theeffectofpreparationmethod!on …...kamilla!hall!kragelund!! ! studienummer201503524!!! 3!...

84
THE EFFECT OF PREPARATION METHOD ON PRESCHOOL CHILDREN’S LIKING OF A FAMILIAR AND AN UNFAMILIAR VEGETABLE OVER MERE EXPOSURES Master’s thesis in Molecular Nutrition and Food Technology Submitted 01.06.2017 Kamilla Hall Kragelund, student no. 201503524 Supervisors: Ulla Kidmose and Babara Vad Andersen Department of Food Science, AU Årslev Institute of Science and Technology, Aarhus university

Upload: others

Post on 21-Aug-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

 

 THE  EFFECT  OF  PREPARATION  METHOD  ON  

PRESCHOOL  CHILDREN’S  LIKING  OF  A  FAMILIAR  AND  AN  UNFAMILIAR  VEGETABLE  OVER    

MERE  EXPOSURES      

                                             

   

Master’s  thesis  in  Molecular  Nutrition  and  Food  Technology  Submitted  01.06.2017  

 Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund,  student  no.  201503524  

Supervisors:  Ulla  Kidmose  and  Babara  Vad  Andersen  Department  of  Food  Science,  AU  Årslev  

Institute  of  Science  and  Technology,  Aarhus  university        

Page 2: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  2  

Abstract  Despite   the  health  benefits,  Danish   children  do  not  meet   the  national   recommendations   for  

daily  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables.  Studies  show  that  the  food  preferences  are  formed  in  the  

childhood  based  on  internal  and  external  factors,  but  can  be  influenced  by  learning  strategies.  

This   study   aims   to   investigate   if   three   different   preparation  methods   have   an   effect   on   the  

children’s  liking  of  a  familiar  vegetable  (carrot)  versus  an  unfamiliar  vegetable  (parsnip)  over  

mere   exposure.   The   263   participating   children   were   divided   into   six   exposure   groups  

according   to  what   they  were   going   to   be   exposed   to   e.g.   carrot   baked   (CBA,   n=42),   carrot  

boiled  (CBO,  n=45),  carrot  raw  (CR,  n=43),  parsnip  baked  (PBA,  n=45),  parsnip  boiled  (PBO,  

n=43)   or   parsnip   raw   (PR,   n=45).   At   pre-­‐‑test   and   post-­‐‑test,   all   the   children   tasted   all   six  

combinations  in  order  to  function  as  each  other’s  control  group.    

 

The  results  showed  that  there  was  no  significant  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  within  the  

exposure  groups,  however  there  was  significant  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  between  the  

exposed   children  and   the  non-­‐‑exposed   children   from  pre-­‐‑test   to  post-­‐‑test,   except   in   the  CR  

and   CBA   groups.   The   children   gave   the   familiar   carrot   a   higher   liking   compared   to   the  

unfamiliar  parsnip  and  even  though  the  liking  of  the  unfamiliar  parsnip  showed  an  increasing  

tendency,   the   familiar   raw   carrot   remained   the   most   liked   from   pre-­‐‑test   to   post-­‐‑test.   The  

study  also  found  that  the  raw  preparation  method  did  not  influence  the  liking  of  the  familiar  

carrot   over   mere   exposures,   as   the   liking   remained   the   same,   however   the   liking   of   the  

unfamiliar   parsnip   increased   slightly.   Also,   both   the   baked   carrot   and   parsnip   increased  

slightly  in  liking  as  well  as  the  boiled  carrot  and  parsnip.  The  results  showed  that  the  liking  of  

preparation  method  in  general  was  higher  in  the  familiar  carrot  compared  to  the  unfamiliar  

parsnip.   Furthermore,   the   gender   of   the   children   did   not   affect   the   liking   of   the   vegetables  

except   the   boys   had   a   higher   liking   of   PR   at   the   pre-­‐‑test.   However,   the   children’s   level   of  

neophobia  had  an  effect  on  the  liking  as  the  most  neophobic  children  had  the  lowest  liking  of  

the  vegetables  and  the  children  with  the   lowest   level  of  neophobia  had  the  highest   liking  of  

vegetables.  Hence,  it  can  be  concluded  that  different  preparation  methods  and  mere  exposure  

can   improve   the   liking   for   vegetables.  However,  more   research   is   needed   in   relation   to   the  

different  preparation  methods,  mere  exposure  and  food  neophobia  in  children.    

   

Page 3: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  3  

Resume  (in  Danish)  

På  trods  af,  at  forskning  viser,  at  grøntsager  er  sunde  for  os  og  indeholder  vigtige  vitaminer,  

mineraler  og  fibre,  så  er  indtaget  af  grøntsager  blandt  danske  børn  ikke  højt  nok.  I  Danmark  

anbefales   det,   at   børn   i   alderen  4-­‐‑10   år   indtager   300-­‐‑500   gram  grøntsager   om  dagen,  men  

ifølge  en  undersøgelse  er  det  kun  21  %  af  de  danske  børn,  der  opfylder  kravene  (Fagt,  2015).  

Dette  betyder,  at  der  stadig  er  et  stort  behov  for  at  undersøge,  hvad  der  kan  få  børnene  til  at  

øge   deres   indtag   af   grøntsager.   Undersøgelser   viser,   at   et   menneskes   præferencer   for  

fødevarer   primært   dannes   i   barndommen   og   at   barnet   ofte   tager   sine   kostvaner  med   ind   i  

voksenlivet   (2017).   Derudover   spiser   barnet,   hvad   det   kan   lide,   hvilket   i   barndommen   kan  

være  præget  af  fødevare  neofobi,  der  blandt  andet  er  beskrevet  som  en  manglende  lyst  til  at  

smage  på  en  ukendt   fødevare   (Birch  &  Fisher,   1998).  Tidligere   studier   viser,   at   indlærings-­‐‑

strategien  gentagen  eksponering  af  en  fødevare  kan  forbedre  børns  accept  af  en  i  begyndelsen  

ukendt/ikke   vellidt   grøntsag   samt   at  måden,   hvorpå   grøntsagen   er   tilberedt   også   påvirker  

børnenes  accept.  Derfor  ønsker  dette  studie  at  undersøge,  hvordan  forskellige  tilberednings-­‐‑

metoder   samt   gentagen   eksponering   påvirker   børnenes   liking   af   to   forskellige   grøntsager,  

hvoraf  én  er  velkendt  for  dem  og  den  anden  er  mindre  velkendt.    

 

De   263   deltagende   børn   blev   inddelt   i   seks   forskellige   grupper,   som   var   opkaldt   efter   den  

grøntsag  og  tilberedningsmetode,  som  børnene  i  gruppen  skulle  eksponeres  for  otte  gange  i  

perioden   med   gentagen   eksponering;   carrot   baked   (CBA),   carrot   boiled   (CBO),   carrot   raw  

(CR),  parsnip  baked  (PBA),  parsnip  boiled  (CBO)  og  parsnip  raw  (PR).  Til  prætesten  og  post-­‐‑

testen   smagte   alle   børnene   på   alle   smagsprøver   for   på   den   måde   at   kunne   være   kontrol-­‐‑

gruppe  for  hinanden.  Resultaterne  viste,  at  gentagen  eksponering  ikke  havde  nogen  effekt  på  

børnenes   liking   indenfor   eksponeringsgrupperne,  men   at   der   til   gengæld   var   en   signifikant  

effekt  af  gentagen  eksponering  imellem  de  børn,  der  var  blevet  eksponeret  sammenlignet  med  

de  børn,  der   ikke  var  blevet  eksponeret  bortset   fra  grupperne  CR  og  CBA.  Børnene  gav  den  

velkendte   gulerod  højere   liking   end  den  ukendte  pastinak  ved  prætest   og   selvom   liking   for  

den   ukendte   pastinak   viste   en   stigende   tendens   gennem   de   gentagne   eksponeringer,   så  

forblev  liking  for  den  velkendte  gulerod  højere  end  for  pastinakken.  Resultaterne  viste  også,  

at  den  rå   tilberedningsmethode   ikke  påvirkede   liking  af  den  velkendte  gulerod,  men  gav  en  

lille  stigning  i  liking  ved  pastinakken  i  løbet  af  de  gentagne  eksponeringer.  Både  den  bagte  og  

den  kogte  tilberedningsmetode  viste  en  stigende  tendens  i  både  gulerod  og  pastinak.  

Page 4: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  4  

Børnenes  køn  havde  generelt  ikke  nogen  effekt  på  liking  bortset  fra,  at  drengene  havde  højere  

liking  af  PR  ved  prætest.  Børnenes  neofobiske  status  havde  effekt  på  børnenes  liking,  idet  de  

børn,  der  var  i  gruppen  ”Ikke  neofobisk”  havde  højere  liking  af  grøntsagerne  end  de  børn,  der  

var   i   gruppen   ”Neofobisk”.   Baseret   på   disse   resultater,   konkluderes   det,   at   gentagen  

eksponering  samt  forskellige  tilberedningsmetoder  kan  anvendes  til  at  forbedre  børns  accept  

af  grøntsager,  hvilket  er  i  overensstemmelse  med  allerede  eksisterende  litteratur  på  området.      

Page 5: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  5  

Preface  

This  report  was  written  for  a  60  ECTS  master  project  at  the  master’s  programme  Molecular  

Nutrition  and  Food  Technology  at  Institute  of  Science  and  Technology,  Aarhus  University.  The  

experimental   work   of   the   study   was   performed   at   four   public   schools   in   Odense   in  

collaboration  with   Department   of   Food   Science,   AU   Årslev.   Postdoc   Barbara   Vad   Andersen  

and  associate  professor  Ulla  Kidmose  were  supervisors.    

 

The  author  of   this  study  wish   to  give   thanks   to   the  children,  parents  and   teachers   from  the  

four  schools   for  participating   in   the  study.  Also,  a   thank   to  Grete  Nielsen  and  Brian  Due   for  

their   help   in   carrying   out   the   study   and   to   Ulla   Kidmose   and   Barbara   Vad   Andersen   for  

supervision  through  the  process.    

 

 

 

 

 

This  thesis  has  been  prepared  by  

 

____________________________________________________      __________________    

Signature  of  the  author                                                                                            Date    

   

Page 6: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  6  

Table  of  contents  

1.  Introduction  ............................................................................................................................................  8  1.1  The  objective  and  hypotheses  of  the  study  ...........................................................................................  9  1.2  Guidance  for  the  Reader  ..............................................................................................................................  9  1.3  Explanation  of  terms  .................................................................................................................................  10  1.4  Abbreviations  and  symbols  .....................................................................................................................  10  

2.  Theoretical  Background  ..................................................................................................................  11  2.1  Children’s  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables  ..........................................................................................  11  2.2  Vegetables  –  carrots  and  parsnips  ........................................................................................................  12  2.3  Development  of  children’s  food  preferences  ....................................................................................  13  2.4  Strategies  to  increase  children’s  acceptance  of  fruit  and  vegetables  .......................................  15  2.4.1  Mere  exposure  ..........................................................................................................................................................  15  2.4.2  Preparation  methods  ............................................................................................................................................  16  

2.5  Food  Neophobia  ..........................................................................................................................................  18  2.6  Measuring  tools  for  children’s  liking  ...................................................................................................  19  2.7  Literature  study  ..........................................................................................................................................  20  2.7.1  Mere  exposure  ..........................................................................................................................................................  20  2.7.2  Food  neophobia  .......................................................................................................................................................  21  2.7.3  Preparation  methods  ............................................................................................................................................  22  

2.8  Sub-­‐‑conclusion  .............................................................................................................................................  23  3.  Materials  and  methods  .....................................................................................................................  24  3.1  Overall  study  design  ..................................................................................................................................  24  3.1.1  Structure  of  the  study  design  .............................................................................................................................  24  3.1.2  The  phases  of  the  study  ........................................................................................................................................  25  3.1.3  Scale  for  measurement  of  liking  .......................................................................................................................  26  

3.2  Participants  ..................................................................................................................................................  26  3.2.1  Recruitment  of  the  test  participants  ...............................................................................................................  26  3.2.2  Test  participants  .....................................................................................................................................................  27  3.2.3  Randomization  of  participants  for  mere  exposure  phase  .....................................................................  27  3.2.4  Randomization  of  participants  for  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  ....................................................................  28  

3.3  Experimental  procedure  ..........................................................................................................................  29  3.3.1  Pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  procedure  .....................................................................................................................  29  3.3.2  Mere  exposure-­‐‑test  ................................................................................................................................................  29  3.3.3  Deviations  from  test  procedure  ........................................................................................................................  30  

3.4  Experimental  stimuli  .................................................................................................................................  30  3.4.1  Choice  of  vegetables  and  preparation  methods  ........................................................................................  30  

3.5  Statistical  data  processing  .......................................................................................................................  32  3.5.1  Pre-­‐‑test  liking  ...........................................................................................................................................................  32  3.5.2  Test  for  the  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  within  the  groups  ......................................................  33  3.5.3  Test  for  the  effect  of  gender  on  liking  ............................................................................................................  33  3.5.4  Test  for  the  effect  of  level  of  neophobia  on  liking  .....................................................................................  33  3.5.5  Correlation  tests  ......................................................................................................................................................  33  

4.  Results  ...................................................................................................................................................  34  4.1  Participants  ..................................................................................................................................................  34  4.2  Pre-­‐‑test  liking  ...............................................................................................................................................  34  4.3  Mere  exposure  .............................................................................................................................................  36  4.4  Effect  of  gender  on  liking  .........................................................................................................................  40  4.5  Effect  of  level  of  neophobia  on  liking  ...................................................................................................  41  

Page 7: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  7  

4.6  The  correlation  tests  .................................................................................................................................  44  5.  Discussion  .............................................................................................................................................  48  Strengths  and  limitations  ................................................................................................................................  54  

6.  Conclusion  ............................................................................................................................................  56  7.  Perspectives  .........................................................................................................................................  57  8.  References  ............................................................................................................................................  58  9.  APPENDIX  .............................................................................................................................................  64  Appendix  A  ...........................................................................................................................................................  65  Appendix  B  ...........................................................................................................................................................  69  Appendix  C  ...........................................................................................................................................................  72  Appendix  D  ...........................................................................................................................................................  73  Appendix  E  ...........................................................................................................................................................  74  Appendix  F  ...........................................................................................................................................................  75  Appendix  G  ...........................................................................................................................................................  77  Appendix  H  ...........................................................................................................................................................  79  Appendix  I  ............................................................................................................................................................  80  Appendix  J  ............................................................................................................................................................  83  Appendix  K  ...........................................................................................................................................................  84  

 

   

Page 8: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  8  

1.  Introduction  Years   of   medical   and   nutritional   research   have   shown   several   health   benefits   from   high  

consumption  of   fruits   and  vegetables   including   reduced   risk  of   life-­‐‑threatening  diseases   for  

instance   cardiovascular   conditions,   type   II   diabetes,   obesity   and   certain   types   of   cancers  

(Reetica  Rekhy,  2014).  Fruits  and  vegetables  are  important  for  human  health  as  they  contain  

important  vitamins,  minerals  and  fibers,  which  have  a  preventing  effect  on  diseases  (Astrup,  

2005).  The  World  Health  Organization   (WHO)  has  estimated   that  approximately  1.7  million  

deaths  yearly  or  2.8  %  of  all  deaths  yearly  are  related  to  the  current  low  intake  of  fruits  and  

vegetables   (2013).   Beyond   that,   a   study   from   2014   suggests   that   there   is   “an   inverse  dose  

response  relationship  between  fruit  and  vegetables  consumption  and  all  cause  mortality”  (Wang  

X.,   2014).   Therefore,   a   high   intake   of   fruits   and   vegetables   is   important   in   a   healthy   diet.  

However,  the  global  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables  does  not  yet  meet  the  recommendations.  

In  Europe,  the  daily  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables  for  an  adult  is  220  g,  which  is  only  half  of  

the  recommended  intake  (2011).  In  Denmark,  the  national  recommendation  is  600  g  of  fruits  

and  vegetables  per  day  for  adults  and  for  children  in  the  age  group  4-­‐‑10  years,  which  is  the  

relevant   age   for   the   present   study,   the   recommendation   is   300-­‐‑500   g   per   day.   Also   in  

Denmark,   the  daily   recommendations   are  not  met,   as   only  21%  of   the  Danish   children   and  

young  people  meet  the  daily  intake  (Fagt  &  Matthiessen,  2017).  

 

Studies  show  that  food  preferences  are  formed  in  the  childhood  and  that  the  food  preferences  

remain  the  same  into  adulthood  (2017)  (2004).  However,  children  tend  to  eat  what  they  like  

and   this   can   be   influenced  by   food  neophobia,  which   is   described   as   a   reluctance   to   eat   or  

taste  novel   foods  (Birch  &  Fisher,  1998).  But,  previous  studies  have  successfully  shown  that  

the   learning   strategy  mere   exposure   of   a   vegetable   has   had   an   effect   and   furthermore   can  

improve  the  children’s  accept  of  an  initially  disliked  vegetable  to  the  same  level  as  an  initially  

liked  vegetable  (2012)  (2007).  Other  studies  have  similar  found  that  the  preparation  method  

of   vegetable   also   influences   the   liking   of   it   due   to   sensory   attributes   (Gertrude  G.   Zeinstra,  

2010).   Therefore,   the   present   study   aims   to   investigate   how   three   different   preparation  

methods   influence  preschool  children’s   liking  of  a   familiar  and  an  unfamiliar  vegetable  over  

mere  exposure.      

Page 9: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  9  

1.1  The  objective  and  hypotheses  of  the  study  

The  overall  objective  of  this  study  is  to  investigate  how  three  different  preparation  methods  

influence  the  liking  of  a  familiar  vegetable  and  an  unfamiliar  vegetable  over  the  strategy  mere  

exposure.        

 

Following  are  the  hypotheses  in  relation  to  the  overall  objective  of  the  study:  

1.   The  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  unfamiliar)  has  an  effect  on  liking  

2.   The  type  of  preparation  method  has  an  effect  on  liking  

3.   The  type  of  vegetable  and  preparation  method  has  an  effect  on  liking  

4.   The  strategy  mere  exposure  has  an  effect  on  liking  

 

Additional  to  the  hypotheses  to  overall  objective  of  the  study,  there  are  also  some  hypotheses  

related  to  the  questionnaires,  which  can  be  seen  below:  

5.   The  type  of  gender  has  an  effect  on  liking  

6.   The  level  of  food  neophobia  has  an  effect  on  liking  

7.   There  is  a  correlation  between  the  liking  and  the  intake  of  respectively  carrots  and  

parsnips  reported  by  the  parents  

8.   There  is  a  correlation  between  the  liking  of  respectively  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  

by  the  parents  and  the  children’s  liking  of  carrots  and  parsnips  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  

 

1.2  Guidance  for  the  reader  

This   thesis   is   written   in   English   as   most   literature   regarding   the   topic   is   in   English   and  

therefore  it  is  most  natural  to  write  the  following  in  the  same  languish.  The  thesis  starts  with  

a   theoretical   background   in   section   2   involving   a   description   of   the   development   of   food  

preferences   in   children,   the   learning   strategy  mere   exposure   and   preparation  methods.   In  

section  3,   the  materials  and  method  of   the  study   is  described  together  with  a  description  of  

the  different  phases  of   the  study,   the  recruitment  of   the  participants  and  the  statistical  data  

processing.   The   results   are   presented   in   section   4   and   discussed   in   section   5.   Afterwards,  

there  is  a  conclusion  and  a  perspective  of  the  study.    

   

Page 10: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  10  

1.3  Explanation  of  terms  

In  order  to  reduce  confusion  of  the  terms  that  are  often  used  in  the  literature  regarding  the  

topic  of  this  study  for  instance  food  acceptance,  food  preferences  and  liking,  an  explanation  of  

the  terms  used  in  this  study  will  be  given  here.  However,  when  the  literature  is  described,  the  

terms   that   they  have  been  used  will   of   course  be  used.  According   to  Cardello   et   al.   (2000),  

there   are   two   important   variables   in   studies   of   consumer   food   behavior;   behavioral   and  

attitudinal,  of  which  the  behavioral  variables  consists  of  measurements  such  as  intake  and  the  

attitudinal   variable   includes   the   affective   responses   to   food   such   as   liking/disliking.   In  

relation   to   this,   the   term  food  preference   is  used  as  a  behavioral  measure,  when  one  prefer  

something   over   another   in   contrast   to   the   term   food   acceptance,   which   is   used   as   an  

attitudinal  measure  for  prepared  food  that  is  actually  tasted.  This  means,  that  in  this  study  the  

children’s   liking   of   a   familiar   and   an   unfamiliar   vegetable   is   used   as   an   attitudinal  

measurement  for  vegetable  acceptance,  which  can  give  an  idea  about  the  children’s  intake  of  

vegetables.    

 

1.4  Abbreviations  and  symbols  

CBA:  baked  carrot    

CBO:  boiled  carrot    

CR:  raw  carrot    

PBA:  baked  parsnip    

PBO:  boiled  parsnip    

PR:  raw  parsnip    

 

 

Page 11: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  11  

2.  Theoretical  Background  

2.1  Children’s  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables  

In   the   following   section,   the   current   intake   of   fruits   and   vegetables   among   children  will   be  

reviewed.  As  fruits  and  vegetables  are  usually  being  discussed  as  a  united  group  of  foods,  they  

will  also  be  mentioned  together  as  one  group  of  foods  here.    

 

As  mentioned   in   the   introduction,   a  high   intake  of   fruit   and  vegetables   is   important   for   the  

individual  human  health  and  for  the  public  health  as   fruit  and  vegetables  contain   important  

vitamins,  minerals  and  fibers.  This  means  that  they  have  a  preventive  impact  on  development  

of  different  diseases  such  as  cardiovascular  diseases  and  some  types  of  cancer  (Astrup,  2005).  

In  Denmark,   the  national  board  of  health   recommend   that  4-­‐‑10  years  old   children  eat  300-­‐‑

500  gram  of   fruits  and  vegetables  each  day.  A  report  written  by  Fagt  et  al.   (2017)   from  the  

Technical  University  of  Denmark  (DTU)  concerning  the  Danish  people’s  dietary  habits  shows  

that  Danish  children  and  young  people  between  4-­‐‑17  years  in  general  eat  inadequate  amounts  

of   fruits   and   vegetables   as   only   21%   of   them   meet   the   national   recommendations.  

Additionally,  the  data  shows  that  the  4-­‐‑10-­‐‑year-­‐‑old  children,  who  are  relevant  for  the  present  

study,  have  an  intake  of  approximately  80  g  more  fruits  and  vegetables  per  day  compared  to  

the   older   children   in   the   report.   In   general,   the   intake   of   fruits   and   vegetables   for   children  

between  4  and  17  years  has  been  unchanged  since  2005  as  there  has  been  an  increase  in  the  

intake  of  vegetables  for  the  4-­‐‑10  years  old  and  a  decrease  in  the  intake  of  fruits  among  the  11-­‐‑

17  years.  However,  an  increase  in  the  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables  of  50  g  per  day  among  

the  4-­‐‑6-­‐‑years   old  means   that   the  proportion   that   comply  with   the   recommendations   in   the  

age-­‐‑group  4-­‐‑6  years  old  increases  from  25%  to  40%  in  the  period  from  2005  to  2013  (Fagt  &  

Matthiessen,  2017).  In  further  details,  results  from  the  Danish  eating  habits  2011-­‐‑2013  shows  

that  boys  between  4-­‐‑9  years  in  average  eats  158  g  of  vegetables  per  day  and  that  girls  in  the  

same  age  group  eats  in  average  157  g  of  vegetables  per  day  (Fagt,  2015).  

 

All  these  results  show  that  there  has  been  an  increase  in  the  intake  of  vegetables  for  children  

between  4-­‐‑6  years,  but  there  is  still  a  requirement  for  a  further  improvement  as  around  60%  

of  them  still  do  not  have  a  high  enough  intake  compared  to  the  recommendations.    

   

Page 12: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  12  

2.2  Vegetables  –  carrots  and  parsnips  

As   mentioned   in   the   previous   section,   vegetables   are   important   for   human   health   as   they  

contain   essential   vitamins,   minerals   and   fibers   as   well   as   many   other   health-­‐‑beneficial  

compounds   (Astrup,   2005).   In   the   present   study   two   vegetables   a   carrot   and   a   parsnip   is  

chosen  as   the   test  vegetables  because  the  carrot   is  a   familiar  vegetable  and  the  parsnip   is  a  

unfamiliar  vegetable.  Carrots  are  one  of  the  most  popular  vegetables  especially  in  Europe  and  

they  are  found  in  many  different  forms  both  fresh,  frozen,  “cut-­‐‑and-­‐‑peel”  etc.,  which  makes  it  

easy   for   the   consumer   to   use   them   in   their   everyday   life   (Tanumihardjo,   Suri,   Simon,   &  

Goldman,  2016).  This  makes  the  carrot  one  of  the  most  consumed  coarse  vegetable,  which  is  

supported  by  a  study  from  Beck    (2014),  which  showed  that  90  %  of  Danish  adult  consumers  

had  a  high  intake  of  carrots.  In  comparison,  only  50-­‐‑60  %  of  the  adult  consumers  consumed  

parsnips   minimum   once   a   month,   which   makes   it   a   less   eaten   and   less   familiar   vegetable  

compared  to  the  carrot.  The  consumer’s  high  intake  and  general  liking  for  carrots  are  related  

to  a  perceived  sweetness  of  carrots  (Varming,  et  al.,  2004).  However,  vegetables  in  general  are  

often  characterized  as  having  a  bitter  taste,  which  is  one  of  the  primary  causes  of  low  intake  

and  low  acceptance  from  especially  children.  Many  different  compounds  have  been  related  to  

the  bitter  taste  in  carrots,  but  the  polyacetylenes  falcarionol  (FaOH),  falcarindiol  (FaDOH)  and  

falcarindiol  3-­‐‑acetate  (FaDOAc)  have  been  shown  to  have  the  major  impact  on  the  bitter  taste  

in   carrots   (Kreutzmann,   Christensen,   &   Edelenbos,   2008).   The   polyacetylenes   also   occur  

naturally   in   parsnips.   Minimal   processing   results   in   a   reduction   of   the   compounds   due   to  

peeling  of  the  vegetable  skin  in  which  a  high  distribution  of  the  compounds  is  placed  and  also  

heat  processing  reduces  the  levels  of  polyacetylene  (Koidis,  Rawson,  &  Brunton,  2015).    

 

Carrots   and   parsnips   are   root   crops   and   they   are   related   to   each   other   despite   they   are  

different  vegetables.  They  both  have  a  long  history  –  the  carrot  was  being  used  in  Central  Asia  

and  Afghanistan  1100  years  ago  and  the  parsnip  was  consumed  in  Roman  times.  The  carrot  

comes  in  a  variety  of  different  colors,  but  in  this  study  the  focus  will  be  on  the  orange  carrot.  

Carrots  are  available  in  large  parts  of  the  world  and  function  well  in  cooler  climates  and  both  

parsnips   and   carrots   can   be   eaten   during   the   winter,   where   many   vegetables   are   not   in  

season.  If   they  are  stored  in  refrigerator,   the  shelf   life  can  be  up  to  a  month  (Tanumihardjo,  

Suri,  Simon,  &  Goldman,  2016).  

 

Page 13: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  13  

Carrots   are   a   significant   source   of   a-­‐‑carotene   and   b-­‐‑carotene,   which   can   be   cleaved   into  

respectively   one   or   two  molecules   of   vitamin  A   in   the   human   body.   This  makes   carrots   an  

important  source  of  vitamin  A,  which  is  essential  for  reproduction,  healthy  eyes,  cell  growth  

and  strengthening  of   the   immune  system.  Both  carrots  and  parsnips  contain   fibers  with  3  g  

fiber   per   100   g   raw   carrot   and   5   g   fiber   per   100   g   parsnip.   Beyond   all   this,   carrots   have  

antioxidant   capacity   as   both  a-­‐‑carotene   and  b-­‐‑carotene   are   antioxidant   and   therefore   they  

can  capture  free  radicals  (Tanumihardjo,  Suri,  Simon,  &  Goldman,  2016).    

 

2.3  Development  of  children’s  food  preferences  

In  this  section,  the  development  of  children’s  food  preferences  and  the  factors  that  influence  it  

will  be  described.  According  to  Helland  et  al.   (2017)  children’s   food  preferences  are   formed  

when  the  child  is  approximately  2-­‐‑3  years  old  and  they  tend  to  persist  into  adulthood.  There  

are  several  factors  that  affect  the  development  of  the  children’s  food  preferences  and  they  are  

shown  in  figure  1  below,  which  is  inspired  by  a  review  by  Johnson  et  al.  (2016).    

 

 Figure  1:  The  internal  and  external  factors  and  the  learning  strategies  that  influence  the  development  of  children’s  food  preferences.    

Vegetable  consumption

Internal  factors:Genetic  predispositionAgeNeophobia

External  factors:Early  learning  experiencesParent  neophobiaHome  environmentSocioeconomic  statusVegetable  availabilityPreparation  method  of  vegetable

Learning  strategies:Mere  exposureFlavour-­‐‑flavour  learningFlavour-­‐‑nutrient  learningReward

Page 14: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  14  

 

The  internal  factors  are  as  followed  genetic  predispositions,  age  and  level  of  neophobia.  Some  

children  have  a  genetically  predisposed  response  to  bitter  taste  as  they  are  sensitive  to  6-­‐‑n-­‐‑

propylthiouracil   (PROP)   and   therefore   rate   vegetables   as  more   bitter.   This  might   result   in  

negative  experiences  related  to  vegetables  and  a  lower  intake  (Bell  &  Tepper,  2006).  The  age  

of   the   children   is   also   an   important   factor,   as   children   have   an   innate   taste   preference   for  

sweet  and  salty   in  their  early  childhood  and  an   inborn  distaste   for  bitter.  Though,   this  taste  

preference  declines  with  age  (Mennella,  2014).  The  last  internal  factor  the  level  of  neophobia  

is   also   related   to   age,   as   it   often   starts   at   the   age   of   2   years   old   and   is   at   its   height   up   to  

preschool  age,  but  in  most  cases  it  declines  with  age  after  this  period.  However,  it  can  result  in  

a  “rejection  of  foods  that  are  novel  or  unknown  to  the  child”  (Doveya,  Staples,  Gibson,  &  Halfor,  

2007).   The   external   factors   consist   of   early   learning   experiences,   parent   neophobia,   home  

environment,   socioeconomic   status,   vegetable   availability   and   preparation   method   of   the  

vegetables.   In  general,   these  external   factors  show  that  young  children  to  a  great  extent  are  

influenced   by   their   home   environment   and   their   parent’s   intake   of   vegetables   (Larson   &  

Story,  2009).  Studies  have  also  shown  that  the  children’s  intake  of  vegetables  is  influenced  by  

socioeconomic  factors  in  the  family  as  the  availability  and  accessibility  of  vegetables  in  lower  

socioeconomic  families  is  associated  with  economic  expenses  (Cullen,  et  al.,  2003).    In  recent  

years,  the  preparation  method  of  the  vegetable  has  also  been  shown  as  a  factor  that  influences  

the  consumption  of  vegetables,  as  some  preparation  methods  e.g.  boiled  are  more  wanted  by  

children   than   other   preparation  methods   (Zeinstra,   2010).   However,   this  will   be   described  

more  in  details  later.    

 

Even   though   the   children’s   food   preferences   are   formed   based   on   the   above-­‐‑mentioned  

factors,   studies   have   shown   that   there   are   some   learning   strategies   that   can   be   used   to  

increase   the   children’s   familiarity   of   vegetables   and   thereby   improve   the   acceptance   and  

consumption  (Johnson,  2016).  The  most  studied  strategies  are  mere  exposure,  flavour-­‐‑flavour  

and  flavour-­‐‑nutrient  learning  and  the  use  of  rewards.  Mere  exposure  is  the  strategy  that  has  

shown   the  most   consistent   effects   on   children’s   vegetable   acceptance   (Lakkakula   A,   2010)  

(Hausner,  Olsen,  &  Møller,  2012).    

Page 15: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  15  

 

2.4  Strategies  to  increase  children’s  acceptance  of  fruit  and  vegetables  

As   just   described,   children’s   food  preferences   are  determined  of   both   internal   and   external  

factors.  But  even  though  the  child’s  food  preferences  are  formed  early  in  life,  they  can  still  be  

modified   by   different   strategies   for   instance   mere   exposure,   flavour-­‐‑flavour   and   flavour-­‐‑

nutrient   learning  (Cardello  A.   ,  Schutz,  Snow,  &  Lesher,  2000).  These   learning  strategies  can  

be  used   to  modify   children’s  acceptance   for   food  and  as   relevant   for   this   study   they  can  be  

used  to  increase  the  liking  of  a  familiar  and  an  unfamiliar  vegetable  among  school  children.  In  

the   following   section,   the   strategy   mere   exposure   and   the   external   factor   the   preparation  

method  will  be  described  based  on  the  objective  to  increase  the  liking  of  vegetables.  

 

2.4.1  Mere  exposure  

Several   studies   (2010)   (2012)   (2012)  have   tested   the  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  children’s  

acceptance   of   different   types   of   food   including   vegetables.   The   strategy   mere   exposure  

function  by  frequently  repeated  exposures  of  the  stimulus  and  thereby  creating  an  increased  

familiarity  with  the  food.  In  order  for  mere  exposure  to  be  a  success,  it  is  important  that  the  

experience  with   the   tasting   of   the   food  does   not   produce   any   negative   affect   as   this  would  

probably  decrease  the  acceptance.  The  associations  that  the  children  have  with  the  tasting  of  

the  food  products  are  therefore  important  for  their  liking  (Zajonc,  1968).    

 

The   number   of   mere   exposures   that   are   needed   to   change   the   children’s   acceptance   for  

vegetables  varies  slightly  depending  on   the  study.  The  number  of  mere  exposures  seems   to  

depend  on  different  factors  for  instance  the  type  of  food,  as  only  three  exposures  are  sufficient  

for  acceptance  of  fruit,  whereas  vegetables  require  up  to  eight  or  nine  exposures  (Horne  P.  J.,  

2004).   For   instance,  Maier   et   al.   (2007)   compared  an   initially  disliked  and  an   initially   liked  

vegetable  and  showed  that  after  seven  to  eight  exposures  most  infants  accepted  the  initially  

disliked  vegetable.  Another  study  concluded  that  “there  was  a  tendency  for  highly  liked  foods  to  

remain  so  and  for  least  liked  foods  to  become  more  liked  over  time”  (Brandi  Y.  Rollins,  2010).  A  

study   from   2011   showed   that   the   children  who  were   exposed   to   an   initially   disliked   food  

(snack  bar)  increased  their  acceptance  of  the  food  after  nine  exposures  and  the  children  that  

were   exposed   to   an   initially   liked   food   (also   snack   bar)   showed   a   stable   liking   across   the  

Page 16: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  16  

exposures   (Hausner,   Hartvig,   Reinbach,   Wendin,   &   Bredie,   2012).   Based   on   the   above-­‐‑

mentioned  results,  approximately  eight  or  nine  mere  exposures  are  required  before  there  is  

an  effect  in  the  acceptance  of  the  initially  disliked  food.      

 

2.4.1.2  The  boredom  effect  

The  boredom  effect  is  typically  seen  in  studies  with  10-­‐‑20  mere  exposures  and  will  result  in  

decreased   acceptance   of   the   food   (R.   Bornstein,   1990).   The   boredom   effect   can   be   seen   in  

children  that  have  been  exposed  to  a  food  repeatedly  and  this  can  be  a  limiting  factor  for  the  

effect   of  mere   exposure   due   to   the   increased   level   of   familiarity   (Mojet,   2008).   A   study   by  

Olsen   et   al.   (2012)   showed   that   liking   for  most   vegetables   decreased   during   the   exposure  

period  even  though  the  opposite  was  expected.  They  discuss  that  a  reason  for  this  result  could  

be  due  to  boredom  as  the  daily  exposures  might  have  been  too  extensive,  too  monotonous  and  

the  children  got  bored  of  the  vegetables  and  the  repetition.  Results  from  a  study  by  Hausner  et  

al.  (2012)  suggests  that  an  exposure  interval  of  2-­‐‑3  days  may  prevent  the  boredom  effect  as  

the  acceptance  for  the  snack  bar  in  their  study  was  constant  over  exposures.      

 

Due  to  the  results  mentioned  above,  it  is  important  to  be  aware  of  the  boredom  effect  in  the  

research  primarily  about  mere  exposure  as  there  will  be  a  risk  of  it  affecting  the  results.  The  

results  could  also  be  transferred  to  the  reality  as  the  boredom  effect  could  also  occur  here  if  

the  same  food  is  repeatedly  being  served  for  a  child.  However,  the  risk  of  boredom  effect   in  

reality  is  smaller  compared  to  the  risk  in  mere  exposure  studies  due  to  naturally  more  varied  

food  in  reality.    

 

2.4.2  Preparation  methods  

Today,  there  is  limited  research  on  how  the  different  preparation  methods  influence  the  liking  

of  vegetables  among  children.  A  study  by  Zeinstra  et  al.  (2010)  showed  that  the  preparation  

method  influenced  the  liking  of  for  instance  carrots  and  that  the  children  preferred  boiled  and  

steamed  compared  to  mashed,  grilled,  stir-­‐‑fried  and  deep-­‐‑fried.  This  was  explained  by  the  fact  

that   boiling   was   the   most   familiar   preparation   method   of   carrots   and   because   of   sensory  

attributes,  as  the  boiling  and  steaming  resulted  in  the  most  uniform  surface  of  the  carrots,  the  

typical  carrot  taste,  colour  and  crunchiness.  Beyond  that,  the  absence  of  brown  colouring  was  

also  described  as  being  an  important  factor.  Therefore,  the  study  concluded  that  the  liking  of  

Page 17: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  17  

vegetables  was  dependent  of  a  mixture  of  familiarity  of  the  taste,  a  uniform  appearance  and  a  

texture  that  the  children  can  handle  due  to  their  physical  development  of  teeth,  muscles  etc.  

(Gertrude   G.   Zeinstra,   2010).   These   results   regarding   the   sensory   attributes   are   consistent  

with  the  results  found  by  Donadini  et  el.  (2012)  as  they  found  that  nearly  half  of  the  children  

related  their  acceptance  of  the  vegetable  to  the  sensory  characteristics.  For  instance,  a  sweet  

taste   and   the   original   colour   of   the   vegetable   increased   the   acceptance,   whereas   brown  

colouring  and  a  tough  texture  had  a  negative  impact  on  acceptance.    

 

Results  from  another  study  by  Poelman  et  al.  (2013)  also  showed  that  children’s  acceptance  

for  vegetables  in  this  case  Brassica  vegetables  was  influenced  by  the  preparation  method,  but  

that  it  was  the  preparation  time  that  had  the  greatest  effect  on  acceptance.  The  study  showed  

that   the   children   accepted   boiling   and   steaming   equally   much,   but   that   the   medium  

preparation   time  was  preferred  over   the   short   and   the   long  preparation   time.  The  medium  

preparation  time  in  the  study  was  6  minutes  and  this  resulted  in  a  medium  firm  texture  of  the  

samples.   These   findings   are   in   accordance   with   Szczesniak   et   al.   (1972)   who   found   that  

children  prefer  raw  to  cooked  vegetables.  Another  study  by  Poelman  et  al.  (2011)  also  found  

that  children  liked  the  boiled  vegetables  most  compared  to  baked/stir  fried.  The  study  found  

that  type  of  preparation  method  had  a  greater  influence  on  the  children  who  had  a  reported  

lower   liking   of   vegetables   compared   to   the   children   that   had   a   higher   liking   of   vegetables.  

These  results  were  also  supported  by  a  study  from  2015  by  the  same  author  Poelman  et  al.  

(2015)   who   found   that   boiling   and   steaming   were   preferred   over   roasting   and   frying   in  

carrots.      

 

All   these   above-­‐‑mentioned   results   show   that   the   preparation  method   is   important   for   the  

acceptance  of  vegetables  among  children  and  therefore  it  is  one  of  the  factors  that  can  be  used  

to  increase  the  intake  of  vegetables  in  children.  However,  it  is  important  to  remember  that  the  

most  liked  preparation  method  is  specific  to  the  vegetable  as  raw  for  instance  is  preferred  in  

some   vegetables,  when   boiling   is   preferred   in   others   (Poelman,   Delahunty,   &   Graaf,   2015).  

However,   more   research   on   the   preparation   methods   is   needed   as   some   of   the   existing  

research  are  based  on  results  from  questionnaires  instead  of  actual  tasting.    

 

Page 18: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  18  

2.5  Food  Neophobia    

The  acceptance  of   food  products   is  highly   influenced  by   the  child’s   level  of   food  neophobia,  

which   is   defined   as   “the  rejection  of   foods   that  are  novel  or  unknown  to   the  child”.   (Doveya,  

Staples,  Gibson,  &  Halfor,  2007)  The  term  has  been  described  by  Rozin  (1979)  as  a  survival  

mechanism  that  was  evolutionary  beneficial  to  help  children  avoid  consumption  of  potentially  

poisonous  plants,  toxic  chemicals  and  others  the  environment  without  parents  or  other  older.  

Food   neophobia   is   therefore   a   natural   survival   mechanism  whereby   the   child   rejects   food  

products   that   it   has   no   experiences   with.   However,   this   survival-­‐‑way   of   looking   at   food  

neophobia  is  not  as  relevant  today  as  it  used  to  be,  as  todays  food  is  generally  safe  to  eat  and  

therefore   there   is   no   reason   for   this   protective   mechanism.   Today   this   mechanism   only  

function   as   a   reduction   in   the   child’s   dietary   variety   (Nicklaus   S.   ,   2009).   Therefore,   the  

challenge  of  food  neophobia  today  is  more  related  to  the  reduced  intake  of  important  macro-­‐‑  

or  micronutrients.  For   instance,  a  study  by  Falciglia  et  al.   (2000)  showed  that  children  with  

high  level  of  food  neophobia  had  a  lower  intake  of  vitamin  E  compared  to  the  less  neophobic  

children.   Another   study   showed   that   neophobic   individuals   had   deficiencies   in  magnesium,  

monounsaturated  fats  and  protein  (Capiola  &  Raudenbush,  2012).  As  relevant  for  this  study,  a  

study  from  2013  showed  that  people  with  higher  levels  of  food  neophobia  had  a  lower  intake  

of  vegetables  compared  to  persons  with  lower  levels  of  food  neophobia  (Siegrist,  Hartmann,  &  

Keller,  2013).  

 

Food  neophobia  usually   starts  developing  as   the  child   is  around   two  years  old  and   it  peaks  

somewhere   between  2   and  6   years   and   then   it   gradually   decreases  with   age   and   obtains   a  

stable   level   in   adulthood.   Even   though   food   neophobia   tend   to   be   strongly   related   to   age,  

other   factors   also   influence   the   child’s   level   of   food   neophobia   for   instance   the   individual  

child’s   personality.   The  personality   plays   a   role   in   the   acceptance   of   new   foods   as   children  

with   a   more   sensation   seeking   personality   tend   to   have   a   lower   level   of   food   neophobia  

because  of  a  lower  general  neophobia.  And  opposite,  children  with  different  levels  of  anxiety  

tend  to  be  positively  related  to  food  neophobia  (Doveya,  Staples,  Gibson,  &  Halfor,  2007).    

 

As   mentioned   before,   the   children’s   food   preferences   are   established   early   in   life   and  

therefore  it  is  relevant  if  breastfeeding  or  formula  feeding  influences  food  neophobia.  Studies  

show  that  infants  that  have  been  breast-­‐‑fed  increased  their  intake  of  novel  vegetables  in  their  

Page 19: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  19  

weaning   period   in   a   greater   amount   compared   to   formula   fed   infants.   It   functions   as   a  

domino-­‐‑effect  as   the   infants  who  were   introduced   to  a  higher  variety  of  vegetables  early   in  

their   weaning   period,   also   tended   to   accept   new   food   in   general   more   rapidly   (Maier,  

Chabanet,  Schaal,  Issanchou,  &  Leathwood,  2007).  Beyond  that,  children  are  also  believed  to  

have  an  innate  tendency  to  prefer  sweet  and  fatty  energy-­‐‑dense  food  compared  to  the  bitter  

taste,  which  often  characterizes   the   taste  of  vegetables.  Therefore,  children  probably  have  a  

genetic  resistance  against  vegetables  (Lehto,  et  al.,  2015).  

 

Through   the   years,   there   have   been   developed   many   different   tools   for   measuring   food  

neophobia  in  children.  According  to  a  recent  review  by  Damsbo-­‐‑Svendsen  et  al.  (2017),  seven  

instruments  are  available  for  measuring  food  neophobia  in  children  depending  on  the  child’s  

age.  The  present  study  uses  the  Food  Neophobia  Scale  for  Children  (FNSC)  from  1994,  which  

is  an  adjusted  version  of  the  Food  Neophobia  Scale  (FNS).  It  was  developed  for  children  in  the  

age  group  between  5  and  11  years  and  is  a  ten-­‐‑item  measure  and  can  be  seen  in  appendix  A  

(Pliner,  1994).  

 

2.6  Measuring  tools  for  children’s  liking  

In   the  previous  sections,   the  strategy  mere  exposure  and  the   factor  of  preparation  methods  

used  in  this  study  for  increasing  the  liking  of  a  familiar  and  an  unfamiliar  vegetable  have  been  

described  and  in  this  section  the  measuring  tools  for  the  children’s  liking  will  be  described.    

 

Today,  there  is  a  variety  of  measuring  tools  for  food  acceptance  testing  as  it  is  both  possible  to  

measure  a  direct  intake  of  the  vegetable  in  the  test  and  there  are  also  several  different  scales,  

of  which  some  of  the  scales  e.g.  pictorial  scales  are  invented  specially  for  children.  However,  

by  using   these   scales   for   children,   it   is   important   to  use   age-­‐‑appropriate   scales,   so   that   the  

children   do   not   get   distracted   by   the   pictures   and   it   is   essential   that   the   children   have   the  

cognitive  skills  and  thereby  understand  that  the  faces  on  the  pictures  are  supposed  to  show  

their   response   to   the   food   (Lawless  &  Heymann).  According   to  Chen  et  al.   (1996)  a  7-­‐‑point  

facial  scale  can  be  used  with  children  from  5  years  and  older,  which  is  why  it  was  used  in  the  

present  study,  4-­‐‑years-­‐‑old  can  use  a  5-­‐‑point  scale  and  3-­‐‑years  old  children  have  shown  to  able  

to   use   a   3-­‐‑point   facial   scale.   By   using   these   scales,   the   children   express   their   liking   for   the  

tasted  vegetable  or  other  food  and  this  is  a  measurement  for  the  children’s  acceptance  of  the  

Page 20: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  20  

vegetable,  which  was  also  described  in  the  explanation  of  terms  in  the  introduction.  Previous  

studies  have  also  used  these  scales  as  Maier  et  al.  used  the  9-­‐‑point  scale  together  with  intake  

(2007),  Hausner  et  al.  used  a  7-­‐‑point  smiley  scale  (2012)  and  Lakkakula  et  al.  used  a  3-­‐‑point  

Likert  scale  (2010).  However,  in  a  greater  perspective,  the  objective  is  to  increase  the  intake  

of   vegetables   in   children   and   therefore   the  most   accurate  measurement   is   to  measure   the  

intake  directly.  This  was  done  by  Maier  et  al.   in   infants  (2007)  and  by  Hausner  et  al.   in  2-­‐‑3  

years  old  children  (2012).  

 

2.7  Literature  study  

As  already  mentioned  in  the  introduction,  children  do  not  consume  the  recommended  amount  

of  vegetables  and  children’s  liking  of  vegetables  is  one  of  the  key  predictors  for  the  children’s  

consumption  of  vegetables  (Gibson,  Wardle,  &  Watts,  1998).  Johnson  et  al.  (2016)  found  that  

children’s  food  preferences  are  formed  in  the  childhood  and  that  children  tend  to  take  their  

eating  habits  with  them  into  adulthood.  This  makes  it  important  to  study,  which  factors  that  

influence   the   children’s   acceptance   of   vegetables   and   which   strategies   that   have   been  

successfully  used  to  increase  the  children’s  acceptance  of  vegetables.  Therefore,  the  following  

section  will  describe  some  of  the  already  existing  literature  related  to  the  topic  of  this  study.  

2.7.1  Mere  exposure    

In  the  following  section,  the  studies  that  have  already  investigated  the  relationship  between  

mere   exposure   of   an   initially   disliked   vegetable   (or   another   food)   and   the   acceptance   of   it  

compared  to  an  initially  liked  food  will  be  described.    

A  study  by  Maier  et  al.  (2007)  investigated  the  effect  of  repeated  exposures  of  vegetable  purée  

on  acceptance  in  7-­‐‑month  old  infants.  They  compared  an  initially  liked  and  an  initially  disliked  

vegetable  purée  and  the  results  showed  that  after  eight  exposures,  the  intake  of  the  initially  

disliked   vegetable   purée   was   similar   to   the   initially   liked   purée.   The   acceptance   was  

maintained  even  after  nine  months,  which   shows   that   the   effect  was  persistent.  A   study  by  

Hausner   et   al.   (2012)   investigated   the   effect   of   mere   exposure   of   an   initially   disliked   and  

initially   liked  snack  bar   in  9-­‐‑11-­‐‑year-­‐‑old  children.  The  results  showed  that  the  children  that  

had  been  exposed  to  the  initially  disliked  snack  bar  increased  their  acceptance  for  the  disliked  

snack  bar  after  nine  exposures  to  the  same  level  as  the  acceptance  of  the  initially  liked  snack  

Page 21: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  21  

bar.   Lakkakula   et   al.   (2010)   tested   repeated   exposure   in   vegetables   by   low-­‐‑income   school  

children   and   the   results   showed   that   the   number   of   children   who   liked/or   liked   a   lot   for  

previously  disliked  vegetables  were  at  the  most  after  eight  or  nine  tasting  of  the  vegetables.  

These   studies   indicate   that   the   number   of  mere   exposures   that   are   needed   to   increase   the  

acceptance  of  an  initially  disliked  food  are  eight  or  nine  exposures.  Then  Hausner  et  al.  (2012)  

compared   the   strategies   mere   exposure,   flavour-­‐‑flavour   learning   and   flavour-­‐‑nutrient  

learning.  The  results  showed  that  the  intake  of  a  novel  vegetable  purée  increased  most  due  to  

the  mere  exposure  and  this  was  seen  already  after  5  exposures.  The  flavour-­‐‑flavour  learning  

strategy   needed   10   exposures   to   change   the   intake   and   flavour-­‐‑nutrient   strategy   did   not  

succeed   in   changing   the   intake.   Other   studies   have   also   tried   to   study   the   effect   of   several  

strategies  such  as  a  study  by  Fildes  et  al.   (2014),  who  tested   if  a  daily   intake  at  home  of  an  

initially  disliked  vegetable   for  14  days   together  with  a   reward  could   increase   the  children’s  

acceptance.   The   results   showed   that   the   14   exposures   and   a   reward   were   effective   in  

increasing  the  children’s  acceptance  of  an  initially  disliked  vegetable.  However,  the  study  also  

mention   that  maybe  14  exposures  were  not  necessarily   for  all   children,  maybe   fewer   could  

have  been  used.    

These   findings   show   that  mere  exposure   can  be  used  as  a  powerful  mechanism   to   improve  

and   increase   the   acceptance   of   initially   disliked   foods   in   children   -­‐‑   also   in   a   long-­‐‑term  

perspective.  However,   the  number  of  exposures   that  are  needed  are  still  not  consistent  and  

therefore  more  research  is  needed.    

2.7.2  Food  neophobia  

One  of  the  factors  that  highly  influence  the  children’s  food  preferences  and  the  acceptance  of  

foods  is  the  child’s  level  of  neophobia.  Several  studies  have  studied  the  effect  of  neophobia  in  

relation  to  food  acceptance  and  some  of  them  will  be  described  here.  Food  neophobia  is  often  

measured  by  the  Food  Neophobia  Scale  (FNS)  developed  by  Pliner  and  Hobden  in  1992  and  

according  to  a  study  by  Damsbo-­‐‑Svendsen  et  al.  (2017),  the  FNS  continue  to  give  reliable  and  

valid  results,  even  though  some  of  the  questions  are  not  relevant  today  due  to  a  changed  food  

selection.  Most  studies  today  rely  on  parents  reports  about  the  children’s  level  of  neophobia,  

but   a   study   by   Laureati   et   al.   (2015)   tried   to   develop   their   own   neophobia   scale   for   the  

children  based  on  the  Food  Neophobia  Scale  (FNS).  The  results  from  this  study  showed  that  

Page 22: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  22  

the  new  questionnaire  could  be  used  by  children  at  the  age  of  eight  years  and  older,  but  not  by  

children  at  6-­‐‑years  as  they  did  not  respond  repeatable  at  the  questionnaires.    

 

Falciglia   et   al.   (2000)   divided   the   children   of   their   study   into   three   groups   based   on   their  

scores  from  the  FNS  and  results  from  the  study  showed  that  the  neophobic  group  had  a  higher  

intake   of   saturated   fat,   a   lower   overall   “Health   Eating   Index”   score   and   less   food   variety  

compared   to   children   without   food   neophobia.   This   is   consistent   with   the   findings   from   a  

study  by  Helland  et  al.  (2017),  who  found  that  children  with  high  levels  of  neophobia  had  less  

frequent   intake  of  vegetables.   In  relation  to  mere  exposure,   this  could  indicate  that  children  

with  a  high  level  of  neophobia  need  more  exposures  in  order  to  improve  their  acceptance  for  

a  novel  food  compared  too  children  with  lower  levels  of  neophobia.    

2.7.3  Preparation  methods    

One  of  the  factors  that  can  influence  the  children’s  acceptance  of  vegetables  is  the  preparation  

method  of  the  vegetable.  Currently,  there  is  not  much  scientific  research  available  on  the  topic  

and   therefore   it  has  only  been  possible   to   find  a   few  articles.  However,   the  existing   studies  

have   in   general   found   that   the   preparation   method   influences   the   acceptance   of   the  

vegetables,  which  will   be   described   here.   In   2011  Poelman   et   al.   (2011)   investigated   if   the  

children’s  acceptance   for  vegetables  was   influenced  by   the  preparation  method.  They   found  

that  the  preparation  method  did  affect  the  acceptance  for  vegetables  (cauliflower  and  beans)  

as  boiled  vegetables  were  accepted  more  than  baked/stir  fried.  The  low  acceptance  for  baked  

samples  was  explained  by  a  high  odour  intensity  and  the  presence  of  browned  flavour,  which  

was  not  present  in  the  boiled  samples.  Again  in  2013,  Poelman  et  al.  (2013)  showed  that  the  

children’s   acceptance   for   broccoli   and   cauliflower  was   affected   by   the   preparation  method,  

but  that  the  preparation  time  had  a  greater  effect  on  acceptance  as  the  medium  preparation  

time  of  6-­‐‑8.5  minutes  depending  om  vegetable  and  preparation  method  was  most  wanted  for  

both  vegetables  compared  to  the  short  and  the  long  preparation  times.  Later,  Poelman  et  al.  

(2015)  also  studied  children’s  experiences,   liking  and  intake  of  vegetables  with  reference  to  

preparation  practices  at  home  by  using  a  questionnaire,  which  was  answered  by  the  parents.  

The   results   showed   that  preparation  method   influenced  vegetable  acceptance  and   intake   in  

children  and  also  that  the  effect  of  preparation  methods  was  vegetable  specific.  Zeinstra  et  al.  

(2010)   investigated   the  effect  of  preparation  method  on  sensory  preferences   for  vegetables  

Page 23: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  23  

among  4-­‐‑12  years  old  children  and  young  adults  up  to  the  age  of  25.  They  found  that  all  age  

groups   preferred   boiled   and   steamed   vegetables   over   stir-­‐‑fried,   grilled,   mashed   and   fried  

vegetables.   Beyond   that,   they   found   that   liking   of   the   vegetables   was   positively   associated  

with   a  uniform  surface   and  a  well-­‐‑known   taste  of   vegetables,  moderately   associated  with   a  

granular  texture  and  negatively  associated  with  brown  colouring.    

2.8  Sub-­‐‑conclusion  

In  the  above-­‐‑mentioned  sections,  it  is  described  that  the  intake  of  fruits  and  vegetables  among  

children  today  do  not  meet  the  recommendations,  even  though  they  are  important  for  human  

health.  The  children’s   food  preferences  are   formed  based  on  both   internal   factors   including  

neophobia  and  external  factors  including  the  preparation  method  of  the  vegetable.    The  food  

preferences   can   be   affected   by   different   learnings   strategies   for   instance   mere   exposure,  

flavor-­‐‑flavor  learning  and  flavor-­‐‑nutrient  learning.  Studies  have  shown  that  the  strategy  mere  

exposure  has   succeeded   in   increasing   the   acceptance  of   initially   disliked   foods   to   the   same  

level  as  an   initially   liked   foods  and  other  studies  shown  that   the  preparation  method  of   the  

vegetable   influences   the   acceptance   for   the   vegetables   among   children   due   to   sensory  

attributes.    

Page 24: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  24  

3.  Materials  and  methods  In  the  following  section,  the  method  of  the  study  will  be  described  including  a  description  of  

the  study  design,   the  conduction  of   the  study,   the  participants  and  the  statistics  used   in   the  

handling  of  the  data.    

 

3.1  Overall  study  design  

The  study  is  a  Mere  Exposure  study  and  the  participants  were  randomized  into  one  of  the  six  

exposure   groups,  which   include   two  vegetables   (carrot   and  parsnip)   and   three  preparation  

methods  (baked,  boiled  and  raw).  The  samples  were  called  carrot  baked  (CBA),  carrot  boiled  

(CBO),  carrot  raw  (CR),  parsnip  baked  (PBA),  parsnip  boiled  (PBO)  and  parsnip  raw  (PR).  The  

study  design  was  classified  into  three  phases:    

1.   Pre-­‐‑test:  all  children  tasted  samples  of  both  CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO  and  PR  and  noted  

their  liking  for  each  sample.  

2.   Mere  exposure:  each  of  the  six  groups  of  children  was  exposed  to  a  sample  of  either  

CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO  or  PR  depending  on  which  group  they  were  in.  This  was  

repeated  eight  times  during  four  weeks  and  the  children  noted  their  liking  each  time.  

3.   Post-­‐‑test:  all  children  tasted  samples  of  both  CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO  and  PR  again  and  

noted  their  liking  for  each  sample.  

 

3.1.1  Structure  of  the  study  design  

In  the  table  below,  the  overall  structure  of  the  study  design  is  outlined.  The  table  shows  a  list  

of  all  the  six  exposure  groups  and  shows  what  the  participants  in  the  different  groups  tasted  

at   the   pre-­‐‑test,   the   mere   exposures   and   the   post-­‐‑test.   This   design,   where   all   participants  

tasted  the  same  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  and  at  the  post-­‐‑test,  was  chosen  so  that  the  children  all  could  

function  as  each  other’s  control  group.  This  design  enabled  that  the  development  in  liking  for  

the  exposed  children  in  the  six  different  exposure  groups  could  be  compared  to  the  pre-­‐‑  and  

post-­‐‑test  liking  of  the  non-­‐‑exposed  children/the  control  group.  For  instance,  the  liking  of  CR  

at  pre-­‐‑  and  post-­‐‑test  of  the  children  who  were  exposed  to  CR  was  compared  to  the  liking  at  

pre-­‐‑  and  post-­‐‑test  of  the  children,  who  were  not  exposed  to  CR  in  the  mere  exposure  period.    

 

 

Page 25: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  25  

Table  1:  Overview  of  the  study  design.  The  groups  carrot  baked  (CBA),  carrot  boiled  (CBO),  carrot  raw  (CR),  parsnip  baked  (PBA),  parsnip  boiled  (PBO)  and  parsnip  raw  (PR)  are  listed  in  the  first  column.  In  the  columns  “Pre-­‐‑test”,  “Mere  exposure”  and  “Post-­‐‑test”,  it  is  noted  what  the  participants  in  the  different  groups  tasted  at  the  pre-­‐‑test,  mere  exposures  and  post-­‐‑test.    

Group   Pre-­‐‑test   Mere  exposure   Post-­‐‑test  

CBA   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR   8  ´  CBA   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR  

CBO   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR   8  ´  CBO   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR  

CR   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR   8  ´  CR   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR  

PBA   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR   8  ´  PBA   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR  

PBO   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR   8  ´  PBO   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR  

PR   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR   8  ´  PR   CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO,  PR  

 

3.1.2  The  phases  of  the  study  

In  the  following  section,  the  three  phases  of  the  study  will  be  described  in  detail.    

 

3.1.2.1  Pre-­‐‑test  

The   first   phase   of   the   study   was   the   pre-­‐‑test.   All   the   children   were   exposed   to   an  

approximately  7  g  sample  weigh  as  seen  in  figure  4  picture  B  of  both  CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO  

and   PR   served   in   transparent   plastic   jars   with   lids   (Abena   A/S,   Aabenraa,   Denmark),   and  

noted  their  liking  on  a  7-­‐‑point  facial  scale,  which  will  be  described  later  in  this  section,  after  

each  sample.  The  samples   can  be  seen   in   figure  4  picture,  C,  D,  E  and  F.  The  six   samples  of  

vegetables  were  served  in  a  randomised  order  between  classes  to  eliminate  a  serving  order  

effect.    

 

3.1.2.2  Mere  exposure  

The  second  phase  of  the  study  was  the  mere  exposure  phase,  which  took  place  two  times  per  

week   for   four  weeks.   The   children  were   exposed   to   an   approximately   7   g   sample   of   either  

CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO  or  PR  depending  on  which  group  they  were  in.  After  the  tasting,  they  

noted   their   liking   on   the   7-­‐‑point   facial   scale.   To   simplicity,   the   randomization   of   the   six  

exposure  groups  was  done  in  between  the  classes,  so  all  the  children  in  the  same  class  were  in  

the  same  exposure  group  –  except  from  one  class  in  which  the  children  were  mixed  between  

the  exposure  groups.    

 

Page 26: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  26  

3.1.2.3  Post-­‐‑test  

The  third  phase  of  the  study  was  the  post-­‐‑test,  which  was  an  exact  repetition  of  the  pre-­‐‑test.  It  

took  place  at  the  four  schools  on  their  last  day  of  the  study.  Again,  the  children  were  exposed  

to  an  approximately  7  g  sample  of  both  CBA,  CBO,  CR,  PBA,  PBO  and  PR  served  in  transparent  

plastic   jars  with  lids  (Abena  A/S,  Aabenraa,  Denmark),  and  noted  their   liking  on  the  7-­‐‑point  

facial  scale.  The  six  samples  were  served  in  the  same  randomised  order  as  at  the  pre-­‐‑test.  

 

3.1.3  Scale  for  measurement  of  liking  

As  a  measuring  tool  for  liking,  the  7-­‐‑point  facial  scale  was  used  based  on  a  study  by  Chen  et  al.  

(Chen,  1996),  in  which  it  is  concluded  that  the  7-­‐‑point  facial  hedonic  scale  is  a  reliably  scale  

for  5-­‐‑year  old  children.  The  scale  can  be  seen  in  the  figure  2  below.  

 

 

Figure  2:  7-­‐‑point  facial  scale.  

 

3.2  Participants  

3.2.1  Recruitment  of  the  test  participants  

The   children  were   recruited   from   four   public   schools   in   Odense.   They  were   all   children   in  

preschool  classes  in  the  age  group  between  five  and  seven  years  old.  The  schools  that  wanted  

to   participate   were   informed   about   the   study   at   a   personal   meeting.   The   parents   of   the  

participating  children  were  asked  for  acceptance  through  a  questionnaire  (appendix  A)  about  

Page 27: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  27  

the  children’s  liking  and  intake  of  carrots  and  parsnips  during  a  week.  Data  from  43  children  

were   excluded   from   the   analysis   due   to  missing   acceptance   from   the   parents   and   the   final  

number   of   participating   children  was   263.   However,   the   teachers   on   all   four   schools  were  

contacted  several  times  in  an  attempt  to  get  more  accepts  from  parents.    

 

3.2.2  Test  participants  

The  participants  in  the  study  were  children,  who  were  attending  preschool  classes  in  public  

schools  in  Odense.  The  inclusion  criteria  were  therefore  as  following:  

1.   Attending  preschool  class  in  public  schools  in  Odense  

2.   Healthy  and  no  food  allergy  for  relevant  vegetables  

3.   Acceptance  from  parents  

 

3.2.3  Randomization  of  participants  for  mere  exposure  phase  

The   recruited   children   were   randomized   into   six   exposure   groups   for   the   mere   exposure  

phase.  The  randomization  was  based  on  the  fact  that  all  children  in  the  same  class  should  be  

in   the  same  exposure  group  for  practical  reasons  –  and  only   in  one  class,   the  children  were  

mixed.  The  randomization  subdivision  is  seen  in  the  table  below  and  the  randomization  of  the  

mixed  class  can  be  seen  in  appendix  C.  

 Table  2:  Randomization  of  the  classes  for  the  mere  exposure  phase.  CBA=carrot  baked,  CBO=carrot  boiled,  CR=carrot  raw,  PBA=parsnip  baked,  PBO=parsnip  boiled  and  PR=parsnip  raw.  

School/class   Mere  exposure-­‐‑group  

Hunderupskolen   0.A   CR  

0.B   CBO  

0.C   CBA  

Rosengårdskolen   0.A   CBO  

0.B   CR  

0.C   Mixed  (appendix  C)  

Sct.  Hans  skolen   0.X   PR  

0.Y   PBO  

0.Z   PBA  

Page 28: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  28  

Tingløkkeskolen   0.A   PBA  

0.B   PBO  

0.C   PR  

0.D   CBA    

3.2.4  Randomization  of  participants  for  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  

For  the  pre-­‐‑test  and  the  post-­‐‑test,  the  samples  from  the  six  exposure  groups  were  served  in  

the   following   randomized  order  as   seen   in   the   table  3  below.  The  same  structure  was  used  

both  for  the  pre-­‐‑test  and  the  post-­‐‑test.    

Table  3:  Randomization  of  serving  order  at  the  pre-­‐‑  and  the  post-­‐‑test.  CBA=carrot  baked,  CBO=carrot  boiled,  CR=carrot  raw,  PBA=parsnip  baked,  PBO=parsnip  boiled  and  PR=parsnip  raw.  

School/class   Serving  order  at  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  

Hunderupskolen   0.A   PR,  CBO,  PBA,  PBO,  CR,  CBA  

0.B   PBO,  CR,  CBO,  PBA,  CBA,  PR  

0.C   CR,  CBA,  PBO,  PR,  PBA,  CBO  

Rosengårdskolen   0.A   PBA,  CR,  CBA,  CBO,  PR,  PBO  

0.B   CBO,  CBA,  CR,  PBO,  PR,  PBA  

0.C   CBA,  PBA,  PR,  CR,  CBO,  PBO  

Sct.  Hans  skolen   0.X   PR,  CBA,  CBO,  PBA,  CR,  PBO  

0.Y   PBO,  PBA,  CR,  CBA,  PR,  CBO  

0.Z   CR,  PR,  PBA,  PBO,  CBO,  CBA  

Tingløkkeskolen   0.A   CR,  CBO,  CBA,  PR,  PBA,  PBO  

0.B   PBA,  PBO,  PR,  CBA,  CBO,  CR  

0.C   CBO,  PR,  PBA,  CR,  CBA,  PBO  

0.D   CBA,  PBO,  CR,  CBO,  PBA,  PR  

 

 

 

Page 29: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  29  

3.3  Experimental  procedure  

The  general  test  procedure  is  described  in  the  following  section.  Furthermore,  the  deviations  

from  the  normal  procedure  are  also  described.      

3.3.1  Pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  procedure    

All   tests   took  place   in   the   preschool   classrooms   at   the   four   participating   schools.   The   tests  

were  carried  out   in  the   late  mornings  between  9  am  and  12  am  depending  on  the  class  and  

school.  All  tests  were  carried  out  before  the  children  had  lunch.  The  children  were  placed  in  

their   normal   seats   to   make   the   situation   most   natural   for   them.   First,   the   children   were  

introduced  to  the  7-­‐‑point  facial  scale  where  the  head  of  the  study  went  through  all  the  seven  

faces   on   the   scale   and   the   children  were   giving   examples   of   food   they   associated  with   the  

specific  face.  Then  the  teacher  handed  out  the  facial  scales,  which  had  the  specific  child’s  name  

and   the  exposure  group  e.g.  CR  on   it,  while   two  assistants  handed  out   the   samples   and   the  

dividers,  which  can  be  seen  in  figure  3.  The  dividers  were  placed  between  the  children,  so  that  

they  could  not  see  each  other’s  facial  features  and  evaluation,  when  they  tasted  the  samples  to  

avoid  interaction.  The  children  were  told  to  sit  quiet,  taste  their  sample  and  note  their  liking.  

Afterwards,   when   they   were   finished,   they   put   up   their   hand   and   the   facial   scales   were  

collected.  This  procedure  was  repeated  six  times  at  both  the  pre-­‐‑test  and  the  post-­‐‑test.  After  

the  post-­‐‑test,  each  class  were  giving  a  box  of  local  apple  juice  as  thanks.        

 

3.3.2  Mere  exposure-­‐‑test  

The  mere  exposure  tests  also  took  place  in  the  preschool  classrooms  between  9  am  and  12  am  

depending  on  the  class  and  school.  The  children  were  placed  in  their  normal  seats,  and  they  

had  a  short  introduction  each  time  to  the  facial  scale  and  to  what  was  going  to  happen.  Then  

the  dividers  were  placed  between  the  children  and  the  teacher  handed  out  the  facial  scales  to  

each  child.  The  samples  of  vegetables  were  handed  out  and  the  children  were  told  to  taste  the  

sample  quiet  and  note  with   ticking  which  smiley   they  associated  with   the  sample.  Then   the  

scales  were  collected  and  all  the  plastic  jars  were  thrown  out  or  handed  to  the  teacher.    

 

Page 30: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  30  

   Figure  3:  The  pictures  show  the  test  setup  in  one  class  with  the  dividers  between  the  children.    

3.3.3  Deviations  from  test  procedure  

The  following  observations  have  been  done  during  the  test  carrying-­‐‑out  in  general  for  all  the  

participating  classes.  First,  many  of  the  children  were  not  present  at  all  the  mere  exposures  or  

at  the  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  and  therefore  they  did  not  taste  the  samples  the  amount  of  times  

that  was  planned  for  the  study.  Next,  some  of  the  classes  often  had  young  substitute  teachers  

who  did  not  know  the  children  and  had  problems  to  calm  the  children  down.  This  resulted  in  

noise  and  classroom  disruptions  during  the  tasting.  Furthermore,  in  some  few  instances,  the  

children   just   had  had   fruit   before   the   tasting,  which   could   affect   the   liking  of   the   vegetable  

sample  tasting  compared  to  when  they  did  not  just  have  had  fruit  before  the  tasting.  

 

3.4  Experimental  stimuli  

3.4.1  Choice  of  vegetables  and  preparation  methods  

The   present   study   aims   to   compare   the   development   in   a   familiar   and   in   an   unfamiliar  

vegetable.  The  vegetables  were  chosen  based  on  a  study  by  Beck    (2014),  which  showed  that  

90  %  of  adult  Danish  consumers  had  a  high  intake  of  carrots  and  50-­‐‑60  %  of  the  consumers  

consumed   parsnips   minimum   once   a   month   –   therefore   the   carrot   was   categorized   as   a  

familiar   vegetable   and   the   parsnip   was   categorized   as   an   unfamiliar   vegetable.   This   was  

supported  by   the  questionnaires   in   the  present   study.  Three  different  preparation  methods  

were   chosen:  baked,  boiled  and   raw.  They  were   chosen  based  on   the  assumption   that  both  

Page 31: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  31  

vegetables  are  normally  prepared  in  these  ways.  A  group  of  four  adults  staff  members  related  

to  the  study  tasted  and  tested  the  baked,  boiled  and  raw  carrots  and  parsnips  before  the  study  

start   to   find   the   optimal   size,   weight   and   preparation   time   of   the   baked,   boiled   and   raw  

vegetables.  The  protocol  for  the  preparation  of  the  vegetables  can  be  seen  in  appendix  B.  The  

vegetables   were   delivered   by   AU   Årslev.   The   used   carrot   cultivar   was   New   Hall   and   the  

parsnips  were  a  mixture  of  the  cultivars  Picador  and  Aromata.  All  the  vegetables  were  stored  

in  a  cold  room  at  1°C  in  AU  Årslev  during  the  study.    

 

 

Page 32: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  32  

 Figure  4:    A:  The  cutting  of  the  vegetables.  B:  weighing  7  g  of  a  vegetable.  C:  The  carrots  in  transparent  plastic  jars,  D:  The  parsnips  in  transparent  plastic  jars,  E:  carrots  ready  for  serving  and  F:  parsnips  ready  for  serving.  

3.5  Statistical  data  processing  

All  the  data  from  the  questionnaires  and  the  children’s   liking  replies  from  both  pre-­‐‑test,   the  

mere  exposure  phase  and  post-­‐‑test  were  entered  in  Excel  and  descriptive  statistics  including  

mean,  standard  deviation  and  standard  error  of  mean  (SEM)  were  made.  The  statistical  data  

processing  was  done  in  XLSTAT  19.01  (Addinsoft,  New  York,  United  States),  which  bases  its  

calculations  on  least  squares  mean.      

 

3.5.1  Pre-­‐‑test  liking  

The  effect  of  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  unfamiliar)  was  tested  by  a  one-­‐‑way  ANOVA,  

as  there  is  only  one  factor  –  the  type  of  vegetable.  The  null  hypothesis  (H0)  was  that  the  type  

of  vegetable  did  not  have  an  effect  on  the  liking.  Afterwards,  a  one-­‐‑way  ANOVA  was  done  to  

see   if   the   preparation   method   had   an   effect   on   the   liking   with   the   H0   that   the   type   of  

preparation  method  did  not  have  an  effect  on  the  liking.  Both  tests  were  based  on  the  liking  

scores  from  the  pre-­‐‑test.  In  case  of  a  p-­‐‑value  <0.05,  the  supplemental  post  hoc  test  Tukey  HSD  

was  run  to  see  between  which  groups  the  differences  occurred.    

 

A   two-­‐‑way   ANOVA   was   done   to   test   if   the   effect   of   the   type   of   vegetable   (familiar   versus  

unfamiliar)  and  type  of  preparation  method  had  an  effect  on  the  liking  with  the  H0  that  there  

was  no  effect  of  the  type  of  vegetable  and  type  of  preparation  method  on  liking.  The  test  was  

also   based   on   the   liking   scores   from   the   pre-­‐‑test.   Also   here,   a   supplemental   post   hoc   test  

Page 33: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  33  

Tukey  HSD  was  run.    

 

3.5.2  Test  for  the  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  within  the  groups  

A  two-­‐‑way  ANOVA  test  was  done  to  test  if  the  mere  exposure  within  the  six  different  exposure  

groups  had  an  effect  on  the  liking  scores  from  pre-­‐‑test  through  all  exposure  tests  to  post-­‐‑test  

with  the  H0  that  mere  exposure  did  not  have  an  effect  on  liking.    

 

3.5.3  Test  for  the  effect  of  gender  on  liking  

The  effect  of  gender  on  liking  was  tested  by  a  one-­‐‑way  ANOVA  based  on  the  liking  scores  from  

the  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  with  the  H0  that  gender  did  not  have  an  effect  on  the  liking.  

 

3.5.4  Test  for  the  effect  of  level  of  neophobia  on  liking  

The  children  were  divided   into   three  groups  based  on   their  scores   from  the  FNSC  based  on  

literature  (Falciglia,  Couch,  Gribble,  Pabst,  &  Frank,  2000).  The  groups  were  as  followed:  Not  

neophobic  (scores  from  0-­‐‑30),  Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic  (scores  from  31-­‐‑40)  and  

Neophobic  (scores  from  41-­‐‑70).  The  effect  of  the  level  of  neophobia  on  liking  was  tested  by  a  

one-­‐‑way  ANOVA  based  on   the   liking   scores   from   the  pre-­‐‑test   and   the  post-­‐‑test  with   the  H0  

that  the  level  of  neophobia  did  not  have  an  effect  on  the  liking  score.  Beyond  this,  a  two-­‐‑way  

ANOVA  was  done  to  see   if   the   level  of  neophobia  and  the  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  

unfamiliar)  had  an  effect  on  the  liking  score.    

 

3.5.5  Correlation  tests  

Pearson’s   correlation   test  was   used   to   test   the   correlation   between   the   children’s   liking   of  

carrots  and  parsnips   in  relation  to  the  children’s   intake  of  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  

parents.  Finally,  the  correlation  between  the  children’s  liking  of  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  

by  parents  in  relation  to  the  children’s  liking  score  of  the  carrots  and  parsnips  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  

was  also  tested  by  the  Pearson’s  correlation  test.    

Page 34: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  34  

4.  Results  In  the  following  section,  the  key  results  of  the  study  will  be  presented  starting  with  the  basic  

data   on   the   participants,   followed   by   the   results   concerning   the   type   of   vegetable,   type   of  

preparation  method  and  mere  exposure.  And  at  last,  the  results  based  on  the  questionnaires  

completed   by   the   parents   are   presented.   In   case   of   significant   differences   between   groups,  

they  will  be  marked  with  an  A,  B,  C  etc.  depending  on  their  grouping  from  the  Tukey  HSD  test.  

 

4.1  Participants  

The  distribution  of  the  participants  is  shown  in  table  4.  The  table  shows  that  the  total  number  

of  participants  were  263  children,  of  which  148  were  boys  and  115  were  girls.  This  results  in  

a  percentage  of  boys  of  56.3%  and  a  percentage  of  girls  of  43.7%.    

 Table  4:  Distribution  of  participants  in  the  six  exposure  groups:  baked  carrot  (CBA),  boiled  carrot  (CBO),  raw  carrot  (CR),  baked  parsnip  (PBA),  boiled  parsnip  (PBO)  and  raw  parsnip  (PR).  

  CBA   CBO   CR   PBA   PBO   PR   Total   %  

Girls   19   19   20   18   17   22   115   43.7  

Boys   23   26   23   27   26   23   148   56.3  

Total   42   45   43   45   43   45   263   100  

4.2  Pre-­‐‑test  liking  

First,  the  effect  of  the  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  less  unfamiliar)  on  liking  was  tested  

based  on  data  from  the  pre-­‐‑test.  The  results  showed  that  there  was  a  significant  effect  of  the  

type   of   vegetable   on   the   liking   score   with   a   p-­‐‑value=<0.0001   (appendix   D)   as   the   familiar  

carrots   had   a   significantly   higher   liking   score   with   a   LS   mean   of   4.6   compared   to   the  

unfamiliar   parsnips,  which   had   a   LS  mean   liking   score   of   3.6.   This   is   illustrated   in   figure   5  

below.      

 

Page 35: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  35  

 Figure  5:  Pre-­‐‑test  mean  liking  for  carrots  and  parsnips.  X-­‐‑axis  shows  the  type  of  vegetable  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

 

The  effect  of  the  preparation  method  on  liking  at  pre-­‐‑test  was  also  tested.  The  results  showed  

that  the  type  of  preparation  method  had  a  significant  effect  on  the  liking  scores  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  

with   a   p-­‐‑value=<0.0001   (appendix   E).   The   raw   preparation   method   had   the   significantly  

highest  liking,  the  baked  preparation  method  had  the  significantly  second  highest  liking  and  

the  boiled  preparation  method  had  the  significantly  lowest  liking  score  (appendix  E),  which  is  

illustrated  in  figure  6  below.    

 

 Figure  6:  Pre-­‐‑test  liking  for  preparation  methods.  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  type  of  preparation  method  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Carrot Parsnip

Liking  score

Vegetable

Type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  unfamiliar)  and  liking  at  pre-­‐‑test

AB

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Baked Boiled Raw

Liking  score

Preparation  method

Type  of  preparation  method  and  liking  at  pre-­‐‑test

A

BC

Page 36: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  36  

The  results  from  the  test  of  the  effect  of  the  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  unfamiliar)  and  

the  type  of  preparation  method  showed  that  there  was  a  significant   interaction  effect  of   the  

type   of   vegetable   and   the   type   of   preparation   method   on   liking   with   a   p-­‐‑value=0.0001  

(appendix  F).  As  seen  in  figure  7  below,  the  raw  carrot  (CR)  had  a  significantly  higher  liking  

compared  to  the  other  five  groups  and  the  boiled  parsnip  (PBO)  had  the  significantly  lowest  

liking.  The   remaining   four   groups   (CBA,  CBO,  PBA  and  PR)  did  not  differ   significantly   from  

each  other.    

 

 Figure  7:  Interaction  effect  of  the  type  of  vegetable  and  the  type  of  preparation  method  on  pre-­‐‑test  liking  between  all  six  exposure  groups:  baked  carrots  (CBA),  boiled  carrots  (CBO),  raw  carrots  (CR),  baked  parsnips  (PBA),  boiled  parsnips  (PBO)  and  raw  parsnips  (PR).  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  type  of  vegetable  and  preparation  method  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  score  from  1-­‐‑7.    

4.3  Mere  exposure    

The  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  within  the  six  exposure  groups  was  tested  and  the  result  

showed  that  there  were  no  significant  differences  in  liking  within  the  groups  from  pre-­‐‑test  to  

post-­‐‑test.  However,  as  it  can  be  seen  in  figure  8  the  liking  was  in  general  increasing  and  the  

highest  liking  score  was  obtained  at  exposure  number  8  in  the  groups  CBA,  PBA,  PBO  and  PR.  

In  the  CBO  group  the  highest   liking  was  obtained  at   the  post-­‐‑test,  while  the   liking   in  the  CR  

group  was  almost  unchanged  from  pre-­‐‑test  to  post-­‐‑test.  Figure  8  also  shows  that  CR  had  the  

highest   liking   through  pre-­‐‑test   to  post-­‐‑test   and   that  PBO  had   the   lowest   liking  almost   from  

pre-­‐‑test  to  post-­‐‑test.  In  relation  to  the  effect  of  preparation  method  on  liking,  figure  8  shows  

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

CBA CBO CR PBA PBO PR

Liking  score

Preparation  method  and  vegetable

Type  of  vegetable  and  type  of  prepraration  method  -­‐‑ liking  at  pre-­‐‑test

B B

A

B

CB

Page 37: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  37  

that  the  raw  preparation  method  did  not  influence  the  liking  of  the  familiar  carrot  over  mere  

exposures,   as   the   liking   remained   the   same,   however   the   liking   of   the   unfamiliar   parsnip  

increased  slightly.  Also,  both  the  baked  carrot  and  parsnip  increased  slightly  in  liking  as  well  

as   the   boiled   carrot   and   parsnip.   Figure   8   shows   that   the   liking   of   preparation  method   in  

general  was  higher  in  the  familiar  carrot  compared  to  the  unfamiliar  parsnip.  It  was  also  seen  

that  the  liking  of  PR,  CBA,  PBA  and  PBO  decreased  from  exposure  no.  8  to  post-­‐‑test.  

 

 

 Figure  8:  Liking  from  pre-­‐‑test  to  post-­‐‑test  among  the  exposed  children.  X-­‐‑axis  shows  the  exposure  number  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  score  from  1-­‐‑7.    

 

It  was  also  tested,  if  there  were  any  differences  in  liking  between  the  exposure  groups  at  the  

different   exposure   numbers.   The   results   showed   that   there   was   a   significant   difference  

(p=<0.0001)  in  the  liking  between  the  groups  at  the  pre-­‐‑test.  CR  had  significantly  higher  liking  

compared   to   PBO,   PBA,   CBA,   CBO   and   PR.   PR   and   CBO   had   significantly   higher   liking   than  

PBO.  At  the  first  exposure,  there  was  a  significant  difference  in  liking  between  the  groups  as  

CR  had  significantly  higher   liking  compared  to  PBO  and  PBA  (p=0.003).  At   the  second  mere  

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Pretest 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Posttest

Liking  score

Liking  in  the  mere  exposure  period  

CBA CBO CR PBA PBO PR

Page 38: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  38  

exposure,  CR  had  significantly  higher  liking  compared  to  PBO  and  CBA  (p=0.002).  At  the  third  

mere  exposure,  CR,  CBO  and  PR  had  the  significantly  highest  liking  compared  to  PBO.  At  the  

fourth  mere  exposure,  CR  had  a  significantly  higher  liking  compared  to  PBO  and  PBA,  whereas  

PR   and   CBO   had   a   significantly   higher   liking   compared   to   PBO   (p=0.0001).   At   the   fifth  

exposure,  CR  had  a  significantly  higher  liking  compared  to  PBO  and  PBA,  whereas  CBO  had  a  

significantly  higher  liking  compared  to  PBO  (p=0.001).  At  the  sixth  mere  exposure,  there  was  

also  a  significant  difference  (p=0.003)   in   the   liking  between  the  exposure  groups  as  CR  and  

CBO  had  the  significantly  highest  liking  compared  to  PBO.  At  the  second  last  mere  exposure,  

CR   had   a   significantly   higher   liking   than   PBA   (p=0.05).   There  was   no   significant   difference  

between  the  groups  at  mere  exposure  eight,  but   in   the  post-­‐‑test  CR  had  significantly  higher  

liking  compared  to  PBA,  PBO  and  CBA.  CBO  had  significantly  higher  liking  than  PBA  and  PBO  

(p=<0.0001)  (appendix  G).    

 

The  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  was  also  tested  between  the  exposed  children  and  the  

non-­‐‑exposed  children  at  pre-­‐‑test,  the  4.  exposure  (only  exposed  children)  and  post-­‐‑test.  The  

results   (appendix  G)   are   illustrated   in   figure  9  below.  The   results   show   that   there  were  no  

differences   in   liking   between   exposed   and   non-­‐‑exposed   children   at   pre-­‐‑test   or   post-­‐‑test  

within   the  CBA   (figure  9,  A)   and   the  CR   (figure  9,  E)   group.   In   the  CBO   (figure  9,  C)   group,  

results  showed  that  the  non-­‐‑exposed  children  had  a  significantly  lower  liking  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  

and  post-­‐‑test  compared  to  the  exposed  children’s  liking  at  the  4.  exposure  and  post-­‐‑test.  In  the  

PBA-­‐‑group   (figure   9,   B),   the   exposed   children   had   a   significantly   higher   liking   at   the   4.  

exposure   compared   to   the   non-­‐‑exposed   children’s   liking   at   the   post-­‐‑test.   In   the   PBO-­‐‑group  

(figure  9,  D),  the  exposed  children  had  a  significantly  higher  liking  at  the  post-­‐‑test  compared  

to   the  non-­‐‑exposed   children’s   liking   at   post-­‐‑test.   In   the  PR  group   (figure  9,   F),   the   exposed  

children  had  a   significantly  higher   liking  at  pre-­‐‑test,  4.   exposure  and  post-­‐‑test   compared   to  

the  non-­‐‑exposed  children’s  liking  at  post-­‐‑test.  These  results  show  that  the  mere  exposure  has  

had  an  effect  compared  to   the  non-­‐‑exposed  children  within  all  exposure  groups  except  CBA  

and  CR.    

 

 

Page 39: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  39  

 Figure  9:  A:  Mean  liking  for  exposed  and  non-­‐‑exposed  children  at  pre-­‐‑test,  4.  exposure  (only  exposed  children)  and  post-­‐‑test.  A:  carrot  baked  (CBA),  B:  parsnip  baked  (PBA),  C:  carrot  boiled  (CBO),  D:  parsnip  boiled  (PBO),  E:  carrot  raw  (CR)  and  F:  parsnip  raw  (PR).  

 

1234567

Liking  score

CBA  A

1234567

Liking  score

PBAB

A

ABBAAB

1234567

Liking  score

CBOC

ABB

AA

B

1234567

Liking  score

PBOD

AB AB ABA

B

1234567

Liking  score

CRE

1234567

Liking  score

PRF

ABAA A

B

Page 40: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  40  

4.4  Effect  of  gender  on  liking  

The  effect  of  gender  on  liking  was  tested  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  and  the  results  (appendix  H)  showed  

that  there  were  no  significant  differences  in  liking  between  the  gender  in  the  different  groups  

except   in   the   PR   group   (p-­‐‑value=0.026),   where   the   boys   had   a   significantly   higher   liking  

compared  to  the  girls  (appendix  H),  which  is  illustrated  in  figure  10.    

 

 Figure  10:  Pre-­‐‑test:  liking  distributed  in  gender  in  the  six  exposure  groups  baked  carrot  (CBA),  boiled  carrot  (CBO),  raw  carrot  (CR),  baked  parsnip  (PBA),  boiled  parsnip  (PBO)  and  raw  parsnip  (PR).  The  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

There  were  also  no  significant  differences  (appendix  H)   in   the   liking  between  gender  at   the  

post-­‐‑test,  which  is  illustrated  in  figure  11  below.    

 Figure  11:  Post-­‐‑test:  liking  distributed  in  gender  in  the  six  exposure  groups  baked  carrot  (CBA),  boiled  carrot  (CBO),  raw  carrot  (CR),  baked  parsnip  (PBA),  boiled  parsnip  (PBO)  and  raw  parsnip  (PR).  The  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

1234567

CBA CBO CR PBA PBO PR

Mean  liking

Pre-­‐‑test  liking  and  gender

Boy Girl

A B

1234567

CBA CBO CR PBA PBO PR

Mean  liking

Post-­‐‑test  liking  and  gender

Boy Girl

Page 41: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  41  

4.5  Effect  of  level  of  neophobia  on  liking  

The  children  were  divided  into  three  neophobia  groups  based  on  their  score  from  the  FNSC  

and  table  5  shows  the  distribution  in  numbers,  whereas  figure  12  shows  the  distribution  in  %.  

 Table  5:  The  number  of  children  in  the  three  Neophobia-­‐‑groups.  

Groups   Number  of  children  Not  neophobic   84  Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic   73  Neophobic   105  Total   262  

Figure  12:  The  %-­‐‑distribution  of  the  neophobia  groups.  

The  effect  of  the  level  of  neophobia  on  liking  both  at  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  was  tested  and  the  

results  showed  that  there  was  a  significant  effect  of  the   level  of  neophobia  on  liking  at  both  

pre-­‐‑test   (p-­‐‑value=0.000)   and   post-­‐‑test   (p-­‐‑value=<0.0001)   (appendix   I).   At   the   pre-­‐‑test,   the  

group  “Not  neophobic”  had  a  significantly  higher  liking  compared  to  the  “Neophobic”,  which  

can  be  seen  in  figure  13.  

 

Not  neophobic32%

Neither  neophobic  or  not  

neophobic28%

Neophobic40%

%-­‐‑Distribution  of  neophobia

Page 42: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  42  

 Figure  13:  Pre-­‐‑test  liking  and  level  of  neophobia.  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  three  neophobia-­‐‑groups  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.    

 

At   the   post-­‐‑test,   the   group   “Not   neophobic”   had   the   significantly   highest   liking   score,   the  

“Neither  neophobic  or  not”  group  had   the  second  highest   liking  and   the  group   “Neophobic”  

had  the  significantly  lowest  liking  (appendix  I).  This  is  illustrated  in  figure  14  below.    

 

 Figure  14:  Post-­‐‑test  liking  and  level  of  neophobia.  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  three  neophobia-­‐‑groups  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.    

 

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Not  neophobic Neither  neophobic  or  not Neophobic

Mean  liking  

Level  of  neophobia

Pre-­‐‑test  liking  and  level  of  neophobia

A BAB

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Not  neophobic Neither  neophobic  or  not Neophobic

Mean  liking

Level  of  neophobia

Post-­‐‑test  liking  and  level  of  neophobia

A B C

Page 43: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  43  

Furthermore,  it  was  also  tested  at  both  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test  if  there  was  an  interaction  effect  

of   the   type  of   vegetable   (familiar   versus   less  unfamiliar)   and   the   level   of   neophobia   on   the  

liking.   At   the   pre-­‐‑test,   the   results   showed   that   there  was   a   significant   difference   (p-­‐‑value=  

0.0001)   in   liking,   which   is   illustrated   in   figure   15.   The   “carrot   not   neophobic”   group   had  

significantly   higher   liking   than   “parsnip   neophobic”,   “parsnip   neither   neophobic   or   not”,  

“parsnip   not   neophobic”   and   “carrot   neophobic”.   “Carrot   neither   neophobic   or   not”   had  

significantly  higher   liking  than  “parsnip  neophobic”,   “parsnip  neither  neophobic  or  not”  and  

“parsnip   not   neophobic”.   “Carrot   neophobic”   had   significantly   higher   liking   compared   to  

“parsnip   neophobic”   and   “parsnip   neither   neophobic   or   not”.   All   p-­‐‑values   can   be   seen   in  

appendix   I.  As   seen   in   figure  15,   the   familiar   carrot  had   the  highest   liking   regardless  of   the  

level  of  neophobia  and  the  unfamiliar  parsnip  has  the  lowest  liking  regardless  of  the  level  of  

neophobia.      

 

 Figure  15:  Pre-­‐‑test:  liking,  level  of  neophobia  and  type  of  vegetable.  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  interaction  between  type  of  vegetable  and  neophobia-­‐‑group  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

 

At   the  post-­‐‑test,   the   results   also   showed   a   significant   difference   (p-­‐‑value  =   <0.0001)   in   the  

liking,  which  can  be  seen  in  the  figure  16  below.  Again,  it  is  seen  that  the  familiar  carrot  had  

the  highest   liking   compared   to   the  unfamiliar  parsnip   regardless  of   level  of  neophobia.  The  

results  showed  that  the  two  groups  “carrot  not  neophobic”  and  “carrot  neither  neophobic  or  

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Carrot  -­‐‑ Not  neophobic

Carrot  -­‐‑ Neither  neophobic  or  not

Carrot  -­‐‑Neophobic

Parsnip  -­‐‑ Not  neophobic

Parsnip  -­‐‑ Neither  neophobic  or  not

Parsnip  -­‐‑Neophobic

Mean  liking

Pre-­‐‑test  liking,  level  of  neophobia  and  type  of  vegetable

A

DDCDBC

AB

Page 44: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  44  

not”  had  significantly  higher   liking  than  “parsnip  neophobic”,   “parsnip  neither  neophobic  or  

not”,   “parsnip  not   neophobic”   and   “carrot   neophobic”.   Furthermore,   the   “carrot   neophobic”  

group  had  significantly  higher  liking  compared  to  “parsnip  neophobic”  and  “parsnips  neither  

neophobic   or   not”   and   that   “parsnip   not   neophobic”   had   significantly   higher   liking   than  

“parsnip  neophobic”.  All  p-­‐‑values  can  be  seen  in  appendix  I.  

 

 Figure  16:  Post-­‐‑test:  liking,  level  of  neophobia  and  type  of  vegetable.  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  interaction  between  type  of  vegetable  and  neophobia-­‐‑group  and  the  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

 

4.6  The  correlation  tests  

The   correlation   between   the   children’s   intake   and   the   children’s   liking   of   both   carrots   and  

parsnips   reported  by  parents  was   tested.  The   results   showed  a   significant   (p-­‐‑value=  0.000)  

positive,  medium-­‐‑strong  correlation  between  the  children’s  liking  and  the  children’s  intake  of  

carrots  reported  by  parents  with  a  Pearson’s  coefficient  =  0.6  (appendix  J).  The  liking  and  the  

intake  of  carrots  in  percentage  can  be  seen  figure  17  and  18  below.  The  results  also  showed  a  

significant   (p-­‐‑value=0.000)   positive,   medium-­‐‑strong   relation   between   the   children’s   liking  

and  the  children’s  intake  of  parsnips  reported  by  parents  with  a  Pearson’s  coefficient  =  0.520  

(appendix  J).  The  liking  and  the  intake  of  parsnips  in  percentage  can  be  seen  in  figure  19  and  

20  below.    

 

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Carrot  -­‐‑ Not  neophobic

Carrot  -­‐‑ Neither  neophobic  or  not

Carrot  -­‐‑Neophobic

Parsnip  -­‐‑ Not  neophobic

Parsnip  -­‐‑ Neither  neophobic  or  not

Parsnip  -­‐‑Neophobic

Mean  liking

Post-­‐‑test  liking,  level  of  neophobia  and  type  of  vegetable

AA

B BC CD D

Page 45: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  45  

 Figure  17:  A:  Liking  of  carrots  in  %  reported  by  parents.  

 

 Figure  18:  Intake  of  carrots  in  %  reported  by  parents.  

 

2% 1% 2%

7%

22%

42%

24%

Liking  of  carrots  in  %  reported  by  parents

Not  like  at  all

Not  like  

Not  like  a  little

Neither  like  or  not  like

Like  a  little

Like  

Like  a  lot

23%

53%

13%

9%

2%

Intake  of  carrots  in  %  reported  by  parents

Daily

2-­‐‑6  times  per  week

1  time  per  week

<  1  time  per  week

Never

Page 46: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  46  

 Figure  19:  A:  Liking  of  parsnips  in  %  reported  by  parents.  

Figure  20:  Intake  of  parsnips  in  %  reported  by  parents  

The  correlation  of   the  children’s   liking  of   carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  parents  and   the  

children’s  liking  of  carrots  (CBA,  CBO  and  CR)  and  parsnips  (PBA,  PBO  and  PR)  at  pre-­‐‑test  was  

also  tested.  In  the  CBA  group,  the  results  showed  a  significant  (p=0.005)  Pearson’s  coefficient  

of  0.178  and  there  was  therefore  a  weak  positive  relationship  between  the  parents  reported  

liking  and  the  children’s  liking  for  CBA  at  the  pre-­‐‑test.  In  the  CR  group,  there  was  a  significant  

(p-­‐‑value=0.000)  positive  Pearson  coefficient  of  0.368,  which  means  that  also  here  there  was  a  

32%

15%13%

29%

9%

1% 1%

Liking  of  parsnips  in  %  reported  by  parents

Not  like  at  all

Not  like  

Not  like  a  little

Neither  like  or  not  like

Like  a  little

Like  

Like  a  lot

0%0%3%

39%

58%

Intake  of  parsnips  in  %  reported  by  parents

Daily

2-­‐‑6  times  per  week

1  time  per  week

<  1  time  per  week

Never

Page 47: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  47  

correlation  between   the  parents   reported   liking  and   the   children’s   liking  of  CR.   In   the  PBO,  

there   was   a   significant   (p-­‐‑value=0.004)   positive   correlation   with   a   Pearson   coefficient   of  

0.200.  In  the  groups  CBO,  PBA  and  PR  the  results  showed  no  significant  correlation  between  

the  liking  reported  by  parents  and  the  children’s   liking  at  pre-­‐‑test.  The  results  are  shown  in  

figure  21,  where  a  star  marks  the  significant  correlations.    

 

 Figure  21:  Correlation  between  parents  reported  liking  and  children's  pre-­‐‑test  liking.  The  x-­‐‑axis  shows  the  correlation  between  the  children’s  liking  reported  by  parents  and  the  six  exposure  groups  baked  carrot  (CBA),  boiled  carrot  (CBO),  raw  carrot  (CR),  baked  parsnip  (PBA),  boiled  parsnip  (PBO)  and  raw  parsnip  (PR).  The  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  liking  scale  from  1-­‐‑7.  

 

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

CBA CBO CR PBA PBO PR

Mean  liking

Correlation  between  liking  reported  by  parents  and  children's  pre-­‐‑test  liking

Parent Child

*

*

*

Page 48: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  48  

5.  Discussion  In  the  following  section,  the  presented  results  from  the  study  will  be  discussed  in  reference  to  

what  was  expected  based  on  literature.    

 

The   results   showed   that   the   children   gave   the   familiar   carrot   a   significantly   higher   liking  

compared   to   the  unfamiliar  parsnip  at   the  pre-­‐‑test.  This  was  expected  based  on  a  previous  

mentioned  study  by  Beck  (2014)  who  documented  that  90%  of  the  Danish  consumers  have  a  

high  intake  of  carrots.  This  was  also  supported  by  the  results  from  the  questionnaires,  as  the  

parents  reported  that  66%  of  the  children  liked  a  lot/liked  carrots  (figure  17)  and  76%  of  the  

children   consumed   carrots   daily   or   2-­‐‑6   times   per   week   and   89%   consumed   carrots   in   an  

interval   of   once   a  week   to   daily   (figure  18).   Beck   (2014)   also   found   that   only   50-­‐‑60  of   the  

Danish   consumers   had   parsnips   minimum   once   a   month.   This   was   also   supported   by   the  

results  from  the  questionnaires,  where  the  parents  reported  that  47%  of  the  children  did  not  

like   parsnips   (figure   19),   58  %   of   the   children   never   consumed   parsnips   and   97  %   of   the  

children  had  an  intake  of  parsnips  in  an  interval  from  less  than  once  a  week  to  never  (figure  

20).   These   results  where   a   familiar   vegetable   has   higher   liking   compared   to   an   unfamiliar  

vegetable  is  consistent  with  the  results  of  a  study  by  Dinnella  et  al.  (Dinnella,  et  al.,  2016)  who  

found  a  close  positive  relationship  between  familiarity  and  the  stated  liking.    

   

The  results  from  the  effect  of  preparation  methods  showed  that  the  highest  ratings  of   liking  

for   carrots   and   parsnips  were   given,   when   the   children   ate   the   two   vegetables   “raw”.   The  

preparation  method  baking  had  the  significantly  second  highest  liking  and  the  boiling  had  the  

significantly  lowest  liking.  Based  on  the  answers  from  the  questionnaires,  it  was  also  expected  

that   the   raw  preparation  method  would  have   the  highest   liking   as  many   children  often   eat  

raw  carrots  (appendix  K).  Raw  vegetables  are  in  other  words  familiar  to  the  children  and  two  

studies  also  point  out  that  it  seems  as  if  children  like  crunchiness  in  vegetables  (1998)  (1995),  

even  though  raw  vegetables  can  be  difficult  for  some  children  to  manage,  as  they  can  be  hard  

to  chew.  However,  this  liking  of  a  crunchy  texture  could  be  the  reason  why  raw  parsnips  were  

also  liked.  Other  studies  by  Poelman  et  al.  (2011)  and  Zeinstra  et  al.  (2010)  found  that  boiled  

vegetables  had  the  highest  acceptance  compared  to  the  other  tested  preparation  methods  e.g.  

baked,  grilled,  mashed,  stir-­‐‑fried  and  deep  fried.  However,  none  of  the  studies  used  raw  as  a  

Page 49: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  49  

preparation   method.   According   to   Zeinstra   et   al.   (2010),   the   high   acceptance   of   boiled  

vegetables   in   their   study  was   related   to   the   familiar   vegetable   colour   and   taste,   a   uniform  

surface  without  brown  colouring  and  the  relatively  crunchy  texture.  They  also  found  that  one  

important  explanation  for  the  high  acceptance  of  boiled  vegetables  was  that  the  children  were  

most  familiar  with  this  preparation  method.  In  general,  the  liking  of  vegetables  was  strongly  

influenced  by  the  appearance  of  the  vegetable  and  a  uniform  surface  with  no  brown  colouring  

was  related  to  a  high  liking.  However,  the  boiled  vegetables  were  the  least  liked  in  the  present  

study  and   this  could  be  due   to   the   fact   that   the  vegetables  were  served  cold  and   this   is  not  

how  the  children  are  familiar  with  boiled  vegetables  e.g.  from  dinner  at  home.    

 

The  interaction  effect  between  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  unfamiliar)  and  preparation  

method  showed  that  the  raw  carrot  had  the  significantly  highest  liking  and  the  boiled  parsnip  

had  the  significantly   lowest   liking.  This  was  also  expected,  as   the  children  are  most   familiar  

with   the   raw   carrots   and   probably   not   familiar   with   the   boiled   parsnips   based   on   the  

questionnaires  (appendix  K).    

 

As  mentioned  in  the  introduction,  the  carrot  and  parsnip  were  chosen  in  the  present  study  to  

investigate  the  development  in  liking  over  mere  exposures  of  a  familiar  versus  an  unfamiliar  

vegetable.  This  is  in  the  present  study  related  to  previous  studies  that  have  tested  an  initially  

liked  and  disliked   food.  For   instance,  Maier  et  al.   (2007)  showed  that  after  eight  exposures,  

the  intake  of  the  initially  liked  and  disliked  vegetables  had  reached  the  same  level  for  most  of  

the  participants.  Additionally,  Rollins  et  al.   (2010)  also  found  that  highly   liked  food  remains  

liked  and  the  disliked  foods  become  more  liked  after  a  while.  The  same  was  seen  in  a  study  by  

Hausner  et   al.   (2012),  where  most  of   the  participants  who  had  been  exposed   to  an   initially  

disliked  food  increased  their  acceptance  for  the  food  after  nine  exposures  to  the  same  level  as  

the  initially  liked  food.  Based  on  these  studies,  it  was  expected  in  our  study  that  the  familiar  

carrot  would  have  a  higher  liking  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  compared  to  unfamiliar  parsnip,  but  that  the  

liking   of   the   parsnip   would   increase   during   the   mere   exposures   as   the   children   got   more  

familiar   with   it.   This   means   that   after   the   period   of   mere   exposure,   the   liking   of   the   two  

vegetables  would  be  more  similar,  as  the  liking  of  the  familiar  carrot  would  remain  the  same  

or   maybe   increase   slightly.   However,   this   was   not   the   case   in   the   present   study   as   the  

unfamiliar   parsnip   did   not   reach   the   same   level   of   liking   as   the   familiar   carrot.   As   seen   in  

Page 50: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  50  

figure  8,  the  vegetables  and  preparation  methods  that  had  the  highest  liking  at  pre-­‐‑test  were  

CR,   then  PR  and   then  CBO,  which  means   that  CBA  had   the   third   lowest   liking,  PBA  had   the  

second  lowest  liking  and  PBO  had  the  lowest  liking.  At  the  post-­‐‑test,  CR,  CBO  and  PR  had  the  

highest  liking  and  CBA,  PBA  and  PBO  had  the  lowest  liking.  This  means  that  CR  remained  the  

most   liked   vegetable,   which   was   expected,   and   then   PBO   also   remained   the   least   liked  

vegetable  and  did  not  reach  the  same  liking  as  the  familiar  carrot.  This  could  be  explained  by  

the  limitations  of  the  study,  which  are  described  in  detail  in  the  end  of  the  discussion.    

 

The  results  from  the  effect  of  mere  exposure  (figure  8)  showed  that  there  was  no  significant  

effect   of  mere   exposure   on   liking  within   the   exposure   groups   from  pre-­‐‑test   to   post-­‐‑test   or  

between  some  of  the  exposures  among  the  exposed  children.  But  even  though  there  was  no  

significant   difference   in   liking,   the   results   showed   a   tendency   as   the  mean   liking   increased  

and   had   the   highest   liking   at   exposure   no.   8   in   the   groups   CBA,   PBA,   PBO   and   PR   and   the  

highest  liking  at  post-­‐‑test  in  the  CBO  group.  The  liking  in  the  CR  group  was  unchanged,  which  

is  consistent  with  literature  (Caton,  et  al.,  2013).  The  results  showed  that  the  raw  preparation  

method  did  not   influence  the  liking  of  the  familiar  carrot  over  mere  exposures,  as  the   liking  

remained  the  same,  however  the  liking  of  the  unfamiliar  parsnip  increased  slightly.  Also,  both  

the   baked   carrot   and   parsnip   increased   slightly   in   liking   as   well   as   the   boiled   carrot   and  

parsnip.  The  results  showed  that  the  liking  of  preparation  method  in  general  was  higher  in  the  

familiar  carrot  compared  to  the  unfamiliar  parsnip.  These  results  indicate  that  mere  exposure  

and  preparation  methods  did  have  some  effect  on  liking  of  the  vegetables,  however  it  was  not  

enough  to  make  the  unfamiliar  parsnip  as  liked  as  the  familiar  carrot.  This  can  be  explained  by  

the  fact  that  there  are  several  factors  in  the  study,  which  can  have  influenced  the  results  of  the  

effect  of  mere  exposure.  First,  a  higher  number  of  repeated  exposures  could  be  necessarily  to  

obtain  a  significant  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking.  The  literature  that  are  concerned  with  

this  topic  is  not  consistent  with  the  number  of  exposures  needed  to  see  an  increase  in  liking,  

acceptance  or  intake.  One  study  found  an  increase  in  intake  after  five  exposures  (2012),  two  

other  studies  have  found  an  effect  after  eight  to  nine  exposures  (Lakkakula  A,  2010)  (2007),  

while  another  study  needed  up  to  15  exposures  to  enhance  the  preference  (Horne  P.  J.,  2004).  

These  inconsistent  numbers  of  mere  exposures  are  probably  due  to  different  types  of  foods,  

age  groups  and  study  designs  in  the  literature.  For  instance,  vegetables  need  a  higher  number  

of  exposures  compared  to  fruits,  the  age  groups  are  differing  from  infants  to  school  children  

Page 51: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  51  

or  even  older  and  the  design  of  the  study  are  often  designed  with  a  pre-­‐‑test,  a  post-­‐‑test,  a  pre-­‐‑

determined  number  of  mere  exposures,  which  can  give  some  limitations.  An  important  factor  

that  might  have  influenced  the  results  of  no  significant  effect  of  mere  exposure  on  liking  in  our  

study   is   the   children’s   level   of   neophobia.   Almost   40%  of   the   children  were   categorized   as  

“Neophobic”,  which  means  that  they  might  require  more  support  and  adaption  compared  to  

the  other  children,  and  the  mere  exposure  might  not  be  successful  by  itself  in  increasing  these  

children’s   liking  (Wild,  Graaf,  &   Jager,  2017).  This  will  be  discussed   later.  Other   factors   that  

can  have  influenced  the  results  could  be  the  effect  of  group  dynamics  in  the  class  room,  noises  

and  not  sedentary  children  that  have   influenced  the  other  children  as   they  were  tasting   the  

samples.  Also,   the   temperature  of   the  vegetables  can  have   influenced  the   final  result  as   it   is  

not   common   to   eat   boiled   and   baked   carrot   and   parsnip   cold.   As  mentioned   shortly   in   the  

results  associated  figure  8,   it   is  also  relevant  to  mention  that  the  liking  of  PR,  CBA,  PBA  and  

PBO   decreased   from   exposure   no.   8   to   post-­‐‑test.   This   can   possibly   be   explained   by   the  

boredom  effect  –  not  due  to  the  number  of  exposures,  but  due  to  the  high  number  of  tastings  

at  the  post-­‐‑test.  

 

As  mentioned   in   the   section  Materials   and  methods,   the   study  was   designed   so   that   it  was  

possible   to   compare   the  non-­‐‑exposed   children’s   liking  with   the   exposed   children’s   liking   at  

pre-­‐‑test   and   post-­‐‑test   and   in   that   way   see   if   there   was   an   effect   of   being   exposed   to   an  

vegetable/preparation   method   compared   to   not   being   exposed.   Therefore,   it   was   tested   if  

there   was   a   difference   in   the   liking   between   the   exposed   children   and   the   non-­‐‑exposed  

children  within  all  six  exposure  groups  at  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test.  Here,  the  results  showed  that  

there   was   no   significant   difference   in   liking   between   the   exposed   children   and   the   non-­‐‑

exposed  children  in  the  CR  and  CBA  group.  In  relation  to  the  CR,  this  result  was  expected,  as  

the  raw  carrot  was  the  initially  most  liked  and  most  frequently  consumed  (appendix  K).  In  the  

CBO  group,  results  showed  that  the  non-­‐‑exposed  children  had  a  significantly   lower   liking  at  

the  pre-­‐‑test   and  post-­‐‑test   compared   to   the  exposed   children’s   liking  at   the  4.   exposure  and  

post-­‐‑test.   In   the   PBA-­‐‑group,   the   exposed   children   had   a   significantly   higher   liking   at   the   4.  

exposure   compared   to   the  non-­‐‑exposed   children’s   liking   at   the  post-­‐‑test.   In   the  PBO-­‐‑group,  

the  exposed  children  had  a   significantly  higher   liking  at   the  post-­‐‑test   compared   to   the  non-­‐‑

exposed  children’s  liking  at  post-­‐‑test.  In  the  PR  group,  the  exposed  children  had  a  significantly  

higher   liking   at   pre-­‐‑test,   4.   exposure   and  post-­‐‑test   compared   to   the  non-­‐‑exposed   children’s  

Page 52: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  52  

liking  at  post-­‐‑test.  These  results  show  that   the  mere  exposure  has  had  an  effect  on   liking   in  

this   study   when   the   liking   from   mere   exposure   is   compared   to   the   liking   from   the   non-­‐‑

exposed   children/control   group.   Therefore,   this   study   support   the   already   existing   studies,  

who  have  also  found  an  effect  of  mere  exposure  (2007)  (2012)  (2012).    

 

In   relation   to   the   effect   of   gender   on   liking,   it   was   expected   that   there   was   no   difference  

between  the  gender  based  on  a  study  by  Fagt  et  al.  (2015)  about  the  Danish  people’s  dietary  

habits   from   2011   to   2013.   This   study   showed   that   there   was   almost   no   difference   in   the  

average  daily  intake  of  vegetables  between  boys  and  girls  in  the  age  group  4-­‐‑9  years  old.  The  

boys  had  an  average  daily  intake  of  158  grams  and  the  girls  had  an  average  daily  intake  of  157  

grams.  The  results  from  this  study  shows  that  the  boys  have  a  significantly  higher  liking  of  the  

raw  parsnip  (PR)  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  compared  to  the  girls.  But  otherwise,  there  are  no  significant  

differences  between  the  genders  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  or  at  the  post-­‐‑test.  The  significant  difference  

in  the  PR-­‐‑group  at  the  pre-­‐‑test  could  be  due  to  randomness,  as  there  is  no  explained  cause  of  

this.   While   our   results   showed   no   significant   difference   in   liking   of   vegetables   between  

gender,   a   review   of   Rasmussen   et   al.   (2006)   found   that   “girls  tend  to  have  a  higher  or  more  

frequent  intake  of  most  of  fruit  and/or  vegetables  than  boys”  in  twenty-­‐‑seven  out  of  49  papers.  

Beyond  that,  in  eighteen  out  49  papers  they  found  no  difference  in  the  intake.  This  shows  that  

there  is  some  inconsistence  regarding  the  difference  in  intake  or  liking  between  gender.    

 

One  of  the  factors  that  have  already  been  mentioned  to  influence  the  acceptance  of  vegetables  

is  the  children’s  level  of  neophobia  and  therefore  it  was  relevant  to  classify  the  children  of  the  

study   in   different   groups   based   on   their   level   of   neophobia   to   see   the   effect   of   the   level   of  

neophobia   on   liking.   The   level   and   grouping   of   neophobia   was   calculated   based   on   the  

parent’s  answers  at  the  Food  Neophobia  Scale  for  Children  (FNSC),  which  showed  that  almost  

40%   of   the   children   were   categorized   as   “Neophobic”.   The   results   of   the   effect   of   level   of  

neophobia   on   liking   showed   that   the   “Not   neophobic”   group   had   the   significantly   highest  

liking   compared   to   the   “Neophobic”   group   at   the   pre-­‐‑test.   At   the   post-­‐‑test,   the   group   “Not  

neophobic”   had   the   significantly   highest   liking   again,   and   the   group   “Neophobic”   had   the  

significantly  lowest  liking.  These  results  were  expected  as  it  was  anticipated  that  the  children  

with   a   general   low   level   of   neophobia  would   also   have   a   higher   liking   of   both   the   familiar  

carrot   and   the   unfamiliar   parsnip   compared   to   the   children   with   a   general   high   level   of  

Page 53: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  53  

neophobia   as   they  do  not   like  new   foods.   This   also  means   that   these   results   are   consistent  

with   already   existing   literature   as   Howard   et   al.   (2012)   found   that   food   neophobia   was  

associated  with  liking  and  trying  fewer  vegetables.  The  level  of  neophobia  was  also  tested  to  

see  the  response  to  a  familiar  vegetable  compared  to  an  unfamiliar  vegetable.  It  was  expected  

that   the   familiar   carrot   in   general   would   have   a   higher   liking   compared   to   the   unfamiliar  

parsnip  regardless  of  level  of  neophobia.  Again,  the  results  showed  (figure  15+16)  what  was  

expected,  as   the  children   in  all   three  groups  of   levels  of  neophobia  gave   the  carrot  a  higher  

liking  compared  to  the  parsnip.  The  “Not  neophobic”  group  with  carrot  had  the  highest  liking  

and  the  “Neophobic”  group  with  the  parsnip  had  the  lowest  liking  at  both  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑

test.  Again,  this  is  consistent  with  the  questionnaires,  in  which  the  parents  reported  that  the  

children   in   general   often   had   carrots   and   almost   never   had   parsnips   (appendix   K).  

Furthermore,  our  results  also  indicate  that  the  answers  to  FNSC  from  the  parents  had  a  high  

validity   as   their   answers   matches   the   children’s   liking.   As   the   results   from   the   FNSC   are  

consistent  with  the  results  from  the  children’s  liking  at  both  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test,  this  shows  

that  the  FNSC  can  still  be  used  today  even  though  it  was  developed  in  1994.  Recent  studies  by  

Damsbo-­‐‑Svendsen  et  al.  (2017)(2017)  have  questioned  the  applicability  of  the  FNSC  today,  as  

the   accessibility   of   foods   from   ethnic   countries,   which   are   the   content   of   some   of   the  

questions  in  the  FNSC,  has  increased  since  then  and  therefore  some  of  the  questions  are  not  

relevant  anymore.    

 

The  final  results  about  the  correlation  between  the  children’s  liking  and  the  children’s  intake  

of  both  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  the  parents  showed  a  significant,  positive,  medium-­‐‑

strong  correlations  coefficient  of  0,6  between  the  children’s  liking  and  the  children’s  intake  of  

carrots   reported   by   the   parents.   The   same  was   the   case  with   the   children’s   liking   and   the  

children’s  intake  of  parsnips  reported  by  parents,  which  gave  a  result  of  a  significant,  positive  

and  medium   strong   correlation   coefficient   of   0,52.   These   results   showed   that   there   was   a  

stronger   correlation  between   the   liking   and   intake  of   carrots   compared   to  parsnips.  As   the  

Pearson’s  coefficient  is  not  close  to  1  in  any  of  the  cases,  it  showed  that  there  was  not  a  strong  

relationship   between   the   children’s   liking   and   intake   reported   by   the   parents,  which   could  

indicate   that   even   though   the   children   have   a   high   liking   as   for   the   carrot,   it   does   not  

necessarily  mean  that  they  have  a  similar  high  intake  and  reverse  with  the  parsnip.  Otherwise  

the  results  show  that  the  validity  of  the  questionnaires  are  not  100  %,  but  this  has  not  been  

Page 54: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  54  

the  case  in  the  other  cases  where  results  from  the  questionnaires  have  been  used.  Therefore,  

this  could  indicate  that  it  is  not  necessarily  possible  to  equate  liking  of  a  food  100  %  with  the  

intake  of  a   food  based  on  questionnaires  –  but   it  still  gives  an   indication  of   the  relationship  

between  liking  and  intake.   In  this  case,   the   indication  of  the  relationship  between  children’s  

liking  and  intake  reported  by  parents   is  that  when  the  children  have  a  high  liking,   they  also  

have  a  high  intake  and  reverse  with  the  parsnip  –  a  low  liking  and  a  low  intake.  

 

In  extension  of   the  previous  section,   the  relationship  between  the  children’s   liking  reported  

by   the  parents  and  the  children’s   liking  at  pre-­‐‑test  was  also   tested.   It  showed  no  significant  

correlation  between  liking  of  carrots  reported  by  parents  and  children’s  liking  of  CBO  and  also  

no   significant   correlation   between   children’s   liking   of   parsnips   reported   by   parents   and  

children’s   liking   of   PBA   and   PR.   However,   in   the   carrots-­‐‑group   there   was   a   significant  

correlation   between   the   children’s   liking   of   carrots   reported   by   parents   and   the   children’s  

liking  of  CBA  and  CR  at  pre-­‐‑test  with  the  strongest  relation  to  the  CR.  This  could  indicate  that  

the  parents  particularly  have  thought  of  raw  carrots,  when  they  reported  the  children’s  liking  

of  carrots  together  with  the  fact  that  the  CR  had  the  highest  liking  of  the  different  preparation  

methods   in   the   carrot-­‐‑group  at  pre-­‐‑test.  This   is   supported  by   the   fact   that   the  parents   also  

reported  that  the  carrots  often  were  eaten  raw  (appendix  K).  In  the  parsnip-­‐‑group,  there  was  

only   a   significant   correlation   between   the   children’s   liking   of   parsnips   reported  by   parents  

and  PBO  at  pre-­‐‑test.  This  could  be  due  to  the  fact  that  the  parents  reported  that  the  children  

did  not  like  parsnips  and  PBO  had  the  lowest  liking  at  pre-­‐‑test  of  all  the  parsnips  preparation  

methods.    

 

Strengths  and  limitations  

The  present  study  has  both  strengths  and  limitations.  The  major  strength  of  the  present  study  

is  that  the  results  are  based  on  actual  tasting  and  not  only  data  from  questionnaires.  Beyond  

that,   the   tasting   sessions   took   place   in   a   familiar   and   trusted   environment   for   the   children  

compared   to   tastings   in   laboratories.   This   means   that   the   setting   does   not   influence   the  

results  negatively  as   related   to  an  unnatural   and   forced   situation   in  a   laboratory.  However,  

the  chosen  setting  might  cause  some  other  problems,  which  will  be  discussed  further  down  in  

this  section.    

 

Page 55: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  55  

The  present  study  also  has  some  limitations.  First,  there  is  a  limitation  in  study-­‐‑design  as  the  

study   only   is   looking   at   the   short-­‐‑term   outcomes   of   mere   exposure   of   the   three   different  

preparation  methods  of  the  two  different  vegetables.  So,  the  lack  of  follow-­‐‑up  measurements  

is  a  limitation  as  some  other  studies  have  prioritised  both  3-­‐‑  and  6-­‐‑months  follow-­‐‑up  to  see  

the  long-­‐‑term  effect  of  the  mere  exposures  as  this  is  very  relevant  to  see  if  the  effects  persists  

(2007).     In   relation   to   this,   a   second   limitation   to   the   study   design   is   that   there  were   only  

scheduled   eight   mere   exposures,   which   were   enough   to   see   a   result   according   to   some  

literature,   but   this   was   not   realistic   as   many   of   the   children   had   several   sick   days   and  

therefore  were  not  exposed  to  a  vegetable  eight  times  before  the  post-­‐‑test.    

 

In  some  of  the  classes,  the  test  setup  functioned  as  expected,  but  in  some  of  the  other  classes  

there  were  several  problems,  which  could  have  an  influence  on  the  results  from  the  children  

in   the   respective   classes,   which   is   a   limitation.   For   instance,   there   were   many   different  

teachers  or  substitutes  during  the  tastings  and  this  resulted  in  some  cases  in  turbulence  in  the  

classroom  and  this  caused  increased  talking  between  the  children  while  they  were  tasting  the  

vegetables.   This   could   therefore   also   have   influenced   the   results.   The   7-­‐‑point   facial   scale,  

which  was  used  as  a  measuring  tool  for  the  children’s  liking  of  the  two  vegetables,  can  have  

been   a   limitation   as   it   has  might   been   too  detailed  or   complicated  with   the   seven  different  

faces  for  some  of  the  children  who  are  slower  in  their  cognitive  development  (Chen,  1996).    

 

A  last  limitation  of  the  study  design  is  that  the  samples  with  the  vegetables  were  served  cold,  

which  is  not  how  boiled  and  baked  carrots  and  parsnips  usually  are  eaten,  but  it  was  the  only  

way   that   was   logistical   and   practical   possible   and   in   that   way   all   the   samples   were   also  

comparable,   as   they   all   had   the   same   temperature,   shape   and   weight.   But   because   of   this  

limitation,  there  can  be  some  challenges  in  transmitting  the  results  from  the  boiled  and  baked  

vegetables  directly  into  the  homes,  where  the  food  will  be  eaten  warm  and  also  together  with  

other  foods.  Whereas,  especially  the  samples  with  the  raw  carrots  were  served  in  the  way  the  

children  would  usually  eat  them.    

   

Page 56: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  56  

6.  Conclusion  The  overall  objective  of  the  present  study  was  to  investigate  how  three  different  preparation  

methods   influenced   the   liking   of   a   familiar   carrot   and   an   unfamiliar   parsnip   over   mere  

exposures.   The   study   showed   that   the   children   liked   the   familiar   carrot   better   than   the  

unfamiliar  parsnip  and   that   the   children  preferred  both  vegetables   raw  compared   to  baked  

and  boiled.  The  study  found  that  the  learning  strategy  mere  exposure  did  not  have  any  effect  

on   liking   within   the   six   exposure   groups,   but   that   the   children   who   were   exposed   to   a  

vegetable  increased  their  liking  significantly  compared  to  the  children  who  were  not  exposed,  

except  in  the  CR  and  CBA  group.  Hence,  it  can  be  concluded  based  on  the  results  of  this  study  

that  mere   exposure   has   an   effect   on   liking   of   both   a   familiar   and   an   unfamiliar   vegetable,  

which  is  consistent  with  the  literature.  The  study  found  that  the  raw  preparation  method  did  

not  influence  the  liking  of  the  familiar  carrot  over  mere  exposures,  as  the  liking  remained  the  

same,   however   the   liking   of   the   unfamiliar   parsnip   increased   slightly.   Also,   both   the   baked  

carrot  and  parsnip   increased  slightly   in   liking  as  well   as   the  boiled  carrot  and  parsnip.  The  

results   showed   that   the   liking  of  preparation  method   in  general  was  higher   for   the   familiar  

carrot   compared   to   the  unfamiliar  parsnip.  Based  on   these   results,   it   can  be  concluded   that  

there  is  a  tendency  of  an  increase  in  liking  in  all  preparation  methods  over  mere  exposure  and  

therefore  that  preparation  methods  and  mere  exposure  cane  be  used  to  improve  the  liking  of  

vegetables.  

 

The   liking  of  both   the   familiar   carrot   and   the  unfamiliar  parsnip  was  not   influenced  by   the  

gender   of   the   children   -­‐‑   except   that   the   boys   had   a   higher   liking   of   PR   in   the   pre-­‐‑test.  

However,  the  study  found  that  food  neophobia  had  an  effect  on  liking  as  the  children  with  low  

neophobic  status  had  the  highest  liking  and  the  children  with  high  neophobic  status  had  the  

lowest   liking  at  both  pre-­‐‑test  and  post-­‐‑test.  This  could  indicate  that  that  neophobic  children  

need   more   exposures   and/or   other   learning   strategies   in   order   to   increase   their   liking   of  

familiar   and   unfamiliar   vegetables.   The   results   also   showed   that   the   familiar   carrot   had   a  

higher  liking  compared  to  the  unfamiliar  parsnip  regardless  of  neophobic  status.  Based  on  the  

results   from   the  neophobic   status,   it   can  be  concluded   that   food  neophobia   is  an   important  

factor   that   influences   the   children’s   liking  of  vegetables.  However,  more   research   is  needed  

both  in  relation  to  preparation  methods,  mere  exposure  and  food  neophobia.  

Page 57: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  57  

7.  Perspectives  As  the  current  intake  of  vegetables  among  children  still  do  not  meet  the  recommendations,  it  

remains  important  to  investigate  which  factors  and  learning  strategies  that  can  improve  the  

acceptance  and  intake.  The  overall  objective  of  the  present  study  was  to  investigate  how  three  

preparation  methods  influenced  the  liking  of  a  familiar  carrot  and  an  unfamiliar  parsnip  over  

mere   exposures.   The   study   concluded   that   the   different   preparation   methods   and   mere  

exposure  can   improve   the   liking   for  vegetables.  This  means   that   the   learning  strategy  mere  

exposure   and   different   preparation  methods   can   be   used   both   at   homes   by   parents   and   in  

institutions   in  order   to   increase   the   liking  and   improve   the  acceptance  of  both   familiar  and  

unfamiliar  vegetables  in  children.  This  is  important  in  order  to  improve  the  human  health  in  a  

bigger  perspective.  However,  more  research  is  needed  on  the  implement  of  using  the  strategy  

mere  exposure  at  home  by  the  parents,  in  institutions,  schools  etc.    

 

In  future  research,  it  could  be  interessting  also  to  involve  the  parents  at  home  in  the  period  of  

mere  exposure  of  the  different  preparation  methods  as  this  is  more  natural  at  home.  It  would  

also  be  interesting  to  have  a  higher  number  of  exposures  and  some  follow-­‐‑up  measurements  

to  see   if   the  possible  effect   is  maintained  after  months.   In  general,  more  research  about   the  

effect  of   the  preparation  method  and  mere  exposure  strategy   is  needed   to   fully  understand  

the  effect  of  it.  

   

Page 58: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  58  

8.  References  Astrup,  A.  (2005).  Kostradene  2005.  Danmarks  Fødevareforskning.  

Baxter,  Jack,  &  Schröder.  (1998).  The  use  of  repertory  grid  method  to  elicit  perceptual  data  

from  primary  school  children.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(9),  p.  73.80.  

Beck,  T.  K.  (2014).  Impact  of  consumers'  bitter  taste  phenotype,  familiarity,  liking,  

demography  and  food  life  style  on  intake  of  bitter  tasting  coarse  vegetables.  PhD  

Thesis,  Science  and  Technology.  

Bell,  K.,  &  Tepper,  B.  (2006).  Short-­‐‑term  vegetable  intake  by  young  children  classified  by  6-­‐‑n-­‐‑

propylthoiuracil  bitter-­‐‑taste  phenotype.  American  Journal  of  Clinical  Nutrition(84),  pp.  

245–251.  

Betty  Ruth  Carruth,  P.  Z.  (2004).  Prevalence  of  Picky  Eaters  among  Infants  and  Toddlers  and  

Their  Caregivers’  Decisions  about  Offering  a  New  Food.  

Birch,  L.  L.,  &  Fisher,  J.  O.  (1998).  Development  of  eating  behaviours  among  children  &  

adolescents.  Pediatrics(101),  pp.  539–549.  

Brandi  Y.  Rollins,  E.  L.  (2010).  Stability  and  change  in  snack  food  likes  and  dislikes  from  5  to  

11  years.  Appetite(55),  pp.  371–373.  

Capiola,  A.,  &  Raudenbush,  B.  (2012).  The  Effects  of  Food  Neophobia  and  Food  Neophilia  on  

Diet  and  Metabolic  Processing.  Food  and  Nutrition  Sciences(3),  pp.  1397-­‐‑1403.  

Cardello,  A.,  Schutz,  H.,  Snow,  C.,  &  Lesher,  L.  (2000).  Predictors  of  food  acceptance,  

consumption  and  satisfaction  in  specific  eating  situations.  Food  Quality  and  

Preference(11),  pp.  201-­‐‑216.  

Caton,  S.  J.,  Ahern,  S.  M.,  Remy,  E.,  Nicklaus,  S.,  Pam,  B.,  &  Hetherington,  M.  M.  (2013).  

Repetition  counts:  repeated  exposure  increases  intake  of  a  novel  vegetable  in  UK  pre-­‐‑

school  children  compared  to  flavour–flavour  and  flavour–nutrient  learning.  British  

Journal  of  Nutrition(109),  pp.  2089–2097.  

Chen.  (1996).  Age  Appropriate  Hedonic  Scales  to  measure  Food  Preferrences  of  Young  Children.  

Journal  of  Sensory  Studies.  

Coulthard,  H.,  Harris,  G.,  &  Emmett,  P.  (2010).  Long-­‐‑term  consequences  of  early  fruit  and  

vegetable  feeding  practices  in  the  United  Kingdom.  Public  Health  Nutrition(13),  pp.  

2044-­‐‑2051.  

Page 59: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  59  

Cullen,  K.,  Baranowski,  T.,  Owens,  E.,  Marsh,  T.,  Rittenberry,  L.,  &  de  Moor,  C.  (2003).  

Availability,  accessibility,  and  preferences  for  fruit,  100%  fruit  juice,  and  vegetables  

influence  children’s  dietary  behavior.  Health  Educ  Behav(30),  pp.  615–26.  

Damsbo-­‐‑Svendsen  M.,  F.  M.  (2017).  Development  of  novel  tools  to  measure  food  neophobia  in  

children.  Appetite.  

Damsbo-­‐‑Svendsen,  M.,  Bom  Frøst,  M.,  &  Olsen,  A.  (2017).  A  review  of  instruments  developed  

to  measure  food  neophobia.  Appetite.  

Dinnella,  C.,  Morizet,  D.,  Masi,  C.,  Cliceri,  D.,  Depezay,  L.,  Appleton,  K.  M.,  .  .  .  Monteleone,  E.  

(2016).  Sensory  determinants  of  stated  liking  for  vegetable  names  and  actual  liking  for  

canned  vegetables:  A  cross-­‐‑country  study  among  European  adolescents.  Appetite(107),  

pp.  339-­‐‑347.  

Donadini,  G.,  Fumi,  M.,  &  Porretta,  S.  (2012).  Influence  of  preparation  method  on  the  hedonic  

response  of  preschoolers  to  raw,  boiled  or  oven-­‐‑baked  vegetables.  LWT  -­‐‑  Food  Science  

and  Technology(49),  pp.  282-­‐‑292.  

Doveya,  T.  M.,  Staples,  P.  A.,  Gibson,  E.  L.,  &  Halfor,  J.  C.  (2007).  Food  neophobia  and  

‘picky/fussy’  eating  in  children:  A  review.  

Fagt,  S.  (2015).  Danskernes  kostvaner  2011-­‐‑2013,  Hovedresultater.  DTU  Fødevareinstituttet,  

Afdeling  for  Ernæring.  

Fagt,  S.,  &  Matthiessen,  J.  (2017).  Kostens  betydning  for  børn  og  unges  sundhed  og  overvægt:  

2000-­‐‑2013.  Afdeling  for  Risikovurdering  og  Ernæring.  DTU  Fødevaresinstituttet.  

Falciglia,  G.,  Couch,  S.,  Gribble,  L.,  Pabst,  S.,  &  Frank,  R.  (2000).  Food  neophobia  in  childhoold  

affects  dietary  variety.  Journal  of  the  American  Dietetic  association(12),  pp.  1474-­‐‑1478.  

Fildes,  A.,  Jaarsveld,  C.  H.,  Wardle,  J.,  &  Cooke,  L.  (2014).  Parent-­‐‑Administered  Exposure  to  

Increase  Children’s  Vegetable  Acceptance:  A  Randomized  Controlled  Trial.  Academy  of  

Nutrition  and  Dietetics.(6),  pp.  881-­‐‑888.  

Foundation,  T.  L.  (2011).  How  can  the  consumption  of  vegetables  in  Europe  be  increased?  

France.  

Gertrude  G.  Zeinstra,  M.  K.  (2010).  The  influence  of  preparation  method  on  children’s  liking  

for  vegetables.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(21),  pp.  906–914.  

Gibson,  E.  L.,  Wardle,  J.,  &  Watts,  C.  J.  (1998).  Fruit  and  vegetable  consumption,  nutritional  

knowledge  and  beliefs  in  mothers  and  children.  Appetite(31),  pp.  205–228.  

Page 60: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  60  

Hausner,  H.,  Hartvig,  D.  L.,  Reinbach,  H.  C.,  Wendin,  K.,  &  Bredie,  W.  L.  (2012).  Effects  of  

repeated  exposure  on  acceptance  of  initially  disliked  and  liked  Nordic  snack  bars  in  9-­‐‑

11  year-­‐‑old  children.  Clinical  Nutrition(31),  pp.  137-­‐‑143.  

Hausner,  H.,  Olsen,  A.,  &  Møller,  P.  (2012).  Mere  exposure  and  flavour–flavour  learning  

increase  2–3  year-­‐‑old  children’s  acceptance  of  a  novel  vegetable.  Appetite(58),  pp.  

1152–1159.  

Helland,  S.  H.,  Bere,  E.,  Bjørnara,  H.  B.,  &  Øverby,  N.  C.  (2017).  Food  neophobia  and  its  

association  with  intake  of  fish  and  other  selected  foods  in  a  Norwegian  sample  of  

toddlers:  A  cross-­‐‑sectional  study.  Appetite(114),  pp.  110-­‐‑117.  

Horne,  P.  J.  (2004).  Increasing  children’s  fruit  and  vegetable  consumption.  A  peer-­‐‑modelling  

and  rewards-­‐‑based  intervention.  European  Journal  of  Clinical  Nutrition(58),  pp.  1649-­‐‑

1660.  

Howard,  A.  J.,  Mallan,  K.  M.,  Byrne,  R.,  Magarey,  A.,  &  Daniels,  L.  A.  (2012).  Toddlers’  food  

preferences.  The  impact  of  novel  food  exposure,  maternal  preferences  and  food  

neophobia.  Appetite  (59),  pp.  818-­‐‑825.  

Jennifer  M.  Schindler,  D.  C.  (2012).  Assessing  the  effect  of  food  exposure  on  children's  

identification  and  acceptance  of  fruit  and  vegetables.  Eating  Behaviors.  

Johnson,  S.  L.  (2016).  Developmental  and  Environmental  Influences  on  Young  Children’s  

Vegetable  Preferences  and  Consumption.  Advanced  Nutrition  (Suppl)(7),  pp.  2200S-­‐‑

31S.  

Kirby  S,  B.  T.  (1995).  Children’s  fruit  and  vegetable  intake:  Socioeconomic,  adult  child,  

regional,  and  urban-­‐‑rural  influences.  Journal  of  Nutrition  Education(27),  pp.  261-­‐‑271.  

Koidis,  A.,  Rawson,  A.,  &  Brunton,  N.  (2015).  Effect  of  Different  Types  of  Processing  and  

Storage  on  the  Polyacetylene  Profile  of  Carrots  and  Parsnips.  In  V.  Preedy,  Processing  

and  Impact  on  Active  Components  in  Food  (pp.  45–53).  Academic  Press.  

Kreutzmann,  S.,  Christensen,  L.  P.,  &  Edelenbos,  M.  (2008).  Investigation  of  bitterness  in  

carrots  (Daucus  carota  L.)  based  on  quantitative  chemical  and  sensory  analyses.  

LWT(41),  pp.  193-­‐‑205.  

Lakkakula  A,  G.  J.  (2010).  Repeated  taste  exposure  increases  liking  for  vegetables  by  low-­‐‑

income  elementary  school  children.  Appetite(55),  pp.  226-­‐‑231.  

Larson,  N.,  &  Story,  M.  (2009).  A  review  of  environmental  influences  on  food  choices.  Ann  

Behav  Med  (Suppl)(38),  pp.  S56-­‐‑S73.  

Page 61: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  61  

Laureati,  M.,  Bergamaschi,  V.,  &  Pagliarini,  E.  (2015).  Assessing  childhood  food  neophobia:  

Validation  of  a  scale  in  Italian  primary  school  children.  Food  Quality  and  

Preference(40),  pp.  8-­‐‑15.  

Lawless,  H.,  &  Heymann,  H.  (n.d.).  Sensory  Evaluation  of  Food,  Principles  and  Practices  (Second  

Edition  ed.).  Springer.  

Lehto,  E.,  Ray,  C.,  Haukkala,  A.,  Yngve,  A.,  Thorsdottir,  I.,  &  Roos,  E.  (2015).  Predicting  gender  

differences  in  liking  for  vegetables  and  preference  for  a  variety  of  vegetables  among  

11-­‐‑year-­‐‑old  children.  Appetite.  

Maier,  A.,  Chabanet,  C.,  Schaal,  B.,  Issanchou,  S.,  &  Leathwood,  P.  (2007).  Effects  of  repeated  

exposure  on  acceptance  of  initially  disliked  vegetables  in  7-­‐‑month  old  infants.  Food  

Quality  and  Preference(18),  pp.  1023–1032.  

Mennella,  J.  (2014).  Ontogeny  of  taste  preferences:  basic  biology  and  impli-­‐‑  cations  for  health.  

American  Journal  of  Clinical  Nutrition  (Suppl)(99),  pp.  704S–7011S.  

Mojet,  E.  P.  (2008).  Boredom  and  the  reasons  why  some  new  food  products  fail.    

Nicklaus.  (2004).  A  prospective  study  of  food  preferences  in  childhood.  Food  Quality  and  

Preference(15),  pp.  805-­‐‑818.  

Nicklaus,  S.  (2009).  Development  of  food  variety  in  children.  Appetite(52),  pp.  253-­‐‑255.  

Olsen,  A.,  Ritz,  C.,  Kraaij,  L.,  &  Møller,  P.  (2012).  Children’s  liking  and  intake  of  vegetables:  A  

school-­‐‑based  intervention  study.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(23),  pp.  90-­‐‑98.  

Pliner,  P.  (1994).  Development  of  measures  of  food  neophobia  in  children.  Appetite,  pp.  147-­‐‑

163.  

Poelman,  A.  A.,  Delahunty,  C.  M.,  &  Graaf,  C.  d.  (2015).  Vegetable  preparation  practices  for  5–6  

years  old  Australian  children  as  reported  by  their  parents;  relationships  with  liking  

and  consumption.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(42),  pp.  20–26.  

Poelman,  A.,  &  Delahunty,  C.  (2011).  The  effect  of  preparation  method  and  typicality  of  colour  

on  children’s  acceptance  for  vegetables.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(22),  pp.  355–364.  

Poelman,  A.,  Delahunty,  C.,  &  de  Graaf,  C.  (2015).  Vegetable  preparation  practices  for  5–6  

years  old  Australian  children  as  reported  by  their  parents;  relationships  with  liking  

and  consumption.  (42),  pp.  20–26.  

Poelman,  A.,  Delahunty,  C.,  &  Graaf,  C.  d.  (2013).  Cooking  time  but  not  cooking  method  affects  

children’s  acceptance  of  Brassica  vegetables.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(28),  pp.  441–

448.  

Page 62: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  62  

Rasmussen,  M.  (2006).  Determinants  of  fruit  and  vegetable  consumption  among  children  and  

adolescents:  a  review  of  the  literature.  Part  I:  quantitative  studies.  International  

Journal  of  Behavioral  Nutrition  and  Physical  Activity.  

R.  Bornstein,  A.  K.  (1990).  Boredom  as  a  Limiting  Condition  on  the  Mere  Exposure  Effect.  

Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology(5),  pp.  791-­‐‑800.  

Reetica  Rekhy,  R.  M.  (2014).  Promoting  consumption  of  fruit  and  vegetables  for  better  health.  

Have  campaigns  delivered  on  the  goals?  Appetite(79),  pp.  113-­‐‑123.  

Rollins  B,  L.  E.  (2010).  Stability  and  change  in  snack  food  likes  and  dislikes  from  5  to  11  years.  

Appetite(55),  pp.  371-­‐‑373.  

Rozin,  P.  (1979).  Preference  and  affect  in  food  selection.  Preference  Behavior  and  

Chemoreception,  pp.  289-­‐‑302.  

Sara  M.  Ahern,  S.  J.  (2013).  Eating  a  Rainbow.  Introducing  vegetables  in  the  first  years  of  life  in  

3  European  countries.  

S.  Ahern,  S.  C.  (2014).  The  root  of  the  problem:  increasing  root  vegetable  intake  in  preschool  

children  by  repeated  exposure  and  flavour  flavour  learning.  Appetite(80),  pp.  154-­‐‑160.  

Siegrist,  M.,  Hartmann,  C.,  &  Keller,  C.  (2013).  Antecedents  of  food  neophobia  and  its  

association  with  eating  behavior  and  food  choices.  Food  Quality  and  Preference(30),  pp.  

293-­‐‑298.  

Sullivan,  S.  A.  (1994,  2).  Infant  dietary  experience  and  acceptance  of  solid  foods.  

Pediatrics(93),  pp.  271–277.  

Szczesniak,  A.  (1972).  Consumer  awareness  of  and  attitudes  for  food  texture:  II.  Children  and  

teenagers.  Journal  of  Texture  Studies(3),  pp.  206-­‐‑217.  

Tanumihardjo,  S.,  Suri,  D.,  Simon,  P.,  &  Goldman,  I.  (2016).  Vegetables  of  Temperate  Climates:  

Carrot,  Parsnip,  and  Beetroot.  The  Encyclopedia  of  Food  and  Health(5),  pp.  387-­‐‑392.  

Varming,  C.,  Jensen,  K.,  Møller,  S.,  Brockhoff,  P.  B.,  Christiansen,  T.,  Edelenbos,  M.,  .  .  .  Poll,  L.  

(2004).  Eating  quality  of  raw  carrots––correlations  between  flavour  compounds,  

sensory  profiling  analysis  and  consumer  liking  test.  Food  Quality  and  PReference(15),  

pp.  531-­‐‑540.  

Wang  X.,  Q.  Y.  (2014).  Fruit  and  vegetable  consumption  and  mortality  from  all  causes,  

cardiovascular  disease,  and  cancer:  systematic  review  and  dose-­‐‑response  meta-­‐‑

analysis  of  prospective  cohort  studies.  

Page 63: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  63  

WHO.  (2013).  Promoting  fruit  and  vegetable  consumption  around  the  world.  Information  

sheet.  World  Health  Organization.  

Wild,  V.  W.,  Graaf,  C.  d.,  &  Jager,  G.  (2017).  Use  of  Different  Vegetable  Products  to  Increase  

Preschool-­‐‑Aged  Children’s  Preference  for  and  Intake  of  a  Target  Vegetable:  A  

Randomized  Controlled  Trial.  JOURNAL  OF  THE  ACADEMY  OF  NUTRITION  AND  

DIETETICS(6),  pp.  859-­‐‑866.  

Zajonc,  R.  B.  (1968).  Attitutidinal  effects  of  mere  exposure.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  

Psychology(9).  

Zeinstra.  (2010).  The  influence  of  preparation  method  on  children’  s  liking  for  vegetables.  

Food  Quality  and  Preference(21),  pp.  906-­‐‑914.  

 

 

 

 

   

Page 64: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  64  

9.  APPENDIX    

Page 65: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  65  

Appendix  A  

Spørgeskema  til  forældre      Kære  forældre,   Dit  barns  klasse  indgår  i  en  undersøgelse,  som  udføres  af  en  specialestuderende  ved  Århus  Universitet.  Formålet  er  at  undersøge,  hvordan  tilberedningsmetode  påvirker,  hvor  godt  børn  kan  lide  grøntsager. I  aftale  med  dit  barns  skole  serverer  vi  i  løbet  af  november  og  december  måned  smagsprøver  af  gulerødder  og  pastinak,  som  dit  barn  angiver,  hvor  godt  han/hun  kan  lide.    For  at  få  et  indblik  i  børnenes  spisevaner  bedes  du  venligst  udfylde  spørgeskemaet  herunder.  Besvar  venligst  spørgsmålene  med  ét  svar,  medmindre  andet  er  angivet.  Bemærk,  hvis  du  har  flere  børn  i  samme  klasse,  udfyld  venligst  et  spørgeskema  for  hvert  barn.    Ved  at  underskrive  på  sidste  side  angiver  du,  at  Århus  Universitet  må  bruge  besvarelserne  inden  for  denne  undersøgelses  formål.  Besvarelserne  behandles  fortroligt  og  anonymt.  Aflever  venligst  spørgeskemaet  til  klasselæren  senest  d.  14/11.  Ved  spørgsmål,  kontakt  venligst:  [email protected]    Dit  barns  navn      

Dit  barns  køn

q   Dreng  q   Pige  

Dit  barns  fødselsdato  og  år    Dit  barns  højde       Dit  barns  vægt        Lider  dit  barn  af  fødevareallergi  eller  intolerance?

q   Nej  q   Ja,  beskriv  

hvilke__________________________________________________________________________________    

Hvor  ofte  spiser  dit  barn  gulerødder?   q   Dagligt    q   2-­‐‑6  gange  om  ugen  q   En  gang  om  ugen  

Page 66: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  66  

q   Sjældnere  end  en  gang  om  ugen    q   Aldrig  

Hvordan  spiser  dit  barn  oftest  gulerødder?  (Angiv  venligst  1,  2  og  3  ud  for  de  mest  anvendte  tilberedningsmetoder,  hvor  1  =  den  hyppigste  tilberedningsmetode)

q   Kogt    q   Grillet  q   Stegt    q   Bagt    q   Dampet    q   Rå  q   Andet  –  angiv  tilberedningsmetode  

____________________________________________________________    Hvor  godt  kan  dit  barn  lide  gulerødder?  (Sæt  1  kryds)  ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Kan  over-­‐‑  hovedet  ikke  lide  

    Kan  hverken  lide  eller  ikke  lide  

    Kan  ekstremt  godt  lide  

 Hvor  ofte  spiser  dit  barn  pastinak?

q   Dagligt    q   2-­‐‑6  gange  om  ugen    q   En  gang  om  ugen  q   Sjældnere  end  en  gang  om  ugen    q   Aldrig  

Hvordan  spiser  dit  barn  oftest  pastinak?  (Angiv  venligst  1,  2  og  3  ud  for  de  mest  anvendte  tilberedningsmetoder,  hvor  1  =  den  hyppigste  tilberedningsmetode)

q   Kogt    q   Grillet    q   Stegt  q   Bagt  q   Dampet    q   Rå    q   Andet  –  angiv  tilberedningsmetode  

____________________________________________________________    Hvor  godt  kan  dit  barn  lide  pastinak?  (Sæt  1  kryds)  ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Kan  over-­‐‑  hovedet  ikke  lide  

    Kan  hverken  lide  eller  ikke  lide  

    Kan  ekstremt  godt  lide  

Page 67: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  67  

 

Angiv  med  1  kryds,  hvor  enig  du  er  i  følgende  udsagn:            

Mit  barn  smager  konstant  på  ny  og  anderledes  mad  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Mit  barn  stoler  ikke  på  ny  mad  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Hvis  mit  barn  ikke  ved,  hvad  der  er  i  maden,  vil  han/hun  ikke  spise  det  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Mit  barn  kan  godt  lide  mad  fra  andre  lande  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Mit  barn  synes,  at  etnisk  mad  ser  for  underligt  ud  til  at  spise  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

     Når  vi  er  ude  og  spise,  vil  mit  barn  gerne  prøve  ny  mad  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 

Page 68: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  68  

Mit  barn  er  bange  for  spise  noget,  som  han/hun  ikke  har  fået  før  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Mit  barn  stiller  store  krav  til  maden,  som  han/hun  spiser  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Mit  barn  spiser  næsten  hvad  som  helst  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

 Mit  barn  kan  lide  at  prøve  nye  etniske  restauranter  

☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐   ☐  

Meget  uenig  

Uenig   Lidt  uenig   Neutral   Lidt  enig   Enig   Meget  enig  

       Forældreunderskrift  for  accept  af  deltagelse  ____________________________________________________________      Tak  for  deltagelsen!    

 

   

Page 69: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  69  

Appendix  B  

Protocol:  preparation  method  of  CBA,  CBO,  PBA  og  PBO  

 

Materialer:  

-­‐‑   Friske  gulerødder  +  pastinakker  (står  i  kølerummet)  

-­‐‑   Snittemaskine  med  E/S  5mm  klinge  

-­‐‑   Bakke/pose  

-­‐‑   Vægt  

-­‐‑   Små  plastik  smørbægre  +  låg  

-­‐‑   Tusch    

-­‐‑   Gryde  +  låg  

-­‐‑   Bageplade,  bagepapir  og  stanniol    

 

Kort  oversigt:  

  Gulerødder   Pastinakker  

Rå   5  mm  tykkelse  

7  g  +/-­‐‑  1  g  

5  mm  tykkelse  

7  g  +/-­‐‑  1  g  

Kogt   5  mm  tykkelse  

Kogetid:  6  min.    

7  g  +/-­‐‑  1g  

5  mm  tykkelse  

Kogetid:  6  min.    

7  g  +/-­‐‑  1g  

Bagt   5  mm  tykkelse  

240°C  i  12  min.  med  bagepapir  under  

og  med  stanniol  henover  

7  g  +/-­‐‑  1g  

5  mm  tykkelse  

200°C  i  12  min.  med  bagepapir  under  og  

stanniol  henover  

7  g  +/-­‐‑  1g  

 

Rå  gulerødder  +  pastinakker:  

-­‐‑   Tag  det  antal  plastik  smørbægre  +  låg,  som  svarer  til  elevantal  til  rå  gulerødder  

-­‐‑   Noter  CR  eller  PR  på  låget  af  dem  alle  

-­‐‑   Gulerødder  afvejes  

-­‐‑   Pastinakker  afvejes  

-­‐‑   Tag  en  skræller  og  skræl  det  hele  

Page 70: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  70  

-­‐‑   Skær  enderne  af,  kun  lige  det  nødvendige  

-­‐‑   Ved  pastinakkerne  skæres  den  bredeste  del  af,  som  ikke  kan  komme  ned  i  maskinen  

-­‐‑   Sæt  snittemaskinen  til  og  sæt  en  pose  på  der,  hvor  stykkerne  kommer  ud  

-­‐‑   Gulerødder  køres  gennem  snittemaskinen  med  klingen  E/S  5mm  

-­‐‑   Pastinakker  køres  gennem  snittemaskinen  med  klingen  E/S  5mm  

-­‐‑   Placer  et  plastik  smørbæger  på  vægten  og  afvej  7  g  (+/-­‐‑  1)  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Dette  gentages  indtil  der  er  lavet  smagsprøver  til  alle  dem,  der  skal  have  rå  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Placer  et  plastik  smørbæger  på  vægten  og  afvej  7  g  (+/-­‐‑  1g)  pastinak  

-­‐‑   Dette  gentages  indtil  der  er  lavet  smagsprøver  til  alle  dem,  der  skal  have  rå  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Stil  alle  prøverne  i  en  kasse  på  køl  natten  over  

 

Bagte  pastinakker:  

-­‐‑   Tænd  ovnen  på  200°C  og  læg  bageplader  med  bagepapir  klar  ved  siden  af  

-­‐‑   Tag  det  antal  plastik  smørbægre  +  låg,  som  svarer  til  elevantal  til  bagte  pastinakker  

-­‐‑   Noter  PBA  på  låget  

-­‐‑   Pastinakkerne  afvejes  

-­‐‑   Pastinakkerne  skrælles  og  enderne  skæres  af    

-­‐‑   Den  bredeste  del,  som  ikke  kan  komme  ned  i  maskinen  skæres  af  

-­‐‑   Sæt  snittemaskinen  til  og  sæt  en  pose  på  der,  hvor  stykkerne  kommer  ud  

-­‐‑   Brug  klingen  med  5  mm  og  kør  alle  pastinakkerne  igennem  maskinen  

-­‐‑   Alle  stykkerne  fordeles  jævnt  ud  på  bagepapiret  

-­‐‑   Når  ovnen  er  200°C,  sættes  pladen  ind  og  et  stopur  sættes  på  12  minutter  

-­‐‑   Når  stopuret  ringer,  tages  pladen  ud  og  pastinakkerne  afkøles  

-­‐‑   Placer  et  plastik  smørbæger  på  vægten  og  afvej  7  g  (+/-­‐‑  1g)  bagt  pastinak  

-­‐‑   Stil  alle  prøverne  i  en  kasse  på  køl  

 

Bagte  gulerødder:  

-­‐‑   Tænd  ovnen  på  240°C  og  læg  bageplader  med  bagepapir  klar  ved  siden  af  

-­‐‑   Tag  det  antal  plastik  smørbægre  +  låg,  som  svarer  til  elevantal  til  bagte  gulerødder  

-­‐‑   Noter  CBA  på  låget  

-­‐‑   Gulerødderne  afvejes  

Page 71: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  71  

-­‐‑   Gulerødderne  skrælles  og  enderne  skæres  af    

-­‐‑   Sæt  snittemaskinen  til  og  sæt  en  pose  på  der,  hvor  stykkerne  kommer  ud  

-­‐‑   Brug  klingen  med  5  mm  og  kør  alle  gulerødderne  igennem  maskinen  

-­‐‑   Alle  stykkerne  fordeles  jævnt  ud  på  bagepapiret  med  stanniol  over,  som  skal  sættes  

godt  fast  

-­‐‑   Når  ovnen  er  240°C,  sættes  pladen  ind  og  et  stopur  sættes  på  12  minutter  

-­‐‑   Når  stopuret  ringer,  tages  pladen  ud  og  pastinakkerne  afkøles  

-­‐‑   Placer  et  plastik  smørbæger  på  vægten  og  afvej  7  g  (+/-­‐‑  1g)  bagt  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Stil  alle  prøverne  i  en  kasse  på  køl  

 

Kogte  gulerødder  +  pastinakker:  

-­‐‑   Hæld  3  liter  vand  i  minimum  to  gryder  og  stil  dem  på  komfuret,  som  tændes  på  6  

-­‐‑   Tag  det  antal  plastik  smørbægre  +  låg,  som  svarer  til  elevantal  til  rå  gulerødder  

-­‐‑   Noter  CBO  eller  PBO  på  låget  af  dem  alle  

-­‐‑   Gulerødder  afvejes  

-­‐‑   Pastinakker  afvejes  

-­‐‑   Tag  en  skræller  og  skræl  det  hele  

-­‐‑   Skær  enderne  af,  kun  lige  det  nødvendige  

-­‐‑   Ved  pastinakkerne  skæres  den  bredeste  del  af,  som  ikke  kan  komme  ned  i  maskinen  

-­‐‑   Sæt  snittemaskinen  til  og  sæt  en  pose  på  der,  hvor  stykkerne  kommer  ud  

-­‐‑   Gulerødder  køres  gennem  snittemaskinen  med  klingen  E/S  5mm  

-­‐‑   Pastinakker  køres  gennem  snittemaskinen  med  klingen  E/S  5mm  

-­‐‑   Når  vandet  koger,  fordeles  pastinakstykkerne  og  gulerod  stykkerne  i  hver  deres  gryde  

-­‐‑   Sæt  låget  på  og  sæt  stopuret  på  6  minutter,  når  vandet  koger  voldsomt  skrues  der  ned  

på  3  

-­‐‑   Når  stopuret  ringer,  hældes  vandet  fra  og  stykkerne  afkøles  

-­‐‑   Placer  et  plastik  smørbæger  på  vægten  og  afvej  7  g  (+/-­‐‑  1)  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Dette  gentages  indtil  der  er  lavet  smagsprøver  til  alle  dem,  der  skal  have  kogt  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Placer  et  plastik  smørbæger  på  vægten  og  afvej  7  g  (+/-­‐‑  1g)  pastinak  

-­‐‑   Dette  gentages  indtil  der  er  lavet  smagsprøver  til  alle  dem,  der  skal  have  kogt  gulerod  

-­‐‑   Stil  alle  prøverne  i  en  kasse  på  køl  natten  over  

Page 72: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  72  

Appendix  C  

Randomization  for  mere  exposre  of  Rosengårdskolen,  O.C    PLAN FOR ROSENGÅRDEN 0.C Exposure 1-8

   

Rosengården,  0.CAri Gawad CRBertha Hjuler Petersen CRHicham Abou Lebde CRJasmin Sharilou Hossein CBOJohan Lisberg Højgaard CBOJustin Nicky Westphal CBOMaj Trieu Hansen CBOMartha Arum Bentzen CBAMathilde Fuglsang Nielsen CBAMohammad Fadi Saleh CBAMuhammed Ibrahim Ali CBANellie Christiane Binar PRTimur Efe Kursyk Edeer PRTristian Jørgensen Mikkelsen PRWilliam Hovman Elley PBOFrederikke Barslund Madsen PBOIsmo Ali Hassan Abdallah PBOMathias Borup Nedergaard PBOMona Bassam PBASebastian Borup Christensen PBASophia Nørmand Johansen PBAUmaiza Ali PBA

Page 73: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  73  

Appendix  D  

Hypothesis:  The  type  of  vegetable  (familiar  versus  unfamiliar)  has  an  effect  on  liking    

Tested  

groups  

Observations   Mean  liking   Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA  p-­‐‑

value  

Conclusion  

All  groups  

at  pre-­‐‑test  

1478   4.1   2.4   <0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

 

 

Tukey  HSD  test  –  analysis  of  the  differences  between  the  categories:  

Comparisons  groups   Difference   Pr  >  Diff   Significant  

Carrot  vs  parsnip   1.0   <  0.0001   Yes    

Vegetable  category   LS  means  liking   Grouping  

Carrot   4.6   A  

Parsnip   3.6   B  

 

 

   

Page 74: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  74  

Appendix  E  

Hypothesis:  The  type  of  preparation  method  has  an  effect  on  liking    

Tested  

groups  

Observations   Mean  liking   Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA  p-­‐‑

value  

Conclusion  

All  groups  

at  pre-­‐‑test  

1478   4.1   2.4   <0.0001   H0  is  

rejected  

 

 

Tukey  HSD  test  –  analysis  of  the  differences  between  the  categories:  

Comparisons  groups   Difference   Pr  >  Diff   Significant  

Raw  vs  boiled   1.6   <0.0001   Yes  

Raw  vs  baked   1.1   <0.0001   Yes  

Baked  vs  raw   0.5   0.002   Yes    

Preparation  method  category   LS  means  liking   Grouping  

Baked   5.0   A  

Boiled   3.9   B  

Raw   3.4   C  

 

 

   

Page 75: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  75  

Appendix  F  

Hypothesis:  The  type  of  vegetable  and  the  type  of  preparation  method  has  an  effect  on  

liking  

 

Tested  

groups  

Observations   Mean  liking   Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA  p-­‐‑

value  

Conclusion  

All  groups  

at  pre-­‐‑test  

1478   4.1   2.4   <0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

 

 

Tukey  HSD  test  –  analysis  of  the  differences  between  the  categories:  

Comparisons  groups   Difference   Pr  >  Diff   Significance  

Carrot  raw  vs  Parsnip  boiled   3.0   <  0.0001   Yes  

Carrot  raw  vs  parsnip  baked   2.1   <  0.0001   Yes  

Carrot  raw  vs  carrot  boiled   2.1   <  0.0001   Yes  

Carrot  raw  vs  carrot  baked   2.0   <  0.0001   Yes  

Carrot  raw  vs  parsnip  raw   1.9   <  0.0001   Yes  

Parsnip  raw  vs  parsnip  boiled   1.1   <  0.0001   Yes  

Parsnip  raw  vs  parsnip  baked   0.3   0.779   No  

Parsnip  raw  vs  carrot  boiled   0.2   0.894   No  

Parsnip  raw  vs  carrot  baked   0.1   0.998   No  

Carrot  baked  vs  parsnip  boiled   1.0   <  0.0001   Yes  

Carrot  baked  vs  parsnip  baked   0.2   0.952   No  

Carrot  baked  vs  carrot  boiled   0.1   0.988   No  

Carrot  boiled  vs  parsnip  boiled   0.9   <  0.0001   Yes  

Carrot  boiled  vs  parsnip  baked   0.1   1.000   No  

Parsnip  baked  vs  parsnip  boiled   0.9   0.000   Yes  

 

   

Page 76: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  76  

 

Category   LS  means  liking   Grouping  

Carrot  raw  (CR)   5.9   A  

Parsnip  raw  (PR)   4.1   B  

Carrot  baked  (CBA)   4.0   B  

Carrot  boiled  (CBO)   3.8   B  

Parsnip  baked  (PBA)   3.8   B  

Parsnip  boiled  (PBO)   2.9   C  

 

 

   

Page 77: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  77  

Appendix  G  

Hypothesis:  The  strategy  mere  exposure  has  an  effect  on  liking  

 

Effect  of  mere  exposure  within  the  six  exposure  groups  –  exposed  children:  

Tested  groups   Observations   Mean  

liking  

Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA    

p-­‐‑value  

Conclusion  

Carrot  baked   397   4.4   2.2   0.247   H0  is  accepted  

Carrot  boiled   422   4.9   2.0   0.059   H0  is  accepted  

Carrot  raw   390   5.6   2.0   0.579   H0  is  accepted  

Parsnip  baked   419   4.1   2.3   0.506   H0  is  accepted  

Parsnip  boiled   392   3.6   2.5   0.377   H0  is  accepted  

Parsnip  raw   422   4.7   2.4   0.931   H0  is  accepted  

 

Differences  in  liking  between  the  exposure  groups  at  the  exposure  numbers:  

Tested  

groups  

Observations   Mean  liking   Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA  p-­‐‑

value  

Conclusion  

Pre-­‐‑test   251   4.1   2.3   <  0.001     H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  1   263   4.3   2.2   0.003   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  2   262   4.4   2.3   0.002   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  3   262   4.5   2.3   0.040   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  4   261   4.6   2.3   0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  5   260   4.5   2.3   0.001   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  6   257   4.7   2.2   0.003   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  7   235   4.7   2.3   0.050   H0  is  rejected  

Exposure  8   144   5.0   2.2   0.365   H0  is  accepted  

Post-­‐‑test   249   4.7   2.4   <  0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

 

 

 

 

 

Page 78: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  78  

 

 

LS  means  for  exposure  number:  

Exposure  no.   CBA   CBO   CR   PBA   PBO   PR  Pretest   3.8   4.3   6.0   3.4   2.9   4.4  1   4.2   4.5   5.4   3.9   3.5   4.3  2   4.1   4.6   5.6   4.2   3.7   4.4  3   4.3   5.0   5.0   4.3   3.7   4.9  4   4.5   4.9   5.8   4.4   3.1   4.9  5   4.4   5.1   5.5   4.0   3.6   4.7  6   5.0   5.3   5.4   4.3   3.7   4.8  7   4.7   4.7   5.6   4.0   4.2   4.8  8   5.2   5.4   5.6   4.6   4.3   5.0  Posttest   4.4   5.6   5.9   3.7   3.8   4.8    

 

Test  between  exposed  and  non-­‐‑exposed  children:  

  Observations   Mean  liking   Std.  deviation   P-­‐‑value   Significant  

CBA   533   4.0   2.4   0.412   No  

CBO   540   3.9   2.4   <0.0001   Yes  

CR   539   5.9   1.8   0.982   No  

PBA   540   3.6   2.4   0.002   Yes  

PBO   525   2.8   2.3   0.009   Yes  

PR   535   3.9   2.3   <0.0001   Yes  

 

Page 79: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  79  

Appendix  H  

Hypothesis:  The  type  of  gender  has  an  effect  on  liking  

 

Tested  groups   Observations   Mean  

liking  

Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA    

p-­‐‑value  

Conclusion  

CBA  pre-­‐‑test   246   4.0   2.3   0.324   H0  is  accepted  

CBA  post-­‐‑test   245   4.0   2.5   0.248   H0  is  accepted  

CBO  pre-­‐‑test   247   3.8   2.4   0.923   H0  is  accepted  

CBO  post-­‐‑test   248   3.7   2.5   0.643   H0  is  accepted  

CR  pre-­‐‑test   248   5.9   1.7   0.761   H0  is  accepted  

CR  post-­‐‑test   249   5.9   1.9   0.696   H0  is  accepted  

PBA  pre-­‐‑test   248   3.8   2.3   0.548   H0  is  accepted  

PBA  post-­‐‑test   247   3.2   2.4   0.420   H0  is  accepted  

PBO  pre-­‐‑test   242   2.9   2.2   0.271   H0  is  accepted  

PBO  post-­‐‑test   241   2.7   2.4   0.083   H0  is  accepted  

PR  pre-­‐‑test   247   4.1   2.2   0.026   H0  is  rejected  

PR  post-­‐‑test   243   3.6   2.4   0.351   H0  is  accepted  

 

 

Tukey  HSD  test:  

Comparisons  groups   Difference   Pr  >  Diff   Significant  

Boy  PR  pre-­‐‑test  vs  girl  PR  pre-­‐‑test   0.6   0.026   Yes  

 

Category   LS  means  liking   Groups  

Boy   4.3   A  

Girl   3.7   B  

Page 80: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  80  

Appendix  I  

Hypothesis:  The  level  of  neophobic  status  has  an  effect  on  liking  

 

Pre-­‐‑test:  level  of  neophobic  status  and  liking  

Tested  groups   Observations   Mean  

liking  

Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA    

p-­‐‑value  

Conclusion  

Pre-­‐‑test  all   1472   4.1   2.4   0.000   H0  is  rejected  

 

Tukey  HSD  test:  Pre-­‐‑test  level  of  neophobic  status  and  liking  

Comparisons  groups   Difference   Pr  >  Diff   Significant  

Not  neophobic  vs  Neophobic   0.6   <  0.0001   Yes  

Not  neophobic  vs  Neither  neophobic  or  not  

neophobic  

0.4   0.071   No  

Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic  vs  

Neophobic  

0.3   0.208   No  

 

 

Pre-­‐‑test:  neophobia,  liking  and  familiar  vs.  unfamiliar  vegetable  

Tested  groups   Observations   Mean  

liking  

Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA    

p-­‐‑value  

Conclusion  

Pre-­‐‑test  all   1472   4.1   2.4   <  0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

 

   

Category   LS  means  liking   Groups  

Not  neophobic   4.4   A  

Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic   4.1   A-­‐‑B  

Neophobic   3.8   B  

Page 81: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  81  

 

 

 

Post-­‐‑test:  neophobia  and  liking  

Tested  groups   Observations   Mean  

liking  

Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA    

p-­‐‑value  

Conclusion  

Pre-­‐‑test  all   1467   3.9   2.5   <  0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

 

Comparisons  groups   Difference   Pr  >  Diff   Significant  

Not  neophobic  vs  Neophobic   1.1   <  0.0001   Yes  

Not  neophobic  vs  Neither  neophobic  or  not  

neophobic  

0.5   0.008   Yes  

Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic  vs  

Neophobic  

0.6   0.002   Yes  

 

 

   

Category   LS  means  liking   Groups  

Carrot  +  Not  neophobic   5.0   A  

Carrot  +  Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic   4.6   A-­‐‑B  

Carrot  +  Neophobic   4.2   B-­‐‑C  

Parsnip  +  Not  neophobic   3.9   C-­‐‑D  

Parsnip  +  Neither  neophobic  or  not  

neophobic  

3.5   D  

Parsnip  +  Neophobic   3.4   D  

Category   LS  means  liking   Groups  

Not  neophobic   4.4   A  

Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic   3.9   B  

Neophobic   3.4   C  

Page 82: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  82  

Post-­‐‑test:  neophobia,  liking  and  familiar/unfamiliar  vegetable  

Tested  groups   Observations   Mean  

liking  

Std.  

deviation  

ANOVA    

p-­‐‑value  

Conclusion  

Pre-­‐‑test  all   1467   3.9   2.5   <  0.0001   H0  is  rejected  

 

 

 

 

Category   LS  means  liking   Groups  

Carrot  +  Not  neophobic   5.3   A  

Carrot  +  Neither  neophobic  or  not  neophobic   4.7   A  

Carrot  +  Neophobic   3.9   B  

Parsnip  +  Not  neophobic   3.6   B-­‐‑C  

Parsnip  +  Neither  neophobic  or  not  

neophobic  

3.2   C-­‐‑D  

Parsnip  +  Neophobic   2.8   D  

Page 83: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  83  

Appendix  J  

Hypothesis:  there  is  a  correlation  between  the  children’s  liking  of  carrots  and  parsnips  

and  the  children’s  intake  of  carrots  and  vegetables  reported  by  parents  

  Carrots   Parsnips  Pearson’s  coefficient:  parents  reported  liking  and  intake  

0.606   0.520  

P-­‐‑values   0.000   0.000   Hypothesis:  there  is  a  correlation  between  the  children’s  liking  of  carrots  and  parsnips  

reporten  by  parents  and  the  children’s  liking  at  pre-­‐‑test  

 

  CBA   CBO   CR   PBA   PBO   PR  Pearson’s  coefficient  Parents  liking  -­‐‑  liking  

0.178   0.065   0.368   0.123   0.200   0.074  

p-­‐‑value   0.005   0.315   0.000   0.073   0.004   0.284  

Page 84: THEEFFECTOFPREPARATIONMETHOD!ON …...Kamilla!Hall!Kragelund!! ! Studienummer201503524!!! 3! Resume(inDanish)! Påtrodsaf,at!forskningviser,at!grøntsagerersundeforosogindeholdervi

    Kamilla  Hall  Kragelund       Studienummer  201503524    

  84  

Appendix  K  

 

Figur  22:  Preparation  methods  of  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  parents.  The  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  number  of  children  

Figur  23:  The  intake  of  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  parents.  The  y-­‐‑axis  shows  the  number  of  children  

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

Boiled Grilled Fried Baked Steamed Raw Andet

Num

ber  of  children

Preparation  methods  of  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  parents

Carrot Parsnip

020406080100120140160

Daily 2-­‐‑6  times  per  week

1  time  per  week <  1  time  per  week

Never

Num

ber  of  children

Intake  of  carrots  and  parsnips  reported  by  parents

Carrot Parsnip