the role of prolactin in pregnancy-induced changes in food intake

85
The Role of Prolactin in Pregnancy-Induced Changes in Food Intake Natasha Danielle Stenhouse A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Biomedical Science with Honours at the University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. 2012

Upload: others

Post on 03-Feb-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

The Role of Prolactin in Pregnancy-Induced

Changes in Food Intake

Natasha Danielle Stenhouse

A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of

Bachelor of Biomedical Science with Honours

at the University of Otago, Dunedin,

New Zealand.

2012

ii

Abstract

Pregnancy is associated with a significant increase in food intake. This

occurs in order to supply the mother with sufficient energy to support both

herself and the developing fetus throughout pregnancy and lactation when

metabolic demand is high. This increase in food intake occurs despite an

elevation in circulating levels of the appetite-suppressing hormone leptin.

Hence, pregnancy is considered a state of leptin resistance. The hormone

prolactin, the levels of which are significantly increased during pregnancy,

is thought to be involved in increasing food intake during pregnancy.

Exogenous administration of prolactin in rodents is associated with

increased food intake and reduced response to leptin. As such, it is possible

that the pregnancy-induced increases in prolactin may have the same effect.

We hypothesised that prolactin acts centrally to mediate the changes in food

intake that occur during pregnancy. Thus, the aim of this experiment was to

measure food intake during pregnancy in mice that specifically lack

prolactin receptors in the brain.

To characterise food intake in these mice, food intake and bodyweight

were measured daily in non-pregnant, pregnant and lactating neuron-

specific prolactin receptor knockout mice and controls. This neuron-specific

prolactin receptor knockout model has lost prolactin receptors throughout

iii

the brain and thus exhibits impaired central prolactin response.

Immunohistochemistry for phosphorylated signal transducer and activator

of transcription 5 (pSTAT5), a marker of prolactin receptor activation, was

performed in order to assess the success of the knockout animal. Finally,

because there appeared to be a difference in bodyweight in the non-pregnant

knockout animals, non-pregnant mice were subjected to a fast and refeed

protocol and then placed on a high fat diet in order to further characterise

their food intake regulatory systems.

Food intake increased significantly over pregnancy and was higher still

during lactation in both animal groups. Bodyweight also increased over

pregnancy, then remained stable during the lactation period. Food intake

was significantly higher throughout pregnancy in the knockout animals

compared with the controls. Unexpectedly, food intake and bodyweight

were significantly higher in the non-pregnant knockout animals compared

with the controls suggesting a basal difference in bodyweight regulation.

Immunohistochemistry for pSTAT5 demonstrated significant, but not

complete loss, of the prolactin receptor throughout the brain, specifically in

the arcuate nucleus and medial preoptic area. There was no significant

difference between the animal groups in the fast and refeed protocol. The

high fat diet data demonstrated that the knockout animals on a high fat diet

gained significantly more weight than the control animals on a control diet.

iv

The results obtained did not support our hypothesis that prolactin action

in the brain critically mediates changes in food intake during pregnancy.

However, interpretation of these results was complicated by the non-

pregnant animal results and the incomplete nature of the knockout. It

remains possible the remaining prolactin-responsive neurons in the brain

may mediate prolactin action to stimulate food intake. This latter

interpretation is supported by the changes in bodyweight in the non-

pregnant knockout animals. Despite the failure to support or disprove our

hypothesis, a significant step towards characterisation of this knockout

model was made.

v

Acknowledgements

I can't believe it's almost over. This has been such a big year and there

are so many people who have been so important in making sure this was a

success that I really wouldn't be able to name all of them by name.

First off, I would like to thank my supervisors Sharon and Dave for

everything, I really can't fit it all in here. Thank you Dave, for your

unending wisdom and guidance throughout the year. You were always

available even when in a different hemisphere! And Sharon for the patience

and support you've shown throughout this whole process. And for always

having the answer.

To the rest of the Grattan Lab, for answering endless questions about any

number of things throughout the year and for patiently listening to all my

presentations! Special thanks to Ilona. You always had time for me no

matter how busy you were. Whether I had questions, concerns, proof

reading that I needed help with, it didn't matter you were always available.

To the rest of the CNE and the department of Anatomy for supporting

my project and providing constant entertainment during the long hours

spent in the lab.

vi

I would like to thank my amazing family for tolerating me throughout

this process. I'm sure it has been as trying for you all as it has for me. All

my friends, Papi for all the help formatting and proofreading and Jessie, for

always being there and making sure I stayed semi-sane and stopped me

rewriting my whole thesis! I'd like to thank my Dad for his proofreading

skills, , Matt who has provided endless support and pretended to know what

I was talking about when even I didn't and Allegra for being the greatest. I

couldn't have done it without all you guys!

Natasha

vii

Contents

Abstract ii  

Acknowledgements v  

Contents vii  

List of Figures xi  

List of Abbreviations 1  

1.0  Introduction 3  1.1: Overview ........................................................................................... 3  

1.2 Food intake regulation in the non-pregnant individual ...................... 3  

1.3 Energy Homeostasis During Pregnancy ............................................. 5  

1.4 Prolactin .............................................................................................. 6  

1.5 The role of prolactin in leptin resistance ............................................ 9  

2.0  Methods 12  2.1 Overview .......................................................................................... 12  

2.2 Generation of Conditional Knockout Mice ...................................... 12  

2.3 Animals ............................................................................................. 13  

2.4: Experiment 1: Food intake, body weight and litter growth

during different reproductive states ............................................. 14  

2.4.1 Estrous Cycle Assessment ................................................... 14  

2.5: Experiment 2: Confirmation of prolactin receptor deletion in

the brain ........................................................................................ 15  

viii

2.5.1 Immunohistochemistry for pSTAT5 ................................... 17  

2.5.2 Immunohistochemistry for GFP .......................................... 19  

2.5.3 Immunohistochemical Analysis .......................................... 20  

2.6 Experiment 3: Postfasting feeding behaviours and response to

a high fat diet ................................................................................ 20  

2.6.1 Fast and Refeed Study ......................................................... 21  

2.6.2 High Fat Diet Study ............................................................ 21  

2.7 Statistical Analysis ........................................................................... 22  

3.0  Results 23  3.1 Experiment 1 .................................................................................... 23  

3.1.1 Neuron-specific prolactin receptor knockout mice

have abnormal estrus cycles ........................................... 23  

3.1.2 Food intake and bodyweight during different

reproductive states .......................................................... 26  

3.1.3 Litter Weight Gain During Lactation .................................. 27  

3.2 Experiment 2 .................................................................................... 31  

3.2.1 pSTAT5 Immunohistochemistry ......................................... 31  

3.2.2 GFP Immunohistochemistry ............................................... 31  

3.3 Experiment 3 .................................................................................... 39  

3.3.1 Fast and Refeed: .................................................................. 39  

3.3.2 Response to a High Fat Diet ................................................ 39  

4.0  Discussion 42  

ix

4.1 Overview .......................................................................................... 42  

4.2 Success of the Knockout and Immunohistochemistry for

pSTAT5: ....................................................................................... 43  

4.3 Knockout mice have abnormal estrous cyclicity .............................. 46  

4.4 Food Intake and Bodyweight During Different Reproductive

States ............................................................................................ 49  

4.5 Litter Characteristics ........................................................................ 54  

4.6 Response to a Fast and Refeed protocol and a High Fat Diet

study ............................................................................................. 56  

4.7 Limitations ........................................................................................ 57  

4.8 Conclusions ...................................................................................... 59  

5.0  References 60  

Appendices 69  Appendix A: Fixatives ............................................................................ 69  

0.2 M Phosphate Buffer   (1 litre) ..................................... 69  

4% (w/v) Paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M PB .................................... 69  

30% Sucrose Solution .................................................................. 70  

Appendix B: Solutions Used For Immunhistochemistry ........................ 71  

Cryoprotectant   (1 litre) ............................................... 71  

10x Tris-buffered saline (TBS) stock solution   (2 litres) ......... 71  

1x TBS Working Solution   (1 litre) ...................... 71  

TBS+0.3% Triton X   (100 ml) ......................................... 72  

x

Blocking solution   (100 ml) ............................................ 72  

Hydrogen peroxide solution   (50 ml) ................................. 72  

Avidin-Biotin complex solution   (25 ml) ............................. 72  

xi

List of Figures

Figure 1.1 Diagrammatic representation of prolactin receptor

signalling via the JAK/STAT pathway ................................ 7

Figure 3.1 Estrus cyclicity ................................................................... 25

Figure 3.2 Food intake during different reproductive states ............... 28

Figure 3.3 Bodyweight during different reproductive states ............... 29

Figure 3.4 Litter growth during lactation ............................................ 30

Figure 3.5 Prolactin-induced pSTAT5 in the medial preoptic

area ..................................................................................... 34

Figure 3.6 Prolactin-induced pSTAT5 in the paraventricular

nucleus of the hypothalamus ............................................. 35

Figure 3.7 Prolactin-induced pSTAT5 in the arcuate nucleus of

the hypothalamus ............................................................... 36

Figure 3.8 Prolactin-induced pSTAT5 in the ventromedial

nucleus of the hypothalamus ............................................. 37

Figure 3.9 Green fluorescent protein in the paraventricular

nucleus of the hypothalamus ............................................. 38

Figure 3.10 Animal response to a fast and refeed protocol ................... 40

Figure 3.11 Animal response to a high fat diet ..................................... 41

xii

Figure 4.1 Diagrammatic representation of short loop negative

feedback of prolactin secretion in the arcuate

nucleus ............................................................................... 48

1

List of Abbreviations

ABC avidin-biotin-complex

AGRP agouti-related peptide

ARC arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus

DAB diaminobenzidine

DPX 1,3-diethyl-8-phenylxanthine

GFP green-fluorescent protein

HFD high fat diet

i.p. intraperitoneal

JAK janus kinase

kg kilogram

M molar

ME median eminence

mg milligram

ml millilitre

mm millimetre

MPOA medial preoptic area

mRNA messenger ribonucleic acid

NPY neuropeptide Y

OC optic chiasm

POMC pro-opiomelanocortin

2

PVN paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus

STAT3 signal transducer and activator of transcription 3

STAT5 signal transducer and activator of transcription 5

pSTAT5 phosphorylated signal transducer and activator of

transcription 5

TBS tris-buffered saline

TIDA tuberoinfudibular dopaminergic

VMH ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus

3V third ventricle

ɑ-MSH ɑ-melanocyte stimulating hormone

µl microlitre

µm micrometre

3

1.0 Introduction

1.1: Overview

The expression “eating for two” is often used to justify the significant

increase in food intake associated with pregnancy. Pregnant females must

not only ensure their own energy needs are met, they must also supply

nutrients to the growing fetus and produce energy stores that can be used

during the metabolically-demanding period of lactation. However, this

change in food intake begins long before it is required by any significant

alteration in metabolic activity. Thus, early pregnancy is characterised by a

period of positive energy balance. The increase in food intake during

pregnancy occurs despite an increase in levels of the appetite suppressing

hormone leptin. This suggests that the body must somehow override the

normal food intake regulatory mechanisms in order to facilitate the state of

pregnancy-induced hyperphagia (1).

1.2 Food intake regulation in the non-pregnant individual

In the non-pregnant individual, energy balance is tightly regulated

through the actions of various homeostatic mechanisms. Long-term energy

4

regulation is achieved through the activity of hormones such as leptin and

insulin. Mice unable to produce leptin (ob/ob mice) and those lacking a

functional leptin receptor (db/db) have considerably increased food intake

and are grossly obese (2-4). These animals demonstrate the critical role

leptin plays in food intake regulation.

Leptin, which is produced in the periphery, acts centrally to reduce food

intake. It is secreted predominantly by adipocytes in proportion to the fat

mass of an individual, with the stomach and the placenta producing lesser

amounts (5, 6). Leptin's food intake regulatory actions are mediated through

a number of different pathways. In the arcuate nucleus (ARC) of the

hypothalamus, a key site of food intake regulation, leptin acts directly upon

pro-opiomelanocortin neurons (POMC) and neuropeptide-Y (NPY)/agouti-

related peptide (AGRP) expressing neurons (7). These anatomically distinct

neuronal populations have opposing effects. POMC neuron activation

causes release of the anorectic peptide ɑ-melanocyte-stimulating hormone

(ɑ-MSH) whilst activation of NPY/AGRP expressing neurons increases the

release of these two orexigenic neuropeptides. Leptin stimulates POMC

neuron activation and thus increases ɑ-MSH levels while simultaneously

inhibiting NPY/AGRP neurons (8). This causes an overall increase in

anorectic and decrease in orexigenic signals respectively, with a consequent

decrease in food intake.

5

In addition to leptin, there are other factors involved in the regulation of

food intake. Both the stomach and the gut release hormones on a daily basis

which act, in the short term, to either stimulate or suppress appetite (9).

These include hormones such as cholecystokinin, an anorectic peptide that

is released from the gut (9) and ghrelin, a centrally-acting orexigenic

peptide that is released from the stomach (10). These signals combine to

form an integrated system which ensures energy balance is appropriately

regulated in the non-pregnant individual.

1.3 Energy Homeostasis During Pregnancy

A pregnancy-associated increase in food intake has been well established

in both rats and mice (11-15). A number of alterations in the homeostatic

mechanisms responsible for regulating energy balance facilitate the

development of this pregnancy-induced hyperphagia (16). Pregnancy is

associated with a paradoxical increase in leptin levels (11, 17, 18). In a non-

pregnant individual, this state of hyperleptinemia would cause a reduction

in food intake. However, pregnancy is thought to induce a state of central

leptin resistance, which means that these leptin-induced appetite-

suppressive effects are lost. This pregnancy-induced leptin resistance is

associated with a decrease in the leptin-induced activation of signal

transducer and activator of transcription 3 (STAT3) and downregulation of

6

leptin receptor mRNA in the hypothalamus (11, 19). This decrease in leptin

responsiveness means that hyperphagia can be maintained throughout

pregnancy and excess energy consumed stored as fat deposits. The

mechanism by which pregnancy-induced leptin resistance develops is not

clear, but it is thought to be caused by changes in hormone levels during

pregnancy.

1.4 Prolactin

During pregnancy, there is an increase in prolactin receptor activity due

to increasing levels of prolactin and its placental homologues, placental

lactogens (20). Prolactin plays a wide range of roles in the body, including

development and maintenance of the mammary glands (21), stimulation of

neurogenesis (22) and maternal behavioural adaptations (23). It has also

been implicated in this state of pregnancy-induced central leptin resistance.

The hormone prolactin is also thought to play a role in the regulation of

energy balance. Prolactin receptors have been identified in a number of

tissue types such as adipose and liver tissue (24), and in areas of the brain

involved in maintaining energy homeostasis including the ARC,

paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) and ventromedial

nucleus of the hypothalamus (VMH) (25). Prolactin exerts its effects

7

through signalling via the JAK/STAT pathway (see figure 1.1) (26). When

prolactin binds to its receptor, receptor-associated JAK proteins are

activated. These JAK proteins phosphorylate signal transducer and activator

of transcription 5 (STAT5) molecules. These phosphorylated STAT5

(pSTAT5) molecules dimerise, then translocate to the nucleus where they

regulate gene transcription.

Figure 1.1: Prolactin signalling via the JAK/STAT pathway. Adapted

from Grattan, 2002, Reproduction (27)

8

Numerous studies have supported a role for prolactin in increasing food

intake in both birds (28-30) and mammals (31, 32). Noel and Woodside (32)

demonstrated that in female rats, prolactin administered peripherally and

centrally increased food intake independent of the dose. Further research

confirmed the results of the above study but whether the observed effect

was dose dependent is inconclusive (33, 34). It seems that the appearance of

these prolactin-induced hyperphagic effects depends on the length of

treatment time. Prolactin treatment for seven days or less has been shown to

be ineffective. In order for a significant hyperphagic effect to be displayed,

a treatment period of nine (35) or more (31, 36) days is required.

Furthermore, constantly elevated prolactin levels are more effective at

inducing hyperphagia than phasic prolactin administration (34). The route

of prolactin administration must also be considered, as it may influence the

mechanism by which this hyperphagia occurs. Indeed, research has shown

that prolactin administered peripherally can induce acyclicity in female rats

(37). Acyclicity is also associated with increased food intake.

Initially, it was proposed that the hyperphagic effects of prolactin were

possibly mediated through activation of receptors for an alternative peptide.

However, injection of an anti-prolactin receptor antibody antagonised this

prolactin-induced increase in food intake suggesting that this effect was a

direct consequence of prolactin receptor activation (30). While it is

9

hypothesised that prolactin promotes hyperphagia through altering the leptin

response in both pregnant and non-pregnant animals, the exact role

prolactin plays in this process has not yet been determined.

1.5 The role of prolactin in leptin resistance

Attenuation of the leptin response after simultaneous treatment with both

prolactin and leptin in non-pregnant rats has further supported the role of

prolactin in the induction of leptin resistance. Central leptin injections (4

µg) were sufficient to considerably reduce food intake in saline-infused rats.

In contrast, this treatment did not reduce food intake in rats subjected to

chronic infusions of prolactin (5µg/h) for 10 days. There was also a

significant decrease in pSTAT3 expression (a marker of leptin response) in

the prolactin treated rats compared with control (34). The appetite

suppressive effects of leptin were also lost in pseudopregnant rats treated

with chronic prolactin infusion further supporting this idea of prolactin-

induced central leptin resistance (38).

It has been proposed that prolactin may impair the leptin response by

inducing downregulation of leptin receptors in the brain. Decreased leptin

receptor mRNA expression was seen in leptin resistant male rats following

prolonged leptin infusion (39). Additionally, Ladyman and Grattan

10

demonstrated that leptin mRNA expression in the VMH is significantly

lower during pregnancy but whether this is a direct result of prolactin

signalling is yet to be determined (19). It is also a possibility that prolactin

may directly interfere with leptin signalling. Prolactin acts to increase

expression of suppressor of cytokine signalling 3 molecule (SOCS3) (40,

41). SOCS3 expression causes subsequent downregulation of STAT

signalling and consequently, impairs leptin signalling which is dependent on

STAT3 activation (42). However, both of these theories require neurons that

are dually responsive to prolactin and leptin. Prolactin receptors have been

identified in a number of neurons in the VMH and paraventricular nucleus

(PVN) but colocalisation of leptin receptors on these neurons has not yet

been demonstrated.

There is significant evidence suggesting that prolactin can induce

changes in food intake. As such, we hypothesised that prolactin is critically

involved in causing leptin resistance and therefore increasing food intake

during pregnancy. Assessing food intake in pregnant, prolactin-receptor

knockout animals would help clarify the role of prolactin in these changes

but global loss of the prolactin-receptor causes infertility. Recent advances

in transgenic techniques have facilitated the development of a novel,

neuron-specific prolactin receptor knockout mouse. The aim of this project

was to measure food intake during pregnancy in both neuron-specific

11

prolactin receptor knockout and control mice. If prolactin plays an

important role in pregnancy-related central leptin resistance, then the

hyperphagic state that would normally occur during pregnancy would not be

observed.

12

2.0 Methods

2.1 Overview

This thesis was comprised of three experimental components. The aim of

the first component was to assess daily food intake, body weight and litter

weight (where applicable) in non-pregnant, pregnant and lactating neuron-

specific prolactin receptor knockout animals and controls. The second

experiment was to confirm loss of the prolactin receptor in the brain of

these mice to validate the experimental model. This was achieved through

immunohistochemistry for pSTAT5 as a marker of functional prolactin

response. Based on initial observations in experiment 1 that suggested

appetite might be different in the knockout mice, a third experiment was

added to the project. The third experiment was to examine the post-fasting

feeding behaviours of animals and changes in body weight when fed a high

fat diet in non-pregnant neuron-specific prolactin receptor knockout animals

and controls.

2.2 Generation of Conditional Knockout Mice

Animals used in the experiments were CamK-II-Cre;Prlrflox/flox adult

female mice. These animals were generated using two different transgenic

13

strains. A female prolactin receptor floxed mouse (Prlrflox/flox), in which the

critical coding region for the prolactin receptor gene was flanked by loxP

sites, was mated with a CamKII-Cre expressing male animal, which

expresses cre-recombinase neuron-specifically (43).A second round of

mating between the heterozygous CamKII-Cre;Prlrflox/wt and Prlrflox/flox

generated animals which were homozygous for the floxed gene and either

cre-positive or cre-negative. These cre-positive mice would undergo cre-

mediated recombination of the coding sequence between the loxP sites and

thus functional loss of the prolactin receptor in cells expressing the CamKII

promoter. Control mice were homozygotic for the floxed gene but did not

express cre. The floxed prolactin receptor construct was such that when

recombined by cre, it would activate green fluorescent protein (GFP), as an

indicator of successful recombination.

2.3 Animals

Animals were individually housed in controlled temperature (22 ± 1°C)

and lighting (12L:12D cycles, with lights-on at 0600 h). All animals were

allowed free access to food (unless otherwise stated) and water. All animal

manipulations and protocols required for this thesis were approved by the

University of Otago Committee on Ethics in the Care and Use of Laboratory

Animals (Ethics Number 106/10).

14

2.4: Experiment 1: Food intake, body weight and litter growth

during different reproductive states

The food intake and body weight of female, virgin mice were monitored

daily using a FX300i Wp scale (range 0-320g, accuracy 0.001g, M&D

Company Ltd). Estrous cyclicity was also monitored using daily vaginal

smearing. After 14 days, females were paired with male mice in order to

achieve pregnancy and were checked daily for the presence of a vaginal

plug. Appearance of a vaginal plug was used to indicate pregnancy day 1.

Food intake and body weight were measured throughout gestation until

parturition on day 19. Litters were normalised to five pups and

measurements including bodyweight, food intake and pup weight

recommenced six days post-delivery in order to monitor growth and food

intake during lactation without interfering with establishment of maternal

behaviour. Measurements were taken until weaning at 21 days postpartum.

2.4.1 Estrous Cycle Assessment

The estrous cycle was assessed through daily vaginal smearing between

0900 and 1000 hours. This involved insertion of a small wire loop into the

vaginal opening of a mouse which was gently rotated to collect cells from

the vaginal epithelium. Any displaced cells collected on the loop were then

15

transferred to a glass slide. The slide was then stained with 3 drops of

0.05% Toluidine blue and left for 1 minute. The cells were then examined

under a light microscope at low magnification. The stage of the estrous

cycle was based on epithelial cell morphology and distribution using the

following guidelines:

Proestrus: Predominantly round, nucleated epithelial cells arranged

in clusters.

Estrus: Large numbers of enucleated, cornified epithelial cells.

Metestrus: Cornified epithelial cells and large numbers of small,

granular leukocytes.

Diestrus: Small number of epithelial cells and/or leukocytes.

2.5: Experiment 2: Confirmation of prolactin receptor deletion

in the brain

At the conclusion of experiment 1, all pups were culled and the female

mice left for 1-3 weeks before the brains were collected for

immunohistochemistry for prolactin-induced pSTAT5 as a marker of

functional prolactin response in the brain. These animals were subjected to a

16

series of subcutaneous bromocriptine (5 mg/ml, 200 µl volume) injections

twenty four hours, twelve hours and two hours prior to perfusion in order to

suppress endogenous prolactin. Forty minutes before perfusion, animals

were administered prolactin (5mg/kg) via intraperitoneal (i.p) injection.

Mice were then anesthetised using a single i.p. injection of sodium

pentobarbitone (15 mg/ml, 150 µl volume). Once loss of the pedal

withdrawal reflex indicated that the animal was deeply anesthetized, a small

incision was made in the abdomen just below the final rib exposing the

diaphragm. The diaphragm was then pierced using surgical scissors and the

rib cage removed allowing access to the heart. A 22 gauge needle was then

inserted into the left ventricle of the heart and 20 mls of 4%

paraformaldehyde (Appendix A) injected over two minutes. Rigidity of the

body was used to indicate successful perfusion. Brains were collected and

post-fixed for 1 hour in the same fixative then placed in 30% sucrose

(Appendix A) in phosphate buffered saline (Appendix A) at 4oC until they

sank. They were then flash frozen using powdered dry ice and stored at -

80oC until tissue processing.

The brain stem and cerebellum were removed from the posterior aspect

of the mouse brain with a single cut through the level of the pons and the

brain was mounted cut surface down in TissueTek® (O.C.T.

Compound;Sakura Finetek USA Inc., CA, USA) onto a cryostat chuck.

17

Thirty µm thick coronal sections were sliced at -21°C in three series, each

containing sections 90 µm apart. Slices were collected from the beginning

of the medial preoptic area (MPOA) (0.26 mm relative to Bregma) to the

end of the VMH (approximately -2.06 mm relative to Bregma) and placed

in a 24 well multiplate (44). Sections were placed in cryoprotectant

(Appendix B) and refrigerated at -20°C until further processing.

2.5.1 Immunohistochemistry for pSTAT5

Immunohistochemistry allows for visualisation of a protein of interest

through utilising the binding properties of an antibody conjugated to a

visual marker to a specific antigen. A tissue specimen is exposed to primary

antibody specific for the desired protein. A biotinylated secondary antibody

is then added which binds to the primary antibody amplifying the signal and

this complex is then visualised with a streptavidin complex bound to

horseradish peroxidase, using diaminobenzidine (DAB) as the colour

reagent. Appendix B describes the details of all buffer and solution

protocols used in the immunohistochemical process.

One series of sections was processed for immunohistochemistry for

pSTAT5. All steps were carried out on an orbital shaker unless otherwise

stated. Sections were washed in 1x tris-buffered saline (TBS; Appendix B)

18

6 times for 10 minutes each, to remove any remaining cryoprotectant. Any

further wash steps involved 3 times 10 minute TBS washes. Antigen

retrieval was then achieved through transferring sections into just boiled

0.01 M Tris (pH 10) for 5 minutes. Sections were then cooled for 10

minutes on the bench and placed in blocking solution (see Appendix B) for

10 minutes and then washed. The sections were then incubated for 36 hours

at 4oC in 1 ml of blocking solution (Appendix B) containing the primary

antibody (rabbit anti-pSTAT5, tyr 649, Cell Signalling Technology) at a

1:1000 dilution and 2% normal goat serum. Following this, sections were

washed and then incubated at room temperature for 90 minutes in

biotinylated goat anti-rabbit antibody (1:300 dilution, BA-1000, Vector

Laboratories, Inc., CA, USA). This solution was washed off and sections

incubated in Vectastain Elite avidin-biotin complex (ABC) solution (Vector

Laboratories, Inc., CA, USA; Appendix B). After 60 minutes the sections

were washed again and then reacted with nickel-enhanced DAB (Vector

Laboratories, Inc., CA, USA). The DAB reaction progress was monitored

under a light microscope to ensure sufficient visualisation of the desired

proteins. A final wash was performed. The sections were then float-

mounted onto ATS-coated slides in TBS+0.3% TritonX and left to dry

overnight. Slides were dehydrated using graded alcohol (3 minutes per step)

and then two 3-minute xylene incubations. Finally, a few drops of 1,3-

19

diethyl-8-phenylxanthine (DPX; Merck (N.Z.) Ltd, N.Z.) were added and

the slide coverslipped and left to dry.

2.5.2 Immunohistochemistry for GFP

A second series of sections was processed for immunohistochemistry for

green fluorescent protein. All steps took place on an orbital shaker at room

temperature except where stated. Sections were washed in TBS six times for

10 minutes each to remove any residual cryoprotectant. All subsequent

wash steps involve 3 washes in TBS for ten minutes each. Tissue was

incubated, without shaking, in 0.3% H2O2 for 10 minutes and washed.

Sections were placed in blocking solution (Appendix B) for 1 hour then

directly transferred into 2 ml of primary antibody (1:20,000 dilution, anti-

GFP rabbit polyclonal [A6455], Invitrogen) diluted in blocking buffer for

72 hours at 4oC then washed. Following this, sections were transferred into

secondary antibody (1:200 dilution, biotinylated goat anti-rabbit, Vector

BA-1000) in TBS+0.3% TritonX for 1 hour and washed. While the sections

were incubating, the ABC (Vector Laboratories, Inc., CA, USA; Appendix

B) solution was made and left at room temperature for 30 minutes. Sections

were then transferred to the ABC solution for 60 minutes then washed.

Tissue was reacted with DAB complex and the reaction monitored under a

light microscope. Following this, the sections were placed in a final wash.

20

Sections were then float-mounted, dehydrated and coverslipped using the

procedure detailed above.

2.5.3 Immunohistochemical Analysis

All immunohistochemical analysis was purely qualitative with no formal

quantitative analysis performed. Each section was photographed using an

Olympus AX70 research microscope (10x objective) with a digital camera

attached. For the pSTAT5 immunohistochemical staining, four different

brain regions were assessed: the MPOA, PVN, ARC and VMH. Each region

was defined using the mouse brain atlas (44). Each of these regions in a

knockout animal was compared to a similar area in a control animal to

examine the differential pSTAT5 expression. A similar procedure was used

to assess GFP staining however only the PVN was assessed in these slides

due to difficulties with tissue sections.

2.6 Experiment 3: Postfasting feeding behaviours and response

to a high fat diet

The original goal of our experiments was to describe food intake and

bodyweight changes in neuron-specific prolactin receptor knockout animals

and controls. However, based on initial observations from experiment 1 that

21

the non-pregnant knockout animals displayed higher bodyweight and food

intake compared with control, a third experiment was added. In order to

gain further insight into the possible mechanisms behind these increases in

bodyweight and food intake in our non-pregnant knockout animals a fast

and refeed study and a high fat diet study were performed (45).

2.6.1 Fast and Refeed Study

Fourteen female mice (6 controls and 8 knockouts) were fasted for 24

hours. Food was returned and food intake and body weight measurements

taken daily for seven days post-fast.

2.6.2 High Fat Diet Study

Mice included in the fast and refeed study and an additional 8 mice were

placed on either a high fat (45% kilojoules from fat, D12451, Research

Diets, NJ, USA) diet (HFD) (control n=5, knockout n=7) or a control diet

(10% kilojoules from fat, D1245B, Research Diets, NJ, USA) (control n=5,

knockout n=5). Food intake and body weight were measured daily for five

days. After this initial period the measurements were taken every three days

for a further three weeks.

22

2.7 Statistical Analysis

Daily food intake and bodyweight in non-pregnant animals and

cumulative litter weight gain from day 7 to day 19 was assessed using a

paired, Students t-test. Data collected for daily food intake and bodyweight

during pregnancy and lactation and litter weight over lactation were

analysed using repeated measures ANOVA to assess the effect of

interaction, time and animal group. Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used

where necessary. One way ANOVA with a Dunnet post-hoc test was then

performed on daily food intake and bodyweight data during pregnancy and

lactation for each animal group to determine the point in time at which these

parameters changed. Grubbs outliers test was used to assess whether there

were any significantly outlying data points in cumulative litter weight gain

from day 7 to day 19 data. Food intake post high fat diet administration was

analysed using a two-way ANOVA. Bonferroni post-hoc tests were

performed where appropriate. In all instances, results were deemed

statistically significant at P value <0.05.

23

3.0 Results

3.1 Experiment 1

3.1.1 Neuron-specific prolactin receptor knockout mice have abnormal

estrus cycles

Time spent in each stage of the estrous cycle was different between the

two knockout and control animals (figure 3.1A). Both groups spent 8% of

time in proestrus. However, time spent in estrus and in metestrus and

diestrus differed. The control animals spent considerably more time in

estrus (34.0%) compared with the knockout animals (11%). By contrast, the

knockout animals spent the majority of their time in met/diestrus (81.4%)

while the control animals spent 57.0% of time met/diestrus. This abnormal

estrous cyclicity was indicative of a pseudopregnancy state.

There was considerable disparity between the knockout and control

groups in the number of animals that were housed with a male mouse and

achieved pregnancy (figure 3.1B). In the wildtype group, 72.7% (16 of 22)

of animals that were mated became pregnant. This percentage decreased in

the knockout animals with only 42.8% (12 of 28) of the animals house with

a male achieving pregnancy. It also took significantly longer for knockout

24

animals to get pregnant (19 days) compared with the controls (12 days)

(figure 3.1C).

25

Figure 3.1: A: Columns represent percentage of time spent in each stage of the estrus cycle over a 14 day period (n=9 per group). B: Columns represent percentage of animals that were housed with a breeder male that achieved pregnancy (n control= 22, n knockout= 28). C: Columns represent mean±SEM number of days from placing a female with a breeder male to pregnancy (n control= 22, n knockout= 28). * p<0.05. Animals that did not get pregnant after 4 weeks were allocated a value of 28. Individual points represent individual animal values.

A

B

Proestrus Estrus Metestrus/Diestrus0

20

40

60

80

100

ControlKnockout

Stage of Estrous Cycle

% o

f Tim

e

C

Control Knockout0

10

20

30 *

Num

ber

of d

ays

to p

regn

ancy

Control Knockout0

20

40

60

80

% a

nim

als

hous

ed w

ith a

mal

e th

atac

hiev

ed p

regn

ancy

26

3.1.2 Food intake and bodyweight during different reproductive states

Mean food intake over the fourteen day period in non-pregnant control

animals was 3.7±0.05 g. Unexpectedly, food intake was significantly higher

in our knockout animals with mean food intake in this group 4.0±0.04 g

(figure 3.2A). During pregnancy, food intake increased significantly over

the course of pregnancy with food intake significantly higher by day 10 in

the control and day 15 in the knockout animals. Furthermore there was a

significant effect of genotype between the two animal groups with the

knockouts eating significantly more overall during pregnancy than the

controls (figure 3.2B).

There was no overall difference in food intake during lactation between

the knockout and control groups (figure 3.2C). However, food intake did

change over time, with food intake in the control animals starting to

increase significantly compared to day 6 of lactation by day 12. In contrast,

food intake was not significantly different from day 6 at any point during

lactation in the knockout group.

Bodyweight was significantly higher in the non-pregnant knockout

animals compared with controls (figure 3.3A). During pregnancy,

bodyweight increased dramatically in both animal groups with the control

27

animals displaying significantly increased bodyweight by day 11 of

pregnancy and the knockout animals by day 12 (3.3A). There was no

significant difference between the knockout and control animals in overall

bodyweight gain during this time. From day 6 of lactation, bodyweight

remained relatively stable in both animal groups (3.3B). Again, no

significant difference in overall weight gain between the two animal groups

was found.

3.1.3 Litter Weight Gain During Lactation

During the experiment, the subjective impression was that the litter

produced by the knockout animals had significantly lower growth rates

during lactation compared with control. While there was a significant

difference in litter growth pattern over time, there was no significant overall

difference in growth between the two groups (figure 3.4A). Furthermore,

there was no significant difference in cumulative litter weight gain from day

7 to day 19 between the knockout and control litters (figure 3.4B). One

animal appeared to be different from the rest of the group when litter weight

gain from day 7 to day 19 was analysed. However, a Grubbs outlier test

demonstrated that this point was not a significant outlier. Nevertheless,

removal of this point brings the litter growth from day 7 to day 19 to

statistical significance.

28

Figure 3.2: A: Bars represent mean±S.E.M food intake over a 14 day

period in virgin mice (n=9 per group). * p<0.05 B: Daily food intake over the 18 days of pregnancy prior to birth on day 19 (n control=10, n knockout=8). Values represent mean±SEM. * significant with respect to food intake on day 1 in control animals p<0.05. ** significant with respect to food intake on day 1 in control animals p<0.05. C: Daily food intake from day 6 to day 20 of lactation (n control=10, n knockout=8). Values represent mean±SEM. * significant with respect to food intake on day 6 in control animals p<0.05.

Control Knockout0

1

2

3

4

5

*

Food

Inta

ke (g

)

6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 206

8

10

12

14 ControlKnockout

*

Day of Lactation

Food

inta

ke (g

)

A

B

C

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 180

2

4

6

8 ControlKnockout

Day of Pregnancy

*

*

*

Food

inta

ke (g

)

29

Figure 3.3: A: Columns represent the mean±S.E.M bodyweight over a 14 day period in virgin mice (n=9 per group). * p<0.05 B: Daily bodyweight over the 18 days of pregnancy prior to birth on day 19 (n control=10, n knockout=8). Values represent mean±SEM. * significant with respect to bodyweight on day 6 in control animals p<0.05. ** significant with respect to food intake on day 1 in control animals p<0.05. C: Daily bodyweight from day 6 to day 21 of lactation (n control=10, n knockout=8). Values represent mean±SEM.

6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 2020

25

30

35 ControlKnockout

Day of Lactation

Bod

ywei

ght (

g)

A

B

C

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 180

10

20

30

40

50ControlKnockout

*

**

Day of Pregnancy

Bod

ywei

ght (

g)Control Knockout

0

5

10

15

20

25 ***

Body

wei

ght (

g)

30

Figure 3.4: A: Litter weight from day 6 to day 20 of lactation. (n litters

control=10, n litters knockout=8). Values represent mean±SEM. B: Columns represent mean±S.E.M pup growth from day 7 to day 19 of lactation. Individual points represent individual animal values.

Control Knockout

0

10

20

30

Pup

grow

th f

rom

day

7 t

o da

y 19

(g)

A

B

6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 200

10

20

30

40

50 ControlKnockout

Day of lactation

Litt

er w

eigh

t (g

)

31

3.2 Experiment 2

3.2.1 pSTAT5 Immunohistochemistry

The MPOA, PVN, ARC and VMH were qualitatively assessed for

pSTAT5 expression (figure 3.5). Prolactin treatment resulted in our control

animals displaying dense staining for pSTAT5 in all brain regions assessed.

However, our knockout animals displayed differential pSTAT5 distribution

based on the brain region of interest. There was considerable pSTAT5

expression in the MPOA of the control animals. In the knockout animal

brain, there was only scattered pSTAT5 expression in this area (figure 3.6).

The PVN of the hypothalamus showed some pSTAT5 expression in the

control animals. By contrast, there was no staining seen in this area in the

knockout animals (figure 3.7). The ARC showed extensive, but not

complete, loss of prolactin induced pSTAT5 in the knockout animals

compared with the controls (figure 3.8). There was almost complete loss of

pSTAT5 staining in the VMH in the knockouts compared with controls

(figure 3.9).

3.2.2 GFP Immunohistochemistry

Technical difficulties meant that only limited brain regions were

assessable. The PVN of the hypothalamus showed considerable GFP

32

expression in the knockout animals compared with control (figure 3.10).

This corroborates with the pSTAT5 data which showed no pSTAT5 staining

in this area.

33

Figure 3.5: Diagram of coronal mouse brain section indicating areas

assessed for the presence of cells positive for pSTAT5 and GFP. Areas of interest included medial preoptic area (top), paraventricular nucleus (middle), arcuate nucleus and ventromedial nucleus (bottom).

34

Figure 3.6: Representative images of pSTAT5 staining in the medial preoptic area (outlined in red) in control (top) and knockout (bottom) animals. Black dots (indicated by red arrows) indicate a cell positive for pSTAT5 and therefore the prolactin receptor. Inset scale bar approximately 100µm. 3V=Third ventricle, OC=optic chiasm.

35

Figure 3.7: Representative images of pSTAT5 staining in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (outlined in red) in control (top) and knockout (bottom) animals. Black dots (indicated by red arrows) indicate a cell positive for pSTAT5 and therefore the prolactin receptor. Inset scale bar approximately 100µm. 3V=Third ventricle

36

Figure 3.8: Representative images of pSTAT5 staining in the arcuate

nucleus of the hypothalamus (outlined in red) in control (top) and knockout (bottom) animals. Black dots (indicated by red arrows) indicate a cell positive for pSTAT5 and therefore the prolactin receptor. Inset scale bar approximately 100µm. 3V=Third ventricle, ME=median eminence

37

Figure 3.9: Representative images of pSTAT5 staining in the

ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus (outlined in red) in control (top) and knockout (bottom) animals. Black dots (indicated by red arrows) indicate a cell positive for pSTAT5 and therefore the prolactin receptor. Inset scale bar approximately 100µm. 3V=Third ventricle

38

Figure 3.10: Representative images of GFP staining in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (outlined in red) in control (top) and knockout (bottom) animals. Black dots (indicated by red arrows) indicate a cell positive for GFP. Inset scale bar approximately 100µm.3V=Third ventricle

3V

3V  

3V  

39

3.3 Experiment 3

3.3.1 Fast and Refeed:

There was no significant difference in food intake immediately after the

fast, basal food intake levels or food intake over the 7 days after the fast

between the two animal groups (figures 3.11A, 3.11 B and 3.11C).

However, the knockout animals had significantly higher mean bodyweight

(25.1±0.9 g) compared with the controls (21.3±0.6 g) (figure 3.11D ).

3.3.2 Response to a High Fat Diet

There was a decrease in food intake after the first day animals were

placed on a high fat diet (figure 3.12A). This change did not differ

significantly between the knockout and control animal groups. The

knockout animals placed on a high fat diet gained significantly more weight

(1.8±0.6 g) than the control animals on the same diet (figure 3.12B).

40

Figure 3.11: A and B: Columns represent mean±S.E.M food intake immediately post-fast and basal food intake respectively. C: Food intake for the 7 days postfast each point represents mean±S.E.M. D: Columns represent mean±S.E.M bodyweight over the 7 day measurement period (n control=6, n knockout=8). * p<0.05.

Control Knockout0

2

4

6

Food

Inta

ke P

ost-F

ast (

g)

Control Knockout0

2

4

6

Bas

al F

ood

Inta

ke (g

)

1 2 3 4 5 6 70

2

4

6

8

10ControlKnockout

Day Post-Fast

Food

Inta

ke (g

)

Control Knockout0

10

20

30*

Body

wei

ght (

g)

A

C

B

D

41

Figure 3.12: A: Food intake over the initial four day period post

administration of HFD or control. Points represent mean±S.E.M. B: Change in bodyweight from day 1 to day 25. * significant with respect to control animals on a control diet p<0.05

1 2 3 40

2

4

6

8Control CDControl HFDKnockout CDKnockout HFD

Day

Food

Inta

ke (g

)

-2

-1

0

1

2

3Control CDControl HFDKnockout CDKnockout HFD

*

Cha

nge

in b

odyw

eigh

tfr

om d

ay 1

to d

ay 2

5 (g

)A

B

42

4.0 Discussion

4.1 Overview

Pregnancy is associated with a substantial increase in energy

requirements. A number of adaptations during pregnancy, such as increased

food intake and increased nutrient absorption, allow the maternal body to

meet these demands. Pregnancy-induced hyperphagia is accompanied by a

paradoxical increase in leptin levels, suggesting that the maternal brain is

leptin resistant. Indeed, exogenous leptin administration to pregnant rodents

failed to elicit appetite-suppressive effects such as those seen in non-

pregnant animals (11, 46). Pregnancy is also associated with

downregulation of leptin receptor mRNA and leptin-induced STAT3

phosphorylation, further supporting this idea of pregnancy-induced leptin

resistance accompanied by hyperphagia (19, 46). The exact mechanism by

which these changes occur is yet to be determined, however the pregnancy

hormone prolactin is thought to be involved. There is significant evidence

demonstrating that exogenous prolactin can increase food intake in a

number of species (29, 31, 35). Additionally, central prolactin infusion has

been shown to inhibit responses to exogenous leptin through induction of

central leptin resistance (34, 38). Thus, the aim of this study was to block

43

prolactin action in the brain using a recently developed transgenic knockout

mouse to see if this prevented the expected increases in food intake during

pregnancy.

To investigate the role of prolactin in pregnancy-induced changes in

food intake, we measured food intake during pregnancy in a neuron-specific

prolactin-receptor knockout mouse. There was an overall difference in food

intake during pregnancy between the two animal groups. However, this

trend was the reverse of what we would expect, with the knockout animals

showing increased food intake compared with the controls. Furthermore

there was a significant increase in both food intake and bodyweight in our

non-pregnant knockout animals compared with controls. These unexpected

changes in our non-pregnant knockout group suggested that further

characterisation of this knockout model was necessary in order to elucidate

the possible mechanisms by which these changes arose.

4.2 Success of the Knockout and Immunohistochemistry for

pSTAT5:

A critical issue that must be considered when interpreting the data is

whether the experimental model had achieved the expected deletion of the

44

prolactin receptor from the brain. To determine the success of the CamK-II-

Cre;Prlflox/flox knockout animal model used, pSTAT5 immunohistochemistry

was performed to assess the extent of prolactin receptor signalling loss on

neurons. Prolactin-induced pSTAT5 provides a well characterised marker

for prolactin receptor expression and functional activity in neurons (25).

The results showed differential staining patterns between the knockout and

control animals. While the knockout animals expressed considerably less

pSTAT5 than the controls, there was still staining in areas such as the ARC

and MPOA. This indicated that there were still a number of cells expressing

the prolactin receptor.

There are a number of possible reasons why this knockout model was

incomplete. It is possible that the CamKII promoter is not expressed in all

neurons. Therefore, in those cells lacking the promoter prolactin receptors

would not be excised. The pattern of expression of the CamKII promoter in

cells of the hypothalamus is a source of controversy. It has been

successfully used in previous experiments to delete estrogen receptor-ɑ

specifically out of neurons (47). However, other descriptions of CamKII

expression show incomplete or scattered distribution in the hypothalamus

which is perhaps more consistent with our data (48). Alternatively, the

prolactin receptor may be expressed by other cell types, such as glia, that do

not express CamKII-Cre. These cells may influence the activity of

45

surrounding neurons thus indirectly influencing food intake. While it is

clear that there is still some prolactin receptor expression in the brains of the

knockout animals, the specific cell types retaining the receptor have not

been determined. As such, the incomplete nature of the knockout animal

must be taken into consideration when interpreting results.

Interestingly, the control animals displayed pSTAT5 staining in the PVN

post-weaning. Previous research has found that pSTAT5 is not expressed in

the PVN of virgin, diestrous animals in response to exogenous prolactin

administration (49). However, during lactation, multiple brain regions,

including the PVN, become more sensitive to prolactin (49). This is an

adaptive response which facilitates the prolactin-induced changes that are

necessary for healthy pregnancy and lactation. The results from the present

study, showing expression of pSTAT5 in the PVN in a post-weaning animal

suggest that this increased sensitivity to prolactin during pregnancy and

lactation may persist post-weaning and not return to non-pregnant levels.

This augmented prolactin response in the brain as a result of reproductive

experience is consistent with previous studies in rats (50, 51) and suggests

that this might be a phenomenon common to several species.

Immunohistochemistry for green fluorescent protein was also used to

assess cre-mediated recombination of the prolactin receptor floxed gene and

46

therefore identify the cells which had lost the prolactin receptor. While

difficulties with the tissue sections limited the number of brain regions

assessable, there was significant expression of the protein throughout the

brains of the knockout animals. The PVN showed significant GFP

expression in the knockout mice. This is consistent with our pSTAT5 results

which showed complete loss of prolactin-induced pSTAT5 in the PVN.

4.3 Knockout mice have abnormal estrous cyclicity

As part of the characterisation process of this mouse, estrous cyclicity

was monitored over a 14 day period. These results provided the first

indication of the physiological differences occurring in the knockout group

due to loss of the prolactin receptor. The knockout animals spent

considerably more time in met/diestrus and less time in estrus compared

with the control animals. These changes were likely due to the animals

displaying a state of pseudopregnancy induced by raised prolactin levels.

Mice, like other rodents, have a four day long estrous cycle (52). The

luteal phase of this cycle is only a few hours long in the absence of mating.

When mating occurs, stimulation of the cervix in the female causes

prolactin surges which maintain the corpus luteum allowing for a

subsequent rise in progesterone levels. These mating-induced prolactin

47

surges last long enough for embryo implantation and placental

development. After this point, the placenta produces placental lactogens

(prolactin homologues) in quantities sufficient to maintain high

progesterone levels and thus the viability of the pregnancy (20). In a normal

animal, the corpus luteum would degenerate rapidly if ovulation occurred in

the absence of mating because they lack the high prolactin levels that are

essential for luteotrophic support. In contrast to this, when the

hyperprolactinemic knockout animals ovulate, their high prolactin levels act

to maintain the corpus luteum for an extended period of time. This results in

the animal displaying a pseudopregnancy, a prolonged period of diestrous-

like smears resembling early pregnancy but in the absence of a fetus.

Furthermore, because ovulation triggers the development of the corpus

luteum and this prolonged, anovulatory, pseudopregnant state, animals do

not ovulate as frequently. This is possibly why our knockout animals spent

only a very small proportion of time in the estrus stage of the cycle.

The disrupted estrous cycles of these animals were also reflected in the

difficulties encountered when trying to get sufficient female mice pregnant.

Less than half of the knockout animals that were placed with a breeder male

achieved pregnancy. Moreover, the time taken to achieve pregnancy after

females were placed with a male was significantly longer in the knockout

animals compared with controls. Because the knockout animals ovulate so

48

infrequently, there are considerably fewer opportunities for mating and thus

pregnancy. Consequently, a large number of animals had to be mated for a

prolonged period of time in order to get a sufficient number of pregnant

knockout mice for our experiments.

The hyperprolactinemia exhibited by the knockout animals is likely due

to impaired negative feedback on prolactin secretion. Previous data from

our laboratory demonstrates that there is significant loss of prolactin

receptor on the tuberoinfundibular dopaminergic (TIDA) neuron population

of the arcuate nucleus in the CamK-II-Cre;Prlrflox/flox mice (Kokay pers

comm). The TIDA neurons mediate the short loop negative feedback

system critically involved in regulating prolactin secretion (see Ben-

Jonathan and Hnasko (53) for a review) (figure 4.1).

Figure 4.1: Short loop negative feedback of prolactin secretion Grattan

2002 Reproduction (27)

49

The TIDA neurons release dopamine, which in turn inhibits prolactin

secretion from the anterior pituitary gland. Increased prolactin levels

stimulate the TIDA neurons to increase dopamine secretion and therefore

inhibit prolactin secretion from the anterior pituitary. In this way, prolactin

negatively regulates itself. Loss of the prolactin receptor on the TIDA

neurons of the knockout animals impedes this negative feedback system. As

such, their prolactin levels are significantly higher than controls. In fact,

radioimmunoassay results from our laboratory found that prolactin levels

were up to 3 times higher in the knockouts compared with the controls

(Brown pers comm). These chronically high prolactin levels may be

inducing these changes in the natural reproductive cycle of the mouse

through actions in the ovary.

4.4 Food Intake and Bodyweight During Different

Reproductive States

The non-pregnant knockout animals had significantly higher food intake

and bodyweight compared with the control animals. It is the

hyperprolactinemic nature of the knockout animals, as described above, that

is hypothesised to cause these significant increases in food intake and

bodyweight in the non-pregnant state. However, there are multiple

50

mechanisms by which these high prolactin levels may cause these changes.

A number of studies have found that increasing prolactin levels through

administration of exogenous prolactin increases food intake in a variety of

animal species (28, 29, 31, 33, 35, 36). Thus, it is possible that the increased

endogenous prolactin levels displayed by the knockout animals may have

had similar consequences. Alternatively, as described above, it is possible

that the high prolactin levels maintain the corpus luteum for an extended

period of time. Because of this, the animal is also exposed to increased

progesterone levels. High progesterone levels have been associated with an

increase in food intake, an increase in water retention and consequently an

increase in bodyweight. Additionally, acyclicity has been associated with an

increase in food intake in non-pregnant rats (37). Further research as to

whether the neurons retaining the prolactin receptor are involved in food

intake or not would help clarify whether these differences are caused by

raised prolactin levels or raised progesterone levels.

Food intake was significantly higher by day 15 of pregnancy in the

knockout animals and day 10 in the control animals. This result is consistent

with our hypothesis that the prolactin receptor is involved in increasing food

intake in the earlier stages of pregnancy when metabolic demand is not the

primary driving factor. However, it is more likely that this delayed increase

in food intake during pregnancy is caused by the pseudopregnant state

51

exhibited by the non-pregnant animals. The hyperprolactinemia causes the

non-pregnant animals to display a hormone profile essentially identical to

that of a pregnant animal (54-56). These hormonal changes are likely why

the knockout animals exhibit increased food intake. Therefore, the

development of pregnancy, as distinct from pseudopregnancy, is unlikely to

change this hormone-driven hyperphagia any further.

The knockout animal group displayed significantly higher food intake

over pregnancy compared with the control animals. These data did not

support our hypothesis that prolactin receptor action is critically involved in

mediating changes in food intake over pregnancy. In fact, this trend was the

reverse of what we anticipated, as it was hypothesised that due to loss of the

prolactin receptor in the brain, the expected increases in food intake during

pregnancy would be inhibited in the knockout group. It is likely that these

results were influenced by the non-pregnant animal data. The non-pregnant

knockout animals ate significantly more and thus it is possible that this

hyperphagic state was maintained throughout the course of the pregnancy

which is reflected in the overall increased food intake seen in this group.

However, a number of caveats with the experiment make it difficult to draw

any firm conclusions as to the mechanisms behind these changes.

52

Interpretation of these results was complicated by the pSTAT5

immunohistochemistry results because we could not determine whether the

cells retaining the prolactin receptor were involved in food intake

regulation. If the cells involved in food intake regulation had lost the

prolactin receptor, this would suggest that prolactin is possibly not as

critical to the regulation of food intake during pregnancy as was previously

thought. Alternatively, failure to lose prolactin receptors from neurons

involved in food intake would mean that the hyperprolactinemia in these

animals would be acting on the remaining prolactin receptor sensitive

neurons, potentially continuing to stimulate food intake. As such, until the

neuronal populations retaining the prolactin receptor are identified, no firm

conclusions from our pregnancy data can be drawn. Additionally, the

significant changes seen in our non-pregnant knockout animals suggested

that their normal food intake regulatory mechanisms were altered. Because

we were unable to determine how these changes in food intake regulation

arose in our non-pregnant animals, it is difficult to speculate as to how

pregnancy might change this further.

Bodyweight displayed a significant increase over pregnancy but there

was no significant difference between the two animal groups. It was

anticipated that both groups would exhibit an increase over the course of the

pregnancy due to growth of the developing fetuses. However, we

53

hypothesised that, should the increases in food intake during pregnancy be

inhibited in the knockout group, bodyweight gain in these animals would be

lower to reflect this. Because we did not see the expected decreases in food

intake in the pregnant knockout animals, any decreases in bodyweight as a

direct consequence of this also did not occur.

Food intake and bodyweight were also monitored in both animal groups

throughout the 21 day lactation period. There was no overall significant

difference in food intake or bodyweight between the two animal groups.

However, the control group showed a significantly greater increase in food

intake over the course of lactation compared to the knockouts. Furthermore,

food intake during lactation in the knockout animals did not differ

significantly at any point compared with day 6 of lactation, whereas the

control animals showed a significant increase in food intake by day 12 of

lactation compared with day 6.

Significant differences in food intake between the knockout and control

animals were not necessarily expected as there are different factors driving

the increased food intake in lactation compared with pregnancy. During

pregnancy, increases in food intake occur in anticipation of the changes in

metabolic demand that lie ahead. This means that non-metabolic signals,

such as prolactin, must be present to cause these changes. During lactation,

54

the metabolic demands are sufficient to signal to the body that increased

food intake is necessary. Because these signals are metabolic as opposed to

hormonal, it is anticipated that changes in prolactin receptor expression

should not alter food intake during lactation significantly. The significant

increase in food intake over the lactation period may reflect differences in

metabolic demand between the two groups as a downstream effect of

prolactin receptor loss. These changes may be occurring due to impaired

maternal behaviour and decreased milk production or delivery. This

decrease in milk production causes a decline in metabolic demand, and food

intake decreases to ensure appropriate energy balance is maintained. This

hypothesis is further supported by the decrease in rate of knockout litter

weight gain which occurred at a similar time point to the decreases in food

intake during lactation in this animal group.

4.5 Litter Characteristics

Preliminary observations suggested that the knockout animal litters

weights were significantly lower than their control counterparts although

statistical analysis showed no significant difference in cumulative pup

growth from day 7 to day 19 between the knockout and control groups.

Initial assessment of the data suggested that one litter in the knockout group

was considerably heavier than the others. However, this point was not

55

considered a significant outlier when subjected to a Grubbs outliers test.

Despite this, removal of that one point brought the results to statistical

significance. Additionally, litter growth over time was significantly lower in

the knockout compared with the control animals.

These differences in litter growth may be due to differences in maternal

behaviour between the two animal groups. Prolactin has been demonstrated

as critical to the development of maternal behaviours. In fact, centrally

administered exogenous prolactin is sufficient in rats to cause

reproductively inexperienced females to exhibit maternal behaviour towards

pups (57). Furthermore, mice that are heterozygous for a null mutation in

the gene encoding the prolactin receptor have been observed to have

impaired maternal behaviour development (23). Therefore, it would be

expected that significant loss of the prolactin receptor in critical areas, such

as the MPOA, would have similar effects. Impairment of normal behaviours

due to this, such as time spent suckling, could influence the feeding habits

of the pups and thus their weight gain. Alternatively, there may be inherent

characteristics of the pups that cause them to display different growth

patterns compared with control litters. A cross-fostering study, in which

knockout litters are placed with control mothers and vice versa, would help

determine whether these changes occurred due to differences in the pup

phenotype or due to changes in maternal behavioural characteristics.

56

4.6 Response to a Fast and Refeed protocol and a High Fat

Diet study

The difference in bodyweight and food intake in the non-pregnant

knockout animals was unexpected and prompted us to undertake additional

investigations. Previous research suggests that a fast and refeed protocol

and a high fat diet experiment are the first steps to investigating a

bodyweight phenotype in a specific knockout population (see review by

Ellacott et al., (45)). When an animal is fasted, a post-fast hyperphagic

response normally occurs to compensate for the period of negative energy

balance. Failure to refeed may indicate defects in the animals’ ability to

maintain energy homeostasis. Alternatively, excessive refeeding responses

may suggest that the animals’ satiety signals are altered. There was no

significant difference in the refeeding response of our animals. However,

there was a significant increase in bodyweight between the two groups. This

confirms the results we found in our non-pregnant animals and provides

further evidence for an altered food intake-regulatory system in our

knockout animals.

57

Response to a HFD can also be used to indicate possible dysfunction in

energy regulation. There was a dramatic increase in food intake in all

groups over the first day the animals were placed on a HFD. However, this

significant increase lasted only the first day. Bodyweight gain was

significantly higher in the knockout animals on the HFD compared with the

control animals on the control diet. There also was a trend towards

significantly higher weight gain in the knockout animals on the control diet.

While this difference was not significant, it is likely that increasing our

group sizes would cause this result to reach statistical significance. This

increased bodyweight in the knockout animals on both diets, further

supports the idea that these knockout animals have some impairment in

bodyweight homeostasis which requires further investigation.

4.7 Limitations

A number of limitations with the methods that were used may have

influenced the results obtained. The key problem with this experiment was

the failure of our knockout model to lose the prolactin receptor in all areas

of the brain. As was discussed earlier, there is debate as to the expression of

the CamKII promoter within cells of the hypothalamus. However, because it

58

had been successfully used in previous experiments to knock out estrogen

receptors from neurons, it was anticipated that it could also be used to

knock the prolactin receptor out neuron-specifically. In order to rectify this

problem, our laboratory is currently breeding a prolactin receptor floxed

mouse with a nestin-cre expressing mouse. The nestin promoter is

expressed during early embryonic development in progenitor cell types of

the neuroectoderm, developing mesonephros and the somites (58). It is

anticipated that this nestin promoter will be more successful in achieving

global neuronal loss of the prolactin receptor compared with the CamKII

promoter.

The most likely target of prolactin in food intake regulation is the

oxytocin neuronal population in the PVN where we seemed to have

achieved deletion of the prolactin receptor. This suggests that there may be

other brain areas involved in prolactin-induced changes in food intake

regulation. The brainstem is well established as one of the centres involved

in food intake regulation and it has also been shown to express the prolactin

receptor during lactation (49). This suggests that it may have been

responsible for mediating the changes we saw.

Food intake data collection is also likely to be prone to error. In the

mouse, food consumption is so small that any losses that may occur through

59

means such as transfer to scales or dropping into the bottom of the cage, can

significantly skew results. Furthermore, as such small amounts are

consumed, significant changes may be too small to detect using

conventional statistical analysis.

4.8 Conclusions

While our data did not support the hypothesis that prolactin is critically

involved in increasing food intake during pregnancy, a number of factors

limited our ability to draw finite conclusions about the results. Nevertheless,

a significant step towards characterising this knockout mouse model was

taken. Obesity and excess weight gain during pregnancy is now being

considered 'the most common clinical risk factor encountered in obstetric

practice' (59). It is implicated in a number of pregnancy-related

complications including pre-eclampsia and gestational diabetes. Through

improving comprehension of the role of prolactin in appetite and weight

regulation in pregnancy we can better understand how excess weight gain

and obesity during pregnancy occurs and how dysregulation of this pathway

may affect the outcome of pregnancy.

60

5.0 References

1. Augustine RA, Ladyman SR, Grattan DR. From feeding one to feeding

many: hormone-induced changes in bodyweight homeostasis during

pregnancy. The Journal of Physiology. 2008;586(2):387-97.

2. Pelleymounter MA, Cullen MJ, Baker MB, Hecht R, Winters D, Boone

T, et al. Effects of the obese gene product on body weight regulation in

ob/ob mice. Science. 1995;269(5223):540-3.

3. Lee G-H, Proenca R, Montez JM, Carroll KM, Darvishzadeh JG, Lee JI,

et al. Abnormal splicing of the leptin receptor in diabetic mice. Nature.

1996;379(6566):632-5.

4. Chen H, Charlat O, Tartaglia LA, Woolf EA, Weng X, Ellis SJ, et al.

Evidence that the diabetes gene encodes the leptin receptor: identification of

a mutation in the leptin receptor gene in db/db mice. Cell. 1996;84(3):491-

5.

5. Bado A, Levasseur S, Attoub S, Kermorgant S, Laigneau JP, Bortoluzzi

MN, et al. The stomach is a source of leptin. Nature. 1998;394(6695):790-3.

6. Masuzaki H, Ogawa Y, Sagawa N, Hosoda K, Matsumoto T, Mise H, et

al. Nonadipose tissue production of leptin: leptin as a novel placenta-

derived hormone in humans. Nature Medicine. 1997;3(9):1029-33.

7. Hahn TM, Breininger JF, Baskin DG, Schwartz MW. Coexpression of

Agrp and NPY in fasting-activated hypothalamic neurons. Nature

Neuroscience. 1998;1(4):271-2.

61

8. Cowley MA, Smart JL, Rubinstein M, Cerdán MG, Diano S, Horvath

TL, et al. Leptin activates anorexigenic POMC neurons through a neural

network in the arcuate nucleus. Nature. 2001;411(6836):480-4.

9. Gibbs J, Young RC, Smith GP. Cholecystokinin decreases food intake in

rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology.

1973;84(3):488-95.

10. Nakazato M, Murakami N, Date Y, Kojima M, Matsuo H, Kangawa

K, et al. A role for ghrelin in the central regulation of feeding. Nature.

2001;409(6817):194-8.

11. Ladyman SR, Grattan DR. Region-Specific Reduction in Leptin-

Induced Phosphorylation of Signal Transducer and Activator of

Transcription-3 (STAT3) in the Rat Hypothalamus Is Associated with

Leptin Resistance during Pregnancy. Endocrinology. 2004;145(8):3704-11.

12. Cripps AW, Williams VJ. The effect of pregnancy and lactation on

food intake, gastrointestinal anatomy and the absorptive capacity of the

small intestine in the albino rat. The British Journal of Nutrition.

1975;33(1):17-32.

13. Seeber RM, Smith JT, Waddell BJ. Plasma Leptin-Binding Activity

and Hypothalamic Leptin Receptor Expression During Pregnancy and

Lactation in the Rat. Biology of Reproduction. 2002;66(6):1762-7.

14. Rocha M, Bing C, Williams G, Puerta M. Pregnancy-induced

hyperphagia is associated with increased gene expression of hypothalamic

agouti-related peptide in rats. Regulatory Peptides. 2003;114(2–3):159-65.

15. Speakman JR, McQueenie J. Limits to Sustained Metabolic Rate: The

Link between Food Intake, Basal Metabolic Rate, and Morphology in

62

Reproducing Mice, Mus musculus. Physiological Zoology. 1996;69(4):746-

69.

16. Cummings DE, Overduin J. Gastrointestinal regulation of food intake.

The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2007;117(1):13-23.

17. Butte NF, Hopkinson JM, Nicolson MA. Leptin in human

reproduction: serum leptin levels in pregnant and lactating women. The

Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1997;82(2):585-9.

18. Amico JA, Thomas A, Crowley RS, Burmeister LA. Concentrations

of leptin in the serum of pregnant, lactating, and cycling rats and of leptin

messenger ribonucleic acid in rat placental tissue. Life Sciences.

1998;63(16):1387-95.

19. Ladyman SR, Grattan DR. Suppression of Leptin Receptor Messenger

Ribonucleic Acid and Leptin Responsiveness in the Ventromedial Nucleus

of the Hypothalamus during Pregnancy in the Rat. Endocrinology.

2005;146(9):3868-74.

20. Cerruti R. Mammogenic Activities of the Mid-Gestational Mouse

Placenta. Endocrinology. 1960;67(6):884-7.

21. Liu X, Robinson GW, Wagner KU, Garrett L, Wynshaw-Boris A,

Hennighausen L. Stat5a is mandatory for adult mammary gland

development and lactogenesis. Genes & Development. 1997;11(2):179-86.

22. Shingo T, Gregg C, Enwere E, Fujikawa H, Hassam R, Geary C, et al.

Pregnancy-stimulated neurogenesis in the adult female forebrain mediated

by prolactin. Science Signalling. 2003;299(5603):117.

63

23. Lucas BK, Ormandy CJ, Binart N, Bridges RS, Kelly PA. Null

Mutation of the Prolactin Receptor Gene Produces a Defect in Maternal

Behavior. Endocrinology. 1998;139(10):4102-7.

24. Ling C, Hellgren G, Gebre-Medhin M, Dillner K, Wennbo H,

Carlsson B, et al. Prolactin (PRL) Receptor Gene Expression in Mouse

Adipose Tissue: Increases during Lactation and in PRL-Transgenic Mice.

Endocrinology. 2000;141(10):3564-72.

25. Brown RSE, Kokay IC, Herbison AE, Grattan DR. Distribution of

prolactin-responsive neurons in the mouse forebrain. The Journal of

Comparative Neurology. 2010;518(1):92-102.

26. Schindler C, Darnell JE. Transcriptional Responses to Polypeptide

Ligands: The JAK-STAT Pathway. Annual Review of Biochemistry.

1995;64(1):621-52.

27. Grattan D. Behavioural significance of prolactin signalling in the

central nervous system during pregnancy and lactation. Reproduction.

2002;123(4):497-506.

28. Buntin JD. Time course and response specificity of prolactin-induced

hyperphagia in ring doves. Physiol Behav. 1989;45(5):903-9.

29. Buntin JD, Figge GR. Prolactin and growth hormone stimulate food

intake in ring doves. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1988;31(3):533-40.

30. Li C, Kelly PA, Buntin JD. Inhibitory effects of anti-prolactin

receptor antibodies on prolactin binding in brain and prolactin-induced

feeding behavior in ring doves. Neuroendocrinology. 1995;61(2):125-35.

64

31. Byatt JC, Staten NR, Salsgiver WJ, Kostelc JG, Collier RJ.

Stimulation of food intake and weight gain in mature female rats by bovine

prolactin and bovine growth hormone. American Journal of Physiology -

Endocrinology And Metabolism. 1993;264(6):E986-E92.

32. Noel MB, Woodside B. Effects of systemic and central prolactin

injections on food intake, weight gain, and estrous cyclicity in female rats.

Physiology & Behavior. 1993;54(1):151-4.

33. Sauve D, Woodside B. The effect of central administration of

prolactin on food intake in virgin female rats is dose-dependent, occurs in

the absence of ovarian hormones and the latency to onset varies with

feeding regimen. Brain Research. 1996;729(1):75-81.

34. Naef L, Woodside B. Prolactin/Leptin Interactions in the Control of

Food Intake in Rats. Endocrinology. 2007;148(12):5977-83.

35. Gerardo-Gettens T, Moore BJ, Stern JS, Horwitz BA. Prolactin

stimulates food intake in a dose-dependent manner. The American Journal

of Physiology. 1989;256(1 Pt 2):R276-80.

36. Moore BJ, Gerardo-Gettens T, Horwitz BA, Stern JS.

Hyperprolactinemia stimulates food intake in the female rat. Brain Research

Bulletin. 1986;17(4):563-9.

37. Tarttelin MF, Gorski RA. Variations in food and water intake in the

normal and acyclic female rat. Physiology & Behavior. 1971;7(6):847-52.

38. Augustine RA, Grattan DR. Induction of Central Leptin Resistance in

Hyperphagic Pseudopregnant Rats by Chronic Prolactin Infusion.

Endocrinology. 2008;149(3):1049-55.

65

39. Martin RL, Perez E, He YJ, Dawson R, Jr., Millard WJ. Leptin

resistance is associated with hypothalamic leptin receptor mRNA and

protein downregulation. Metabolism. 2000;49(11):1479-84.

40. Steyn FJ, Anderson GM, Grattan DR. Hormonal Regulation of

Suppressors of Cytokine Signaling (SOCS) Messenger Ribonucleic Acid in

the Arcuate Nucleus during Late Pregnancy. Endocrinology.

2008;149(6):3206-14.

41. Anderson GM, Beijer P, Bang AS, Fenwick MA, Bunn SJ, Grattan

DR. Suppression of prolactin-induced signal transducer and activator of

transcription 5b signaling and induction of suppressors of cytokine

signaling messenger ribonucleic acid in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus of

the rat during late pregnancy and lactation. Endocrinology.

2006;147(10):4996-5005.

42. Bjorbak C, Lavery HJ, Bates SH, Olson RK, Davis SM, Flier JS, et al.

SOCS3 mediates feedback inhibition of the leptin receptor via Tyr985. J

Biol Chem. 2000;275(51):40649-57.

43. Tsien JZ, Chen DF, Gerber D, Tom C, Mercer EH, Anderson DJ, et

al. Subregion-and cell type-restricted gene knockout in mouse brain. Cell.

1996;87(7):1317-26.

44. Paxinos G, Franklin K. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates:

compact second edition. San Diego: Academic. 2003.

45. Ellacott KLJ, Morton GJ, Woods SC, Tso P, Schwartz MW.

Assessment of Feeding Behavior in Laboratory Mice. Cell Metabolism.

2010;12(1):10-7.

66

46. Ladyman SR, Fieldwick DM, Grattan DR. Suppression of leptin-

induced hypothalamic JAK/STAT signaling and feeding response during

pregnancy in the mouse. Reproduction. 2012.

47. Wintermantel TM, Campbell RE, Porteous R, Bock D, Gröne H-J,

Todman MG, et al. Definition of Estrogen Receptor Pathway Critical for

Estrogen Positive Feedback to Gonadotropin-Releasing Hormone Neurons

and Fertility. Neuron. 2006;52(2):271-80.

48. Gavériaux-Ruff C, Kieffer BL. Conditional gene targeting in the

mouse nervous system: Insights into brain function and diseases.

Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 2007;113(3):619-34.

49. Brown RSE, Herbison AE, Grattan DR. Differential Changes in

Responses of Hypothalamic and Brainstem Neuronal Populations to

Prolactin During Lactation in the Mouse. Biology of Reproduction.

2011;84(4):826-36.

50. Anderson GM, Grattan DR, van den Ancker W, Bridges RS.

Reproductive Experience Increases Prolactin Responsiveness in the Medial

Preoptic Area and Arcuate Nucleus of Female Rats. Endocrinology.

2006;147(10):4688-94.

51. Sjoeholm A, Bridges RS, Grattan DR, Anderson GM. Region-,

Neuron-, and Signaling Pathway-Specific Increases in Prolactin

Responsiveness in Reproductively Experienced Female Rats.

Endocrinology. 2011;152(5):1979-88.

52. Hilliard J. Corpus luteum function in guinea pigs, hamsters, rats, mice

and rabbits. Biology of Reproduction. 1973;8(2):203-21.

67

53. Ben-Jonathan N, Hnasko R. Dopamine as a prolactin (PRL) inhibitor.

Endocrine reviews. 2001;22(6):724-63.

54. Smith MS, Freeman ME, Neill JD. The Control of Progesterone

Secretion During the Estrous Cycle and Early Pseudopregnancy in the Rat:

Prolactin, Gonadotropin and Steroid Levels Associated with Rescue of the

Corpus Luteum of Pseudopregnancy. Endocrinology. 1975;96(1):219-26.

55. Freeman ME, Smith MS, Nazian SJ, Neill JD. Ovarian and

Hypothalamic Control of the Daily Surges of Prolactin Secretion During

Pseudopregnancy in the Rat. Endocrinology. 1974;94(3):875-82.

56. Freeman ME, Neill JD. The Pattern of Prolactin Secretion during

Pseudopregnancy in the Rat: A Daily Nocturnal Surge. Endocrinology.

1972;90(5):1292-4.

57. Bridges RS, Numan M, Ronsheim PM, Mann PE, Lupini CE. Central

prolactin infusions stimulate maternal behavior in steroid-treated,

nulliparous female rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

of the United States of America. 1990;87(20):8003-7.

58. Dubois NC, Hofmann D, Kaloulis K, Bishop J, Trumpp A. Nestin-Cre

transgenic mouse line Nes-Cre1 mediates highly efficient Cre/loxP

mediated recombination in the nervous system, kidney, and somite-derived

tissues. Genesis. 2006;44(8):355-60.

59. Bhattacharya S, Campbell D, Liston W, Bhattacharya S. Effect of

Body Mass Index on pregnancy outcomes in nulliparous women delivering

singleton babies. BMC Public Health. 2007;7(1):168.

68

69

Appendices

Appendix A: Fixatives

0.2 M Phosphate Buffer (1 litre)

Sodium dibasic anhydrous (Na2HPO4) 22.72 g

Sodium bonobasic 1x hydrous (NaH2PO4.H2O) 5.52 g

Dissolve in 900 ml milliQ-H2O

Store at room temperature

4% (w/v) Paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M PB

In fumehood:

Dissolve 4g paraformaldehyde powder in 50 ml of 55O C milliQ-H2O.

Continue heating on stirrer unit with thermometer to 60 degrees.

Once solution is 60 degrees add 2-3 drops of 1 M NaOH.

Continue stirring until all the paraformaldehyde powder has dissolved

and the solution is clear.

Cool solution on bench until room temperature

Make up to 100 ,l with 0.2 M phosphate buffer.

Filter solution.

70

Ensure pH is 7.3-7.4

Store at 4OC until use.

For transcardial mouse perfusion use 20 mL per animal.

30% Sucrose Solution

Sucrose 30 g

Dissolve in 100 ml 0.1 M phosphate buffer

Store at 4OC until use.

71

Appendix B: Solutions Used For Immunhistochemistry

Cryoprotectant (1 litre)

0.1 M PB 500 ml

NaCl 9 g

Sucrose 300 g

Polyvinyl pyrrolidine (PVP-40) 10 g

Ethylene glycol 300 ml

Make up to 1 litre final volume with milliQ-H2O.

Store at 4OC until use.

10x Tris-buffered saline (TBS) stock solution (2 litres)

100 mM Tris 24.2 g

1. M NaCl 1 752 g

Dissolve in 1800ml milliQ-H2O

Adjust pH to 7.6 with concentrated HCl.

Make up to final volume of 2 litres with milliQ-H2O

1x TBS Working Solution (1 litre)

Dilute 100 ml of the stock solution (10x TBS) in 900 ml milliQ-H2O.

Store at room temperature.

72

TBS+0.3% Triton X (100 ml)

Triton-X 300 μl

1x Tris-buffered saline 100 ml

Blocking solution (100 ml)

TBS+0.3% TritonX 100 ml

Bovine serum albumin 0.25 g

Normal goat serum 2000 μl

Hydrogen peroxide solution (50 ml)

1x TBS 28.5 ml

Methanol 20 ml

30% Hydrogen peroxide 1.5 ml

Avidin-Biotin complex solution (25 ml)

Made up using manufacturers instructions

60 minutes prior to use:

Prepare enough for 1 ml per well:

TBS 2 5 ml

Solution A 10 μl

Solution B 10 μl

73