the why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

45
The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals Dan Weary

Upload: others

Post on 04-Oct-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Dan Weary

Page 2: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals
Page 3: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Part 1: Methods used to draw inferences regarding felt emotions?

• Acute response to noxious stimuli

• Responses with and without targeted drugs

• Motivation and conditioning tests

• Drug discrimination and generalization

Part 2: How do such feelings contribute to the experience of suffering?

Page 4: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Anand & Craig, 1996

Acute response to noxious stimuli: e.g. heel prick in infants

Page 5: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Before palpation During palpation

Stojkov et al. 2015. J. Dairy Sci. 98:5352-5361

Acute response to noxious stimuli: e.g. uterine palpation in metritic cows

Page 6: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

Passive Rectal palpation Uterine palpation

Bac

k A

rch

(cm

²)Healthy

Metritic

Stojkov et al. 2015. J. Dairy Sci. 98:5352-5361

Acute response to noxious stimuli: e.g. uterine palpation in metritic cows

Page 7: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Acute responses to noxious stimuli are intuitively compelling, but:

• Responses may not be pain specific

• Both response and lack of response can be difficult to interpret

Page 8: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Part 1: Procedures used to draw inferences regarding felt emotions?

• Acute response to noxious stimuli

• Responses with and without targeted drugs

• Motivation and conditioning tests

• Drug discrimination and generalization

Page 9: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals
Page 10: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals
Page 11: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

no relief local anasthesic local anasthesic and sedative

Nu

mb

er

head jerks

leg movements

Grondahl-Nielsen et al. 1999

Pain control: e.g. Intra-operative pain

Page 12: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Faulkner and Weary 2000. J. Dairy Sci. 83:2037-2041

Pain control: e.g. post-operative pain

Page 13: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Acute responses to noxious stimuli are intuitively compelling, but:

• Responses may not be pain specific

• Both response and lack of response can be difficult to interpret

Page 14: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Part 1: Procedures used to draw inferences regarding felt emotions?

• Acute response to noxious stimuli

• Responses with and without targeted drugs

• Motivation and conditioning tests

• Drug discrimination and generalization

Page 15: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Injection method:

Approach-avoidance testing

Ede et al., 2018. Sci. Reports 8:9443

Page 16: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Approach-avoidance testing

Ede et al., 2018. Sci. Reports 8:9443

Page 17: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Motivational tests provide pre-defined response measures with high consistency, but:

• Require inferences about motivation to access reward

• Motivation may vary with type of reward

• Some tests rely on an acute response (e.g. withdraw/escape)

Page 18: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Conditioned place aversion

Ede et al., in prep.

Page 19: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Conditioned place aversion

Ede et al., in prep.

Page 20: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Part 1: Procedures used to draw inferences regarding felt emotions?

• Acute response to noxious stimuli

• Responses with and without targeted drugs

• Motivation and conditioning tests

• Drug discrimination and generalization

Page 21: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Gauvin & Holloway, 1991. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 39: 521-523.

0

20

40

60

80

100

Saline Pentylenetetazole Cat on catnip

Drug discrimination and generalization:e.g. rats on PTZ

Res

po

nse

s (%

)

Treatment

Page 22: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Design features

Drug discrimination and generalization

Motivational testing

Analgesics and controls

Response to noxious stimuli

Response measures

Functional, specific, consistent

Function unclear, non-specific, variable

Inferences regarding felt affect?

Weary et al., 2017. Advances in the Study of Behavior 49:27-48

Page 23: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Part 1: Procedures used to draw inferences regarding felt emotions?

Part 2: How do such feelings contribute to the experience of suffering?

Page 24: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Usage in the medical literature

concurrent negative affects:

The patient required “small doses of codeine” for pain when she thought it was due to sciatica, but much higher doses were required when she was diagnosed with cancer.

Cassell, 1982

Page 25: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Usage in the medical literature

concurrent negative affects:

“In the month between an irregular chest X ray and results of the biopsy, I enjoyed very good health in the presence of serious illness… this turned that month into a controlled experiment in pure suffering.”

Frank, 2001, p. 354

Page 26: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Usage in the medical literature

concurrent negative affects:

“The tendonitis caused extraordinary pain … but I knew what was happening and had reasonable assurance that the acute phase would not last long… So here is the reverse experiment: pain with more annoyance than suffering.”

Frank, 2001, p. 354

Page 27: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Usage in the medical literature

Mood state:

Low mood

Reduced abilityto perform highly motivated tasks

Page 28: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Usage in the medical literature

Anhedonia:

Reduced motivation to perform previously rewarding tasks

Low mood

Page 29: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Usage in the medical literature

Loss of control:

“Suffering can start with anguish over the possibility that if the symptom continues, the patient will be overwhelmed or lose control”

Cassell, 1999, p. 531

Page 30: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Applying this understanding to animals?

Page 31: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

concurrent negative affects:

e.g. pain + fear

Page 32: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

anhedonia:

Evidence of anhedonia:

• reduced appetite

• reduced grooming

• reduced anticipatory behaviours

Page 33: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Assessing mood:

Page 34: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Training task

Positiveapproach

formilk reward

Negativedo not

approach; time-out

Page 35: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals
Page 36: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Cognitive Bias Task

ambiguous ambiguous ambiguous

Do calves approach these ambiguous screens?

Positiveapproach

formilk reward

Negativedo not

approach; time-out

Page 37: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

0

20

40

60

80

100

positive nearpositive

halfway near negative

negative

Screensapproached

(%)

Generalization curve

Neave et al., 2013. PLoS ONE 8(12): e80556

Page 38: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals
Page 39: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

0

20

40

60

80

100

Before disbudding

After disbudding

positive nearpositive

halfway near negative

negative

Screensapproached

(%)**

Cognitive bias after disbudding

Neave et al., 2013. PLoS ONE 8(12): e80556

Page 40: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Lecorps et al., in prep.

Page 41: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Lecorps et al., in prep.

^

and anhedoniaCognitive bias during post-operative pain

Page 42: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals
Page 43: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Control:

Page 44: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals

Pain is more likely to contribute to suffering when combined with:• Fear• Lack of control

Suffering might be identified by:• Reduced performance of motivated

behaviours• Other indicators of low mood

Suffering summary

Weary, 2014. Dilemmas in Animal Welfare, CABI, pp. 188-202.

Page 45: The why and how of assessing pain and suffering in animals