the prevalence and prevention of crosstalk: a multi-institutional study

7
This article was downloaded by: [University of Victoria] On: 18 November 2014, At: 15:17 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The Journal of Social Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vsoc20 The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study John E. Edlund a , Austin Lee Nichols b , Bradley M. Okdie c , Rosanna E. Guadagno d , Cassie A. Eno e , Jeremy D. Heider f , Edward J. Hansen g , Brad J. Sagarin g , Ginette Blackhart h , Catherine A. Cottrell i & Kenneth Tyler Wilcox a a Rochester Institute of Technology b University of Navarra c The Ohio State University at Newark d National Science Foundation e Bellevue University f Southeast Missouri State University g Northern Illinois University h East Tennessee State University i New College of Florida Accepted author version posted online: 13 Dec 2013.Published online: 08 Apr 2014. To cite this article: John E. Edlund, Austin Lee Nichols, Bradley M. Okdie, Rosanna E. Guadagno, Cassie A. Eno, Jeremy D. Heider, Edward J. Hansen, Brad J. Sagarin, Ginette Blackhart, Catherine A. Cottrell & Kenneth Tyler Wilcox (2014) The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study, The Journal of Social Psychology, 154:3, 181-185, DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2013.872596 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2013.872596 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or

Upload: kenneth-tyler

Post on 22-Mar-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

This article was downloaded by: [University of Victoria]On: 18 November 2014, At: 15:17Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Journal of Social PsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vsoc20

The Prevalence and Prevention ofCrosstalk: A Multi-Institutional StudyJohn E. Edlunda, Austin Lee Nicholsb, Bradley M. Okdiec, Rosanna E.Guadagnod, Cassie A. Enoe, Jeremy D. Heiderf, Edward J. Hanseng,Brad J. Sagaring, Ginette Blackharth, Catherine A. Cottrelli &Kenneth Tyler Wilcoxa

a Rochester Institute of Technologyb University of Navarrac The Ohio State University at Newarkd National Science Foundatione Bellevue Universityf Southeast Missouri State Universityg Northern Illinois Universityh East Tennessee State Universityi New College of FloridaAccepted author version posted online: 13 Dec 2013.Publishedonline: 08 Apr 2014.

To cite this article: John E. Edlund, Austin Lee Nichols, Bradley M. Okdie, Rosanna E. Guadagno,Cassie A. Eno, Jeremy D. Heider, Edward J. Hansen, Brad J. Sagarin, Ginette Blackhart,Catherine A. Cottrell & Kenneth Tyler Wilcox (2014) The Prevalence and Prevention ofCrosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study, The Journal of Social Psychology, 154:3, 181-185, DOI:10.1080/00224545.2013.872596

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2013.872596

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or

Page 2: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f V

icto

ria]

at 1

5:17

18

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

The Journal of Social Psychology, 154: 181–185, 2014Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLCISSN: 0022-4545 print / 1940-1183 onlineDOI: 10.1080/00224545.2013.872596

REPLICATIONS AND REFINEMENTS

The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: AMulti-Institutional Study

JOHN E. EDLUNDRochester Institute of Technology

AUSTIN LEE NICHOLSUniversity of Navarra

BRADLEY M. OKDIEThe Ohio State University at Newark

ROSANNA E. GUADAGNONational Science Foundation

CASSIE A. ENOBellevue University

JEREMY D. HEIDERSoutheast Missouri State University

EDWARD J. HANSENBRAD J. SAGARIN

Northern Illinois University

GINETTE BLACKHARTEast Tennessee State University

Address correspondence to John E. Edlund, Rochester Institute of Technology, Department of Psychology, 18 LombMemorial Dr., Rochester, NY 14623, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f V

icto

ria]

at 1

5:17

18

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

182 THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CATHERINE A. COTTRELLNew College of Florida

KENNETH TYLER WILCOXRochester Institute of Technology

ABSTRACT. It is a common problem in psychology subject pools for past study participants toinform future participants of key experimental details (also known as crosstalk). Previous research(Edlund, Sagarin, Skowronski, Johnson, & Kutter, 2009) demonstrated that a combined classroomand laboratory treatment could significantly reduce crosstalk. The present investigation tested a labo-ratory-only treatment for the prevention of crosstalk at five universities, along with institutional-levelmoderators of crosstalk. Results indicated the presence of crosstalk at all universities and that the lab-oratory-based treatment was effective in reducing crosstalk. Importantly, crosstalk rates were higher(but successfully neutralized) in research pools with higher research credit requirements. Therefore,this research provides valuable guidance regarding crosstalk prevalence and its minimization byresearchers.

Keywords: crosstalk, data integrity, participant pools, research methods, social influence

NON-NAÏVE RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS ARE a significant problem for psychologicalresearchers (Aronson, Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Gonzales, 1990). A non-naïve participant is aparticipant who (through any number of means) knows critical information related to a partic-ular study. Importantly, non-naïve participants may behave differently from naïve participants(Nichols & Maner, 2008). The primary reasons why research participants lose their naïveté toa particular research study are that they decipher the hypothesis of the researcher through theresearch protocol (Aronson et al., 1990) or arrive with foreknowledge of the research. Participantcrosstalk is problematic and occurs when previous participants reveal key experimental infor-mation to future participants (Edlund, Sagarin, Skowronski, Johnson, & Kutter, 2009). Edlundand colleagues (2009) investigated two techniques for reducing crosstalk (a classroom basedtechnique and a combined classroom and laboratory-only technique). In combination, these twotechniques successfully decreased detectable crosstalk from 3% to 0.004%.

The current study tested a laboratory-only treatment for preventing crosstalk at five differentuniversities. We predicted the detection of crosstalk at every university, and that a laboratory-onlytreatment would significantly reduce the prevalence of crosstalk across the different universities.Additionally, we examined contextual variables across the universities to explain differences incrosstalk prevalence and treatment effectiveness.

METHOD

Participants

A total of 3,607 students participated across five universities.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f V

icto

ria]

at 1

5:17

18

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

EDLUND ET AL. 183

Materials

A pretest determined the normal range of guesses for the number of gumballs in a glass jar. Usingthis knowledge, we artificially set the reported number of gumballs in the jar to a non-commonguessing point (i.e., not a multiple of 100), within one standard deviation of the average guessduring the pretest, and at a value that was different from the actual number of gumballs in the jar.

Procedure

At the conclusion of unrelated experiments, participants were given the opportunity to guess thenumber of gumballs in a glass jar. Following the methodology laid out in Edlund and colleagues’study (2009), we tested an experimental condition where only a laboratory-based treatment wasused (p. 639). In the experimental condition experimenters said: “Thanks for trying. I would liketo ask that you not tell anyone about this experiment to help keep guesses normal. Is that okaywith you?” After receiving verbal agreement the experimenters dismissed the participants fromthe laboratory. In the control condition, no verbal request was made (p. 637).

Additionally, we assessed how onerous the requirement was for the research alternative(number of papers) and the length of time required to complete the research participationrequirement.1

RESULTS

First, we examined the effectiveness of our treatment by collapsing across all universities anddid not find an overall effect of the laboratory-only treatment of crosstalk, χ2(1, N = 3607) =1.40, pFisher’s exact = .16 (see Table 1 for a detailed breakdown of the crosstalk prevalence ratesby condition). Further, condition order did not impact the results b = .63, S.E.b = .46, Wald(1 df ) = 2.11, p > .10.

An inspection of the means suggested that the baseline rate of crosstalk varied by institution.Therefore, we explored potential moderators of crosstalk by entering each moderator into sepa-rate logistic regression analyses. The nature of the alternative requirements (number of papers)interacted with experimental condition to predict the prevalence of crosstalk, such that higher

TABLE 1Rates of Crosstalk Across Conditions

Condition

University Control Experimental

East Tennessee State University 3/146 (2.0%) 3/83 (3.6%)Northern Illinois University 2/478 (0.5%) 1/243 (0.5%)Stephen F. Austin State University 2/403 (0.5%) 2/223 (1.0%)University of Alabama 5/392 (1.3%) 1/243 (0.5%)University of Florida 8/680 (1.2%) 2/716 (0.3%)

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f V

icto

ria]

at 1

5:17

18

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

184 THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

requirements led to increased rates of crosstalk in the control condition. Rates in the experimen-tal condition were similar regardless of the requirements (Table 1), b = .78, S.E.b = .40, Wald(1 df ) = 4.70, p = .03, Cramer’s V = 0.52. The length of time (number of hours to completethe requirement) required to complete the research requirement also interacted with experimen-tal condition to predict the prevalence of crosstalk such that higher participant requirements ledto increased rates of crosstalk in the control condition, while rates in the experimental condi-tion were similar to the rates found in universities that did not have high requirements; b = .45,S.E.b = .21, Wald (1 df ) = 4.73, p = .03, Cramer’s V = 0.53.

DISCUSSION

We investigated the prevalence of participant crosstalk across five different universities andfound evidence of crosstalk at each. Given that our institution characteristics (location, size, etc.)varied greatly, our data indicate the importance of crosstalk and of its consideration by psy-chology subject pool coordinators and researchers using introductory psychology participants.More importantly, the rate of crosstalk and success of its treatment varied between institu-tions. Specifically, institutions that demanded more out of their research participants had higherrates of crosstalk. Of course, the astute reader might note that several other variables appearto co-vary with institutional requirements (such as the size of the university’s student body orprestige of the university). However, we measured variables related to these institutional-levelvariables (e.g. the size of the introduction to psychology pool), and they did not impact the results.Additionally, Northern Illinois University (NIU) reported substantially less crosstalk in the cur-rent sample than was reported in Edlund and colleagues’ study (2009). We believe this differencefurther supports our argument that the requirements of introduction to psychology courses mat-ter; NIU’s research requirements are lower now than they were in Edlund and colleagues’ study(2009).

These findings have important implications for researchers collecting data from psychologysubject pools. First, crosstalk occurred at every university suggesting that researchers need tobe concerned with its detection–independent of institution. Second, crosstalk occurs more fre-quently in participant pools that demand more from their participants indicating the importanceof employing techniques to reduce crosstalk for researchers at universities with more demandingpsychology subject pools. In our study, we demonstrated that a laboratory-based treatment (con-sisting of one sentence) successfully reduced rates of crosstalk at these high demand schools.Future research should consider these findings and examine factors that will help reduce anddetect participant crosstalk.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Preliminary results were presented at the 2011 Southeastern Society of Social Psychologistsannual meeting. We wish to thank our many undergraduate research assistants who allowed us tocomplete this project.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f V

icto

ria]

at 1

5:17

18

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

EDLUND ET AL. 185

NOTE

1. We also assessed several other potential variables that varied between the universities. These were non-significant(and available from the authors upon request).

AUTHOR NOTES

John E. Edlund is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at the Rochester Institute of Technology. His research interestsinclude research methodology, romantic relationships, and personality. Austin Lee Nichols is an Assistant Professor at theUniversity of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain. His research interests include leadership, personality, impression management,and methodology. Bradley M. Okdie is affiliated with The Ohio State University at Newark. Rosanna E. Guadagno isaffiliated with the National Science Foundation. Cassie A. Eno is affiliated with Bellevue University. Jeremy D. Heideris affiliated with the Department of Psychology at Southeast Missouri State University. Edward J. Hansen is affiliatedwith Northern Illinois University. Brad J. Sagarin is affiliated with Northern Illinois University. Ginette Blackhartis affiliated with East Tennessee State University. Catherine A. Cottrell is affiliated with the New College of Florida.Kenneth Tyler Wilcox is affiliated with Rochester Institute of Technology.

REFERENCES

Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Gonzales, M. H. (1990). Methods of research in social psychology.New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Edlund, J. E., Sagarin, B. J, Skowronski, J. J., Johnson, S., & Kutter, J. (2009). Whatever happens in the laboratory staysin the laboratory: The prevalence and prevention of participant crosstalk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,35, 635–642. doi:10.1177/0146167208331255

Nichols, A. L. & Maner, J. (2008). The good-subject effect: Investigating participant demand characteristics. The Journalof General Psychology, 135(2), 151–165. doi:10.3200/GENP.135.2.151-166

Received November 2, 2013Accepted December 1, 2013

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f V

icto

ria]

at 1

5:17

18

Nov

embe

r 20

14