the temporal paradox of hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · the temporal...

11
The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic plasticity Friedemann Zenke 1 , Wulfram Gerstner 2 and Surya Ganguli 1 Hebbian plasticity, a synaptic mechanism which detects and amplifies co-activity between neurons, is considered a key ingredient underlying learning and memory in the brain. However, Hebbian plasticity alone is unstable, leading to runaway neuronal activity, and therefore requires stabilization by additional compensatory processes. Traditionally, a diversity of homeostatic plasticity phenomena found in neural circuits is thought to play this role. However, recent modelling work suggests that the slow evolution of homeostatic plasticity, as observed in experiments, is insufficient to prevent instabilities originating from Hebbian plasticity. To remedy this situation, we suggest that homeostatic plasticity is complemented by additional rapid compensatory processes, which rapidly stabilize neuronal activity on short timescales. Addresses 1 Department of Applied Physics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA 2 Brain Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences and School of Computer and Communication Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Fe ´ de ´ rale de Lausanne, EPFL, CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland Corresponding author: Zenke, Friedemann ([email protected]) Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166-176 This review comes from a themed issue on Neurobiology of learning and plasticity Edited by Leslie Griffith and Tim Vogels http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.015 0959-4388/# 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved Introduction More than half a century ago, Donald Hebb [1] laid down an enticing framework for the neurobiological basis of learning, which can be succinctly summarized in the well-known mantra, ‘neurons that fire together wire together’ [2]. However, such dynamics suffers from two inherent problems. First, Hebbian learning exhibits a positive feedback instability: those neurons that wire together will fire together more, leading to even stronger connectivity. Second, such dynamics alone would lead to all neurons in a recurrent circuit wiring together, precluding the possibility of rich pat- terns of variation in synaptic strength that can encode, through learning, the rich structure of experience. Two fundamental ingredients required to solve these pro- blems are stabilization [3], which prevents runaway neural activity, and competition [4 ,5,6 ], in which the strengthening of a synapse may come at the ex- pense of the weakening of others. In theoretical models, competition and stability are often achieved by augmenting Hebbian plasticity with addi- tional constraints [3,5,7 ]. Such constraints are typically implemented by imposing upper limits on individual synaptic strengths, and by enforcing some constraint on biophysical variables, for example, the total synaptic strength or average neuronal activity [6 ,7 ,812]. In neurobiology, forms of plasticity exist which seemingly enforce such limits or constraints through synaptic scal- ing in response to firing rate perturbations [13,14], or through stabilizing adjustments of the properties of plas- ticity in response to the recent synaptic history, a phe- nomenon known as homeostatic metaplasticity [6 ,11,15,16]. Overall, synaptic scaling and metaplasti- city, as special cases of homeostatic mechanisms that operate over diverse spatiotemporal scales across neuro- biology [1722], are considered key ingredients that contribute both stability and competition to Hebbian plasticity by directly affecting the fate of synaptic strength. The defining characteristic of homeostatic plasticity is that it drives synaptic strengths so as to ensure a homeo- static set point [23,24 ], such as a specific neuronal firing rate or membrane potential. However, it is important that this constraint is implemented only on average, over long timescales, thereby allowing neuronal activity to fluctuate on shorter timescales, so that these neuronal activity fluctuations, which drive learning through Hebbian plas- ticity, can indeed reflect the structure of ongoing experi- ence. This requisite separation of timescales is indeed observed experimentally; forms of Hebbian plasticity can be induced on the timescale of seconds to minutes [2528], whilst most forms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity operate over hours or days [14,24 ,29]. This separation of timescales, however, raises a temporal paradox: homeo- static plasticity then may become too slow to stabilize the fast positive feedback instability of Hebbian learning. Indeed modeling studies that have attempted to use homeostatic plasticity mechanisms to stabilize Hebbian learning [11,30,31 ,32 ,33,34] were typically required to speed up homeostatic plasticity to timescales that are orders of magnitude faster than those observed in experi- ments (Figure 1). Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166176 www.sciencedirect.com

Upload: others

Post on 01-Aug-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning andhomeostatic plasticityFriedemann Zenke1, Wulfram Gerstner2 and Surya Ganguli1

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

Hebbian plasticity, a synaptic mechanism which detects and

amplifies co-activity between neurons, is considered a key

ingredient underlying learning and memory in the brain.

However, Hebbian plasticity alone is unstable, leading to

runaway neuronal activity, and therefore requires stabilization

by additional compensatory processes. Traditionally, a

diversity of homeostatic plasticity phenomena found in neural

circuits is thought to play this role. However, recent modelling

work suggests that the slow evolution of homeostatic plasticity,

as observed in experiments, is insufficient to prevent

instabilities originating from Hebbian plasticity. To remedy this

situation, we suggest that homeostatic plasticity is

complemented by additional rapid compensatory processes,

which rapidly stabilize neuronal activity on short timescales.

Addresses1 Department of Applied Physics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA

94305, USA2 Brain Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences and School of Computer

and Communication Sciences, Ecole Polytechnique Federale de

Lausanne, EPFL, CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland

Corresponding author: Zenke, Friedemann ([email protected])

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166-176

This review comes from a themed issue on Neurobiology of learning

and plasticity

Edited by Leslie Griffith and Tim Vogels

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.015

0959-4388/# 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved

IntroductionMore than half a century ago, Donald Hebb [1] laid

down an enticing framework for the neurobiological

basis of learning, which can be succinctly summarized

in the well-known mantra, ‘neurons that fire together

wire together’ [2]. However, such dynamics suffers

from two inherent problems. First, Hebbian learning

exhibits a positive feedback instability: those neurons

that wire together will fire together more, leading to

even stronger connectivity. Second, such dynamics

alone would lead to all neurons in a recurrent circuit

wiring together, precluding the possibility of rich pat-

terns of variation in synaptic strength that can encode,

through learning, the rich structure of experience. Two

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

fundamental ingredients required to solve these pro-

blems are stabilization [3], which prevents runaway

neural activity, and competition [4�,5,6�], in which

the strengthening of a synapse may come at the ex-

pense of the weakening of others.

In theoretical models, competition and stability are often

achieved by augmenting Hebbian plasticity with addi-

tional constraints [3,5,7�]. Such constraints are typically

implemented by imposing upper limits on individual

synaptic strengths, and by enforcing some constraint

on biophysical variables, for example, the total synaptic

strength or average neuronal activity [6�,7�,8–12]. In

neurobiology, forms of plasticity exist which seemingly

enforce such limits or constraints through synaptic scal-

ing in response to firing rate perturbations [13,14], or

through stabilizing adjustments of the properties of plas-

ticity in response to the recent synaptic history, a phe-

nomenon known as homeostatic metaplasticity

[6�,11,15,16]. Overall, synaptic scaling and metaplasti-

city, as special cases of homeostatic mechanisms that

operate over diverse spatiotemporal scales across neuro-

biology [17–22], are considered key ingredients that

contribute both stability and competition to Hebbian

plasticity by directly affecting the fate of synaptic

strength.

The defining characteristic of homeostatic plasticity is

that it drives synaptic strengths so as to ensure a homeo-

static set point [23,24�], such as a specific neuronal firing

rate or membrane potential. However, it is important that

this constraint is implemented only on average, over long

timescales, thereby allowing neuronal activity to fluctuate

on shorter timescales, so that these neuronal activity

fluctuations, which drive learning through Hebbian plas-

ticity, can indeed reflect the structure of ongoing experi-

ence. This requisite separation of timescales is indeed

observed experimentally; forms of Hebbian plasticity can

be induced on the timescale of seconds to minutes [25–28], whilst most forms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity

operate over hours or days [14,24�,29]. This separation of

timescales, however, raises a temporal paradox: homeo-

static plasticity then may become too slow to stabilize the

fast positive feedback instability of Hebbian learning.

Indeed modeling studies that have attempted to use

homeostatic plasticity mechanisms to stabilize Hebbian

learning [11,30,31�,32�,33,34] were typically required to

speed up homeostatic plasticity to timescales that are

orders of magnitude faster than those observed in experi-

ments (Figure 1).

www.sciencedirect.com

Page 2: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

The temporal paradox of Hebb and homeostatic plasticity Zenke, Gerstner and Ganguli 167

Figure 1

inst.ms

second

minute

hourday

week

hour

day

week

second

minute

hour

minute

10min

hour

∗ network simulations

Weigh t normalizatio n in models

von de r Malsbur g [5]Mille r an d MacK ay [7•], Laz ar et al . [109]

Litwin-Kum ar an d Doiro n [55•] ∗

van Rossu m et al . [53] , Zen ke et al . [31•] ∗

Toyoizum i et al . [32•]

Synaptic scalin g in ex periments

Aot o et al . [68] , Ibat a et al . [69]

Greenhill et al . [111]

Turrigian o et al . [13]Li et al . [116]

Kaneko et al . [114]Goel et al . [113]

Model s of metaplastici ty

Clopat h et al . [37•] ∗El Boustan i et al . [72 ] ∗

Gjorgjiev a et al . [54]Jedlicka et al . [104•]

Benuskov a an d Abraha m [117]

Zenke et al . [31•] ∗Pfiste r an d Gerstne r [36]

Metaplastici ty in ex periments

Mocket t et al . [115]

Huang et al . [110]

Christie an d Abraha m [112]

(a) (b)

(c) (d)

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Comparison of the typical timescale of different forms of homeostatic plasticity in models and in experiments. (a) Weight normalization in models.

Here we plot the characteristic timescale on which synaptic weights are either normalized or scaled. (b) Synaptic scaling in experiments. Here we

plot the typical time at which synaptic scaling is observed. (c) Models of metaplasticity. We plot the characteristic timescale on which the learning

rule changes. (d) Metaplasticity in experiments. Here we show the typical timescale at which metaplasticity is observed. Literature referenced in

the figure [5,7�,13,31�,32�,36,37�,53,54,55�,68,69,72,104�,109–117].

This temporal paradox could have two potential resolu-

tions. First, the timescale of Hebbian plasticity, as cap-

tured by recent plasticity models fit directly to data from

slice experiments [28,35,36,37�,38], may overestimate the

rate of plasticity that actually occurs in vivo. This overes-

timate could arise from differences in slice and in vivopreps, or because complex nonlinear synaptic dynamics,

both present in biological synapses and useful in learning

and memory [39–41], are missing in most, but not all [42–44], data-driven models. While slow plasticity may be a

realistic possibility in cortical areas exhibiting plastic

changes over days [45,46], it may not be a realistic

resolution in other areas, like the hippocampus, which

must rapidly encode new episodic information [47,48].

The second potential resolution to the paradoxical sepa-

ration of timescales between Hebbian and homeostatic

plasticity may be the existence of as yet unidentified

rapid compensatory processes (RCPs) that stabilize Heb-

bian learning. Below, we explore both the theoretical

utility and potential neurobiological instantiations of

these putative RCPs.

The temporal paradox of Hebbian andhomeostatic plasticityTo understand the theoretical necessity for RCPs to

stabilize Hebbian plasticity, it is useful to view a diversity

of synaptic learning models through the unifying lens of

control theory (Figure 2a). Here we can view the ‘fire

www.sciencedirect.com

together, wire together’ interplay of neuronal activity and

Hebbian synaptic plasticity as an unstable dynamical

system. Also, we can view a compensatory process as a

feedback controller that observes some aspect of either

neuronal activity or synaptic strength, and uses this

observation to compute a feedback control signal which

then directly affects synaptic strength so as to stabilize

global circuit activity. Indeed homeostatic plasticity is

often thought of as a negative feedback control process

[23,24�,49,50]. In general, the delay in any feedback

control loop must be fast relative to the time-scale over

which the unstable system exhibits run-away activity

[51]. If the loop is slightly slow, the run-away will start

before the stabilizing feedback arrives, generating oscil-

lations within the system. In some cases, if the loop is

even slower, the unstable runaway process might escape

before stabilization is even possible (Figure 2b).

Such oscillations and run-away are demonstrated in

Figure 3 for several compensatory mechanisms with

feedback timescales that are chosen to be too slow,

including the Bienenstock–Cooper–Munro (BCM) rule

[6�,52], and triplet spike-timing-dependent plasticity

(STDP) [36] with either synaptic scaling [13,53] or a

metaplastic sliding threshold, as stabilizing controllers.

For example, the BCM rule (Figure 3a) can be thought of

as a feedback control system where the controller

observes a recent average of the postsynaptic output firing

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Page 3: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

168 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

Figure 2

Synaptic strength

Neuronal activi ty

System:

Controller

External stimuli

Plastici ty

ObservationsControl signal 0

10

20 τ = 10ms

−100

1020 τ = 200ms

−450

4590 τ = 300ms

500ms

Ang

le(d

eg)

ΘF

(a) (b)

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

A control theoretic view of homeostatic plasticity and Hebbian learning. (a) The coupled dynamics of Hebbian plasticity and neural activity

constitutes an unstable dynamical system. Homeostatic plasticity, and more generally any compensatory mechanism, can be viewed as a

controller that observes aspects of neuronal activity and synaptic strengths, and uses these observations to compute a feedback control signal

that acts on synaptic dynamics so as to stabilize circuit properties. (b) The cart pole problem is a simple example of stabilizing a non-linear

dynamical system with feedback. The task of the controller is to exert horizontal forces on the cart to maintain the rod (m = 1 kg) in an upright

position. For simplicity we assume a weightless cart with no spatial constraints on the length of the track. The controller has access to an

exponential average (time constant t) over recent observations of both the angle u and angular velocity u˙of the pole. For small values of t the

controller can successfully maintain the rod upright (u � 0; top panel). However, as t and the associated time lag in the observed quantities gets

larger, oscillations arise (middle panel) and eventually the system becomes unstable (bottom panel).

rate of a neuron, and uses this information to control both

the sign and amplitude of associative plasticity; if the

recent average is high (low), plasticity is modulated to

be anti-Hebbian (Hebbian). However, to achieve stability,

the BCM controller must average recent output-activity

over a short enough time-scale to modulate plasticity

before this activity itself runs away (Figure 3a;

[11,31�,32�]). This result is not limited to BCM like rate

models, but applies equally to STDP models which rely on

similar metaplasticity processes to ensure stability

(Figure 3bc; [31�,36,37�,54]). These modified STDP rules

can be thought of as employing feedback controllers which

also observe a recent average of output firing rate, but use

this information to modulate the STDP window, thereby

stabilizing the system by changing relative rates of long-

term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD).

Another class of models relies on re-normalization of

afferent synaptic weights to stabilize Hebbian plasticity.

We distinguish between models with instantaneous algo-

rithmic re-normalization of the weights [4�,5,7�,55�] and

models which proportionally scale synaptic weights in an

activity dependent way [8,31�,56]. In the former case the

controller observes total synaptic strength and uses this

information to adjust synapses to keep this total strength

constant. In the latter case, the controller observes neu-

ronal firing rate, or recent average thereof, and uses this

information to proportionally adjust afferent synaptic

weights to enforce either a specified target output, or a

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

total synaptic strength. Just as in the case of metaplasticity

discussed above, the temporal average of the activity

sensor has to be computed over a short timescale, related

to the timescale over synaptic strengths and neuronal

activity change, to ensure stability (Figure 3d

[31�,32�,33]). Moreover, the rate at which the synaptic

scaling process itself causes synaptic strengths to scale

must be finely tuned to a narrow parameter regime that is

neither too fast nor too slow [31�,32�,33,53]; if too slow,

then stabilization is not possible, while if too fast, the

stabilization process overshoots, causing oscillations.

Finally, some STDP models can be intrinsically stable,

especially in feedforward circuits. One example are

pair-based STDP models in which the integral of the

STDP window is slightly biased toward depression.

When weights are additionally limited by hard bounds,

this can lead to bimodal weight distributions and firing

rate stabilization [9,57]. However, these learning rules

typically require fine-tuning and become unstable

when input correlations are non-negligible. Other sta-

ble models arise from a weight dependence in the

learning rule such that high (low) synaptic strength

makes LTP (LTD) weaker [53,58–62]. Such a stabiliz-

ing weight dependence is advantageous because it can

lead to more plausible unimodal weight distributions as

observed empirically [60,62]. However, the unimodal

weight distribution, by precluding multi-stability in the

configurations of synaptic strengths, typically leads to

www.sciencedirect.com

Page 4: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

The temporal paradox of Hebb and homeostatic plasticity Zenke, Gerstner and Ganguli 169

Figure 3

0

1

2

3

0 20 40 60 80 100

Act

ivity

(a.u

.)

Time ( τω )

νin νout νout νoutω

0

30

60

−5 0 5 10 15 20 25

Plasticity enabled

Firi

ngra

te(H

z)

Synaptic scalingMetaplasticity

−5 0 5 10 15 20 25

−5 0 5 10 15 20 25

Synaptic scalingMetaplasticity

−5 0 5 10 15 20 25

−5 0 5 10 15 20 25

Neu

ron

(×10

00)

Neu

ron

(×10

00)

Time (s)

receiving corr elate d input

(a)

(b)

(c)

(d)

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Instability in different plasticity models. (a) Unstable oscillations in the

BCM model for a simple feed-forward circuit [6�,11]. Model:

twdwdt ¼ aninnoutðnout�b~n2

out), tcddt

~nout ¼ nout�~nout with nout ¼ wnin,

tw ¼ 0:9tc; nin � 1 in arbitrary units (a.u.) and a and b are dimensionful

scalar constants that ensure correct units; we take simply

a = b = 1. Here ~nout can be thought of as the observation of a

controller, corresponding to an average of output neuronal activity nout

over time-scale tc. Moreover, the multiplicative term in parenthesis in

the weight dynamics can be interpreted as a control signal that

modulates both the sign and amplitude of associative plasticity,

dictating a stabilizing anti-Hebbian rule if the recent average ~nout is too

large. If the control dynamics tc is too slow relative to the synaptic

plasticity dynamics tw, unstable oscillations arise. (b–d) Runaway

activity in a recurrent neural network simulation consisting of

25 000 excitatory and inhibitory integrate-and-fire neurons and plastic

excitatory synapses using a minimal triplet STDP model [36] with

different homeostatic mechanisms such as sliding threshold

metaplasticity (violet) and synaptic scaling (green), both described in

detail in [31�]. (b) Population firing rate as a function of time. (c,d)

Raster plot of spiking activity. The bottom 3 groups of 100 neurons

received rate modulated spiking input with 100ms correlation time

constant to emulate sensory input to a small set of neurons. The

timescale of sliding threshold metaplasticity was tc = 3 min. The

timescale for the rate detector and the scaling dynamics for synaptic

scaling were tc = 10 s and tscl = 1 hour respectively. Because of these

slow stabilization dynamics, the fast interplay between Hebbian

plasticity and recurrent network dynamics leads to rapid population

firing rate destabilization within 10–20 s for both learning rules.

www.sciencedirect.com

the rapid erasure of synaptic memory traces in the pres-

ence of background activity driving plasticity [61,63].

Moreover, when such weight dependent synaptic learning

rules are embedded in a recurrent neuronal circuit without

any additional control mechanisms, they can succumb to

runaway neural activity as experience dependent neural

correlations emerge in the recurrent circuit [61,64,65].

In summary, empirical findings and control-theoretic con-

siderations suggest that compensatory mechanisms capable

of stabilizing Hebbian plasticity must operate on tightly

constrained timescales. In practice, such compensatory

mechanisms must act on similar or even faster timescales

than Hebbian plasticity itself [31�,32�,36,37�,54,55�,56].

STDP, as one of the most common manifestations of

Hebbian plasticity in the brain, can be induced in a matter

of seconds to minutes [25–27,66,67]. Homeostatic plastici-

ty, on the other hand, acts on much longer timescales of

hours to days [14,16,19,29,68–70]. This separation of time-

scales poses a temporal paradox as it renders most data-

driven STDP models unstable [3,71]. In spiking network

models, this instability has severe consequences

(Figure 3b–d); it precludes the emergence of stable syn-

aptic structures or memory engrams [31�,61,64], unless the

underlying plasticity models are augmented by RCPs

[31�,55�,56,72,73�]. Overall, these considerations suggest

that one or more RCPs exist in neurobiological systems,

which are missing in current plasticity models.

Putative rapid compensatory processesWhat putative RCPs could augment Hebbian plasticity

with the requisite stability and competition? Here we

focus on several possibilities, operating at either the

network, the neuronal or the dendritic level

(Figure 4a). However, we note that these possibilities

are by no means exhaustive.

At the network level, recurrent or feedforward synaptic

inhibition could influence and potentially stabilize plas-

ticity at excitatory synapses directly. For instance, [74]

demonstrated via dynamic-clamp that a simulated in-

crease in total excitatory and inhibitory background con-

ductance, which could originate from elevated levels of

network activity, rapidly reduces the amplitude of LTP,

but not LTD, of the STDP window. This rapid effect may

be mediated through changes in calcium dynamics in

dendritic spines and could constitute an RCP. Another

study [75] showed that global inhibition in a rate-based

network model is sufficient to stabilize plasticity at excit-

atory synapses with a sliding presynaptic and fixed post-

synaptic plasticity threshold. Finally, using a model-based

approach, Wilmes et al. [76] have proposed that dendritic

inhibition could exert binary switch-like control over

plasticity by gating back-propagating action potentials.

Other modeling studies have suggested a role for

inhibitory synaptic plasticity (ISP) [77,78], instead of

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Page 5: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

170 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

Figure 4

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Stimulation times:

Pop

ulat

ion

rate

(H

z) 60

0

400

units

3736.8 37.2 37.4 37.6

Time (min)

Spikes:

active cell assembly

(a) (b)

neuronal-levelheterosynaptic

branch-specificsingle

synapselevel

Inhibitoryplasticity

(a) Schematic representation of the many potential loci of rapid compensatory processes. (b) Example of a plastic network model displaying

attractor dynamics in three distinct cell assemblies. Because these dynamics outlast the stimulus that triggers them they have been considered as

neural substrate for working memory. The top panel shows a spike raster, whereas the bottom panel shows the population firing rates of the three

cell assemblies. This particular model uses an augmented triplet STDP learning rule (cf. Figure 3) in which heterosynaptic plasticity and single-

synapse-level plasticity have been added as stabilizing RCPs. Moreover, this model relies on a slow form of inhibitory plasticity which normalizes

the overall network activity and ensures that cell assemblies do not grow indefinitely over time by recruiting additional neurons. This provides a

proof of principle that a biologically inspired learning rule can indeed be stabilized by a sensible combination of RCPs, whereas the same learning

rule endowed with slow compensatory mechanisms leads to run-away dynamics (cf. Figure 3b–d). Figure adapted from Zenke et al. [73].

non-plastic inhibition, in stabilizing Hebbian plasticity. It

has been suggested, for instance, that ISP in conjunction

with a fixed plasticity threshold at the excitatory synapse

could have a similar effect as the sliding threshold in the

BCM model [79]. Finally, in some experiments excitatory

and inhibitory plasticity are not integrated as indepen-

dent events, but can influence each other. For instance,

there are cases in which induction of ISP alone can flip the

sign of subsequent plasticity at excitatory synapses [80].

While inhibition and ISP may act as RCPs, a clear picture

of how these elements tie together has not yet emerged.

Further experimental and theoretical work is required to

understand their potential for acting as RCPs.

Neuromodulation may also play an important role in

stabilizing plasticity. Neuromodulators have been impli-

cated in both homeostatic signalling [21] and in gating the

expression of synaptic plasticity [81–84]. However, to

successfully serve as RCP, neuromodulatory mechanisms

have to either drive rapid compensatory changes directly,

or substantially reduce the average rate of Hebbian plas-

ticity in vivo to enable slower forms of homeostatic

plasticity to preserve stability. While a detailed account

of the role of neuromodulators goes beyond the scope of

this article (but see [84,85] for reviews), here we note that

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

several neuromodulators at least partially meet one of

these core requirements for RCPs. For example, nitric

oxide (NO), which is involved in synaptic homeostasis

and plasticity [86], is released in response to increased

NMDA activity and decays within some tens of seconds

[87]. Because cell membranes are permeable to NO, the

molecule diffuses rapidly and thus could potentially act

as a fast proxy of bulk neuronal activity that can be read

out locally [88]. Similarly, dopaminergic transmission can

be fast [89] and is known to affect induction, consolida-

tion and possibly maintenance of synaptic long-term

plasticity [82,84,90–92]. A sensible gating strategy imple-

mented by dopamine or other neuromodulators could

result in a drastically reduced and more manageable

average rate of plasticity in vivo. In contrast, in vitro such

neuromodulatory mechanisms might be disengaged,

thus potentially creating less natural and much faster

plasticity rates compared to in vivo. Apart from the

neuromodulatory system, recent work has highlighted

the complexity of the local interactions between astro-

cytes and synaptic plasticity which may also act as RCPs

[93–95]. However, to unequivocally answer which

aspects of neuromodulation and glial interactions consti-

tute suitable RCPs will require further experimental and

theoretical work.

www.sciencedirect.com

Page 6: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

The temporal paradox of Hebb and homeostatic plasticity Zenke, Gerstner and Ganguli 171

A more well studied possibility for an RCP is hetero-

synaptic plasticity, which operates at the level of indi-

vidual neurons or potentially dendritic branches.

Heterosynaptic plasticity refers to a non input-specific

change at other synapses onto a neuron that are not

directly activated [30,96,97,98�,99�]. Its viability as

putative RCP arises from the fact that some forms of

heterosynaptic plasticity can be induced rapidly, and

moreover, similar to synaptic scaling, can show aspects

of weight normalization [97]. For instance, a rapid form

of heterosynaptic plasticity, in which neuronal bursting

causes bi-directional weight-dependent changes in af-

ferent synapses, has been observed recently [98�,100�].While the observed weight-dependence is reminiscent

of Oja’s rule [8], as strong synapses weaken, it is not

identical because weak synapses can also strengthen.

Nevertheless, this form of heterosynaptic plasticity has

been shown to prevent runaway of LTP in models

of feedforward circuits [30] and has been demonstrated

to co-occur with Hebbian plasticity in experiments

[100�].

Recently, the utility in stabilizing runaway LTP has also

been demonstrated in a recurrent network model of

spiking neurons [73], in which a similarly burst depen-

dent form of heterosynaptic plasticity is crucial to ensure

stable formation and recall of Hebbian cell assemblies

(Figure 4b). Moreover, the work suggests a potential role

of heterosynaptic plasticity in triggering the reversal of

LTP and LTD [101].

At the level of dendritic branches, a recently described

form of structural heterosynaptic plasticity [99�],involves local dendritic competition between synapses.

Specifically, glutamate induced structural synaptic

potentiation of a set of clustered dendritic spines causes

shrinkage of nearby, but unstimulated spines. Interest-

ingly, even when structural potentiation was switched

off through inhibition of Ca2+/calmodulin dependent

protein kinase II (CaMKII), the heterosynaptic effect

persists, suggesting a model in which spines send and

receive shrinkage signals instead of competing for

limited resources. Moreover, the observed dendritic

locality is consistent with work on local or branch

specific conservation of total synaptic conductance

[97,102].

Finally, in some cases heterosynaptic plasticity might not

actually be heterosynaptic, as it may still depend on low

levels of spontaneous synaptic activity in unstimulated

synapses [103]. A recent biophysical model derived from

synaptic plasticity data [104�] actually requires such low-

levels of activity to match the data. Interestingly, this

model suggests such ‘heterosynaptic’ plasticity could

arise as a consequence of a rapid (timescale �12 s) ho-

meostatic sliding threshold possibly related to autopho-

sphorylation of CaMKII.

www.sciencedirect.com

While heterosynaptic plasticity, like synaptic scaling, can

have a stabilizing effect, it is distinct from synaptic scaling

in two ways. First, heterosynaptic plasticity need not

multiplicatively scale all weights in the same manner.

Second, it is unclear whether heterosynaptic plasticity in

general drives neuronal activity variables to a specific set

point, like synaptic scaling does [24�]. Thus, while het-

erosynaptic plasticity has the rapidity to act as a putative

RCP, its functional utility in storing memories requires

further empirical and theoretical study, as it may lack the

ability to precisely preserve ratios of synaptic strengths.

However, its clear functional utility in preventing insta-

bility (Figure 3d), along with even an approximate pres-

ervation in ratios of strengths, could potentially endow

the interaction of heterosynaptic plasticity and Hebbian

learning with the ability to stably learn and remember

memories. Further network modeling, building on prom-

ising heterosynaptic plasticity models [30,73�,104�], could

be highly instructive in elucidating the precise properties,

beyond rapidity, a putative RCP must obey in order to

provide appropriate competition and stability to Hebbian

plasticity.

ConclusionThe trinity of Hebbian plasticity, competition and stabil-

ity are presumed to be crucial for effective learning and

memory. However, a detailed theoretical and empirical

understanding of how these diverse elements conspire to

functionally shape neurobiological circuits is still missing.

Here we have focused on one striking difference between

existing models and neurobiology: the paradoxical sepa-

ration of timescales between Hebbian and homeostatic

plasticity. In models, such a separation of timescales

typically leads to instability, unless plasticity is con-

strained by RCPs that act much faster than observed

forms of homeostatic plasticity. In principle, RCPs could

be implemented at various spatial scales. Here we have

primarily discussed different forms of heterosynaptic

plasticity and processes involving synaptic inhibition as

possible candidates. However, rapid processes involving

neuromodulation, glial interactions, or intrinsic plasticity

[105–108] could also constitute RCPs. Thus, identifying

the key neurobiological processes that provide stability

and competition to Hebbian learning rules remains an

important direction for future research.

Conflict of interest statementNothing declared.

Acknowledgements

FZ was supported by the SNSF (Swiss National Science Foundation). SGwas supported by the Burroughs Wellcome, Sloan, McKnight, Simons andJames S. McDonnell Foundations and the Office of Naval Research. WGwas supported for this work by the European Research Council under grantagreement number 268689 (MultiRules) and by the European Community’sSeventh Framework Program under grant no. 604102 (Human BrainProject).

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Page 7: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

172 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

References and recommended readingPapers of particular interest, published within the period of review,

have been highlighted as:

� of special interest

1. Hebb DO: The Organization of Behavior: A NeuropsychologicalTheory. New York: Wiley & Sons; 1949.

2. Shatz CJ: The developing brain. Sci Am 1992, 267:60-67 http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/the-developing-brain/.

3. Abbott LF, Nelson SB: Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast. NatNeurosci 2000, 3:1178-1183 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/81453ISSN: 1097-6256, http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v3/n11s/full/nn1100_1178.html.

4.�

Rochester N, Holland J, Haibt L, Duda W: Tests on a cellassembly theory of the action of the brain, using a large digitalcomputer. IEEE Trans Inf Theory 1956, 2:80-93 http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TIT.1956.1056810 ISSN: 0096-1000.

Of historical interest as probably the earliest modeling study — theauthors show that Hebbian LTP needs to be complemented by twomechanisms: (i) LTD formulated as an early version of a covariance ruleand (ii) and a rapid heterosynaptic mechanism formulated as normal-ization of the sum over all weights of afferent synapses.

5. von der Malsburg C: Self-organization of orientation sensitivecells in the striate cortex. Kybernetik 1973, 14:85-100 ISSN:0023-5946.

6.�

Bienenstock E, Cooper L, Munro P: Theory for the developmentof neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocularinteraction in visual cortex. J Neurosci 1982, 2:32-48 In: http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/abstract/2/1/32.

This classic modeling paper for metaplasticity via a sliding thresholdbetween LTP and LTD links thorough mathematical analysis to experi-mental data available at that time.

7.�

Miller KD, MacKay DJ: The role of constraints in Hebbianlearning. Neural Comput 1994, 6:100-126.

A careful and transparent mathematical study on the role of synapticconstraints in Hebbian learning.

8. Oja E: Simplified neuron model as a principal componentanalyzer. J Math Biol 1982, 15:267-273 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00275687 ISSN: 0303-6812, http://www.springerlink.com/content/u9u6120r003825u1/abstract/.

9. Kempter R, Gerstner W, van Hemmen J: Hebbian learning andspiking neurons. Phys Rev E 1999, 59:4498-4514 http://dx.doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevE.59.4498 ISSN: 1063-651X, 1095-3787, http://infoscience.epfl.ch/record/97790.

10. Song S, Miller KD, Abbott LF: Competitive Hebbian learningthrough spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity. NatNeurosci 2000, 3:919-926 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/78829 In:http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v3/n9/full/nn0900_919.html.

11. Cooper LN, Intrator N, Blais BS, Shouval HZ: Theory of CorticalPlasticity. New Jersey: World Scientific; 2004978-981-238-746-2.

12. Brito CSN, Gerstner W: Nonlinear Hebbian learning as a unifyingprinciple in receptive field formation. PLOS Comput Biol 2016,12:e1005070 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005070ISSN: 1553-7358, http://journals.plos.org/ploscompbiol/article?id=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005070.

13. Turrigiano GG, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB:Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocorticalneurons. Nature 1998, 391:892-896 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/36103 ISSN: 0028-0836.

14. Turrigiano GG: The self-tuning neuron: synaptic scaling ofexcitatory synapses. Cell 2008, 135:422-435 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.008 ISSN: 0092-8674, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092867408012981.

15. Abraham WC, Bear MF: Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synapticplasticity. Trends Neurosci 1996, 19:126-130 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(96)80018-X ISSN: 0166-2236, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S016622369680018X.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

16. Abraham WC: Metaplasticity: tuning synapses and networksfor plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008, 9:387 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2356 ISSN: 1471-003X, http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v9/n5/abs/nrn2356.html.

17. Davis GW: Homeostatic control of neural activity: fromphenomenology to molecular design. Annu Rev Neurosci 2006,29:307-323 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135751.

18. Marder E, Goaillard J-M: Variability, compensation andhomeostasis in neuron and network function. Nat Rev Neurosci2006, 7:563-574 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1949 ISSN: 1471-003X, http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v7/n7/abs/nrn1949.html.

19. Rabinowitch I, Segev I: Two opposing plasticity mechanismspulling a single synapse. Trends Neurosci 2008, 31:377-383http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.05.005 ISSN: 0166-2236,http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0166223608001483.

20. Turrigiano G: Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptichomeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinement. AnnuRev Neurosci 2011, 34:89-103 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153238.

21. Turrigiano GG: Homeostatic synaptic plasticity: local andglobal mechanisms for stabilizing neuronal function. ColdSpring Harb Perspect Biol 2012, 4:a005736 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a005736 ISSN: 1943-0264, http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/content/4/1/a005736.

22. Gjorgjieva J, Drion G, Marder E: Computational implications ofbiophysical diversity and multiple timescales in neurons andsynapses for circuit performance. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2016,37:44-52 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.12.008 ISSN:0959-4388, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0959438815001865.

23. Cannon WB: The Wisdom of the Body. New York: Norton; 1967:.ISBN: 0-393-00205-5 978-0-393-00205-8.

24.�

Turrigiano GG: The dialectic of Hebb and homeostasis. PhilosTrans R Soc B 2017, 372:20160258 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0258 ISSN: 0962-8436, 1471-2970, http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/372/1715/20160258.

A broad review of synaptic homeostatic plasticity which also addressesopen questions regarding timescales of plasticity and homeostasis.

25. Markram H, Lubke J, Frotscher M, Sakmann B: Regulation ofsynaptic efficacy by coincidence of postysnaptic AP andEPSP. Science 1997, 275:213-215.

26. Bi G-Q, Poo M-M: Synaptic modifications in culturedhippocampal neurons: dependence on spike timing, synapticstrength, and postsynaptic cell type. J Neurosci 1998,18:10464-10472 In: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/18/24/10464.abstract.

27. Zhang LI, Tao HW, Holt CE, Harris WA, Poo M-m: A criticalwindow for cooperation and competition among developingretinotectal synapses. Nature 1998, 395:37-44 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/25665 ISSN: 0028-0836, http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v395/n6697/abs/395037a0.html.

28. Froemke RC, Dan Y: Spike-timing-dependent synapticmodification induced by natural spike trains. Nature 2002,416:433-438 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/416433a ISSN: 0028-0836, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11919633.

29. Watt AJ, Desai NS: Homeostatic plasticity and STDP: keeping aneuron’s cool in a fluctuating world. Front Synaptic Neurosci2010, 2:5 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00005 In: http://www.frontiersin.org/synaptic_neuroscience/10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00005/abstract.

30. Chen J-Y, Lonjers P, Lee C, Chistiakova M, Volgushev M,Bazhenov M: Heterosynaptic plasticity prevents runawaysynaptic dynamics. J Neurosci 2013, 33:15915-15929 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5088-12.2013 ISSN: 0270-6474,1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/33/40/15915.

31.�

Zenke F, Hennequin G, Gerstner W: Synaptic plasticity in neuralnetworks needs homeostasis with a fast rate detector. PLoS

www.sciencedirect.com

Page 8: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

The temporal paradox of Hebb and homeostatic plasticity Zenke, Gerstner and Ganguli 173

Comput Biol 2013, 9:e1003330 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003330.

Mathematical analysis of network dynamics in rate neurons indicating theneed for RCP implemented through either weight normalization or meta-plastic sliding threshold.

32.�

Toyoizumi T, Kaneko M, Stryker MP, Miller KD: Modeling thedynamic interaction of Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity.Neuron 2014, 84:497-510 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.036 ISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.cell.com/article/S0896627314008940/abstract.

The authors show that the time course drop of synaptic strength observedin ocular deprivation experiments cannot be captured by a standard bcmmodel. Instead the authors suggest a model in which plasticity is intrin-sically bistable whereas homeostasis is multiplicative

33. Yger P, Gilson M: Models of metaplasticity: a review ofconcepts. Front Comput Neurosci 2015:138 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2015.00138 In: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fncom.2015.00138/full.

34. Zenke F, Gerstner W: Hebbian plasticity requires compensatoryprocesses on multiple timescales. Philos Trans R Soc B 2017,372:20160259 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0259 ISSN:0962-8436, 1471-2970, http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/372/1715/20160259.

35. Senn W, Markram H, Tsodyks M: An algorithm for modifyingneurotransmitter release probability based on pre- andpostsynaptic spike timing. Neural Comput 2001, 13:35-67 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976601300014628 ISSN: 0899-7667,http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976601300014628.

36. Pfister J-P, Gerstner W: Triplets of spikes in a model of spiketiming-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 2006, 26:9673-9682http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1425-06.2006 ISSN:0270-6474, 1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/26/38/9673.

37.�

Clopath C, Busing L, Vasilaki E, Gerstner W: Connectivity reflectscoding: a model of voltage-based STDP with homeostasis. NatNeurosci 2010, 13:344-352 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2479ISSN: 1097-6256, http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v13/n3/full/nn.2479.html.

Voltage-based plasticity model with parameters extracted from experi-mental data (see also [104�]).

38. Graupner M, Brunel N: Calcium-based plasticity model explainssensitivity of synaptic changes to spike pattern, rate, anddendritic location. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2012, 109:3991-3996http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1109359109 ISSN: 0027-8424,1091-6490, http://www.pnas.org/content/109/10/3991.

39. Fusi S, Drew PJ, Abbott LF: Cascade models of synapticallystored memories. Neuron 2005, 45:599-611 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.001 ISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15721245.

40. Lahiri S, Ganguli S: A memory frontier for complexsynapses.Advances in Neural Information ProcessingSystems. Tahoe, USA: Curran Associates, Inc.; 2013, 1034-1042In: http://papers.nips.cc/paper/4872-a-memory-frontier-for-complex-synapses.

41. Benna MK, Fusi S: Computational principles of synapticmemory consolidation. Nat Neurosci 2016 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.4401. ISSN: 1097-6256, http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/vaop/ncurrent/full/nn.4401.html.

42. Clopath C, Ziegler L, Vasilaki E, Busing L, Gerstner W: Tag-trigger-consolidation: a model of early and late long-term-potentiation and depression. PLoS Comput Biol 2008,4:e1000248 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000248.

43. Barrett AB, Billings GO, Morris RGM, van Rossum MCW: Statebased model of long-term potentiation and synaptic taggingand capture. PLoS Comput Biol 2009, 5:e1000259 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000259.

44. Ziegler L, Zenke F, Kastner DB, Gerstner W: Synapticconsolidation: from synapses to behavioral modeling. JNeurosci 2015, 35:1319-1334 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3989-14.2015 ISSN: 0270-6474, 1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/35/3/1319.

www.sciencedirect.com

45. Rittenhouse CD, Shouval HZ, Paradiso MA, Bear MF: Monoculardeprivation induces homosynaptic long-term depression invisual cortex. Nature 1999, 397:347-350 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/16922 ISSN: 0028-0836, http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v397/n6717/abs/397347a0.html.

46. Rose T, Jaepel J, Hubener M, Bonhoeffer T: Cell-specificrestoration of stimulus preference after monoculardeprivation in the visual cortex. Science 2016, 352:1319-1322http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.aad3358 ISSN: 0036-8075,1095-9203, http://science.sciencemag.org/content/352/6291/1319.

47. Nakazawa K, Sun LD, Quirk MC, Rondi-Reig L, Wilson MA,Tonegawa S: Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucialfor memory acquisition of one-time experience. Neuron 2003,38:305-315 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00165-XISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S089662730300165X.

48. Lee I, Kesner RP: Differential contributions of dorsalhippocampal subregions to memory acquisition and retrievalin contextual fear-conditioning. Hippocampus 2004, 14:301-310http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10177 ISSN: 1098-1063, http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.10177/abstract.

49. O’Leary T, Wyllie DJA: Neuronal homeostasis: time for achange? J Physiol 2011, 589:4811-4826 http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2011.210179 ISSN: 1469-7793, http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1113/jphysiol.2011.210179/abstract.

50. Williams A, O’Leary T, Marder E: Homeostatic regulation ofneuronal excitability. Scholarpedia 2013, 8:1656 http://dx.doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.1656 ISSN: 1941-6016, http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Homeostatic_Regulation_of_Neuronal_Excitability.

51. Astrom KJ, Murray RM: Feedback Systems: An Introduction forScientists and Engineers. Princeton University Press; 2010:. ISBN:978-1-4008-2873-9, google-Books-ID: cdG9fNqTDS8C.

52. Udeigwe LC, Munro PW, Ermentrout GB: Emergent dynamicalproperties of the BCM learning rule. J Math Neurosci 2017, 7:2http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13408-017-0044-6 ISSN: 2190-8567,https://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s13408-017-0044-6.

53. van Rossum MCW, Bi GQ, Turrigiano GG: Stable Hebbianlearning from spike timing-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci2000, 20:8812-8821 ISSN: 0270-6474, 1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/20/23/8812.

54. Gjorgjieva J, Clopath C, Audet J, Pfister J-P: A triplet spike-timing–dependent plasticity model generalizes theBienenstock–Cooper–Munro rule to higher-orderspatiotemporal correlations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011,108:19383-19388 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105933108 In:http://www.pnas.org/content/108/48/19383.abstract.

55.�

Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B: Formation and maintenance ofneuronal assemblies through synaptic plasticity. Nat Commun2014, 5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ncomms6319 In: http://www.nature.com/ncomms/2014/141114/ncomms6319/full/ncomms6319.html.

This article demonstrates stable learning and transient recall (�100 ms) ofHebbian cell assemblies in a recurrent network model through thecombination of a plausible model of stdp with two homeostatic pro-cesses: a fast one that is implemented as a renormalization of the sum ofall synaptic weights onto a given neuron every 20 ms, plus a rapid firingrate homeostasis (FRH) implemented by plasticity isp following a modelby [77] (see also [73]).

56. Yger P, Harris KD: The Convallis rule for unsupervised learningin cortical networks. PLoS Comput Biol 2013, 9:e1003272 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003272.

57. Kempter R, Gerstner W, van Hemmen JL: Intrinsic stabilizationof output rates by spike-based Hebbian learning. NeuralComput 2001, 13:2709-2741 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976601317098501 ISSN: 0899-7667, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11705408.

58. Kistler WM, Hemmen JL: Modeling synaptic plasticity inconjunction with the timing of pre-and postsynaptic action

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Page 9: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

174 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

potentials. Neural Comput 2000, 12:385-405 In: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/089976600300015844.

59. Rubin J, Lee DD, Sompolinsky H: Equilibrium properties oftemporally asymmetric Hebbian plasticity. Phys Rev Lett 2001,86:364-367 http://dx.doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.86.364.

60. Gutig R, Aharonov R, Rotter S, Sompolinsky H: Learning inputcorrelations through nonlinear temporally asymmetricHebbian plasticity. J Neurosci 2003, 23:3697-3714 ISSN:0270-6474, 1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/23/9/3697.

61. Morrison A, Aertsen A, Diesmann M: Spike-timing-dependentplasticity in balanced random networks. Neural Comput 2007,19:1437-1467 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/neco.2007.19.6.1437ISSN: 0899-7667, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17444756.

62. Gilson M, Burkitt AN, Grayden DB, Thomas DA, Hemmen JL:Emergence of network structure due to spike-timing-dependent plasticity in recurrent neuronal networks. I. Inputselectivity–strengthening correlated input pathways. BiolCybern 2009, 101:81-102 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00422-009-0319-4 ISSN: 0340-1200, 1432-0770, http://www.springerlink.com/content/8441p585w93u0576/.

63. Billings G, van Rossum MCW: Memory retention and spike-timing-dependent plasticity. J Neurophysiol 2009, 101:2775-2788 http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.91007.2008 In: http://jn.physiology.org/content/101/6/2775.abstract.

64. Kunkel S, Diesmann M, Morrison A: Limits to the development offeed-forward structures in large recurrent neuronal networks.Front Comput Neurosci 2011, 4:160 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2010.00160 In: http://www.frontiersin.org/Computational_Neuroscience/10.3389/fncom.2010.00160/pdf/abstract.

65. Delattre V, Keller D, Perich M, Markram H, Muller EB: Network-timing-dependent plasticity. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2015, 220http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00220 In: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fncel.2015.00220/full.

66. Bi G-q, Poo M-m: Synaptic modification of correlated activity:Hebb’s postulate revisited. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 24:139-166.

67. Markram H, Gerstner W, Sjostrom PJ: A history of spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Front Synaptic Neurosci 2011, 3:4 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2011.00004 In: http://www.frontiersin.org/Synaptic_Neuroscience/10.3389/fnsyn.2011.00004/pdf/abstract.

68. Aoto J, Nam CI, Poon MM, Ting P, Chen L: Synaptic signalingby all-trans retinoic acid in homeostatic synaptic plasticity.Neuron 2008, 60:308-320 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.012 ISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627308007071.

69. Ibata K, Sun Q, Turrigiano GG: Rapid synaptic scaling inducedby changes in postsynaptic firing. Neuron 2008, 57:819-826http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.031 ISSN: 0896-6273,http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627308002134.

70. Yee AX, Hsu Y-T, Chen L: A metaplasticity view of theinteraction between homeostatic and Hebbian plasticity. Phil.Trans. R. Soc. B 2017, 372:20160155 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0155 ISSN: 0962-8436, 1471-2970, http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/372/1715/20160155.

71. Babadi B, Abbott LF: Stability and competition in multi-spikemodels of spike-timing dependent plasticity. PLOS ComputBiol 2016, 12:e1004750 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004750 ISSN: 1553-7358, http://journals.plos.org/ploscompbiol/article?id=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004750.

72. El Boustani S, Yger P, Fregnac Y, Destexhe A: Stable learning instochastic network states. J Neurosci 2012, 32:194-214 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-11.2012 In: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/32/1/194.abstract.

73.�

Zenke F, Agnes EJ, Gerstner W: Diverse synaptic plasticitymechanisms orchestrated to form and retrieve memories inspiking neural networks. Nat Commun 2015, 6 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ncomms7922.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Here the authors demonstrate stable learning and recall of Hebbian cellassemblies in a recurrent network model by combining a plausible stdpmodel with heterosynaptic and transmitter-induced plasticity which bothact as RCPs to prevents runaway activity (see also [55�])

74. Delgado JY, Gomez-Gonzalez JF, Desai NS: Pyramidal neuronconductance state gates spike-timing-dependent plasticity. JNeurosci 2010, 30:15713-15725 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3068-10.2010 ISSN: 0270-6474, http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3068-10.2010.

75. Lim S, McKee JL, Woloszyn L, Amit Y, Freedman DJ,Sheinberg DL, Brunel N: Inferring learning rules fromdistributions of firing rates in cortical neurons. Nat Neurosci2015 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.4158. ISSN: 1097-6256,http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/vaop/ncurrent/full/nn.4158.html.

76. Wilmes KA, Sprekeler H, Schreiber S: Inhibition as a binaryswitch for excitatory plasticity in pyramidal neurons. PLOSComput Biol 2016, 12:e1004768 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004768 ISSN: 1553-7358, http://journals.plos.org/ploscompbiol/article?id=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004768.

77. Vogels TP, Sprekeler H, Zenke F, Clopath C, Gerstner W:Inhibitory plasticity balances excitation and inhibitionin sensory pathways and memory networks. Science 2011,334:1569-1573 http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1211095 In:http://www.sciencemag.org/content/334/6062/1569.abstract.

78. Luz Y, Shamir M: Balancing feed-forward excitation andinhibition via Hebbian inhibitory synaptic plasticity. PLoSComput Biol 2012, 8:e1002334 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002334.

79. Clopath C, Vogels TP, Froemke RC, Sprekeler H: Receptive fieldformation by interacting excitatory and inhibitory synapticplasticity. bioRxiv 2016:066589 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/066589In: http://www.biorxiv.org/content/early/2016/07/29/066589.

80. Wang L, Maffei A: Inhibitory plasticity dictates the sign ofplasticity at excitatory synapses. J Neurosci 2014, 34:1083-1093 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4711-13.2014 ISSN:0270-6474, 1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/34/4/1083.

81. Bear MF, Singer W: Modulation of visual cortical plasticity byacetylcholine and noradrenaline. Nature 1986, 320:172-176.

82. Frey U, Morris RGM: Synaptic tagging: implications for latemaintenance of hippocampal long-term potentiation. TrendsNeurosci 1998, 21:181-188 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01189-2 ISSN: 0166-2236, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0166223697011892.

83. Zhang JC, Lau PM, Bi GQ: Gain in sensitivity and loss intemporal contrast of STDP by dopaminergic modulation athippocampal synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009,106:13028-13033.

84. Pawlak V, Wickens JR, Kirkwood A, Kerr JND: Timing is noteverything: neuromodulation opens the STDP gate. FrontSynaptic Neurosci 2010, 2:146 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00146 ISSN: 1663-3563, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21423532.

85. Fremaux N, Gerstner W: Neuromodulated spike-timing-dependent plasticity, and theory of three-factor learning rules.Front Neural Circuits 2016, 85 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2015.00085 In: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fncir.2015.00085/full.

86. Hardingham N, Dachtler J, Fox K: The role of nitric oxide in pre-synaptic plasticity and homeostasis. Front Cell Neurosci 2013,7:190 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2013.00190.

87. Crespi F, Rossetti ZL: Pulse of nitric oxide release in responseto activation of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the ratstriatum: rapid desensitization, inhibition by receptorantagonists, and potentiation by glycine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther2004, 309:462-468 http://dx.doi.org/10.1124/jpet.103.061069ISSN: 0022-3565, 1521-0103, http://jpet.aspetjournals.org/content/309/2/462.

88. Sweeney Y, Hellgren Kotaleski J, Hennig MH: A diffusivehomeostatic signal maintains neural heterogeneity and

www.sciencedirect.com

Page 10: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

The temporal paradox of Hebb and homeostatic plasticity Zenke, Gerstner and Ganguli 175

responsiveness in cortical networks. PLoS Comput Biol 2015,11:e1004389 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004389.

89. Schultz W: Multiple dopamine functions at different timecourses. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007, 30:259-288.

90. Gu Z, Yan Z: Bidirectional regulation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II activity by dopamine D4 receptorsin prefrontal cortex. Mol Pharmacol 2004, 66:948-955 http://dx.doi.org/10.1124/mol.104.001404 ISSN: 0026-895X, 1521-0111, http://molpharm.aspetjournals.org/content/66/4/948.

91. Redondo RL, Morris RGM: Making memories last: the synaptictagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci 2011, 12:17-30 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2963 ISSN: 1471-003X, http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v12/n1/abs/nrn2963.html.

92. Lisman J: Glutamatergic synapses are structurally andbiochemically complex because of multiple plasticityprocesses: long-term potentiation, long-term depression,short-term potentiation and scaling. Philos Trans R Soc B 2017,372:20160260 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0260 ISSN:0962-8436, 1471-2970, http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/372/1715/20160260.

93. Min R, Nevian T: Astrocyte signaling controls spike timing-dependent depression at neocortical synapses. Nat Neurosci2012, 15:746-753 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3075 ISSN: 1097-6256, http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v15/n5/full/nn.3075.html.

94. Volterra A, Liaudet N, Savtchouk I: Astrocyte Ca2+ signalling: anunexpected complexity. Nat Rev Neurosci 2014, 15:327-335http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3725 ISSN: 1471-003X, http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v15/n5/abs/nrn3725.html.

95. De Pitta M, Brunel N, Volterra A: Astrocytes: orchestratingsynaptic plasticity? Neuroscience 2016, 323:43-61 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.04.001 ISSN: 0306-4522,http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0306452215003188.

96. Lynch GS, Dunwiddie T, Gribkoff V: Heterosynaptic depression:a postsynaptic correlate of long-term potentiation. Nature1977, 266:737-739 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/266737a0 In: http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v266/n5604/abs/266737a0.html.

97. Royer S, Pare D: Conservation of total synaptic weight throughbalanced synaptic depression and potentiation. Nature 2003,422:518-522 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature01530 ISSN: 0028-0836, http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v422/n6931/full/nature01530.html.

98.�

Chistiakova M, Bannon NM, Bazhenov M, Volgushev M:Heterosynaptic plasticity multiple mechanisms and multipleroles. Neuroscientist 2014, 20:483-498 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858414529829 ISSN: 1073-8584, 1089-4098, http://nro.sagepub.com/content/20/5/483.

A comprehensive review of heterosynaptic plasticity in experiments andits putative role as a RCP.

99.�

Oh W, Parajuli L, Zito K: Heterosynaptic structural plasticity onlocal dendritic segments of hippocampal CA1 neurons. CellRep 2015, 10:162-169 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2014.12.016 ISSN: 2211-1247, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2211124714010456.

This article shows that when a cluster of spines is stimulated throughglutamate uncaging, which under control conditions leads to structuralgrowth which is presumably related to Hebbian plasticity, nearby unsti-mulated spines shrink which corresponds to heterosynaptic depression.

100.�

Volgushev M, Chen J-Y, Ilin V, Goz R, Chistiakova M, Bazhenov M:Partial breakdown of input specificity of STDP at individualsynapses promotes new learning. J Neurosci 2016, 36:8842-8855 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0552-16.2016 ISSN:0270-6474, 1529-2401, http://www.jneurosci.org/content/36/34/8842.

A recent experimental study with additional modelling demonstrating thatheterosynaptic plasticity co-occurs with input-specific homosynapticplasticity and can effectively stabilize runaway LTP.

101. Zhou Q, Tao HW, Poo M-M: Reversal and stabilization ofsynaptic modifications in a developing visual system. Science2003, 300:1953-1957 http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1082212

www.sciencedirect.com

In: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/sci;300/5627/1953.

102. Bourne JN, Harris KM: Coordination of size and number ofexcitatory and inhibitory synapses results in a balancedstructural plasticity along mature hippocampal CA1 dendritesduring LTP. Hippocampus 2011, 21:354-373 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20768 ISSN: 1098-1063, http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.20768/abstract.

103. Abraham WC, Logan B, Wolff A, Benuskova L: HeterosynapticLTD in the dentate gyrus of anesthetized rat requireshomosynaptic activity. J Neurophysiol 2007, 98:1048-1051http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00250.2007 ISSN: 0022-3077, 1522-1598, http://jn.physiology.org/content/98/2/1048.

104.�

Jedlicka P, Benuskova L, Abraham WC: A voltage-based STDPrule combined with fast BCM-like metaplasticity accounts forLTP and concurrent ‘‘heterosynaptic’’ LTD in the dentategyrus in vivo. PLoS Comput Biol 2015, 11:e1004588 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004588.

A biophysical plasticity model is presented and fitted to experimentaldata. Interestingly, best fits are obtained for parameters which aresuggestive of a rapid (�10 s) sliding threshold mechanism reminiscentof the bcm model.

105. Daoudal G, Debanne D: Long-term plasticity of intrinsicexcitability: learning rules and mechanisms. Learn Mem 2003,10:456-465 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.64103 ISSN: 1072-0502,1549-5485, http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/10/6/456.

106. Sjostrom P, Rancz E, Roth A, Hausser M: Dendritic excitabilityand synaptic plasticity. Physiol Rev 2008, 88:769 http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00016.2007 ISSN: 0031-9333, http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&btnG=Search&q=intitle:Dendritic+Excitability+and+Synaptic+Plasticity#0.

107. Johnston D, Narayanan R: Active dendrites: colorful wings ofthe mysterious butterflies. Trends Neurosci 2008, 31:309-316http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2008.03.004 ISSN: 0166-2236,http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0166223608001197.

108. O’Leary T, Williams A, Franci A, Marder E: Cell types, networkhomeostasis, and pathological compensation from abiologically plausible ion channel expression model. Neuron2014, 82:809-821 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.002ISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S089662731400292X.

109. Lazar A, Pipa G, Triesch J: SORN: a self-organizing recurrentneural network. Front Comput Neurosci 2009, 3 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/neuro.10.023.2009 ISSN: 1662-5188, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2773171/.

110. Huang YY, Colino A, Selig DK, Malenka RC: The influence of priorsynaptic activity on the induction of long-term potentiation.Science 1992, 255:730-733 ISSN: 0036-8075..

111. Greenhill SD, Ranson A, Fox K: Hebbian and homeostaticplasticity mechanisms in regular spiking and intrinsic burstingcells of cortical layer 5. Neuron 2015, 88:539-552 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.025 ISSN: 0896-6273,http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627315008144.

112. Christie BR, Abraham WC: Priming of associative long-termdepression in the dentate gyrus by theta frequency synapticactivity. Neuron 1992, 9:79-84 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0896-6273(92)90222-Y ISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/089662739290222Y.

113. Goel A, Jiang B, Xu LW, Song L, Kirkwood A, Lee H-K: Cross-modal regulation of synaptic AMPA receptors in primarysensory cortices by visual experience. Nat Neurosci 2006,9:1001-1003 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1725 ISSN: 1097-6256,http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v9/n8/abs/nn1725.html.

114. Kaneko M, Stellwagen D, Malenka RC, Stryker MP: Tumornecrosis factor-a mediates one component of competitive,experience-dependent plasticity in developing visual cortex.Neuron 2008, 58:673-680 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.023 ISSN: 0896-6273, http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627308003772.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Page 11: The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic … · 2017. 6. 28. · The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic 1 plasticity Friedemann Zenke , Wulfram Gerstner2

176 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

115. Mockett B, Coussens C, Abraham WC: NMDA receptor-mediated metaplasticity during the induction of long-termdepression by low-frequency stimulation. Eur J Neurosci 2002,15:1819-1826 http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02008.x ISSN: 1460-9568, http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02008.x/abstract.

116. Li L, Gainey MA, Goldbeck JE, Feldman DE: Rapid homeostasisby disinhibition during whisker map plasticity. Proc Natl Acad

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2017, 43:166–176

Sci U S A 2014, 111:1616-1621 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1312455111 ISSN: 0027-8424, 1091-6490, http://www.pnas.org/content/111/4/1616.

117. Benuskova L, Abraham W: STDP rule endowed with the BCMsliding threshold accounts for hippocampal heterosynapticplasticity. J Comput Neurosci 2007, 22:129-133 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10827-006-0002-x ISSN: 0929-5313, http://www.springerlink.com/content/b51224m81192h232/abstract/.

www.sciencedirect.com