the impact of simulated astigmatism on …the impact of simulated astigmatism on functional measures...

133
THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE Sung-Hua Yang B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Applied Science (Research) Vision and Driving Laboratory School of Optometry and Vision Science Faculty of Health Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation Queensland University of Technology

Upload: others

Post on 25-Apr-2020

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL

PERFORMANCE

Sung-Hua Yang B.Optom., MSc.

May, 2015

Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of

Master of Applied Science (Research)

Vision and Driving Laboratory

School of Optometry and Vision Science

Faculty of Health

Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation

Queensland University of Technology

Page 2: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

i

Keywords

Astigmatic blur

Astigmatic axis

Astigmatic power

Visual acuity

Reading rates

Visual processing speeds

Reading eye movements

Chinese characters

Page 3: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

ii

Abstract

Uncorrected refractive error, including astigmatism, is a leading cause of reversible

visual impairment. While the ability to perform vision-related daily activities is

reduced when people are not optimally corrected, only limited research has

investigated the impact of uncorrected astigmatism. Given the capacity to perform

vision-related daily activities involves integration of a range of visual and cognitive

cues, this research examined the impact of simulated astigmatism on visual tasks that

also involved cognitive input. The research also examined whether the higher levels

of complexity inherent in Chinese characters makes them more susceptible to the

effects of astigmatism. The effects of different powers of astigmatism, as well as

astigmatism at different axes were investigated in order to determine the minimum

level of astigmatism that resulted in a decrement in visual performance.

Study 1 investigated the impact of three powers (0.50 DC, 1.00 DC, 2.00 DC) of

against-the-rule astigmatism (ATR) and three different axes of astigmatism (ATR,

with-the-rule (WTR) and oblique (OBL)) on distance and near visual acuity, and

reading rates for six font sizes. Participants included 12 young, visually normal

adults (mean age 23.9 ± 5.5 years; range 18 to 34 years). Distance visual acuity was

measured using a high contrast logMAR chart at 4 m, while near visual acuity and

reading rates were tested using a series of Bailey-Lovie reading charts. The order of

testing was randomised, and participants were required to watch a movie (at 40 cm)

for 10 minutes to standardise the level of blur adaptation. A short break (5 minutes)

with clear vision was provided on completion of each testing condition. Distance and

near visual acuity significantly (p<0.05) decreased as a function of increasing levels

of cylindrical power for ATR astigmatism. Importantly, even small amounts of ATR

astigmatism (as low as 0.50 DC) had a significant negative impact on visual acuity.

Similarly, the axis of astigmatism differentially impacted on both distance and near

visual acuity compared with best-correction, where OBL astigmatism had a

significantly greater detrimental effect than either WTR or ATR astigmatism.

Reading rate for all six font sizes significantly decreased with increasing power of

ATR astigmatism and as a function of astigmatic axis. Furthermore, there were

significant interactions between font size and astigmatic power, where the effects of

Page 4: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

iii

astigmatism were greater for smaller font sizes. In addition, reading rate with OBL

astigmatism was significantly slower than for the other axes of astigmatism.

Study 2 investigated the impact of simulated astigmatism of the same powers and

axes as in Study 1 on outcome measures that involved the integration of vision and

cognitive skills. Participants included 12 visually normal young adults (mean age

24.8 ± 5.3 years; range 18 to 34 years). Outcome measures included distance and

near visual acuity, and tasks involving visual and cognitive skills (Developmental

Eye Movement Test (DEM), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), and Trail

Making Tests A and B). Increasing levels of ATR astigmatism increased visual

processing time on both the Trails A and B tests and also reduced DSST scores,

relative to the best-corrected condition and all conditions were significantly different

to one another. While all three axes of astigmatism decreased performance on the

Trails A and B tests and the DSST, there were no between axis differences, with the

exception of the Trails B where both WTR and OBL astigmatism were worse than

ATR. Performance on these tests is thus clearly affected by the presence of

astigmatism even at low levels, but there is little axis dependency. Conversely,

performance on the DEM was more robust to the effects of simulated astigmatism.

Collectively, the results of Study 2 suggest that performance of tasks involving the

integration of vision and cognitive skills, visual scanning and visual processing are

influenced by the level of astigmatism, but are less axis dependent. This slowing of

visual scanning and visual processing in the presence of astigmatism has implications

for performance of everyday activities.

Study 3 evaluated the impact of astigmatism on visual acuity and reading

performance using Chinese rather than English characters, in order to determine

whether the higher levels of complexity inherent in Chinese characters makes them

more susceptible to the effects of astigmatism. Participants included 12, visually

normal young adults (mean age 23.5 ± 2.4 years; range 20 to 28 years), for whom

Chinese was their first spoken and written language. Distance visual acuity was

tested as for Studies 1 and 2, while near visual acuity and reading rate were tested

using Chinese near vision charts. Astigmatism had a significantly greater negative

impact on reading rates using Chinese compared to the English symbols investigated

in Study 1. Reading rates for Chinese text became slower with increasing powers of

Page 5: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

iv

ATR astigmatism, even as low as 0.50 DC. Astigmatic axis also impacted on reading

rate, where simulated astigmatism reduced reading rate for all font sizes compared to

optimal correction, with OBL astigmatism having the greatest detrimental effect. The

finding that astigmatism had a greater negative impact on reading rates for

Chinese compared to English characters is likely to reflect the relative visual

complexity and higher levels of cognitive processing required for reading

Chinese characters. This suggests that correction of astigmatism is even more

important for individuals reading Chinese than English text and that astigmatism

as low as 0.50 DC can be problematic.

In summary, different astigmatic powers resulted in reduced distance and near visual

acuity and also lowered reading rates and visual processing speeds. Oblique

astigmatism had the greatest negative impact, with the majority of outcomes being

affected to a similar extent by WTR and ATR astigmatism. Collectively, these

findings are important for better understanding the influence of astigmatism on

functional vision and assist eye care practitioners in recognising the importance of

correcting astigmatism, even for levels as low as 0.50 DC.

Page 6: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

v

Table of Contents

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 1 1.1 Background .................................................................................................................................. 1 1.2 Aims of this research ................................................................................................................... 2 1.3 Thesis outline ............................................................................................................................... 3 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................................. 6 2.1 Optics of the eye .......................................................................................................................... 6 2.2 Astigmatism ................................................................................................................................. 7

2.2.1 Symmetry of astigmatism between right and left eyes ................................................... 10

2.2.2 Association between spherical refractive error and astigmatism .................................... 10

2.2.3 The influence of genetics on astigmatism ....................................................................... 12

2.2.4 Prevalence and changes of astigmatism with age ........................................................... 12

2.2.5 Blur adaptation and astigmatism ..................................................................................... 14

2.2.6 The correction of astigmatism ........................................................................................ 14 2.3 Refractive error and quality of life ............................................................................................. 15 2.4 Refractive error and reading ...................................................................................................... 16 2.5 Refractive error and driving ....................................................................................................... 17 2.6 Impact of impaired vision on tasks involving cognitive skills ................................................... 18 2.7 Impact of astigmatism on visual acuity ...................................................................................... 19 2.8 Impact of astigmatism on reading performance ......................................................................... 21 2.9 Testing visual acuity and reading with Chinese characters ........................................................ 23 2.10 Rationale .................................................................................................................................... 24 CHAPTER 3: IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATIC REFRACTIVE BLUR ON VISUAL ACUITY AND READING.................................................................................................................. 27 3.1 Introduction ................................................................................................................................ 27 3.2 Methods ..................................................................................................................................... 28

3.2.1 Participants ..................................................................................................................... 28

3.2.2 Screening procedures ...................................................................................................... 28

Page 7: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

vi

3.2.3 Protocol ........................................................................................................................... 30

3.2.4 Distance and near visual acuity ...................................................................................... 32

3.2.5 Reading rate .................................................................................................................... 33

3.2.6 Statistical analysis ........................................................................................................... 33

3.3 Results ........................................................................................................................................ 34

3.3.1 Powers of ATR astigmatism ........................................................................................... 34

3.3.2 Axes of astigmatism ....................................................................................................... 40 3.4 Discussion .................................................................................................................................. 44 3.5 Conclusion ................................................................................................................................. 46 CHAPTER 4: IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATIC REFRACTIVE BLUR ON TESTS INVOLVING VISION AND COGNITION ...................................................................................... 48 4.1 Introduction ................................................................................................................................ 48 4.2 Method ....................................................................................................................................... 49

4.2.1 Participants ..................................................................................................................... 49

4.2.2 Protocol ........................................................................................................................... 49

4.2.3 Distance and near visual acuity ...................................................................................... 50

4.2.4 Developmental Eye Movement Test (DEM test) ............................................................ 50

4.2.5 Digit Symbol Substitution test (DSST)........................................................................... 51

4.2.6 Trail Making Tests A and B (TMT A and TMT B) ........................................................ 52

4.2.7 Statistical analysis ........................................................................................................... 53 4.3 Results ........................................................................................................................................ 53

4.3.1 Powers of ATR astigmatism ........................................................................................... 53 4.3.2 Axes of astigmatism ....................................................................................................... 59

4.4 Discussion .................................................................................................................................. 66 4.5 Conclusion ................................................................................................................................. 68 CHAPTER 5: IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM BLUR ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE USING CHINESE CHARACTERS..................... 70 5.1 Introduction ................................................................................................................................ 70

Page 8: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

vii

5.2 Method ....................................................................................................................................... 71

5.2.1 Participants ..................................................................................................................... 71

5.2.2 Protocol ........................................................................................................................... 71

5.2.3 Distance and near visual acuity ...................................................................................... 72

5.2.4 Reading rate .................................................................................................................... 73

5.2.5 Statistical analysis ........................................................................................................... 74 5.3 Results ........................................................................................................................................ 75

5.3.1 Powers of ATR astigmatism ........................................................................................... 75

5.3.2 Axes of astigmatism ....................................................................................................... 79

5.3.3 Comparison of the reduction in reading performance with astigmatism for English and Chinese characters ...................................................................................... 82

5.4 Discussion .................................................................................................................................. 89 5.5 Conclusion ................................................................................................................................. 93 CHAPTER 6: CONCLUSIONS ........................................................................................................ 94 6.1 Impact of astigmatism on distance and near visual acuity ......................................................... 95 6.2 Impact of astigmatism on reading rate (English characters) ...................................................... 96 6.3 Impact of astigmatism on functional visual tasks (DEM, DSST, and TMT A and B) ............... 96 6.4 Impact of astigmatism on reading rate (Chinese characters) ..................................................... 98 6.5 Summary and Implications ........................................................................................................ 99 APPENDICES ................................................................................................................................... 102

Appendix 1 Participant information and consent form ............................................................ 102 REFERENCES .................................................................................................................................. 105

Page 9: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

viii

List of Figures Figure 2-1 – Total wavefront aberrations (left), sphero-cylinder components (centre)

and higher order aberration components (right). Total equals the sum of the sphero-cylinder and higher order components; colours at the red end of the spectrum indicate higher dioptric powers... ...............................................................6

Figure 2-2 – Schematic representation of the eye’s refracting surfaces; the front and back surfaces of the cornea and the front and back surfaces of the crystalline lens. GRIN is gradient refractive index. ....................................................................8

Figure 2-3 – With-the-rule astigmatism (left) has the steeper corneal meridian along the vertical meridian (90 ± 30 degrees), oblique astigmatism (centre) has the steeper corneal meridian between (30 to 60 and 120 to 150 degrees, while against-the rule astigmatism (right) has the steeper corneal meridian along the horizontal meridian (180 ± 30 degrees); colours at the red end of the spectrum indicate steeper curvatures.. .....................................................................10

Figure 2-4 – The effect of different axes of astigmatism on image quality. In myopic ATR the vertical detail is blurred, in myopic OBL the oblique detail is blurred and in myopic WTR the horizontal detail is blurred. ..................................19

Figure 3-1 – Sample of Bailey-Lovie reading chart................................................................33

Figure 3-2 – Group mean distance visual acuity as a function of three different powers of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ............35

Figure 3-3 – Group mean near visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................37

Figure 3-4 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ......................................................................................................................38

Figure 3-5 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different levels of spherical blur and ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .........39

Figure 3-6 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................40

Figure 3-7 – Group mean near visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).........................................................41

Figure 3-8 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different astigmatic axis (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .....................43

Figure 4-1 – Schematic representation of the different components of the DEM test. ...........51

Figure 4-2 – Sample of DSST and Trail Making tests. ...........................................................52

Figure 4-3 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .........................54

Page 10: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

ix

Figure 4-4 – Group mean near visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................56

Figure 4-5 – Mean DEM adjusted vertical and horizontal time with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .................57

Figure 4-6 – Mean DSST score with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ...............................................................58

Figure 4-7 – Mean TMT time with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ...............................................................59

Figure 4-8 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................60

Figure 4-9 – Group mean near visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).........................................................62

Figure 4-10 – Mean DEM adjusted vertical and horizontal time with astigmatism on different axes (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .......................63

Figure 4-11 – Mean DSST score with astigmatism on different axes (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ...............................................................64

Figure 4-12 – Mean Trail Making test time with astigmatism on different axes (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).........................................................65

Figure 5-1 – Sample of a Chinese (7 strokes per character) near visual acuity chart .............73

Figure 5-2 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .......................755

Figure 5-3 – Group mean near visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................76

Figure 5-4 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ......................................................................................................................78

Figure 5-5 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................79

Figure 5-6 – Group mean near visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).........................................................81

Figure 5-7 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different astigmatic axis (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .....................82

Figure 5-8 – Reduction in the overall percentage reading rate as a function of the three different powers of astigmatism for the English and Chinese characters (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). .............................................85

Figure 5-9 – Overall reduction in percentage reading rate for all power of ATR astigmatism combined as a function of character size for the Chinese and English charts (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). ......................86

Page 11: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

x

Figure 5-10 – Overall reduction in percentage reading rate for 2.00 DC of astigmatism for the three axes combined as a function of font/symbol size for the Chinese and English charts (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).. ..............88

Figure 5-11 – Sample of similarity between Chinese characters ............................................92

Page 12: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

xi

List of Tables Table 2-1 – Summary of studies comparing the effect of astigmatic axis orientation on

visual performance. .................................................................................................23

Table 3-1 – Luminance and illuminance levels of charts .......................................................32

Table 3-2 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different powers of ATR astigmatism compared to baseline ....................................................................................................................36

Table 3-3 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) for the effect of three different levels of spherical blur and astigmatism compared with baseline condition ...........39

Table 3-4 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different axes of astigmatism compared with baseline condition ..................................................................................................................42

Table 4-1 – Luminance and illumination of charts .................................................................50

Table 4-2 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different powers of ATR astigmatism ........................................55

Table 4-3 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of 3 different axes of astigmatism compared with baseline condition ..................................................................................................................61

Table 5-1 – Luminance and illuminance of near charts ..........................................................72

Table 5-2 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different powers of ATR astigmatism compared to baseline ....................................................................................................................77

Table 5-3 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of 3 different axes of astigmatism compared with baseline condition ..................................................................................................................80

Table 5-4 – Group mean percentage reduction (± standard deviation) in reading rate relative to the optimal correction for the 3 levels of astigmatism and for each of the six font sizes for the English (Study 1) and Chinese (Study 3) characters .................................................................................................................84

Table 5-5 – Group mean percentage reduction (± standard deviation) in reading rate relative to the optimal correction for the 3 axes of astigmatism and for each of the six font sizes for the English (Study 1) and Chinese (Study 3) charts. .........87

Page 13: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

QUT Verified Signature

Page 14: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

xii

Acknowledgements

I would like to appreciate my supervisor Professor Joanne Wood for her invaluable

advice, assistance and support throughout my career. I would also thank my associate

supervisor Professor Michael Collins for all of his generosity, assistance and

encouragement.

Thanks to everyone at Vision and Driving Laboratory, and Contact Lens and Visual

Optics Laboratory, particularly Alexander Black, Rodney Jensen, Fan Yi, and

Catherine Foster for their advice and encouragement throughout my journey. Thanks

also to Philippe Lacherez for his invaluable statistical advice. In addition, I wish to

thank Sumithira Narayanasamy for her generosity in time and encouragement. Many

thanks to the participants, without their generosity of time and enthusiasm, none of

these studies would have been possible.

A special thanks to my parents for their understanding, enduring support, and

guidance. Finally, I would like to thank Cuihong Miao for her continued support,

patience, and understanding during my studies.

Page 15: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

1

Chapter 1: Introduction

1.1 Background

Uncorrected refractive error is a leading cause of reversible visual impairment

(Coleman et al., 2006) and has been shown to reduce distance visual acuity and near

work as well as functional tasks of daily living such as reading and driving (Higgins

et al., 1998; Chung et al., 2007; Wood et al., 2014). While there has been extensive

research into the effects of spherical refractive blur on clinical and functional tests of

vision, there is only limited research investigating the effects of blurred vision caused

by uncorrected astigmatic refractive error and this is an important issue given the

relatively high prevalence of astigmatism in the population (Fan et al., 2004; Read et

al., 2007; Ferrer-Blasco et al., 2009; Hashemi et al., 2011).

Some studies have investigated the impact of astigmatism on visual acuity (Villegas

et al., 2006), and functional visual performance measures such as reading

(Casagrande et al., 2013). However, investigation of the effect of a range of

astigmatic powers and axis orientations on these measures has not been undertaken

and was one of the main aims of the studies in this thesis. In addition, given that the

capacity to perform vision-related daily activities involves the integration of a range

of visual and cognitive cues and does not simply depend on high contrast visual

acuity, this research also examined the impact of simulated astigmatism on a range of

tasks involving both visual and cognitive function, including visual search and speed

of visual processing tasks.

This research also examined whether the higher levels of complexity inherent in

Chinese characters makes them more susceptible to the effects of simulated

astigmatism. Chinese characters are different to English characters in that they can

have up to 18 strokes in a single character and these strokes can be various lengths

and at a wide variety of angles. Therefore reading Chinese characters in the presence

of astigmatic blur presents a unique challenge in terms of spatial resolution and

Page 16: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

2

cognitive demand. A particular aim of this research was to determine the minimum

level of astigmatism that impacted on visual performance when assessed using

Chinese characters, as well as the axis of astigmatism that had the greatest negative

impact on performance. This is a potentially important issue given the relatively high

prevalence of astigmatism in the Chinese population (Chen, et al., 2013).

1.2 Aims of this research

The overall aim of this research was to investigate how different levels and axes of

astigmatic blur impact on functional measures of visual performance. In order to

achieve this aim three studies were conducted. These studies, along with their

specific aims are described below.

Study 1. Impact of simulated astigmatic refractive blur on functional measures

of visual performance (distance visual acuity, near visual acuity and reading

rate)

The aim of Study 1 was to investigate how uncorrected astigmatism, in terms of both

power and axis, impacts upon distance and near visual acuity and reading

performance. This study extends previous work by testing at lower levels of

astigmatism and also testing at a range of axes of astigmatism.

Hypothesis 1: That the power and axis of astigmatism will influence distance and

near visual acuity and reading rates.

Study 2. Impact of simulated astigmatic refractive blur on tests involving both

visual and cognitive input

The aim of Study 2 was to investigate how uncorrected astigmatism, both in terms of

the power of the cylinder and its axis, impacts upon performance of tasks involving

the integration of both visual and cognitive skills including the Developmental Eye

Movement Test (DEM), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), and Trail Making

Tests A and B.

Page 17: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

3

Hypothesis 2: That the power and axis of astigmatism will impact upon performance

of tasks involving the integration of both visual and cognitive skills, including the

DEM test, DSST, and Trail Making Tests A and B.

Study 3. Impact of simulated astigmatism on functional measures of visual

performance using Chinese characters (near visual acuity and reading rate)

The aim of Study 3 was to investigate the impact of simulated astigmatism of

different powers and axes on near visual acuity and reading rates using Chinese

characters, which are hypothesised to present a higher level of visual and cognitive

effort than the standard English letter charts employed in Experiments 1 and 2 of this

thesis. The differential impact of astigmatism on reading rates measured using

Chinese characters was compared to reading rates with English characters measured

in Study 1.

Hypothesis 3: That the power and axis of astigmatism will have an influence near

visual acuity and reading rate using charts based on Chinese characters.

Hypothesis 4: That there will be a difference in the effects of the power and axis of

astigmatism on near visual acuity and reading rate using charts based on English

versus Chinese characters, with the effects being greater for Chinese characters.

1.3 Thesis outline

This thesis is structured as follows:

Chapter 1 briefly summarises the research background, the overall research aims as

well as specific aims of each of the three experiments, and provides an outline of

each of the chapters.

Chapter 2 includes a review of the literature and provides an overview of

astigmatism and its impact on visual function. Topics include the optics of the eye,

Page 18: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

4

an overview of astigmatism, the impact of refractive error on quality of life, reading

and driving performance, the impact of impaired vision on tasks involving cognitive

skills, the impact of astigmatism on visual acuity and reading performance and

testing visual acuity and reading with Chinese characters. The overall rationale of the

research is also provided.

Chapter 3 describes the study design, research methodology and results for Study 1

which involved investigation of the impact of different powers and axes of

astigmatism on binocular distance visual acuity, binocular near visual acuity, and

binocular reading performance for six different font sizes. Participants included those

for whom English is their first spoken and written language.

The powers of astigmatism included in the experiments described in Chapters 3, 4

and 5 included: - 0.25 DS / 0.50 DC x 90, - 0.50 DS / 1.00 DC x 90 and - 1.00 DS /

2.00 DC x 90. To test the effect of the axis of astigmatism in all three experiments,

the following powers and axes of astigmatism were included; - 1.00 DS / 2.00 DC x

90, - 1.00 DS / 2.00 DC x180 and - 1.00 DS / 2.00 DC x 135 (right eye) and 45 (left

eye). The mean spherical equivalent was maintained as plano for all of the simulated

astigmatic conditions in the three experiments. Spherical blur control conditions were

also used to measure distance visual acuity in Study 1 only, including: + 0.25 DS, +

0.50 DS and + 1.00 DS, in order to match the best sphere of the three cylinder

powers.

The following two chapters present the research examining the impact of

astigmatism on the binocular performance of tasks that involve vision and cognitive

skills such as the DEM, DSST, and Trail Making A and B Tests (Chapter 4), and the

impact of astigmatism on binocular near visual acuity and binocular reading

performance involving Chinese symbols for Chinese speaking participants (Chapter

5). Chapter 5 also includes a comparison between the percentage reductions in

reading rates for English and Chinese characters (collected in Experiments 1 and 3

respectively).

Page 19: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

5

Finally, Chapter 6 presents an overview and summary of the major findings of this

thesis. Implications for further research are also presented in this chapter.

Page 20: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

6

Chapter 2: Literature Review

This chapter provides a review of relevant literature regarding uncorrected refractive

errors, especially astigmatism, and how they impact upon vision and functional

performance tasks, such as driving and reading, in order to provide a clear framework

for the design of the experiments described in this thesis.

2.1 Optics of the eye

The optical properties of the human eye determine the focus of light on the retina and

the optical quality of the retinal image. Any mismatch between the optical power of

the ocular components and the axial length of the eye results in optical defocus and a

blurred retinal image. The ability of the eye to resolve an image on the retina is

determined by the optical quality of that image, the resolution of the photoreceptors

and the subsequent neural mechanisms that process the visual information.

Optical focusing errors can be expressed in a number of ways. In terms of wavefront

optics, the aberrations of the eye are classified as lower order (e.g., prism, defocus,

astigmatism) and higher order (e.g., coma, spherical aberration) (Figure 2-1). The

clinical correction of refractive errors is normally limited to correction of the spherical

and astigmatic components, although the correction of higher orders is possible with

spectacle lenses, contact lenses, refractive surgery or adaptive optics (Dai, 2008).

Figure 2-1 – Total wavefront aberrations (left), sphero-cylinder components (centre) and higher order aberration components (right). Total equals the sum of the sphero-cylinder and higher order components; colours at the red end of the spectrum indicate higher dioptric powers.

Page 21: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

7

Spherical (defocus) errors result from the total power of the refracting surfaces

focusing light in front of the retina (myopia) or behind the retina (hyperopia). While

these spherical refractive errors can result from excessive power in the cornea or

crystalline lens, they typically arise because the axial length of the eye is either too

long (myopia) or too short (hyperopia) in comparison to the optical power of that

particular eye (Deller et al., 1947). The following sections will deal specifically with

the optical characteristics of astigmatism of the human eye.

2.2 Astigmatism

Astigmatism describes the refractive condition where parallel rays of light entering

the eye are focused at two distinct focal lines that are perpendicular to one another,

rather than at a single focal point. The majority of astigmatism of the eye typically

occurs as a result of differences in the anterior corneal surface curvature of the two

principal meridians, while the other refractive surfaces of the eye, including the

posterior cornea, and the front and back surfaces of the lens also contribute to the total

astigmatism. Because of the greater refractive index difference at the anterior corneal

surface (n=1air to n=1.376cornea) compared to that at the posterior corneal surface

(n =1.376cornea to n=1.336aqueous) (Figure 2-2), the posterior corneal surface contributes

less astigmatism, even if the difference in radius of curvature between the two

principal meridians is identical on the front and back surfaces. The astigmatism of the

crystalline lens can arise from a range of factors including toric surface curvatures of

the front and back surfaces of the lens, decentration or tilting of the lens with respect

to the line of sight, or potentially due to asymmetric refractive index distribution

across the lens. The lens has a gradient refractive index which is higher in the nucleus

(about n=1.41), decreasing towards the cortex (about n=1.35) (Pierscionek et al.,

1989).

Page 22: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

8

The astigmatism induced by the anterior cornea is often known as corneal

astigmatism, while the astigmatism induced by the other refractive surfaces is known

as internal astigmatism. The astigmatism measured by keratometers is often called

corneal astigmatism instead of anterior corneal astigmatism, because the keratometer

has historically been calibrated with a refractive index of 1.3375, instead of the true

refractive index of the cornea (n=1.376) (Phillips et al., 1997). By using a lower

refractive index for the keratometer calibration, this reduces the anterior corneal

surface power by about 10%, assuming that the posterior cornea will contribute -10%

of the anterior corneal power (e.g., if anterior is + 2.00 DC x 90, then posterior is

assumed to be - 0.20 DC x 90 or + 0.20 DC x 180).

The total ocular astigmatism is the combination of anterior corneal and internal

astigmatism and the difference between the total and anterior corneal astigmatism is

sometimes also referred to as residual astigmatism. The approximate ratio of the

contribution of anterior corneal and internal astigmatism to the total astigmatism is

defined by Javal’s rule (Grosvenor, 1978):

Figure 2-2 – Schematic representation of the eye’s refracting surfaces; the front and back surfaces of the cornea and the front and back surfaces of the crystalline lens. GRIN is gradient refractive index.

Page 23: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

9

Total astigmatism (DC) = 1.25 x anterior corneal astigmatism (DC) + 0.50 DC ATR

This rule is based on the probability that the posterior cornea only contributes a small

amount of ATR astigmatism and the rule does not apply for cases of oblique

astigmatism. The posterior corneal surface in adults contributes astigmatic refractive

power of about 0.30 D ATR (Ho et al. 2009). Grosvenor et al. (1988) revisited this

relationship between corneal and total astigmatism and suggested a simplified version

of Javal’s rule. The slope of the regression was found to be close to 1, and the y

intercept close to + 0.50 (i.e., + 0.50 DC ATR). Keller et al. (1996) studied the

relationship between corneal and total astigmatism using videokeratoscopy across a

range of entrance pupil sizes from 2 to 7 mm and compared this with subjective

refraction for the same entrance pupil sizes. The linear regressions were similar for

all pupil sizes and were consistent with the simplified Javal’s rule described by

Grosvenor, et al. (1988).

The axis of astigmatism is typically classified as with-the-rule (WTR) astigmatism,

against-the-rule (ATR) astigmatism or oblique (OBL) astigmatism. In WTR the minus

correcting cylinder axis is near to horizontal (between 150 and 30 degrees) (Baldwin

et al., 1981; Hashemi, et al., 2011). This is typically due to the vertical meridian of the

cornea being steeper than the horizontal meridian (Hayashi et al., 1995) (Figure 2-3).

In ATR astigmatism the minus correcting cylindrical axis is near to vertical (60 to 120

degrees) and it typically occurs because the steepest meridian of the anterior corneal

surface is near to the horizontal axis (Hayashi, et al., 1995; Chen, Zuo, et al., 2013).

In OBL astigmatism the minus correcting cylinder axes are between the WTR and

ATR axes (30 to 60 and 120 to 150 degrees) (Bennett, 1984; Saunders, 1986) and it is

the least common form of astigmatism (Satterfield, 1989).

Page 24: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

10

2.2.1 Symmetry of astigmatism between right and left eyes

McKendrick and Brennan (1996) reported that the mean value of the cylindrical

power and axis were similar between the right and left eyes of patients within their

cohort, but there was no distinct predominance of axes showing direct or mirror

symmetry. Studies by Dunne et al. (1994) and Garcia et al. (2003) have found

evidence for mirror symmetry of axes between eyes. In a more recent study,

Guggenheim et al. (2008) also provided evidence that the axes of astigmatism in

fellow eyes exhibit mirror rather than direct symmetry.

The following sections provide an overview of the association between astigmatism

and other refractive errors, the influence of genetics on the development of

astigmatism and the changes in astigmatism that occur with ageing.

2.2.2 Association between spherical refractive error and astigmatism

In a study of the wavefront aberrations of the eye, Porter et al. (2001) reported that

astigmatism (≥ 0.25 D) exists in 73% of adult human eyes with myopic refractive

error. Both spherical and astigmatic refractive errors are also often found together in

children (Fan, et al., 2004; Lai et al., 2010), with strong evidence showing an

association between the presence of astigmatic and spherical refractive errors (Fulton

Figure 2-3 – With-the-rule astigmatism (left) has the steeper corneal meridian along the vertical meridian (90 ± 30 degrees), oblique astigmatism (centre) has the steeper corneal meridian between (30 to 60 and 120 to 150 degrees, while against-the rule astigmatism (right) has the steeper corneal meridian along the horizontal meridian (180 ± 30 degrees); colours at the red end of the spectrum indicate steeper curvatures.

Page 25: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

11

et al., 1982; Goss et al., 1990; Gwiazda et al., 2000; Farbrother et al., 2004; Heidary et

al., 2005). The relationship between astigmatism and myopia was studied by

Gwiazda, et al. (2000) in a longitudinal study from 1974 to 2000 which followed 245

participants for up to 23 years. Their results showed that infantile astigmatism was

associated with increased astigmatism and myopia during the school years, and that

ATR astigmatism was significantly associated with higher levels of myopia after the

age of seven.

Farbrother, et al. (2004) also noted an association between the development of myopia

and astigmatism in a large study of 90,884 subjects. They showed a strong association

between the astigmatic axis (especially WTR astigmatism) and high levels of

spherical refractive error (both hyperopia and myopia). They also reported that

subjects with low levels of ametropia, especially myopic subjects (21-30 years of

age), were more likely to exhibit ATR astigmatism. In a later study (Heidary, et al.,

2005), found a high prevalence of astigmatism within a population (217 participants)

that had high levels of myopia and the severity of myopia was found to be associated

with astigmatism, which was predominantly WTR. A number of studies have also

found an association between the presence of astigmatism and anisometropia

(Yamashita et al., 1999; Qin et al., 2005; Linke et al., 2011). However, some studies

have failed to find n association between astigmatism and the presence of myopia

(Pärssinen, 1991) or myopia progression (Goss, et al., 1990).

A role for astigmatism in the development of spherical refractive error has been

proposed by various authors (Howland, 1982; Gwiazda, et al., 2000). Gwiazda, et al.

(2000) noted that ATR astigmatism in infants was associated with a higher prevalence

of myopia and astigmatism during childhood and suggested that astigmatism interacts

with the emmetropisation process in the developing eye. Buehren et al. (2007)

analysed the optical changes in the cornea due to eyelid pressure following reading

and proposed that hyperopic defocus and ATR astigmatism would provide the best

retinal image and that this subsequently could promote the eye to become myopic

with ATR astigmatism. Vincent et al. (2013) also examined the optical changes

occurring in the cornea after reading in the fellow eyes of anisomyopes. Greater levels

Page 26: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

12

of ATR were noted in the more myopic eye and this led to the proposal by these

authors that astigmatism may influence eye growth in anisomyopia.

2.2.3 The influence of genetics on astigmatism

The development of astigmatism also appears to be influenced by genetic factors.

Wixson (1965) demonstrated that the corneal powers of children were influenced by

the corneal power of their parents’ eyes, while Clementi et al. (1998) analysed

familial trends in corneal astigmatism and found that when families with high levels

of astigmatism are included, modelling suggests a degree of heritability of

astigmatism.

Studies of twins have compared monozygotic and dizygotic twins to examine the

effect of genetics on astigmatic refractive errors. However the results have been

ambiguous. Teikari and O’Donnell (1989) and Valluri et al. (1999) found no

substantial differences in astigmatism between monozygotic and dizygotic twins.

However, Teikari et al. (1989) found more similarity in the axes of astigmatism in

monozygotic twins compared to dizygotic twins. Hammond et al. (2001) studied the

refractive error similarity of over 500 twin pairs and found some heritability of

astigmatism, but less than that for spherical refractive errors. In recent genome-wide

association studies, various potential candidate genes associated with astigmatism

development have been identified (Fan et al., 2011; Han et al., 2011; Lopes et al.,

2013)

2.2.4 Prevalence and changes of astigmatism with age

The prevalence of astigmatism, along with its power and axis, are reported to change

across the lifespan. Infants are reported to show a high prevalence of astigmatism

during the first year of life (Atkinson et al., 1980), which has been shown to reduce

over the subsequent years up until school age (Dobson et al., 1984; Gwiazda et al.,

1984; Rowland et al., 1985; Abrahamsson et al., 1990; Ehrlich et al., 1997).

Page 27: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

13

Dobson, et al. (1984) examined the data of 281 children aged up to 9.5 years old with

astigmatism (≥ 1.00 D). They found a change of astigmatic axis with age, with those

astigmatic children under 3.5 years old showing predominantly ATR astigmatism,

transitioning to be predominantly WTR astigmatism for those over 5.5 years old. Fan,

et al. (2004) found a higher incidence of astigmatism in Chinese preschool children

than that previously reported, with 55.8 % of school-age students having at least 0.50

D of astigmatism which was predominantly WTR in axis.

The prevalence of astigmatism in adults over 30 years of age was reported to be

24.5% in Handan, China (Liang et al., 2009). In older populations older than 65 years

of age, the prevalence was as high as 74% in Taiwan (Cheng et al., 2003). Ferrer-

Blasco, et al. (2009) noted that the mean level of astigmatism in an adult population

less than 40 years of age was 0.75 DC, and that the average level of astigmatism

increased slightly in older populations up to 70 years of age.

A change is consistently reported in the average astigmatic axis in adults with

increasing age. In general, there tends to be a shift from predominantly WTR towards

ATR astigmatism with increasing age in adults (Anstice, 1971; Saunders, 1986).

Hashemi, et al. (2011) showed that both ATR and OBL astigmatism increased with

age, however, the increase in ATR (45.6%) with age was greater than the increase in

OBL astigmatism (33.7%) with age. These changes in total astigmatic axis seem to be

due to changes in corneal astigmatism, with various studies showing that corneal

astigmatism changes from predominantly WTR in young adults to more oblique axes

and ATR in older adults (Anstice, 1971; Baldwin, et al., 1981; Liu et al., 2011).

In summary, astigmatism is a common refractive error and demonstrates changes in

both magnitude and axis with increasing age. Astigmatism changes from being

predominantly ATR in infants to being predominantly WTR around 5-6 years of age.

For young adults, a low magnitude of astigmatism (0.25 DC) is common. With-the-

rule astigmatism is most common in adults, with OBL astigmatism being the least

prevalent. In adults, corneal astigmatism shows a change from being predominantly

WTR in young adults to more OBL and ATR with increased age.

Page 28: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

14

The following two sections provide an overview of some of the issues associated with

the correction of astigmatism, including adaption of the visual system to the presence

of astigmatism, and the various optical means to correct astigmatism.

2.2.5 Blur adaptation and astigmatism

When exposed to spherical refractive blur, the visual system demonstrates the ability

to partially adapt to the blur, so that visual acuity improves over time in the presence

of blur (Pesudovs et al., 1993; Mon-Williams et al., 1998). The time course of this

adaptation is rapid and plateaus after around 6 minutes (Khan et al. 2013). A similar

phenomenon has been shown to occur with astigmatic blur (Sawides et al., 2010;

Ohlendorf et al., 2011; Vinas et al., 2012; Kompaniez et al., 2013; Vinas et al., 2013).

This process of adaptation appears to be sensitive to both the power and the axis of

the astigmatism (Sawides, et al., 2010; Ohlendorf, et al., 2011), with larger amounts

of astigmatic blur resulting in greater levels of adaptation, and adaptation to

astigmatic blur along the vertical meridian resulting in clear images appearing blurred

along the horizontal meridian. The short-term impact of astigmatic blur also appears

to be lessened when the axis of the induced astigmatism matches the natural axis of

astigmatism and is worse when the axis is perpendicular (Vinas, et al., 2013).

2.2.6 The correction of astigmatism

The optical correction of astigmatism can take many forms including spectacles,

contact lenses and refractive surgery. Lenses to correct astigmatism are described as

cylindrical or toric and the axes of the maximum and minimum powers of the

cylindrical lens are orthogonal. The axes of the cylindrical lens are aligned with the

axes of the astigmatism of the eye to provide optical correction.

In spectacle correction, the axis of the cylindrical lens is appropriately aligned in the

spectacle frame to match the eye’s astigmatism. Contact lens correction is more

complex. Soft toric contact lenses require some form of stabilisation in the lens to

maintain the correct axis of the cylindrical lens power. Because soft toric contact

lenses are more difficult to fit, are available only in limited powers and axes of

Page 29: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

15

cylinder and are more expensive than soft spherical contact lenses, low levels of

astigmatism (<1.00 DC) are often left uncorrected in soft contact lens wearers

(Richdale et al 2007). Any unwanted rotation of the soft toric lens whilst in the eye

can also result in residual uncorrected astigmatism (Chamberlain et al 2011).

Rigid spherical contact lenses naturally correct a large proportion (89%) of the

anterior astigmatism through the tear lens that forms beneath the contact lens (Read et

al., 2014). If a spherical rigid contact lens still leaves residual astigmatism, this can

be corrected with a toric rigid lens design (Phillips, et al., 1997). Lower levels of

astigmatism can also be corrected through the emerging procedure of astigmatic

orthokeratology (Chen et al., 2012; Chen, et al., 2013), where the rigid contact lens is

designed to alter the astigmatic shape of the anterior corneal surface. The surgical

correction of astigmatism can also involve reshaping of the corneal surface (e.g.,

LASIK for astigmatism) (Koller et al., 2006; Bababeygy et al., 2008) or the

implantation of intra-ocular toric lenses during cataract surgery (Ahmed et al., 2010;

Holland et al., 2010).

The remainder of this literature review focuses on the functional impacts of

astigmatism. Topics to be considered include refractive error and quality of life,

refractive error and reading, refractive error and driving performance, impact of

impaired vision on tasks involving cognitive skills, the impact of astigmatism on

visual acuity and reading performance, and testing visual acuity and reading with

Chinese characters.

2.3 Refractive error and quality of life

Refractive error has received increasing attention because of its high prevalence in

many regions of the world (Katz et al., 1997; Wu et al., 2001; Xu et al., 2005; Vitale

et al., 2008). The World Health Organization (WHO) and the International Agency for

the Prevention of Blindness (IAPB) have highlighted the fact that uncorrected

refractive error is a leading cause of visual impairment (World Health Organisation,

2007). A number of studies have assessed vision-related quality of life using a

standardised 25-item vision-specific quality of life (VQOL) instrument, the National

Page 30: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

16

Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire (Nirmalan et al., 2005; Suzukamo et al.,

2005). Using this instrument, a measurable impact of correctable visual impairment

on VQOL has been found in various cross-sectional studies. Nirmalan, et al. (2005)

reported that uncorrectable and correctable refractive error both reduced VQOL and

visual acuity. In another study, participants who received optimal correction had

greater improvements in near and distance visual acuity, and VQOL than those who

were inaccurately-corrected, or uncorrected (Coleman, et al., 2006).

Rahi et al. (2008) reported that the vision loss caused by uncorrected refractive error

not only decreased VQOL but also increased the difficulty when performing a range

of vision-related tasks, including reading and driving (Margolis et al., 2002). Previous

researchers have also shown that refractive error significantly impacts on performance

of vision-related daily living activities that relates to the associated decrease in visual

acuity and vision function (Lamoureux et al., 2004). These results suggest that

refractive errors not only impact on visual acuity, but have the potential to also

decrease many aspects of vision-related function and quality of life. The effects of

refractive error on functional tasks including reading and driving are described in the

following sections.

2.4 Refractive error and reading

Reading is an important skill that is used in many daily tasks. The impact of refractive

error on reading performance depends to some extent on chart design. Distance and

near visual acuity charts can make use of isolated letters and lines of letters, while

isolated words and continuous text are also used in near visual acuity charts (Bailey et

al., 2013). However, these forms of charts have different levels of legibility and

require different cognitive abilities for optimal performance. Bailey, et al. (2013)

emphasised that it is better to assess near visual acuity using letter-by-letter scoring,

while unrelated words can be used as an indicator of near reading ability. Legge et al.

(1989) developed the MNREAD charts based on continuous text with a logarithmic

size progression that can also be used for assessing reading speed. More recently,

Wolffsohn and Cochrane (2000) introduced another reading chart, known as the

Practical Near Acuity Chart (PNAC), which used a single paragraph of three related

Page 31: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

17

words on each line (3-letter word, 4-letter word, and 5-letter word). Near acuity

measured with the PNAC and Bailey-Lovie charts was highly correlated (r = 0.97),

although the time taken to measure near acuity with the PNAC was faster than with

Bailey-Lovie charts.

West et al. (2002) demonstrated that the visual acuity loss caused by uncorrected

refractive error contributed to deficits in performance of vision-related daily tasks

such as reading and near work. Uncorrected refractive errors have been shown to have

a significant negative effect on reading performance (Taylor, 2000). Chung et al.

(2007) using the MNREAD Acuity Charts also found a relationship between

increasing amounts of spherical blur and reduced maximum reading rates, threshold

print size and reading acuity.

2.5 Refractive error and driving

The efficient integration and timely processing of visual information from various

sources are two important components of an individual’s ability to complete daily

tasks. Wood et al. (2009) suggested that visual impairment degraded visual processing

speeds and impacted not only on the ability to undertake visual tasks, but also on the

ability to apprehend and quickly process sensory information. For example, drivers

with visual blur are likely to have difficulty reading highway signs in the distance

(such as signs of speed limit, stop, and exit) which is important for safe vehicle

control decisions (Schieber, 2004). Kline et al. (1999) reported that night-time sign

recognition was significantly reduced when visual acuity was degraded, and the

ability to detect road hazards and avoid them was significantly worse when

participants were driving with visual impairment. However there is only a weak

association between reduced visual acuity and driving crash risk (Owsley et al., 1999;

Owsley et al., 2001; Owsley et al., 2010).

Blurred vision due to spherical refractive errors has been found to impair a number of

aspects of driving performance, including total driving time, hazard avoidance and

sign recognition, while lane-keeping and gap judgements appear to be relatively

unaffected by blur (Higgins, et al., 1998). Spherical blur as low as 0.50 DS reduced

Page 32: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

18

overall measures of driving performance on a closed road at night but not during the

daytime (Wood, et al., 2014), as well as the ability to detect and recognise pedestrians

at night (Wood et al., 2012). Chu et al. (2010) also showed that bifocal contact lenses

significantly reduced many aspects of night-time driving performance on a closed

road circuit.

2.6 Impact of impaired vision on tasks involving cognitive skills

Functional vision can be considered to involve the integration of information

requiring both visual and cognitive skills. Dickinson et al. (1991) noted that reading

rate should not be the sole measure of the capacity for reading and suggested using

tests that require additional cognitive processing ability to better reflect performance

on everyday tasks. The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) and the Trail Making

A and B Tests (TMT) are standardised tests that require integration of visual ability,

including visual search as well as cognitive skills. A number of studies have reported

that performance on these types of tests is impacted by various forms of visual

impairment.

Wood, et al. (2009) reported that visual impairment comprising reduced contrast

sensitivity that was simulated using specially designed filters, significantly increased

the time taken to complete the DSST and the TMT A and B in young adults. These

results were also shown to extend to older adults, where the time taken to complete

selected cognitive tasks that involved speed of visual processing was significantly

impaired when visual input was degraded using filters that reduced contrast sensitivity

in a similar manner to that of early cataracts (See et al., 2010; Wood, et al., 2012).

Similarly, Hunt et al. (2010) reported that speed of visual information processing was

slowed for older adults when the appearance of paper-based versions of the DSST

charts was blurred, rather than blurred using optical lenses or filters as was the case in

other studies. The productivity of computer users was found to be negatively impacted

by impaired vision induced by 2.00 DC of imposed astigmatism (Daum et al., 2004)

and 1.50 DC astigmatism (Sheedy et al., 1990), with the time to complete tasks

increased by up to 29% and 12%, respectively. However, the impact of astigmatic

blur on the performance of laboratory-based or everyday functional tasks involving

Page 33: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

19

the integration of vision and cognitive skills has received relatively little attention,

with the majority of literature focusing on the impact of astigmatic blur on

standardised measures of visual acuity as described in the following sections.

2.7 Impact of astigmatism on visual acuity

The effects of astigmatism on visual function are determined by the meridional blur

induced by the condition. The focal planes of astigmatism are orthogonal and in the

case of ATR (negative correction cylinder axis x 90 degrees), the more myopic

meridian tends to blur vertical detail, whereas in WTR (negative correcting cylinder

axis x 180 degrees) the more myopic meridian blurs the horizontal detail (Figure 3-4).

The magnitude of retinal blur depends on a range of factors such as pupil size and the

interaction with other optical aberrations of the eye, such as spherical error and higher

order aberrations.

The location of the astigmatic focal planes with respect to the retina will be also be

influenced by the eye’s accommodation response. Astigmatism can be present in a

variety of forms, for example: as a crossed cylinder of +0.50DS/-1.00 DC x 90, as a

simple hyperopic astigmatism of -1.00 DC x 90, or as simple myopic astigmatism of

+1.00 DC x 180. In the case of crossed cylinder astigmatism (+0.50DS/-1.00 DC x

90), the circle of least confusion (as defined by geometrical optics) is placed at the

retina. In the case of simple hyperopic (-1.00 DC x 90) or myopic (+1.00 DC x 180)

astigmatism, one focal plane is at the retina while the other focal plane is behind

(hyperopic) or in front (myopic) of the retina. In an eye with active accommodation,

the level of accommodation response and the target vergence will therefore determine

the relative locations of the orthogonal astigmatic focal planes with respect to the

Figure 2-4 – The effect of different axes of astigmatism on image quality. In myopic ATR the vertical detail is blurred, in myopic OBL the oblique detail is blurred and in myopic WTR the horizontal detail is blurred.

Page 34: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

20

retina. Stark et al (2003) provided a detailed summary of these possible focussing

strategies and they included focusing using the least accommodative response, the

most accommodative response, the circle of least confusion, other intermediate focal

positions within the astigmatic interval, the more ‘‘vertical’’ focal detail, the more

‘‘horizontal’’ focal detail, the response closest to the resting or tonic level of

accommodation, or dynamic/fluctuating accommodation to periodically optimize each

focal plane.

Atchison et al. (2009) used adaptive optics to test the subjective limits of the

perception of spherical and crossed cylinder astigmatic blur. They found that the

average astigmatic blur detection was about 90% of the dioptric value of spherical

blur (for defocus considered to be “just noticeable” this was 0.18 DS and 1.01 DS for

“bothersome” blur). Both Applegate et al. (2003) and Rocha et al. (2007) also found

that the blur detection threshold of crossed cylinder astigmatism was similar to that

found for defocus. However, these authors also noted variability between subjects in

the expected improvement associated with correcting low amounts of natural

astigmatism (< 0.50 DC) and concluded that under clinical conditions, there was little

benefit in correcting these small amounts of astigmatism.

Watanabe et al. (2013) examined the effect of hyperopic astigmatism with cycloplegia

on visual acuity in 0.50 DC steps, from 0.50 to 2.50 DC, and reported that visual

acuity was significantly negatively correlated with increasing cylinder power. Pesala

et al. (2014) measured the impact of uncorrected astigmatism on visual acuity in

pseudophakic eyes in 0.50 DC steps up to ± 2.50 DC, and reported that both

increasing negative and positive astigmatism had a similar linear effect in decreasing

distance visual acuity. Near visual acuity has the potential to be improved with

positive astigmatism, since the myopic astigmatic focal plane provides some

improvement in near image quality along one meridian (Casagrande, et al., 2013).

Most studies that have investigated the impact of the axis of astigmatism on visual

acuity have reported that ATR astigmatism causes a greater loss of visual acuity than

WTR or OBL astigmatism. Studies by both Wolffsohn et al. (2011) and Wills et al.

Page 35: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

21

(2012) noted that simulated ATR crossed cylinder astigmatism had more impact on

visual acuity and reading performance than WTR crossed cylinder astigmatism,

particularly for smaller print size targets. Atchison, et al. (2009) also reported higher

levels of sensitivity to the detection of blur with ATR than WTR crossed cylinder

astigmatism. In addition, Yamamoto et al. (2014) reported that patients who had ATR

astigmatism following monofocal intraocular lens implantation had significantly

worse visual acuity that those with WTR astigmatism. Importantly, Vinas, et al.

(2013) noted that the impact of astigmatic blur was dependent on the orientation of

induced astigmatism and that this effect was significantly influenced by the eyes

natural axis of astigmatism.

Of relevance to the impact of astigmatism on visual acuity are the prescribing patterns

of optometrists with respect to astigmatic correction. O’Leary and Evans (2003) found

that the majority of optometrists would correct astigmatism at levels of 0.75 DC and

above. In a study comparing spherical and toric soft contact lenses, Richdale et al.

(2007) concluded that the correction of 0.75 DC to 1.00 DC astigmatism produced a

clinically significant improvement in visual acuity. Hayashi et al. (2010) also

concluded that for patients with intraocular lens implants (IOL) following cataract

extraction, it was clinically advisable to consider toric IOLs when the level of

astigmatism reaches 1.00 DC.

In summary, both the level of astigmatism and the orientation of the astigmatic axis

impact on distance and near visual acuity. Most clinical studies suggest that a

threshold of the order of 0.75 DC requires correction to improve visual acuity,

however, astigmatic blur can be detected at much lower values.

2.8 Impact of astigmatism on reading performance

As stated in the previous sections, functional vision testing is an important method to

define visual ability along with standard visual acuity testing (Kaido et al., 2006).

Yamaguchi et al. (2011) suggested that a functional vision assessment should involve

daily tasks, such as reading. Dickinson, et al. (1991) imposed simulated crossed

cylinder astigmatism and noted that reading rates were impaired by astigmatism,

Page 36: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

22

while Whittaker and Lovie-Kitchin (1993) also found that reading tasks became more

difficult in the presence of uncorrected astigmatism.

In a detailed study of the effects of astigmatism on reading, Wills, et al. (2012) found

that simulated crossed cylinder astigmatic blur reduced reading rates compared with

baseline for all of the tested levels and axes of astigmatism (1.00 and 2.00 DC, ATR

and WTR astigmatism). They noted that reading rate for N16 print size was decreased

by 10% with 2.00 DC WTR astigmatism, while for smaller print sizes of N12 and

N10, the reduction of reading rate was up to 24% with 1.00 DC ATR astigmatism and

both ATR and WTR astigmatism at 2.00 DC. They noted that ATR astigmatism had a

greater impact on reading fluency than WTR astigmatism, even for small amounts of

astigmatism. Wolffsohn et al. (2011) also reported that reading speeds were decreased

with increasing levels of uncorrected crossed cylinder astigmatism, but only for

powers of astigmatism of 3.00 DC or greater. They also showed that uncorrected

crossed cylinder ATR astigmatism resulted in a greater reduction in reading rate

compared to OBL astigmatism.

Kobashi et al. (2012) investigated the effects of ATR, WTR, and OBL myopic

astigmatism at 1.00, 2.00 and 3.00 DC with cycloplegia and found that reading

performance decreased significantly with increasing levels of astigmatism. In

addition, they reported that the axis of each level of astigmatism impacted on reading

acuity and the maximum reading speed, with performance with OBL astigmatism

being worse than that with ATR and WTR astigmatism. However, there was no

significant difference in reading performance between ATR and WTR astigmatism.

Casagrande, et al. (2013) used cycloplegia and 0 DC, -0.75 DC, and -1.50 DC of ATR

and WTR astigmatism and found that increasing astigmatism resulted in reduced

reading acuity, with WTR astigmatism causing a greater reduction in reading acuity

and slower reading speeds compared with ATR astigmatism. Table 2-1 summarises

previous studies that have compared the effect of the axis of astigmatism on visual

performance. Overall, most studies have demonstrated that ATR causes a greater loss

of performance than WTR or OBL astigmatism, although this is not always the case.

Page 37: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

23

(ATR=against-the-rule astigmatism; WTR = with-the-rule astigmatism; OBL=oblique astigmatism; MNRead-J = Japanese version of the Minnesota Low Vision Reading Test acuity charts)

2.9 Testing visual acuity and reading with Chinese characters

Only a limited number of studies have used visual acuity charts that have included

Chinese characters to determine distance or near visual performance (Woo et al.,

Author (Date)

Subjects (n)

Age mean +/- SD

Visual performance

Testing chart

Axis

Trindade et al. (1997)

20

71.75

± 7.47

Distance VA

Near VA

Snellen

Jaeger

WTR = ATR

ATR > WTR

Remón et al. (2006) 4

20 Distance VA Letters or Landolt C

ATR = WTR = OBL

Atchison, et al. (2009)

6 31

± 10 Blur limit

Letters ATR > WTR

Wolffsohn, et al. (2011) 21

58.9

± 2.8

Distance VA

Near VA

logMAR

logMAR

ATR > WTR = OBL

ATR > WTR = OBL

Reading rate Standardised

texts ATR > OBL

Wills, et al. (2012) 30

21.7

± 3.4

Distance VA

Near VA

Bailey-Lovie

Bailey-Lovie

ATR > WTR

ATR > WTR

Reading rate Bailey-Lovie ATR > WTR

Kobashi, et al. (2012) 38

44.9

± 19.5

Distance VA Landolt-C OBL > WTR = ATR

Reading MNRead J OBL > WTR = ATR

Casagrande, et al. (2013)

23

26

± 6

Near VA

Reading speed

Snellen

Standardised sentences

WTR > ATR

WTR > ATR

Table 2-1 – Summary of studies comparing the effect of astigmatic axis orientation on visual

performance

Page 38: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

24

1980; Johnston, 1985), despite the fact that a large proportion of the world’s

population consists of readers of Chinese. While English characters are mostly

composed of 3-5 strokes (depending on the font type), Chinese characters can range in

complexity from 2-18 strokes per character (Zhang et al., 2007). A recent study

explored whether reading visual span (number of characters seen in each fixation) is

affected by the level of complexity of Chinese text (Wang et al., 2014). These authors

demonstrated in a group of bilingual (English and Chinese) subjects that as the

complexity of the Chinese characters increased, the visual span was found to get

smaller, suggesting that reading rate would also be likely to diminish.

Zhang et al. (2009) also explored the effect of Chinese character complexity by

investigating the legibility of Chinese characters of different levels of spatial

complexity both with and without the spherical blur of up to + 2.00 DS. Their study

demonstrated that Chinese characters with more strokes resulted in lower levels of

visual acuity, but that the introduction of blur caused a similar rate of loss of visual

acuity with Chinese and Snellen E optotypes. The link between reading rates, blur and

Chinese character complexity is clearly a complex topic and importantly, the

influence of astigmatic blur of different levels of power and axes on visual acuity and

reading performance with Chinese text has not been investigated.

2.10 Rationale

In summary, a number of researchers have shown that uncorrected spherical refractive

error and simulated visual impairment result in problems in the performance of a

number of vision-related daily living tasks and activities (Rahi, et al., 2008; Wood et

al., 2010). These tasks include, for example, reading performance and performance on

a variety of cognitive tests (Taylor, 2000; Wood, et al., 2009; Wood, et al., 2010).

Importantly, there has been only limited research investigating the effects of blurred

vision resulting from uncorrected astigmatic refractive error on functional measures

such as reading performance (Villegas et al., 2014). While the above research findings

clearly highlight the important relationship between visual blur and performance

measures that require integration of vision and cognition, the issue of uncorrected

astigmatism has rarely been considered; this is important given the relatively high

Page 39: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

25

prevalence of astigmatism of various axes in both children as well as in adult

populations.

Studies that have explored the impact of spherical blur on visual performance have

demonstrated that as the magnitude of spherical blur increases, visual performance

decreases. However, the impact of astigmatic blur on visual function is much less well

understood. Astigmatic blur has two important features, power and axis. While

increasing the power of astigmatism has been shown to cause greater losses of vision,

the impact of axis is less consistent and potentially depends on the interaction between

the blurred meridian (axis) of the retinal image and the orientation of the salient

features within the scene or visual task being undertaken. Orientation dependency of

astigmatism is a key factor in understanding its impact on the performance of visual

tasks.

In Experiment 1, the influence of a comprehensive range of astigmatic powers and

axes on visual performance for distance visual acuity, near visual acuity and reading

rate for a range of font sizes was investigated. The effect of equivalent levels of

spherical blur on distance visual acuity were also studied and compared to that

resulting from astigmatic blur.

In Experiment 2, the effect of astigmatic blur was investigated for a range of tests that

involve visual resolution, eye movements and cognitive demands including visual

processing speeds. This second experiment investigated how astigmatic blur impacts

on the performance of those visually-related tasks that reflect the way in which we

make use of vision under more complex everyday situations.

Finally, in Experiment 3 the impact of astigmatism on visual acuity and reading rates

with Chinese characters was investigated and compared to the effect of astigmatism

on English text. Chinese text was chosen since it differs from English text in that it

contains more spatially complex characteristics, with details at a variety of angles

within a symbol. Given the orientation dependent nature of astigmatic blur, it was

Page 40: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

26

considered that Chinese symbols may present a particular challenge in the presence of

astigmatic blur.

Page 41: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

27

Chapter 3: Impact of simulated astigmatic refractive blur on visual acuity and reading 3.1 Introduction

It is well known that astigmatism influences both distance and near visual acuity

(Trindade, et al., 1997; Kobashi, et al., 2012). Studies have shown an approximately

linear reduction in distance visual acuity with increasing cylindrical power, with a loss

of 1-2 lines of logMAR distance visual acuity per dioptre of cylindrical power,

although the method of inducing astigmatic error varied amongst the studies (Remón,

et al., 2006; Atchison et al., 2011; Watanabe, et al., 2013). The impact of astigmatism

on visual acuity has also been shown to depend on the axis of astigmatism. Visual

acuity has been shown to be reduced to a greater extent when the axis of crossed

cylinder astigmatism is either ATR (Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Wills, et al., 2012) and/or

OBL (Wolffsohn, et al., 2011). However, other authors have failed to find a difference

in the effect of different crossed cylinder astigmatic axis on visual acuity (Ohlendorf

et al 2011).

While there is significant evidence showing that astigmatic blur reduces visual acuity,

there has been only limited research investigating the effect of astigmatism on

functional visual performance. Wills, et al. (2012) reported that reading rate was

reduced by crossed cylinder astigmatism, depending on the power and axis of

astigmatism. Similarly, Wolffsohn, et al. (2011) reported that reading speed was

reduced for uncorrected crossed cylinder astigmatism, however, they also reported

that other functional measures including glare sensitivity and performance on a

computer-based simulated driving performance task were unaffected by astigmatism.

The aim of this study was to investigate how simulated crossed cylinder astigmatism,

in terms of both power and axis, impacts upon distance and near visual acuity and

reading performance. This first study extends previous work by testing at lower levels

of astigmatism and at a range of axes.

Page 42: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

28

3.2 Methods

3.2.1 Participants

Participants included 12 young adults (mean age 23.92 ± 5.49 years; range 18 to 34

years) and included 8 females and 4 males. All participants gave informed consent

and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (Ethics

forms in Appendix 1).

The inclusion criteria included: (1) age between 18-35 years; (2) first spoken and

written language to be English; (3) good general health with no self-reported

neurological illness or cognitive impairment; (4) no history of significant ocular

disease or injury; (5) normal binocular vision function (no history of strabismus or

ocular motility disorders); (6) astigmatism less than or equal to 0.50 DC to minimise

the habitual adaption to uncorrected astigmatism; and (7) best corrected visual acuity

of 0.00 logMAR or better in both eyes.

3.2.2 Screening procedures

All participants underwent a screening procedure to determine whether they met the

study inclusion criteria. The tests included in the vision screening assessment were:

(a) Case history

Each participant was interviewed to obtain information regarding their ocular

history, including whether they had any significant history of ocular trauma or

surgery and their general health (including medication use).

(b) Refractive error

Each participant’s refractive status was determined by an experienced and

qualified optometrist using non-cycloplegic retinoscopy followed by

monocular subjective refraction using a phoropter and standard cross cylinder

procedures and binocular balancing using alternate occlusion.

Page 43: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

29

(c) Best corrected visual acuity

Monocular and binocular visual acuity with the optimal refractive correction

were assessed at both distance and near using a high contrast Early Treatment

Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) chart at 4 m and a Bailey-Lovie near

reading card at 40 cm, following measurement of refractive error. Visual

acuity was scored on a letter by letter basis with each letter corresponding to a

score of -0.02 log units (Bailey et al., 1976).

(d) Stereoacuity

Stereoacuity was assessed using the TNO test (Grafton Optical, Herfordshire),

to ensure that participants had stereoacuity of at least 60 seconds of arc. Each

participant (with the optimal refractive correction in the trial frame) was

presented with the test plates, beginning with the largest disparity and asked to

identify the orientation of the missing sector of a circle in each plate. The

smallest disparity at which both test plates were correctly identified was

recorded.

(e) Accommodation amplitude

Monocular and binocular amplitudes of accommodation were determined

using a N8 size letter on a near visual acuity test chart (Rabbetts & Elliott,

2007). The target was presented at approximately 40 cm and slowly brought

towards the participant, who was asked to report when the letter first appeared

blurred and could not be made clear. The distance from the spectacle plane

(with optimal correction in trial frame) to the blur point was measured and

converted to dioptres. Three measurements were taken and the average of

these measures was calculated. To determine if the participant had sufficient

amplitude of accommodation, the measured accommodative amplitude was

compared to that derived through the following formula: 18.5 - 0.3 (age in

years) (Scheiman et al., 2008).

(f) Near point of convergence

The near point of convergence was assessed using the push up method using a

single 6/9 letter target. The target was brought towards the participant, who

was asked to report when diplopia was first noticed. The distance from the

Page 44: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

30

spectacle plane (with optimal correction in trial frame) to the point where

diplopia was first reported, or a deviation of the eyes was observed by the

examiner, was measured and recorded; (Scheiman & Wick, 2008).

Measurements were conducted three times and the average of these measures

was calculated; a level of ≤ 8 cm was considered normal.

(g) Near dissociated horizontal heterophoria

The near horizontal phoria was assessed using the near Howell-Dwyer phoria

card at 33 cm. A 6 base down (BD) prism lens was held in front of the

participant’s right eye (with the optimal refractive correction in the trial frame)

and the participant asked to identify the number (and colour of the band in

which the number was located) that the top arrow on the card was aligned.

(h) Near horizontal fusional vergence

Both positive and negative fusional vergence at near were measured using the

step vergence technique with a prism bar. For assessing positive fusional

vergence, base out (BO) prism was introduced in front of the participant’s

right eye, and gradually increased from a lower to a higher magnitude until

diplopia was first reported, or the examiner noticed an outward movement of

the eye. The magnitude of the prism was then gradually reduced until single

vision was reported. The same procedure was repeated using base in (BI)

prism to assess negative fusional vergence (Scheiman, et al., 2008). Inclusion

criteria required that the near phoria could be compensated for by the near

fusional vergence ranges, in accordance with Sheard’s criterion for discomfort

in binocular vision. Using Sheard’s criterion, the fusional reserve range needed

to be at least twice the magnitude of the phoria to ensure comfortable

binocular vision (Scheiman, et al., 2008).

3.2.3 Protocol

Visual acuity (distance and near visual acuity) and reading performance (reading

rate), were measured binocularly for six different visual conditions, which included

three different powers of ATR astigmatism and three different axes of 2.00 DC

Page 45: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

31

astigmatism. The mean spherical equivalent was maintained as plano for all of the

simulated astigmatism conditions:

(1) Optimal sphero-cylinder correction in both right and left eyes (baseline control

condition),

(2) - 0.25 DS / + 0.50 DC x 90 (ATR) in both the right and left eyes,

(3) - 0.50 DS / + 1.00 DC x 90 (ATR) in both the right and left eyes,

(4) - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 90 (ATR) in both the right and left eyes,

(5) - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 180 (WTR) in both the right and left eyes,

(6) Right eye: - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 135 (OBL) &

Left eye: - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 45 (OBL).

Distance visual acuity was also measured with an additional three visual conditions

which included levels of spherical blur that were equivalent to the level of blur of the

astigmatic conditions (Atchison, et al., 2011):

(7) + 0.25 DS before both right and left eyes,

(8) + 0.50 DS before both right and left eyes,

(9) + 1.00 DS before both right and left eyes,

The blurring lenses were worn in a trial frame and the inter-pupillary distance was

adjusted for each individual participant. Each participant completed a practice reading

task to ensure that they were familiar with the requirements of the study.

The order of testing of the different visual conditions was randomised between

participants to minimise order and fatigue effects. Prior to testing visual acuity or

reading rate, the participants were required to watch a movie (from a distance of 40

cm) for 10 minutes, in order to allow for blur adaptation to the testing lenses. This

adaptation duration was based on the period required for blur adaptation in previous

research (Khan et al., 2013). A short break (5 minutes) with clear vision was provided

when testing with each visual condition was completed.

Page 46: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

32

The room illumination levels were measured with the IM-2D lux meter (Topcon,

Tokyo) as presented in Table 3-1. The luminance levels of the charts were measured

with a BM-7 luminance colorimeter (Topcon, Tokyo) and are also presented in Table

3-1 along with the Weber contrast levels derived from these chart measurements.

Luminance (of

chart) (cd/m2)

Illumination (lux)

(at chart) Weber contrast (%)

Distance visual acuity

chart 192 650 95

Near visual acuity

and reading rate charts 196 680 92

3.2.4 Distance and near visual acuity

Binocular distance visual acuity was measured using ETDRS visual acuity charts

(Good-Lite Co.) at a working distance of 4 m using a working distance correction of +

0.25 DS. Participants were required to read the letters on each line, starting from the

top line of the ETDRS chart and encouraged to provide their ‘best guess’ for letter

identification if they were unsure of a letter. Termination of testing occurred when

participants made a line of errors. Each correct letter was scored as 0.02 log units.

Three different ETDRS charts were used for testing and the order of charts was

randomised between participants.

Near visual acuity was measured using a series of Bailey-Lovie reading charts which

include 30 unrelated words per page, with words presented from N80 to N5 print size

(Figure 3-1), Testing was conducted at 40 cm, with the charts mounted on a stand to

assist in maintaining a standard working distance and the logMAR acuity values

corrected for this testing distance. Participants were required to read the words on

each line of the chart starting from the top line and reading left to right, and

encouraged to provide their ‘best guess’ for any words they were unsure of and to

Table 3-1 – Luminance and illuminance levels of charts

Page 47: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

33

continue reading until a line of errors was made. Seventeen different reading charts

were available and the order of charts was randomised between subjects.

3.2.5 Reading rate

Reading rate was recorded using a series of Bailey-Lovie reading charts at 40 cm

mounted on a stand as for near visual acuity described above for font sizes in the

range of N20 to N3. Participants were instructed to read aloud the words on the chart

as quickly as possible, reading from left to right from the top of the chart (N20), and

to continue guessing the words until instructed to stop. The investigator instructed the

participant to stop reading when a full line of errors was made and the reading time

was digitally recorded (Sony IC recorder ICD-UX512F, Sony Inc, Japan) and later

analysed. Participants were required to read two different charts under each visual

condition. The use of a number of different Bailey-Lovie reading acuity charts

minimised potential learning effects between tests for different visual conditions. The

reading rate of each participant was calculated and converted from the time taken by

each participant to read the words on each line, into a score of words read per minute

(for each individual line from N20 to N6).

3.2.6 Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using the Statistical Package for the Social

Sciences (SPSS) Version 21.0 (IBM Corp., United States). The outcome measures

were visually inspected with box plots and histograms to confirm approximate normal

distribution of the data. Statistical normality tests were also undertaken using the

Figure 3-1 – Sample of a Bailey-Lovie reading chart.

Page 48: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

34

Shapiro-Wilks test and these confirmed that the distributions were approximately

normal and therefore the use of parametric statistics was appropriate. One-way

repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to examine the influence

of various levels and axes of simulated astigmatism on distance visual acuity, near

visual acuity, and reading rate. Two-way, repeated measures ANOVA were used to

examine the interaction between different levels and axes of simulated astigmatism,

print size, and reading rate. A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically

significant. In the analyses of the data for this Experiment as well as for Experiments

2 and 3, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction for degrees of freedom was used where

the assumptions of sphericity were violated. In addition, given the exploratory nature

of the Experiments described in this thesis, multiple adjustments were not strictly

required and would have been overly conservative, potentially masking important

findings (Perneger, 1998; Bender et al., 2001). Therefore in all of the analyses for

Experiments 1-3, p-values were not adjusted for multiple comparisons; however, the

possibility of chance findings due to type I errors must be considered when

interpreting the results.

3.3 Results

3.3.1 Powers of ATR astigmatism

Table 3-2 shows the group mean values and results of the statistical analyses for the

outcome measures (distance visual acuity, near visual acuity and reading rate) for

three powers of induced ATR astigmatism compared with baseline.

Distance visual acuity was significantly reduced in the presence of simulated

astigmatism (p<0.001) (Table 3-2). As the power of ATR astigmatism increased

distance visual acuity worsened compared to baseline (optimal correction) (Figure

3-2). Post-hoc testing showed that all powers of ATR astigmatism significantly

reduced distance visual acuity compared with the optimally corrected condition

(Table 3-2). Post-hoc testing also indicated that distance visual acuity was

significantly different between the three different cylindrical powers, where visual

acuity significantly worsened as the power of the ATR astigmatism increased.

Page 49: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

35

Figure 3-2 – Group mean distance visual acuity as a function of three different powers of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 50: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

36

Visual Conditions

One-way ANOVA

Measure Baseline

- 0.25 DS / - 0.50 DS / - 1.00 DS /

P + 0.50 DC + 1.00 DC + 2.00 DC df F value

x 90 x 90 x 90

VA (logMAR)

Distance -0.17 -0.11 -0.01 0.24

3, 33 76.68 < 0.001 ± 0.10 ± 0.09 b ± 0.10 a ± 0.10 a

Near -0.07 -0.05 -0.03 0.13

1.57, 17.3 24.42 < 0.001 ± 0.08 ± 0.07 ± 0.06 ± 0.10 a

Reading rate (words/minute)

N20 155.40

± 15.57

148.72 141.27 135.56 1.54, 16.89 46.33 < 0.001

± 13.70 b ± 14.84 a ± 15.57 a

N16 146.20

± 14.52

139.41

± 14.48 b

131.55

± 15.03 a

127.59

± 14.07 a 1.91, 21.02 46.34 < 0.001

N12 135.94

± 13.66

130.75

± 13.28 a

121.69

± 11.49 a

116.04

± 12.85 a 3, 33 57.19 < 0.001

N10 132.69

± 16.03

123.61

± 14.97 a

115.45

± 14.19 a

106.77

± 13.35 a 1.82, 20.01 60.17 < 0.001

N8 123.20

± 15.40

114.08

± 14.36 a

106.26

± 14.13 a

98.34

± 14.63 a 1.55, 17.08 57.08 < 0.001

N6 113.12

± 15.19

103.92

± 16.20 a

97.95

± 15.77 a

81.75

± 21.50 a 1.25, 13.70 25.87 < 0.001

Post-hoc testing of the effects of induced astigmatism versus best-corrected value, (ap < 0.001, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.05)

Table 3-2 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different powers of ATR astigmatism compared to baseline

Page 51: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

37

Near visual acuity was also significantly worsened (p < 0.001) in the presence of

simulated ATR astigmatism (Table 3-2); as the power of astigmatism increased, near

visual acuity worsened compared with the baseline (optimal correction) (Figure 3.3)

Post-hoc testing showed that only the 2.00 DC ATR astigmatism significantly reduced

near visual acuity relative to baseline (p < 0.001), while 0.50 DC and 1.00 DC of

induced ATR astigmatism did not significantly reduce near visual acuity relative to

best-corrected near visual acuity (Table 3-2).

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA (within factors: cylinder power and reading

font size), demonstrated that there was a significant main effect of cylindrical power

(F (1.92, 21.13) = 104.04; p < 0.001) and font size (F (1.77, 19.42) = 73.55; p < 0.001) on

reading rate but the interaction did not quite reach significance (F (2.96, 32.60) = 2.74; p =

0.06). A series of one-way ANOVAs demonstrated that reading rate for all six font

sizes (N20, N16, N12, N10, N8, and N6) significantly decreased reading rate (p <

0.001) compared with baseline (optimal correction) for increasing powers of ATR

astigmatism. The change in reading rate as a function of the power of ATR

Figure 3-3 – Group mean near visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 52: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

38

astigmatism and font size is given in Figure 3-4. Post-hoc testing demonstrated that all

levels of ATR astigmatism significantly reduced reading rates for all font sizes

compared to baseline (Table 3-2).

Comparison of the decrease in distance visual acuity for the three different levels of

spherical blur (0.25 DS, 0.50 DS, and 1.00 DS) with the 3 levels of induced ATR

astigmatism are shown in Figure 3-5 and Table 3-3.

Figure 3-4 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 53: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

39

Visual Conditions

+ 0.25 DS

-0.25 DS /

+ 0.50 DC

x 90

+0.50 DS

-0.50 DS /

+1.00 DC

x 90

+1.00 DS

-1.00DS /

+2.00 DC

x 90

Distance

VA

(logMAR)

-0.11

± 0.12

-0.11

± 0.09

-0.03

± 0.12

-0.02

± 0.10

0.20

± 0.18

0.24

± 0.10

Figure 3-5 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different levels of spherical blur and ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Table 3-3 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) for the effect of three different levels of spherical blur and astigmatism compared with baseline condition

Page 54: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

40

A two way repeated measures ANOVA, (within factors included blur levels (3) and

whether the blur was induced by a spherical or sphero-cylindrical correction (2))

indicated that there was a significant effect of increasing blur (F (1.27,12.40) = 82.54; p <

0.001) but no significant effect of whether the blur was induced by a spherical or

sphero-cylindrical lens (F (1, 11) = 0.42; p = 0.53) and no significant interaction effect

(F (2, 22) = 0.320; p = 0.73).

3.3.2 Axes of astigmatism

Table 3-4 shows the group mean values and results of the statistical analyses for the

outcome measures (distance visual acuity, near visual acuity and reading rate) for

three different axes of induced astigmatism (ATR, WTR, and OBL) with a constant

cylindrical power of 2.00 DC, compared with the best-corrected condition.

Distance visual acuity was reduced for all of the astigmatic axes compared with

baseline (Figure 3-6). While this reduction reached significance (p < 0.001), post-hoc

testing indicated there was no difference in distance visual acuity between the ATR,

WTR and OBL axes.

Figure 3-6 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 55: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

41

Near visual acuity was also reduced for all axes of induced astigmatism compared to

baseline (Figure 3-7) and this reduction reached significance (p < 0.001). Post-hoc

testing showed that all axes of induced astigmatism significantly reduced near visual

acuity compared with optimally corrected baseline acuity, but there was no significant

difference between axes.

Figure 3-7 – Group mean near visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean)

Page 56: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

42

Visual Conditions One-way ANOVA

Measure Baseline - 1.00 DS / - 1.00 DS / - 1.00 DS /

P + 2.00 DC + 2.00 DC + 2.00 DC df F value x 90 x 180 x 45 & 135

VA (logMAR)

Distance -0.17 0.24 0.26 0.28

1.81, 19.90 67.15 < 0.001 ± 0.10 ± 0.10 a ± 0.10 a ± 0.12 a

Near -0.07 0.13 0.14 0.20

3, 33 24.51 < 0.001 ± 0.08 ± 0.10 a ± 0.09 a ± 0.09 a Reading rate (words/minute)

N20 155.40 135.56 129.71 127.80

3, 33 23.55 < 0.001 ± 15.58 ± 15.60 a ± 11.58 a ± 15.95 a

N16 146.20 127.59 117.99 114.74

3, 33 35.37 < 0.001 ± 14.52 ± 14.07 a ± 14.29 a ± 16.10 a

N12 135.94 116.04 109.11 108.51

3, 33 38.84 < 0.001 ± 13.66 ± 12.85 a ± 13.72 a ± 12.72 a

N10 132.69 106.77 99.67 97.80

3, 33 42.51 < 0.001 ± 16.03 ± 13.35 a ± 16.25 a ± 14.71 a

N8 123.20 98.34 89.19 86.06

3, 33 46.74 < 0.001 ± 15.40 ± 14.63 a ± 20.42 a ± 20.65 a

N6 113.13 81.75 76.84 64.46

1.70, 18.71 28.10 < 0.001 ± 15.19 ± 21.50 a ± 25.38 a ± 25.65 a

Table 3-4 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different axes of astigmatism compared with baseline

condition

Post-hoc testing of the effects of induced astigmatism versus baseline value, (ap < 0.001, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.05).

Page 57: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

43

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA (within factors: cylinder axis and reading

font size), demonstrated that there was a significant main effect of cylindrical axis (F

(2.08, 22.82) = 68.34; p < 0.001) and font size (F (1.81, 19.82) = 87.16; p < 0.001) on reading

rate and there was also a significant interaction effect (F (4.87, 53.59) = 2.56; p = 0.039).

A series of one-way ANOVAs demonstrated that reading rate for all six different

font sizes (N20, N16, N12, N10, N8, and N6) significantly (p < 0.001) decreased

compared with baseline (optimal correction) for 2.00 D astigmatism along the three

axes. The reduction in reading rate as a function of level of axis of astigmatism and

font size is shown in Figure 3-8. Post-hoc testing indicated that the relationship

between the axis of astigmatism and the decrease in reading rate varied as a function

of font size as reflected by the significant interaction effect. Reading rate with OBL

astigmatism was significantly slower than ATR for all font sizes: N20 (p = 0.013),

N16 (p = 0.001), N12 (p = 0.002), N10 (p = 0.008), N8 (p = 0.011), and N6 (p =

0.003), and slower than WTR astigmatism for N6 (p = 0.003). A significant

difference between reading rates between ATR and WTR was only apparent at font

sizes of N16 (p = 0.021), and N8 (p = 0.033).

Figure 3-8 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different astigmatic axis (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 58: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

44

3.4 Discussion

This experiment assessed the influence of simulated astigmatism on standard

measures of visual function and reading performance (distance visual acuity, near

visual acuity, and reading rate). To characterise the effects of astigmatism on

distance visual acuity and near visual function performance, three different powers

(0.50 DC, 1.00 DC, and 2.00 DC) of simulated ATR astigmatism, and three different

axes to induce ATR, WTR, and OBL astigmatism for an astigmatic power of 2.00

DC were evaluated. Three different levels of spherical blur (0.25 DS, 0.50 DS and

1.00 DS) that were equivalent to the level of blur of the astigmatic conditions for

distance visual acuity were also included to determine whether there is any

difference between low levels myopia and astigmatism when the level of dioptric

blur is matched.

Different powers of simulated astigmatism were found to impair both distance and

near visual acuity. The decrease in visual acuity was greater for higher levels of

astigmatism, nevertheless, even low powers of astigmatism (0.50 DC) reduced

distance but not near visual acuity, where ATR astigmatism only significantly

reduced near visual acuity for a power of 2.00 DC. These findings are in general

agreement with previous studies (Raasch, 1995; Wolffsohn, et al., 2011), although

some studies did not compensate for the effective spherical blur induced by different

powers of simulated astigmatism. Importantly, even small amounts of ATR

astigmatism as low as 0.50 DC had a significant negative impact on distance visual

acuity. This is in general support of the finding by Guo and Atchison (2010) that the

minimum level of induced crossed cylinder astigmatism required for participants to

just notice a decrease in the clarity of a 6/7.5 letter was 0.28 DC. Watanabe, et al.

(2013) also reported that even small levels of hyperopic astigmatism with

cycloplegia ( ≥ 0.50 DC) at some axes impaired functional distance visual acuity

which involved continuous measurement of visual acuity and was more sensitive to

astigmatic blur than standard measures of visual acuity.

The effect of different levels of astigmatic power on near visual acuity was less than

that for distance, where it was only when the level of astigmatism reached 2.00 DC

Page 59: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

45

that a significant decrease in near visual acuity was recorded. Casagrande, et al.

(2013) reported that near visual acuity started to decrease compared to baseline for

powers of 1.50 DC hyperopic astigmatism, while other studies have found significant

reductions in near visual acuity for powers as low as 1.00 DC depending on the axis

of the crossed cylinder astigmatism (Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Wills, et al., 2012).

Interestingly, there were no significant differences between spherical and astigmatic

blur on distance visual acuity. This finding does not agree with that of Atchison, et

al. (2011) who reported that crossed cylinder astigmatism of 0.75 D produce losses in

visual acuity that were twice those produced by defocus of the same spherical blur

strength.

The axis of astigmatism was also found to differentially impact on both distance and

near visual acuity compared with the baseline best-corrected condition. However,

while both distance and near visual acuity were significantly reduced for all axes of

2.00 DC astigmatism compared to baseline, there were no significant differences

between axes. These findings support those of Ohlendorf, et al. (2011), who also

failed to find a difference in the effect of different crossed cylinder astigmatic axes

on distance visual acuity. Wolffsohn, et al. (2011), however, reported that crossed

cylinder ATR and OBL astigmatism had a greater detrimental impact on distance and

near visual acuity than WTR astigmatism, while Kobashi, et al. (2012) reported that

eyes with OBL myopic astigmatism had lower visual acuity than those with either

ATR or WTR astigmatism. It should be noted that while we failed to find any

significant difference in the influence of different astigmatic axes on distance and

near visual acuity, the eyes with OBL astigmatism showed slightly worse visual

acuity than with WTR and ATR astigmatism.

The reading rates recorded for the best-corrected baseline condition were consistent

with the participant’s education level and age range (Legge et al., 1985; Akutsu et

al., 1991; Elliott et al., 2001) and similar to those reported by Wills, et al. (2012),

who tested participant with similar education levels and age ranges to those in this

experiment. Reading rate was found to be affected by different powers of ATR

Page 60: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

46

astigmatism, where reading rate became slower as cylindrical power increased. This

is in general agreement with previous studies, where increasing levels of spherical

blur increased reading difficulty and these effects were greater for smaller font sizes

(Bailey et al., 1980; Chung, et al., 2007; Bailey, et al., 2013). Conversely,

Wolffsohn, et al. (2011) reported that reading rate only decreased significantly for

crossed cylinder astigmatic powers of 3.00 DC or greater, however, they only tested

reading rate for one font size that was two lines larger than the threshold near visual

acuity. Wills, et al. (2012) also noted that reading rate decreased to a greater extent

for font sizes of N12 and smaller.

We also found that OBL astigmatism resulted in the greatest decrease in reading

rates and this is in line with previous studies which have also reported that OBL

astigmatism had the greatest detrimental effect on reading rates (Kobashi, et al. 2012;

Wolffsohn, et al. (2011). However, although Kobashi, et al. (2012) reported that

OBL myopic astigmatism reduced reading rate more than WTR and ATR, their

results showed that ATR impacted on reading performance slightly more than WTR.

Reading rates only differed significantly between ATR and WTR astigmatism in this

study for the smallest font size tested, with performance with ATR astigmatism being

slightly better than for WTR astigmatism. Conversely, Wills et al (2011) reported

that crossed cylinder ATR astigmatism had a greater detrimental effect on reading

rates relative to WTR astigmatism but the experimental design adopted in that study

was slightly different to that used here. Overall, our findings demonstrated that the

effects of astigmatic axis became greater for the smaller font sizes, an effect that was

also apparent but didn’t quite reach significance for astigmatic power.

3.5 Conclusion

In summary, the findings from this study expand on current knowledge regarding the

relationship between different powers and axes of uncorrected astigmatism and

distance and near visual acuity and reading performance. We demonstrated that OBL

astigmatism has a significantly greater impact on reading rates than other axes of

astigmatism. Further studies are needed to identify the impact of simulated

astigmatism on distance and near visual acuity and reading rates for different levels

Page 61: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

47

of OBL astigmatism (which had the greatest detrimental effect), as well as to explore

the effects of both habitual and simulated uncorrected astigmatism on the

performance of participants of different age groups.

Page 62: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

48

Chapter 4: Impact of simulated astigmatic refractive blur on tests involving vision and cognition

4.1 Introduction

Degradation of visual input can lead to slowing of visual processing, even when the

degradation of vision (either through spherical blur or reduced contrast) is simulated

in otherwise visually normal young adults (Dickinson, et al., 1991; Gilmore et al.,

2006; Wood, et al., 2009). It has been hypothesised that one effect of degrading visual

input is that it makes the initial stages of visual processing more cognitively effortful.

The resulting demand on cognitive resources in turn reduces those available for other

higher level cognitive processes, including the encoding of new information in

memory, and operations such as comprehension of written information (Wingfield et

al., 2005).

While the effects of spherical blur and reduced contrast have been explored in relation

to their impact on various tests of cognitive processing (Lindenberger et al., 2001;

Anstey et al., 2006; Gilmore, et al., 2006; Wood, et al., 2009; Wood et al., 2011), the

effects of other commonly occurring refractive conditions, such as astigmatism have

not been explored. Indeed, given the findings from previous studies (Wills, et al.,

2012; Casagrande, et al., 2013) and those from Study 1 of this thesis, that even small

amounts of crossed cylinder astigmatism can have a negative impact on distance

visual acuity, near visual acuity, and reading performance, it is considered likely that

uncorrected astigmatism may also influence performance on tasks involving rapid

visual processing and visual search.

Based on previous literature in this area, it is thus likely that astigmatic blur will

influence many aspects of functional visual performance that involve both visual and

cognitive input, however, the impact of different levels and powers of astigmatism has

not been explored. Hence, the aim of this study was to investigate the effects of

Page 63: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

49

simulated astigmatism, both in terms of the power of the cylinder and its axis, on

performance of tasks involving the integration of both visual and cognitive skills. The

standardised tests selected for inclusion in this study included the Developmental Eye

Movements (DEM) test that measures reading-related eye movements, the Digit

Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) and the Trail Making A and B tests (TMAT and

TMBT) which measure aspects of visual search and speed of visual processing.

4.2 Method

4.2.1 Participants

Participants included 12 visually normal young adults (mean age 24.75 ± 5.26 years;

range 18 to 34 years) and included 8 females and 4 males. All participants provided

informed consent and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of

Helsinki (Ethics forms in Appendix 1).

The inclusion criteria and the screening procedures for this study were the same as for

Study 2 described in Chapter 3 of this thesis.

4.2.2 Protocol

Visual acuity (distance and near visual acuity), reading eye movements and cognitive

test performance were measured binocularly using commonly used chart-based tests.

Testing was conducted for the different conditions of simulated astigmatic blur as per

Study 1, where the blurring lenses were worn in a trial frame and the inter-pupillary

distance was adjusted for each individual participant. The blurring conditions tested

included three levels of ATR astigmatic blur (0.50 D, 1.00 D, and 2.00 D) and three

axes of 2.00 D astigmatism (ATR, WTR and OBL) compared to the best corrected

condition. The mean spherical equivalent was maintained as plano for all of the

simulated astigmatic conditions. As for Study 1, the order of testing of the visual

conditions was randomised between subjects to minimise order effects. Prior to

testing, the participants watched a movie (from a distance of 40 cm that matched the

near vision testing distance) for 10 minutes, in order to allow adaptation to the testing

Page 64: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

50

lenses as discussed previously, with a short break (5 minutes) with clear vision being

provided when the testing of each visual condition was complete.

The illumination levels for testing were measured with IM-2D lux meter (Topcon,

Tokyo) as presented in Table 4-1 for the DEM, DSST and Trail making tests; the

values for the distance and near visual acuity charts were the same as for Study 1

(Table 3-1). The luminance of the charts were measured with the BM-7 luminance

colorimeter (Topcon, Tokyo) and are also presented in Table 4-1 along with the

Weber contrast values derived from the chart measurements.

Luminance (of chart) (cd/m2)

Illumination (lux) (at chart)

Weber contrast (%)

DEM 190 680 93

DSST 190 840 93

Trail Making tests 190 840 93

4.2.3 Distance and near visual acuity

Binocular distance and near visual acuity were measured as for Study 1 for the five

astigmatic blur conditions and the best corrected baseline condition.

4.2.4 Developmental Eye Movement Test (DEM test)

The DEM test was chosen to assess functional reading-related eye movement

performance given that this test is designed to control for rapid automised naming

(RAN) skills (Garzia et al., 1990). The reading eye movements (DEM) test consists of

a pre-test and two subtests which consist of 40 numbers arranged in two vertical

columns (tests A and B) and a subtest with 80 numbers with irregular spacing and

arranged in 16 horizontal rows (test C) (Figure 4-1).

Table 4-1 – Luminance and illumination of charts

Page 65: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

51

Participants were required to read aloud the single digit numbers which were arranged

either vertically (Charts A and B) or horizontally (Chart C), and mounted on a stand at

a working distance of 40 cm, as quickly and accurately as possible for each of the six

visual conditions under binocular viewing conditions. Responses were also digitally

recorded (Sony IC recorder ICD-UX512F, Sony Inc, Japan) as has been undertaken

previously (Pang et al., 2010). The time taken to read aloud the 80 numbers in both

the four vertical columns (tests A and B) known as the vertical time and the 16

horizontal rows (test C) known as the horizontal time was derived post-testing from

the audio recordings. The number of substitution errors (“s” errors), omission errors

(“o” errors), addition errors (“a” errors) and transposition errors (“t” errors) were also

recorded. The testing times of the vertical columns (tests A and B) and horizontal

rows (test C) were adjusted for the errors made by participants, according to the test

instructions, and the DEM ratio (derived by dividing the horizontal time by the

vertical time) calculated for each of the visual conditions.

4.2.5 Digit Symbol Substitution test (DSST)

The DSST (Wechsler, 1981) is a measure of general information processing speeds

(Spreen et al., 1998) and has been widely used in studies of cognitive ageing. The pen

and paper version of the test was used, where the test stimulus is printed on white A4

Figure 4-1 – Schematic representation of the different components of the DEM test.

Page 66: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

52

paper and includes a key at the top of the page which specifies the particular symbols

that correspond to each numerical digit from 1 - 9 (Figure 4-2).

Participants were instructed to write the correct symbol corresponding to the random

array of digits as quickly and accurately as possible according to the coding key. The

DSST score was recorded as the number of correct symbols recorded in 90 seconds.

Both speed and accuracy were equally emphasized in the instructions to ensure

consistency (Wong et al., 1991). Participants were specifically instructed to use their

non-writing hand as a page support to prevent them from pointing to the coding key.

4.2.6 Trail Making Tests A and B (TMT A and TMT B)

The pencil and paper version of the TMT from the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychology

Test Battery was used (Spreen, et al., 1998) which assesses motor speed, visual

attention, mental flexibility and motor function (Lezak, 1995). This is a timed task

consisting of two sub-tests: Parts A and B which are both presented on white paper

(Figure 4-2). Part A consists of 25 randomly positioned encircled numbers and the

participants were required to join the numbers in chronological order (i.e. 1-2-3-4

etc.). Part B consists of randomly positioned encircled numbers and letters and

Figure 4-2 – Sample of DSST and Trail Making tests.

Page 67: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

53

participants were instructed to join the numbers and letters in alternating order (i.e. 1-

A-2-B-3-C, etc.). Any errors made were pointed out by the examiner immediately and

corrected before continuing the sequence. A participant’s score was taken as the time

to complete the test to the nearest tenth of a second.

Different versions of the DEM, DSST and TMT A and B were produced

electronically and printed on paper as per the originals, in order that different versions

of the tests could be presented for the different visual conditions in order to minimise

familiarity effects. The order of the visual conditions and versions of the tests was

also randomised for each participant.

4.2.7 Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis of the results was performed using the Statistical Package for the

Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 21.0 (IBM Corp., United States). As for Study 2, the

outcome measures were visually inspected with box plots and histograms to confirm

approximate normal distribution of the data. Statistical normality tests were also

undertaken using the Shapiro-Wilks test and confirmed that the distributions were

approximately normal and therefore the use of parametric statistics was appropriate.

One-way repeated measures ANOVA were used to examine the influence of various

levels and axes of simulated astigmatism on distance visual acuity, near visual acuity,

DEM, DSST and TMT A and TMT B. A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered

statistically significant.

4.3 Results

4.3.1 Powers of ATR astigmatism

Group mean values and the results of the statistical analyses for the outcome measures

(distance visual acuity, near visual acuity, DEM test, DSST, and TMTA and B) for

the three levels of induced ATR astigmatism compared with baseline are given in

Table 4-2. As for the participants in Study 1, simulated ATR astigmatism reduced

distance visual acuity relative to baseline (Figure 4-3), with all levels of astigmatism

significantly reducing performance compared with the best-corrected baseline

Page 68: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

54

condition and visual acuity being worsened to a greater extent as the power of

astigmatism increased. Post-hoc testing also demonstrated that all levels of ATR

astigmatism were significantly different to one another as well as relative to baseline.

Figure 4-3 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different levels of ATR

astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 69: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

55

Visual Conditions One-way ANOVA

Measure Baseline - 0.25 DS / - 0.50 DS / - 1.00 DS /

P + 0.50 DC + 1.00 DC + 2.00 DC df F value x 90 x 90 x 90

VA (logMAR)

Distance -0.18 -0.11 -0.02 0.24

1.90, 20.84 92.96 < 0.001 ± 0.09 ± 0.10 a ± 0.12 a ± 0.08 a

Near -0.09 -0.05 -0.02 0.14

1.87, 20.54 37.34 < 0.001 ± 0.06 ± 0.07 a ± 0.07 a ± 0.06 a DEM, DSST, and TMT A and B

DEM Adj vertical time (s) 24.80 24.92 25.89b 26.40b

1.82, 20.01 4.25 0.032 ± 3.99 ± 3.31 ± 3.54 ± 3.94

DEM Adj horizontal time (s) 25.29 25.94 26.42 26.67

3, 33 1.51 > 0.05 ± 3.78 ± 4.91 ± 4.46 ± 2.94

DEM Ratio 1.02 1.04 1.02 1.02

1.71, 18.85 0.10 > 0.05 ± 0.07 ± 0.09 ± 0.09 ± 0.13

DSST (correct responses) 77.50 75.33 73.50 70.17

3, 33 30.75 < 0.001 ± 9.50 ± 9.17 a ± 8.41 a ± 9.61 a

TMTA time for completion (s) 30.54 33.75 37.63 42.75

1.86, 20.40 29.51 < 0.001 ± 4.14 ± 6.03 a ± 7.33 a ± 7.35 a

TMTB time for completion (s) 44.36 47.65 51.55 57.58

1.63, 17.95 38.04 < 0.001 ± 7.83 ± 8.50 b ± 8.14 a ± 8.35 a

Post-hoc testing of the effects of induced astigmatism versus baseline value, (ap < 0.001, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.05).

Table 4-2 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different powers of ATR astigmatism

Page 70: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

56

Near visual acuity was also worsened compared with the best-corrected condition for

increasing power of ATR astigmatism (Figure 4-4); with the difference reaching

significance. Post-hoc testing showed that all levels of induced ATR astigmatism

significantly reduced near visual acuity compared with optimally corrected baseline

acuity, and that 2.00 DC ATR astigmatism was also significantly different to all other

levels of astigmatism, but that 0.50 D and 1.00 D levels were not different to one

another (Table 4-2). These results are in general accord with those for the participants

in Study 1, where higher levels of ATR astigmatism reduced near visual acuity but for

the Study 2 participants these differences reached significance.

The DEM results showed that both the adjusted vertical and horizontal times

increased (worse performance) with increasing powers of ATR astigmatism relative to

baseline (Figure 4-5), however, only the differences for vertical times reached

significance Post-hoc testing showed that the DEM vertical adjusted times were

significantly different from one another for all but the comparison of the 0.50 DC

level and baseline and between the 1.00 and 2.00 DC levels (Table 4-2). Neither the

Figure 4-4 – Group mean near visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 71: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

57

DEM adjusted time (horizontal) nor the DEM ratio were significantly affected by

different powers of ATR astigmatism compared with baseline (optimal correction).

The DSST score presented as the number of DSST symbols correctly recognised

within a 90 second time period decreased for increasing powers of ATR astigmatism

compared to baseline (Figure 4-6) and these differences reached significance. Post-

hoc testing showed that all levels of ATR astigmatism significantly reduced the DSST

score compared with the baseline condition (Table 4-2) and that the DSST scores for

all levels of astigmatism were significantly different to one another.

Figure 4-5 – Mean DEM adjusted vertical and horizontal time with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 72: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

58

The time taken to complete both the TMT A and B tests was also increased (i.e.

performance was worsened) with increasing powers of ATR astigmatism relative to

the baseline condition (Figure 4-7), with the differences reaching statistical

significance (p < 0.001) (Table 4-2). Post-hoc testing showed that all levels of ATR

astigmatism significantly increased the TMT A and B times compared with the

optimally corrected baseline condition and all were significantly different from one

another (Table 4-2).

Figure 4-6 – Mean DSST score with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 73: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

59

4.3.2 Axes of astigmatism

Table 4-3 shows the group mean values and results of the statistical analysis of the

outcome measures (distance and near visual acuity, DEM, DSST, TMT A and B) for

three different axes of induced astigmatism (ATR, WTR, and OBL) all 2.00 DC

power, compared with baseline.

As for Study 1, distance visual acuity reduced in the presence of simulated

astigmatism at the three different axes compared to baseline (Figure 4-8) and this

difference was significant (p < 0.001 (Table 4-3). Post-hoc testing showed that all

axes of simulated astigmatism significantly reduced distance visual acuity compared

with optimally corrected baseline acuity, but there was no significant difference in

visual acuity between the three different axes of astigmatism.

Figure 4-7 – Mean TMT time with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 74: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

60

Figure 4-8 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 75: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

61

Visual Conditions One-way ANOVA

Measure Baseline - 1.00 DS / - 1.00 DS / - 1.00 DS /

P + 2.00 DC + 2.00 DC + 2.00 DC df F value x 90 x 180 x 45 & 135

VA (logMAR)

Distance -0.18 0.24 0.22 0.27

3, 33 59.87 < 0.001 ± 0.09 ± 0.08 a ± 0.09 a ± 0.12 a

Near -0.09 0.14 0.14 0.21

3, 33 43.168 < 0.001 ± 0.06 ± 0.06 a ± 0.09 a ± 0.09 a DEM, DSST, and TMT A and B

DEM Adj vertical time (s) 24.80 26.40 27.05 26.46

3, 33 4.49 0.009 ± 3.99 ± 3.94 ± 3.33 c ± 3.82

DEM Adj horizontal time (s) 25.29 26.67 27.70 27.25

1.91, 21.04 4.02 0.035 ± 3.78 ± 2.94 ± 4.79 ± 3.94 a

DEM Ratio 1.02 1.02 1.02 1.03

3, 33 0.03 > 0.05 ± 0.07 ± 0.13 ± 0.11 ± 0.06

DSST (correct responses) 77.50 70.17 70.33 67.67

3, 33 27.71 < 0.001 ± 9.50 ± 9.61 a ± 9.53 a ± 9.83 a

TMT A time for completion (s) 30.54 42.75 44.46 44.85

1.92, 21.17 22.40 < 0.001 ± 4.14 ± 7.35 a ± 8.53 a ± 11.30 b

TMT B time for completion (s) 44.36 57.58 60.45 62.84

1.92, 21.08 41.62 < 0.001 ± 7.83 ± 8.35 a ± 9.34 a ± 12.73 a

Table 4-3 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of 3 different axes of astigmatism compared with

baseline condition

Post-hoc testing of the effects of induced astigmatism versus baseline value (ap < 0.001, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.05).

Page 76: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

62

Near visual acuity was decreased by astigmatism for all three different axes

compared with baseline (Figure 4-9) and this difference reached significance (Table

4-3). Post hoc testing also indicated that as well as all astigmatic conditions being

significantly different to baseline, near visual acuity was also significantly worse for

the OBL relative to ATR astigmatism.

Figure 4-9 – Group mean near visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 77: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

63

The adjusted vertical and horizontal times with the DEM were longer in the presence

of simulated astigmatism at different axes (Figure 4-10), and these differences

reached significance (Table 4-3). Post-hoc testing showed that the differences were

only significant between baseline and astigmatism at the three different axes but the

vertical time was not significantly different between axes (Table 4-3). DEM adjusted

horizontal time was also increased by simulated astigmatism (Figure 4-10). Post-hoc

testing showed that these differences were only significantly longer for WTR and

OBL astigmatism compared to baseline (Table 4-3). The DEM ratio was not

significantly affected by simulated astigmatism for any of the axes investigated in

this study.

Figure 4-10 – Mean DEM adjusted vertical and horizontal time with astigmatism on different axes (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 78: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

64

The DSST score was reduced in the presence of astigmatism for all of the three axes

of astigmatism (Figure 4-11). Post-hoc testing showed that the DSST score was

significantly worse relative to baseline for all different axes of astigmatism (Table

4-3). In addition, DSST scores were significantly lower in the presence of OBL

astigmatism than either ATR or WTR astigmatism, which were not significantly

different to one another.

Figure 4-11 – Mean DSST score with astigmatism on different axes (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 79: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

65

The time to complete the TMT A was increased (worse) in the presence of different

axes of simulated astigmatism (Figure 4-12) and this difference reached significance

(p < 0.001) (Table 4-3). Post-hoc testing showed that all three axes of simulated

astigmatism significantly increased TMT A test time compared with baseline (Table

4-3), but there were no significant differences in time taken to complete the test

between the three axes tested.

Similarly, the time for completion of the TMT B was increased (worse performance)

in the presence of astigmatism at the three different axes (Figure 4-12) and again this

difference reached significance (p < 0.001) (Table 4-3). Post-hoc testing showed that

all three astigmatic conditions were longer than baseline and that WTR and OBL

astigmatism also resulted in significantly longer times to complete the TMT B than

ATR astigmatism

Figure 4-12 – Mean Trail Making test time with astigmatism on different axes (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 80: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

66

4.4 Discussion

This study assessed the influence of crossed cylinder astigmatism on reading eye

movements and tests of visual processing speeds and cognitive performance.

Simulated crossed cylinder astigmatism impaired both distance and near visual

acuity for different powers and axes as was seen for the participants in Study 1 of

this thesis and as has been reported in previous studies using various methods to

induce astigmatic errors (Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Wills, et al., 2012; Casagrande, et

al., 2013; Watanabe, et al., 2013; Yamamoto, et al., 2014).

Reading eye movements as measured with the DEM were only impacted by higher

levels of ATR astigmatism, with the vertical adjusted time being more sensitive to

the impact of astigmatism than horizontal times; adjusted vertical reading times were

significantly slower for the higher powers of ATR astigmatism compared to baseline,

but there were no between axis differences. The horizontal adjusted reading times

were however, only affected for higher powers of WTR and OBL astigmatism (2.00

DC) and the ratio of vertical to horizontal reading times was unaffected by

astigmatism. These findings suggest that the horizontal adjusted times of the DEM

are less affected by astigmatism and while the vertical times were impacted this was

only the case for higher powers of WTR and OBL, suggesting a differential impact

of astigmatism. Our finding that performance on the DEM seems to be relatively

robust to the effects of simulated crossed cylinder astigmatism is in accord with

Wills, et al. (2012) who reported that crossed cylinder astigmatic blur resulted in

slower horizontal and vertical adjusted reading times on the DEM, but that astigmatic

blur did not impact on the DEM ratio and that there was little effect of simulated

astigmatism on eye movements when measured using the Visagraph instrumentation.

Performance on the DSST was significantly decreased with increasing powers of

astigmatism and was significantly reduced relative to the baseline condition even for

the lowest level of 0.50 DC (ATR). DSST scores were also significantly different to

one another for all powers of astigmatism indicating that progressive increases in the

level of ATR astigmatism reduce the ability of participants to complete the DSST.

This study also found that OBL astigmatism had the greatest detrimental effect on

Page 81: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

67

performance on the DSST, compared to either WTR or ATR astigmatism. The

finding that astigmatic blur impacts on the DSST is in support of previous studies

that have either used spherical blurring lenses on a modified version of the Digit

Symbol Coding and Symbol Search tests (Bertone et al., 2007) or blurred versions of

the DSST (Hunt, et al., 2010). Bertone, et al. (2007) represented their visual

impairments in terms of quite significant and specific losses in visual acuity (6/6 vs

6/12 and 6/18) resulting from spherical blur, while in the study of Hunt, et al. (2010)

blur was simulated by manipulating the appearance of the paper-based versions of

the DSST charts rather than blurring the vision of participants using optical lenses as

was the case in our study. To date, there have been no studies of the effects of

simulated astigmatism on cognitive test performance that involve visual search and

speed of visual processing, particularly not at the low levels of astigmatism that were

included here. It is likely that the decrement in performance from simulated

astigmatism seen here for the DSST results from a slowing of the speed of cognitive

processing of information, as well as impairment in the perception of the presented

stimuli, it is also possible that working memory could be affected by blur as

suggested by Hunt, et al. (2010).

The findings for the TMT A and B tests also demonstrated that simulated

astigmatism resulted in longer times to complete testing, with all levels of ATR

resulting in increased times and OBL astigmatism having the greatest detrimental

effect. Interestingly, while Hunt, et al. (2010) found no difference in the amount of

time required to complete the TMT A and B tests with increasing blur, our study

showed that astigmatic blur even as low as 0.50 DC slowed performance. This

finding is consistent with the suggestion by Salthouse (2011) that the TMT involve

both speed and fluid cognitive abilities. Therefore the presence of astigmatic blur of

different powers and axes is likely to have increased search times and decreased

visual processing speed. Interestingly, while Skeel et al. (2003) found that the TMT

B test was differentially impacted to a greater extent by uncorrected refractive error

compared to the TMT A, our findings suggest that the results of both tests were

impacted by simulated astigmatic blur which also concurs with previous research

which found that reduced contrast sensitivity also slows performance on both the

TMT A and B tests (Wood, et al., 2009; Wood, et al., 2011).

Page 82: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

68

Importantly, a number of the cognitive tests included here have been shown to be

predictive of driving performance and crash risk, thus the implications of the findings

are that uncorrected astigmatism could have a detrimental impact on driving safety.

For example, Stutts et al. (1998) reported an association between reduced

performance on the TMT and increased crash risk. Similarly, Daigneault et al. (2002)

reported that participants with a history of crashes performed more poorly on several

measures of executive function including the TMT compared to those with no crash

history. Furthermore, Anstey et al. (2005) in their multifactorial model of safe

driving, which was based on a review of empirical studies of predictors of safe

driving and crash risk, as well as an understanding of the age-related changes in

cognition, sensory function and motor abilities, concluded that a number of measures

of vision, physical function and cognition, including executive function were

associated with unsafe driving outcomes. More recently, Friedman et al. (2013)

reported that performance on the TMT B was significantly associated with crash

rates in older adults. In our study, simulated astigmatism, in terms of powers and

axes, resulted in slowing in performance on these measures of executive function and

processing speeds, thus it is possible that uncorrected astigmatism could be

associated with increased crash risk.

Collectively the findings reported in this study highlight the importance of correction

of astigmatism at levels as low as 0.50 DC, regardless of the axis of astigmatism, for

optimum performance on a range of tests that involve both visual and cognitive skills

and are linked to the performance of everyday tasks including reading and driving.

4.5 Conclusion

This study demonstrated that even low levels of astigmatism can impact on reading

eye movements to a lesser extent and particularly cognitive test performance that

involves visual search and speed of visual processing. Oblique astigmatism had

greater effects on performance than either WTR or ATR astigmatism. Given that

many functional tasks such as driving involve rapid visual search and processing,

these findings suggest that uncorrected astigmatism may have an effect on many

important activities of daily living. There may also be a significant impact of

Page 83: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

69

uncorrected or under-corrected astigmatism on a child’s capacity to perform many

classroom learning tasks that involve both vision and cognitive skills. Further study

in this area should focus on the impact of uncorrected astigmatism upon the ability to

detect relevant targets within more complex (or crowded) visual arrays and extend to

practical assessments of tasks of everyday living including driving, given the link of

many of the tests included here with driving performance and safety.

Page 84: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

70

Chapter 5: Impact of simulated astigmatism blur on functional measures of visual performance using Chinese characters

5.1 Introduction

As discussed elsewhere in this thesis, astigmatism has a detrimental influence on a

range of measures of visual function including visual acuity (Raasch, 1995; Kobashi,

et al., 2012; Read, et al., 2014). Interestingly, Little et al. (2012) reported that

uncorrected astigmatism differentially affected visual acuity depending on the format

of the letter chart, where simulated astigmatism had the greatest effect on crowded

letter charts with higher levels of complexity.

Uncorrected astigmatism also impacts on tasks that involve the integration of both

vision and cognitive skills, such as reading (Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Rosenfield et al.,

2012; Wills, et al., 2012; Watanabe, et al., 2013), and on cognitive test performance

as demonstrated in Study 2. Reading tasks that involve Chinese characters are

particularly challenging because they involve a range of complicated structures,

variability of categories, and similarity between characters (Stallings, 1976; Mori et

al., 1984; Hildebrandt et al., 1993). However, although reading Chinese characters is

likely to be a more difficult task than reading in other languages, only a limited

number of studies have measured the factors that affect near visual acuity using

Chinese characters (Johnston, 1985; Hao et al., 1997). This is an important gap in the

literature given that a large proportion of the world’s population are readers of

Chinese and the prevalence of astigmatism is relatively high in the Chinese population

(Chen et al 2013). This study therefore evaluated the impact of astigmatism on visual

acuity and reading performance using Chinese charts which are hypothesised to

present a higher level of visual and cognitive effort than measurements using standard

English letter charts as employed in Studies 1 and 2 of this thesis.

Page 85: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

71

5.2 Method

5.2.1 Participants

Twelve young adults (mean age 23.50 ± 2.43 years; range 20 to 28 years) consisting

of 7 females and 5 males were recruited to participate in this study. All participants

were required to have Chinese as their first spoken and written language in order that

they would be able to fluently read the Chinese characters included in the study. All

other inclusion and exclusion criteria as well as the screening procedures were the

same as for Studies 1 and 2.

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (Ethics

forms in Appendix 1) and was approved by the QUT Human Research Ethics

Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants following a

full explanation of the experimental procedures, with the option to withdraw from the

study at any time.

5.2.2 Protocol

Distance visual acuity was measured binocularly as for Studies 1 and 2 using standard

ETDRS charts, while near visual acuity and reading rate were measured using letter

charts incorporating Chinese characters. Testing was conducted for the different

conditions of simulated astigmatic blur as per Studies 1 and 2, where the blurring

lenses were worn in a trial frame and the inter-pupillary distance was adjusted for

each individual participant. The blurring conditions tested included three levels of

ATR astigmatic blur (0.50 D, 1.00 D, and 2.00 D) and three axes of 2.00 D

astigmatism (ATR, WTR and OBL) compared to the best corrected condition. The

mean spherical equivalent was maintained as plano for all of the simulated astigmatic

conditions. As for Studies 1 and 2, the order of testing of the visual conditions was

randomised between subjects to minimise order effects and adaptation periods were

included.

The illumination levels for the near charts incorporating Chinese characters were

measured with IM-2D lux meter (Topcon, Tokyo) as presented in Table 5-1. The

Page 86: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

72

illumination levels for testing were measured with IM-2D lux meter (Topcon, Tokyo)

as presented in Table 4-1; the values for the distance and near visual acuity were the

same as for Studies 1 and 2 (Table 3-1). The luminance of the charts measured with

the BM-7 luminance colorimeter (Topcon, Tokyo) are also presented in Table 4-1

along with the Weber contrast values derived from these measurements.

5.2.3 Distance and near visual acuity

Binocular distance visual acuity was measured using the ETDRS charts as described

for Studies 1 and 2.

A series of Chinese near visual acuity charts were used for binocular testing of near

visual acuity. These near vision charts were based on those designed by Hao et al

(1997) and included only those Chinese characters with 7 strokes which more closely

represent the form of Chinese characters used in everyday Chinese writing. Six

different charts were used with each chart consisting of 87 unrelated characters, with

text size from N100 to N3 (Figure 5-1) where each character represented a word.

Testing was conducted with the charts mounted on a stand at a working distance of 40

cm and the order of charts was randomised between participants. Participants were

instructed to read each character on each line commencing from the top line of the

chart and were encouraged to attempt to read characters when unsure. Measurement

was terminated when a line of errors was made

Luminance (of

chart) (cd/m2)

Illumination

(lux) (at chart) Weber contrast (%)

Chinese near charts 196 680 92

Table 5-1 – Luminance and illuminance of near charts

Page 87: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

73

5.2.4 Reading rate

Reading rate was measured using the six different Chinese reading charts at 40 cm.

for text sizes from N20 to N3. A different Chinese reading chart was used for each

visual condition, which minimised potential learning effects between testing for the

different blur conditions. During testing, participants were required to read aloud the

characters on the charts as quickly as possible, from left to right from the top of the

chart (N20) and to guess any words they were unsure of until instructed to stop

reading; testing was terminated when the participant made a full line of errors.

Reading time was digitally recorded (Sony IC recorder ICD-UX512F, Sony Inc,

Figure 5-1 – Sample of a Chinese (7 strokes per character) near visual acuity chart

Page 88: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

74

Japan) and later analysed. The reading rate of each participant was calculated and

converted from the time taken by each participant to read the words on each line, into

a score of characters read per minute (for each individual line from N20 to N6) as for

Study 1.

5.2.5 Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis of the results was performed using the Statistical Package for the

Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 21.0 (IBM Corp., United States). As for Studies 1

and 2 in this thesis, visual inspection of the outcome measures was undertaken with

box plots and histograms to confirm approximate normal distribution of the data.

Statistical normality tests were also undertaken using the Shapiro-Wilks test,

confirming that the distributions were approximately normal and that parametric

statistics were appropriate to use. One-way repeated measures ANOVA were used to

examine the influence of various levels and axes of simulated astigmatism on distance

visual acuity, near visual acuity, and reading rate. Two-way, repeated measures

ANOVA were used to examine the interaction between different levels and axes of

simulated astigmatism, print size, and reading rate. A p-value of less than 0.05 was

considered statistically significant. In addition, the group mean reduction in

percentage reading rate was calculated for each of the six font sizes relative to the

baseline condition for that size, for both the data collected for Chinese characters as

well as that collected in Study 1 for English characters. A three-way repeated

measures ANOVA with two within factors (cylinder power and character size) and

character type included in the charts as the between subjects factor was used to

examine whether the effect of cylinder power and font size was greater or less for

reading rates measured with Chinese or English characters. A further three-way

repeated measures ANOVA with two within factors (cylinder axis and character size)

and character type as the between subjects factors examined the relative effects of

astigmatic axis on reading rates measured using Chinese and English characters.

Page 89: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

75

5.3 Results

5.3.1 Powers of ATR astigmatism

Table 5-2 shows the group mean values and results of the statistical analyses of the

outcome measures for three levels of induced ATR astigmatism compared with the

baseline best-corrected condition. Distance visual acuity decreased for increasing

powers of simulated ATR astigmatism compared with baseline (optimal correction)

(Figure 5-2), following similar trends to those recorded for the participants in Studies

1 and 2. Post-hoc testing showed that the reduction in distance visual acuity resulting

from all powers of ATR astigmatism was significantly different from baseline and

that the differences between different powers of astigmatism were also all

significantly different from one another (Table 5-2).

Near visual acuity was also reduced with increasing powers of simulated ATR

astigmatism (Figure 5-3). Post-hoc testing showed that all tested levels of induced

ATR astigmatism significantly reduced near visual acuity compared with the baseline

condition (Table 5-2) and all differences between powers were significant.

Figure 5-2 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 90: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

76

Figure 5-3 – Group mean near visual acuity with different levels of ATR astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 91: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

77

Visual Conditions One-way ANOVA

Measure Baseline - 0.25 DS / - 0.50 DS / - 1.00 DS / P + 0.50 DC + 1.00 DC + 2.00 DC df F value

x 90 x 90 x 90 Visual Acuity (logMAR)

Distance -0.02 0.04 0.11 0.20

1.81, 19.96 20.15 < 0.001 ± 0.08 ± 0.90 a ± 0.09 a ± 0.08 a

Near 0.13 0.16 0.18 0.21

3, 33 22.04 < 0.001 ± 0.02 ± 0.04 a ± 0.04 a ± 0.04 a Reading rate (characters/minute)

N20 253.09 224.50 200.06 178.13

3, 33 61.24 < 0.001 ± 29.87 ± 21.82 a ± 28.36 a ± 27.19 a

N16 221.72 198.07 179.84 160.13

3, 33 79.70 < 0.001 ± 24.03 ± 28.48 a ± 29.10 a ± 24.75 a

N12 196.00 174.48 160.21 137.49

3, 33 57.32 < 0.001 ± 29.44 ± 28.46 a ± 25.45 a ± 15.52 a

N10 174.96 161.25 146.29 124.50

3, 33 52.87 < 0.001 ± 26.15 ± 26.36 a ± 19.70 a ± 14.55 a

N8 161.48 145.11 131.75 109.75

3, 33 51.03 < 0.001 ± 25.53 ± 21.72 a ± 19.25 a ± 16.83 a

N6 147.99 129.10 112.02 90.20

3, 33 35.78 < 0.001 ± 32.53 ± 24.29 a ± 29.54 a ± 22.35 a

Table 5-2 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of three different powers of ATR astigmatism compared to baseline

Post-hoc testing of the effects of induced astigmatism versus baseline value, (ap < 0.001, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.05).

Page 92: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

78

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA (within factors: cylinder power and character

size), demonstrated that there was a significant main effect of cylindrical power (F

(2.05, 22.58) = 104.90; p < 0.001) and character size (F (2.35, 25.81) = 122.57; p < 0.001) on

reading rate with a significant interaction between the two factors (F (5.08, 55.96) = 2.60;

p = 0.03). A series of one way ANOVAs demonstrated that reading rate for all six

character sizes (N20, N16, N12, N10, N8, and N6) significantly (p < 0.001) decreased

reading rate and these differences were all significant compared with baseline

(optimal correction) (Table 5-2). Post-hoc testing also demonstrated that reading rate

was significantly different between all levels of ATR astigmatism, where reading rate

decreased significantly for each increase in power of the ATR astigmatism. The effect

of different powers of ATR astigmatism as a function of text size is shown in Figure

5-4.

Figure 5-4 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different levels of astigmatic power (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 93: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

79

5.3.2 Axes of astigmatism

Table 5-3 shows the group mean data and results of the statistical analysis of the

outcome measures for the three different axes of induced astigmatism compared with

the baseline condition. Distance visual acuity was worsened by simulated astigmatism

for all three axes tested here (Figure 5-5) and these differences reached statistical

significance (Table 5-3). Post hoc testing demonstrated that distance visual acuity was

significantly reduced by astigmatism of 2.00 D for all axes of astigmatism included

here relative to baseline and that OBL astigmatism reduced distance visual acuity to a

significantly greater extent than either ATR or WTR astigmatism, which were not

significantly different to one another.

Figure 5-5 – Group mean distance visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 94: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

80

.

Visual Condition One-way ANOVA

Measure Baseline - 1.00 DS / - 1.00 DS / - 1.00 DS /

P + 2.00 DC + 2.00 DC + 2.00 DC df F value x 90 x 180 x 45 & 135

Visual acuity (logMAR)

Distance -0.02 0.20 0.20 0.30 3, 33 26.20 < 0.001 ± 0.08 ± 0.08 a ± 0.07 a ± 0.11 a

Near 0.13 0.21 0.22 0.29 3, 33 29.27 < 0.001 ± 0.02 ± 0.04 a ± 0.06 a ± 0.08 a Reading rate (characters/minute)

N20 253.09 178.13 179.85 164.10 3, 33 77.70 < 0.001 ± 29.87 ± 27.19 a ± 33.90 a ± 32.01 a

N16 221.72 160.13 157.37 143.64 3, 33 138.53 < 0.001 ± 24.03 ± 24.75 a ± 30.18 a ± 34.22 a

N12 196.00 137.49 139.40 126.35 1.78, 19.55 124.99 < 0.001 ± 29.44 ± 15.52 a ± 22.24 a ± 22.99 a

N10 174.96 124.50 124.21 112.62 3, 33 83.98 < 0.001 ± 26.15 ± 14.55 a ± 23.99 a ± 25.44 a

N8 161.48 109.75 109.62 95.94 3, 33 54.71 < 0.001 ± 25.53 ± 16.83 a ± 23.19 a ± 24.78 a

N6 147.99 90.20 93.21 78.66 3, 33 52.68 < 0.001 ± 32.53 ± 22.35 a ± 27.30 a ± 28.39 a

Post-hoc testing of the effects of induced astigmatism versus baseline value, (ap < 0.001, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.05).

Table 5-3 – Group mean data (± standard deviation) and one-way ANOVA results for the effect of 3 different axes of astigmatism compared

with baseline condition

Page 95: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

81

Near visual acuity measured with the Chinese characters was also impacted by the

axis of simulated astigmatism (Figure 5-6) and this effect was significant (Table 5-3).

Post-hoc testing showed that all axes of 2 D of induced astigmatism significantly

reduced near visual acuity compared with optimally corrected baseline acuity, and

that near visual acuity for OBL astigmatism was significantly worse than ATR and

WTR, but that near visual acuity measures with ATR and WTR were not significantly

different from one another.

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA (within factors: cylinder axis and character

size), demonstrated that there was a significant main effect of cylindrical axis (F (2.32,

25.55) = 139.77; p < 0.001) and character size (F (1.94, 21.36) = 94.27; p < 0.001) on

reading rate with a significant interaction between the two factors (F (4.99, 54.93) = 3.04;

p = 0.017). A series of one-way ANOVAs demonstrated that reading rate for all six

font sizes (N20, N16, N12, N10, N8, and N6) was significantly (p < 0.001) decreased

in the presence of 2 D of astigmatism at all 3 axes and that these differences were all

significant compared with baseline (optimal correction) (Table 5-3). Post-hoc testing

Figure 5-6 – Group mean near visual acuity with different axes of astigmatism (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 96: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

82

also demonstrated that reading rate was significantly different between OBL

astigmatism and both ATR and WTR astigmatism which were not significantly

different from one another. The decrease in reading rate as a function of astigmatic

axis and text size is shown in Figure 5-7.

5.3.3 Comparison of the reduction in reading performance with astigmatism for

English and Chinese characters

Table 5-4 shows the group mean reduction in percentage reading rate as a function of

level of ATR astigmatism and font size relative to the optimal best-corrected

condition for both the English and Chinese characters.

Figure 5-7 – Mean reading rate; reading rate of different text size and with different astigmatic axis (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 97: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

83

A three-way repeated measures ANOVA with two within factors (cylinder power and

font size) and character type (Chinese and English) as the between subjects factors

indicated that there were significant main effects of cylinder power (F (1.68, 36.96) =

181.50; p < 0.001) and font size (F (3.43, 75.37) = 8.508; p < 0.001) and a significant

effect of character type (F (1, 22) = 21.19; p < 0.001). There were also a series of

significant interaction effects between cylinder power and character type (F (1.68, 36.96)

= 12.50; p < 0.001), where increasing cylinder powers had a significantly greater

effect on Chinese than English characters. The interaction between font size and

cylinder power was also significant (F (3.41, 36.96) = 4.67; p = 0.003), while the

interaction between font size and character type just reached significance (F (1.68, 36.96)

= 2.73; p = 0.043).

Page 98: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

84

Visual Conditions

Character

Type Text size

- 0.25 DS / + 0.50 DC x 90 - 0.50 DS / + 1.00 DC x 90 - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 90

Percentage reduction in reading rate compared to the best corrected (baseline) condition

English

N20 4.16 ± 3.36 9.00 ± 4.74 12.72 ± 5.31 N16 4.58 ± 3.97 10.01 ± 5.30 12.67 ± 5.03 N12 3.82 ± 1.90 10.37 ± 3.94 14.62 ± 4.74 N10 6.76 ± 4.17 12.89 ± 5.01 19.30 ± 6.78 N8 7.34 ± 3.80 13.78 ± 3.44 20.06 ± 7.81 N6 8.35 ± 4.49 13.59 ± 5.34 27.91 ± 14.44

Chinese

N20 10.90 ± 6.22 20.75 ± 9.13 29.33 ± 9.66 N16 10.90 ± 5.64 19.11 ± 7.27 27.91 ± 6.02 N12 10.87 ± 7.07 17.93 ± 8.34 29.30 ± 5.66 N10 7.77 ± 6.34 15.90 ± 7.18 28.19 ± 7.54 N8 9.77 ± 6.70 17.82 ± 8.26 31.48 ± 9.50 N6 11.52 ± 10.93 23.70 ± 14.68 38.54 ± 9.66

Table 5-4 – Group mean percentage reduction (± standard deviation) in reading rate relative to the optimal correction for the 3 levels of astigmatism and for each of the six font sizes for the English (Study 1) and Chinese (Study 3) characters.

Page 99: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

85

The interactions involving character type which are of particular interest here are

shown in Figure 5-8 and Figure 5-9. Figure 5-8 demonstrates that the percentage

reduction in reading rate relative to baseline for the Chinese characters was greater

than that for the English characters.

The significant interaction between font size and character type reveals that the

pattern of reduction in reading rate differs when reading is recorded using the Chinese

compared to the English charts. For the Chinese characters, there is double the

percentage reduction in reading rate at N20 (20.32% reduction for the Chinese vs

8.63% for the English characters) when the data for the three powers of ATR

combined are considered, however, the difference in percentage reduction between

groups reduces as target size decreases up to N10, after which performance using the

two different charts follow a similar pattern (Figure 5-9).

Figure 5-8 – Reduction in the overall percentage reading rate as a function of the three different powers of astigmatism for the English and Chinese characters (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 100: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

86

Table 5-5 shows the group mean reduction in percentage reading rate as a function of

the three different axes of 2.00 DC astigmatism and font size relative to the optimal

correction for the English and Chinese charts.

Figure 5-9 – Overall reduction in percentage reading rate for all powers of ATR astigmatism combined as a function of character size for the Chinese and English charts (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 101: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

87

Visual Conditions

Character

Type

Text size - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 90 - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 180 - 1.00 DS / + 2.00 DC x 45

Percentage reduction in reading rate compared to the best corrected (baseline) condition

English

N20 12.72 ± 5.31 16.03 ± 8.30 17.69 ± 6.68 N16 12.67 ± 5.03 19.15 ± 7.77 21.46 ± 8.02 N12 14.62 ± 4.74 19.57 ± 8.26 20.21 ± 4.39 N10 19.30 ± 6.78 24.65 ± 10.18 26.24 ± 7.38 N8 20.06 ± 7.81 28.19 ± 10.22 30.59 ± 11.81 N6 27.91 ± 14.44 33.08 ± 16.28 44.01 ± 16.36

Chinese

N20 29.33 ± 9.66 28.98 ± 10.46 35.22 ± 10.61 N16 27.91 ± 6.02 29.39 ± 8.08 35.83 ± 10.27 N12 29.30 ± 5.66 28.79 ± 5.77 35.60 ± 6.07 N10 28.19 ± 7.54 29.24 ± 7.43 36.17 ± 8.45 N8 31.48 ± 9.50 32.16 ± 9.88 40.85 ± 11.86 N6 38.54 ± 9.66 37.15 ± 12.12 46.82 ± 14.76

Table 5-5 – Group mean percentage reduction (± standard deviation) in reading rate relative to the optimal correction for the 3 axes of astigmatism and for each of the six font sizes for the English (Study 1) and Chinese (Study 3) charts.

Page 102: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

88

A three-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted as for cylinder power and

indicates that there were significant main effects of cylinder axis (F (1.63, 35.95) =

14.82; p < 0.001) and font size (F (2.54, 55.82) = 27.19; p < 0.001) and a significant

effect of character type (F (1, 22) = 22.82; p < 0.001). There was also a significant

interaction effect between font size and character type (F (2.54, 55.82) = 3.33; p = 0.033),

but no other significant interaction effects. The interaction between font size and

character type is given in Figure 5-10, showing data for 2.00 DC of astigmatism for

the three astigmatic axes combined as a function of font size when reading

performance was assessed using the English and Chinese charts.

As for cylinder power, the percentage relative reduction in reading rate was

consistently higher for the Chinese characters, however, the pattern of reduction in

reading rate appears to follow a different pattern than that for the English characters.

For both different levels of ATR astigmatism and for 2.00 D of cylinder power for

the three axes, the effects are greater for Chinese characters than English characters

Figure 5-10 – Overall reduction in percentage reading rate for 2.00 DC of astigmatism for the three axes combined as a function of font/symbol size for the Chinese and English charts (error bars represent the standard error of the mean).

Page 103: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

89

and exhibit a different pattern of reduction as a function of font size, with the %

reduction being relatively flat from N20 to N10 and then increasing thereafter,

whereas for English characters the reduction in reading rate is more systematically

related to decreasing font size.

5.4 Discussion

This study assessed the impact of simulated astigmatism on distance visual acuity

using English letter charts and for near visual acuity and reading rates using Chinese

character charts for a group of young adults whose first language was Chinese. The

effects of three different levels of ATR astigmatism (0.50 DC, 1.00 DC, and 2.00

DC) and three different axes of 2.00 DC astigmatism (ATR, WTR, and OBL) were

investigated.

Both distance visual acuity measured using English characters and near visual acuity

measured using Chinese characters were significantly reduced by all powers of ATR

astigmatism compared to the baseline condition and the differences between powers

were also all significantly different from one another. These findings suggest that for

Chinese adults, uncorrected ATR astigmatism for powers as low as 0.50 DC can

have a significant negative impact on visual acuity, which is in agreement with the

findings of Study 1 in our study. In addition, our finding that even low powers of

astigmatism can significantly reduce distance and near visual acuity is in accord with

those of Atchison, et al. (2009) and Guo, et al. (2010), who reported that levels of

crossed cylinder astigmatism as low as 0.28 D on average resulted in a noticeable

reduction in the level of clarity of 6/7.5 (0.1 logMAR) letters. Interestingly, we found

a similar pattern of results for near visual acuity as measured using Chinese

characters, relative to distance visual acuity, where even low levels of ATR

astigmatism had an impact on near visual acuity and this was a case for all symbol

sizes assessed in this study from N20 to N6.

Distance visual acuity was also reduced by 2.00 DC of simulated astigmatism for all

of the axes (ATR, WTR and OBL) assessed here compared to the optimum

correction, and for OBL astigmatism compared to ATR and WTR astigmatism which

Page 104: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

90

were not significantly different from one another. A very similar pattern of results

was evident for near visual acuity. Our findings suggest that near visual acuity

regardless of whether it is measured using English or Chinese characters is affected

by the magnitude of induced astigmatism and is impacted to a greater extent for OBL

rather than WTR or ATR astigmatism. These findings are in line with previous

findings for English symbols (Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Kobashi, et al., 2012). The

finding that ATR and WTR crossed cylinder astigmatism did not differentially

impact on visual acuity is in agreement with Kobashi, et al. (2012) who used myopic

astigmatism, while other studies have suggested that the effects of experimentally or

surgically induced ATR astigmatism had a greater negative impact on visual acuity

(Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Wills, et al., 2012; Yamamoto, et al., 2014).

Overall, the reading rate for Chinese characters was higher than that measured for the

English characters in Study 1, which is in general agreement with other studies that

have indicated that reading rates of upwards of 240 words per minutes are normal for

reading Chinese characters (Sun et al., 1985). These higher reading rates are partly

explained by Wei et al. (2013) who reported that the visual search and strategies for

saccade target selection adopted by readers of Chinese characters attempt to identify

as many characters as possible on each fixation and are different to those of readers

of English text.

Reading rates were found to be significantly reduced for all powers of ATR

astigmatism compared to the optimally corrected baseline condition, even for

astigmatism as low as 0.50 DC, which is in general agreement with our findings for

English characters (Study 1). Other authors have also investigated these effects on

reading rate measured using English characters, but for only selected powers of

astigmatism. For example, Wolffsohn, et al. (2011) reported that reading rate was

significantly reduced for simulated crossed cylinder astigmatism of powers of 3.00

DC or greater, while Wills, et al. (2012) investigated the effects of 1.00 DC and 2.00

DC powers of crossed cylinder astigmatism and found that the impact was text size

dependent, with only text sizes of N12 and smaller being impacted by the lower

astigmatic powers. Kobashi, et al. (2012) also found that reading speed was affected

by simulated myopic astigmatism of as little as 1.00 DC. Our findings extend the

Page 105: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

91

evidence in this area by showing that for reading rates using Chinese characters, even

low levels of astigmatism (0.50 DC) can have a detrimental effect on reading

performance, while other studies have found no influence of astigmatism axes of the

same power on visual acuity (Remón, et al., 2006; Ohlendorf, et al., 2011).

Methodological differences in terms of how astigmatism was simulated as well as the

way in which visual acuity was measured may explain these differences in results.

The orientation of axis of astigmatism was also found to impact on reading rate when

assessed using Chinese characters, where simulated astigmatism reduced reading rate

for all character sizes compared to optimal correction, and OBL astigmatism had a

greater detrimental effect than either ATR and WTR astigmatism which were not

significantly different from one another. Kobashi, et al. (2012) using Japanese

characters also reported that individuals with OBL myopic astigmatism had worse

reading performance than those with WTR or ATR myopic astigmatism. However

Wolffsohn, et al. (2011) reported that reading speed for English text was

significantly worse for uncorrected crossed cylinder ATR astigmatism than for OBL

astigmatism.

This study also allowed comparison of the reduction in reading rate in the presence

of simulated astigmatism when viewing English characters (assessed in Study 1) with

that of Chinese characters as assessed in Study 3, by converting the reading rates into

percentage reductions relative to the optimum baseline correction for each

symbol/font size for the two groups. While the percentage reduction in reading rates

increased as a function of the level of ATR astigmatism for both English and Chinese

reading charts, this effect was significantly greater for the Chinese characters. This

finding is likely to reflect the relative visual complexity and higher levels of

cognitive processing required to read Chinese characters. For example, 2,500

characters are frequently used in Chinese compared to the 26 letters in English and

the information content of each Chinese character is much higher than in each

English letter (Wei, et al., 2013). Indeed, as suggested by Garner (1970), Chinese

characters do not provide simple visual patterns in terms of information recognition,

compared to the Roman alphabet. English letters are highly consistent, involving

regular characters of simple strokes and with low “within-character” crowding, while

Page 106: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

92

Chinese characters are compounds of pictographs, where even one character might

consist of several pictographs, which increase the “within-character” crowding

(Zhang, et al., 2007). In addition, the complicated structure and mixed components of

Chinese characters are key factors that influence visual search efficiency (Yeh et al.,

2002). The results of Study 2 indicate that simulated astigmatism reduced

performance on the DSST and TMT A and B, which involve executive function as

well as speed of visual search. This reduction in visual search and processing speeds

in the presence of astigmatism may partly explain the greater effect of astigmatism

on reading of Chinese characters, given that at smaller character sizes, visual defocus

would increase the “within-character” crowding and the “between-character”

similarity (Figure 5-11), which may increase character identification time.

Recently Wang, et al. (2014) compared the visual span, which is the number of

letters/symbols that can be read without moving the eyes, for participants reading

lower case English letters in comparison to Chinese characters of different levels of

complexity. They demonstrated that as the complexity and crowding of the Chinese

characters increased, the size of visual span decreased along with reading

performance. Our finding that astigmatism had a greater effect on the Chinese

characters is likely to reflect a decrease in visual span and reading strategies as well

as increased speed of visual processing as discussed above. However, we did not

assess eye movements in this study which would be useful in future studies to better

understand the role of astigmatic blur on reading performance using different

complexities of Chinese symbols.

Figure 5-11 – Sample of similarity between Chinese characters

Page 107: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

93

Our findings have important implications, suggesting that the correction of

astigmatism is even more important for those individuals reading Chinese than

English text and that level of astigmatism as low as 0.50 DC can be problematic.

Oblique astigmatism was also found to have a greater detrimental impact on reading

rates than other axes which was also evident for English text.

5.5 Conclusion

This study demonstrated that simulated astigmatism has a detrimental effect on

visual acuity and reading performance which use Chinese characters rather than

English characters. Simulated astigmatism had a significant detrimental impact on

near visual acuity and reading performance, even for modest amounts of astigmatic

blur as low as 0.50 DC and OBL astigmatism had a significantly greater impact on

visual performance than either ATR or WTR astigmatism. Importantly, the effect of

simulated astigmatism was greater for reading rates assessed using Chinese

characters relative to English characters (as assessed in Study 1) which has important

implications given that uncorrected astigmatism is relatively high in the Chinese

population (Chen et al 2013).

Page 108: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

94

Chapter 6: Conclusions

Refractive error, especially astigmatism, is one of the leading causes of visual

impairment in younger populations. While it has been suggested that ability to

perform vision-related daily activities will be reduced when people are not optimally

corrected in terms of their refractive error (West, et al., 2002; Rosenfield, et al.,

2012), there is only limited research that has investigated the impact of uncorrected

astigmatism on the performance of visual tasks relevant to everyday activities. Given

the capacity to perform vision-related daily activities involves the integration of a

range of visual and cognitive cues and does not simply depend on high contrast

visual acuity, this research examined the impact of simulated astigmatism on a range

of tasks that involved both visual and cognitive skills. A particular focus of the

studies included in this thesis was to investigate the effects of different powers of

astigmatism as well as axes of astigmatism, in order to determine the minimum level

of astigmatism that resulted in a decrement in functional performance, as well as the

axis of astigmatism that had the greatest negative impact on measures of visual and

cognitive performance.

In Study 1 the impact of three different powers (0.50 D, 1.00 D, 2.00 D) of ATR

astigmatism and three different axes of astigmatism (ATR, WTR and OBL) of 2.00

DC on standard visual outcome measures including distance and near visual acuity,

and reading rates for six different font sizes were examined. A similar experimental

approach was adopted in Study 2, which looked at the effects of the same powers and

axes of simulated astigmatism as for Study 1 on outcome measures that involved the

integration of vision and cognitive skills, including the DEM test, DSST, and TMT A

and B. In Study 3, the effect of the same different levels and axes of astigmatism on

reading Chinese rather than English characters was also investigated in order to

determine whether the higher levels of complexity inherent in Chinese characters

makes them more susceptible to the effects of simulated astigmatism. The following

sections summarise the collective findings of these three studies with the aim of

Page 109: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

95

providing a better understanding of the differential impact of astigmatism on

functional measures of vision and cognitive performance.

6.1 Impact of astigmatism on distance and near visual acuity

Distance and near visual acuity were found to significantly worsen as a function of

increasing levels of cylindrical power for simulated crossed cylinder ATR

astigmatism. This is in general agreement with previous studies (Raasch, 1995;

Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Singh et al., 2013), although some studies have not

compensated for the effective spherical blur induced by different powers of

simulated astigmatism. Importantly, even small amounts of ATR astigmatism, at

levels as low as 0.50 DC, had a significant negative impact on visual acuity.

Watanabe, et al. (2013) also found that hyperopic astigmatism (with cycloplegia) of

≥ 0.50 DC WTR and ≥ 1.00 DC ATR caused a measureable loss of visual acuity

under certain testing conditions.

Similarly, the axis of astigmatism was found to differentially impact on both distance

and near visual acuity compared with baseline, where OBL astigmatism had a greater

detrimental effect than either WTR and ATR which is line with previous findings

(Wolffsohn, et al., 2011; Kobashi, et al., 2012). Interestingly, while Vinas, et al.

(2013) also showed that the impact of myopic astigmatism on visual acuity was

dependent on the orientation of the induced astigmatism, they indicated that previous

exposure to astigmatism played a significant role in this relationship, with a strong

bias towards the natural axis of astigmatism. This is the reason why the inclusion

criteria for this study required that none of the participants had astigmatism of more

than 0.25 DC. We also found that there was no significant difference in ATR and

WTR induced astigmatism in terms of the detrimental impact on visual acuity in

agreement with the findings of Kobashi, et al. (2012). Conversely, other studies have

suggested that the effects of experimentally or surgically induced ATR astigmatism

had a greater negative impact on visual acuity than WTR astigmatism (Yamamoto, et

al. 2014).

Page 110: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

96

In summary, the results of this study suggest that levels of ATR astigmatism as low

as 0.50 DC may have a detrimental impact on visual acuity and that OBL

astigmatism had the greatest negative impact on both distance and near visual acuity.

6.2 Impact of astigmatism on reading rate (English characters)

Reading rate for all six font sizes significantly decreased with increasing power of

ATR astigmatism and as a function of the axis of astigmatism. Furthermore, there

were interactions between text sizes and powers of astigmatism, where the effects of

astigmatism were greater for the smaller text sizes. As expected, reading speeds were

faster for the larger font sizes, which is in line with previous research (Chung et al.,

1998; Wills, et al., 2012). In addition, the orientation of the astigmatic axis was also

found to be important, where, reading rate with OBL astigmatism was significantly

slower than for the other axes of astigmatism, which has been previously reported by

Wolffsohn, et al. (2011).

6.3 Impact of astigmatism on functional visual tasks (DEM, DSST, and TMT

A and B)

Different levels of astigmatism and the orientation of the axis of astigmatism

differentially impacted on performance on a range of functional tests including the

DEM test of reading eye movements, DSST test and the TMT A and B. Performance

on the DEM test has been shown to be related to reading performance and visual

processing speeds, even though it is not directly correlated with saccadic eye

movement skills (Ayton et al., 2009), while the DSST and TMT A and B tests are

used to assess aspects of cognitive performance including executive function and

provide measures of speed of visual processing (Skeel, et al., 2003; Bertone, et al.,

2007; Hunt, et al., 2010; Salthouse, 2011).

In terms of the level of ATR astigmatism, all powers of astigmatism increased visual

processing time on both the TMT A and B tests and reduced DSST scores, relative to

baseline and all conditions were significantly different to one another. This indicates

that as the level of astigmatism increases, speeds of visual processing are slowed and

that these effects are significant even for the lowest level of astigmatism considered

here (0.50 DC). This study is the first to investigate the effects of astigmatism on

Page 111: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

97

these types of cognitive tests that involve visual search skills as well as speed of

visual processing, but it is possible to compare the data with those of Hunt, et al.

(2010) where spherical blur was simulated using image processing techniques and

the resulting images then printed, with participants completing them in the normal

pen and paper format. Contrary to our findings, Hunt, et al. (2010) found a

differential effect of simulated optical blur on these tests, where performance on the

DSST but not the TMT tests was impacted by blur. Importantly, their approach was

different to that included in this thesis in a number of ways and the capacity of image

processing techniques to produce exact levels of blur was not validated. Indeed, our

results are more in line with those which simulated the effects of reduced contrast at

the eye and showed that reduced contrast sensitivity resulted in slower visual

processing speeds measured with both the DSST and TMT A and B tests in both

young and older adults (Wood, et al., 2009; Wood, et al., 2010).

In terms of the axis of astigmatism, while all three axes of astigmatism resulted in a

decrease in performance on the TMT A and B tests and the DSST, there were no

between-axis differences, with the exception of the TMT B where both WTR and

OBL astigmatism had a greater negative impact compared to ATR astigmatism.

Performance on these tests is thus clearly impaired in the presence of astigmatism,

even at low levels, but there is little axis dependency. Conversely, performance on

the DEM seemed to be much more robust to the effects of simulated astigmatism, in

accord with Wills, et al. (2012) who also found little effect of crossed cylinder

astigmatism on eye movements when measured using the Visagraph instrumentation.

These results suggest that performance of tasks involving the integration of vision

and cognitive skills, visual scanning sequencing and motor speeds were influenced

by the level of astigmatism but were less dependent on the orientation of the axis of

astigmatism. This slowing of visual processing speeds observed in the presence of

astigmatism has implications for a range of aspects of everyday activities.

Importantly, tests such as the TMT B and DSST have been shown to be predictive of

driving performance and crash risk (Anstey, et al., 2005; Friedman, et al., 2013). Our

finding suggests that uncorrected astigmatism could have a detrimental impact on

driving safety and highlights the importance of optimum correction of astigmatism at

levels as low as 0.50 DC, regardless of the axis of astigmatism. Future work should

Page 112: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

98

systematically explore the role of uncorrected astigmatism and driving performance

measured on closed road circuits (which allow manipulation of visual status in a safe

and controlled environment (Wood, 2002)) as well as the role of uncorrected

astigmatism on crash risk.

6.4 Impact of astigmatism on reading rate (Chinese characters)

Astigmatism was found to have a greater impact on reading rates using Chinese

characters which involve a greater level of complexity than that involved in reading

English letters, as explored in Study 1. Reading rates for Chinese text became slower

with increasing power of astigmatism, even for powers of astigmatism as low as 0.50

DC and reading rate with OBL astigmatism was slower than with ATR and WTR

astigmatism. The orientation of the axis of astigmatism was also found to impact on

reading rates when assessed using Chinese characters, where simulated astigmatism

reduced reading rate for all font sizes compared to optimal correction, and OBL

astigmatism had a greater detrimental effect than either ATR and WTR astigmatism,

which were not significantly different from one another. Kobashi, et al. (2012) using

Japanese characters also reported that individuals with OBL astigmatism had worse

reading performance than those with WTR or ATR astigmatism.

When comparing the reading rates obtained for Chinese compared to English

characters, it was noted that reading rates were higher in the baseline condition for

Chinese characters. This is supported by previous studies which have indicated that

reading rates of upwards of 240 words per minutes is normal for Chinese characters

(Sun, et al., 1985). These higher reading rates may be explained by Wei, et al.

(2013), who found that readers of Chinese characters use a different processing-

based strategy for saccade target selection than do readers of English text and

identify as many characters as possible on each fixation.

The greater percentage decrease in reading rates with astigmatism at even low levels

and for all axes assessed using Chinese characters compared to English letters is

likely to reflect the relative visual complexity and higher levels of cognitive

processing required to read Chinese characters. For example, there are 2,500

characters frequently used in Chinese compared to the 26 letters in English and the

Page 113: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

99

information contained in each Chinese character is much higher than in each English

letter (Wei, et al., 2013). Our findings have important implications, suggesting that

the correction of astigmatism is even more important for those individuals reading

Chinese than English text, and that astigmatism as low as 0.50 DC can be

problematic, which is highly relevant given the high prevalence of astigmatism in

Chinese populations. Oblique astigmatism was also found to have a greater

detrimental impact on reading rates than other axes, which was also evident for

English text.

6.5 Summary and Implications

This thesis investigated the impact of different levels of astigmatism and the impact

of different axes of astigmatism on standard measures of distance visual acuity, near

visual acuity and reading rates for both English and Chinese characters, reading eye

movements and tests of cognitive processing speed.

Different levels of astigmatic power not only resulted in reduced distance and near

visual acuity, but also lowered reading rates, reduced eye movement accuracy and

visual processing speeds as measured by the TMT A and B and the DSST.

Astigmatism orientated at an oblique axis had the greatest negative impact on all

aspects of performance measured, with the majority of the outcomes being affected

to a similar extent by WTR and ATR astigmatism.

Collectively, these findings are important for better understanding the influence of

astigmatism on functional vision and demonstrate that different levels of astigmatic

power not only resulted in reduced distance and near visual acuity, but also lowered

reading rates and visual processing speeds. Oblique astigmatism had the greatest

negative impact on performance compared to WTR and ATR astigmatism which

affected most aspects of performance to a similar extent.

The direction of blur which astigmatism creates in an image is likely to be an

important factor in respect to the functional impact of different astigmatic blur

conditions. For example, ATR astigmatism produces blur of the vertical elements of

the image whereas WTR astigmatism produces blur of the horizontal elements. In a

Page 114: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

100

visual task such as an English visual acuity chart or reading, the ATR blur reduces

the legibility of the vertical character elements which are the predominant component

of the characters. On the other hand, WTR astigmatism impairs legibility of the

horizontal elements of the characters and the spacing between the lines of text.

Oblique astigmatism blurs both horizontal and vertical components of English text

and it is probably not surprising therefore that in the studies reported in this thesis,

OBL astigmatism tended to have the greatest impact on visual acuity and functional

tests of vision. It would be of interest in future research to examine the interactions

between axis/level of astigmatism and visual task performance, where the orientation

of the critical detail within the tasks was carefully controlled. This could be

augmented by analysis of the orientation of critical details within everyday visual

tasks using Fourier analysis of scenes.

The design of this study also meant that only three levels of simulated ATR

astigmatism and three different axes of 2.00 DC could be assessed, given time

constraints and potential fatigue of the participants. It would be useful to

systematically assess the impact of different powers of WTR and OBL astigmatism,

as well as to determine the effect of astigmatic correction in habitual astigmats.

In addition, this thesis did not measure eye movements while participants were

reading the Chinese and English text which would have been useful in exploring why

simulated astigmatism had a greater negative impact on reading Chinese characters.

Reading eye movements have been investigated for different languages that use

either spaced and unspaced (like Chinese characters) writing systems (Rayner, 1998;

Rayner et al., 2007; Li et al., 2011). In addition, Kajii et al. (2001) measured the eye

movement strategies adopted when reading Japanese text (which is unspaced like

Chinese text) and reported that the preferred viewing location was the first character

of the word, which is also found for English readers when reading unspaced text.

Thus the spacing of Chinese text may be one of the factors that interact with the

impact of astigmatism on reading rates. It would be useful in future studies to

investigate the impact of astigmatism on patterns of eye movements, search

strategies and speed of visual processing in populations using different reading

strategies.

Page 115: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

101

An important clinical implication of the studies reported in this thesis is that

correction of small amounts of astigmatism (e.g. 0.50 DC) should be considered

given the impact of simulated ATR astigmatism of this magnitude on functional

measures that involve visual and cognitive skills. The functional impact of OBL

astigmatism was also worse than either WTR or ATR and therefore is even more

important in terms of appropriate correction. In addition, astigmatism was found to

have a greater negative impact on reading rates measured for Chinese characters

rather than English text, which reinforces the importance of correction for this group,

particularly as Chinese populations have relatively high levels of astigmatism.

In summary, the findings outlined in this thesis are important for better

understanding of the influence of astigmatism on functional vision. They also assist

eye care practitioners in recognising the importance of correcting astigmatism, even

for levels as low as 0.50 DC, since functional vision performance can be adversely

affected even at these low levels of astigmatism.

Page 116: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

102

Appendices

Appendix 1

Participant information and consent form

PARTICIPANT INFORMATION FOR QUT RESEARCH PROJECT

The Impact of Astigmatism on Vision Performance QUT Ethics Approval Number 1300000792

RESEARCH TEAM Principal Researcher: Mr. Sung-Hua Yang HDR Student Associate Researchers: Professor Joanne Wood Principal Supervisor Professor Michael Collins Associate Supervisor School of Optometry and Vision Science, Faculty of Health, Queensland University of Technology (QUT)

DESCRIPTION This project is being undertaken as part of the Masters degree of Sung-Hua Yang.

The purpose of this project is to investigate the impact of astigmatism on vision performance.

The research team is looking for participants who are aged 18 to 35 years old, speak English or Chinese as their first language and have healthy eyes and normal vision.

PARTICIPATION Prior to the study the research team will conduct a brief eye examination to check your eye health, but no eye drops will be required.

During the study you will complete visual tests with letter charts, word charts, and reading tests. While you perform these visual tests you will wear different types of lenses that blur your vision. Short breaks will be permitted between the test sessions.

The initial vision screening will take about 15 minutes and the main study will require an additional 2 hours of testing time. The tests that we propose to use include reading rate (speed of reading a paragraph of text), developmental eye movement (a test requiring eye movements between two columns of numbers in a determined sequence), digit symbol substitution (a test requiring matching of different symbols which correspond to digits in a fixed period of time) and trails tests (a test of connecting numbers and letters in a determined sequence). All these procedures will be conducted at the School of Optometry and Vision Science at QUT (O Block, Kelvin Grove Campus).

Your participation in this project is entirely voluntary. If you do agree to participate you can withdraw from the project without comment or penalty. If you withdraw, on request any identifiable information already obtained from you will be destroyed. Your decision to participate or not participate will in no way impact upon your current or future relationship with QUT (for example your grades, employment or ongoing clinical care).

Page 117: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

103

EXPECTED BENEFITS It is not expected that this project will directly benefit you. We will appreciate your participation. To compensate you for your contribution and to help towards travel costs, the research team will provide you with a $20 gift voucher.

RISKS There will be minimal risks associated with your participation in this project. The vision tests and cognitive tests are standard clinical and research tests. Some participants may feel some eye discomfort or headaches when performing the tests with blurred vision. However, we will cease the tests if this happens and we will provide sufficient breaks between measurement sessions to give your eyes some rest. It should be noted that if you do agree to participate, you can withdraw at any time during the project without comment or penalty.

PRIVACY AND CONFIDENTIALITY All information collected during this study will be treated confidentially. The data will be coded as non-identifiable and will be stored securely in the School of Optometry and Vision Science. It is possible that the non-identifiable data collected in this project could be aggregated with future research data.

CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE We would like to ask you to sign a written consent form (enclosed) to confirm your agreement to participate.

QUESTIONS / FURTHER INFORMATION ABOUT THE PROJECT If have any questions or require further information please contact one of the research team members below.

Mr Sung-Hua Yang 3138 5673 [email protected] Prof Joanne Wood 3138 5701 [email protected] Prof Michael Collins 3138 5702 [email protected]

CONCERNS / COMPLAINTS REGARDING THE CONDUCT OF THE PROJECT QUT is committed to research integrity and the ethical conduct of research projects. However, if you do have any concerns or complaints about the ethical conduct of the project you may contact the QUT Research Ethics Unit on 3138 5123 or email [email protected]. The QUT Research Ethics Unit is not connected with the research project and can facilitate a resolution to your concern in an impartial manner.

Thank you for helping with this research project. Please keep this sheet for your information.

Page 118: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

104

CONSENT FORM FOR QUT RESEARCH PROJECT

The Impact of Astigmatism on Vision Performance QUT Ethics Approval Number 1300000792

RESEARCH TEAM CONTACTS Mr Sung-Hua Yang 3138 5673 [email protected] Prof Joanne Wood 3138 5701 [email protected] Prof Michael Collins 3138 5702 [email protected]

STATEMENT OF CONSENT

By signing below, you are indicating that you:

• Have read and understood the information document regarding this project

• Have had any questions answered to your satisfaction

• Understand that if you have any additional questions you can contact the research team

• Have been informed that the project is for the purpose of research and not for treatment of your eyes.

• Understand that you are free to withdraw at any time, without comment or penalty.

• Understand that you can contact the Research Ethics Unit on 3138 5123 or email [email protected] if you have concerns about the ethical conduct of the project.

• Understand that non-identifiable data collected in this project may be used as comparative data in future projects.

• Agree to participate in the project.

Name

Signature

Date

Please return this sheet to the investigator.

Page 119: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

105

References

Abrahamsson, M., Fabian, G., Andersson, A. K., & Sjöstrand, J. (1990). A

longitudinal study of a population based sample of astigmatic children I. Refraction and amblyopia. Acta Ophthalmologica, 68(4), 428-434.

Ahmed, I. I. K., Rocha, G., Slomovic, A. R., Climenhaga, H., Gohill, J., Grégoire,

A., & Ma, J. (2010). Visual function and patient experience after bilateral implantation of toric intraocular lenses. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 36(4), 609-616.

Akutsu, H., Legge, G. E., Ross, J. A., & Schuebel, K. J. (1991). Psychophysics of

Reading—x. Effects of age-related changes in vision. Journal of Gerontology, 46(6), 325-331.

Anstey, K. J., Butterworth, P., Borzycki, M., & Andrews, S. (2006). Between-and

within-individual effects of visual contrast sensitivity on perceptual matching, processing speed, and associative memory in older adults. Gerontology, 52(2), 124-130.

Anstey, K. J., Wood, J., Lord, S., & Walker, J. G. (2005). Cognitive, sensory and

physical factors enabling driving safety in older adults. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(1), 45-65.

Anstice, J. (1971). Astigmatism: its components and their changes with age.

American Journal of Optometry and Archives of American Academy of Optometry, 48(12), 1001-1006.

Applegate, R. A., Marsack, J. D., Ramos, R., & Sarver, E. J. (2003). Interaction

between aberrations to improve or reduce visual performance. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 29(8), 1487-1495.

Atchison, D. A., Guo, H., Charman, W. N., & Fisher, S. (2009). Blur limits for

defocus, astigmatism and trefoil. Vision Research, 49(19), 2393-2403. Atchison, D. A., & Mathur, A. (2011). Visual acuity with astigmatic blur. Optometry

and Vision Science, 88(7), 798-805. Atkinson, J., Braddick, O., & French, J. (1980). Infant astigmatism: its disappearance

with age. Vision Research, 20(11), 891-893. Ayton, L. N., Abel, L. A., Fricke, T. R., & McBrien, N. A. (2009). Developmental

eye movement test: what is it really measuring? Optometry and Vision Science, 86(6), 722-730.

Page 120: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

106

Bababeygy, S. R., Zoumalan, C. I., & Manche, E. E. (2008). Visual outcomes of wavefront-guided laser in situ keratomileusis in eyes with moderate or high myopia and compound myopic astigmatism. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 34(1), 21-27.

Bailey, I. L., & Lovie-Kitchin, J. E. (2013). Visual acuity testing. From the

laboratory to the clinic. Vision Research, 90, 2-9. Bailey, I. L., & Lovie, J. (1976). New design principles for visual acuity letter charts.

Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 53(11), 740. Bailey, I. L., & Lovie, J. (1980). The design and use of a new near-vision chart.

American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 57(6), 378-387. Baldwin, W. R., & Mills, D. (1981). A longitudinal study of corneal astigmatism and

total astigmatism. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 58(3), 206-211.

Bender, R., & Lange, S. (2001). Adjusting for multiple testing - when and how?

Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 54(4), 343-349. Bennett, A. (1984). A new approach to the statistical analysis of ocular astigmatism

and astigmatic prescriptions. In The Frontiers of Optometry: First International Congress (Vol. 2, pp. 35-42).

Bertone, A., Bettinelli, L., & Faubert, J. (2007). The impact of blurred vision on

cognitive assessment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(5), 467-476.

Buehren, T., Collins, M. J., & Davis, B. (2007). Potential higher-order aberration

cues for sphero-cylindrical refractive error development. Optometry and Vision Science, 84(3), 163-174.

Casagrande, M., Baumeister, M., Bühren, J., Klaproth, O. K., Titke, C., & Kohnen,

T. (2013). Influence of additional astigmatism on distance-corrected near visual acuity and reading performance. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 98, 24-29.

Chen, C., Cheung, S. W., & Cho, P. (2013). Myopia control using toric

orthokeratology (TO-SEE study). Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54(10), 6510-6517.

Chen, C. C., Cheung, S. W., & Cho, P. (2012). Toric orthokeratology for highly

astigmatic children. Optometry and Vision Science, 89(6), 849–855. Chen, W., Zuo, C., Chen, C., Su, J., Luo, L., Congdon, N., & Liu, Y. (2013).

Prevalence of corneal astigmatism before cataract surgery in Chinese patients. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 51(10), 1348-1355.

Page 121: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

107

Cheng, C. Y., Hsu, W. M., Liu, J. H., Tsai, S. Y., & Chou, P. (2003). Refractive errors in an elderly Chinese population in Taiwan: the Shihpai Eye Study. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 44(11), 4630-4638.

Chu, B. S., Wood, J. M., & Collins, M. J. (2010). The effect of presbyopic vision

corrections on nighttime driving performance. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 51(9), 4861-4866.

Chung, S. T., Mansfield, J. S., & Legge, G. E. (1998). Psychophysics of reading.

XVIII. The effect of print size on reading speed in normal peripheral vision. Vision Research, 38(19), 2949-2962.

Chung, S. T. L., Jarvis, S. H., & Cheung, S. H. (2007). The effect of dioptric blur on

reading performance. Vision Research, 47(12), 1584-1594. Clementi, M., Angi, M., Forabosco, P., Di Gianantonio, E., & Tenconi, R. (1998).

Inheritance of astigmatism: evidence for a major autosomal dominant locus. American Journal of Human Genetics, 63(3), 825-830.

Coleman, A. L., Yu, F., Keeler, E., & Mangione, C. M. (2006). Treatment of

uncorrected refractive error improves vision‐specific quality of life. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 54(6), 883-890.

Dai, G.-m. (2008). Wavefront optics for vision correction (Vol. 179): SPIE press

Bellingham, WA. Daigneault, G., Joly, P., & Frigon, J.-Y. (2002). Executive functions in the

evaluation of accident risk of older drivers. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(2), 221-238.

Daum, K. M., Clore, K. A., Simms, S. S., Vesely, J. W., Wilczek, D. D., Spittle, B.

M., & Good, G. W. (2004). Productivity associated with visual status of computer users. Optometry-Journal of the American Optometric Association, 75(1), 33-47.

Deller, J., O'connor, A., & Sorsby, A. (1947). X-ray measurement of the diameters of

the living eye. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 456-467.

Dickinson, C., & Rabbitt, P. (1991). Simulated visual impairment: Effects on text

comprehension and reading speed. Clinical Vision Sciences, 6(4), 301-308. Dobson, V., Fulton, A., & Sebris, S. (1984). Cycloplegic refractions of infants and

young children: the axis of astigmatism. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 25(1), 83-87.

Dunne, M., Elawad, M. E., & Barnes, D. A. (1994). A study of the axis of orientation

of residual astigmatism. Acta Ophthalmologica, 72(4), 483-489.

Page 122: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

108

Ehrlich, D. L., Braddick, O. J., Atkinson, J., Anker, S., Weeks, F., Hartley, T., Wade, J., & Rudenski, A. (1997). Infant emmetropization: longitudinal changes in refraction components from nine to twenty months of age. Optometry and Vision Science, 74(10), 822-843.

Elliott, D. B., Patel, B., & Whitaker, D. (2001). Development of a reading speed test

for potential-vision measurements. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 42(8), 1945-1949.

Fan, D., Rao, S. K., Cheung, E., Islam, M., Chew, S., & Lam, D. (2004).

Astigmatism in Chinese preschool children: prevalence, change, and effect on refractive development. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 88(7), 938-941.

Fan, Q., Zhou, X., Khor, C.-C., Cheng, C.-Y., Goh, L.-K., Sim, X., Tay, W.-T., Li,

Y.-J., Ong, R. T.-H., Suo, C., Cornes, B., Ikram, M. K., Chia, K.-S., Seielstad, M., Liu, J., Vithana, E., Young, T. L., Tai, E. S., Wong, T.-Y., Aung, T., Teo, Y.-Y., & Saw, S.-M. (2011). Genome-wide meta-analysis of five Asian cohorts identifies PDGFRA as a susceptibility locus for corneal astigmatism. PLoS Genetics, 7(12), e1002402.

Farbrother, J. E., Welsby, J. W., & Guggenheim, J. A. (2004). Astigmatic axis is

related to the level of spherical ametropia. Optometry and Vision Science, 81(1), 18-26.

Ferrer-Blasco, T., Montés-Micó, R., Peixoto-de-Matos, S. C., González-Méijome, J.

M., & Cerviño, A. (2009). Prevalence of corneal astigmatism before cataract surgery. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 35(1), 70-75.

Friedman, C., McGwin, G., Ball, K. K., & Owsley, C. (2013). Association between

higher order visual processing abilities and a history of motor vehicle collision involvement by drivers ages 70 and over. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54(1), 778-782.

Fulton, A., Hansen, R., & Petersen, R. (1982). The relation of myopia and

astigmatism in developing eyes. Ophthalmology, 89(4), 298-302. Garcia, M. L., Huang, D., Crowe, S., & Traboulsi, E. I. (2003). Relationship between

the axis and degree of high astigmatism and obliquity of palpebral fissure. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 7(1), 14-22.

Garner, W. R. (1970). Good patterns have few alternatives: Information theory's

concept of redundancy helps in understanding the gestalt concept of goodness. American Scientist, 34-42.

Garzia, R. P., Richman, J. E., Nicholson, S. B., & Gaines, C. S. (1990). A new

visual-verbal saccade test: The developmental eye movement test (DEM). Journal of the American Optometric Association, 61(2), 124-135.

Page 123: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

109

Gilmore, G. C., Spinks, R. A., & Thomas, C. W. (2006). Age effects in coding tasks: Componential analysis and test of the sensory deficit hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 21(1), 7-18.

Goss, D. A., & Shewey, W. B. (1990). Rates of childhood myopia progression as a

function of type of astigmatism. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 73(5), 159-163.

Grosvenor, T. (1978). Etiology of astigmatism. American Journal of Optometry and

Physiological Optics, 55(3), 214-218. Grosvenor, T., Quintero, S., & Perrigin, D. (1988). Predicting refractive astigmatism:

a suggested simplification of Javal's rule. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 65(4), 292-297.

Guggenheim, J. A., Zayats, T., Prashar, A., & To, C. H. (2008). Axes of astigmatism

in fellow eyes show mirror rather than direct symmetry. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 28(4), 327-333.

Guo, H., & Atchison, D. A. (2010). Subjective blur limits for cylinder. Optometry

and Vision Science, 87(8), E549-E559. Gwiazda, J., Grice, K., Held, R., McLellan, J., & Thorn, F. (2000). Astigmatism and

the development of myopia in children. Vision Research, 40(8), 1019-1026. Gwiazda, J., Scheiman, M., Mohindra, I., & Held, R. (1984). Astigmatism in

children: changes in axis and amount from birth to six years. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 25(1), 88-92.

Hammond, C. J., Snieder, H., Gilbert, C. E., & Spector, T. D. (2001). Genes and

environment in refractive error: the twin eye study. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 42(6), 1232-1236.

Han, S., Chen, P., Fan, Q., Khor, C.-C., Sim, X., Tay, W.-T., Ong, R. T.-H., Suo, C.,

Goh, L.-K., & Lavanya, R. (2011). Association of variants in FRAP1 and PDGFRA with corneal curvature in Asian populations from Singapore. Human Molecular Genetics, 20, 3693–3698.

Hao, Y. M., & Johnston, A. W. (1997). An evaluation of logMAR vision test charts

for near vision using Chinese characters. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 80(5), 178-186.

Hashemi, H., Khabazkhoob, M., Yekta, A. A., Jafarzadehpur, E., Emamian, M. H.,

Shariati, M., & Fotouhi, A. (2011). High prevalence of astigmatism in the 40‐to 64‐year‐old population of Shahroud, Iran. Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 40(3), 247-254.

Hayashi, K., Hayashi, H., & Hayashi, F. (1995). Topographic analysis of the changes

in corneal shape due to aging. Cornea, 14(5), 527-532.

Page 124: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

110

Hayashi, K., Manabe, S., Yoshida, M., & Hayashi, H. (2010). Effect of astigmatism on visual acuity in eyes with a diffractive multifocal intraocular lens. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 36(8), 1323-1329.

Heidary, G., Ying, G. S., Maguire, M. G., & Young, T. L. (2005). The association of

astigmatism and spherical refractive error in a high myopia cohort. Optometry and Vision Science, 82(4), 244-247.

Higgins, K., Wood, J., & Tait, A. (1998). Vision and driving: selective effect of

optical blur on different driving tasks. Human Factors, 40(2), 224-232. Hildebrandt, T. H., & Liu, W. (1993). Optical recognition of handwritten Chinese

characters: advances since 1980. Pattern Recognition, 26(2), 205-225. Ho, J. D., C. Y. Tsai, and S. W. Liou, (2009) Accuracy of corneal astigmatism

estimation by neglecting the posterior corneal surface measurement. American Journal of Ophthalmology 147: 788-95.

Holland, E., Lane, S., Horn, J. D., Ernest, P., Arleo, R., & Miller, K. M. (2010). The

AcrySof Toric intraocular lens in subjects with cataracts and corneal astigmatism. Ophthalmology, 117(11), 2104-2111.

Howland, H. C. (1982). Infant eyes: optics and accommodation. Current Eye

Research, 2(3), 217-224. Hunt, L. A., & Bassi, C. J. (2010). Near-vision acuity levels and performance on

neuropsychological assessments used in occupational therapy. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 64(1), 105-113.

Johnston, A. (1985). Near visual acuity tests using Chinese characters and the

logMAR principle. Singapore Medical Journal, 26, 448-455. Kaido, M., Dogru, M., Yamada, M., Sotozono, C., Kinoshita, S., Shimazaki, J., &

Tsubota, K. (2006). Functional visual acuity in Stevens-Johnson syndrome. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 142(6), 917-922.

Kajii, N., Nazir, T. A., & Osaka, N. (2001). Eye movement control in reading

unspaced text: The case of the Japanese script. Vision Research, 41(19), 2503-2510.

Katz, J., Tielsch, J. M., & Sommer, A. (1997). Prevalence and risk factors for

refractive errors in an adult inner city population. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 38(2), 334-340.

Keller, P. R., Collins, M. J., Carney, L. G., Davis, B. A., & Van Saarloos, P. P.

(1996). The relation between corneal and total astigmatism. Optometry and Vision Science, 73(2), 86-91.

Page 125: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

111

Khan, K. A., Dawson, K., Mankowska, A., Cufflin, M. P., & Mallen, E. A. (2013). The time course of blur adaptation in emmetropes and myopes. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 33(3), 305-310.

Kline, D. W., Buck, K., Sell, Y., Bolan, T. L., & Dewar, R. E. (1999). Older

observers' tolerance of optical blur: Age differences in the identification of defocused text signs. Human Factors, 41(3), 356-364.

Kobashi, H., Kamiya, K., Shimizu, K., Kawamorita, T., & Uozato, H. (2012). Effect

of axis orientation on visual performance in astigmatic eyes. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 38(8), 1352-1359.

Koller, T., Iseli, H. P., Hafezi, F., Mrochen, M., & Seiler, T. (2006). Q-factor

customized ablation profile for the correction of myopic astigmatism. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 32(4), 584-589.

Kompaniez, E., Sawides, L., Marcos, S., & Webster, M. A. (2013). Adaptation to

interocular differences in blur. Journal of Vision, 13(6), 19, 1-14. Lai, Y. H., Hsu, H. T., Wang, H. Z., Chang, C. H., & Chang, S. J. (2010).

Astigmatism in preschool children in Taiwan. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 14(2), 150-154.

Lamoureux, E. L., Hassell, J. B., & Keeffe, J. E. (2004). The determinants of

participation in activities of daily living in people with impaired vision. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 137(2), 265-270.

Legge, G. E., Pelli, D. G., Rubin, G. S., & Schleske, M. M. (1985). Psychophysics of

reading—I. Normal vision. Vision Research, 25(2), 239-252. Legge, G. E., Ross, J. A., Luebker, A., & Lamay, J. M. (1989). Psychophysics of

reading. VIII. The Minnesota low-vision reading test. Optometry and Vision Science, 66(12), 843-853.

Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University

Press. Li, X., Liu, P., & Rayner, K. (2011). Eye movement guidance in Chinese reading: Is

there a preferred viewing location? Vision Research, 51(10), 1146-1156. Liang, Y. B., Wong, T. Y., Sun, L. P., Tao, Q. S., Wang, J. J., Yang, X. H., Xiong,

Y., Wang, N. L., & Friedman, D. S. (2009). Refractive errors in a rural Chinese adult population: the Handan eye study. Ophthalmology, 116(11), 2119-2127.

Lindenberger, U., Scherer, H., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). The strong connection

between sensory and cognitive performance in old age: not due to sensory acuity reductions operating during cognitive assessment. Psychological Bulletin, 16(2), 196-205.

Page 126: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

112

Linke, S. J., Richard, G., & Katz, T. (2011). Prevalence and associations of anisometropia with spherical ametropia, cylindrical power, age, and sex in refractive surgery candidates. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 52(10), 7538-7547.

Little, J. A., Molloy, J., & Saunders, K. J. (2012). The differing impact of induced

astigmatic blur on crowded and uncrowded paediatric visual acuity chart results. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 32(6), 492-500.

Liu, Y.-C., Chou, P., Wojciechowski, R., Lin, P.-Y., Liu, C. J.-L., Chen, S.-J., Liu,

J.-H., Hsu, W.-M., & Cheng, C.-Y. (2011). Power vector analysis of refractive, corneal, and internal astigmatism in an elderly chinese population: the shihpai eye study. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 52(13), 9651-9657.

Lopes, M. C., Hysi, P. G., Verhoeven, V. J., Macgregor, S., Hewitt, A. W.,

Montgomery, G. W., Cumberland, P., Vingerling, J. R., Young, T. L., & van Duijn, C. M. (2013). Identification of a candidate gene for astigmatism. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 54(2), 1260-1267.

Margolis, M. K., Coyne, K., Kennedy-Martin, T., Baker, T., Schein, O., & Revicki,

D. A. (2002). Vision-specific instruments for the assessment of health-related quality of life and visual functioning. Pharmacoeconomics, 20(12), 791-812.

McKendrick, A. M., & Brennan, N. A. (1996). Distribution of astigmatism in the

adult population. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 13(2), 206-212.

Mon-Williams, M., Tresilian, J. R., Strang, N. C., Kochhar, P., & Wann, J. P. (1998).

Improving vision: neural compensation for optical defocus. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 265(1390), 71-77.

Mori, S., Yamamoto, K., & Yasuda, M. (1984). Research on machine recognition of

handprinted characters. Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence (4), 386-405.

Nirmalan, P. K., Tielsch, J. M., Katz, J., Thulasiraj, R. D., Krishnadas, R.,

Ramakrishnan, R., & Robin, A. L. (2005). Relationship between vision impairment and eye disease to vision-specific quality of life and function in rural India: the Aravind Comprehensive Eye Survey. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 46(7), 2308-2312.

O'Leary, C. I., & Evans, B. J. (2003). Criteria for prescribing optometric

interventions: literature review and practitioner survey. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 23(5), 429-439.

Ohlendorf, A., Tabernero, J., & Schaeffel, F. (2011). Visual acuity with simulated

and real astigmatic defocus. Optometry and Vision Science, 88(5), 562-569.

Page 127: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

113

Owsley, C., & McGwin Jr, G. (2010). Vision and driving. Vision Research, 50(23), 2348-2361.

Owsley, C., Stalvey, B., Wells, J., & Sloane, M. E. (1999). Older drivers and

cataract: driving habits and crash risk. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 54(4), 203-211.

Owsley, C., Stalvey, B. T., Wells, J., Sloane, M. E., & McGwin Jr, G. (2001). Visual

risk factors for crash involvement in older drivers with cataract. Archives of Ophthalmology, 119(6), 881-887.

Pärssinen, O. (1991). Astigmatism and school myopia. Acta Ophthalmologica, 69(6),

786-790. Pang, P. C., Lam, C. S., & Woo, G. C. (2010). Factors affecting accuracy in the

Developmental Eye Movement Test measurement for Cantonese‐speaking children. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 93(5), 341-348.

Perneger, T. V. (1998). What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. British Medical

Journal, 316(7139), 1236-1238. Pesala, V., Garg, P., & Bharadwaj, S. R. (2014). Image quality analysis of

pseudophakic eyes with uncorrected astigmatism. Optometry and Vision Science, 91(4), 444-451.

Pesudovs, K., & Brennan, N. A. (1993). Decreased uncorrected vision after a period

of distance fixation with spectacle wear. Optometry and Vision Science, 70(7), 528-531.

Phillips, A. J., Speedwell, L., Stone, J., & Hough, T. (1997). Contact lenses:

Butterworth-Heinemann Oxford, England. Pierscionek, B. K., & Chan, D. Y. (1989). Refractive index gradient of human lenses.

Optometry and Vision Science, 66(12), 822-829. Porter, J., Guirao, A., Cox, I. G., & Williams, D. R. (2001). Monochromatic

aberrations of the human eye in a large population. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 18(8), 1793-1803.

Qin, X.-J., Margrain, T. H., To, C. H., Bromham, N., & Guggenheim, J. A. (2005).

Anisometropia is independently associated with both spherical and cylindrical ametropia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 46(11), 4024-4031.

Raasch, T. W. (1995). Spherocylindrical refractive errors and visual acuity.

Optometry and Vision Science, 72(4), 272-275. Rabbetts, R. B., & Elliott, D. B. (2007). Assessment of binocular vision. In D. B.

Elliott (Ed.), Clinical Procedures in Primary Eye Care (pp. 151-217): Elsevier Health Sciences UK.

Page 128: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

114

Rahi, J., Peckham, C., & Cumberland, P. (2008). Visual impairment due to undiagnosed refractive error in working age adults in Britain. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 92(9), 1190-1194.

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years

of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372-422. Rayner, K., Li, X., Williams, C. C., Cave, K. R., & Well, A. D. (2007). Eye

movements during information processing tasks: Individual differences and cultural effects. Vision Research, 47(21), 2714-2726.

Read, S., Collins, M., & Carney, L. (2007). A review of astigmatism and its possible

genesis. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 90(1), 5-19. Read, S. A., Vincent, S. J., & Collins, M. J. (2014). The visual and functional

impacts of astigmatism and its clinical management. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 34(3), 267-294.

Remón, L., Tornel, M., & Furlan, W. D. (2006). Visual acuity in simple myopic

astigmatism: influence of cylinder axis. Optometry and Vision Science, 83(5), 311-315.

Richdale, K., Berntsen, D. A., Mack, C. J., Merchea, M. M., & Barr, J. T. (2007).

Visual acuity with spherical and toric soft contact lenses in low-to moderate-astigmatic eyes. Optometry and Vision Science, 84(10), 969-975.

Rocha, K. M., Vabre, L., Harms, F., Chateau, N., & Krueger, R. R. (2007). Effects of

Zernike wavefront aberrations on visual acuity measured using electromagnetic adaptive optics technology. Journal of Refractive Surgery, 23(9), 953-959.

Rosenfield, M., Hue, J. E., Huang, R. R., & Bababekova, Y. (2012). The effects of

induced oblique astigmatism on symptoms and reading performance while viewing a computer screen. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 32(2), 142-148.

Rowland, H. C., & Sayles, N. (1985). Photokeratometric and photorefractive

measurements of astigmatism in infants and young children. Vision Research, 25(1), 73-81.

Salthouse, T. A. (2011). What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making

performance? Intelligence, 39(4), 222-232. Satterfield, D. (1989). Prevalence and variation of astigmatism in a military

population. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 60(1), 14-18. Saunders, H. (1986). Changes in the orientation of the axis of astigmatism associated

with age. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 6(3), 343-344.

Page 129: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

115

Sawides, L., Marcos, S., Ravikumar, S., Thibos, L., Bradley, A., & Webster, M. (2010). Adaptation to astigmatic blur. Journal of Vision, 10(12), 22.

Scheiman, M., & Wick, B. (2008). Clinical management of binocular vision:

heterophoric, accommodative, and eye movement disorders: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Schieber, F. (2004). Highway research to enhance safety and mobility of older road

users. Transportation in an Aging Society, 125-144. See, A. Y., Anstey, K. J., & Wood, J. M. (2010). Simulated Cataract and Low

Contrast Stimuli Impair Cognitive Performance in Older Adults: Implications for Neuropsychological Assessment and Everyday Function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 18(1), 1-21.

Sheedy, J. E., Bailey, I. L., Fong, D., Ostermeier, M., & Stanton, S. (1990). Task

performance and contrast polarity on hard copy and video displays. In SC-DL Tentative (Vol. 1249, pp. 45-53): International Society for Optics and Photonics.

Singh, A., Pesala, V., Garg, P., & Bharadwaj, S. R. (2013). Relation between

uncorrected astigmatism and visual acuity in pseudophakia. Optometry and Vision Science, 90(4), 378-384.

Skeel, R. L., Nagra, A., VanVoorst, W., & Olson, E. (2003). The relationship

between performance-based visual acuity screening, self-reported visual acuity, and neuropsychological performance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17(2), 129-136.

Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests:

Administration, norms, and commentary: Oxford University Press. Stallings, W. (1976). Approaches to Chinese character recognition. Pattern

Recognition, 8(2), 87-98. Stark, L. R., Strang, N. C., & Atchison, D. A. (2003) Dynamic accommodation

response in the presence of astigmatism. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics and Image Science, 20(12), 2228-2236.

Stutts, J. C., Stewart, J. R., & Martell, C. (1998). Cognitive test performance and

crash risk in an older driver population. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 30(3), 337-346.

Sun, F., Morita, M., & Stark, L. W. (1985). Comparative patterns of reading eye

movement in Chinese and English. Perception and Psychophysics, 37(6), 502-506.

Page 130: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

116

Suzukamo, Y., Oshika, T., Yuzawa, M., Tokuda, Y., Tomidokoro, A., Oki, K., Mangione, C. M., Green, J., & Fukuhara, S. (2005). Psychometric properties of the 25-item national eye institute visual function questionnaire (NEI VFQ-25), japanese version. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 3(1), 65-76.

Taylor, H. (2000) Refractive errors: magnitude of the need. Community Eye Health,

13,1-2. Teikari, j., O'Donnell, J. J., Kaprio, J., & Koskenvuo, M. (1989). Genetic and

environmental effects on oculometric traits. Optometry and Vision Science, 66(9), 594-599.

Teikari, J. M., & O'Donnell, J. J. (1989). Astigmatism in 72 twin pairs. Cornea, 8(4),

263-266. Trindade, F., Oliveira, A., & Frasson, M. (1997). Benefit of against-the-rule

astigmatism to uncorrected near acuity. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 23(1), 82-85.

Valluri, S., Minkovitz, J. B., Budak, K., Essary, L. R., Walker, R. S., Chansue, E.,

Cabrera, G. M., Koch, D. D., & Pepose, J. S. (1999). Comparative corneal topography and refractive variables in monozygotic and dizygotic twins. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 127(2), 158-163.

Villegas, E., Alcon, E., & Artal, P. (2006). The effect of correcting small

astigmatisms on visual acuity. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 47(5), 1173.

Villegas, E. A., Alcón, E., & Artal, P. (2014). Minimum amount of astigmatism that

should be corrected. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 40(1), 13-19.

Vinas, M., de Gracia, P., Dorronsoro, C., Sawides, L., Marin, G., Hernández, M., &

Marcos, S. (2013). Astigmatism impact on visual performance: meridional and adaptational effects. Optometry and Vision Science, 90(12), 1430-1442.

Vinas, M., Sawides, L., De Gracia, P., & Marcos, S. (2012). Perceptual adaptation to

the correction of natural astigmatism. PloS One, 7(9), 1-10. Vincent, S. J., Collins, M. J., Read, S. A., Carney, L. G., & Yap, M. K. (2013).

Corneal changes following near work in myopic anisometropia. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 33(1), 15-25.

Vitale, S., Ellwein, L., Cotch, M. F., Ferris III, F. L., & Sperduto, R. (2008).

Prevalence of refractive error in the United States, 1999-2004. Archives of Ophthalmology, 126(8), 1111-1119.

Wang, H., He, X., & Legge, G. E. (2014). Effect of pattern complexity on the visual

span for Chinese and alphabet characters. Journal of Vision, 14(8), 6, 1-17.

Page 131: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

117

Watanabe, K., Negishi, K., Kawai, M., Torii, H., Kaido, M., & Tsubota, K. (2013). Effect of experimentally induced astigmatism on functional, conventional, and low-contrast visual acuity. Journal of Refractive Surgery, 29, 19-24.

Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised

(WAIS-R): San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wei, W., Li, X., & Pollatsek, A. (2013). Word properties of a fixated region affect

outgoing saccade length in Chinese reading. Vision Research, 80, 1-6. West, S., Rubin, G., Broman, A., Munoz, B., Bandeen-Roche, K., & Turano, K.

(2002). How does visual impairment affect performance on tasks of everyday life?: The SEE Project. Archives of Ophthalmology, 120(6), 774-780.

Whittaker, S. G., & Lovie-Kitchin, J. (1993). Visual requirements for reading.

Optometry and Vision Science, 70(1), 54-65. Wills, J., Gillett, R., Eastwell, E., Abraham, R., Coffey, K., Webber, A., & Wood, J.

(2012). Effect of simulated astigmatic refractive error on reading performance in the young. Optometry and Vision Science, 89(3), 271-276.

Wingfield, A., Tun, P. A., & McCoy, S. L. (2005). Hearing loss in older adulthood

what it is and how it interacts with cognitive performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 144-148.

Wixson, R. (1965). Refraction pedigrees: part two the cornea. American Journal of

Optometry and Archives of American Academy of Optometry, 42 (10), 615-618.

Wolffsohn, J. S., Bhogal, G., & Shah, S. (2011). Effect of uncorrected astigmatism

on vision. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 37(3), 454-460. Wolffsohn, J. S., & Cochrane, A. L. (2000). Design of the low vision quality-of-life

questionnaire (LVQOL) and measuring the outcome of low-vision rehabilitation. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 130(6), 793-802.

Wolffsohn, J. S., & Cochrane, A. L. (2000). The practical near acuity chart (PNAC)

and prediction of visual ability at near. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 20(2), 90-97.

Wong, J. L., & Gilpin, A. R. (1991). Effects of speed vs accuracy instructions and

field dependence on college students' digit-symbol matching performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73(1), 314-314.

Woo, G., & Lo, P. (1980). A Chinese word acuity chart with new design principles.

Singapore Medical Journal, 21(5), 689-692. World Health Organisation (2007) Global Initiative for the Elimination of Avoidable

Blindness. Action Plan 2006-2011. WHO, 2007.

Page 132: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

118

Wood, J., Chaparro, A., Anstey, K., Hsing, Y., Johnsson, A., Morse, A., & Wainwright, S. (2009). Impact of simulated visual impairment on the cognitive test performance of young adults. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 100(3), 593-602.

Wood, J., Chaparro, A., Anstey, K., Lacherez, P., Chidgey, A., Eisemann, J., Gaynor,

A., & La, P. (2010). Simulated visual impairment leads to cognitive slowing in older adults. Optometry and Vision Science, 87(12), 1037-1043.

Wood, J. M. (2002). Age and visual impairment decrease driving performance as

measured on a closed-road circuit. Human Factors, 44(3), 482-494. Wood, J. M., Collins, M. J., Chaparro, A., Marszalek, R., Carberry, T., Lacherez, P.,

& Chu, B. S. (2014). Differential effects of refractive blur on day and nighttime driving performance. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 55(4), 2284-2289.

Wood, J. M., Lacherez, P., Black, A. A., Cole, M. H., Boon, M. Y., & Kerr, G. K.

(2011). Risk of falls, injurious falls, and other injuries resulting from visual impairment among older adults with age-related macular degeneration. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 52(8), 5088-5092.

Wood, J. M., Tyrrell, R. A., Chaparro, A., Marszalek, R. P., Carberry, T. P., & Chu,

B. S. (2012). Even moderate visual impairments degrade drivers' ability to see pedestrians at night. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 53(6), 2586-2592.

Wu, H. M., Seet, B., Yap, E. P. H., Saw, S. M., Lim, T. H., & Chia, K. S. (2001).

Does education explain ethnic differences in myopia prevalence? A population-based study of young adult males in Singapore. Optometry and Vision Science, 78(4), 234-239.

Xu, L., Li, J., Cui, T., Hu, A., Fan, G., Zhang, R., Yang, H., Sun, B., & Jonas, J. B.

(2005). Refractive error in urban and rural adult Chinese in Beijing. Ophthalmology, 112(10), 1676-1683.

Yamaguchi, T., Negishi, K., & Tsubota, K. (2011). Functional visual acuity

measurement in cataract and intraocular lens implantation. Current Opinion in Ophthalmology, 22(1), 31-36.

Yamamoto, T., Hiraoka, T., Beheregaray, S., & Oshika, T. (2014). Influence of

simple myopic against-the-rule and with-the-rule astigmatism on visual acuity in eyes with monofocal intraocular lenses. Japanese Journal of Ophthalmology, 58(5), 409-414.

Yamashita, T., Watanabe, S., & Ohba, N. (1999). A longitudinal study of cycloplegic

refraction in a cohort of 350 Japanese schoolchildren. Anisometropia. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 19(1), 30-33.

Page 133: THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON …THE IMPACT OF SIMULATED ASTIGMATISM ON FUNCTIONAL MEASURES OF VISUAL PERFORMANCE . Sung-Hua Yang . B.Optom., MSc. May, 2015 . Submitted in

119

Yeh, S.-L., & Li, J.-L. (2002). Role of structure and component in judgments of visual similarity of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(4), 933-947.

Zhang, J.-Y., Zhang, T., Xue, F., Liu, L., & Yu, C. (2009). Legibility of Chinese

characters in peripheral vision and the top-down influences on crowding. Vision Research, 49(1), 44-53.

Zhang, J. Y., Zhang, T., Xue, F., Liu, L., & Yu, C. (2007). Legibility variations of

Chinese characters and implications for visual acuity measurement in Chinese reading population. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 48(5), 2383-2390.