the role of gender in young children’s selective trust of ...molly a. schlesinger & rebekah a....

25
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hmep20 Download by: [University of California, Riverside Libraries] Date: 12 June 2017, At: 11:38 Media Psychology ISSN: 1521-3269 (Print) 1532-785X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hmep20 The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of Familiar STEM Characters Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of Familiar STEM Characters, Media Psychology To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2017.1328311 Published online: 12 Jun 2017. Submit your article to this journal View related articles View Crossmark data

Upload: others

Post on 24-Sep-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hmep20

Download by: [University of California, Riverside Libraries] Date: 12 June 2017, At: 11:38

Media Psychology

ISSN: 1521-3269 (Print) 1532-785X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hmep20

The Role of Gender in Young Children’s SelectiveTrust of Familiar STEM Characters

Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert

To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The Role of Gender inYoung Children’s Selective Trust of Familiar STEM Characters, Media Psychology

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2017.1328311

Published online: 12 Jun 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Page 2: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust ofFamiliar STEM CharactersMolly A. Schlesinger and Rebekah A. Richert

Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside, California, USA

ABSTRACTFemale characters are less likely to engage with science, technol-ogy, engineering, and math (STEM) content than male characterson young children’s television shows. The current study examinedhow preschool-aged children’s selective trust of male and femalecharacters to teach STEM differed by child gender, and how trustrelates to children’s character identification. Forty-eight 3- to 6-year-old children’s selective trust of male and female characters toteach STEM content, gendered character identification, and perso-nal interest in STEM activities was measured. Boys and girls hadsimilar interest in STEM and had greater identification with same-gender characters. Although boys had significantly greater trust inmale characters, girls had similar levels of trust in male and femalecharacters. Overall, children had greater trust of male characters toteach STEM content, but this effect was driven by boys, indicatingidentification and selective trust are related, but not identical con-structs. The discussion considers how representations of femaleand male characters on television may impact children’s trust ofeducational media characters to teach STEM.

Evenwith the increased presence of positive female characters on television duringthe last 40 years, women and girls are still underrepresented (Collins, 2011).Content analyses of United States television programming have reported femalecharacters tend to be more passive and helpless, display less cleverness andindependence, and speak less often than male characters (Baker & Raney, 2007;Biddle, 2016; Collins, 2011; Hentges & Case, 2013; Hetsroni & Lowenstein, 2014;Ryan, 2010; Steyer, 2014). In syndicated cartoons airing in the early 2000s in theUnited States, 30% of lead characters and 36% of minor characters were girls orwomen, and male characters were more likely to display cleverness and achievegoals than female characters (Aubrey & Harrison, 2004). Unpublished work hasfound that preschool television programs airing in 2015–2016 in the United Statesfeaturing male characters included significantly more school readiness skill

CONTACT Molly A. Schlesinger, [email protected] 900 University Ave., Department ofPsychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside, CA 92521, USA.This research was funded by a National Science Foundation Collaborative Research Grant (DRL-1252146) and a University ofCalifornia, Riverside Committee on Research grant to R. A. Richert. The manuscript was written with the support of aNational Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (GRFP) to M. A. Schlesinger. The authors thank Israel Flores forassisting with data collection and Rachel Flynn for support during early versions of project.

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGYhttps://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2017.1328311

© 2017 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

Page 3: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

curriculum, and male characters spoke more often than in shows with femalemain characters or ensemble casts (Biddle, 2016).

The underrepresentation of female characters is principally notable with tele-vision programs featuring STEM (science, technology, engineering, and mathe-matics) content. As of 2012, 31% of characters in U.S. television shows created foryoung children were female, and only 13% of female characters had careers inSTEM fields (Smith, Choueiti, Prescott, & Pieper, 2012). Otherwise, althoughresearch on STEM media is growing, there is very little research on televisionprograms for young children that teach STEM content (Richert & Schlesinger,2016; Schlesinger, Flynn, & Richert, 2016; Schroeder & Kirkorian, 2016). Becauserecent research has demonstrated that the trust children have in familiar char-acters to teach STEM relates to children’s learning of STEM concepts from thosecharacters (Schlesinger et al., 2016), and given the differences in gender represen-tations in children’s programming, the current study examined how children’strust in familiar characters to teach STEM related to identification with charactersand differed by character gender.

A focus on characters featured in television and video programming iswarranted because although interactive media use is increasing, televisionremains a huge part of preschoolers’ lives. By age 4, almost all children watchtelevision shows daily and about 80% additionally watch shows on mobiledevices (Kabali et al., 2015). With the increases in availability and variability inaccessing television programming, media characters and their related messagesabout gender roles continue to be a dominant part of young children’s lives.

Children’s developing perceptions of gender roles and gender stereotypes areimpacted by their exposure to messages about what is appropriate for their owngender and others (Bandura, & Bussey, 2004; Gerbner, Gross, Morgan,Signorielli, & Shanahan, 2002; Hentges & Case, 2013). Social cognitive theorypostulates children are more likely to mimic behaviors of and engage in obser-vational learning processes from same-gender models (Bandura, 1997; Bandura,& Bussey, 2004). Because under the umbrella of social cognitive theory, childrenare more likely to learn content from same-gender characters whether due togreater trust, identification, familiarity, or other social cognitive mechanisms, itis important to remember that female characters are less likely to achieve theirgoals, speak less, and aremore passive thanmale characters (Aubrey &Harrison,2004; Bandura, 1997; Bandura, & Bussey, 2004; Biddle, 2016; Keys, 2016). Thecurrent study considers how these portrayals relate to children’s selective trust ofcharacters, and could impact children’s learning educational content anddevelop their concepts of gender roles. Overall, the theoretical importance ofthis study is situated in the recognition that preschool-aged children can learnboth educational content and gender stereotypes from educational media char-acters, both of which impact children’s development. The implications of chil-dren having lower trust in female characters as sources of STEM information isevident in research about children’s selective trust.

2 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 4: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Selective trust

Selective trust is the development of children’s recognition of characteristics andtraits in others (Harris, 2012). Children depend on others to learn about theirworld; and much like adults, young children discriminate between who they canand cannot trust to provide accurate information. Although children refine theirabilities to make accurate judgments of others’ trustworthiness throughout devel-opment, by age 4, children are sensitive to characteristics and traits of potentialinformants, and evaluate who they can trust to provide relevant and accurateinformation, also known as selective trust (Harris, 2012; Woolley & Ghossainy,2013). The growing body of selective trust literature typically evaluates the traitsthat make an informant or character more “trustworthy,” with the goal ofexamining the social cognitive processes involved in young children’s evaluationsof others and how trust relates to learning. Selective trust is typically evaluated bypresenting two novel informants (or informants who the children have notencountered outside of a lab) or characters with distinct traits, and children areasked to choose which character they would ask if they wanted to learn moreabout a relevant domain. In the selective trust literature, the term “trust” refers to apreference or desire to learn from a particular character or informant.

This work has found 3- and 4-year-olds can identify reliable, unreliable, andignorant informants at above chance levels (Corriveau & Harris, 2009; Corriveau,Pickard, & Harris, 2011; Jaswal, McKercher, & VanderBorght, 2008; Koenig &Harris, 2005; Nurmsoo & Robinson, 2009). Studies that have presented childrenwith two informants who are each experts in unique domains (or one expert andsomeone with an unknown expert status) have indicated young children over-whelmingly claim the informants described as experts as having greater knowledgeabout their expert domain than would be expected by chance (Koenig & Jaswal,2011; Lutz & Keil, 2002). Overall, there is little evidence of a global or halo effect inyoung children’s trust (Koenig & Jaswal, 2011). The selective trust literatureprovides evidence that young children are adept at recognizing traits consistentwith trustworthiness (e.g., reliability, expertise), and children are more likely tolearn from a character or informant who displays positive or knowledgeable traits.

However, limited research has examined how gender relates to selective trust. Astudy addressing children’s gender-preferences and gender-stereotype awarenessfound that 4- to 6-year-old childrenweremore likely to trust and learn from same-gender than other-gender informants about objects in a gender-neutral color (Ma& Woolley, 2013). Other research has revealed 4- to 7-year-old children hadgreater trust of a same-gender informant only when that informant was consis-tently reliable (Taylor, 2013). In a study focused on 4- to 8-year-old children’s trustof male and female child informants with gender counter-stereotyped interestsand expertise, 4- and 5-year-olds had greater trust of same-gender informantsregardless of the informants’ interest and expertise (Boseovski, Hughes, & Miller,2016). When expertise was equal between same-gender and other-gender

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 3

Page 5: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

informants, 3- to 4-year-old children learned more often from same-genderinformants; however, when the other-gender informant had relevant expertisethat the same-gender informants did not, children learned from the other-genderinformants (Terrier, Bernard, Mercier, & Clément, 2016). Other research focusingon the impact of stereotypes indicated 3- to 5-year-old children have been morelikely to associate male-stereotyped activities with male experts; the same associa-tion was not found for female-stereotyped activities with female experts(Shenouda & Danovitch, 2013). Overall, the existing literature has indicated thatchildren initially gravitate toward same-gender informants when all other factorsare held constant; however, children will guide their selective trust by other traits(e.g., expertise, reliability) over more salient similarities (e.g., gender).

In sum, the selective trust literature has provided evidence that young childrenhave greater trust of knowledgeable and same gender characters. However, verylittle research has examined children’s trust of familiar media characters who aredynamic, have flexible personality traits, and whose knowledgeability differsdepending on the episode narrative and curriculum. This lack of research issurprising given that children readily learn from familiar media characters(Schlesinger et al., 2016). To fill a gap in the literature about children’s trust offamiliar media characters, from the perspective that there are limited competentfemale media characters teaching STEM, the current study examined if children’strust in familiar television characters as sources of STEM information varied bychild gender or character gender.

Two hypothesized patterns of relations could be derived from prior research.First, children could demonstrate a global same-gender preference, such that girlshave significantly higher trust of female characters and boys have significantlyhigher trust of male characters. This pattern would suggest that children believethe presentations of male and female characters’ STEM knowledgeability andexpertise are essentially “equal,” and therefore, children are basing characterpreference on gender-similarity (e.g., Boseovski, et al., 2016; Terrier et al., 2016)or other positive traits (e.g., identification). Second, children could demonstrate amale preference, such that children prefer male characters to teach them STEMcontent. The male preference would suggest that even at this young age childrenare sensitive to cultural messages that male characters are more likely to be expertsin STEM than female characters (e.g., Boseovski et al., 2016), even in circum-stances when both male and female characters are well matched in their time onscreen and curriculum expertise. Both pathways have implications for the rela-tions between trust and learning. Under the social cognitive theory framework,children are more likely to learn from models to whom they feel more similar;however, according to prior literature, children’s trust that a character will provideaccurate and relevant information likely plays a large role. Therefore, it is impor-tant to focus on children’s selective trust of characters, as children may be morelikely to learn from characters they trust. In order to examine potential

4 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 6: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

mechanisms through which children’s own gender relates to their trust in char-acters, the current study also examined children’s identification with characters.

Identification

In addition to gender-based expectations about expertise, identification (i.e.,perception of similarity) is an integral social cognitive aspect of observationallearning (Bandura, 1997); and it has been argued that identification with mediacharacters may lead to greater attention to and therefore greater learning fromthose characters (Calvert, Strong, Jacobs, & Conger, 2007; Richert, Robb, & Smith,2011; Schlesinger et al., 2016). Gender plays an important role in identification,especially for young children, as gender is one of the earliest salient features ofidentity (Kohlberg, 1996; Maccoby, 1998). Although the evidence is inconsistent,there is some evidence that preschool-aged children have greater attention to,learning from, and identification with members of the same-gender group, othertraits can influence identification beyond gender similarity, including perceptionsof intelligence or expertise (Bandura, & Bussey, 2004; Halim, Ruble, & Tamis-LeMonda, 2013; Hentges & Case, 2013; Hoffner, 1996; Lemish, 2010; Luecke-Aleska, Anderson, Collins, & Schmitt, 1995).

Perceptions of trust, expertise, or reliability may operate similarly to percep-tions of intelligence during early childhood, therefore, it is hypothesized that therewill be positive relations between identification and trust in the current study, suchthat childrenmay have stronger identification with characters in the gender groupthat they believe to be more trustworthy sources of information, reflecting thedesire to be like characters who have intelligence or knowledgeability, includingthose with STEM expertise. Therefore, the current study included measures ofcharacter identification to examine relations with selective trust.

STEM

The current study focused on STEM for several reasons. The United States isplacing increased emphasis on STEM education by integrating STEM lessonsinto preschool curriculum and educational television curriculum (CaliforniaDepartment of Education, 2013). STEM, by definition, is an interdisciplinarycollective of scientific, technological, engineering, and mathematics concepts,activities, and perspectives (Ejiwale, 2013). A primary concern of this study wasto capture STEM from a collectivist perspective, by both collapsing concepts thatfall under discrete domains and by including concepts that fall under multipleSTEM domains.

Although girls typically outperform boys in STEMdomains, gender stereotypesimplying girls are less interested and able to engage in STEMare still prevalent andimpact decisions to pursue STEM throughout the lifespan, leading to continuedgender imbalance in STEM fields (Aschbacher, Li, & Roth, 2010; Hyde, Lindberg,

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 5

Page 7: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Linn, Ellis, & Williams, 2008; National Science Foundation, 2014; Yazilitas,Svensson, de Vries, & Saharso, 2013). In addition to focusing on characters, thecurrent study also evaluated children’s interest in a variety of STEM activities toexamine if boys and girls have different interests in STEM during the preschoolyears, prior to beginning formal schooling when gendered interests become morepervasive (Maccoby, 1998). With female characters still underrepresented inchildren’s media and female characters less likely to be involved in STEM activ-ities, focusing on children’s trust of male and female familiar media characters toteach STEM curriculum is imperative for evaluating the impact media representa-tion has on young children.

The current study

The current study had multiple hypotheses. First, in an exploratory analysisexamining potential differences between children’s interest in STEM during thepreschool years, it was hypothesized that boys and girls would have similar levelsof interest in STEM. Second, examining how gender is involved in identification(or perceived similarity) to characters, it was hypothesized that children wouldhave stronger identification with same gender characters, than other-genderedcharacters; that is, highly gendered identification. Third, based on the lack ofcompetent female characters who teach STEM, it was hypothesized that childrenwould have greater trust in male characters, even when both male and femalecharacters are shown as having similar amounts of STEM knowledgeability andexpertise. Finally, due to the conceptual overlap between identification and trust, itwas hypothesized that identification and trust would be significantly related, andidentification may be the mechanism that explains children’s patterns of trust insame- or other-gender characters.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-six children were interviewed (56.3% female), who ranged in age from 3years, 9 months to 6 years, 11 months (M = 5.35, SD = 0.85). The race andethnicity breakdown of the sample was diverse, representing the population ofSouthern California: 29.7% European American, 14.1% Hispanic or LatinoAmerican, 4.7% African American, 3.2% Asian American, and 48.4% multi-ethnic. Of those identifying multiple races or ethnicities, 17.2% identified asboth Hispanic American and European American, 10.9% identified as bothAfrican American and European American, and remaining participants identifiedas African American/Asian American (4.7%), Asian American/EuropeanAmerican/Hispanic American (3.1%), African American/European American/Hispanic American (3.1%), African American/Hispanic American (1.5%),

6 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 8: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

European American/Hispanic American/Native American (1.5%), AfricanAmerican/Asian American/Hispanic American (1.5%), Asian American/Hispanic American (1.5%), Asian American/European American (1.5%), andHispanic American/Native American (1.5%). In terms of parents’ educationlevel, 6.1% had a high school diploma or general education diploma; 46.9% hadsome college, a vocational degree, or associate’s degree; 28.6% had bachelor’sdegrees; and 14.3% had advanced degrees. With regards to yearly income, 17.4%of families reported earning less than $29,999 per year, 23.9% of families earningbetween $30,000–49,999, 23.9% of families earning between $50,000–89,000,23.9% of families earning between $90,000–149,000, 4.7% of families earningmore than $150,000 yearly, and 6.5% not reporting.

Families were from Southern California, and were recruited through a labora-tory database, community flyers, and community events to visit the on-campuslaboratory for one 90-min session. To participate, English had to be the primarylanguage spoken in the home, but parents and children did not have to be nativeEnglish speakers. Children received a small toy for their time, and parents received$20 compensation for travel costs. Eight children were interviewed, but notincluded in the following analyses due to substantial missing data (four childrenasked to end the study early, four children displayed behavioral issues and couldnot complete the study). The final sample was 48 participants (Mage= 5.40, SD =0.82, 55% female) in the following analyses. Therewere no significant demographicdifferences between children who were included or dropped from analyses.

Materials

Children watched four 90-s video clips, each featuring an animated educationalmedia character. All four clips demonstrated the characters helping others bysolving difficult engineering-related problems, as well as displaying critical think-ing and highly-developed socioemotional skills. One clip featuredDora fromDorathe Explorer helping a friend cross a windy river (Calvert et al., 2007; Richert &Schlesinger, 2016). One clip featured Diego from Go Diego Go rescuing flightlessbirds from the ocean (Richert & Schlesinger, 2016). One clip featured Sid from Sidthe Science Kid getting a large stuffed animal onto a bed (Richert & Schlesinger,2016).One clip featuredGabriela from Sid the ScienceKid lifting a life-size truck offthe ground. Canonically, both Dora and Diego are well matched; they both speakSpanish, have anthropomorphized animal friends, regularly rescue animals, teacha range of STEM, problem-solving, verbal, and socio-emotional content, and air onNick Jr. Sid and Gabriela are similarly well matched, as inquisitive and prosocialscientists and problem solvers in their classroom and at home, and air on PBS-related channels. For the purposes of the analyses, Dora and Gabriela are groupedas female characters, and Sid and Diego are grouped as male characters.

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 7

Page 9: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Measures

STEM interestUsing a smiley face scale, children were asked how interested they were in learningabout eight STEM concepts on a +2 (like a lot, big smiley face) to 0 (neutral,straight line smiley face) to –2 (dislike a lot, big frowning face) smiley face scale.Children’s responses to each question were transformed to +1 (like a lot or like alittle) and 0 (neutral, dislike a little, dislike a lot), to capture positive responses.There were two at least two questions representing each STEM domain; however,some questions represented multiple categories. Concepts were drawn from acombination of educational television curriculum and state curriculum guidelines(California Department of Education, 2013). Participants were asked about howmuch they were interested in learning about animals, health, computers, phones,bicycles, counting, addition, and types of experimenting. Children’s positiveresponses to these eight questions were summed, creating a STEM interest scoreranging from 0 to 8 (Crohnbach’s α = .72), with higher scores indicating greaterinterest in STEM concepts and STEM learning.

Gendered identification. Identification with characters was evaluated usingSchlesinger et al.’s (2016) measure; children were verbally asked how much they(a) like, (b) were like, and (c) wanted to be like each character. Participants couldrespond a lot (2), a little (1), or not at all (0) to each question. The responses to allthree questions were averaged for both female characters (α = .81) and both malecharacters (α = .72), creating two identification scores, ranging from 0 (weakidentification) to 1 (moderate identification) to 2 (strong identification). An identi-fication difference score was created by subtracting the female character identifi-cation score from the male identification score. This resulted in a final genderedidentification score ranging from +2 (indicating stronger identification with malecharacters) to –2 (indicating stronger identification with female characters).Means closer to 0 indicate either weak identification generally or equally strongidentification with both genders.

Gendered selective trust. The measure of children’s selective trust in differentcharacters was adapted from and administered consistent with previous mea-sures of children’s selective trust of familiar characters, which typically evaluatewho young children would prefer to learn from or would ask to learn somethingnew (Boseovski et al., 2016; Schlesinger et al., 2016). Participants were shownimages the characters from the video clips and were told, “I am going to tell youabout different things these characters know about, and you tell me who youwould watch on TV to learn about those things.” Participants were then asked topick which character they would watch to help them learn about the eight STEMconcepts from the STEM interest measure first, second, third, and last, or not atall. Howmany times both male characters were selected first were summed for a

8 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 10: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

male character trust score, and how many times both female characters wereselected first were summed for a female character trust score, ranging from 0 to8, with higher scores indicating greater trust. A trust difference score was createdby subtracting the female character trust score from the male trust score. Thisresulted in a final gendered trust score ranging from +8 (indicating strongertrust with male characters) to –8 (indicating stronger trust with female char-acters). Means closer to 0 indicate similar levels of trust with both male andfemale characters.

Exposure. Because the clips were chosen from readily available children’sprograms, parents reported their children’s exposure to the television seriesfrom which the clips were drawn. Parents indicated how often their childwatched each of the characters (0 = never, 1 = once a month, 2 = 2–3 times amonth, 3 = once a week, 4 = 2–3 times a week, 5 = once a day, 6 =more than oncea day). An exposure difference score was created by subtracting the femalecharacter exposure from the male exposure (α = .77). This resulted in a finalgendered exposure score ranging from +6 (indicating greater exposure to themale characters) to –6 (indicating greater exposure to the female characters).Means closer to 0 indicate either little exposure generally or equal exposure tocharacters of both genders. Additionally, parents indicated whether their chil-dren would (1) or would not (0) recognize each character.

ProcedureThe study took place in a university laboratory testing room arranged like aliving room. After obtaining parental consent and child assent, parents com-pleted a survey with demographic and media exposure questions adjacent to thetesting room, where children and parents could talk to each other, but parentswere discouraged from disrupting the interview. Child participants providedratings of their personal STEM interest, then watched short video clips in whichthey were reintroduced to the familiar media characters, and played gamesevaluating identification and trust of familiar characters. In each video clip,the main characters displayed knowledgeability and expertise for solving engi-neering problems, mathematics abilities, scientific knowledgeability, and tech-nological tools. (Note: An analysis of children’s learning from these clips ispresented elsewhere [Richert & Schlesinger, 2016]; however, the measures pre-sented in this manuscript were designed to test the hypotheses related tocharacter perceptions and are thus the focus of the analyses.)

Results

Preliminary analyses first tested relations of all variables with child age, race andethnicity, character exposure, family income, and parent education. Analyses thenexamined if participants demonstrated gender-stereotyped STEM interests,

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 9

Page 11: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

followed by examining gendered identification. The primary analyses tested theimpact of selective trust by child gender, child age, and identification.

Preliminary analyses

There were no significant relations between exposure, STEM interest, characteridentification, and character trust with race and ethnicity,1 family income, andparent education. Therefore, race and ethnicity, family income, and parent educa-tion were excluded from further analyses. A correlation matrix for the variablescan be found in Table 1. For the full sample, age was positively correlated withgendered trust, r = .32, p = .027; the older children were the greater their trust ofmale characters. Similarly, gendered identification and gendered trust were posi-tively correlated, r = .40, p = .005; children with stronger identification with malecharacters and weaker identification with female characters had greater trust ofmale characters and weaker trust of female characters. For girls alone, there wereno significant relations between age, exposure, STEM interest, gendered identifi-cation, and gendered trust (see Table 2). For boys in the sample, age was positivelycorrelated with gendered trust, r = .51, p = .02, and STEM interest was negativelycorrelated with exposure, r = -.55, p = .02 (see Table 3). Therefore, age, identifica-tion, and gender will be considered in the final selective trust analyses.

Gendered exposure

Parents reported their children watched the television series a few times a month(female characters:M = 1.80, SD = 1.11; male characters:M = 1.58, SD = .95). Novariables were significantly correlated with exposure (see Table 1). Additionally, allparents noted their children would recognize both main characters from the clips.

An independent samples t test examined how boys’ and girls’ characterexposure differs. Boys and girls had similar amounts of exposure to the char-acters, t(47) = .57, p > .10, Cohen’s d = .21. Because there were no differences in

Table 1. Correlation Matrix (With Means and Standard Deviations) for Child Age, Gender, STEMInterest, Character Exposure, Identification, and Trust for the Full Sample.

1 2 3 4 5 6

5.40 6.02 –0.18 0.21 1.10 0.45Mean (SD) (0.82) (1.87) (0.47) (0.72) (3.48) (0.50)

1. Age —2. STEM interest 0.085 —3. Exposure 0.046 –0.174 —4. Identification 0.017 –0.001 –0.001 —5. Trust 0.317* –0.033 0.059 0.399** —6. Gender 0.127 –0.213 0.090 0.668*** 0.497*** —

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

10 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 12: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

exposure by participant gender and exposure was unrelated to any other vari-ables, exposure was not included in the following analyses.

STEM interest

Overall, children had relatively high personal interest in STEM (M = 6.02,SD = 1.87). Independent samples t tests were run to examine differences inSTEM interest by child gender, and indicated no significant gender differ-ences in children’s STEM interest, t(47) = 1.48, p = .15, Cohen’s d = .81. Inother words, there was no evidence that during this period in development(preschool and kindergarten) girls are less interested in engaging in STEMthan boys. Because STEM interest was not significantly related to any othervariables, STEM interest was not considered in further analyses.

Gendered identification

To examine if participants had similar identification with male and femalecharacters, or if their identification was skewed towards their own gender, One-sample t tests were run for boys and girls separately (see Figure 1). If identificationscores do not differ from 0, children have similar levels of identification with maleand female characters. A one sample t test indicated girls’ identification scoreswere significantly different from 0, t(27) = 2.126, p = .043, Cohen’s d = .82, such

Table 2. Correlation Matrix (With Means and Standard Deviations) for Child Age, STEM Interest,Character Exposure, Identification, and Trust for Girls.

1 2 3 4 5

5.31 6.38 –0.22 –0.22 –0.44Mean (SD) (0.82) (1.70) (0.52) (0.54) (3.29)

1. Age —2. STEM Interest 0.253 —3. Exposure –0.018 0.270 —4. Identification –0.186 0.184 –0.188 —5. Trust 0.148 0.148 –0.078 0.158 —

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 3. Correlation Matrix (With Means and Standard Deviations) for Child Age, STEM Interest,Character Exposure, Identification, and Trust for Boys.

1 2 3 4 5

5.52 5.60 –0.13 0.73 3.00Mean (SD) (0.82) (2.01) (0.43) (053) (2.74)

1. Age —2. STEM Interest –0.027 —3. Exposure 0.096 0.540* —4. Identification 0.023 0.210 0.075 —5. Trust 0.513* 0.017 0.183 0.021 —

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 11

Page 13: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Table4.

Multip

leRegression

Predictin

gTrustby

ParticipantAg

e,ParticipantGender,Identification,

andaGenderby

IdentificationInteraction.

95%

CIforβ

Variable

BBSE

βt

pLower

Upp

erFchange

Adjusted

R2R2

change

Mod

el1

(Con

stant)

–6.173

3.210

–1.923

0.061

–12.631

0.285

5.25*

0.081

0.101

Age

1.347

0.588

0.317*

2.292

0.026

0.165

2.529

Mod

el2

(Con

stant)

–6.270

2.837

–2.210

0.032

–11.980

–0.560

14.19***

0.283

0.212

Age

1.098

0.524

0.258*

2.096

0.042

0.044

2.151

Gender

3.218

0.854

0.464***

3.767

0.000

1.498

4.937

Mod

el3

(Con

stant)

–6.344

2.842

–2.232

0.031

–12.069

–0.620

0.86

0.280

0.013

Age

1.142

0.527

0.269*

2.169

0.035

0.082

2.203

Gender

2.499

1.155

0.361*

2.164

0.036

0.173

4.825

Identification

0.741

0.800

0.153

0.927

0.359

–0.870

2.353

Mod

el4

(Con

stant)

–6.442

2.859

–2.253

0.029

–12.204

–0.680

0.57

0.273

0.009

Age

1.184

0.532

0.279*

2.225

0.031

0.111

2.256

Gender

2.864

1.258

0.413*

2.277

0.028

0.329

5.399

Identification

1.287

1.083

0.266

1.188

0.241

–0.895

3.468

Gender×Identification

–1.218

1.620

–0.179

–0.752

0.456

–4.484

2.047

Note:*p

<.05,

**p<.01,

***p

<.001.

12 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 14: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

that girls had significantly stronger identification with female characters thanwould be expected by chance. Boys’ identification scores also were significantlydifferent from 0, t(21) = 6.40, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 2.79, such that boys hadsignificantly stronger identification with male characters than would be expectedby chance.

To fully examine differences between boys’ and girls’ identification scores,an Independent Samples t-test indicated boys and girls had significantlydifferent identification scores from each other, t(47) = 6.16, p < .001,Cohen’s d = 1.78. Overall, boys’ identification scores (M = 0.74, SD = 0.54)represent stronger identification with male characters, and girls’ identifica-tion scores (M = -0.22, SD = 0.54) represent stronger identification withfemale characters (see Figure 1).

Gendered trust

To examine if participants had similar trust of male and female characters, or iftheir trust was skewed towards their gender or male characters, One-sample ttests were run for boys and girls separately (see Figure 2). The one sample t testindicated girls’ trust scores did not significantly differ from 0, t(26) = .70, p =.49, Cohen’s d = .28; girls had similar levels of trust for male and femalecharacters. Conversely, boys’ trust scores were significantly different from 0,t(21) = 5.13, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 2.24; boys had significantly stronger trust ofmale characters than would be expected by chance. An independent samples ttest indicated boys and girls had significantly different trust scores, t(47) =

Figure 1. Gendered identification by participant gender, with scores higher than 0 indicatinggreater identification with male characters and scores lower than 0 indicating greater identifica-tion with female characters.

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 13

Page 15: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

3.08, p = .03, Cohen’s d = .43; specifically, boys’ (M = 2.52, SD = 3.53) trustscores were significantly higher than girls’ (M = -0.44, SD = 3.29) trust scores(see Figure 2).

As noted above, both age and identification were significantly related to trust.To examine the significant relations between identification and trust with childgender, a hierarchical multiple regression was conducted. Model 1 added theeffect of age, and was significant, R2adjusted = .081, F (1, 47) = 5.25, p = .026. Agewas significantly positively predictive of gendered trust, the older children were,the greater their trust of male characters. Age accounted for 8.1% of the variancein selective gendered trust.

Model 2 added the effect of participant gender, and was significantly betterthan Model 1, R2adjusted = .28, R2change = .212, F (1, 46) = 14.19, p < .001. Genderwas significantly positively predictive of gendered trust; boys’ selective trust was0.464 points greater than girls’. Age was still significantly positively predictive.Model 2 accounted for an additional 21.2% of the variance in children’s gen-dered trust.

Model 3 added the effect of gendered identification, and was not signifi-cantly better than the previous models, R2adjusted = .28, R2change = .013, F (1, 45)= .86, p > .10. Both age and gender were still positively predictive of trust.Model 4 added the effect of a participant gender by identification interaction,and was not significantly better than the previous models, R2adjusted = .27,R2change = .009, F (1, 44) = .57, p > .10. Both age and gender were still positivelypredictive of trust.

Overall, all four models accounted for 31.4% of the variance in children’sgendered trust. Although identification and trust were significantly

Figure 2. Gendered trust by participant gender, with scores higher than 0 indicating greater trustwith male characters and scores lower than 0 indicating greater trust with female characters.

14 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 16: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

correlated, age and child gender explain significantly more of the variance inchildren’s trust patterns than identification.

Discussion

The goal of the current study was to examine how participant and charactergender relate to children’s identification with animated characters from populartelevision programs that include STEM curriculum, and children’s selective trustin those characters to teach STEM content. Findings suggested that identifica-tion and trust have different patterns of relations with gender, such that girls andboys both demonstrated a same-gender preference for character identification,and only boys had significantly higher trust of male characters, whereas girls hadsimilar trust of male and female characters. Of note is that children’s interest inSTEM activities did not differ by child gender and was unrelated to characterperception variables. The current data do not allow us to determine a) if this lackof relation between children’s interests and character perceptions indicates thatchildren’s character perceptions are unrelated to their own interests during thepreschool years, b) if this lack of relation is true globally during the preschoolyears or is only true in regards to children’s STEM interests and perceptionsabout characters in STEM domains, or c) if preschoolers’ vary more broadly intheir STEM interests than is represented in the sample in the current study.Thus, future research should continue to examine the role that media charactersplay in the development of children’s interest in STEM activities. To contributeto this line of inquiry, our discussion focuses on the relations between children’strust in characters as sources of STEM information and their identification withthose characters within a sample of children who had relatively similar and highdegrees of interest in STEM themselves.

Selective trust

In regards to selective trust, we initially hypothesized two possible patterns basedon prior research examining how gender relates to children’s trust: either childrenwould display a same-gender preference, basing trust on gender similarity, orchildren would have stronger trust of male characters, reflecting cultural stereo-types that male characters are more likely to have STEM competence (Boseovskiet al., 2016; Terrier et al., 2016). Overall, children had positive selective trust scores,indicating children generally had greater trust of male characters; but whencharacter trust was further unpacked, we found this was driven by boys’ signifi-cantly greater trust of male characters, compared to girls having similar trust inmale and female characters.

If we examined character trust in relation to gender alone, it would seemchildren recognized that male characters are more generally portrayed asSTEM experts and that female characters are less often portrayed as competent

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 15

Page 17: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

in STEM. These patterns suggest differences in how gender-stereotyped infor-mation in children’s programs becomes incorporated into children’s perspec-tives of characters depending on children’s identification with on screencharacters. Importantly, all the characters in the clips displayed similar com-petency and knowledge for solving engineering problems, as well as otherSTEM content. Thus, children’s responses likely reflect a stereotype built froma mass media context in which female characters are less likely than malecharacters to be clever, successful, and have more positive engagements withSTEM (Aubrey & Harrison, 2004).

Identification

An important pathway through which exposure to characters relates to children’sself-perceptions and learning is the extent to which children identify with thecharacters (Bandura, 1997). In the current study, children had somewhat strongeridentification with same gender than other gender characters, consistent withsocial cognitive theory (Bandura, & Bussey, 2004). This finding somewhat con-trasts results from previous studies suggesting no gender differences in children’sidentification with male characters (Schlesinger et al., 2016), but some genderdifferences for female characters, such as girls having stronger identification with afemale character than boys (Calvert et al., 2007). However, neither previous studyexplicitly compared characters of different genders. Our findings that children hadgreater identification with same-gender characters suggest that during early child-hood, character identification may reflect ingroup biases constructed of easy torecognize, salient cues (e.g., gender), rather than other, less salient informationadditionally used to construct identification later in development (e.g., spokenlanguage, interests, goals; Maccoby, 1988).

The possibility that identification was a proxy for ingroup bias is important fortwo reasons, one methodological and one theoretical. Methodologically, if thecharacter identification variable used in the current study is a proxy for preschoo-lers’ ingroup biases, thismay statistically account for ingroup biases in the analysesof selective trust. Given prior research on gender and trust, it is possible ouridentification variable reflected same-group favoritism (Taylor, 2013). Taylor(2013) noted that when children did not choose an other-gender reliable infor-mant over a same-gender unreliable informant, it was not because they believedthe other gender informants to be untrustworthy, but rather children likely had aningroup bias toward same-gender reliable informants. Although in the hierarch-ical multiple regression, identification did not account for a significant amount ofthe variance in selective trust, identification was included in the character trustanalyses to account for the impact of identification as well as possible ingroupbiases. If identification is a proxy for same group favoritism, the current studystatistically accounted for the contribution of same group favoritism in children’strust perceptions.

16 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 18: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

On a theoretical level, character identification during early childhood may be avery different construct than later in development, and may not predict behaviorbeyond early childhood or short-term learning from characters. Research onchildren’s emotional relationships with media characters has indicated childrenengage in “break ups” with characters, which is likely caused by some of the samesocial cognitive processes that inform how much children like, are like, and wantto be like particular characters (Bond&Calvert, 2014; Brunick, Putnam,McGarry,Richards, & Calvert, 2016). As such, children’s identification with characters mayshift from being based on salient category membership like gender, to less salientsimilarity cues or categories throughout development. Future research shouldexamine how children’s qualitative character identification differs at uniquetimes in development and how the construct of character identification mayinclude different factors at specific times in development.

In all likelihood, children’s character identification during the preschoolyears is driven by both an ingroup positivity bias (Taylor, 2013) and thedesire to be like others of the same gender (Bandura, & Bussey, 2004). Thus,the evidence of the same-gender bias in identification operating differentlythan the gendered differences in children’s trust in the characters reveal someimportant psychological processes in how young children conceptualizeeducational media characters, particularly the unique relations between theconstructs of identification and trust.

Trust and Identification

To summarize the patterns above, girls had high identification and selective trustin female characters and boys had high identification and selective trust in malecharacters. Thus, on the surface, the appropriate pieces seem to be in place forobservational learning (Bandura, 1997; Bandura, & Bussey, 2004). When girlsare presented with female characters who are teaching STEM content, girlsidentify with those characters and trust them as sources of STEM information,just as they would with male characters. Similarly, when boys are presented withmale characters who are teaching STEM content, boys identify with thosecharacters and trust them as sources of information. Especially with femalecharacters, identification may further drive selective trust, in that childrenwith stronger identification may develop greater character trust. We hypothe-sized selective trust and identification may demonstrate similar patterns withingender because previous literature has speculated relations between identifica-tion and trust, however, little empirical work has found consistent relationsbetween identification and trust (Bond & Calvert, 2014; Richards & Calvert,2016; Richert et al., 2011; Schlesinger et al., 2016).

The strongly positive relations between identification and selective trustbetween gender, and nonsignificant relations within genders points to the needfor further study of how these constructs relate in early childhood and at different

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 17

Page 19: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

developmental periods. Identification and trust clearly overlap in some circum-stances. One possibility is that the two constructs overlap in their initial formation,but become differentiated over the course of development and through exposureto diverse media characters. In other words, children initially may be drawn tosame-gender characters (e.g., through salient cues about same-gender groups), butunique aspects of individual characters (e.g., competency, reliability, or other lesssalient similarity cues) may impact children’s reevaluations of trust and identifica-tion upon repeated exposure.

The inconsistent correlations between character trust and identification whenboys’ and girls’ variables are examined separately and together also demonstratethe necessity to continue to study how these processes relate to each other and tochildren’s learning from characters during early childhood. Based on cultivationtheory (Gerbner et al., 2002; Hentges & Case, 2013), one primary concern andapplication of these findings is that they may be revealing the way childrenperceive female characters, female peers, and young women. The boys in thisstudy had less positive views of female characters and were less likely to perceivethose characters as STEM experts. The girls in the study did not demonstrate thesame stereotyped expectations. Based on cultivation theory, both boys’ and girls’beliefs about gender roles will develop through exposure to messages on televisionprograms about appropriate behaviors for boys and girls, along with otherinfluences in children’s lives. Overtime, children’s beliefs about gender roles willreflect the behaviors of the characters in their favorite shows, and children mayembody the stereotypes and make assumptions about people in other-gendergroups based on those media messages. These behaviors and beliefs can influencesocial, cognitive, and emotional development and decisions throughout the life-span, contributing to the lack of diversity in STEM fields (Aschbacher, et al., 2010;Gerbner et al., 2002; Hentges & Chase, 2013; National Science Foundation, 2014;Yazilitas et al., 2013).

Exposure

A related question is how do differences in exposure relate to children’s selectivetrust in characters. This study was not designed to examine the construct ofexposure, but we can speculate as to the role exposure may play in children’sdevelopment of trust based on two pieces of information. First, all children werefamiliar with the characters in the study before entering the lab. The quantitativemeasure of exposure (e.g., how often does your child watch this program) was notsignificantly related to character trust or identification, suggesting amount ofexposure to the characters either a) did not relate to character perceptions, b)did not vary greatly enough in this sample to explain variations in characterperception, or c) had reached some critical level for the establishment of characterperceptions. Recent studies have indicated it is central to understand the qualityand types of interactions with characters, rather than quantity of exposure, in

18 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 20: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

order to understand how children develop their perceptions of and emotionalrelationships with characters (Bond & Calvert, 2014; Brunick et al., 2016).

Second, the relatively brief exposure to female characters solving difficultproblems did not result in children having equivalent levels of trust in the femaleandmale characters. A related limitation is that a pretest assessment of children’sbeliefs about these characters was not taken. Although it is unlikely a 90-secondreintroduction to familiar characters would change children’s beliefs about thecharacters (Schlesinger et al., 2016), the fact that following short exposureschildren’s trust was not equivalent for female and male characters makes thediscrepancy in representations of female STEM experts in children’s program-ming all the more problematic, as once preschoolers make judgments aboutunreliable informants, they are unlikely to revise their judgments even afteradditional exposure to the same informants displaying expertise or reliability(Nurmsoo & Robinson, 2009).

Similarly, if preschoolers are exposed to a previously reliable informant beingunreliable, even if there is a good explanation for unreliability (e.g., the informanthad been blindfolded), they are unlikely to trust that informant in the future evenwhen the cause of the unreliability is no longer applicable (e.g., not wearing ablindfold anymore) (Jaswal, et al., 2008; Nurmsoo&Robinson, 2009). Research onchildren’s revisions to how they trust informants has practical implications fortelevision producers. Children’s already established character trust before enteringthe lab was not revised by brief exposure to STEM competency in the currentstudy, and resistance to revising character perceptions may operate similarly out-side of the lab: Preschoolers may be unlikely to trust typically incompetentcharacters who display expertise in a few sporadic episodes, but children mayrevise their trust for inquisitive, and often ignorant, characters who display someknowledgeability in the future. Although, in the current study we were unable toevaluate revisions in children’s selective trust, we did find trust related to othervariables.

Limitations

We have addressed limitations where relevant throughout the discussion; a fewothers warrant additional consideration. A primary limitation is drawn from thedichotomous and stringent ways in which gender is presented in children’sprograms in the United States, and the limited presentation of female characters.Our stimuli, assessments, and analyses focused on an inflexible gender binarythat is exclusively presented in young children’s television programming in theUnited States, and our study did not include questions about general genderstereotypes or about how children conceptualize characters who do not adhereto mainstream categories. In the current study, no parents reported theirchildren as gender nonconforming, agender, transitioning, or displaying incon-sistencies about gender category membership. We have not found mainstream

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 19

Page 21: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

educational television programs targeted to preschool-aged children in theUnited States that incorporate characters outside of the binary gender categories.However, related to our findings that even young children recognize culturalstereotypes of more negative perceptions of girls’ STEM knowledge, parents andprogram creators should consider the potential influences of diverse and flexiblegender representations on children’s perceptions of characters or themselves.Similarly, our study did not evaluate children’s interest in learning or trust ofcharacters to teach other curriculum typically present in young children’seducational media, such as literacy or socio-emotional skills. Although resultscould have differed if we focused on stereotypically feminine or gender neutraldomains, the lack of competent female characters on television generally makesit unlikely that the results would differ; future research should examine howchildren’s trust of media characters differ by domain, particularly important forcharacters who teach a variety of curricula.

Although our sample was diverse in race and ethnicity, increasing general-izability of our results in the United States, due to the degree of diversity, we didnot have large enough groups to analyze how race, ethnicity, or cultural identitymay differentially impact the variables in this study. The social cognitive processesthrough which underrepresented populations in the United States perceive char-acters of diverse races and ethnicities has similar implications as the current study(e.g., poor representations in media), and future research should examine thesefactors (Ryan, 2010).

An additional limitation focuses on the STEM exemplars used in this study.The items meant to capture STEM had wide inclusivity, falling under multipledomains. However, the examples were by no means inclusive of all STEMactivities and concepts, and did not explicitly capture any STEM content pre-sented in the short video clips children watched in the lab, but were reflective ofactivities and learning domains children recognize and are typically presented intelevision series that teach STEM content. A final limitation is that the currentstudy focused on four comparable familiar media characters, but was notinclusive of all male and female media characters who teach STEM. The sampleof characters were chosen because their shows included STEM content and airednew episodes when the study was developed, but as a related limitation bothtelevision programs feature STEM content as well as other curriculum fordeveloping other school readiness skills. As the population of screen-basedcharacters is ever evolving and growing, research should continue to considerthe effects of gender-based messages (whether implied or explicit) on childrenlearning from characters, self-perceptions, and beliefs about people theyencounter in their daily lives outside of the screen.

20 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 22: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Conclusions

In conclusion, despite a lack of gender differences in STEM interest during thepreschool years, preschool-aged children are aware of gender stereotypes inmediaabout STEM, and boys hold those stereotypes more strongly than girls. We foundevidence that children overall have greater trust of male characters to teach STEMcontent, and trust is related to identification. Although, paths between trust ininformants and learning are complex, under circumstances in which trust extendsto learning, fewer positive portrayals of female characters as STEM experts couldhave negative ramifications for girls (Corriveau & Harris, 2009; Corriveau, et al.,2011; Koenig & Harris, 2005; Koenig & Jaswal, 2011; Schlesinger et al., 2016). Wefound evidence that girls want to learn from female characters just asmuch as theywant to learn from male characters; however, if female characters continue to beportrayed as less knowledgeable on educational television programs, girls may losetrust for female characters over time, while continuing to identify with the femalecharacters, which may result in girls assimilating the female characters’ negativetraits into their own self-perceptions. Future research should examine how trustand identification relate to learning, specifically for girls and children in under-represented groups who are poorly represented on children’s television; andmedia procedures should continue to concentrate on increasing the quality ofrepresentation of television characters.

Note

1. Due to the emphasis of Dora and Diego representing Latino, Hispanic, and Spanish-origincultures in the clips and on the respective television series, numerous one-way analyses ofvariance (ANOVA) were conducted examining any differences in exposure and characterbelief variables by character ethnicity. One-way ANOVAs with character ethnicity as theindependent variable and the exposure difference score, F(1,47) = 0.63, p > .10, identificationdifference score, F(1,47) = 0.45, p > .10, and trust difference score, F(1,47) = 0.56, p > .10,indicated no significant differences in character beliefs or exposure by character ethnicity.

References

Aubrey, J. S., & Harrison, K. (2004). The gender-role content of children’s favorite televisionprograms and its links to their gender-related perceptions. Media Psychology, 6(2), 111–146. doi:10.1207/s1532785xmep0602_1

Aschbacher, P. R., Li, E., & Roth, E. J. (2010). Is science me? High school students’ identities,participation and aspirations in science, engineering, and medicine. Journal of Research inScience Teaching, 47(5), 564–582. doi:10.1002/tea.20353

Baker, K., & Raney, A. A. (2007). Equally super?: Gender-role stereotyping of superheroes inchildren’s animated programs. Mass Communication & Society, 10(1), 25–41. doi:10.1080/15205430709337003

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.Bandura, A., & Bussey, K. (2004). On broadening the cognitive, motivational, and socio-

structural scope of theorizing about gender development and functioning: Comment on

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 21

Page 23: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Martin, Ruble, and Szkrybalo. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 691–701, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.691

Biddle, A. M. (2016, June). Dora, Doc, and Daniel Tiger: What is educational televisionteaching preschoolers about gender? Paper presented at the 46th annual meeting of theJean Piaget Society, Chicago, IL.

Bond, B. J., & Calvert, S. L. (2014). A model and measure of US parents’ perceptions of youngchildren’s parasocial relationships. Journal of Children and Media, 8(3), 286–304.doi:10.1080/17482798.2014.890948

Boseovski, J. J., Hughes, C., & Miller, S. E. (2016). Expertise in unexpected places: Children’sacceptance of information from gender counter-stereotypical experts. Journal ofExperimental Child Psychology, 141, 161–176. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.002

Brunick, K. L., Putnam, M. M., McGarry, L. E., Richards, M. N., & Calvert, S. L. (2016).Children’s future parasocial relationships with media characters: The age of intelligentcharacters. Journal of Children and Media, 10(2), 181–190. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1127839

California Department of Education. (2013). Alignment of the preschool learning foundations.Retrieved from http://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/psalignment.asp

Calvert, S. L., Strong, B. L., Jacobs, E. L., & Conger, E. E. (2007). Interaction and participationfor young Hispanic and Caucasian girls’ and boys’ learning of media content. MediaPsychology, 9(2), 431–445. doi:10.1080/15213260701291379

Collins, R. L. (2011). Content analysis of gender roles in media: Where are we now and whereshould we go?. Sex Roles, 64(3), 290–298. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9929-5

Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Preschoolers continue to trust a more accurateinformant 1 week after exposure to accuracy information. Developmental Science, 12(1),188–193. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00763.x

Corriveau, K. H., Pickard, K., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Young children’s selective trust ininformants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1179–1187. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0321

Ejiwale, J. A. (2013). Barriers to successful implementation of STEM education. Journal ofEducation and Learning, 7(2), 63–74. doi:10.11591/edulearn.v7i2.220

Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., Signorielli, N., & Shanahan, J. (2002). Growing up withtelevision: Cultivation processes. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advancesin theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 43–67). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Halim, M. L., Ruble, D. N., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2013). Four-year-olds’ beliefs abouthow others regard males and females. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(1),128–135. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02084.x

Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.

Hentges, B., & Case, K. (2013). Gender representations on Disney Channel, CartoonNetwork, and Nickelodeon broadcasts in the United States. Journal of Children andMedia, 7(3), 319–333. doi:10.1080/17482798.2012.729150

Hetsroni, A., & Lowenstein, H. (2014). Is she an expert or just a woman? Gender differencesin the presentation of experts in TV talk shows. Sex Roles, 70(9–10), 376–386. doi:10.1007/s11199-014-0370-z

Hoffner, C. (1996). Children’s wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favoritetelevision characters. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 40(3), 389–402.doi:10.1080/08838159609364360

Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gendersimilarities characterize math performance. Science, 321, 494–495. Retrieved from http://www.sciencemag.org/content/321/5888/494.full

22 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT

Page 24: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Jaswal, V. K., McKercher, D. A., & VanderBorght, M. (2008). Limitations on reliability:Regularity rules in the English plural and past tense. Child Development, 79(3), 750–760.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01155.x

Kabali, H. K., Irigoyen, M. M., Nunez-Davis, R., Budacki, J. G., Mohanty, S. H., Leister, K. P.,& Bonner, R. L. (2015). Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children.Pediatrics, 136(6), 1–13. doi:10.1542/peds.2015-2151.

Keys, J. (2016). Doc McStuffins and Dora the Explorer: Representations of gender, race, andclass in US animation. Journal of Children and Media, 10(3), 355–368. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1127835

Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P.L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers.Child Development, 76, 1261–1277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x

Koenig, M. A., & Jaswal, V. K. (2011). Characterizing children’s expectations about expertiseand incompetence: Halo or pitchfork effects? Child Development, 82(5), 1634–1647.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01618.x

Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts andattitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–172). Stanford,CA: Stanford University Press.

Lemish, D. (2010). Screening gender on children’s television: The views of producers around theworld. New York, NY: Routledge.

Luecke-Aleska, D., Anderson, D. R., Collins, P. A., & Schmitt, K. (1995). Gender constancyand television viewing. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 773–780. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.773

Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (2002). Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. ChildDevelopment, 73(4), 1073–1084. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00458

Ma, L., & Woolley, J. D. (2013). Young children’s sensitivity to speaker gender when learningfrom others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(1), 100–119. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.638687

Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA:Belknap Press.

National Science Foundation, (2014). Science and engineering indicators 2014. Arlington VA:Author.

Nurmsoo, E., & Robinson, E. J. (2009). Children’s trust in previously inaccurate informantswho were well or poorly informed: When past errors can be excused. Child Development,80(1), 23–27. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x

Richards, M. N., & Calvert, S. L. (2016). Parent versus child report of young children’sparasocial relationships in the United States. Journal of Children and Media, 10(4), 462–480. doi:10.1080/17482798.2016.1157502

Richert, R. A., Robb, M., & Smith, E. (2011). Media as social partners: The social nature ofyoung children’s learning from screen media. Child Development, 82(1), 82–95.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01542.x

Richert, R. A., & Schlesinger, M. A. (2016). The role of fantasy-reality distinctions in preschoolers’learning from educational video. Infant and Child Development. Advance online publication.doi:10.1002/icd.2009

Ryan, E. L. (2010). Dora the Explorer: Empowering preschoolers, girls, and Latinas. Journal ofBroadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(1), 54–68. doi:10.1080/08838150903550394

Schlesinger, M. A., Flynn, R. M., & Richert, R. A. (2016). US preschoolers’ trust of andlearning from media characters. Journal of Children and Media, 10(3), 321–340.doi:10.1080/17482798.2016.1162184

MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 23

Page 25: The Role of Gender in Young Children’s Selective Trust of ...Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert To cite this article: Molly A. Schlesinger & Rebekah A. Richert (2017): The

Schroeder, E. L., & Kirkorian, H. L. (2016). When seeing is better than doing: Preschoolers’transfer of STEM skills using touchscreen games. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–12.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01377

Shenouda, C. K., & Danovitch, J. H. (2013). Does a male nurse know about football?American and Egyptian children’s understanding of gender and expertise. Journal ofCognition and Culture, 13(3–4), 231–254. doi:10.1163/15685373-12342093

Smith, S., Choueiti, M., Prescott, A., & Pieper, K. (2012). Gender role & occupations: A look atcharacter attributes and job-related aspirations in film and television. Los Angeles, CA:Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media.

Steyer, I. (2014). Gender representations in children’s media and their influence. Campus-Wide Information Systems, 31(2–3), 171–180. doi:10.1108/CWIS-11-2013-0065

Taylor, M. G. (2013). Gender influences on children’s selective trust of adult testimony.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 672–690. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.003

Terrier, N., Bernard, S., Mercier, H., & Clément, F. (2016). Visual access trumps gender in 3-and4-year-old children’s endorsement of testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,146, 223–230. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.002

Woolley, J. D., & Ghossainy, E. M. (2013). Revisiting the fantasy-reality distinction: Childrenas naïve skeptics. Child Development, 84(5), 1496–1510. doi:10.1111/cdev.12081

Yazilitas, D., Svensson, J., de Vries, G., & Saharso, S. (2013). Gendered study choice: Aliterature review. A review of theory and research into the unequal representation of maleand female students in mathematics, science, and technology. Educational Research andEvaluation, 19(6), 525–545. doi:10.1080/13803611.2013.803931

24 M. A. SCHLESINGER AND R. A. RICHERT