subjects in soccer

have experience in playing in the over-regional league (Association of Junior

Leagues or higher). This means, the performance level for all the subjects is

high. The concentration test consists of 640 items (2 test sheets with 320

items, 3min time for each) that have to be judged individually. Two kinds of

items have to be marked (circle inside circle with three dots in its center;

square inside circle with two dots in its center). The distribution of the items is

stochastic. But organized line by line, include two target signs, which are to be

marked with a pen, line by line. Perhaps this sort of concentration tests

doesn’t fit practically but only theoretically to the kind of concentration honed

by high level athletes. Or perhaps the sample size in tennis and soccer is not

enough to score sufficient results in such this test.

In-depth interviews with elite tennis athletes have been done in our pilot

study in tennis This study was conducted at the Davidoff Swiss Indoors

Tournament in Basel in November 2009. The study takes a critical look at the

potential effects of mental training on the performance of tennis players.

Thirteen professional tennis players were interviewed to determine their

opinions on the importance of mental skills training, and the specific mental

skills they use before, during, and after a tennis match. Three professional

tennis coaches were also interviewed to determine the specific mental skills

they teach and the strategies they use to teach mental skills. Only one player

said that he didn’t use mental training; the rest of the sample said that mental

Page 138: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

138

training is a very individual process and that the most important mental

training skills were: concentration, positive imagery, self-confidence, and

relaxation. Furthermore, the respondents felt that tennis players should focus

on the process rather than the outcome.

It would be interesting in future studies to examine how precision

changes over a longer period of time after an intervention. Only when it

remained consistently high over time could one speak of the long-term

success of the mental training intervention. Just as with conditioning training

methods, for example, mental training must be regularly implemented with a

clear focus in order to achieve optimal results (Mayer & Hermann, 2010, p.

68).

Although, concentration is very important aspect for closed motor skills

in sport (Arvinen-Barrow et al., 2007; Bös & Schneider, 2003; Weinberg &

Gould, 2007), it is important to appreciate that little guidance is provided by

the literature in this area. Therefore, future research will be needed to study

how precisely these questionnaires measure concentration as in relation to

the skills necessary for elite tennis and soccer players.

The last purpose of this study was to test whether, sex has an influence

on the results in the difference between the precision pre- and post-test in

tennis. As a result, the interaction effect between time and sex is not

significant, but shows a tendency (10% above significance level).

Furthermore, the interaction between group and sex is not significant but

shows also a tendency that women in the intervention group improved more

than the men in the intervention group. These results suggest that sex does

Page 139: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

139

not have a direct effect, but rather, moderates the effects of the intervention

consisting of mental training. Perhaps a difference in the concentration and

motivation of the women results in much faster or more effective learning than

the men. In fact, there are no studies related to this area that have been

found.

Further research is therefore necessary to investigate the effects of sex

on mental training in close and open skills.

A critical issue of this study is related to two aspects, the first is some

intervention teams did not report the frequency of applying the workbook. This

has to be avoided otherwise the influence of the workbook on the efficiency of

the mental training cannot be investigated.

The second aspect deals with the improvement of the study with

regards to its contents. The athletes found the content of the workbook to be

helpful and structured. The only feedback referred to the practical relevance

and the distribution of another questionnaire after the intervention. So to

determine changes in the variables; imagery ability, commitment, somatic

fear, anxiety and concentration another questionnaire at the end of the

intervention should be used. The influence of the changes in the

aforementioned characteristics on the improvement in the TPT and PPT pre-

results and post-results could be analyzed.

Page 140: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

140

7. Conclusion

The results of this study point to some theoretical and practical

conclusions. From a theoretical point of view, the study showed a mental

training resulted in a highly significant improvement on tennis serving

precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on

precision was only marginally significant. This partly confirms the hypothesis.

These results seem to be very logical, in tennis each point must start with a

serve, but in soccer, the whole match may not have any penalty kicks at all.

Another theoretical result of the research is that, self-confidence seems

to have an impact on improving performance when the athlete has more

control over his movements, such as in closed skills. Additionally in tennis, the

difference in TPT pre- and post-test results showed the level of improvement

among the women in the study was larger than the improvement among the

men, although this was only marginally significant. Perhaps a difference in the

concentration and motivation of the women results in much faster or more

effective learning than the men. In fact, there are no studies related to this

area that have been found.

It would also be interesting to study whether there are gender differences in

motivation and the implementation of mental training, and if these produce

different effects on performance.

Furthermore, in both tennis and soccer, our findings may have resulted

from more advanced players being in the habit of trying harder for accuracy;

while younger players focus more on speed.

Page 141: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

141

From a practical standpoint, coaches and psychologists need to be

aware of the specific demands for each sport, and incorporate imagery

intervention during the preparatory and competitive periods of the season.

The coaches and psychologists need to be aware of all available models of

imagery intervention, their specific use, and increase mental skills training

resources (audio and video). This is so they can more effectively help athletes

improve their performance, monitor their pre-competitive anxiety and increase

their levels self-confidence.

Page 142: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

142

8. Zusammenfassung

Ziel dieser Studie war die Überprüfung einer Verbesserung der Präzision

von Freistößen im Fußball und Aufschlägen im Tennis als geschlossene

Lernaufgaben durch mentales Training. Die potentiellen Variablen und deren

Einfluss auf die Effizienz des mentalen Trainings werden in dieser Studie

aufgezeigt. Zu diesen Variablen gehört die Vorstellungsfähigkeit, das

Bekenntnis zur Studie, somatische Angst, Besorgnis und Zuversicht,

Konzentrationsvermögen, Geschlecht und Alter. Der experimentelle Aufbau

der Studie bestand aus einem Vor- und Nachtest und einer Kontrollgruppe.

Die Daten wurden anhand von 60 (38 Männer, 22 Frauen) Amateurathleten

auf einem hohen Tennislevel im Alter zwischen 12 und 35 Jahren in

Deutschland und 54 (Männern) Fußballathleten der dritte Liga im Alter

zwischen 14 und 22 Jahren in Deutschland ermittelt. Alle Teilnehmer waren in

Tennis und Fußballverbänden Baden-Württembergs in Deutschland

registriert. Die Tennis- und Fußballteilnehmer füllten jeweils einen

Fragebogen aus und wurden in zwei Gruppen eingeteilt: Die

Interventionsgruppe (IG) erhielt Technikübungen und mentales Training. Die

Kontrollgruppe (CG) erhielt nur technisches Training (normales Training) über

den gleichen Zeitraum. Analyse der Kovarianz zeigte eine hohe signifikante

Verbesserung im Falle des mentalen Training für die Präzision des

Aufschlages, während der Effekt beim Fußball-Strafstoß nur geringfügig

signifikant war (p=.059). Ergebnisse deuten darauf hin, dass

Selbstbewusstsein scheinbar eine Auswirkung auf die Leistungssteigerung

zeigt, wenn der Athlet mehr Kontrolle über seine Bewegung hat, wie im Falle

Page 143: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

143

der geschlossenen Lernaufgaben. Des Weiteren könnte dieser Effekt durch

erfahrenere Spieler herbeigeführt worden sein, da diese schon mit hohen

Geschwindigkeiten schlagen bzw. schießen und eher die Genauigkeit

trainieren, während jüngere Spieler mehr auf Geschwindigkeit fokussieren.

Zusätzlich ist eine nichtsignifikante Wechselwirkung zwischen Zeit und

Geschlecht erkennbar, eine Tendenz zeigt aber, dass die

Präzisionsverbesserung der Frauen höher war als die der Männer. Es gibt

keinen signifikanten Zusammenhang zwischen Vorstellungsvermögen,

Bekenntnis, somatischer Angst, Besorgnis und Konzentrationsvermögen.

Page 144: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

144

References

Allers, R., & Scheminsky, F. (1926). Über Aktionsstrome der Muskeln bei

motorischen und verwandten Vorgangen. Pflugers Archiv für die gestamte

Physiologie, 212, 169-182.

Anderson, A. (1997). Learning strategies in physical education: Self-talk,

imagery, and goal-setting. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation, and

Dance, 68 (1), 30-35.

Arvinen-Barrow, M., Weigand, D., Thomas, S., Hemmings, B., & Walley, M.

(2007). Elite and novice athletes’ imagery use in open and closed sports.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, 93-104.

Atienza, F., Balaguer, I., & Garcia-Merita, M. (1998). Video modeling and

imaging training on performance tennis service of 9-12- year old children.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87(2), 519-529.

Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral

change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.

Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart, &

Winston.

Bar-Eli, M., Azar, O.H., & Lurie., Y. (2009). (Ir)rationality in action: do soccer

players and goalkeepers fail to learn how to best perform during a penalty

kick?, In M. Raab, J. Johnson & H. Heekeren (Eds.), Mind and motion: the

bidirectional link between thought and action. Elsevier.

Barling, J., & Abel, M. (1983). Self-efficacy beliefs and tennis performance.

Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7(3), 265-272.

Page 145: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

145

Barr, K., & Hall, C. (1992). The use of imagery by rowers. International

Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 243-261.

Barton, K.M.S. (1996). The effect of mental imagery on sport climbing

performance of college students (Master's Thesis - Postgraduate Program in

Physical Education), Madison (Wi): University of Wisconsin.

Baumeister, R.F. (1984). Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and

paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 46(3), 610-20.

Behamonde, R., & Knudson, D. (2003). Linear and angular momentum in

stroke production. In B. Elliot, M. Reid & M. Crespo (Eds.), Biomechanics of

Advanced Tennis. ITF The International Tennis Federation.

Beilock, S.L., Afremow, J.A., Rabe, A.L., & Carr, T.H. (2001). “Don’t miss!”:

The debilitating effects of suppressive imagery on golf putting performance.

Journal of Sport Exercise Psychology, 23, 200-221.

Beilock, S.L., & Gray, R. (2007). Why do athletes “choke” under pressure? In

G. Tenenbaum & R.C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 425-

444). 3rd ed., Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Bird, E.I. (1984). EMG quantification of mental rehearsal. Perceptual and

Motor Skills, 59, 899-906.

BISP. (2011a). Itemanalyse und psychometrische Kennwerte. Zugriff am

08juli2011unterhttp://www.bisp.de/nn_18072/SpoPsy/DE/Diagnostikportal/An

gst/Sportlerfrageboegen/Bogen1/Itemanalyse__CSAI2.html.

Page 146: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

146

BISP. (2011b). Theorie und Werdegang des WAI-S. Retrieved 14/3/2011,

http://www.bisp.de/nn_18072/SpoPsy/DE/Diagnostikportal/Angst/Sportlerfrag

eboegen/Bogen1/Eckdaten.html.

Blair, A., Hall, C., & Leyshon, G. (1993). Imagery effects on the performance

of skilled and novice soccer players. Journal of Sport Sciences, 11(2), 95-

101.

Bloom, G.A., Durand-Bush, N., & Salmela, J.H. (1997). Pre- and

postcompetition routines of expert coaches of team sports. Sport

Psychologist, 11, 127-141.

Bohan, M., Pharmer, J.A., & Stokes, A.F. (1999). When does imagery practice

enhance on a motor task? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88, 651-658.

Bollettieri, N. (1995). Return of serve: My opinion. ITF Coaches Review, 6, 1-

2.

Bowtell, M., King, M., & Pain, M. (2009). Analysis of the keeper-dependent

strategy in the soccer penalty kick. International Journal of Sports Science

and Engineering, 3, 93-102.

Boyd, J., & Munroe, K.J. (2003). The use of imagery in climbing. The Online

Journal of Sport Psychology, 5(2), 15-30.

Braden, V., & Wool, R. (1993). Vic Braden’s mental tennis: how to psych

yourself to a winning game. 1st ed., Vic Braden and Robert Wool.

Brand, R., Ehrlenspiel, F., & Graf, K. (2009). Wettkampf-Angst-Inventar (WAI).

Bonn: Bundesinstitut für Sportwissenschaft.

Page 147: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

147

Brewer, B.W. (2009). Handbook of Sports Medicine and Science. Sport

Psychology, Wiley-BlackWell.

Brewer, B.W., & Shillinglaw, R. (1992). Evaluation of a psychological skills

training workshop for male intercollegiate lacrosse players. Sport

Psychologist, 6, 139-147.

Brouziyne, M., & Molinaro, C. (2005). Mental imagery combined with

physical practice of approach shots for golf beginners. Perceptual and Motor

Skills, 101(1), 203-11.

Bull, S.J. (1991). Personal and situational influences on adherence to mental

skills training. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 121-132.

Butler, R.J. (1996). Sport psychology in action. Oxford, England: Butterworth-

Heinemann.

Bös, K., & Schneider, W. (2003). Vom Tennisstalent zum Spitzenspieler: Eine

Reanalyse von Längsschnittdaten zur Leistungsprognose im Tennis. In A.

Woll (Hrsg.), Miteinander lernen, forschen, spielen. Zukunftsperspektiven für

Tennis (Schriften der Deutschen Vereinigung für Sportwissenschaft: Vol. 131,

S. 11-27). Hamburg: Czwalina.

Callow, N., & Hardy, L. (2001). Types of imagery associated with sport

confidence in netball players of varying skill levels. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 13, 1–17.

Callow, N., Hardy, L., & Hall, C. (2001). The effects of a motivational general-

mastery imagery intervention on the sport confidence of high-level badminton

players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 389-400.

Page 148: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

148

Callow, N., & Waters, A. (2005). The effect of kinesthetic imagery on the sport

confidence of flat-race horse jockeys. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6,

443-459.

Calmels, C., Berthoumieux, C., & d’Arripe-Longueville, F. (2004). Effects of an

imagery training program on selective attention of national softball players.

Sport Psychologist, 18, 272-296.

Calmels, C., d’Arripe-Longueville, F., Fournier, J.F., & Soulard, A. (2003).

Competitive strategies among elite female gymnasts : An exploration of the

relative influence of psychological skills training and natural learning

experiences. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 327-352.

Carlstedt, R.A. (1995). Mentales Tennis – Ihr Schlüssel zum Erfolg. München:

Sportinform.

Cerin, E., Szabo, A., Hunt, N., & Williams, C. (2000). Temporal patterning of

competitive emotions: critical review. Journal of Sport Sciences, 18, 605-626.

Chapman, C., Lane, A.M., Brierley, J.H., & Terry, P.C. (1997). Anxiety, self-

confidence and performance in tae kwon-do. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85,

1275–1278.

Chiappori, P.A., Levitt, S. & Groseclose, T. (2002). Testing Mixed-Strategy

Equilibria When Players Are Heterogeneous: The Case of Penalty Kicks in

Soccer. The American Economic Review, 92, 1138-1151.

Clark, R. (1991). Breakthrough tennis: a revolutionary approach to the game.

USA: Faragut Publishing Company.

Page 149: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

149

Coe, A.O. (2000). The balance between technology and tradition in tennis. In

S. Haake & A.O. Coe (Eds.), Tennis Science and Technology (pp. 3-40).

Oxford: Blackwell.

Coelho, R.W., Campos, W., Silva, S.G., Okazaki, F.H.A., & Keller, B. (2007).

Imagery intervention in open and closed tennis motor skill performance.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 105, 458-468.

Coelho, R.W., Oliveira, S., Elsangedy, H.M., Krinski, K., Colombo, H.,

Buzzachera, C.F., Campos, W., & Silva, S.G. (2008). The effect of imagery in

the tennis service precision and performance. Revista Brasileira de

Cineantropometria & Desempenho Humano, 10(2), 176-183.

Collins, R., Hodges, P.B., & Haven, B.H. (1985). TENNIS: A practical

learning guide. Tichenor Publishing.

Corbin C.B. (1972). Mental Practice. In W.P. Morgan (Ed.), Ergogenic aids

and muscular performance (pp. 94-118). New York: Academic Press.

Cumming, J.L., & Hall, C. (2002). Athlete’s use of imagery in the off-season.

The Sport Psychologist, 16, 160-172.

Cumming, J., Nordin, S.M., Horton, R. & Reynolds, S. (2006). Examining the

direction of imagery and self-talk on dart-throwing performance and self-

efficacy. The Sport Psychologist, 20, 257-274.

Daw, J., & Burton, D. (1994). Evaluation of a comprehensive psychological

skills training program for collegiate tennis players. The Sport Psychologist, 8,

37-57.

Page 150: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

150

De Francesco, C., & Burke, K.L. (1997). Performance enhancement

strategies used in a professional tennis tournament. International Journal of

Sport Psychology, 28, 185-195.

Denis, M. (1985). Visual imagery and the use of mental practice in the

development of motor skills. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science, 10,

45-165.

Donoghue, P.O., & Ballantyne, A. (2004). The impact of speed of service in

Grand Slam singles tennis. In A. Lees, J.F. Kahn & I.W. Maynard (Eds.),

Science and Racket Sports III. Routledge.

Draksal, M., & Nittinger, N. (2002). Mentales Tennis Training, Ein praktisches

Arbeitsbuch für Spieler und Trainer. Verlag Michael Draksal.

Driskell, J., Cooper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance

performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481-492.

DTB, (1995). Tennis – Lehrplan, Band 1, Technik & Taktik. München: BLV

Verlagsgesselschaft.

DTB, (1996). Tennis – Lehrplan, Band 2, Unterricht & Training. München: BLV

Verlagsgesselschaft.

Durand-Bush, N., & Salmela, J.H. (2002). The development and maintenance

of expert athletic performance: Perceptions of world and Olympic champions.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 154-171.

Eberspächer, H. (2004). Mentales Training – Ein Handbuch für Trainer und

Sportler. Copress Verlag in der Stiebner.

Page 151: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

151

Eberspächer, H. (2001). Mentales Training. Das Handbuch für Trainer und

Sportler. München: Copress.

Eberspächer, H. (1995). Mentales Training – Ein Handbuch für Trainer und

Sportler. München: Sportinform Verlag.

Eberspächer, H. (1990). Mentale Trainingsformen in der Praxis – Ein

Handbuch für Trainer und Sportler. Sportinform Verlag.

Efran, J., Lesser, G., & Spiller, M. (1994). Enhancing tennis coaching with

youths using a metaphor model. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 349-359.

Elliot, B.C. (1995). The biomechanics of tennis stroke production. In T.

Reilly, M. Hughes & A. Lees (Eds.), Science and Racket Sports (1st ed.). E &

FN Spon, University Press, Cambridge.

Elliott, B.C., Ackland, T.R., Blanksby, B.A., Hood, K.P., & Bloomfield, J.

(1989). Profiling junior tennis players part 1: morphological, physiological and

psychological normative data. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in

Sport, 21(3), 14-21.

Elliott, B., Ackland, T.R., Blanskby, B.A., & Bloomfield, J. (1990). A

prospective study of physiological and kinanthropometric indicators of junior

tennis performance. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 22,

87-92.

Elliot, B., & Alderson, J. (2003). Biomechanical performance models: the

basis for stroke analysis. In B. Elliot, M. Reid & M. Crespo (Eds.),

Biomechanics of advanced tennis. ITF The International Tennis Federation.

Page 152: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

152

Elliot, B., Mester, J., Kleinöder, H., & Yue, Z. (2003). Loading and stroke

production. In B. Elliot, M. Reid & M. Crespo (Eds.), Biomechanics of

advanced tennis. ITF The International Tennis Federation.

Elliot, B., Reid, M., & Crespo, M. (2003). Biomechanics of advanced tennis.

ITF The International Tennis Federation.

Emery, G., & Hopper, T. (2001). Biomechanical analysis of the tennis serve.

Source: http://education2.uvic.ca/Faculty/thopper/PE117/Pe117/

Assignments/Greg%20Serve%20Analysis.pdf.

Epstein, M.L. (1980). The relationship of mental imagery and mental rehearsal

to performance of a motor task. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 211-220.

Evans, L., Jones, L., & Mullen, R. (2004). An imagery intervention during

the competitive season with an elite rugby union player. Sport Psychologist,

18, 252-271.

Farrow, D., & Kemp, J. (2003). Run like you stole something (the science

behind the score line). Sydney, NSW, Australia: Allen & Unwin.

Fauzee, M., Daud, W., Abdullah, R., & Rashid, S. (2009). The effectiveness of

imagery and coping strategies in sport performance. European Journal of

Social Sciences, 9(1), 97-108.

Feltz, D., & Landers, D. (1983). The effect of mental practice on motor-skill

learning and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5,

25-57.

Feltz, D.L., Landers, D.M., & Becker, B.J. (1988). A revised meta-analysis of

the mental practice literature on motor skill learning. In D. Druckman & J.

Page 153: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

153

Swets (Eds.), Enhancing human performance: Issues, theories and

techniques (pp. 1-65). Washington D.C.: National Academy Press.

Feltz, D.C., & Riessinger, C.A. (1990). Effects of in vivo imagery and

performance feedback on self-efficacy and muscular endurance. Journal of

Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 132-143.

Fery, Y., & Morizot, P. (2000). Kinesthetic and visual image in modeling

closed motor skills: the example of the tennis serve. Perceptual and Motor

Skills, 90, 707-722.

Field, A. (2009). Discovering Statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed., London: Sage.

Fletcher, D., & Hanton, S. (2001). The relationship between psychological

skills usage and competitive anxiety responses. Psychology of Sport and

Exercise, 2, 89-101.

Franks, I.M. & Hanvey, T. (1997). Cues for goalkeepers. Soccer Journal, May-

June, 30-38.

Franks, I.M., McGarry, T., & Hanvey, T. (1999). From notation to training:

analysis of the penalty kick. Insight, 3, 24-6.

Frey, M., Laguna, P.L., & Ravizza, K. (2003). Collegiate athletes’ mental skill

use and perceptions of success: An exploration of the practice and

competition settings. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 115-128.

Gabler, H., & Maier, P. (1998). Das Training der mentalen Fähigkeiten im

Tennis – Übungen zur Praxis des psychologisch orientierten Trainings.

Sportverlag Sindelfingen.

Page 154: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

154

Gallucci, N.T. (2008). Sport Psychology. Performance Enhancement,

Performance Inhibition, Individuals, and Teams (pp. 83-110). Psychology

Press.

Gallwey, T.W. (1977). Tennis und Psyche – Das Innere Spiel. München:

WILA.

Gammage, K.L., Hall, C.R., & Rodgers, W.M. (2000). More about exercise

imagery. Sport Psychology, 14, 348-359.

Garza, D.L., & Feltz, D.L. (1998). Effects of selected mental practice on

performance, self-efficacy, and competition confidence in figure skaters. Sport

Psychologist, 12, 1-15.

Gill, D.L., & Williams, L. (2008). Psychological Dynamics of Sport and

Exercise. 3rd ed., Human Kinetics.

Goudas, M., & Giannoudis, G. (2008). A team-sports-based life-skills program

in a physical education context. Learning and Instruction, 18(6), 528-536.

Gould, D., Damarjian, N., & Greenleaf, C. (2002). Imagery training for peak

performance. In J.L. Van Raalte & B.W. Brewer (Eds.), Exploring sport and

exercise psychology (pp. 49-74). 2nd ed., Washington, DC: American

Psychological Association.

Gould, D., Eklund, R.C., & Jackson, S.A. (1993). Coping strategies used by

U.S. Olympic wrestlers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 64, 83-93.

Gould, D., Finch, L.M., & Jackson, S.A. (1993). Coping strategies used by

national champion figure skaters. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,

64, 453-468.

Page 155: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

155

Gould, D., Medbery, R., Damarjian, N., & Lauer, L. (1999). A survey of mental

skills training knowledge, opinions and practices of junior tennis coaches.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 11, 28-50.

Gould, D., Petlichkoff, L., Hodge, K., & Simons, J. (1990). Evaluating the

effectiveness of a psychological skills educational workshop. The Sport

Psychologist, 4, 249-260.

Gould, D., Tammen, V., Murphy, S.M., & May, J. (1989). An examination of

the U.S. Olympic sport psychology consultants and the services they provide.

Sport Psychologist, 3, 300-312.

Greenspan, M.J., & Feltz, D.L. (1989). Psychological interventions with

athletes in competitive situations: A review. Sport Psychologist, 3, 219-236.

Grosser, M., Kraft, H., & Schönborn, R. (2007). Speed training for tennis. 2nd

ed., Oxford UK: Meyer & Meyer Sport.

Grosser, M., & Schönborn, R. (2008). Training im Kinder- und Jugendtennis.

Aachen: Meyer & Meyer Verlag.

Grosser, M., & Schönborn, R. (2002). Competitive tennis for young players.

Meyer & Meyer Sport.

Gruber, M., & Gollhofer, A. (2004). Impact of sensorimotor training on the rate

of force development and neural activation. European Journal of Applied

Physiology, 92, 98-105.

Guillot, A., Nadrowska, E., & Collet, C. (2009). Using motor imagery to learn

tactical movements in basketball. Journal of Sport Behavior, 32(2), 189-206.

Page 156: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

156

Gregg, M., Hall, C., & Hanton, S. (2007). Perceived Effectiveness of

Heptathletes’ Mental Imagery. Journal of Sport Behavior, 30(4), 398-414.

Grove, J.R., Norton, P.J., Van Raalte, J.L., & Brewer, B.W. (1999). Stages of

change as an outcome measure in the evaluation of mental skills training

programs. Sport Psychologist, 13, 107-116.

Haag, H. (2004). Research Methodology for Sport and Exercise Science.

Verlag Karl Hofmann.

Hakkinen, K., Komi, P.V., & Alen, M. (1985). Effect of explosive type strength

training on isometric force- and relaxation-time, electromyographic and

muscle fibre characteristics of leg extensor muscles. Acta Physiol Scand, 125,

587-600.

Hale, B.D. (1982). The effects of internal and external imagery on muscular

and ocular concomitants. Journal of Sport Psychology, 4, 379-387.

Hale, B.D., & Whitehouse, A. (1998). The effects of imagery-manipulated

appraisal on intensity and direction of competitive anxiety. Sport Psychologist,

12, 40-51.

Hall, C. (2001). Imagery in sport and exercise. In R. Singer, H. Hausenblas &

C. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of research in sport psychology (pp. 529-549).

2nd ed., New York: Wiley.

Hall, C. (2001). Why athletes and exercisers use imagery. Symposium

presented at the annual conference for the Association of the Advancement of

Applied Sport Psychology. Oct. 3-7, 2001, Orlando, FL.

Page 157: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

157

Hall, C. (1998). Measuring imagery abilities and imagery use. In J.L. Duda

(Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 165-

172). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Hall, C. (1995). The motivational function of mental imagery for participation in

sport and exercise. In J. Annett, B. Cripps & H. Steinberg (Eds.), Exercise

addiction: Motivation for participation in sport and exercise (pp. 15-21).

Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society.

Hall, C. (1985). Individual differences in the mental practice and imagery of

motor skill performance. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 10(4),

17-21.

Hall, C., Bucholz, & Fishburne, G. (1992). Imagery and the acquisition of

motor skills. Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences, 17(1), 19-27.

Hall, C., Mack, D., Paivio, A., & Hausenblas, H. (1998). Imagery use by

athletes: Development of the Sport Imagery Questionnaire. Imagery

Journal of Sport Psychology, 29, 73-89.

Hall, C., & Pongrac, J. (1983). Movement Imagery Questionnaire. London,

ON: University of Western Ontario.

Hall, C., Rodgers, W. (1989). Enhancing coaching effectiveness in figure

skating through a mental skills training program. The Sport Psychologist, 3,

142-154.

Hall, C., Rodgers, W., & Barr, K. (1990). The use of imagery by athletes in

selected sports. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 1-10.

Hall., C., Toews, J., & Rodgers, W. (1990). Les aspects motivationnels de

l’imagerie en activites motrices (The motivational function of imagery). Revue

Page 158: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

158

des Sciences et techniques des Activites physiques et Sportives, 22(5), 27-

32.

Hall, N., & Fishburne, G. (2010). Mental imagery research in physical

education. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity, 5(1),

1-17.

Hanrahan, S.J., & Andersen, M.B. (2010). Routledge Handbook of Applied

Sport Psychology (pp. 481-489). Routledge International.

Hardy, L. (1997). Three myths about applied consultancy work. Journal of

Applied Sport Psychology, 9, 107-118 & 277-294.

Hardy, L., & Callow, N. (1999). Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery

perspectives for the enhancement of performance on tasks in which form is

important. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 95-112.

Harris, D.V., & Robinson, W.J. (1986). The effects of skill level on EMG

activity during internal and external imagery. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8,

105-111.

Harwood, C., Cumming, J., & Fletcher, D. (2004). Motivational profiles and

psychological skills use within elite youth sport. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 16, 318-332.

Haslam, I. (1994). The use of imagery by soccer players. Journal of Applied

Sport Psychology, 6, 116-133.

Hausenblas, H.A., Hall, C.R., Rodgers, W.M., & Munroe, J.K. (1999). Exercise

imagery: Its nature and measurement. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,

11, 171-180.

Page 159: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

159

Hays, W.L. (1994). Statistics. 5th ed., Orlando: Harcourt Brace.

Hedelung, K.E., & Rasmusen, A. (1997). Serve and return tactics. ITF Sport

Science and Coaching Review, 13, 6-8.

Heishman, M.F., & Bunker, L. (1989). Use of mental preparation strategies by

international elite female lacrosse players from five countries. Sport

Psychologist, 3, 14-22.

Higger, Y. (1993). Physical training for tennis - an integrating approach. A

presentation from the 8th ITF Worlwide Coaches Workshop, Key Biscayne.

Higger, Y. (2002). Physical training for tennis – the need for better integration.

ITF Coaching and Sport Science Review, 26, 13-14.

Higgins, R., & Lees, A. (1995). A biomechanical analysis of the tennis serve,

examining the effect of altering the foot fault rule. In T. Reilly, M. Hughes & A

Lees (Eds.), Science and Racket Sports. 1st ed., E & FN Spon, University

Press, Cambridge.

Hinshaw, K. (1991). The effects of mental practice on motor skill performance:

Critical evaluation and meta-analysis. Imagination, Cognition and Personality,

11, 3-35.

Hollenbeck, J.R., Klein, H.J., O’Leary, A.M. & Wright, P.M. (1989).

Investigation of the Construct Validity of a Self-Report Measure of Goal

Commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology. 74 (6), 951-956.

Holmes, P., & Collins, D. (2001). The PETTLEP approach to motor imagery: A

functional equivalence model for sport psychologists. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 13, 60-83.

Page 160: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

160

Horn, T.S. (2008). Advances in Sport Psychology. 3rd ed., Miami University,

Oxford, Ohio, Human Kinetics.

Igel, C. (2001). Mentales Training: Zur Wirkung pro- und retrospektiver

Vorstelungsprozesse auf das Bewegunglernen. Sport und Buch Strauß.

Immenroth, M., Haasis, M., Mayer, J., & Eberspächer, H. (2003). Mentales

Training im Lebenslauf (I) – Kinder. In J. Munzert, S. Künzell, H. Maurer, M.

Reiser, N. Schott, & K. Zentgraft (Eds.), Psychomotorische Entwicklung –

Sport und Bewegung im Lebenslauf (p. 65). Giessen: 35. Jahrestagung der

Arbeitsgemeinschaft für Sportpsychologie.

Isaac, A.R. (1992). Mental practice: Does it work in the field? Sport

Psychologist, 6, 192-198.

Jackson, R.C., & Gudgeon, M. (2004). Anticipating serve direction: Implicit

sequence learning in tennis. In A. Lees, J.F. Kahn & I.W. Maynard (Eds.),

Science and Racket Sports III. Routledge.

Jacobson, E. (1930a). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states

during mental activities: Part 1. Imagination of movement involving skeletal

muscle. American Journal of Physiology, 91, 567-606.

- .(1930b). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 2. Imagination and recollection of various muscular acts.

American Journal of Physiology, 94, 27-34.

- .(1930c). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 3. Visual imagination and recollection. American Journal of

Physiology, 95, 694-702.

Page 161: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

161

- .(1930d). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 4. Evidence of contraction of specific muscles during

imagination. American Journal of Physiology, 95, 703-712.

- .(1931a). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 5. Variation of specific muscles contracting during imagination.

American Journal of Physiology, 96, 115-121.

- .(1931b). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 6. A note on mental activities concerning an amputated limb.

American Journal of Physiology, 96, 122-125.

- .(1931c). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental

activities: Part 7. Imagination, recollection, and abstract thinking involving the

speech musculature. American Journal of Physiology, 97, 20-209.

Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action. Blackwell:

Oxford.

Jekauc, D. (2006). Wie wird der Aufschlag präzise? TennisSport

Fachzeitschrift für Tennistraining in Theorie und Praxis, 17 (5), 20-22.

Jekauc, D., & Woll, A. (2006). Effektivität einer ergebnisfokussierten

Mentaltechnik. In M. Raab, A. Arnold, K. Gärtner, J. Köppen, C. Lempertz, N.

Tielemann & H. Castrow (Hrsg.), Zukunft der Sportspiele: fördern, fordern,

forschen (S. 108-110). Flensburg: Flensburg University Press.

Johnson, P. (1982). The functional equivalence of imagery and movement.

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 349-365.

Jones, G. (1995). More than just a game: research developments and issues

in competitive anxiety in sports. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 449-478.

Page 162: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

162

Jones, G. (1995). Psychological preparation in racket sports. In T. Reilly, M.

Hughes & A. Lees (Eds.), Science and Racket Sports. 1st ed., E & FN Spon,

University Press, Cambridge.

Jones, L., & Stuth, G. (1997). The use of mental imagery in athletics. An

overview. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 6(2), 101-115.

Jordet, G. (2009). When Superstars Flop: Public Status and Choking Under

Pressure in International Soccer Penalty Shootouts. Journal of Applied Sport

Psychology, 21(2), 125-130.

Jordet, G. (2005). Perceptual training in soccer: An imagery intervention study

with elite players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 140-156.

Jordet, G., & Hartman, E. (2008). Avoidance motivation and choking under

pressure in soccer penalty shootouts. Journal of Sport & Exercise

Psychology, 30, 452-459.

Jordet, G., Hartman, E., Visscher, C., & Lemmink, K.A. (2007). Kicks from the

penalty mark in soccer: the roles of stress, skill, and fatigue for kick outcomes.

Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(2), 121-9.

Jowdy, D.P., & Harris, D.V. (1990). Muscular responses during mental

imagery as a function of motor skill level. Journal of Sport and Exercise

Psychology, 12, 191-201.

Karageorghis, C.I., & Terry, P.C. (2010). Inside Sport Psychology. Human

Kinetics Publishers.

Kauss, D.R. (2001). Mastering your inner game. Champaign, US: Human

Kinetics.

Page 163: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

163

Kendall, G., Hrycaiko, D., Martin, G.L., & Kendall, T. (1990). The effect of an

imagery rehearsal, relaxation and self-talk package on basketball game

performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 157-166.

Khaled, T. (2004). The effects of mental imagery on the acquisition of motor

performance: A literature review with theoretical implications. Journal of

mental imagery, 28, 79-114.

Kiefer, R. (2011). Improvement of soccer penalty precision through mental

training. (Master Thesis) Faculty of Humanities, University of Konstanz.

Kleinoder, H. (2001). The return of serve. ITF Sport Science and Coaching

Review, 24, 5-6.

Knudson, D. (2006). Biomechanical principles of tennis technique. US:

Racquet Tech Publishing.

Kossert, A.L., & Chandler, K.M. (2007). Exercise imagery: a systematic review

of the empyrical literature. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical

Activity, 2(1), 1-32.

Kosslyn, S.M., Cacioppo, J.T., Davidson, R.J., Hugdahl, K., Lovallo, W.R.,

Spiegel, D., & Rose, R. (2002). Bridging psychology and biology: The analysis

of individuals in groups. The American Psychologist, 57(5), 341-351.

Krolak, A. (1990). Introduction to modern tennis (in Polish). AWF Warszawa.

Kuhn, W. (1988). Penalty-kick Strategies for Shooters and Goalkeepers. In T.

Reilly, K. Davids, W.J. Murphy & A. Lees (Eds.), Science and Football (pp.

489-492). London: E & FN Spon.

Page 164: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

164

Kühnen, P. (2006). game.set.match. Deutschland, Der Weg zum

Spitzenerfolg für “Normal-sterbliche”. New School Verlag.

Lane, J. (1980). Improving athletic performance through visuo-motor behavior

rehearsal. In R. Suinn (Ed.), Psychology in sports: Issues and applications

(pp. 316-320). Minneapolis, MN: Burgess.

Lang, P.J., Kozak, M.J., Miller G.A., Levin, N. & Mclean, A. (1980). Emotional

imagery: conceptual structure and pattern of somato-viceral response.

Psychophysiology, 17, 179-192.

Loehr, J. (1998). Die neue mentale Stärke, Sportliche Bestleistung durch

mentale, emotionale und physische Konditionierung. BLV Verlagsgesellschaft.

Loehr, J. (1989). The mental game: winning at pressure tennis . Plume/

Penguin, New York.

Mackenzie, M. (1993). Tennis – Das Psychospiel – Der Schlüssel zur

Topleistung. Rowohlt Taschenbuch Verlag.

Mahoney, M., & Avener, M. (1997). Psychology of the elite athlete: An

exploratory study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3, 361-366.

Malouff, J.M., McGee, J.A., Halford, H.T. & Rooke, S.E. (2008). Effects of pre-

competition positive imagery and self-instructions on accuracy of serving in

tennis. Journal of Sport Behavior, 3, 264-273.

Mamassis, G., & Doganis, G. (2004). The effects of mental training program

on juniors pre-competitive anxiety, self-confidence and tennis performance.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 118-137.

Page 165: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

165

Martens, R. (2004). Successful Coaching. 3rd ed., Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R.S., Bump, L.A., & Smith, D.E. (1990).

Development and validation of the competitive state anxiety inventory 2

(CSAI-2). In R. Martens, R.S. Vealey & D. Burton (Eds.), Competitive anxiety

in sport (pp. 117-173). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Martin, K.A., & Hall, C.R. (1995). Using mental imagery to enhance intrinsic

motivation. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 54-69.

Martin, K.A., Moritz, S.E., & Hall, C.R. (1999). Imagery use in sport: A

literature review and applied model. Sport Psychologist, 13, 245-268.

Mayer, J., & Hermann, H.D. (2009). Mentales training: Grundlagen und

Anwendung in Sport, Rehabilitation, Arbeit und Wirtschaft. Heidelberg:

Springer Medizin Verlag.

McCullagh, P., & Weiss, M.R. (2001). Modeling: Learning, developmental,

and social psychological considerations. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphy & L.K.

Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 106-125).

2nd ed., New York: MacMillan.

McGarry, T., & Franks, I.M. (2005). The science of match analysis. In T.

Reilly & A.M. Williams (Eds.), Science and soccer. 2nd ed., GB: TJ

International, Padstow, Cornwall.

McGarry, T., & Franks, I.M. (2000). On winning the penalty shoot-out in

soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 401-9.

McKenzie, A., & Howe, B.L. (1997). The effect of imagery on self-efficacy for

a motor skill. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 196-210.

Page 166: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

166

McLean, N., & Richardson, A. (1994). The role of imagery in perfecting

already learned physical skills. In A. Sheikh & E. Korn (Eds.), Imagery in

sports and physical performance (pp. 59-73). Amityville, NY: Baywood

Publishing.

McMorris, T., & Colenso, S. (1996). Anticipation of professional goalkeepers

when facing right- and left-footed penalty kicks. Perceptual and Motor Skills,

82, 931-934.

McMorris, T., Copeman, D., Corcoran, D., Saunders, G., & Potter S. (1993).

Anticipation of soccer goalkeepers facing penalty kicks. In T. Reilly, J. Clarys

& A. Stibbe (Eds.), Science and Football II (pp. 250-253). London: E & FN

Spon.

McMorris, T., Hauxwell, B., Holder, T. (1995). Anticipation of soccer

goalkeepers when facing penalty kicks to the right and left of the goal using

different kicking techniques. In W.K. Simpson, A.D. LeUnes & J.S. Picou

(Eds.), Applied research in coaching and athletics, annual (pp. 32-43).

Boston, MA: American Press.

Meyers, A.W., Whelan, J.P., & Murphy, S.M. (1996). Cognitive behavioural

strategies in athletic performance enhancement. In M. Mersen, R.M. Miller, &

A.S. Belack (Eds.), Progress in behaviour modification (pp. 137-164). Pacific

Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Minas, S.C. (1980). Mental practice of a complex perceptual motor-skill.

Journal of Human Movement Studies, 4, 102-107.

Moran, A. (2004). Sport and exercise psychology: A critical introduction.

Routledge: Hove, England.

Page 167: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

167

Moran, A. (1995). How effective are psychological techniques used to

enhance performance in tennis? The views of some international tennis

coaches. In T. Reilly, M. Hughes & A. Lees (Eds.), Science and Racket

Sports. 1st ed., E & FN Spon, University Press, Cambridge.

Moran, K., Murphy, C., Cahill, G., & Marshall, B. (2009). Can elite tennis

players judge their service speed? 27th International Conference on

Biomechanics in Sports.

Moosbrugger, H., & Goldhammer, F. (2010). FAIR, Frankfurter

Aufmerksamkeits-Inventar.Retrieved10/3/2011,from http://www.assessment

info.de/assessment/seiten/datenbank/

Vollanzeige/vollanzeige-de.asp?vid=416.

Moosbrugger, H., & Oehlschlägel, J. (1996). FAIR. Frankfurter

Aufmerksamkeitsinventar. Testmanual. Bern: Huber.

Moritz, S.E., Hall, C.R., Martin, K.A., & Vadocz, E. (1996). What are confident

athletes imaging? An examination of image content. Sports Psychology, 10,

171-179.

Morris, T., Spittle, M., & Watt, A.P. (2005). Imagery in Sport. Champaign, IL:

Human Kinetics.

Morris, A., & Burwitz, L. (1989). Anticipation and movement strategies in elite

soccer goalkeepers at penalty kicks. Journal of Sports Sciences, 7, 79-80.

Morrisett, L.N. (1956). The role of implicit practice in learning. PhD diss., Yale

University.

Page 168: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

168

Morya, E., Bigatao, H., Lees, A. and Ranvaud, R. (2005). Evolving penalty

kick strategies: World Cup and club matches 2000-2002. Science And

Football Congress, 5, 237-242.

Mulder, T., Zijlstra, S., Zijlstra, W., & Hochstenbach, J. (2004). The role of

motor imagery in learning a totally novel movement. Experimental Brain

Research, 154, 211-217.

Munroe, K., Hall, C., Simms, S., & Weinberg, R. (1998). The influence of type

of sport and time of season on athletes’ use of imagery. The Sport

Psychologist, 12, 440-449.

Munroe, K., Giacobbi, P., Hall, C., & Weinberg, R. (2000). The four W’s of

imagery use: where, when, why, and what. The Sport Psychologist, 14(2),

119-137.

Munroe-Chandler, K., Hall, C., Fishburne, G., Strachan, L. (2007). Where,

when, and why young athletes use imagery: an examination of developmental

differences. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(2), 103-116.

Murphy, S.M., & Jowdy, D.P. (1992). Imagery and mental practice. In T.S.

Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (pp. 221-250). Champaign, IL:

Human Kinetics.

Murphy, S.M., & Martin, K.A. (2002). The use of imagery in sport. In T.S. Horn

(Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (pp. 405-439). 2nd ed.,

Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Murphy, S., Nordin, S., & Cumming, J. (2008). Imagery in Sport, Exercise and

Dancing. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in Sport Psychology. 3rd ed., Human

Kinetics.

Page 169: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

169

Murray, J.F. (1995). The essence of imagery in tennis. Mental Equipment,

Source: http://www.tennisserver.com/mental-equipment/me_8_95.html.

Müller, C. (2011). Die Anwendung von Mentalem Training im Spitzentennis –

eine qualitative Studie. Wissenschaftliche Hausarbeit für das Lehramt an

Gymnasien, Universität Konstanz.

Müller, E. (1989). Sportmotorische Testverfahren zur Talentauswahl im

Tennis (Motor ability tests for talent identification in tennis). Leistungsport, 2,

8-13.

Neck, C.P., & Manz, C.C. (1992). Thought self-leadership: The influence of

self-talk and mental imagery on performance. Journal of Organizational

Behaviour, 13, 681-699.

Nideffer, R.M. (1981). The ethics and practice of applied psychology. Ithaca,

NY: Mouvement Publ.

Nittinger, N. (2009). Psychologisch orientiertes Tennistraining. Stuttgart:

Neuer Sportverlag.

Nittinger, N., & Petersen, C. (2006). Fit to play tennis – High performance

training tips. Racquet Tech Publishing.

Nittinger, N., & Petersen, C. (2003). Fit to play tennis – Practical tips to

optimize training and performance. Published by Fit to Play.

Noel, R. (1980). The effects of visuo-motor behaviour rehearsal on tennis

performance. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 221-226.

Orlick, T., & Partington, J. (1988). Mental links to excellence. The Sport

Psychologist, 2, 105-130.

Page 170: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

170

Page, S.J., Sime, W., & Nordell, K., 1999. The effects of imagery on female

college swimmers’ perceptions of anxiety. Sport Psychologist, 13, 458-469.

Paivio, A. (1985). Cognitive and motivational functions of imagery in human

performance. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 10, S22–S28.

Palacios-Huerta, I. (2003). Professionals Play Minimax. Review of Economic

Studies, 70, 395-415.

Papadelis, C., Kourtidou-Papadeli, C., Bamidis, P., & Albani, M. (2007).

Effects of imagery training on cognitive performance and use of psychological

measures as an assessment tool of mental effort. Brain and Cognition, 64(1),

74-85.

Patrick, T., & Hrycaiko, D. (1998). Effects of a mental training package on an

endurance performance. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 283-299.

Perry, C., & Morris, T. (1995). Mental imagery in sport. In T. Morris & J.

Summers (Eds.), Sport psychology: Theory, apllications and issues (pp. 339-

385). Brisbane, Australia: Wiley.

Pittman, A.M. (1989). Tennis. Scott, Foresman and Company.

Pluim, B. (1999). Conditioning and medical aspects on the female tennis

players. ITF Coaching and Sport Science Review, 18, 13-14.

Ram, N., & McCullagh, P. (2003). Self-modeling: Influence on psychological

responses and physical performance. Sport Psychologist,

17, 220-241.

Page 171: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

171

Reardon, J., & Dekkers, H. (2010). A guide to a mentality: Mindset in a

nutshell. ITF Coaching and sport Science Review, 52(18), 19-20.

Reid, M., Chow, J.W., & Crespo, M. (2003). Muscle activity: an indicator for

training. In B. Elliot, M. Reid & M. Crespo (Eds.), Biomechanics of advanced

tennis. ITF The International Tennis Federation.

Reid, M., & Crespo, M. (2003). Biomechanics of on-court movement. In B.

Elliot, M. Reid & M. Crespo (Eds.), Biomechanics of advanced tennis. ITF The

International Tennis Federation.

Reilly, T. (2007). The science of training – Soccer: a scientific approach to

developing strength, speed and endurance. Routledge.

Rhea, D., Mathes, S.A., & Hardin, K. (1997). Video recall for analysis of

performance by collegiate female tennis players. Perceptual and Motor Skills,

85(3:2), 1354.

Richardson, A. (1967). Mental practice: A review and discussion. Part 2.

Research Quarterly, 38, 263-273.

Rodgers, W., Hall, C., Buckolz, E. (1991). The effects of an imagery training

program on imagery ability, imagery use, and figure skating performance.

Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 109-125.

Roetert, P., & Groppel, J. (2001). World Class Tennis Technique (pp. 207).

Human Kinetics Publishers, Leeds.

Rotella , R.J., Gansneder, B., Ojala, D., & Billing, J. (1980). Cognitions and

coping strategies of elite skiers: An exploratory study of young developing

athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 350-354.

Page 172: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

172

Ryan, E.D., & Simons, J. (1981). Cognitive demand imagery and frequency of

mental practice as factors influencing the acquisition of mental skills. Journal

of Sport Psychology, 4, 35-45.

- .(1983). What is learned in mental practice of motor skills: A test of the

cognitive motor hypothesis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 419-426.

Ryska, T. (1998). Cognitive-behavioral strategies and precompetitive anxiety

among recreational athletes. Psychological Record, 48, 697-708.

Sadeghi, H., Omar-Fauzee, M.S., Jamalis, M., Ab-Latif, R., & Cheric, M.C.

(2010). The mental skills training of University soccer players. International

Education Studies, 3(2), 81-90.

Salmon, G., Hall, C., & Haslam, I. (1994). The use of imagery by soccer

players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 6(1), 116-133.

Savelsbergh, G.J.P., van der Kamp, J., Williams, A.M., & Ward, P. (2005).

Anticipation and visual search behaviour in expert soccer goal keepers.

Ergonomics, 48, 1686- 1697.

Savelsbergh, G.J.P., Williams, A.M., van der Kamp, J., & Ward, P. (2002).

Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. Journal of

Sports Sciences, 20, 279- 287.

Scales, W.D. (1995). The effects of mental imagery on tennis serving

performance across races. Master Thesis - Graduate Program in

Psychology, Lansing (Mi), Michigan State University.

Schmidt, R.A., & Lee, T.D. (1999). Motor control and learning: A behavioural

emphasis. 3rd ed., Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Page 173: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

173

Schmidt, R.A., & Wrisberg, C.A. (2000). Motor learning and performance. A

problem-based learning approach. 2nd ed., USA: Human Kinetics.

Schönborn, R. (2000). Advanced Techniques for Competitive Tennis. 2nd ed.,

Meyer & Meyer Sport.

Schönborn, R. (1999). Advanced techniques for competitive tennis. Aachen:

Meyer & Meyer.

Schönborn, R. (1993). Players’ performance and development. ITF Coaches

Review, 2, 1.

Schuyler, S. (1993). Winning tennis: a complete guide for coaches and

advanced players (pp. 69, 169-170).Prentice Hall.

Schunk, D.H., Hanson, A.R., & Cox, P.D. (1987). Peer-model attributes and

children’s achievement behaviour. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 54-

61.

Schwarzer, D. (2007). Entwicklungsverläufe und Karrieremuster im

internationalen Spitzentennis. University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg.

Schweer, M. (2007). Mentale Fitness im Tennis. Peter Lang GmbH.

Seibold, M. (2011). European Tennis: A comparative analysis of talent

identification and development (TID). Stuttgart: ibidem-Verlag.

Shaw, D.F., & Goodfellow, R. (1997). Performance enhancement and

deterioration following outcome imagery: testing a demand characteristics

explanation. In I.M. Cockeril (Ed.), Cognitive Enhancement in Sport and

Exercise Psychology (pp. 37-43). 1st ed., Leicester: Blackwell Publishers.

Page 174: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

174

Shaw, W.A. (1938). The distribution of muscular action-potentials during

imaging. Psychological Record, 2, 195-216.

- .(1940). The relation of muscular action potentials to imaginal weightlifting.

Archives of Psychology, 247, 50.

Short, S.E., Bruggeman, J.M., Engel, S.G., Marback, T.L., Wang, L.J.,

Willadsen, A. & Short, M.W. (2002). The effect of imagery function and

imagery direction on self-efficacy and performance on a golf-putting task. The

Sport Psychologist, 16, 48-67.

Simons, J. (2000). Doing imagery in the field. In M.B. Andersen (Ed.), Doing

sport psychology. Human Kinetics.

Singer, R.N. (2000). Performance and human factors: considerations about

cognition and attention for self-paced and externally-paced events.

Ergonomics, 43, 1661-1680.

Smith, D., & Collins, D. (2004). Mental practice, motor performance, and the

late CNV. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 412-426.

Smith, D., & Holmes, P. (2004). The effect of imagery modality on golf putting

performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 385-395.

Smith, D., Holmes, P., Whitemore, L., Collins, D., & Devenport, T. (2001). The

effect of theoretically-based imagery scripts on hockey penalty flick

performance. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24, 408-419.

Smith, D., & Wright, C.J. (2008). Imagery and Sport Performance. In A. Lane

(Ed.), Topics in Applied Psychology: Sport and Exercise Psychology. London:

Hodder Education.

Page 175: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

175

Snelleman, L. (1993). Speed training related to co-ordination, anticipation,

footwork and concentration. ITF Coaching and Sport Science Review, 1, 8-9.

Start, K.B., & Richardson, A. (1964). Imagery and mental practice. British

Journal of Education Psychology, 34, 280-284.

Stokell, I. (2002). Coaching women’s soccer: a revolutionary approach to

putting the play back into practice. Contemporary Books.

Stratton, G., Reilly, T., Williams, A.M., & Richardson, D. (2004). Youth soccer:

From science to performance. Routledge.

Suinn, R. (1976). Body thinking for Olympic champs. Psychology Today, 10,

38-43.

Suinn, R.M. (1976a). Visual motor behaviour rehearsal for adaptive

behaviour. In J. Krumboltz & C. Thoresen (Eds.), Counseling methods (pp.

360-366). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Suinn, R.M. (1983). Imagery in sports. In A.A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: Current

theory, research, and application (pp. 507-534). New York: Wiley.

Suinn, R.M. (1993). Imagery. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey & L.K. Tennant

(Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 492-510). New York:

Macmillan.

Surburg, P. (1968). Audio, visual, and audio-visual instruction with mental

practice in developing the forehand tennis drive. Research Quarterly, 39, 728-

734.

Page 176: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

176

Taylor, J. (1995). A conceptual model for integrating athletes’ needs and sport

demands in the development of competitive mental preparation strategies.

The Sport Psychologist, 9, 339-357.

Taylor, J.A., & Shaw, D.F. (2002). The effects of outcome imagery on golf-

putting performance. Journal of Sport Sciences, 20, 607-613.

Taylor, J., & Wilson, G. (2005). Applying Sport Psychology: Four Perspectives

(pp. 117-134). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Terry, P.C. (1995). Mental training for junior tennis players resident at the

Rover L.T.A. school, Bisham Abbey 1987-1993: Issues of delivery and

evaluation. In T. Reilly, M. Hughes & A. Lees (Eds.), Science and Racket

Sports. 1st ed., E & FN Spon, University Press, Cambridge.

Thelwell, R., Greenlees, I., & Weston, N. (2006). Using psychological skills

training to develop soccer performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,

18, 254-270.

Titley, R. (1976). The loneliness of a long-distance kicker. The Athletic

Journal, 57, 74-80.

Unierzyski, P. (1996). A retrospective analysis of junior Grand Slam winners.

ITF Coaching and Sport Science Review, 9, 2.

Unierzyski, P.,& Hurnik, E. (2004). Does practising on clay affect the level of

motor abilities of tennis players? In A. Lees, J.F. Kahn & I.W. Maynard

(Eds.), Science and Racket Sports III. Routledge.

Unierzyski, P., Szczepanowska, E., & Schefke, T. (1998). Training methods

for improving endurance. ITF Coaching and Sport Science Review, 15, 6-8.

Page 177: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

177

Unierzyski, P., & Wieczorek, A. (2004). Comparison of tactical solutions and

game patterns in the finals of two grand slam tournaments in tennis. In A.

Lees, J.F. Kahn, & I.W. Maynard (Eds.), Science and Racket Sports III.

Routledge.

Vadocz, E.A., Hall, C.R., & Moritz, S.E. (1997). The relationship between

cognitive anxiety and imagery use. Journal of Applied Psychology, 9, 241-

253.

Van Aken, I. (1998). Maintaining fitness during tournaments. ITF Coaching

and Sport Science Review, 15, 8-11.

Vealey, R.S. (2007). Mental Skills Training in Sport. In G. Tenenbaum & R.C.

Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psychology (pp. 287-309). 3rd ed.,

Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Vealey, R.S. (2005). Coaching for the inner edge. Morgantown, WV: Fitness

Information Technology.

Vealey, R.S. (1994). Current status and prominent issues in sport psychology

interventions. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 26, 495-502.

Vealey, R.S. (1986). Conceptualization of sport-confidence and competitive

orientation: Preliminary investigation and instrument development. Journal of

Sport Psychology, 8, 221-246.

Vealey, R.S., & Greenleaf, C. (2006). Seeing is believing: Understanding and

using imagery in sports. In J.M. Williams (Ed.), Applied Sport Psychology:

Personal growth to peak performance (pp. 306-348). 5th ed., Boston:

McGraw-Hill.

Page 178: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

178

Vealey, R.S., & Greenleaf, C. (1998). Seeing is believing: Understanding and

using imagery in sport. In J.M. Williams (Ed.), Applied Sport Psychology:

Personal growth to peak performance (pp. 237-269). 3th ed., Mountain View,

CA: Mayfield.

Vealey, R.S., & Walter, S.M. (1993). Imagery training for performance

enhancement and personal development. In J.M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport

psychology (pp. 220-224). 2nd ed., Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.

Velentzas, K., & Heinen T. (2011). Routine integration strategies and their

effects on volleyball serve performance and players’ movement mental

representation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 23, 209-222.

Wakefield, C.J., & Smith, D. (2009). Impact of differing frequencies of

PETTLEP imagery on netball shooting performance. Journal of Imagery

Research in Sport and Physical Activity, 4(1), 1-12.

Watt, A.P., Morris, T., & Andersen , M.B. (2004). Issues in the development of

a measure of imagery ability in sport. Journal of Mental Imagery, 28, 149-180.

Wei, G., & Luo, J. (2010). Sport expert’s motor imagery: Functional imaging of

professional motor skills and simple motor skills. Brain Research, 1341, 52-

62.

Weinberg, R. (2008). Does Imagery Work? Effects on Performance and

Mental Skills. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity,

3(1), Art.1.

Weinberg, R.S., & Gould, D. (2007). Foundations of Sport and Exercise

Psychology. 4th ed., Champaign. IL: Human Kinetics.

Page 179: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

179

White, A., & Hardy, L. (1998). An in depth analysis of the uses of imagery by

high level slalom canoeists and high level artistic gymnasts. The Sport

Psychologist, 12(4), 387-403.

Williams, A.M., & Burtwitz, L. (1993). Advance cue utilisation in soccer. In T.

Reilly, J. Clarys & A. Stibbe (Eds.), Science and Football II (pp. 239-44). E &

FN Spon, London.

Williams, A.M., Davids, K. & Williams, J.G. (1999). Visual Perception and

Action in Sport. London: E & FN Spon.

Williams, A.M. & Reilly, T. (2000). Talent Identification and Development in

Soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 657-667.

Williams, J.M. (1994). Applied Sport Psychology: Personal Growth to Peak

Performance. 3rd ed., California: Mayfield Publishing Company.

Witzig, R. (2006). The global art of soccer. CusiBoy Publishing.

Woolfolk, R.L., Parrish, M.W., & Murphy, S.M. (1985). The effect of positive

and negative imagery on motor skill performance. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 9, 335-341.

Wrisberg, C.A., & Ragsdale, M.R. (1979). Cognitive demand and practice

level: Factors in the mental rehearsal of motor skills. Journal of Human

Movement Studies, 5, 201-208.

Page 180: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

180

Appendices

Inquiries and considerations

A good thesis has to begin with a systematic literature search on the

topic in order to proof the accessibility. The library of the University of

Konstanz was used to search for books and articles on sports science and

sports psychology. Furthermore, available online data banks like SPOLIT and

DOSB and the option for inter-library loan in the University of Konstanz have

been used.

Also, the sport science working group of the University of Konstanz

under the direction of Dr. Darko Jekauc held some meetings on this topic. It

was decided that three interventions dealing with this topic would be done in

three sports (tennis, volleyball, soccer). Results of the intervention in

volleyball are not included in this work.

The result of the meetings was a detailed overview of the upcoming

challenges:

• Duration, extent and development of the intervention

• The design and the construction of a questionnaire that includes all the

relevant constructs of the intervention group and control group

• Processing of an existing workbook

• Literature for the control group in the form of articles

• Gathering ideas for the acquisition of subjects

• A random classification of the possible tennis and soccer clubs in the

intervention and control group

Page 181: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

181

• Organizational things: availability of cameras, recruitment of assistants

for the execution of the intervention or usage of the buses of the university to

get to distant clubs

Afterwards an intervention plan was tested.

Page 182: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

182

Intervention Procedure: Mental Training in Tennis

1. Establishing contact and encouraging collaboration:

a. What is the project about? What is mental training, and what is it

not?

b. What do we want to achieve? What are the goals?

c. How can a coach or player benefit from this project?

d. What is the procedure?

2. Preliminary meeting with the players

a. Presentation of the project

b. Inspiring motivation to participate

c. Clarification of roles

d. Explanation of objectives and expectations

3. First training session

a. Questionnaire

b. Serving practice (warm-up)

c. Viewing of practice videos to analyze personal serving motion

d. Short relaxation (breathing exercise) and visualization of the

trajectory

e. Practice of the short visualization

f. Precision test (Integration of the short visualization into the

routine through practical implementation) (20 serves)

g. Debriefing

4. Second training session

a. Viewing of practice videos to analyze personal serving motion

Page 183: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

183

b. Short relaxation and visualization of the ball’s trajectory

c. Practice of the short visualization through practical

implementation of the entire routine (possible point play)

d. Debriefing

5. Third training session

a. Viewing of practice videos to analyze personal serving motion

b. Short relaxation and visualization of the ball’s trajectory

c. Practice of the short visualization through practical

implementation of the entire routine (possible point play)

d. Concluding discussion

6. Fourth training session

a. Viewing of practice videos to analyze personal serving motion

b. Short relaxation and visualization of the ball’s trajectory

c. Practice of the short visualization through practical

implementation of the entire routine (possible point play)

d. Precision test (20 serves)

e. Concluding discussion

Page 184: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

184

Breathing Relaxation with Self-Instruction and the Positive Visualization

of the Trajectory

Introduction

Find a relaxed position. If sitting: Sit on the bench and put your feet flat on the

ground. Your back should be straight and shoulders should be loose, low, and

relaxed. Your head should be relaxed, supported with minimal muscular effort

by the neck. Your hands can be placed on your thighs or in your lap. Now

close your eyes.

Breathing Relaxation

Breathe deeply in and out six times

While inhaling, pull yourself up straight and bring your shoulder-blades

together

Now exhale and let your shoulders slowly relax

Sit up straight again and inhale deeply, counting slowly to 6

Still sitting up straight, slowly exhale to the count of 6

Repeat this exercise. Relax and enjoy the tranquility

Feel how your stomach lifts and falls while you inhale and exhale

deeply

Concentrate on the long path of your breathing

Let yourself go. Enjoy the peace and harmony of this state

Breath slowly and evenly

Self-Instruction and the Positive Visualization of the Trajectory for the ball

Before you serve each time, please give yourself a silent self-

instruction. Imagine now that you are on the tennis court, about to

Page 185: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

185

serve. You find yourself just behind the baseline, near the center mark.

You practice your first serve, aiming it with high speed at the corner of

the court. There is a target there that you want to hit.

Concentrate now on the ball’s trajectory. You contact the ball high

above your head. The ball leaves the racket, moving toward the

opposite side of the court, crosses over the net and hits the target

directly in the center. You see a clear, positive and precise trajectory

for the ball.

Visualize your serve several times. Concentrate on the ball’s trajectory.

Finish

Flex your arms

Breathe deeply

Open your eyes

Brief Self-Instruction and Positive Visualization on the Court

Position yourself at the baseline. Look directly at the corner where you want

to serve. Bounce the ball several times. Look at the corner again. Close your

eyes and imagine how the ball’s trajectory leads it to the corner and how the

ball hits the center of the target area.

Now serve!

How well did the visualization work?

Why was that so?

Could the routine be changed?

Repeat this visualization procedure several times.

Page 186: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

186

Universität Konstanz Sportwissenschaft

Workbook

Mental Training in

Tennis

Page 187: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

187

Workbook in tennis

Here the reader gets information about the content of the workbook. Because

of formating reasons is is not possible to show a copy of an original workbook.

Block of exercises 1

Week 1

Exercise 1: Picture Exercise

Step 1:

Assume a relaxed body position. Close your eyes and breathe deeply, calmly

and consistently. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and how it

falls when breathing out. Breathe deeply and slowly several times. Slowly

count to six when inhaling and count to six when exhaling.

Step 2:

Imagine your eyes are the lenses of a camera.

Step 3:

Select a section of your environment, for example where you training , a

landscape, your room or another place of your choice.

Step 4:

Look at this place for 1-2 seconds and then abruptly close your eyes. “Take a

picture” of the place with your eyes.

Step 5:

Now imagine seeing that picture clearly in front of your (still) closed eyes.

Observe the picture with all little details as clear as possible with all your

senses. What do you see? What do you smell? What do you feel? What do

you hear? What do you taste?

Page 188: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

188

Step 6:

In case of an unclear picture open your eyes and repeat steps 3 to 5.

Further tips for this exercise:

This exercise can be embedded perfectly in your everyday life. You can

do this everywhere.

In times without contests you can go through this exercise over and

over again and imagine your contest spot.

Try to produce clear and definite pictures with closed eyes.

This exercise trains your senses, concentration, attentiveness and your

imagery ability.

Exercise 2: Remote control exercise

Step 1:

Assume a relaxed body position. Close your eyes and breathe deeply, calmly

and consistently. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and how it

falls when breathing out. Breathe deeply and slowly several times. Slowly

count to six when inhaling and count to six when exhaling.

Step 2:

Imagine holding a remote control. On your remote control you can find buttons

to change certain options.

Loud/silent

Bright/dark

Slow motion/ fast motion

Page 189: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

189

Zoom in/out

Step 3:

Imagine a location of your choice (for example tennis court, shopping mall and

so on). Try to imagine the sounds, surrounding, colors, objects and so on very

exactly.

Step 4:

Use your remote control and change your location down to your whim. In your

imagination you can also introduce buttons for smells, feelings and thoughts.

Further tips on this exercise:

This exercise can be done on a lot of locations. Be creative and imagine

different spots that you like.

The remote control exercise trains your senses, concentration, attentiveness

and your imagery ability.

You can integrate it perfectly in your sport (for example to suppress noise that

distracts you, just grab your remote control and turn down the noise level).

IMPORTANT!!!

You have to train and control this exercise very well. Test it in the training and

see how you can get along with it before trying it in a competition.

Exercise 3: Sustain senses

Step 1:

Assume a relaxed body position. Close your eyes and breathe deeply, calmly

and consistently. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and how it

falls when breathing out. Breathe deeply and slowly several times. Slowly

count to six when inhaling and count to six when exhaling.

Page 190: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

190

Step 2:

Imagine a bucket with tennis balls. Imagine it standing next to you on the red

ash of the tennis court. Take a ball out of it. Think about how many balls could

be left in the bucket.

Step 3:

Now turn towards your ball. Have a close look at it. Imagine it exactly. It's

color, his form. It has a shining yellow color and smells new. It seems that you

are using this ball for the first time.

Step 4:

Imagine yourself bouncing this ball on the court. 1 time, 2 times, 3 times, 4

times. Imagine the sound that you produce by doing so. Meanwhile you smell

the ash of the court. A slight breeze blows that stirs up the dust a little bit.

Step 5:

Take the ball in your hand and throw it in the air, like in a serve. You also

move your right arm like in a serve but instead of ending this movement you

each time catch the ball. You repeat this move: 1 time, 2 times, 3 times, 4

times

Step 6:

Fade out the picture of the ball slowly. You erase yourself from the tennis

court and you slowly sense your body again. Elongate your body and stretch,

then deeply breathe three times. Open your eyes.

Further tips on this exercise:

You can decide freely what kind of objects you want to integrate in your

imagination. It is important for you to generate a clear picture in your

Page 191: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

191

head. This exercise trains your senses, concentration, attentiveness

and your imagery ability.

You will see: the more you repeat this exercise, the better and more

detailed the pictures of the object will be.

This exercise can also easily be integrated in your everyday life.

Exercise block 2

Second week:

Exercise 4: personal winning formula

Step 1:

Assume a relaxed body position. Close your eyes and breathe deeply, calmly

and consistently. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and how it

falls when breathing out. Breathe deeply and slowly several times. Slowly

count to six when inhaling and count to six when exhaling.

Step 2:

Imagine pictures of moments of big access or good performance in your past

(for example a big win, the happy feeling of success, the feeling after a hard

training and so on).

Step 3:

Imagine pictures, symbols or phrases that come to your mind during that. For

example imagine the following situation: After a long hard match you win the

game with a perfect final shot. You hear the applause from the audience.

Imagine accompanying phrases. How did you feel? What did you say to

yourself? How did you motivate you?

Page 192: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

192

For example:

“Today is my day”

“I did my best from the beginning to the end”

“Today I am going to show what I am capable of”

“That is my big day”

“There is a winner hidden inside me”

“Every hit is perfect”

Step 4:

These are just examples. Choose your personal phrase/your personal symbol

that motivate/motivates you. It is to get you on the right energy level in the

training or in the competition.

Further tips on this exercise:

7- Keep your phrase in present tense

8- Form easy sentences

9- Repeat the phrase multiple times and constant

10- Only use positive phrases

11- The phrase has to feel good and has to inspire confidence.

12- Try out your phrase not until training (for example test it in the warm

up phase or just before starting the training)

13- You have to control this first and your phrase has u support you,

before you can use this outside of training.

Exercise 5: Problem box exercise

Page 193: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

193

Step 1:

Get yourself something to write and an envelope. Take on a comfortable

position. Close your eyes and breathe calmly and constantly. Notice how your

stomach rises when breathing in and lowers when breathing out. Breathe in

and out three times. Count slowly to three when breathing in and count to six

when breathing out.

Step 2:

Let your thoughts flow like clouds on the horizon. Take as much time for this

as you need.

Step 3:

Register all occurring unpleasant thoughts that could distract you from the

next match and write them down.

Step 4:

Put what you just wrote down in the envelope (“problem box”) and keep it

there.

Step 5:

After the match open the envelope and read your notes. Try to find strategies

to solve the written down problems.

Further tips on this exercise:

This exercise helps to locate sources of distractions and to lower

nervousness.

Include your coach in step 5. Maybe he can help you to find solution

strategies.

Page 194: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

194

Exercise 6: Your personal match attunement

The goal of this exercise is to write down a flow chart of your attunement to

the match.

Step 1:

Write an announcement for the match. The date and the time of the match

has to be included.

Step 2:

Describe the match situation, the requirements and the technique of your

serve.

Step 3:

The goal is to find your personal corroboration. Find many phrases, pictures

and symbols that motivate you. Write them down first. Here an example:

“I know that I am good” – “I am looking forward to the game” – “I am totally

prepared, physically as well as psychologically” – “When it starts I am going to

be fully concentrated” – “I will act without thinking about the future and the

past!” – “My body bends like a bow at the serve and I am going to hit the ball

at full power with my racket!”

These are just examples. You can change them or add others.

Step4:

Now write this text on a paper.

Further tips on this exercise:

The text has to send out strength and confidence

When updating step 1 and 2 the paper can be used for every match (or

training)

Page 195: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

195

You should write this text in calm surroundings at a calm time of day

Rewrite the text until it is satisfying

You can combine it with pictures that support you in your condition.

You can add them to the text. Here it is important that the pictures

support you or heat up depending on what energy level you can use

better in the training/match and what optimizes your motivation.

You can read your match attunement over again at quiet times.

Important!!!!

Do not use it in matches until you have tested it perfectly. Use it in training

first.

Exercise block 3:

Third week:

Exercise 7: The ideal role model

Step 1:

Imagine a tennis player that embodies all the skills that you miss at yourself.

This can be the best member of your tennis club or a professional that you

saw in the television or somewhere else. Pay attention to the fact that his style

is not so different from yours.

Step 2:

Find a comfortable body position. Close your eyes and breathe calmly and

constantly. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and lowers

Page 196: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

196

when breathing out. Breathe in and out three times. Count slowly to three

when breathing in and count to six when breathing out.

Step3:

Imagine your training place. Instead of yourself, imagine your role model on

the tennis court. Pay attention to how he moves when serving. Watch him a

couple of times. Try to understand the motion sequence. How he tips the ball

on the ground. He throws the ball high into the air. He strikes out. The whole

body is strained and waits for the perfect moment. Finally the ball is hit high

above his head. You follow the flight path. The ball hits his target exactly.

Relax again, breath in and out three times and move yourself back to the

court. Your role model moves perfectly again.

Step 4:

Now concentrate on his/her hands until you have the impression that they are

your own hands. You can now put yourself into this body and feel how it is to

do the perfect serve. Pay attention to the ease, the strength, and the sense of

delight during the movement. When you do the right movement a couple of

times you can evoke these feelings in your room. You feel like you just hit the

perfect serve.

Further tips on this exercise:

The closer you can identify yourself with your role model, the better you can

put yourself into the role model and imagine what this person feels during the

move and what confidence he/she develops, the more efficient the activating

effect for the own movement will be.

Page 197: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

197

Imagine exactly where your shortcomings in the movement are. The goal is

the optimization of the motion sequence.

It holds: Practice makes perfect

Exercise 8: Your perfect image

Step 1:

Assume a relaxed body position. Close your eyes and breathe deeply, calmly

and consistently. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and how it

falls when breathing out. Breathe deeply and slowly several times. Slowly

count to six when inhaling and count to six when exhaling.

Step 2:

Remember your perfect serve. Everything worked out perfectly and you felt

great!

Step 3:

Watch this scene multiple times from different point of views. Find out the

aspect that was responsible for making this serve perfect.

Step 4:

Put yourself in your own person, concentrate on this certain aspect and be

aware of how it feels to do it right.

Step 5:

Watch yourself during the movement. End the exercise with the joy over your

own perfectness. Take this feeling out of your imagination and bring it into the

real room you are in.

Further tips on this exercise:

Page 198: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

198

This exercise can be used when having unprecedented troubles with

the serve. Via the imagination you can recognize what you did

differently in the past.

You can recognize mistakes and regain your old performance.

Practice at a quite location and apply this exercise not until you can

handle it perfectly.

Exercise 9: Replay exercise

The replay is the instantaneous mental repetition of your performance. You

should practice the replay immediately after ending the serve if you have the

time to replay it in your mind.

Step 1:

Assume a relaxed body position. Close your eyes and breathe deeply, calmly

and consistently. Notice how your stomach rises when breathing in and how it

falls when breathing out. Breathe deeply and slowly several times. Slowly

count to six when inhaling and count to six when exhaling.

Step 2:

Imagine the past movement: How did you feel during and while preparing for

the performance?

Step 3:

Imagine running through the whole thing again. How does your body behave,

how does it feel and how much do you control the move? What characteristic

is expressed?

Page 199: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

199

Step 4:

Go back to the start and watch your performance. Did you neglect any parts

of your body? Picture the scene exactly, then put yourself in your own

position and pay attention to what you thought right before the performance.

What did you feel? Did these thoughts help you or distract you?

Step 5:

Is there a change in your technique that you should think about? If so, picture

the whole scene again in your mind, keep all the positive aspects and change

what would improve your performance. Now visualize the improved version.

Further tips on this exercise:

Use the improved version before performing again.

The replay can also be used after a perfect performance in order to

keep the details in your mind.

Practice the replay in the training until you don’t have any more

problems. Then and only then you can use this method in competition.

Goals:

Sit straight but comfortably in a chair. Breathe in and out a couple of times

calmly and constantly. Imagine a triangular target area, on the court. The

target area is formed via the tension belt of the net and the middle line of the

T-filed and the T-field line. Imagine serving the ball in this field. You throw

the ball and perfectly hit the ball with your racket. The ball lands heavy in the

middle of the target area. Your opponent tries to return but can only hit the

ball with the frame of his racket. The ball flies against the fence. You are

Page 200: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

200

proud of your successful serve and clench your fist. Repeat the above-

mentioned exercise a couple of times.

You can replace the target area with other areas and repeat the exercise:

Your target area is a triangle again. It is formed by the t-line, the middle

field line and a virtual line that goes from you to the left corner of the

left t-field.

Or:

Your goal is to hit the body of your opponent. Your target area is a

rectangle. It is formed by two lines that go from you to the opponent

and the t-field line.

End of the workbook

The soccer workbook was nearly identical to the tennis workbook. Besides

replacing serve with penalty kick there were no substantive changes to the

workbook.

Page 201: The Effect of Mental Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis ...the mental training on tennis serving precision, while for soccer penalty kicking the effect of mental training on precision

201

Declaration

“I, Khaled Hegazy, declare that the PhD thesis entitled “The Effect of Mental

Training on Precision Tasks in Tennis and Soccer- A Study on

Educational Technology”

This thesis contains no material that has been submitted previously, for the

award of any other academic degree or diploma. Except where otherwise

indicated, this thesis is my own work”.

Signature Date