theoretical framework and variables affecting the efficacy...

139
Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based Intervention on Rumination Nick Lee A thesis submitted for the degree of Master of Philosophy at The University of Queensland in 2014 School of Psychology

Upload: others

Post on 21-Apr-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness-

Based Intervention on Rumination Nick Lee

A thesis submitted for the degree of Master of Philosophy at

The University of Queensland in 2014

School of Psychology

Page 2: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

Abstract

A large body over the last 20 years has demonstrated that rumination is an important determinant

of depression. Research has also shown that mindfulness practice can reduce rumination and

depression. However, over this period the concept of rumination has evolved. Initially rumination

was understood to be a single static construct that a person would engage in when they experience

depressive mood. The Differential Activation Hypothesis introduced the idea that rumination

involved an interaction between depressive mood and negative cognition. Later, this model was

expanded with negative cognition seen to be the product of the memories of negative events and

influenced by dysfunctional attitude. Rumination started to be viewed as an interactive cycle

between depressive mood, memories of negative event and dysfunctional attitude. A separate but

relevant literature has demonstrated the relationship between emotion and attention. However, to

date no study has investigated the role of innate attention control capacity on the ruminative

process. Similarly, despite attention control being viewed as a component of mindfulness, little

research has investigated the relationship between attention control capacity and mindfulness.

Thus, this study investigates how attention control capacity influences rumination and

mindfulness. 392 adults recruited from two public universities and the general community were

given a series of online survey questionnaires involving their depression and rumination levels,

dysfunctional attitude, attention control, and different facets of trait mindfulness. Results found

that attention control and dysfunctional attitude sequentially mediated the relationship between

depressive mood and rumination. The mindfulness facets of Nonjudging and Nonreactivity also

mediated the relationship between depressive mood and attention control. These results indicate

that our theoretical understanding of the rumination process should include attention control for a

more holistic conception. Further the results suggest that the enhancement of a Nonjudgemental

attitude and Nonreactivity through mindfulness practice has the potential to interfere with the

rumination process by enhancing a person’s attention control capacity and decreasing the

attention allocation effects of emotional arousal.

Page 3: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

Declaration by author

This thesis is composed of my original work, and contains no material previously published

or written by another person except where due reference has been made in the text. I have

clearly stated the contribution by others to jointly-authored works that I have included in

my thesis. I have clearly stated the contribution of others to my thesis as a whole, including statistical

assistance, survey design, data analysis, significant technical procedures, professional editorial

advice, and any other original research work used or reported in my thesis. The content of my

thesis is the result of work I have carried out since the commencement of my research higher

degree candidature and does not include a substantial part of work that has been submitted to

qualify for the award of any other degree or diploma in any university or other tertiary institution.

I have clearly stated which parts of my thesis, if any, have been submitted to qualify for another

award. I acknowledge that an electronic copy of my thesis must be lodged with the University Library

and, subject to the General Award Rules of The University of Queensland, immediately made

available for research and study in accordance with the Copyright Act 1968. I acknowledge that copyright of all material contained in my thesis resides with the copyright

holder(s) of that material. Where appropriate I have obtained copyright permission from the

copyright holder to reproduce material in this thesis.

Page 4: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

Publications during candidature

No publication included

Publications included in this thesis

No publication included

Page 5: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

Contributions by others to the thesis 87 of the collected sample were collect from the University of Adelaide and were given

permission to be used in this thesis.

No other contributions by others.”

Statement of parts of the thesis submitted to qualify for the award of another degree

N/A

Page 6: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

Acknowledgements

I would like to express my special appreciation and thanks to my advisor Professor Dr. Paul Harnett, you have been a tremendous mentor for me. I would like to thank you for encouraging my research and for allowing me to grow as a research scientist. I would also like to thank you for your brilliant comments and suggestions that have made this thesis refined to the level it has achieved.

A special thanks to my family as they have been nothing but supportive during my stay in Brisbane Keywords

Mindfulness, Attention Control, Dysfunctional Attitude, Rumination, Depression, Emotions

Australian and New Zealand Standard Research Classifications (ANZSRC)

ANZSRC code: 170106 Health, Clinical and Counselling Psychology 100%

Fields of Research (FoR) Classification

FoR code: 1701 Psychology 100%

Page 7: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

1

Table of Contents

List of Figures & Tables ...................................................................................................................... 4

List of Abbreviations used in the Thesis .............................................................................................. 5

CHAPTER 1 - Depression Rumination: Introduction & Theoretical Framework............................... 6

1.01 Depression in Australia ................................................................................................................ 6

1.02 Economic Cost of Depression .................................................................................................. 6

1.03 Trends of Depression in the Community ................................................................................. 7

1.04 Depression Relapse .................................................................................................................. 8

1.04.1 Conventional Treatment & Depression Relapse. .............................................................. 9

1.04.2 Cause of Relapse. ............................................................................................................ 10

1.05 Rumination ............................................................................................................................. 10

1.05.1 Response Styles Theory. ................................................................................................. 11

1.05.2 Rumination & Memory Recall. ....................................................................................... 15

1.05.3 Rumination as a Process - Differential Activation Hypothesis....................................... 15

1.05.4 Refinement of Negative Cognition. ................................................................................ 18

1.05.5 Dysfunctional Attitude. ................................................................................................... 21

1.05.6 Implications of DAH on Conventional Treatment Outcomes. ....................................... 23

1.05.7 Unifying the Differential Activation Hypothesis and Response Style Theory. .............. 23

1.06 The role of Attention Control Capacity and Emotions in the rumination process ................. 26

1.06.1 Depression & Emotions.. ................................................................................................ 26

1.06.2 Emotion and Attention. ................................................................................................... 26

1.07 Attention Control ................................................................................................................... 28

1.07.1 Attention Control & Rumination. ................................................................................... 30

1.07 Conclusion ............................................................................................................................. 32

CHAPTER 2 - Mindfulness and Mindfulness-Based Interventions .................................................. 33

2.0 Introduction .............................................................................................................................. 33

2.01.2 Five Aggregates (Skandhas). .......................................................................................... 34

2.01.3 Mindfulness of the Eightfold Path. ................................................................................. 34

2.01.4 Satipatthana Sutta – Guide to Right Mindfulness. .......................................................... 35

2.01.5 Goal of Mindfulness Meditation. .................................................................................... 38

2.01.6 Conclusion. ..................................................................................................................... 38

2.02 Mindfulness in Modern Psychology ...................................................................................... 39

2.02.1 What is Mindfulness in the West? .................................................................................. 39

2.02.2 Trait and State Mindfulness. ........................................................................................... 45

2.03 Mindfulness Based Interventions ........................................................................................... 49

2.03.1 Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy – Mindfulness on Depression. .......................... 49

Page 8: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

2

2.03.2 Content and Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy. ..................................................... 50

2.03.3 Atmosphere of the Class. ................................................................................................ 53

2.03.4 Elements of Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy. ................................................................ 53

2.03.5 Evidence and Rationale of MBCT. ................................................................................. 54

2.03.6 Interacting Cognitive Subsystem. ................................................................................... 57

2.03.7 Mode of Processing. ........................................................................................................ 58

2.03.8 Dialectical Behavioural Therapy – A Mindful Approach to Personality Disorder. ........ 60

2.03.9 Content and Rationale for Mindfulness in DBT ............................................................. 61

2.03.10 Evidence for DBT. ........................................................................................................ 63

2.03.11 Mindfulness & Emotion Regulation. ............................................................................ 64

2.04 Mindfulness & Attention Control .......................................................................................... 66

2.05 Differences between Traditional and Modern Practices ........................................................ 68

2.06 Future of Mindfulness Research ............................................................................................ 69

CHAPTER 3 - Rationale for Present Study ....................................................................................... 70

3.01 Theoretical Approach ............................................................................................................. 70

3.02 Hypothesis .............................................................................................................................. 72

CHAPTER 4 - Methodology.............................................................................................................. 73

4.01 Participants ............................................................................................................................. 73

4.03 Measures ............................................................................................................................ 74

4.03 Ethical Consideration ............................................................................................................. 76

CHAPTER 5 - Results ....................................................................................................................... 79

5.01 Hypothesis 1 (H1). ................................................................................................................. 79

5.02 Hypothesis 2 (H2). ................................................................................................................. 80

5.03 Hypothesis 3 (H3). ................................................................................................................. 81

5.04 Hypothesis 4 (H4). ................................................................................................................. 82

5.04.1 Overall Trait Mindfulness. .............................................................................................. 82

5.04.2 Observing. ....................................................................................................................... 82

5.04.3 Describing. ...................................................................................................................... 83

5.04.4 Acting with Awareness. .................................................................................................. 83

5.04.5 Nonjudging of Inner Experiences ................................................................................... 83

5.04.6 Nonreacting of Inner Experiences ................................................................................... 84

5.05 Post-hoc Analysis. .................................................................................................................. 84

5.05.01 Attention Control. ......................................................................................................... 84

5.05.02 Dysfunctional Attitude. ................................................................................................. 84

CHAPTER 6 - Discussion.................................................................................................................. 85

6.01 Discussion .............................................................................................................................. 85

6.02 Methodological Issues ............................................................................................................ 91

6.03 Future Research...................................................................................................................... 92

Page 9: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

3

6.04 Summery & Conclusion ......................................................................................................... 92

References .......................................................................................................................................... 94

Page 10: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

4

List of Figures & Tables

Figure 1. Leading global causes of “Years” lost due to disability (YLD) in high-income countries in

2004 .....................................................................................................................................................7

Figure.2. Frequency distribution of age of onset of first episode of major depression........................8

Figure 3. Process of Rumination according to the DAH....................................................................18

Figure 4. Process of Rumination according to the ICS......................................................................20

Figure 5. Sequential Meditation of Negative Affect and Rumination................................................71

Figure 6. Conceptual Diagram of Model 6.........................................................................................76

Figure 7. Conceptual Diagram of Model 4.........................................................................................77

Table 1. Correlations between Negative Affect, Attention Control, Dysfunctional Attitude and

Depression and Rumination..............................................................................................................78

Table 2. Correlations between Mindfulness Facets and Negative Affects, Attention Control,

Dysfunctional Attitude, Depression and Rumination........................................................................79

Figure 8. Schematic representation of the rumination model with mindfulness effect.....................85

Figure 9. Schematic representation rumination model.....................................................................88

Page 11: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

5

List of Abbreviations used in the Thesis

AC – Attention Control ATQ – Adult Temperament Questionnaire BPD – Bipolar Personality Disorder CBT – Cognitive Behaviour Therapy DA – Dysfunctional Attitude DAH – Differential Activation Hypothesis DAS – Dysfunctional Attitude Scale DASS-21 – Depression & Anxiety Stress Scale DBT – Dialectical Behavioural Therapy DSM-IV – Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition FD – Formerly Depressed FFMQ – Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire GP – General Practitioners ICS – Interacting Cognitive Subsystem MBCT – Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy MBSR – Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction mD – Mild Depression NA – Negative Affect ND – Never Depressed PD – Personality Disorder PT – Pharmacotherapy PTSD – Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder RSS – Rumination on Sadness Scale RST – Response Styles Theory SSRI – Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors TAU – Treatment as Usual WHO – World Health Organization YLD – Years Lost Due to Disability

Page 12: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

6

CHAPTER 1

Depression Rumination: Introduction & Theoretical Framework

1.01 Depression in Australia

Depression is one of the most prevalent of all psychiatric disorders and perhaps the most

costly. According to the Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS; 2011), around 800,000 Australian

adult sufferer from a depressive illness each year. It is also the third most common illness among

females and the tenth among males, with a 25% and 12.5% chance respectively of suffering from

depression sometime in their lives.

In 2001-02, depression made up for 32% of all mental health issues (SANE, 2005) while in

2008-09, depression accounted for 34.2% (AIHW, 2010). Almost half of the country’s children

displayed symptoms of depression, anxiety or other stress related illness (Anti-Depression

Association of Australia, 2010). The World Health Organization (WHO) has predicted that by 2020,

depression will be the second biggest health problem world-wide, surpassed only by heart disease

(Murray & Lopez, 1996), and is a substantial economic burden on society.

1.02 Economic Cost of Depression

In 2008-09, 21.4 million mental health-related prescriptions were subsidised under the

Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme and Repatriation Pharmaceutical Benefits Scheme, this costed the

Australian Government over $742 million. Of that, 41% were for antidepressants (AIHW, 2011).

Together with Medicare claims for psychotherapy among other services, the problem of depression

will equate to a total of $1 billion in expenditure of the Australian health system annually (Hare,

Driscoll & Stewart, 2009).

A study in the US found that indviduals suffering from depression on average lose almost 6

hours of productive time per week in contrast to just 1.5 hour for people who do not suffer from

depression (Stewart et al., 2003). In 2008, the WHO published their findings regarding the amount

of productive years lost due to disabilities (YLD, productive time that are lost, measured in years) -

both physical and mental. Amongst high-income countries, depression stood out as being the

disability that resulted in the most years lost with a total of 10 million years. This figure is more

than double that of Hearing Loss, the second most disabling affliction with just 4.2 million years.

The pattern is similar for low-income countries, as the leading cause for YLD is still depression

with 55.3 million years lost.

Page 13: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

7

Figure 2. Leading global causes of Years lost due to disability (YLD) in high-income countries in 2004 (WHO, 2008)

*COPD, Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease

In Australia alone, depression-related productivity loss accounts for up to 6 million working

days each year (Andrews et al., 1999) as well as another 12 million days of reduced performances.

Untreated depression can lead to substantial decrease in job performances and on average can cost

an organization almost $10,000 annually, totalling to $4.3 billion in lost productivity per year

(Hilton, 2004).

1.03 Trends of Depression in the Community

The problem of this issue comes into view when we look at it in the context of a trend

toward of increasingly younger onset age for major depression which has been found since the turn

of the twentieth century (Burke, Burke, Rae & Regier, 1991). Studies such as those of Alpert et al.,

(1999) and Fava et al., (1996) indicated that the age of first onset of depression leaned towards the

adolescent age-range. Their findings were also reproduced in Zisook et al. (2007), in the Sequenced

Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) trials. Zisook et al. showed that the mean

age for depression onset in the population for the pre-adult age range was as high as 37% with the

primary age being between the ages of 13 and 15. A more recent study by Williams et al. (2012)

again supports this pattern that more youths are showing signs of depression onset compared to

older people.

0

2

4

6

8

10

Page 14: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

8

Figure 3. Frequency distribution of age of onset of first episode of major depression.

(Williams et al., 2012)

Early onset of depression poses additional concerns regarding the prognosis of the disorder

as younger age of onset is known to be associated with a higher likelihood of suicidality (Van

Noorden et al., 2011) and a greater likelihood of relapse after being treated. In a study by

Thompsom (2008) individuals who experienced their first diagnosis of depression in childhood

were not only more likely have suicidal ideations, but was also found to have higher degrees of

suicidal intent when they develop such ideation.

1.04 Depression Relapse

Depression is a highly recurrent disorder. Weissman and Kasl (1976) found that 67% of

women who had experienced an episode of depression experienced a recurrence of depression.

Coryell et al (1995) found that individuals who have experienced untreated major depressive

disorder have on average 2.2 depressive episodes in their lifetime. This needs to be considered in

context of the fact that approximately half of the people who suffer from depression do not seek

treatment (Hilton, 2004). Even for those who do seek treatment, one of the primary problems with

depression (as with other chronic mental issues) is that when a patient is considered successfully

treated with acute treatment methods there is a significant likelihood that he/she will experience

Page 15: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

9 relapse and recurrence in the future (Judd, 1997). Studies show that for 25-40% of patients, relapse

occurs within can happen after just two years of being treated (Katz & Klerman, 1979) and up to

87% after fifteen years (Keller & Boland, 1998). Each episode increases the chances of another

episode occurring again in the future by 16% (William, Teasdale, Segal, & Kabat-Zinn, 2007). A

longitudinal study in Australia (Brodaty, Luscombe, Peisah, Anstey, & Andreas, 2001) showed that

84% of hospitalized patients who responded positively to treatment have had at least one relapse. In

a meta-analysis by Vittengl, Clark, Dunn, and Jarrett (2007) found that of the 13 studies of CBT

treatment targeting depression reviewed, on average 29% of individuals who were ‘successfully

treated’ experience relapse within one year and 54% relapsed within two years.

Many issues are raised with regard to the continuing prevalence of depression relapse.

Amongst them is the contention that the health care industry may finds itself stretched on resources

which are being spent inefficiently. As the long term benefit for the patient through conventional

treatment may not be long-lasting, patients are forced to continue to spend money, time and energy

seeking treatment for the same condition from which they were supposed to have been ‘successfully

treated’. Similarly, healthcare providers must also devote resources such as psychologists’ work

hours, medication, etc, to help these patients who relapsed. Preventing relapse would be a

significant saving of resources.

1.04.1 Conventional Treatment & Depression Relapse. Current treatments for depression include cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) and pharmacotherapy

(PT). There are a number of relapse prevention methods that are employed in CBT. For example,

patients are encouraged to separate their depressive thoughts into rational and irrational components

and challenge the irrational parts by pitting them against objective proof and evidence (Knaus,

2006). Others include the identification of an individual’s unique vulnerability and triggers that may

induce automatic depressive thoughts. Identifying these and learning specific techniques to

counteract the negative automatic thoughts helps peoples cope better with depression.

Prophylactic treatments such as the use of anti-depressants are often employed to prevent

relapses (American Psychiatric Association, 2008). However, research has indicated that this

approach has moderate success at best. Studies such as that of Hautzinger, de Jong-Meyer, Treiber,

Rudolf, and Thien (1995) show that even with continued medication, a relapse rate as high as 51%

can occur within one year. Goldberg and Kocsis (1996) found that 89% of patients who had shown

positive response to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) displayed re-emergence of

depressive symptoms despite continued treatment. In addition, up to 70% of patients do not wish to

Page 16: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

10 take antidepressants after acute treatment remission (Bockting et al., 2008) significantly increasing

their chances of relapse .

1.04.2 Cause of Relapse. Some theories have postulated that the variables

involved in triggering depression onset are different to those that predispose people to experience

relapse (Lewinsohn, Allen, Seeley, & Gotlib, 1999). While the onset of depression itself may have

been triggered by an external life event that was stressful in nature (Kendler, Thorton, & Gardner,

2001), whether a personis likely to become depressed and relapse after they have been “successfully

treated” has been found to be related to an internal variable known as ‘cognitive reactivity’

(Teasdale, 1988). William, Teasdale, Segal and Kabat-Zinn (2007) argued that the reason why

relapse occur so frequently is because depression has a unique way of forging “a connection in the

brain between sad mood and negative thoughts, so that even normal sadness can reawaken major

negative thoughts”. This in turn triggers more sad moods and even more negative thoughts. This

process is referred to as rumination.

1.05 Rumination

Numerous studies have found connections between rumination and various forms of

depressive symptoms (Wood, Saltzberg, Neale, Stone, & Rachmiel, 1990; Nolen-Hoeksema &

Morrow, 1991; Roberts, Gilboa, & Gotlib, 1998; Nolan, Roberts & Gotlib, 1998; Kuehner &

Weber, 1999; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). Many other studies also support this and indicate

rumination can prolong recovery (Schmaling, Dimidjian, Katon, & Sullivan, 2002), lengthen the

duration of depressive symptoms (Nolen-Hoeksem, Morrow & Fredrickson, 1993), as well as

increase the chance of new episodes (Spasojevic & Alloy, 2001). In fact, Spasojevic and Alloy even

go as far as making the claim that rumination is the most proximal cognitive vulnerability prior to

the emergence of clinically-significant levels of depression.

Over the years, there have been many definitions and ideas as to what ‘rumination’ means.

Some literature describes rumination as being a single static construct that embodies certain themes

while others describe rumination as an outcome of the interaction of different cognitive variables.

On the whole however, certain themes are shared among theorists. For example, according to the

Response Styles Theory (RST; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987) rumination is a process of self-reflection as

well as a repetitive attending to negative emotions. A model known as the Rumination on Sadness

conceptualization, Conway, Csank, Holm, & Blake (2000) defined rumination as repetitive thinking

about sadness, and circumstances related to an individual's sadness. Others such as Lyubomirsky

and Nolen-Hoeksema (1993) defined rumination as a method of coping with depressive mood that

Page 17: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

11 involves self-focused attention. Watkins (2004a) place a larger role of self-focus on his idea of

rumination. He understood rumination as one of two forms of self-focus. Specifically, it is a

maladaptive form labelled conceptual-evaluative while the other was a more adaptive form labelled

experiential self-focus. The key difference between the two is the motivation that drives the styles.

Although the definition of rumination does vary between researchers and their focus can be placed

on different areas, three recurring themes common to all are:

• Self-focus

• Attention demanding

• Negative mood-based or focused.

Because of the static nature of early conceptualization of rumination as a single construct,

these themes are understood here as facets of rumination rather than actual components within a

dynamic cognitive process.

1.05.1 Response Styles Theory. It has been proposed that there are two

ways people deal with depressive moods, through distraction or rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema,

1987). In situations where a person is chronically depressed, they are known to experience intrusive

negative thoughts and memories. When this happens, an individual may attempt to deal with the

intrusion by trying to distract themselves by engaging in nonrelated behaviours as a means to

distract themselves. Morrow and Nolen-Hoeksema (1990) proposes that another group of people

respond with a form of self-focused attention when they become depressed, namely rumination. By

focusing on their symptoms of depression and the causes and consequences of such symptoms,

ruminators are more likely to exacerbate transient depressive mood states leading to a longer

duration and more severe depressive episodes. In an early review by Nolen-Hoeksema (1991)

argued that engaging in rumination had a number of detrimental effects on the prognosis of the

depressed individual. Just and Alloy (1997) found that people who engage in rumination experience

more severe and prolonged depression while people who seek distractions have milder and shorter

episodes. Nolen-Hoeksema addressed the question of how rumination leads to depression. An early

theory was that rumination has an effect of interfering with a person’s ability to engage in

behaviours instrumental to their recovery. Those who are busy engaging in rumination will be less

likely to initiate actions that will result in positive reinforcements or an increased sense of control

such as recreation. The question therefore, is how this occurs. The Response Styles

Theory (RST), proposed by Morrow and Nolen-Hoeksema (1990), argued that ruminators

engage in a maladaptive cognitive style of thinking, such as a higher predisposition to recalling

Page 18: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

12 negative events or associating any given event to a negative outcome. People who suffer from

depression have problems engaging in instrumental behaviours as a result of depressive mood may

impeding their initiative to commit themselves to actions that could solve the problem that gave rise

to the depressive mood in the first place. At the same time, negatively cognition makes it likely that

an individual will believe that their actions will be futile. That is, rumination is a process that

involves depressive mood and negative cognition. The question therefore, is how these two factors

contribute to the ruminative process.

In support of her theory Nolen-Hoeksema (1991a) reported on a study investigating the

response style of people shortly after the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Measures of emotional

health as well as the response styles to negative moods of 137 students were taken 14 days before

the Loma Prieta earthquake hit. It was found that people who were normally predisposed to a

ruminative style of response to depressed mood, were more likely to remain depressed 10 days and

7 weeks after the earthquake than subjects with a less ruminative response style. They also showed

higher levels of trauma-related symptoms in the 10 days after the earthquake. Although the trigger

and emotional component of this study is the post-traumatic grief of the aftermath of the

earthquake, the detrimental effects of the individuals can be attributed to the fact that these people

employed a ruminative response style. It should be noted though, that in this specific circumstance,

where the earthquake was completely unpredicted, the aftermath of the disaster may have had an

effect on the results, primary of which was the fact that there are much greater incentive and

opportunity for people to engage in behaviours such as helping with the relief effort or offering

support for other that results in positive reinforcements. Indeed, it was remarked that almost

everyone was talking about the event in the days after the earthquake struck that even people who

were more likely to ruminate may have talked about their feelings that could have affected the

details of the results. These variables however, did not seem to have affected the overall trend of the

conclusion.

The majority of studies to this point were based on laboratory-induced depressive states. In

order to provide support for the effect of rumination on naturally occurring depression without

additional depressive ‘events’, Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow (1993) conducted a study in which

one group of depressed subjects were asked to focus their attention on their current feeling states

and personal characteristics for 8 minutes. Another group were asked to attend to descriptions of

geographical location and objects. Those in the rumination condition showed an increase in their

depressed mood while those who engaged in the distraction condition had a decrease in their

depressed mood to a point where it was equal to that of a nondepressed person. This study shows

Page 19: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

13 that even without induced sad mood, rumination had an exacerbating effect on the mood of

depressed people. Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow did however, remark that rumination itself is not

dysfunctional. It is only when the act of rumination interacts with depressive mood would it

produce maladaptive effects. Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow and Fredrickson (1993) conducted a study

that looked at the longevity of a person’s depression. In their study, seventy-nine participants were

asked to document their moods and responses to their moods for a period of one month. They found

that 83% of the participants employed a consistent form of response study (rumination or

distraction) throughout the tested period, and that those who used rumination reported more

prolonged depression. An interesting find of this study was that while initial mood of an individual

seemed to be related to whether or not they engaged in rumination, and how much they ruminated

had an independent effect on the duration of their moods.

While the detrimental effects of rumination of depression has been established in these

studies, they are often built on the assumption that rumination exists as an independent construct

with people varying in their predisposition to ruminate. The logical question that follows from this

is, why do people ruminate? More specifically, what may be the contributing factors to their

tendency to employ rumination as a coping mechanism? This question was addressed by Nolen-

Hoeksema, Parker and Judith Larson (1994) in which they conducted a longitudinal study on 253

people. They found that some of the common environmental variables that are related to depression

including lack of social support, concurrent stressors, and higher degree of post-loss depression

were related to higher levels of rumination. They also found that those who employed a ruminative

style of coping developed a greater level of pessimism one month after the death of a loved one.

More importantly, this pessimism resulted in higher levels of depression six months after the event.

Although rumination was correlated with all the noted variables, its effects were still felt even after

controlling for initial depression levels, social support, concurrent stressors, gender, and pessimism.

One of the more interesting findings of this study was the role of social support. Nolen-

Hoeksema’s line of research had, until this point, focused on cognitive responses to depressive

mood. Distractive behaviour, in her view, functioned to take an individual’s mind off their

depressive thoughts. Having a support network that has long been known to buffer the effects of

depression (Aneshensel & Stone, 1982). In the Nolen-Hoeksema studies, social support was only

related to depression through its relationship with rumination. It was theorized that people who have

lower social support were more likely to remain isolated and thus had more time to ruminate on

their problems. In addition, people who do not receive affirmation regarding their views and actions

also have more reason to ruminate. That is, a lack of social support leads to greater rumination. This

Page 20: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

14 study however, does suffer from a few methodological limitations. For example, it is possible that

the people who participated in this study were experiencing less grief than those who did not; as

such they had the capacity and mental space to participate. It is unknown therefore as how greater

level of sadness that is experienced by people who went through the loss of loved ones may affect

their tendency to ruminate. In addition, although all the variables were correlated to rumination, it is

impossible to tell whether they ‘caused’ an individual to ruminate or not. Rather the only conclusion

that can be drawn is that people with lower levels of social support, more concurrent stressors, and

higher degree of post-loss depression were more likely to ruminate. The relationship between

rumination and social support was further investigated by Nolen-Hoeksema and Davis (1999) which

yielded contradicting results. 349 people with terminally ill family members were interviewed

regarding their response style and social support network. It was found that those who employ a

ruminative coping style, were generally more focus on their own emotional reactions to a trauma

and would seek out more social support and are likely to benefit more from this support compared

to those without a ruminative coping style. It was reasoned that this was because ruminators were

more actively and persistently thinking about their loss, its meanings, and their own reactions to the

loss. More critically, compared to nonruminators, there is a desire to share these thoughts with

others. An important prerequisite of this effect is that those who are ruminators are integrated into a

social network so that they feel safe to share their feelings with people of that group. Support from

the social group allows ruminators to better cope with their grief in a more effective manner than

people employing distraction coping. This in turn decreases their distress levels in the 18 months of

losing their loved ones.

Overall, the results of Nolen-Hoeksema’s line of research are built on the foundation of

rumination as a static cognitive construct. When a person is depressed, they have a choice of three

possible methods of how to respond to their condition. They can either choose to engage in a self-

focused thought process referred to as rumination, seek distraction by engaging in other activities so

they do not have to think about the issue that made them depressed in the first place, or they can

seek out social support. Findings indicate that of the three possible responses, rumination leads to

worse outcome in terms of depression prognosis, longevity, severity, and symptomology. Because

Nolen-Hoeksema’s approach focuses on rumination as a single static construct, it does not address

the issues of why rumination is so enduring and why people are unable to stop ruminating in spite

of the fact that such thoughts rarely yield answers to their distress. In order to address this attention-

holding quality of rumination, it is necessary to analyse the construct and break it down to its

constitution parts. Through this analysis, the role of attention control can be brought to light and in

turn, the role of mindfulness can be addressed.

Page 21: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

15

1.05.2 Rumination & Memory Recall. Caldwell and Nolen-Hoeksema

(1999) investigated how dysphoric and nondysphoric students differ in their memory retrieval when

asked to engage in rumination or distraction tasks. Students who engaged in rumination recalled

more negatively biased autobiographical memories when asked for a general recollection of their

past. They reported that negative events happened more frequently in their lives compared to

positive events. This suggests that rumination may have an effect on how people recall. One

criticism of this study is that in one of their experiments, participants engaged in a rumination

process through talking out loud. It is questionable as to whether this method reflects the natural

occurrences of an individual when they are actually ruminating. As most internally processed

rumination are commonly less coherent, organized, or dependent on verbal expression so much.

The issue of distraction is essentially a matter of attention diverting from being self-focus

analysis to something else. A question that arises from the effects of distraction as a coping

mechanism therefore, is whether it is a matter of reducing in analytic thinking or reductions in self-

focus. Watkins and Teasdale (2001) addressed this question in their study in which 36 people were

given questions about their mood, depression levels, self-focus, analytical thinking, and

autobiographical memory. Results of this study found that focus of attention significantly affected

despondent mood. In addition, reduction in self-focus attention had the effect of reducing over-

general memory.

The findings of these studies indicate that rumination, especially when accompanied by

depression, has a significant effect on memory recollection. Depression-driven self-focused

attention somehow affects the cognitive processes that make negative information more accessible.

Given that these memories are made accessible by negative mood, it goes to an understanding with

regards to where the negative content of rumination comes from. The question is, how does this

happen. In order to address this issue, it is necessary to look into the process of rumination itself.

1.05.3 Rumination as a Process - Differential Activation

Hypothesis. One of the landmark models in the research of rumination and its mechanism is

the Differential Activation Hypothesis (DAH) proposed by Teasdale (1983). Unlike Nolen-

Hoeksema’s approach that treats rumination as a static construct that operates when an individual

engages in self-focused thoughts during episodes of depressive moods, Teasdale’s position treats

rumination as an outcome rather than a cognitive strategy.

Page 22: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

16

Teasdale’s theory is concerned with understanding the onset and persistence of depression.

Teasdale first looked into the context-specific encoding and retrieval of state-dependent learning.

Information that was learnt was more likely to be retrieved when the context in which the

information was learnt was replicated. What was extrapolated from this was that the memory, seen

as a network of associations, was linked to the emotions or mood that such memory represented.

The strength of this linkage affects how likely the presence of one activates the other. The presence

of a particular mood elicits specific memories. The implication of this finding was that learned

concepts created during a person’s depressed state (such as ‘worthless’, ‘failure’, etc) are activated

whenever the depressed mood is again present. Indeed, Teasdale (1988) later postulated that when

these negative thoughts are activated by depressed mood, they are often associated with previous

depressing experiences. Furthermore, he listed four characteristics of such negative thoughts:

1. Negatively biased thinking is characteristic of most depressions, even those with a strong

presumed biological component to their aetiology.

2. The content of negative thinking in depressed subjects can be quite widespread, rather than

localised on the focus of specific idiosyncratic schemas.

3. Negative thinking reduces as depression remits, even if the treatment for depression, such as

tricyclic antidepressant medication, does not specifically target the thinking.

4. However, most importantly, negative interpretations of experience may still play an

important role in maintaining depression, so that treatments that target these, such as

cognitive therapy (Beck et al., 1979), are potentially valuable in treating patients in whom

the vicious cycle that has been outlined plays an important part in the maintenance of their

depression.

Although the thoughts of vulnerable individuals may be within parameters of normality

when not experiencing depressive mood, the differences in dysfunctional rumination level between

recovered depressed subjects and controls will likely emerge when such people are subjected to

some form of stimulation that triggers a mild depressive mood state (Lau, Segal, & Williams,

2004). This ‘cognitive reactivity’ as termed by Teasdale, (1988) gave rise to the idea that negative

cognition can, to a certain degree, be both cause and effect of depression.

There is much evidence supporting the cognitive reactivity of DAH. Early studies such as

Teasdale and Dent (1987) looked at the two parts of the rumination cycle. The results showed that

when comparing never depressed (ND) people with formerly depressed (FD) ones, those within the

Page 23: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

17 FD group had higher levels of depression as an enduring characteristic; a higher level of

neuroticism; perceived their personality with greater levels of negativity; and lower levels of

activation when it comes to positive aspects of the self-schema. In addition, when a depressed mood

was induced, FD patients had better recall of self-referred, generalized negative words, indicating

that there was a greater tendency for FD people to activate negative self-schema. People who have

this profile are more vulnerable to becoming seriously depressed when the depressive mood is

triggered. Teasdale and Dent provided evidence that negative feelings are able to trigger negative

cognition processes that kick starts the rumination cycle.

Teasdale (1988) investigated whether the effect of depressive rumination on an individual’s

depression remains mild and temporary, or whether it develops into severe and persistent depressive

disorder. He found that the prognosis of depression can be predicted by whether a mutually

supporting cycle of rumination involving depressed mood and negative cognition can become

established during the onset. The likelihood of this happening is in turn dependent by a complex

interaction between environmental stressors, availability of social support, biological state, as well

as the type of cognitive processes and constructs that are elicited when the depressed state is active.

The form of cognitive processes that are employed during an individual’s depressed times are found

to function as the pattern of thought for the person in their baseline state as well as the processes

most likely to be triggered by depressive mood. So people who are high in neuroticism and have

generalized, global negative views of themselves are more likely to experience severe and

prolonged depression should acute depression be triggered by depressive feelings. This constituted

a “vulnerability to severe and persistent depression that is powerfully related to differences in

patterns of thinking that are activated in the depressed state” (Teasdale, 1988). Teasdale reasoned

that interpreting experience through a lens of negativity can contribute and enhance depressive

symptoms. In addition, the interpretation themselves are dependent on the way in which memory of

experiences are represented when they are elicited by depressed mood. Therefore rather than having

a set of depressive symptoms dictating the quality of experiences for individuals, the interpretation

of the experiences themselves as well as the way in which those experiences are remembered

change the depressive symptoms. The quality of a person’s experiences, be it directed externally

through sensory input or internally in the form of recollection and thought, can both influence the

course of the development of depression in a person. The consequences of this are that depressive

thoughts trigger more sad mood and in turn more negative thoughts. This association is the bond

that holds the depressed mood and negative thoughts together creating a mutually supportive cycle

that Teasdale referred to as rumination. Papageorgiou and Wells (2003) also described rumination

Page 24: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

18 as “cyclic negative thinking” in which depressed mood gives rise to negative thoughts that in turn

deepen the negative emotions experienced (see Figure 4).

Figure 4. Process of Rumination according to the DAH

1.05.4 Refinement of Negative Cognition. Nolen-Hoeksema (1991)

defined rumination as “behaviour and thoughts that focuses an individual’s attention on their

depressive symptoms and on the implication of these symptoms”. While rumination has been

described as a negative process. Rather than an objective analysis of a situation and the symptoms

the individual is experiencing, their reasoning, conclusions as well as the implications of these

conclusions are predisposed to a negative outcome. However, theoretically, even if rumination is

elicit by depressive mood, there is no reason why the outcome on simple reflection of the matter

cannot be positive (or at least objective). If the rumination process was more positive it could

potentially alleviate the depressive mood. It is theoretically possible to engage in a positive form of

rumination in which positive ideas and prospects regarding their situations are considered. This

leads to the question whether it is inevitable that depressive mood must lead to negative cognition?

There are some indications that an additional variable within this process separates reflective

pondering that has an adaptive recovery effect, and depressive rumination that can be detrimental to

mental health. This variable (that is also present among all depressed patients) is a dysfunctional

attitude (DA) that is in operation during rumination. A more comprehensive explanation of DA will

be discussed in the next section, however the general principle behind DA is the ‘tendency’ of

making negative interpretations of events that leads to specific negative thoughts. Fresco, Alloy and

Reilly-Harrington (2006) found that negative thought content was intricately connected with DAs in

depressed adults. A correlation between all three variables of rumination, DA and depression was

Page 25: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

19 found by Ciesla and Roberts (2007). Due to the fact that DA seems to be such an important part of

rumination, it will be discussed in detail below.

The Interacting Cognitive Subsystem

In a further development of the DAH Teasdale and Barnard (1993) proposed the Interacting

Cognitive Subsystem (ICS). The ICS is a unique approach to the issue regarding the relationship

between depressive mood and DA. Rather than being a simple process of depressive moods

activating negative constructs or making them more accessible, the ICS postulates that “negative

depressive thinking reflects a change, with depression, in more generic representations that encode

the interrelationships between constructs, or patterns of constructs” (Teasdale, 1995). Essentially,

the ICS model suggests that the mind operates with a schematic model of the relationship between

constructs that makes up our world. Every experience we have has its own generic features that in

turn can be related to patterns of activation across a number of individual constructs. When a person

becomes depressed, changes occur in the schematic mental models used to interpret experience,

most significant are those that are concerned with their relationship with the world. In addition, the

mental models that become salient when one is depressed are those that are more dependent of

social worth, approval and personal success (Teasdale, 1996). This theory is supported by research

projects including a study that compared depressed and nondepressed people in their sentence

completion ability using functional or dysfunctional words. It was found that depressed people

employ a model in which achievement is closely related to worth and approval. Therefore,

criticisms or failures are seen as 'catastrophic' in terms of general personal inadequacy. This was in

stark contrast to those who were not depressed for whom approval and achievement were less

strongly related to self-worth. In theory, depression can be addressed by engaging in acts that can

either garner approval from others or increase the chance of their success. However some have

argued that the thoughts that are directed towards the achievement of success may reinforce

rumination (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993). This is due to the fact that the efforts that are put in to the

analysis of the situation as well as formulation of plans to achieve success often involve extended

thoughts regarding irremediable personal inadequacies and deficiencies. Therefore, not only are

these thoughts unable to achieve the goal of finding ways to gain approval or achieve success, but

also it reinforces the discrepancies between current situations and the desired self-states. This

discrepancy in turn brings about greater need to think of ways to overcome their shortcomings

which regards more rumination. Hence, the process ultimately supports rather than reduces

depression.

Page 26: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

20

Miranda and Persons’ position (1988) suggested that negative thought content may lie

dormant or inactive unless activated by another process such as a general life stressor that triggers a

depressive mood. In support, Miranda and Person conducted a study that provided evidence that

the level of DA is actually mood-state dependent. According to Gemar, Segal, Sagrati, and Kennedy

(2001), compared to those who have never been depressed, those who have experienced depression

would experience a greater level of DA after a depressive mood has been induced. Studies have

supported this and demonstrated that there is a correlation between DA and induced sad mood

among FD patients but not for ND people. After inducing negative moods, various studies

(Miranda, & Persons, 1988; Miranda, Gross, Persons & Hahn, 1998) discovered that FD people

reported significantly higher level of DAs and depressive mood than ND individuals. As they

induced increasingly higher levels of depressive mood DA showed a corresponding increase.

However, similarly to previous concerns regarding general negative cognition, it could be argued

that even if there was a link between depressive mood and DA, the fact that the depressive moods in

these studies were artificially induced might have affected the results. To address this, Lewinsohn et

al. (1999) looked into the connection between naturally occurring depressed mood and DA levels

among either FD adolescent and an ND group. It was found that there was a significantly higher

correlation between depressive mood and DA level for those who had a history of depression than

those who did not. These results support the theoretical model behind DAH in which negative

constructs and processes are associated with increased risk for depression. However, they are only

activated and detectable when depressed mood are introduced into the cognitive system of the

individual.

Figure 5. Process of Rumination according to the ICS

It should be noted however, that evidence to the contrary has also been found by other

researchers. For example, a similar study by Dykman (1997) did not demonstrate any correlation

Page 27: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

21 between depressive mood and DA. However, this lack of result may be due to methodological

issues.

While the DAH considers negative cognition and depressed mood as a mutually reinforcing

cycle of rumination, the ICS further introduces the idea concept of DA in the process. This variable

of rumination holds significant importance to the overall function of rumination, but also answers

the question previously posed regarding how self-focused attention somehow affects the cognitive

processes that make negative information more accessible. In order to understand this relationship,

it is be necessary to explore the concept of DA in detail.

1.05.5 Dysfunctional Attitude. High levels of negative thoughts and DAs

are associated with the severity of depression (Dent & Teasdale, 1988) and the likelihood of onset

of depression. Alloy et al. (2006) found that individuals who had never suffered from depression yet

still possess high levels of DA were more likely to experience the onset of depression than a control

group. Elevated DA has been found to predict poorer response to treatment (Peselow, Robin, Block,

Barouche, & Fieve, 1990) as well as higher relapse rate (Thase et al., 1992).

Early cognitive models of depression emphasized cognitive vulnerability as a predisposition

of individuals to depression. DA is seen to play both a causal and maintenance role in depression

(Beck, 1967). According to Beck’s theory of depression, negative self-schemata revolving around

themes of inadequacy, failure, loss and worthlessness provide cognitive vulnerability to depressive

symptoms. These schemata manifest as self-worth contingencies such as “if I fail partly, it is as bad

as being a complete failure.” It is believed that when individuals experience negative life events,

they display these DAs via inference and developing a negatively biased analysis of the self, world

and future. In addition, self-schemas normally lie dormant within the mind of the individual until

activated by life events. Another theory of depression, the Hopelessness Theory (Abramson,

Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989), postulates that depressed people attribute negative life events to stable

and global causes. They infer negative consequences will result from negative events and that the

happening of such negative events implies that they are fundamentally flawed or worthless. In this

theory, those that suffers from depression are likely to make negative inferences regarding the

causes, consequences and self-implications of any stressful event, thereby increasing their chances

of developing a sense of hopelessness and in turn, depression.

Although the two theories have their differences, both overlap in that they understand

depression to be a result of cognitive vulnerability of processing or appraisal of life events. It is

theorized that the presence of characteristic DA increases vulnerability to the onset and

Page 28: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

22 maintenance of depression. For example, Beck (1967) believes that individuals that hold an attitude

related to the need to be perfect and gain other’s approval increases the risk of depression. Theorists

such as Teasdale (1988) claims that depressed individuals have a common characteristic that of

inherent bias in virtually all aspects of information processing including perception, attention and

memory. Williams, Mathews and McLeod (1996) showed that compared to non-depressed people,

depressed individuals attend to and recall more negative stimuli than positive ones. As noted

previously, it can be understood therefore that DA is an individual’s tendency to make negative,

self-related associations that leads to a depressive mood.

However, there has been a long-standing debate as to whether DA is a symptom of

depression or whether it is just a mental vulnerability that predisposes people to having the disorder.

This debate is fuelled by the fact that findings regarding the relationship between negative cognition

and depression have yielded mixed results (Lara, Klein & Kash, 2000). While previous findings

show that a DA certainly makes one more vulnerable to the onset of depression, it has also been

shown that a diagnosis of depression is characterized as having a naturally higher level of DA

(Zimmerman, Coryell, Corethal, & Wilson, 1986), so it is uncertain as whether DA is the cause or

merely the symptom of depression. In order to address this theoretical issue, studies using remission

designs have been conducted. The argument has been that if a DA is not a symptom of depression,

meaning its presence is not dependent on episodes of depression, then it should be stable beyond the

episodes. A review of these studies found only limited evidence that negative cognition remains

stable (Just, Abramson & Alloy, 2001). For example, early studies such as those of Silverman,

Silverman and Eardley (1984) as well as Simons, Garfield and Murphy (1984) failed to show clear

differences in DA score between recovered depressed subjects (who are assumed to have increased

risk of depression) and controlled.

The inconsistencies in the research are explained by the DAH. According to the DAH DA

varies as a function of depressed mood. When a person is not depressed DA is not triggered by

negative mood, however increased DA predisposes an individual to depressive disorders. In

analysing each component of rumination, the question of how DAH accounts for the attention

holding property of rumination as well as the method in which mindfulness plays within this

process is brought to the forefront. In order to understand the way in which mindfulness could

operate in rumination, the significance of DAH in terms of its therapeutic application should be

outlined.

Page 29: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

23

1.05.6 Implications of DAH on Conventional Treatment

Outcomes. The DAH can help explain why treating depression using anti-depressants is often

unsuccessful in preventing relapse. Anti-depressants leads to biochemical changes that affect the

mood of the individual by influencing neurotransmitters that bridges communication between brain

cells. This merely has the effect of removing the negative affect component of the ruminative

process. While this treats the ‘trigger’ of depression rumination, DA remains unchanged. That is,

while anti-depressant removes the trigger of rumination, the other components of the rumination

process, including DA and its themes such as inadequacy, failure, loss and worthlessness are all still

intact. The negative thought contents that are generated through the DA are, as Miranda and

Persons (1988) remarked, lying dormant within the mind of the individual. Should the trigger of sad

mood and emotions be reintroduced into the cognitive system, the DA-influenced thought content

that is associated to the sad mood will be once again be prompted to surface to the conscious mind

of the individual and thus the process of rumination restarts.

CBT aims to decrease the level of DA, teaching a specific skill set to depressed patients that

allow them to notice, identify and more importantly, respond to emotional triggers (Linehan, 1993).

In addition, strong emphasis is placed within CBT on cognitive restructuring. This is the process in

which the therapist work together with the patient in order to change certain thinking patterns,

themes as well as and behavioural activations. It is perhaps this additional effort of changing the

ways in which one thinks that, at least within these studies, CBT has a relatively higher success in

alleviating the DA of depressed patients.

Theoretically DA is not completely separated from depression nor is it a subcomponent of

depression. Rather, the presence of depression leads to DA while being supported by it in turn.

Hence rather than a simple either-or question in regard to the nature of the relationship between

depressive mood and DA, the question is how the two influence each other. The clinical implication

of the relationship as outlined by the DAH and ICS is that the disruption of rumination is the key to

recovery, taking the fuel away from the cause of depression. Indeed, the process of cognitive

therapy aims to modify negative cognition with more adaptive ones. Teaching distraction

techniques to patients also help to interfere with the rumination process. Both cases allow for the

breaking of the cycle and taking the patient down the path of recovery.

1.05.7 Unifying the Differential Activation Hypothesis and

Response Style Theory. In 2007 Ciesla and Roberts combined the DAH and the RST

into a single model. Reiterating the position of Nolen-Hoeksema, Ciesla and Roberts (2007) stated

Page 30: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

24 that rumination was not the content of cognition but a process - it was about ‘how’ one thinks.

Perhaps more importantly, although studies to this point were based in the DAH and the theory of

rumination operating as a cycle with depressive mood and negative cognition being mutually

supporting, no study thus far had found direct evidence to support this. Evidence has, at best, found

that they are correlated with each other, but it is only theorized that they support each other in this

fashion. Although the theories developed is sound, the mutual-causation relationship is still not

empirically established. In a series of three studies, Ciesla and Roberts found that within a sample

of people displaying high levels of negative cognition, rumination was more likely to be the

preferred response style. The study also found that following a sad mood induction, the interaction

between rumination (as a response style) and negative cognition predicted change in depressed

mood. While each variable has it’s own maladaptive effect on depressed mood this study showed

that there was a significant interactive effect between the two which results in their amplification. It

is noted here that if one has high self-esteem, ruminative tendency not affect their functional

capacity. It is the position of Ciesla and Roberts (2007) that DA and rumination must operate

together in order for depressive disorders to take place. The lack of one element may still lead to a

relatively adaptive lifestyle. They explain this dual process in the following:

“…the presence of either one of these vulnerabilities on its own is a risk factor for

dysphoric affect, although this risk may be amplified under circumstances in which the

other vulnerability is elicited. For example, a highly ruminative person who has high self-

esteem may function well in general. However, the moderation model suggests that this

individual may become depressed in the face of personally invalidating experiences such

as a job loss, academic failure, or romantic rejection that threatens self-esteem. Similarly,

a relatively nonreflective individual who rarely engages in rumination but who has high

levels of DAs may function well in general. However, this individual may be at risk for

depression under circumstances in which active or distractive coping is less effective,

such as following the death of a loved one or the diagnosis of a serious disease.”

(Ciesla & Roberts, 2007)

This study gives support to the additional vulnerability one faces when rumination and

negative cognition interact with each other and in turn amplifies their effects. This position is

reflected in Robinson and Alloy (1998) in which they showed that DA provides negative thoughts

and inferences but these will be more likely to be depressogenic if the person engages in

rumination. Rumination itself is therefore a nonspecific vulnerability that can fuel various types of

Page 31: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

25 negative mental content. As such, if a person is shy and ruminates on it, it can lead to anxiety,

ruminating on their thrill in gambling can lead to gambling addiction, etc.

Although the interaction between depressive mood, DA and rumination may lead to

depression, some have suggested that in order for our understanding of rumination to be more

holistic, the content of rumination, or the negative life events that one experiences, needs to be

included. The inclusion of this variable was the foundation for the formulation of the Cognitive

Catalysts Model. In a recent study, Ciesla, Felton and Roberts (2012) built on their previous work

by expanding on the interactive effect of rumination with and negative affectivity by adding

negative events to the model. In their study, they looked at different forms of rumination including

reflective pondering and the negative mood-driven brooding and how they interacted with life

events. Their study yielded a number of important findings. Firstly, the effect of negative life

events had most effect in individuals who were either low in self-esteem and high in brooding

tendency, or displayed greater levels of negative attribution style while being high in reflective

pondering. This finding was significant as it implies that dysphoric people, who normally have both

low self-esteem and DA, are more vulnerable to the effects of rumination as it increases their

sensitivity to actual life events. The results supported previous findings that the interaction between

negative feelings, rumination and actual negative life events is a robust effect, and could be

replicated in laboratory environment (Ciesla & Roberts, 2007), has long term detrimental effects

(Robinson & Alloy, 2003) and has a significant effect on the prognosis of an individual’s

depression treatment (Ciesla & Roberts, 2002). Secondly both reflective pondering and brooding

were found to have an interactive effect with stress and negative contents. However unexpectedly,

reflective pondering was not correlated with the level of depression, although it did result in higher

levels of depression over time due to its interaction with stress and negative contents. A second

interesting finding of this study was that both reflective pondering and brooding were found to have

significant interactive effects with life stress and negative contents. What was unexpected however,

was that reflective pondering and not just brooding, was predictive of increase level of depression

over a six week period. Although reflective pondering did not correlation with depression, it did

show an interaction with stress and DA. One possible reason as to why reflective pondering did not

have an effect on depression, is because those who practice reflective pondering lacked negative life

events to dwell on, meaning that their thoughts will not have anything to feed into rumination. This

study resulted in the development of what the authors call, the cognitive catalyst model of

depression. This model looked not only at the fact that depression mood, rumination and DA

interacted to predict depression prognosis, but also that there needs to be some kind of content

(negative life event) for one to ruminate on in order for such effect to take place.

Page 32: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

26 1.06 The role of Attention Control Capacity and Emotions in the rumination process

The present research extends the research summarised above by suggesting that a person’s

capacity for attentional control is an important component of the rumination process. As will be

discussed further in later sections, emotions and an individual’s capacity to control their attention

are intricately linked and therefore the role of attention control on rumination is a logical next step

to the current research.

Negative emotions are one of the most basic symptoms of depression. As outlined

previously, sadness and depressive moods have been intricately linked to rumination, either as the

the cause or the outcome of the process. It is therefore important to briefly discuss some of the basic

symptoms negative emotions have on an individual before elaborating on its connection with their

attention control capacity.

1.06.1 Depression & Emotions. Perhaps the most

important aspect of emotions within a depressed individual is

the fact that they have a symptomatic higher level of emotion

reactivity when exposed to emotionally charged stimuli. Since

the presence of emotions may affect an individual’s attention

control ability (as discussed below), it is therefore logical to

believe that higher levels of emotions can lead to a lowered

level of attention control.

One of the key features of depression is the day-to-day experience of sad mood. Certain

moods predispose the rise of related forms of emotion (Rosenberg, 1998), that investigates the how

depression influences emotional functioning. Early studies have produced evidence indicating that

depression of various forms may have predisposed people to have depressive emotional reactions

when stimuli that are negatively emotionally charged are presented (Golin, Hartman, Klatt, Munz,

& Wolfgang, 1977). The very fact that one is depressed means that their ability to regulate their

own emotional reaction to incoming stimuli has been mildly compromised. In other words, a minor,

negativity-oriented form of emotional dysregulation may be happening within the inner workings of

a depressed patient’s mind.

1.06.2 Emotion and Attention. A person’s emotional reactions are of

importance to understanding the rumination process. An issue arises is why rumination persists and

Page 33: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

27 continues in spite of the sadness it brings? Although we can understand that a negative thought can

lead to a negative feeling and in turn lead to another negative thought, why is it that depressed

people often find themselves unable to disengage from process? Given the heightened emotional

reactivity of an individual when they are depressed, the relationship between emotions and attention

is the critical contribution of this study. One possibility of why ruminator are unable to disengage

their thinking process is because this connection between mood and attention has caused the

individual’s capacity to control their attention to be impeded.

Emotional reactions are believed to be the key signal determining whether a piece of

information is important and deserving priority for attention allocation (Compton, 2003). Moray

(1959) observed that the mentioning of a person’s name will often command more attention even in

the mist of multiple background distractions and this was understood as the early evidence of

emotion’s effect on attention. The name-stimulus is a self-relevant construct that demands a

significant level of emotional arousal (Compton, 2003).

There are two primary lines of research that have produced evidence for the connection

between emotion and attention, one using neuroimaging while the other employs behavioural

science. Neuroimaging technology has given us some of the most convincing evidence of the

connection between emotions and attention. It has been found that the two prefrontal regions, the

orbitofrontal cortex and the anterior cingulated cortex are activated when performing tasks that

demand our attention or emotional evaluation. Other structures such as the lateral prefrontal cortex

amygdale, thalamus, etc. are intricately and reciprocally linked to both these regions, and each also

perform attention and emotionally-related functions (Vuilleumier, Armony & Dolan, 2003). Other

studies show that the visual association cortex (VAC) show a greater level of activation when

presented with images of emotionally positive or negative nature compared to emotionally neutral

images (Lane, Chua & Dolan 1999). These findings indicate that the regions of the brain that

operate with emotion and attention have a reciprocal relationship with each other and are often

activated together. In light of this we can understand how the cognitive functioning of mind often

points towards the phenomena of attention being drawn towards that is emotionally simulating.

In terms of behavioural sciences, modern-day research has established two related findings

that support this connection of emotion and attention. Firstly, emotionally salient stimuli can affect

the allocation of attentions. There have been a number of studies, including that of Fenske and

Eastwood (2003), that show emotional stimuli such as angry faces attract and retain attention to a

greater degree than neutral ones. Secondly, selective attention can be altered by affective states.

This means that depending on the emotional state that one is in, the kind of information that is

Page 34: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

28 deemed to be important can be different. For example, Mathews and Macleod (1994) found

evidence to support the idea that anxious and depressed individuals selectively attend to negative

information while Bradley (1998) found evidence to show that people with anxiety showed

selective attention towards threatening faces.

Most studies employ a form of emotionally arousing visual stimuli and various forms of

cognitive tasks, including image selection, complex information processing, amongst others. Most

popular among these is the probe dot task (MacLeod, Mathews & Tata, 1986) and the Stroop task

(Stroop, 1935). In the probe dot task, two word stimuli are presented in different location on a

screen. After the display is terminated, a neutral probe appears in the former location of one of the

words. Participant’s responses to the probe are timed and used to infer the allocation of attention,

and it is believed that faster responses will indicate a greater level of attention paid to it as opposed

to slower ones. Early studies show that a person’s ability to shift attention towards the neutral probe

is indeed slower if one of the word-stimuli is emotionally charged (Psoner, Snyder & Davidson,

1980). This indicates that attention stays with the emotion longer when compared to neutral stimuli.

The Stroop task, on the other hand, presents a single stimulus with two dimensions (word meaning

and ink colour); attention allocation bias is deduced through the lower speed of deciphering the

colour of depressive modal words rather than neutral words. While it is natural for one to have a

generally lowered speed in colour identification compared to the identifying of the word, when

controlled for this difference, emotionally charged words have been shown to be related to a slower

identification time of the colour compared to emotionally neutral words. This indicates that the

emotions aroused from the word holds attention for a split second longer than words that have little

emotional response.

1.07 Attention Control

Attention control (AC), the ability to direct an individual’s attention, comes in two forms

that are considered related but still distinguishable processes (Posner, Walker, Friedrich & Rafal,

1984). Attention focusing is the process of holding attention to a particular content and attention

shifting is the ability to shift attention towards another object, idea or sensory stimuli. In both

focusing and shifting of attention, there can also be two forms in which they are executed, external

controlled attention and internal controlled attention (Gross & Thompson, 2007). External

controlled attention can be understood as the directing of attention to various sensory inputs, such as

visual or auditory stimuli. Internal controlled attention, on the other hand, is the directing of

attention towards thought content, memories or imagination. High levels of internal controlled

attention seem to be related to the ability to inhibit worrisome thoughts and preventing high anxiety

Page 35: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

29 (Johnson, 2009). Studies support this and have found that certain groups of people exhibit a

deficient ability to direct attention away from emotional thought content and imagery, including

those who have elevated trait anxiety, neuroticism, depressive rumination, and patients of clinical

anxiety (Davis & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Robinson, Wilkowski, Kirkeby & Meier, 2006; Sarason,

1986; Sibrava & Borkovec, 2006; Watkins & Mason, 2002).

Internal controlled attention, measured by an individual’s attention switching or attention

inhibiting ability, may be linked to emotion regulation ability. The relationship between rumination,

emotionally stimulating situations and AC is an interesting one. According to a finding from

Wilkinson and Goodyer (2006), there was no difference in attention switching between the

depressed and the controlled groups. However, in this particular experiment, the Test for Every day

Attention for Children (TEA-CH) the task was performed by the participants was emotionally

neutral. As such, it is understandable that this may not have triggered the ruminative cycle, which is

often triggered by depressive mood. Indeed, it was speculated by Wilkinson and Goodyer (2006)

that the outcome may have been different if ruminators were assigned an emotionally arousing task.

Perhaps unsurprisingly, attentional biases are strongest when the stimuli have personal

importance (Mathews & Klug, 1993). An example of this would be Dalgleish (1995) where expert

bird watchers displayed selective attention towards bird-related words. It is understandable how the

closer to personal experience and importance stimuli has, the more attention it is given. While bird-

related information may be related as a topic of intellectual interest to a group, we can expect the

same phenomena if the stimuli has an emotional content. This relationship is demonstrated by

Schoth and Liossi (2010) where stimuli related to pain was given more attention than neutral stimuli

in a subject group that is suffering from to chronic headache. Thus, the phenomena can be

understood as preoccupation in certain forms of concerns, such as bird watchers with bird-related

information or self-value with those suffering from depression. Such predisposes one to certain

forms of attentional biases. In terms of rumination-related studies, using the Implicit Associate Test

(IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), a study by Gemar, Segal, Sagrati, & Kennedy

(2001) demonstrated that after a depressive mood induction, FD people exhibit greater level of

negative evaluative bias regarding self-referencing information compared to ND. Perhaps more

importantly was that the degree of this attention bias is similar to those who are currently depressed.

This implies that the level of DA that is lying dormant just underneath conscious level of a person

who was previously depression may be just as potent as of one who is experiencing depression and

that it only requires a small amount of sadness for it to be brought to the surface.

Page 36: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

30

1.07.1 Attention Control & Rumination. There has been a history of

research into the relationship AC with rumination. For example, McFarland and Buehler (1998)

conducted a series of five studies that looked at the effects of mood induction on memory retrieval.

The study started out with the assumption that those who engage in self-focused attention to their

emotional state are more likely to have both mood-congruent recall and mood-incongruent recall.

Specifically, which type of recall would be employ was dependent on the manner in which people

focus on their moods. Their study found that when asked to focus on their depressive moods,

dysphoric participants were less likely to use positive memories to overcome their negative moods

compared to those who distracted from self-focus. This study gives grounds to the idea that the

mood in which one focuses on directly affect the level of attention given to the type of memories

that they have access to. Others such as Joormann and Seimer (2004) found that there was a

measurable time difference between dysphoric and nondysphoric people in terms of their recall time

for positive autobiographical memories during rumination as a means to cope with their depressive

mood state. This study implies that there was some kind of attention inhibition in terms of positive

memories retrieval. As noted previously, a study by Lyubomirsky, Caldwell and Nolen-Hoeksema

(1999) found that ruminator has both a recall bias to their negative memories as well as a perception

bias to the frequency of negative events in their lives.

Given the relationship between emotions and attention, this connection between attention

control and memory retrieval, is also supported by years of research regarding the association

between mood and memory. Whether it is explicit memory recall (Derry & Kuiper, 1981), memory

load (Barge & Tota, 1988) and semantic priming (Segal, Gemar, Truchonm Guirguis, & Horowitz,

1995), evidence indicate emotions exert a significant influence on the operation of different

memory-related cognitive processing. This phenomenon can either be mood-congruent (Teasdale,

1983) or mood-dependent (Eich, 1995). Mood-congruent memory is the biased encoding or

retrieval of negative information. People encode or retrieve information based on an emotional state

that matches the tone of the information itself. This concept is not unlike certain aspects of DA as

the things that are recalled are biased towards negative contents. Mood-dependency on the other

hand implies an enhanced likelihood of retrieving information that is encoded in similar mood-

states. A person’s current emotional state increases the likelihood they will retrieve memories that

were learnt during the previous occasion when one was in a similar mood state. The two forms of

memory retrieval are important to cognitive processing, especially with regard to rumination as it

influences ‘when’ something is recalled as well as ‘what’ is being recalled.

Page 37: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

31

This is consistent with Teasdale’s argument that memories recalled are generally associated

with the mood present at the time they were formed. The influence of depressed mood on the

selective processing of negative information is believed to further the negative interpretations of

external events by influencing the information that one is focusing on, as well as how such

information is being categorized (Gotlib & MacLeod, 1997). These in turn further increase negative

moods and thus accelerate the cognitive cycle. Depressive mood are known to have the effect of

increasing the likelihood of one recalling selectively the sad memories from their lives (Bower,

1981; Clark & Teasdale, 1982; Teasdale & Fogarty, 1979); recall misconceived negative

evaluations of otherwise neutral events (Natale & Hantas, 1982); beliefs that their actions within a

social environment to be more negative than it is (Forgas, Bower, & Krantz, 1984); experiencing a

generally lower level of life satisfaction (Schwarz & Clore, 1988); develop a more negative

perception of other people (Fbrgas & Bower, 1987); as well as less likely to hold optimistic illusion

of control (Alloy, Abramson, & Viscusi, 1981).

In 2000, Davis and Nolen-Hoeksema, addressed the question of the commonly observed

cognitive inflexibility of ruminators. In this study rumination levels of 62 participants were

measured as well as various cognitive performance tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

(WCST) that measures cognitive flexibility. It was found that those who employ rumination coping

mechanisms committed significantly more perseverative errors as well as maintained sets

significantly less often than nonruminators. In addition, ruminators were found to be significantly

more inflexibilities than nonruminators even when controlling for general intelligence or depressed

mood. This study postulated two possible explanations for this relationship. First, the tendency to

ruminate during times of sadness may be due to an individual’s cognitive inflexibility and

perseveration. Essentially, people who are unable to inhibit their perseverative tendencies while at

the same time failing to maintain productive lines of reasoning may become trapped in

dysfunctional perseveration on the sadness and events that occupy them. Another possible

interpretation is that preoccupation with an individual’s sadness and conditions in their lives may

sap their confidence in their judgments. This may result in continuing to ruminate and losing the

ability to think flexibly. Whatever the connection is, the cognitive inflexibility as being an integral

part of rumination is of special interest to this study and its importance will be discussed further in a

later section.

Another issue that is of interest regarding rumination is how the self-directness of a person’s

thought can affect the detrimental outcomes of rumination. As noted previously, rumination

includes three key components and one of them is self-focused. The relationship between self-

Page 38: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

32 directed attention and negative cognition was investigated by Ciesla and Roberts (2001). In their

study, 32 patients who were receiving group psychoeducational treatment for depression were given

a series of pre-treatment measure of depression levels, response style, DA, self-consciousness, self-

esteem, etc. It was found that self-directed attention was not significantly correlated with a person’s

negative cognition. However, there was an interaction effect between self-directed thought

(rumination) and DA predicted change of severity regarding the depressive symptoms during the

course of the treatment. Indeed, in spite of the fact that self-consciousness and rumination are both

potentially destructive processes, the effects that they have on treatment outcome are moderated by

the individual’s cognitive style. This confirms previous findings that only through the combination

of DA and rumination that allows these attitudes have effect do the maladaptive effects of

rumination come about. There were also a few interesting finds in this study. Firstly, low self-

directed thought and low DAs was found to be related to lower levels of treatment responses. It is

believed that due to the fact that the particular treatment employed in this study was CBT in nature

and was focuses on changing these variables, it was targeting key features of these individuals’

depressive disorders. In addition those with high levels of self-directed attention combined with a

positive cognitive style showed better treatment responses. This again points to the notion that self-

directed attention is in itself not something that is detrimental to an individual’s depression

prognosis. If one thinks of himself often but has a positive outlook in life, this ruminative process is

actually beneficial to the transitory depressive feeling the one may be experiencing. Indeed, the

capacity to monitor their own thoughts and feelings is an important skill set in CBT.

1.07 Conclusion

Depression is sustained and worsen due to the fact that some people ruminate when they

experience an episode of depressive mood. This response style also predispose one to relapse even

if they have been successfully treated by conventional treatment practices. Rumination is known to

be composed of at least three different components, depressive mood, dysfunctional attitude, and

negative life events. Through cognitive reactivity, these three components are held together in a

mental cycle in which each reinforces each other and locks the individual inside a process known as

rumination. It is also known that a person’s emotional state has an intricate effect on their AC

capacity. It is therefore logical to believe that in some way, an individual’s AC capacity is somehow

intimately linked to the rumination cycle.

Page 39: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

33

CHAPTER 2

Mindfulness and Mindfulness-Based Interventions

2.0 Introduction

Mindfulness is a form of meditative technique that originated in Indian spiritual practices

and has a history for thousands of years. In the past thirty years, there has been a surge of interest

within western psychology and health science in adopting this new method into our modern secular

intervention. Attention Control is one of the core aspects of mindfulness, both as a focus of practice,

and as a part of this phenomenon. Its function within its native philosophical model; transition from

eastern spiritual practice to western intervention technique; clinical results and benefits it has

brought as well as the future of this field shall be explored in this chapter. Special focus would be

looking at how it is related to emotional regulation and attention control.

2.01 Mindfulness and the Buddha’s Diagnosis of the Human Condition

Before looking into mindfulness within the modern, psychological paradigm, it is important

to understand the context from which it was developed. Although there have been mindfulness

practices throughout most of Asia before the rise of Buddhism (Sujato, 2006), it is how it

functioned within Buddhism that is relevant to this study. To understand this issue, the specific

model of the mind that is employed by early Buddhists, as well as the role in which mindfulness

plays within this model needs to be explored.

Over two and a half thousand years ago, a man named Siddhattha Gotama from India sat

under a bodhi tree and began a session of deep meditation. Through this meditation on reality and

the nature of all living things, he found the final answer to the ultimate question of life itself. When

he came out of his meditative state, he was no longer the Siddhattha who went in, for this new man

has overcame his greed, aversion, and delusions of all things, found liberation and enlightenment,

he emerged as the Buddha, the Awakened One.

For the next half a century, he preached and spread his teaching of his discovery to all who was

willing listen. His message was a simple one, yet its meaning was, and still is vastly complex. His

taught the simple lesson of Suffering and Impermanence. Elaborating on this discourse is the

fundamental ‘structured’ lesson, the Four Noble Truths. Found in the very first sermon of the

Buddha after his enlightenment, the Four Noble Truths consists of the following:

Page 40: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

34

1. All existence (including all sentient life) is constituted by composite entities that are

impermanent and subject to dissolution. Further, this impermanence and dissolution is part

and parcel of a virtually endless process of birth, death, and rebirth. This process is called

samsara, and dissatisfaction and Suffering are intrinsic to it.

2. Desire is the primary driving force of samsara and the dissatisfying experiences that are

embedded in it. Desire—above all the desire for self-preservation and self-existence—

engenders mental and physical activities that lead to karmic retribution. The law of karma

ensures that all deeds produce appropriate fruits that condition one’s present life, one’s

future lives, and/or both.

3. Release from samsara and the desire that fuels it can be achieved. Buddhists have come to

designate this state of release as nirvana.

4. The method for achieving nirvana is practicing the noble eightfold path.

(Reynolds & Carbine, 2000)

2.01.2 Five Aggregates (Skandhas). As Dukkha is explained as the

core of Buddhist’s teaching, the following section shall explain this concept with regard to one of

the oldest theory of the mind - the Five Skandhas. Also known as the Five Aggregates of Grasping,

the Five Skandhas is a model in which explains the overall mental activity in its entirety and is also

considered as the mental dynamics in which allows Dukkha to be persist in the world. The problem

with the grasping of the five aggregates is that according to the Doctrine of Dependent Origination,

these are all subjected to constant change. None of these are permanent or certain. As such, the

‘self’ notion from which the grasping of the five aggregates produced itself is impermanent. Any

attempts to gain happiness by satisfying this self is in itself a doomed effort from the beginning. It is

through this understanding that the Buddha realized the primary mechanism from which suffering

arise is the illusion of the self-centered desire (Smith, 1991).

The Five Aggregates model is a more detailed account of Dukkha based on the excessive

investment in sensory perception, the experience of aversive or addictive reactions to these

perceptions, and the resultant cognitive and behavioural patterns that shape our interactions with the

world (Kelly, 2008). The five Skandhas includes Rupa (Form), Vedanna (Sensation), Samjna

(Perception), Samskara (Mental Formation) and Vijnana (Consciousness).

2.01.3 Mindfulness of the Eightfold Path. The Noble Eightfold Path

was the Buddha’s answer to the condition of human suffering. It is a way of life on both a mental

Page 41: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

35 and behavioural level in which one can be alleviated from suffering, so as to live a life free from

miseries and calamities. This way of life is designed to be practiced simultaneously and not

separated as this is a single path with eight aspects and not eight individual paths. Effectively, it is a

guideline in which guides one towards Nirvana. The eight aspect of this path includes:

1. Right view

2. Right intention

3. Right speech

4. Right action

5. Right livelihood

6. Right effort

7. Right mindfulness

8. Right concentration

These eight can be categorized under three primary groups including Panna./Wisdom (1-2),

Sila/Morality (3-5) and Samadhi/Mental Culture (6-8) (Dhammananda, 1993). Within Samadhi, the

element of right mindfulness takes a fundamental role. According to Thanissaro (1996), the Buddha

remarked that mindfulness practices can allow an individual’s training to go through three stages.

These include the focusing in the present moment, noticing how the object changes and the factors

related to the change, as well as having bare attention to the object, without clinging or grasping and

with equanimity. It is therefore the final goal of mindfulness practice, from a Buddhist perspective,

to achieve the relinquishment of a person’s grasping.

2.01.4 Satipatthana Sutta – Guide to Right Mindfulness. The term ‘Mindfulness’ comes from the Pali word ‘Sati.’ There is no direction translation of this

word in English as it is a form of active mental awareness. It involves connotations such as

awareness, attention, as well as remembering. It is defined by Nyaniponika (1973) as being a mode

of consciousness that emphasizes the presence of the mind. There are many aspects of mindfulness

as well as many different levels one can attain. What is important to the present research is that it all

revolves around the issue of mental concentration and how one can bring the mind to a single point

of awareness (attentional control).

Teachings of mindfulness are based primarily on the Satipatthana Sutta of the Majjhima

Nikaya (MN 10). This text documents the four foundations from which mindfulness practice can be

built on. Not coincidently, these ‘pillars’ of mindfulness, including the body, feeling, consciousness

and mental object, correspond to the aggregates of clinging. Meditation of these four aspects of

Page 42: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

36 experience increases capacity to let go of an individual’s attachment and clinging. With regard to

the five aggregates, mindfulness is, from the Buddhists’ perspective, a way to create a space

between the aggregates themselves and the clinging to them. It allows one to see an "aggregate as

an aggregate." The aggregate is not seen to have an independent existence, or a part of their own

identity, but rather is something that is temporary subjected to rising and dissipating. In the process

of one slowly become non-attached to the five aggregates, the concept of a ‘self’ begins to fade.

Mindfulness of the Body. The first foundation for which mindfulness is guided to be based

on, in accordance to the Satipatthana Sutta, is the body. The first level talks about the material

world. This corresponds to the aggregate of matter and has multiple meanings and application in

accordance to the abhidharma tradition. Among these includes the first four of the sixteen stages of

mindfulness of breathing, mindfulness and clear comprehension in the four postures, all physical

activities, the thirty-one parts of the individual’s physical form, the four elements of fire, water,

earth and wind, and lastly, the reflections on repulsiveness (Shaw, 2006). While a complete

explanation of the depth of this level here is beyond the scope of this study, special attention for

mindfulness of breathing as well as physical activity and form may be warranted. Mindfulness of

breathing takes a central role in mindfulness meditation. As remarked by Nyanapoika, (1962),

“Mindfulness of breathing takes first place among the various subjects of meditation. To all

Buddhas, Pacceka-Buddhas, and holy disciples it has been the basis of the attainment of the

Goal, and of their well-being here and now.”

This approach is one of the primary methods in which one achieves tranquillity of the mind.

This is because the breath is considered to stand on the threshold between voluntary and involuntary

bodily function. By bringing the mind to the breath, be it just the sensation of air moving in and out

of the nose, the expanding and contracting of the lungs, or even the movement of the entire body on

each breath, it allows a person to distance themselves from disturbing impressions, mental torrents

and other day-to-day hassles. This process allows the mind to be brought to peace with the breath.

Mindfulness of the body comes in several levels. The first of which is the practitioner’s general

postures. It is an awareness of exactly what the body is doing, be it standing, sitting, walking, etc.

Meditation on this level provides an initial awareness of the impersonal nature of the body

(Nyanapoika, 1962) which aids the letting go of the attachment to it.

Mindfulness of Feeling. The second foundation of mindfulness is that of the Vedenna –

sensation/feeling. This aggregate comes in three primary forms, that of pleasant, unpleasant, or

neutral. In addition, this aggregate is the precondition from which craving and attachment arises.

Page 43: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

37 The importance of being mindful of one’s sensation is that by bringing it to the forefront of

attention, one will become aware of the conditioned and temporary nature of the arising and fading

of this feeling. It is here that through practice, one separates the feeling itself from their self-centred

reference point and the feeling will be seen as just another feeling rather than ‘my feeling’. Hence,

no engagement will be needed (Nyanapoika, 1962).

Mindfulness of Consciousness. Consciousness here refers to the state of mind that one is in

rather than the content, or thought, that is being processed. The aggregate that correspond to this is

both the third (perception) and the fourth (mental formation). Meditative training allows for a

greater level of self-understanding as it is here where the ‘construction’ of our mental objects are

taking place. The primary focus of awareness is the meditator’s continuous mood state and how it

influences the thought pattern one develops (Shaw, 2006). It gives insight as to why one is thinking

what they are thinking. It also an awareness of how past experience can be drawn into the present,

making conditioned association and generating certain through directions (Nyanasamvara, 2004). In

doing so, one slowly decenters themselves from the impact of the thought that demands immediate

reaction. Through practice, it develops not just self-awareness, but also a form of self-honesty and

sincerity which is important to future mental development. Perhaps the most important aspect of the

mindfulness of consciousness is that which concerns with volition (Flickstein, 2007). The generator

of karma, an individual’s volitional responses has a direct impact to their future experiences.

Although it is impossible to avoid the generation of karma completely until one reaches

enlightenment, the degree of karma as well as specifically what ‘type’ of karma being generated can

indeed be influenced. Through mindfulness practice, one slowly distance themselves from the

habitual engagement and slowly be able to see more possibilities of the kinds of responses available.

In choosing what are considered to be wholesome responses, one generates wholesome karma from

which could benefit their future.

Mindfulness of Mental Objects. The focus of the last meditative exercise is on mental

objects. It is anything that the mind encounters in its barest component form. Although it should be

noted here that the Buddhist-specific teachings take a more predominant role. This is because it is

not any mental object that is supposed to be the focus of the pratitioner’s mindfulness but rather

specific ones that through practice, should completely replace the contents of the mind (Nyanapoika,

1962). The Buddha generated a list of such objects which one to meditate upon including the five

hindrances, five aggregates, six sense-bases, seven factors of enlightenment, and the Four Noble

Truths (Trungpa, 2001). Their details will not be elaborated here, but the importance of their role is

that through holding these mental objects in meditation, they give the mind a wholesome direction

Page 44: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

38 of development that will benefit the alleviation of suffering. It is here that mindfulness will be

brought into connection with other teachings of the Buddha’s dharma.

2.01.5 Goal of Mindfulness Meditation. The ultimate goal of

mindfulness meditation is the attainment of Samatha (also known as jhana). Samatha is a spiritual

attainment resulting from prolonged mindfulness practice. It is characterized by complete

concentration and silencing of the mind. In the words of Gunaratana (1980):

“The jhanas themselves are states of deep mental unification characterized by a total immersion of

the mind in its object. They result from the centering of the mind upon a single object with such

degree of attention that inner verbalization, the discursive function of the thought, is arrested and

eventually silenced, brought to a stop.”

Within the Pali canon, there lists nine levels of this mental concentration that can be

achieved. The first four are the results of meditation of form while the other five are through

meditation of the formless. The Anguttara Nikaya (AN 9.36) lists the nine states being:

1. Delightful Sensations

2. Joy

3. Contentment

4. Utter peacefulness

5. Infinity of space

6. Infinity of consciousness

7. No-thingness

8. Neither perception nor non-perception

9. Cessation

It is through the attainment of Samatha that one is able to achieve the concentration

necessary to develop other forms of spiritual attainments via other methods of meditation. It is

important to know that the jhanas, while a remarkable achievement in themselves, are is not

considered to be sufficient for true emancipation (Sarbacker, 2005). The path of a buddhist is

slightly more complex and has other forms of meditative practice that is required to reach the

ultimate goal. However, Samatha is an important prerequisite and a landmark step towards this goal.

2.01.6 Conclusion. Although mindfulness alone is not sufficient to achieve

enlightenment, it is none the less a substantial portion of the path to Buddhahood. The practice of

Page 45: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

39 meditation is intricately linked to the Buddha’s diagnosis of humanity’s suffering as well as the

Buddhist’s model of the human mind. It is through mindfulness that a person’s attachments to the

five aggregates are slowly released, bringing the possibility of liberation from suffering within the

practitioner’s reach. Although these practices have been wide spread throughout Asia for over two

millennia, it has only been introduced to the western world recently.

2.02 Mindfulness in Modern Psychology

After the turn of the century, there has been something of a decline in interest in traditional

therapeutic methods such as cognitive behavioural therapy. As noted in previous chapters, CBT’s

shortcoming in preventing depression relapses has prompted clinicians and researchers to search for

innovative new approaches. It is in the climate of this search that mindfulness practices found its

way into the spotlight of modern day psychology. One of the possible reasons regarding the timing,

proposed by Siegal, Germer and Olendzki (2009), is that young people who were drawn to eastern

spiritualism during the 60s and 70s are now senior clinical researchers and practitioners within the

field of mental health. As beneficiaries of mindfulness practice, it is now that they have the courage

to share their experiences with their clients.

Meditation in its various forms has had a long history in western psychology. The

emergence of eastern meditation in the scene of western psychology studies going back at least

back half a century such as those of Kretschmer (1953). Questions of its suitability as a therapeutic

techniques has also been raised (Trungpa, 2004) but for quite some time, meditation has never

entered mainstream psychology but remained as an alternative, almost fringe form of treatment. It

wasn’t until Jon Kabat-Zinn’s development of the Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) in

1982, a manualized treatment program for managing of chronic pain, that mainstream attention was

drawn to this particular form of treatment. Since then, research on the topic of Mindfulness has

increased exponentially over the past three decades. Mindfulness as a treatment for psychological

disorders have found substantial support and has steadily gathered momentum within the circles of

researchers and practitioners of psychotherapy alike.

2.02.1 What is Mindfulness in the West? The understanding of

mindfulness that is employed in the west bares close resemblance to its Buddhist roots. However as

noted previously, there is no truly satisfactory translation for the word “Sati” in English. Partly

because of translation issues, as well as how it is adapted to the use of the modern world, various

researchers have defined mindfulness in a number of similar, yet slightly different ways. Different

Page 46: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

40 understanding of mindfulness provides pieces of the puzzle of understanding this definition. Some

definitions are shown as follow:

“In mindfulness, the meditator methodically faces the bare facts of his experience, seeing each

event as thought occurring for the first time” (Goleman, 1988)

“Mindfulness in its totality has to do with the quality of awareness that a person brings to activities.”

(Linehan, 1993)

“Mindfulness means paying attention in a particular way; On purpose, in the present moment,

and nonjudgmentally to things as they are.” (Kabat-Zinn, 1994)

Mindfulness has to do with the quality of both awareness and participation that a person brings to

everyday living. It is a way of living awake, with one’s eyes wide open. (Hayes, 2004)

“(Mindfulness is) awareness of present experience with acceptance” (Germer, 2005)

“Mindfulness means paying attention to things as they actually are in any given moment, however

they are, rather than as we want them to be.” (William, Teasdale, Segal, Kabat-Zinn, 2007).

Through these various understanding of Sati, it is impossible to have a single translation that

can accommodate all its qualities. However, certain aspects of mindfulness can be extrapolated

from them and efforts have been made to find an operational definition of this idea (Bishop et al,

2006). From their perspective, mindfulness encompasses at least two components including the self-

regulation of attention in a certain way and the second as having adapting a particular object of

focus. Both of these components also include a number of aspects of qualities that are accepted as

being part of what mindfulness means. The following is a list of qualities of mindfulness that are

common to most of the known conceptualization.

Intentional Attention. One of the aspects of mindfulness that is almost universally accepted

as its composition is that it has both awareness and attention as its core components. It is Brown and

Ryan’s position (2003) that the two are subsumed under consciousness of which they define as

follow:

Consciousness encompasses both awareness and attention. Awareness is the background

“radar” of consciousness, continually monitoring the inner and outer environment. One may be

aware of stimuli without them being at the center of attention. Attention is a process of focusing

conscious awareness, providing heightened sensitivity to a limited range of experience (Westen,

Page 47: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

41

1999). In actuality, awareness and attention are intertwined, such that attention continually

pulls “figures” out of the “ground” of awareness, holding them focally for varying lengths of

time.

A key quality in this is that attention for mindfulness needs to be intentional. This

intentional direction of attention is often contrasted with a state of being often described as

mindlessness, unawareness, or automatic pilot. Kabat-Zinn (2002) described this automatic pilot

state as “…functioning mechanically, without being fully aware of what we are doing or

experiencing…put another way, only half awake.” It is the operation of a person’s routines without

truly actually being aware of what is being done. The attention aspect of mindfulness permits one to

becoming aware of what is happening both internally and externally. Be it the subtle emotional and

thought processes of the inner world as well as the various sensory stimuli of environment. Perhaps

most importantly, mindfulness affects not just our awareness, but it also provides a basis from

which actions and choices are made. In this, intentional awareness allows us to becoming aware of

the emotions that we are experiencing and aware of the choices we make (William, Teasdale, Segal,

Kabat-Zinn, 2007).

Present-Centeredness. Another aspect of mindfulness is that the attention that is being

directed needs to be held in the present moment. It is about what an individual’s sensory input is

receiving right now, which are the particular input that is being attended to, how one is reacting to

them (be it positive, negative or neutral), etc. It allows the mind to remain in the present moment for

a prolonged period of time. In contrast, a person often finds themselves thinking about the past,

predicting the future, fantasizing about other possibilities. This often leads to the process of

elaboration and dramatization of the current situation and allowing an individual’s own emotional

reaction to distort the their views of the state of affairs. However, rather than getting lost in the

dramatization, elaboration and rumination process about what one is experiencing, the possible

consequences and associations, etc. mindfulness allows the directing of the practitioner’s attention

back to the phenomena that are being experienced in the here-and-now of the mind and body

(Teasdale et al., 1994). An important part of the present-centeredness of mindfulness is it

emphasizes on the ability to become aware of one’s own mind wondering away from the present

moment. This awareness of their own mental content is one of the key components of mindfulness

practice which allows the mind to decentre from torrents of the meditator’s internal mental reactivity. It

is important to note here that thoughts and feelings are not suppressed in order to accomplish this, but

rather they are seen as a distraction and attention gently invited back to the present (Teasdale et al,

2000).

Page 48: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

42

Nonjudgmental & Acceptance. Being Nonjudgemental is a quality of mindfulness and is

related to the idea of acceptance as well as ‘seeing things as they are’. It is effectively the

willingness to see things as they are at this present moment while simply allowing them to be

(William, Teasdale, Segal, Kabat-Zinn, 2007). It is impossible for one to observe something,

external or internal to the mind, without also being fully accepting of that which is happening

within the various states of mind. If one is to be mindful of their anger, then it is important that one

is first able accept that one is indeed angry. This goes the same for all different states of the mind be

it emotional or otherwise. It is impossible for one to be mindful and observe something objectively

while at the same time rejecting its existence (Gunaratana, 2002). This state of mind is often in

compared to a judgmental attitude of which the mind constantly evaluates the states of affairs and

comparing them to some kind of standards, be it internally or externally generated. It should be

noted that this process of judgment is often rationalized as being a way to better ourselves or the

situation. However, the reality is that it generates feelings of inadequacies and craving that can

never be satisfied. Having a Nonjudgment attitude is not simply accepting things in spite of their

imperfection, but rather it is not having the standards of ‘good’ or ‘bad’ in the first place so that the

objects of experience can be accepted as they are.

These three components; intentional attention, present-centeredness, and the Nonjudgment,

are considered as being the three core components of the early definition of mindfulness in the

modern world and are often reinforced and practiced in mindfulness-based treatments such as the

MBSR (McCown, Reibel, & Micozzi, 2010). These three components are labelled here as being the

first generation definition of mindfulness.

In spite of the simple and well-accepted status of the first generation definition of

mindfulness, it is considered insufficient as an operational definition that can be employed for the

psychological investigation of the mediating role and mechanisms of mindfulness. Neither is this

definition adequate for the development of measuring instruments of such phenomenon (Bishop et

al, 2006). It is due to these shortcomings that a second generation definition was developed. It is an

attempt at standardizing a definition of mindfulness for modern-day operationalization on a more

scientific and scholarly level. This definition conceptualizes mindfulness as a two-component

construct including Self-Regulating Attention and Orientation to Experience which effectively

separates the ‘act’ of mindfulness with the co-committing ‘attitude’ necessary for mindfulness to

take place. Self-Regulating Attention consists of the intentional direction of experience as well as

the present-centeredness found in the first two components of the first generation definition. The

second, Orientation to Experience involve the attitudes that one needs to have when practicing

Page 49: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

43 mindfulness in order for the directed attention to be defined as ‘mindful’. Kabat-Zinn (1990) listed

seven attitudinal foundations for mindfulness practice, which are directly related to this second

component listed as follow:

Nonjudging. Identical to the third component of the early definition.

Acceptance. Similar to the third component to the first generation definition, acceptance

implies accepting things as they are without the desire for change. Kabat-Zinn’s rationale for this is

that it protects one from becoming attached to various emotions known to the Buddhists as

‘hindrances’ including greed, lust, aversion, hatred and jealousy Kabat-Zinn (1990).

Non-striving. Mindfulness should not be performed in order to accomplish something. The very

desire for that accomplishment conflicts with the attitude of Nonjudgment as it is only due to a

judgment of imperfection of the present situation from which the desire for change arises. In

addition, the desire for achieving something during practice mean that there is an internal torrent of

desire that is exerting an influence on the mind that runs contrary to the state of being mindful

Kabat-Zinn (1990). While it may seem paradoxical regarding to ‘why’ mindfulness is practiced in

the first place, it is better to see change as a pleasant side effect of being mindful rather than the

reason to do so. It is on this point that mindfulness differs from traditional metacognition. While

metacognition if defined as process of monitor and control (Nelson, Stuart, Howard, & Crowley,

1999; Schraw & Moshman, 1995), these concepts imply a need for control and more importantly, a

purpose for which this control is aimed to achieve. The non-striving aspect of mindfulness does not

share these implications. The purpose of mindfulness is to simply be mindful and not to accomplish

anything more. As such, the process of mindfulness is fundamentally different to that of

metacognition and may probably be better compared to a process of observation and guidance.

Beginner’s mind. Beginner’s mind is an attitude of seeing everything as novel and new. Rather

the having any presumptions and expectations of people, places, and things, having a beginner’s

mind is considered to be important in having an openness to the experience necessary for

mindfulness practices and it is this openness that changes the quality of each experience Kabat-Zinn

(1990).

Letting go. Letting go is Kabat-Zinn’s way of expressing being mindfully non-attached to an

individual’s experiences. Observation, rather than engagement, is the operative nature of this

concept. Letting go allows an individual to be open and accepting to the changes of their mood and

situation without a need to judge or react to such changes. The analogy that is used to illustrate this

Page 50: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

44 is of one watching a movie. No matter what happens in the movie, one does not attempt to interact

with the events within.

The other two attitudinal foundations listed by Kabat-Zinn (1990), trust and patience, are not

directed related to the second generation operational definition of mindfulness, but rather more

directed to a practitioner’s attitude towards the idea of mindfulness and the practice of mindfulness.

In a way, these are the attitudes one needs in order be persistence in their practice. Trust is

considered necessary for beginners to maintain diligence in practice. As mindfulness is unlike

modern medicine that has fast effect, it can take some time before the benefits of mindfulness

practice manifests themselves. As such, practitioners are advised to trust the evidence and science

behind the practice, even if the lack of noticeable effect leaves them doubting the process. Similar

to trust, patience is something that students should employ when facing the frustration due to the

lack of progress that they are making in their practice. Although less irrelevant to the issue of

mindfulness definition, it is none the less an important attitudes that students are advised to adopt in

their early practices.

Some scholars in this field would consider that there are additional qualities in mindfulness

that are just as important. Crane (2009) believes that the practice of mindfulness encompasses

qualities such as of kindness and curiosity. Although one can make the argument that by having an

accepting and Nonjudgmental attitude, the qualities of kindness and compassion will emerge

automatically, it is Crane’s position that such attitude needs to be intentionally practiced. Curiosity

could again be a product of being accepting and Nonjudgmental but it has none the less often

appeared separately within the list of qualities that one should have when practicing mindfulness.

Oddly however, this is also one of the qualities that has rarely been elaborated on, nor justified.

Bishop et al (2006), when developing their operational definition of mindfulness, almost

consistently put ‘curiously’ and ‘accepting’ side by side. From their perspective, and curious and

accepting gives room so that all thoughts, feelings and sensations that arise during mindfulness

practice are perceived as relevant, and therefore, subject to observation. It can be said therefore, that

it is a form of motivated acceptance. There has been other concepts that have been introduced into

what mindfulness imply such as Nonreactivity, self-awareness, among others (Shapiro & Swartz,

2000; Reibel et al., 2001) however, close examinations can be found that similar to kindness and

curiosity, they can all be derived from, or a consequences of practicing the original three.

Mindfulness is a practice that has been inherited from the Buddhist tradition into modern

day psychology. While there are still minor variation as to the conceptualization of what

mindfulness is, some building blocks have been established and accepted. Specifically, three core

Page 51: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

45 components including intentional attention, present-centeredness, and the Nonjudgment set the

foundation from the variations of modern day approaches are built on. Although there are other

qualities that other researchers advocate, these three are still universally accepted as being the most

essential.

2.02.2 Trait and State Mindfulness. Although our understanding of

mindfulness in the modern psychological context can have many levels, what may be of significant

importance is that the modern approach often interprets the phenomena of mindfulness as a

cognitive style in that there is a partial trait and state element to it (Sternberg, 2000). It is through

this separation that much of the research in this area has treated the two separately while at the same

time finding connections between the two. This division effectively separates the dispositional

baseline level of mindfulness one experiences on a day to day basis with the level of mindfulness

that one achieves through intentional practices such as meditation. The reason for the importance of

this is that it is through the connection between the two, and the long-term benefits that arises from

intentional practice that a research application of mindfulness becomes meaningful.

In theory, mindfulness practices such as meditation are intended to foster trait mindfulness -

the day-to-day baseline level of being open, accepting, attentiveness and awareness of one’s

experiences. As a trait, there are assumptions that mindfulness possesses certain qualities such as

consistencies that spans across different situations. Over the years, a number of instruments have

been developed to measure trait mindfulness including the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale

(MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003), Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale (CAMS; Feldman et al,

2007), Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FM; Buchheld, Grossman, & Walach, 2001), Five Facet

Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al, 2006) among others. State mindfulness on the other

hand implies the intentional practicing of mindfulness at that moment. In this state, attention to

present-moment experience is intentionally cultivated with an open, nonjudgmental orientation to

experience (Bishop et al., 2004). An individual’s current experience is simply allowed current

thoughts, feelings, and sensations to happen regarding current experience (Hayes et al., 1999). This

often results in states of experiences similar to those found during meditation. The only instrument

that has been developed in order to measure state mindfulness (specifically from sitting meditation)

is the Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS; Bishop et al, 2004)

Segal et al. (2002) proposed that mindfulness is indicative of a certain level of psychological

adjustment should manifest as an inverse relationship with the neuroticism. It is not difficult to see

the reasoning behind such a view. People who first the profile of being neurotic often show higher

Page 52: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

46 susceptibility to distress and less pruned to psychological well-being (Watson & Clark, 1984) that

are known in mindful individuals (Brown et al., 2007). Such views were later validated with studies

such as those of Baer et al (2006) and Brown and Ryan (2003) which yielded significant negative

relationship between trait mindfulness and neuroticism. It was noted by Thompson and Waltz

(2007) however, that it is uncertain how the nature of the relationship between the two. Specifically,

while it is possible that mindfulness lowers neurotic tendency, it is similar likely that neuroticism

interferes with mindfulness levels. Extraversion however, although often associated with positive

affect, were found to have no relationship with trait mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006). Of interest is

that the other of the five domains, conscientiousness and agreeableness, have yet to received

attention in research projects regarding their relationship with mindfulness. Conscientiousness

demonstrates a strong positive relationship with trait mindfulness (Giluk, 2009). However, it is

uncertain as to the specific reason for this. Although both mindfulness and conscientiousness have

elements of attention control and emotional management involved, those very qualities can also be

in contrary with the nonjudgmental attitude necessary for mindfulness. The exact reason for this

relationship therefore, has not yet been established empirically or theoretically. Agreeableness has

been found to have a moderately positive correlation with mindfulness traits (Giluk, 2009). This

was not surprising as qualities of agreeableness such as the capacity to empathize and show

compassion for others are consistent with much of the characteristics of mindfulness practices.

Lastly, openness to experiences shows a small but positive correlation to mindfulness (Giluk, 2009).

Theoretically, this can be explained as both traits share qualities such as receptiveness,

Nonjudgment and acceptance. Of interest to this project, is that there are so far no studies that have

looked into the relationship between a person’s inborn or adult temperament with trait mindfulness.

As such, no findings in this area has been reported

Relationship between trait and state mindfulness has been the focus of researchers for some

time. The development of one have been known to be related to the development of the other. As

attention control is an intricate part of mindfulness, an individual’s cultivation of mindfulness may

translate into a better baseline AC capacity on a day-to-day basis. One of the landmark studies in

this area was by Brown and Ryan (2003) in which demonstrated that the practice of mindfulness

meditation as a positive effect on the level of long term trait mindfulness one experiences.

Measured by the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), participants who took part in

mindfulness meditation practices were asked to report their current state of mindfulness at a quasi-

random point during their day. The results indicated that that momentary-state mindfulness was

significantly correlated with baseline levels of trait mindfulness. There are disagreements however,

as to whether trait mindfulness actually has a positive effect on the practice of mindfulness. It has

Page 53: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

47 been argued that those who have a low level of trait mindfulness are likely to benefit more from

mindfulness intervention and in turn benefit more from such intervention. On the other hand, those

who already have a high level of trait mindfulness may show a ceiling effect in that there isn’t any

room for their mindfulness level of progress (Shapiro, Brown, Thoresen, Plante, 2011). This view is

not unsupported as research such as those of Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, and Toney (2006)

have demonstrated that those with higher levels of trait mindfulness already show greater level

psychological well-being without additional training. In addition, Thompson and Waltz (2007)

found that there are no correlations between trait mindfulness and mindfulness level during

meditation, with Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients reaching only .14. It is important

however, that these data are seen in context. Although general relationship was not found between

the two, it did yield partial support for the presence of a connection. In their study, although

relationship between the two was not found in general, when separating the samples between

experienced and non-experienced meditator, it was found that those who were experienced

meditators yielded a significant relationship between their trait mindfulness and state mindfulness.

Such relationship was not found among non-experienced meditators.

Most studies regarding the role of trait mindfulness often comes in the form of its

relationship with the effects of mindfulness practices rather than the state mindfulness during

practice. For example, the same study by Brown and Ryan (2003) showed that increases in trait

mindfulness have been found to be related various psychological benefits such as the decrease in

anxiety, depressive symptoms, as well as other forms of mood disturbance. Others such as Shapiro,

Brown, Thoresen, and Plante (2011) found that baseline trait mindfulness have significant

moderating effect for MBSR intervention. Those who had higher levels of trait mindfulness before

intervention was found to have better increase in mindfulness, subjective well-being, empathy as

well as greater reduction in stress levels one year after treatment compared to control groups. Such

findings supported the study of Kraemer et al (2006) which too, found trait mindfulness have

predictive power regarding the effects of mindfulness intervention treatments.

Such research finding gives rise to a number of possible relationships between trait

mindfulness, state mindfulness and the benefits that are gained from mindfulness intervention. It is

possible that a high level natural trait mindfulness that comes from an individual’s own personality

does not contribute to their ability to attain a high level of mindfulness during meditative practice.

However meditative practices, none the less, do increase a person’s trait mindfulness. In addition,

this cultivate trait mindfulness is indeed related to an increase of state mindfulness during further

Page 54: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

48 practice. In both cases, high levels of trait mindfulness increase the benefit one gains from

mindfulness-based interventions.

In the end, one of the critical issues regarding mindfulness research is whether the practice

of mindfulness increases a practitioner’s baseline trait mindfulness. In addition, whether the

psychological benefits observed by practice of mindfulness mediate the change in trait mindfulness.

This question is essential to mindfulness research as it addresses the fundamental mechanism of

how the benefits of mindfulness come about by becoming more mindful over time. Carmody and

Baer (2008) looked into this question in their study of 174 individuals in nine MBSR groups. In

their report, they administered survey questionnaires regarding their psychological wellbeing, trait

mindfulness levels before and after an 8-week MBSR program. These surveys include Brief

Symptom Inventory (BSI) (Derogatis 1992), Medical Symptom Checklist (MSCL) (Kabat-Zinn

1982), Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (Cohen et al. 1983; Cohen and Williamson 1988), the Scales of

Psychological Well-Being (Ryff and Keyes 1995) and the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire

(FFMQ) (Baer et al. 2006). It should be noted that the FFMQ contains a dimension of Nonreactivity

which is approximate to the capacity of an individual’s attention control levels. Indeed, the

Nonreactivity is essentially the ability to not allow arousing stimuli to draw a person’s attention to it

so that one is unable to disengage with reacting to the stimuli. The results of these studies confirmed

several important relations regarding mindfulness practices. Firstly, all five facets of trait

mindfulness were significantly increased as a result of the MBSR program. Effect sizes were large

for Observing and Nonreactivity to inner experience and moderate for Describing, Acting with

Awareness, and Nonjudging. This shows that engaging in formal meditative practices has long term

effects on the practitioner’s baseline mindfulness levels. Of interest to this study is that the large

effect size for Nonreactivity indicates that the baseline level of an individual’s AC capacity was

increased because of the MBSR program, Given the emphasis of AC training of MBSR, it is logical

to assume that the practice of mindfulness had a direct effect on increasing the meditator’s AC. The

follow up question therefore, is whether this effect is the reason why psychological and

physiological improvement is achieved. In terms of psychology, time of meditation practices was

significantly correlated to the level of decrease in psychological symptoms, as well as the increase

in trait mindfulness. Similarly, the increase in trait mindfulness was a significant predictor of

decrease in psychological symptoms. Most importantly, the studies show that the relationship

between the time committed to meditative practice and the decrease of an individual’s

psychological symptoms is mediated by the change in trait mindfulness. There are similar results

with regard to lowering perceived stress with the relationship between time of meditation and the

difference in stress level before and after the MBSR program was completely mediated by the

Page 55: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

49 difference in trait mindfulness. It should be noted however, for psychological wellbeing, there was

only partial mediation. This implies that already the change in trait mindfulness may be an

important contributing factor of the change in a person’s psychological wellbeing, there may also be

other variables that mindfulness practiced has cultivated accounting for its’ increase. This study

shows that the benefits of mindfulness intervention and practice can largely be understood as results

from the increase of baseline mindfulness that was developed from the practice. In light of this, the

question becomes, based on the various mindfulness programs that are available, what benefits from

these programs can be gained?

2.03 Mindfulness Based Interventions

In order to exploit the benefits for mindfulness practices for the benefit of the general public,

a number of therapeutic programs that has already been developed that employs mindfulness as

components of their therapy including Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT, Teasdale,

Segal, & Williams, 1995), a program designed for depression; Acceptance and Commitment

Therapy (ACT, Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) a program most employed for addiction

treatment; Dialectical Behavioural Therapy (DBT, Linehan's 1993) developed for borderline

personality disorders; Spiritual Self Schema Development Program (3S) a program designed for

addiction and HIV risk behaviours (Avants, & Margolin, 2004); Hakomi Therapy (Kurtz, 1990),

among others. Each of them employs mindfulness within their intervention that together they are all

recognized as a part of what is known as the ‘third wave’ psychotherapy. Among the similarities

between them, all mindfulness intervention programs employ the three pillars listed previously.

Among them, the most significant to this study is that they all emphasize the cultivation of an

individual’s attention control ability. Specifically how this is adapted depends on the individual

programs as well as the targeted disorder.

2.03.1 Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy –

Mindfulness on Depression. In the recent decades, there have been many studies

that show mindfulness practice and intervention in various forms have beneficial effects across a

vast number of different issues, including that of the psychological such as depression (Kenny &

William, 2007), generalized anxiety disorder (Evans et al, 2008), binge eating (Kristeller & Hallett,

1999), post-traumatic stress disorder (Wolfsdorf & Zlotnick, 2001), substance abuse (Breslin, Zack,

& McMain, 2002), as well as physiological problems such as lowering blood pressure (Barnes et al,

2004), pain management (Morone et al, 2008), chronic fatigue syndrome (Surawy et al, 2005), heart

disease (Tacon et al., 2003), among others. What is of interest to this study is how it is applied to, as

well as known results of treatment of depression.

Page 56: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

50

Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy is a third-wave psychotherapy program that was built

by adapting Kabat-Zinn’s MBSR program and is currently the most commonly employed

mindfulness-based program for the treatment of depression. Fundamentally, MBCT trains an

individual to have the ability to “to recognize and to disengage from mind states characterized by

self-perpetuating patterns of ruminative, negative thought” (Segal et al., 2002, p. 75). In addition,

practitioners are taught to hold a perspective that embodies the core qualities of mindfulness

including openness, curiosity and acceptance. MBCT has demonstrated to receive remarkable

success in its ability to lower relapse rate (Teasdale et al, 2000), with its ability to address both the

negative affect and attention processes of depressed people. Before detailing the rationale and

research findings of this form of treatment, it is important to understand what practices and skills

are taught within the program.

2.03.2 Content and Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy. Conventionally, MBCT is an eight week program compromising of weekly sessions between 2 to

2.5 hours long. Each session will comprise of in-session exercises, mindfulness practices and home

exercises that participants are advised to do each day between sessions. Home practices include

daily formal mindfulness practices lasting a minimum of 45 minutes as well as informal practices

involving an individual’s daily routines. It may include recording of certain experiences one

observed, learn to approach stressful situation differently, or simply giving oneself to mental space

to decentre themselves from the emotions of the situation (Crane, 2009). The first half of the

program often emphasizes on guiding the practitioner’s attention to their internal experiences and

becoming aware of the internal dynamics of the person’s mind. This process is of importance as it is

cultivating the practitioner’s AC capacity. It is through the development of an individual’s ability to

direct attention towards or away from the targeted, grounding objects, be it physical or mental, that

the benefits of the MBCT program can be found. The latter half of the program places emphasis on

external application of such understanding and transformation into everyday issues and daily

challenges.

Within the sessions, formal practices are performed, allowing the various mindfulness skills

and awareness are introduced, developed and refined with each session. Among these are

techniques that teach one to focus their attention onto certain parts of their physical or mental being.

Each of the meditative practices helps to cultivate certain aspect of mindfulness and develops

various benefits that assist in an individual’s therapy. The development of their attention controlling

capacity is an inseparable part of this process.

Page 57: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

51

Body Scan. The body scan is the primary practice employed in MBCT. It is an introduction

from which allows for an individual to first develop the capacity to guide their mind in a manner

that entails the various quality of mindfulness as listed in chapter 3.02.1, onto a more tangible base

– physical sensations. This allows an access to the immediate, non-conceptual based sensations and

intuitive ways of knowing. Commonly lasting up to around 45 minutes, it is often performed while

in a supine position or sitting (Crane, 2009). Operationally, the body scan often begins with a short

session of bringing the mind to an individual’s breath and allowing the mind to clear of other

thoughts. Awareness is then intentionally guided to various regions throughout the body starting

from the toe and moving up towards the head. Attention is paid to specifically where and how the

practitioner’s mind is and it is often here that students are first introduced to issues regarding how

the mind wanders and are repeatedly encouraged to guide to back to the area of the body that the

session has proceed to. Another area of training is for the student’s attitude to allow things as they

are. Due to the various different sensations of the body that might arise, students may have various

judgments and/or desire to affect certain changes. Reminding students to allow things as they are

fosters acceptance and decentring. By practicing the body scan, students learn to ‘be with’ their

bodies in the present-moment, cultivate attentional control of the mind and more importantly,

develop an awareness of when the mind wander and one is switching back into automatic piloting

mode.

Mindful Movement. Mindful movements of various forms are another form of mindfulness

practices geared towards physical sensation. However, rather than a motionless posture, mindful

movement allows for student to experience their body as it moves. A number of techniques are

often introduced but the specific types of movements are often dependent on the teacher’s own

background and experience (McCown, Reibel & Micozzi, 2010). Common mindful movements that

are incorporated into the session include mindfully breathing and walking. Methods that are at the

discretion of the teacher include techniques such as Yoga and Taiji. Mindful movement serves a

number of important functions and cultivates several physical awareness that are not available to the

body scan. For example, it fosters an individual’s awareness to the body in motion, allows one to

become aware of the experiences of their life through sensation through the process of movement,

awareness of their habitual tendencies, discover new possible ways of movement, among others.

Participants are guided to having a sense of their balance that is commonly habitualized in their

daily lives. This process of ‘achieving’ balance rather than simply brings the participant to

becoming aware of their bodily position. More importantly, by experiencing the transitional process

in which one shifts their bodyweight, one becomes aware of the mind’s habit of being focused on

the future and goal of their action rather than the process. Mindful movement in regards to an

Page 58: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

52 individual’s balance brings the mind to the present moment in a way that is more grounded.

Employment of mindful movement allows for one to experience their life in as process

rather than a state that body scan develops. It allows for the practitioner to be grounded in the

sensory experience and are known to give rise to moments of feeling of the body being reconnected

to the mind. This provides a sense of wholeness as well as new possibilities in life. The practice of

mindful movement also permits the possibility of basic application of mindfulness in the

practitioner’s daily life.

Sitting Meditation. Sitting meditation is another mainstay of mindfulness based cognitive

therapy. Often taught two weeks after the body scan has been introduced, sitting meditation entails

the practitioner to position themselves in a sitting position either on the floor (with a cushion or mat)

or on a chair, and intentionally being the body into a upright and relaxed state. Sitting meditation

offers the largest range of different forms of meditation as well as different anchors for the mind -

breathing, physical sensations, emotions and ideas, memories of certain stressful situations, among

others (Crane, 2009). Along with enhancing and cultivating the various skills that are developed in

the previous meditations, sitting meditation allows for the developed of new forms of awareness. It

is also in sitting meditation in which practitioners practice opening the mind to all of these factors

without focus on anyone. Effectively, it is a way to bring to mind to a state of ‘being’ without any

anchor rather than concentration on one thing. In doing so, practitioners are able to have a new

awareness of their relationship with experiences as well as coming to an understanding of how their

own mind operates. Because there no anchor is set, one will be able to become more attuned to the

mind’s reaction to stimuli and ideas, most importantly the affective reaction of pleasant, unpleasant

and neutral to such stimuli and ideas are bring to the surface of awareness as well as how one

normally react to such feelings. This process cultivates an individual’s ability to be attuned to their

experience while at the same time not being lost within them, able to observe the rise and fall of at

each sensation and experience without interpretation.

Three Minute Breathing Space. Three minute breathing space is a form of brief-period

meditation in which one stops what they are doing, and spend three minutes simply breathing.

Practitioners bring their minds to their breath and away from any thoughts and hassle of that

moment. It is a bridge that can bring the core of mindfulness teaching into a person’s daily life. It

teaches one to stop and recognize the habitual actions that he / she is taking at the moment, bring

the mind to a single anchor and away from the other mental events of the time (McCown, Reibel &

Micozzi, 2010). Although only for three minutes, it none the less breaks the rhythm of an

individual’s habits and is able to reinforce their sense of being in the mist of the daily hassles. When

Page 59: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

53 practiced in response to life stressors and emotionally charged situation, it allows for one to pause

and give space so as to gather their minds in the face of these life events. It is through this break in

the practitioner’s routines that bring new possibilities of how one is to respond to the current

situation.

2.03.3 Atmosphere of the Class. In addition to the formal meditation

practices, there are many aspects of a MBCT class that can be considered. For example, the

importance of establishing the environment that supports mindfulness practice is of paramount.

Often, therapists and teachers of mindfulness are required to be seasoned practitioners themselves

before they lead a therapeutic session. This is due to the fact that the teacher of the class must too

embody the qualities of mindfulness including acceptance, compassion and gentle non-striving

(McCown, Reibel & Micozzi, 2010). In addition, the teacher should bring an atmosphere of

curiosity and exploration. By doing so, participants of the program will not only feel safe to open up

to the class, but also follow the lead of the teacher in their exploration and transformation of their

own inner mind. Small group discussions are conducted (either between students and teacher, or

between students themselves) regarding the experiences one had during the week since the previous

session regarding how one has applied their new-found mindfulness in their daily lives. Discussion

also extends to the impact of such events had on them. This process allows students to explore their

challenges and change in a safe and nonjudgmental environment. It also helps to cultivate a sense of

compassion for each other which can then be redirected into oneself, fostering a self-compassion

and acceptance.

2.03.4 Elements of Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy. Certain

techniques from traditional Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy are also integrated into the MBCT

program. There are a number of ways in which these are integrated. The first is the teaching of how

the mind function and bring an understanding to the practitioner regarding the thought processes

that they go through. In addition, CBT’s theories, as well as mindfulness-related approaches of

depression are also taught to the participant so that they become aware of how their conditions are

maintained as well as how mindfulness practices are able to help. Much of the information that are

included in this area are covered within chapter 1, with emotions triggering rumination and

automatic thought processes, leading to greater levels of depressive emotions. Focus have been be

placed on the attitudes that one adopts in any given situation, but more so in stressful and upsetting

circumstances.

Page 60: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

54

Having an understanding of their conditions goes to aid the transformation of the individual

as they are able to conceptualize, become aware of, and more importantly relate differently to their

physical sensations, feelings as well as the inner dynamics of their mind. It allows for one to be

attuned to mood changes that may occur in everyday life and the habitual reactions one may engage

in, thus applying mindfulness to give room and decentre themselves from the mental state at the

time and be able to respond in a way that will foster better mental health. This understanding also

helps with the development of treatment strategies that practitioners can adopt. It is through the

implementation of such CBT techniques that home-exercises and daily practices are designed. In

bringing the practice home and applying them in daily lives, the benefits of the therapy can be

reinforced and maintained, further cultivating the transformation of mindfulness practices.

It should be noted however, that there are considerable differences between MBCT and CBT.

Primary of which is its emphasis on seeing things as they are without an intention to change them.

While CBT hopes to change the actual content of thought for the individual, it has been noted

(Orsillo et al, 2004) that the primary aim of mindfulness is not to change the content but merely the

process in which this content is approached. CBT strives and works to change the thought processes

and behavioural patterns of the individual however with the quality of ‘non-striving’ within MBCT,

change that occurs on the targeted problem is considered as a secondary effect, with the primary

effect being that of the transformation of the individual’s entire outlook and experience of their

lives. With this, much of the techniques that are often employed in CBT such as reinforcements,

targeted actions, or distractions are not commonly utilized by MBCT.

In addition, although both CBT and MBCT involves decentring as one of the critical skills

that are developed through the course of treatment, their overall conceptual understanding and

function are quite different. In CBT, decentring is a capacity that permits one to make changes in

their deep-seated beliefs about other themselves and the world in general (Beck, 1979) while in

MBCT, the skill of decentring in itself, is able to alleviate one from the symptoms of depression

(Teasdale et al, 2002).

2.03.5 Evidence and Rationale of MBCT. The question of how or why

MCBT works can take number of different levels. The first is establishing a working model for

depression from which we can work on. The second is the identification of the various qualities of

mindfulness, finally seeing how these qualities of mindfulness an moderate the variables within

depression model.

Page 61: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

55

In regards to mindfulness, the operational model for depression has been outlined in chapter

1, with negative emotions triggering rumination through differential activation. This ruminative

process in turn generates greater levels of negative feelings and continually feeds the cycle. This

process, outlined by the DAH, not only holds the individual inside a self-perpetuating cycle

emotions and negative thoughts, but also forms the foundation from which relapses occur for those

who are considered as successfully treated by conventional treatment methods. It is also the belief

of this study, that the lack of AC also plays a part in this process and that one of the most critical

aspects of MBCT is the increase of an individual’s baseline AC capacity by developing attention

focus or switching abilities during practice.

As far as the reduction of relapse rate is concerned, there has been ample evidence

demonstrating the efficacy of mindfulness treatment of depression and depression relapse. Two

landmark studies on this issue came just over a decade ago with studies by Teasdale et al. (2000)

and Ma & Teasdale (2004). In Teasdale et al. (2000), 140 patients of depression who had at least

two previous episodes of depressions and haven’t used antidepressants in the previous three months

were randomly assigned to a either MBCT or TAU programme. Follow ups were conducted one

year after treatment was complete. The results of this study showed that for clinical patients who

had three or more depression episodes, the relapse rate was 50% lower for the MBCT group

compared to the TAU group (MBCT = 37% vs. TAU = 66%.) In the second study, Ma & Teasdale

(2004) was an attempt to replicate the results of the previous study and explore the possible

differences of MBCT effectiveness for depression relapses triggered by stressful even or the

participant’s own internal processes. With regard to the first goal, the results were almost identical

when compared to the previous study in which the MBCT group achieved around 50% less relapse

rate compared to the TAU group (MBCT = 36% vs. TAU = 78%.) Of special interest to these

studies was that in both cases, people who experienced two episodes of depression responded to the

MBCT program similarly to the TAU. It was discovered was that this was likely a different

population group. Those who had three episodes or more were more likely to be those who had

difficult childhoods and had struggled with depression from an early age. On the other hand, those

who had two episodes were tended to be those who had normal early life but were struck with major

life events that triggered their depression. Although this may mean the specific number of episodes

may not be a variable in itself, it none the less raises questions regarding how these additional

considerations may affect the efficacy of MBCT.

Looking specifically at the depression side of the issue, MBCT’s effectiveness have been

shown not just in the prevention of relapses, but also in its ability to reduce depressive symptom as

Page 62: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

56 measured by the Beck Depression Inventory (Barnhofer et al, 2009). The degree of symptomology

reduction was proportionate to the level of depression that one was experiencing. More severe

levels depression resulted in better outcome of MBCT. Later studies in this area looked further into

the specific treatment-resistance groups of depression. For example, a study by Eisendrath et al.

(2008) produced evidence that supported the idea mindfulness based interventions are able to help

sufferers of depression even when they have proven to be resistant to antidepressant treatment.

Similarly, MBCT have been shown to be effective for people who are resistant to treatment in both

antidepresseant on top of the more conventional cognitive treatments (Kenny & Williams, 2007). In

addition to evidence showing its success in the treatment of those who are having depression,

research also looked into whether MBCT benefits those who have already met the criteria for

recovery but are still on medication. Kuyken et al. (2008) tackled this issue and conducted a follow

up for people who went through either the MBCT program or continuation of antidepressants. The

results showed that those who went through the MBCT program suffered a relapse rate of 47%

while those in who continued their medication had a 66% relapse rate.

Considering the evidence supporting the benefits of mindfulness interventions for treating

depression, the reasons of explaining this phenomenon rises. It is the position of those who

subscribes to mindfulness as a treatment. The answer to tackling the problem of the reduction of

depression relapse is to target the vulnerabilities that maintains the rumination cycle. Therefore, the

reduction of rumination in itself results in the positive outcome. Early research such as those of

Brown and Ryan (2003) produced evidence showing that the presence of higher levels of trait

mindfulness has the effect of reducing of rumination. As previously noted in section 3.02.2, the

practice of mindfulness enables one to enhance their trait mindfulness level, together with the fact

that this mindfulness intervention program was design for this purpose, it supports the intention that

this program serves to reduce a person’s rumination level. This relationship is supported in a

number of studies such as those of Ramel, Goldin, Carmona and McQuaid (2004), Watkins (2004)

and Broderick (2005) which yielded significant reduction in rumination levels for people with mood

disorders such as depression after going through mindfulness intervention program. It should be

noted, that the study by Ramel, Goldin, Carmona and McQuaid (2004) shows mindfulness

intervention is effective with the rumination-dysfunctional attitude-emotional disturbances

combination. However this particular study was not targeted at clinical samples, nor was it

employing under a structured MBCT program. Rather it utilized the more generalized Mindfulness

Based Stress Reduction (MBSR). Over the years, several models have been developed to explain

why the enhancement of mindfulness is able to reduce rumination.

Page 63: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

57

2.03.6 Interacting Cognitive Subsystem. From the perspective of the

ICS, the mind processes information using three distinct modes including the Mindless Emoting,

Conceptualizing/Doing and Mindful Experiencing/Being. The mindless emoting system indicates

one in which one reacts to feelings without thoughts, awareness, or attention to the bigger picture.

Effectively, the attention is drawn towards the aroused emotions without the capacity to switch

away from it. It is through this system in which one often reacts behaviourally such as aggression

because of anger, flight/fight because of fear, crying because of sadness, are all outcomes of this

system. An individual’s capacity to function when they are mindlessly emoting is limited as they

are unable to direct their attention away from the emotions that they are being bombarded with. The

Conceptualizing/Doing system is one in which the person’s feelings are not attended to but rather

focus is placed on the actual processing and analysis of conceptual contents. Within this state of

mind, one operates on a highly rational, impersonal and goal-oriented mode that focuses on the

cause and effect of the external world. Lastly, Mindful Experiencing/Being is a mode of mind that

recognizes the thoughts and feelings of the individual and being aware of the various internal and

external sensations. In this frame of mind, emotions become experienced directly by the subject

rather than being a conceptualized object that is subjected to rational thought.

It is the position of the ICS that Mindless Emoting and Conceptualizing/Doing systems

bring about vulnerability to depression. In Mindless Emoting, individuals have a tendency to

become identified with their emotional reactions. Rather than being able to recognize that the

emotional reaction as being a mental force that is generated by the situation and their own reaction

to it, the person is immersed within the reaction itself and thus have little to no room for awareness

or reflection. It is believed that one of the ways in which one emote is through cognitive reactivity

(discussed in chapter 1.08.01). Teasdale believes that those who are exceptionally in danger of

depression are those who fluctuate between the Mindless Emoting and Conceptualizing/Doing

systems as this is the basis of which the rumination cycle is maintained. Mindful

Experiencing/Being on the other hand is the only processing system that allows emotional

processing and fostering metacognitive awareness. Metacognitive awareness is generally defined as

the experience of an individual’s thoughts and feelings as mental events, rather than being

synonymous with that person’s self (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993; Teasdale, Segal, & Williams,

1995).

Another aspect of Mindful Experiencing/Being is the promoting of an individual’s capacity

to decentre oneself from his/her experiences. Although similar to metacognitive awareness,

decentring puts emphasis on the practitioner’s awareness that the thoughts and feelings he/she is

Page 64: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

58 experiencing are temporary and transitory rather than true reflections of oneself (Fresco, Flynn,

Mennin & Haigh, 2011). This understanding and attitude developed towards these mental events are

believed to be able to disrupt the connection between emotional reactions with the automatic,

ruminative thought patterns (Teasdale et al., 1995). Through developing this system of the mind,

one is able to develop awareness of the practitioner’s own thoughts and feelings while at the same

time practice Nonreaction to such mental events as they are not reflective, and thus should not be

identified with, oneself. This permits greater level of flexibility to their own responses to these

emotions which thereby lessens the possibility of rumination being the response choice.

In 2002, Teasdale et al conducted a series of three studies looking at the effects of

metacognitive awareness on the prevention of depression relapse. It is believed that an individual’s

vulnerability to relapse is associated with a lack of accessibility of metacognitive sets in relation to

negative thoughts and feelings. In the first study, 40 psychiatric outpatients who were deemed likely

to experience depression relapse were compared with non-depressed controls with regard to their

autobiographical memories. The results confirmed that people within the high risk group for relapse

displayed significantly less evidence of a metacognitive set in relation to negative thoughts and

feelings in the autobiographical memories when prompt by depressive cues. The second study

looked at the extent to which baseline levels of metacognitive awareness was able to predict the

likelihood of relapse in depressed patients. In this study, Teasdale took 158 patients with recent

major depression who were either given antidepressants alone or in conjunction with cognitive

therapy. The same measures of study 1 were taken at various points of the treatment. The results

showed that the chances of relapse were negatively correlated to the chances of depression-related

cues triggering metacognitive sets in a person’s memories with respect to negative thoughts and

feelings. Essentially, this means that the less metacognitively aware one is, the greater the chances

of relapse they will experience. Lastly, the third study confirmed that it is not merely the content of

the participant’s thoughts that is operated on when going through cognitive therapy, rather, training

exclusively aimed at metacognitive awareness such as MBCT, by changing their relationship to

negative thoughts and feelings only, can also significantly reduce relapse or recurrence in

depression. Although these series of studies did not definitively confirm it, it is possible to

extrapolate from these findings that an important effect of cognitive therapy, and any therapy at all,

for depression is mediated through the changes of metacognitive awareness that is enhanced

through the therapy program.

2.03.7 Mode of Processing. The approach taken by Kuyken (2010) focuses

on the mode of processing within the mind. For Kuyken, human behaviour and mental reactions are

Page 65: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

59 based on three primary states - the current state, the goal (or desired) state, and actions in order to

diminish the distances between the two. In order to navigate through these three states, what is

known as the central executive system (Baddeley 1996) functions to weigh up various possible and

alternative goals based on the degree of their importance to the level of to the individual. For those

with mental problems, it is believed that there is a dysfunction in this system by which people are

unable to achieve a particular goal and yet are unable to let it go and switch to another goal

(Watkins 2008). When one is depressed, the goal is effectively to simply ‘be happy’ or to ‘not be

sad.’ Yet in order for actions to be taken so as to close the gap between the current sad state and this

intended state, the current state and the intended state need to be held in the working conscious

mind so as to monitor the progress one makes as the gap is closed. If this was an external issue such

as going to the post office, actual actions can yield objective results leading to the system

acknowledging certain goals have been achieved which lead to the larger goal. This permits the

overall system to be able to operate smoothly. However, because both the current state and the goal

state are both abstract ideas (sad, and be happy), and that these ideas are acted upon as if they are

treated as external, tangible facts, there are little one can do in order to close this gap as there is

nothing one can physically do to deal with abstract ideas. The process of rumination results is

thereby the result of holding these ideas in the conscious mind without shifting to other goals. The

situation is worsen as the process of checking the difference between the two states can serve to

increase the sadness that one is trying to avoid. Secondly, even attempts to suppress the thought and

feelings can result in the content being more strongly associated to the mood (Watkins, 2008).

Within the context of this model, Kuyken listed a number of effects when practicing

mindfulness. When mindfulness is practiced, the abstract ideas are no longer taken as being literal

facts. This result in the processing of such ideas as facts are halted before it begins. Second, it takes

away the significance of such ideas as it provides the individual with direct experience of their

environment which in turn changes the way in which they relate to it. In doing so, the abstract ideas

are not present to interfere with this experience. It is through this mode of the mind that one notices

that the process of thought, while useful at times, can narrow ones’ perception. Thirdly, rather than

the content of thought being stored in the conscious mind, the process itself is brought to focus. By

engaging in meditation, the role of linguistic based content of thought are brought to the foreground

in which our natural inclination to avoid certain thoughts becomes apparent. One becomes aware

that this avoidance, although commonly employed to deal to external threats, are useless when

dealing with internal anxieties. Lastly, by seeing the abstract ideas as being just that, it permits one

to decentre from that perceptive. This permits one to come into contact with a wider range of

possible events and ideas have been ignored previously to make room for the abstract ideas.

Page 66: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

60

Overall, it is believed that the reason why mindfulness is able to decrease rumination level is

through increasing level of the practitioner’s capacity to become aware of their ruminative

tendencies and be able to switch to a Nonruminative mode of the mind. It is through adopting a

mindful approach to their immediate experience and having the Nonjudgmental attitude and

observing their physical sensation and mental thoughts, that one is able to decentre oneself from

their emotions and focusing their attention on the present state. In doing so, MBCT practitioners are

able to hold back the cycle of rumination at its commencement and stop it, from escalating to a

relapse (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002).

2.03.8 Dialectical Behavioural Therapy – A Mindful

Approach to Personality Disorder. Personality disorders,

especially borderline personality disorder, are characterized by

an innate lack of emotional regulation. This, as addressed in

previous sections, could be attributed to, or at least related to,

a lack of attention control ability. One of the critical goals of

DBT is to increase an individual’s capacity to control their

affective states through mindfulness techniques. Considering

the emphasis on attention control development for

mindfulness practice, and the relationship between emotions

and attention, it is logical to assume that the development of

AC capacity of an individual is in some way related to better

emotional management.

Perhaps the earliest attempt to use mindfulness as a treatment option for PD was by (Kutz,

Broysenko & Benson, 1985) in which a mindfulness training method, was designed based on

Kabat-Zinn’s program. It was employed on people who were diagnosed with either narcissistic or

borderline personality disorders. A total of 18 participants were being treated with ongoing

psychodynamic therapy took part in this study and mindfulness training was experimented as an

adjunct to conventional psychotherapy. After a ten-week training program, it was found that there

were significant therapeutic benefits. Both participants and therapists reported that there was a

decrease in anxiety and depression as well as an increase in overall wellbeing. In spite of these

findings however, there were certain methodological flaws that had to be taken into account when

interpreting the results. First of all, the lack of a control group opens the possibility that the

Page 67: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

61 improvement. Might have been due to variables outside of the mindfulness treatment. In addition,

there was a lack of structured interviews or questionnaires to standardize the diagnosis the

individual have made as such the baseline measurement of the participants was difficult to interpret.

Perhaps because of these limitations, later studies that produced dissimilar results gave the subject

higher degrees of clarity as well as complexity to the subject.

Less than a decade later, Marsa Linehan and her colleagues (1991, 1993) developed a

structured and skill-based form of treatment named Dialectical Behavioural Therapy being one of

the most successful treatment available for BPD (Loizzo, 1999). This form of therapy deserves

special attention as it is perhaps the only mindfulness based treatment that has had consistent

evidence in helping treat personality disorder. One of the common symptoms of PD in general, but

especially potent in BPD, is affective dysregulation. It is through emotional dysregulation that

people who suffer from BPD are predisposed not only to intensive and frequent mood swings, but

also impulsive behaviours that can have negative consequences. Targeting this fundamental

symptom of BPD, one of the primary themes of DBT is to address this issue using mindfulness

training.

2.03.9 Content and Rationale for Mindfulness in DBT. DBT is

a form of CBT but with mindfulness and acceptance strategies infused within. The central concept

of ‘dialectic’ is the tension that exists regarding having the individual accepting themselves for

exactly who and how they are at that moment, while at the same time having to push forward to

improve their condition and creating a life that is worth having. The total course of DBT requires a

standard time of one year. Core skills that are trained in DBT include distress tolerance skills,

interpersonal effectiveness skills, emotion regulation skills and mindfulness skills (Linehan, 1993a).

As with other mindfulness-based therapies, DBT’s mindfulness skills involve learning how to

observe and accept the moment-to-moment happenings without judgments or rejections. It should

be noted however, there are some differences between DBT and other mindfulness treatments such

as MBCT and MBSR. Chief among them is the fact that although mindfulness is one of the core

skill that is taught and employed, its scope is not as dominant in DBT. As such, the mindfulness

training that is required for DBT is not as intensive.

The DBT program conceptualizes the mind as always existing in one of three states, the

‘emotion mind,’ the ‘thinking mind’ and the ‘wise mind’. The ‘emotional mind’ refers to state of

mind in which an individual’s thoughts and behaviours are heavily influenced by emotions.

Rational thought, planning and prediction of consequences of actions are difficult. Attempts at

Page 68: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

62 doing so will often find that the perception of the bare facts as distorted, commonly inflated to a

level of importance that is out of proportion to conform to the current emotional state. The

‘reasonable mind’ is viewed as a state in which one rationally and intellectually thinks their way

through a situation with little to no emotional investment. This state of mind is able to perceive facts

without bias, acknowledge empirical proof as well as focus an individual’s attention to the task at

hand. Lastly, the ‘wise mind’ integrations the ‘emotion mind’ with the ‘reasonable mind’ so that a

third element is synthesizes that is unavailable to either states individually. This new element is

intuitive thinking is able go beyond simple emotion or logic. This intuition is able to give a sense of

knowing with regard to what is the right thing to do or how things should be etc. As it integrates

both the reasonable and emotional mind, it is able to think while at the same time taking into

account of their emotions and finding a balance between the two.

It is the DBT’s position that the lack of awareness of an individual’s own emotions is the

primary reason for the impulsive and mood dependent behaviour. Mindfulness practices are used in

order to increase the practitioner’s awareness so that such behaviours can be reduced. To

accomplish this, three ‘what’ skills are taught to the individual along with and three ‘how’ skills.

The three ‘what’ skills include observe, describe and participate while their corresponding ‘how’

skills involves Nonjudgmentally, one-mindfully and effectively. Firmly connected with the ‘how’

skill of Nonjudgmentally, observe, as a skill, is seeing things as they are without judgment or

elaboration at any given situation, even if this situation may be distressing. This ability extends

beyond the sensory input of a person’s five senses and includes any emotional, behavioural and

physiological reaction one has to the situation. There is emphasis here regarding the difference

between the observation of an event and the event itself. It trains one to open themselves to the

moment and experience the happenings internally and externally rather than stopping the emotion

by reacting to the situation or suppressing it. It is through the use of nonreinforced exposure that

automatic fears and avoidances are gradually decreased and eventually extinguished. “Describe” is

a skill that involves putting an individual’s experiences into words. It is considered important that

one is able to verbalize their experience as it is beneficial for the development of communication

skills as well as self-control capacity. The verbalization of the practitioner’s experiences as merely

experiences develops their awareness of the difference between the event and the experience of that

event. This in turn reduces the tendency of a person suffering from DBT to take their emotions and

thoughts literally and confusing their reaction with the objective event. For example, by articulating

their experience of anxiety and fear, an individual will become aware that their subjective

experience only and not that the environment is really threatening. Lastly, “participate” is the skill

in which one takes an active role in their behaviours that they conduct. It is a sense of action

Page 69: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

63 without self-consciousness or self-doubt. In essence, it is action without the mind being preoccupied

with things such as the future, worries or concerns. It also involves mindful action in which one

focuses on the taste of food when they are eating, or the feeling of the sun on their body when they

are sunbathing, etc. It creates a mental space in oneself that frees their mind from the background

noises of their mind, turning away from the automatic pilot mode, and truly engage in the

experiences of their lives with attentiveness (Linehan, 1993a).

2.03.10 Evidence for DBT. The effectiveness of DBT has been subjected to a

number of research studies. The first to utilize a controlled randomized trial was by Linehan et al

(1991). In this study, a total of 44 participants that was diagnosed with BPD using the DSM-III (as

well as similar background in terms of history of suicide, depression, demographics, etc.) were

randomly referred to either DBT or TAU. Over the course of their year-long treatment, they were

assessed every four months. At the end of the treatment, it was found that patients who received

DBT had a significantly fewer days of hospitalization, less suicidal attempts, and increased

retention rate in therapy compared to TAU. However, what was perhaps of great interest in this

study was there was no difference found for depression levels, hopelessness score, suicidal ideation

or reasons for living. This implies that although the DBT had a positive effect on BPD on the

behavioural level, it remained ineffective on the mental level.

In 1994, Linehan and colleagues (Linehan, Tutek, Heard, & Armstrong, 1994) conducted a

study to see if results from DBT were able to be maintained a year after treatment had been

terminated. In this study, 19 participants were given DBT while 20 patients were assigned to TAU.

Assessments were taken periodically for a period of two years. The results of this study revealed

that the DBT group had a higher level of global functioning, less parasuicidal behaviour and less

days of hospitalization compared to the TAU group. There were also less anger, better self-reported

adjustments and higher levels of retention rate were found in this group compared to the TAU (DBT

= 83% vs. TAU = 42%). However, there was no difference found in the area of anxious rumination.

Investigating further, Linehan replicated the study under more rigorous control conditions which

found that DBT patients were less likely attempt suicide, require admission to hospital stay as a

result of suicidal ideation as well as having generally lower medical risk in terms of parasuicidal

behaviours (Linehan et al., 2006b)

There are other studies not led by Linehan that looked into the efficacy of DBT. For

example, one study (Koons, Robins, Tweed, Lynch, et al., 2001) compared the effectiveness of

DBT on women with BPD to a CBT control group. Their findings yielded results that show the

Page 70: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

64 DBT treatment group had lower levels of suicidal ideation, depression levels, ideation, depression,

hopelessness as well as anger expression compared to the control group. Such differences were not

seen in areas such as actual suicidal attempts and behaviours, treatment retention rate as well as the

experience of anger.

As note previously in section 2.03, there is naturally a high comorbidity between BPD and

substance abuse. It was in light of this that Linehan et al (1999) extended her research in DBT by

looking at how DBT performed for hospitalized PD patients who had comorbid drug dependence.

This study, compared the effectiveness of a modified version of DBT (specifically targeting

substance abuse) and TAU, found that there was significant between-group differences in areas of

social and global adjustments. However, it should be noted that no differences were found in terms

of their anger level or parasuicidal behaviour during treatment.

Perhaps the overall picture that has been painted regarding the effectiveness of DBT on PD

is that it develops the individual’s tolerance distressing emotions and gives them an increased range

of response options when distressing situations are encountered. This has shown considerable

success and has thus far been one of the few treatments of BPD that has demonstrated reliable

success.

2.03.11 Mindfulness & Emotion Regulation. Mindfulness’s

operation on BPD has provided an important insight in this question as the fundamental mechanism

of DBT’s effect is based on the ability for mindfulness to aid the regulation of emotions. When

faced with distressing and emotional experiences, people who are low on mindfulness levels

commonly approach them in ways that are commonly either under-engaged or over-engagement

(Hayes & Feldman, 2004). Under-engagement implies a sense of experiential avoidance (Hayes,

Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996) where one tries to change the external environment so

that the negative emotions are not experienced, or it may be the suppression of thought (Wegner,

1994) where the thoughts that could trigger the emotion are deliberately averted. Over-engagement

on the other hand, means that the individual are likely dramatize, elaborate, and overgeneralise

leading to rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991). In both cases, the core problem is a

person’s inability regulate their emotional experiences. Effectively, issue of emotional

dysregulation as discussed in previous chapters can be attributed to both circumstances.

There has been a significant body of literature regarding mindfulness’s role in improving

emotional regulation. Mindfulness is believed to develop an individual’s voluntary exposure to

unpleasant event and distressful experiences which are known to be beneficial in the development

Page 71: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

65 of adaptive emotion regulation (Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2004; Sloan, 2004). One of the

early studies that looked into the relationship between mindfulness and affective reaction to

emotionally charged stimuli was that of Arch & Craske (2006). Within this study, participants were

given either a 15-minute mindful breathing exercise, 15-minute unfocused attention exercise or a

15-minute instructed worry exercise, afterwards all would be exposed to various slides of either

positively, negatively or neutrally charged images. Results indicate that those who were given the

mindful breathing exercise reported a higher overall positive affect when exposed to positive slides.

In addition, they demonstrated higher levels of willingness to be exposed to more negatively

charged pictures. These results imply the process of mindful practice increased not just the

meditator’s positive affect in general, but also their tolerance level of distressing stimuli and were

able to regulate their emotions more effectively. Others such as Shapiro et al. (2007) found that

mindfulness intervention facilitates the significant increase of not just mindfulness levels measured

by the MAAS, but also the decrease in areas such as perceived stress, negative affect, state and trait

anxiety. These findings were similar to a study by McKee and colleagues (2007) in which negative

affectivity and anxiety sensitivity were all reduced in correlation to increased mindfulness levels.

Some evidence also show the increase in mindfulness aid the reduction of reactivity to threat and

subsequent distress in social exclusion Allen & Knight (2005). Lykins and Baer (2009) performed a

study that compared various areas psychological well-being of seasoned meditators and non-

meditators. They found that those who meditated scored significantly lower in thought suppression,

fear of emotion as well as emotion regulation difficulties

There has been a number of different rationale as to why mindfulness is able to aid the

regulation of emotions. Among the earliest theories include those of Linehan (1993), Teasdale,

Segal and Williams (1995) as well as Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, and Freedman (2005) in which

found mindfulness helps one to recognize that distressing thoughts are simply the mental contents

one has and are thereby not taken as accurate representations of reality. It is their position that the

process of decentring involves the subject of perception becomes the object. There has been a long

tradition in developmental psychology in which this process is one of the key mechanisms of

development and growth throughout an individual’s life. (Kegan, 1982). It is through this ability to

decentre oneself to see beyond the object. In this context, when one is able to see the anxiety and

distress, an important perception that comes to mind is that the individual is more than just the

anxiety and distress as only when one is more and such things can one see them as object. Thus,

emotional reactions are therefore limited as this decentring capability is enhanced. An analogy that

Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, and Freedman (2005) makes is the idea of being on the surface of an ocean.

The torrent of waves makes it difficult to see clearly, only when one goes into the water, below the

Page 72: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

66 waves, can one find calmer space to see things. It is through this new vantage point that one is able

to see clearly which in turn fosters greater cognitive as well as behavioural flexibility, liberating

ourselves from the automatic reactions.

Marlatt (1994) on the other hand, uses an opposite analogy when interpreting the

phenomenon placing an emphasis on acceptance. He believes that rather than going underneath the

waves, one instead learns to ‘surf’ the torrents of the mind without giving in to the urges by

accepting them as normal reactions to the situation. It is through this surfing process that one

realizes the temporality of the emotions. An individual’s attention is directed at the emotion directly

and merely watches it as it rises and fades within oneself (Teasdale et al, 2000). As such, rather than

committing oneself to action based on that emotion, mindfulness teaches the individual to hold the

emotion in their attention and allowing the feeling to pass. It should be noted Marlatt’s approach to

mindfulness is the position of addictives behaviours and therefore the analogy needs to be

contextualized within the need of specific tangible substance.

One theory proposed by Glomb, Duffy, Bono and Yang involves the process of decoupling

between the self and their emotions (2011). They believe that due to the fact that mindfulness

involves the disengagement from the conceptual-based thought processes that one habitually

engages in, the practice can provide a more balanced emotional profile for the individual. This

approach is based on the general understanding that when the involvement of the person’s ego in a

negative situation is high, people feel their self-worth is as threatened. (Kernis, Paradise, Whitaker,

Wheatman, & Goldman, 2000). This “threat” generates strong levels of emotional reactions which

is critical for common emotion dysregulation. However, should the person’s ego be separated from

such situation, even if the individual is physically still involved, the same situation will appear

much less threatening. The lack of “threat” to the ego in turn diminishes emotional reactivity. By

undergoing this decoupling process, the individual enters a more impartial state of mind when

facing the situation. Arguments have been made that although the impartial nature of this process

can reduce both negative and positive affect, the freshness of novel experiences and its immediacy

may well likely provide pleasant feelings in itself (Brown & Ryan, 2003).

2.04 Mindfulness & Attention Control

Having an understanding of the benefits of mindfulness to both depression and personality

disorder, the question of the specific mechanism that permits the operation of mindfulness to work

comes into focus. Mindfulness’ effects on issues such as emotion regulation and rumination are

somewhat established. There are however, other effects of mindfulness that are also worth

Page 73: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

67 exploring in the context of this study. For example, the role of attention control is of critical

importance. As noted in previous chapters, there exists a close relationship between emotions and

attention. Emotions, being preconscious reaction to sensory stimuli, allocate attentions that in turn

give rise to conscious experience and thought. As such, mindfulness approach to attention is of

significance to this study.

One theory of emotion regulation was proposed by Gross (2006). His model of emotion is

based on the series of events, both environmental and mental, that occurs constitute to our day to

day experience. In his model, the sequence of events includes situation, attention, appraisal, and

finally response. Attention and appraisal were mental events in which dictates the responses that

one gives to the situation. It is believed that the reason why emotional dysregulation takes place is

that attention is focus on the difficult emotion or source of the emotion itself (Fredericksen, 1998).

Bishop et al (2004) believed that mindfulness can be defined, in part, as the self-regulation of

attention. This regulation includes the capacity of sustaining attention, attention switching, and

inhibition of elaborative processing. As outlined previously in this chapter, attention plays a central

role in the practice of mindfulness, as one of the core tenets of meditative practices is the capacity

to maintain and sustained attention onto the chosen anchor, be it a physical stimuli or the flow of

mental torrent.

There has been increasing evidence to suggest that cultivation of mindfulness has the effect

of increasing an individual’s attentional control capability. Chambers, Lo and Allen (2008) found

that a 10-day meditation retreat resulted in a decrease in reaction time when performing attention

task that was not found in the control group. In addition, depression scores of participants decreased

in proportion to the reaction time. Of interest however, was that mindfulness did not improve

performance in attention-switching task. This implies that although mindfulness training can delay

the engagement to sensory stimuli, its reengagement capabilities are not improved.

Specific population as well as attention skills has also been subjected to research. In 2007,

Jha, Krompinger, and Baime looked into how seasoned meditators who were on a retreat and

beginners who were attending an MBSR course, and a control group would differ in their ability to

perform attention tasks. It was found that the MBSR program has been completed, and those

attended the course increased their attention orienting capacity to a greater degree compared to the

two groups, while those who were on the retreat developed their exogenous alerting capability

beyond their counterparts. It is therefore suggested that different types of attentional skills may be

developed differently depending on a person’s exposure to mindfulness training.

Page 74: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

68

According to Wallance and Shapiro (2006), this capacity to observe the quality of an

individual’s attention, such as becoming inattentive or excited, is called meta-attention. In terms of

the mental mechanism of attention in this context, the self-regulation of attention constantly

interacts with two other variables including the attitude of unconditioned openness of behaviour

toward the tried experience, as well as continuous intention of the purpose of their action towards

the task at hand. Attention control becomes important as it is able to aid one in putting into

perspective the various constituent parts of the person’s mental landscape without the need to

change them. As such, the decentring of oneself from ideas, thoughts and feelings become easier.

It is through the practice of turning an individual’s attention inwards to the emotions as

objects that beneficial outcome becomes possible. Blaser (2012) argues that it is only through this

redirection of attention that it becomes possible to influence the inner world as well as to adapt this

inner world to their present-moment desires. This adaptation builds and remoulds the mental

landscape resulting in more adaptive mental well-being. For example, by adapting an individual’s

inner world to present-moment desires, it becomes possible to see specifically ‘what’ is being

desired and what merely appears to be the object of our desire. For example, desire for specific

financial gains may arise from the opportunity that is presented in the environment. However, this

desire may likely arise from a sense of security that material gains, that require money to acquire,

provide. In turning the individual’s attention inwards and seeing the deeper level desires of their

mind, it may be possible that the inner mental be changed so that the sense of security and the

financial gain of that particular event.

It perhaps should be mentioned that attention control’s role in emotional regulation is not a

modern theory. Within Buddhist scriptures, it has been preached that intentional attending to one’s

own experience cultivates awareness and insight into your own emotional dynamics. In doing so,

one is able to liberate themselves from unpleasant mental states (Ekman, Davidson, Ricard, &

Wallace, 2005).

2.05 Differences between Traditional and Modern Practices

There are a number of differences between what is employed in modern psychology and that

of the Buddhist tradition. Although the conceptual understanding of the practices is similar, there

are differences in terms of the functional application. For example, while mindfulness in the

Buddhist approach places emphasis on all four foundations as outlined by the Satipatthana Sutta,

modern application of mindfulness in psychology deemphasizes the fourth foundation and focuses

primarily on the first three. It is possible that this is due to the fact that the fourth foundation is

Page 75: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

69 intrinsically related to the Buddha’s teaching and has a substantial Buddhist ‘flavour.’ As such, it is

less accepted in the modern, secular application of mindfulness treatment. Related to this point,

rather than considering mindfulness as independent practice as modern day treatment employs,

Buddhist’s approach to mindfulness is that it is one part of a vast system of spiritual practices.

These practices, such as maintaining precepts, and study of the doctrines of Buddhism, needs to be

practiced together with mindful meditation.

Secondly, the Buddhist practice of mindfulness has a clear goal of reaching jhana with

intention of progressing through the various levels of it. Targets are also set so that the

concentration that is attained through jhana is used to develop into further wisdom through other

meditation. Modern practice however, does not consider jhana to be a specific goal as such but

rather focuses simply on the calming of the mind from the torrents of thoughts and emotions. In

doing so, the separation of a person’s emotional reaction to a given situation will be decoupled from

their cognitive or behavioural habits which in turn reduces the effect of the emotion on the

individual.

Lastly, early Buddhist practices of mindfulness are focused on the introspective aspect of

meditation that emphasizes on the mental dynamics, processing and reactions. In contrast, modern

approach takes a broader view and includes physical sensory input. The difference between

Buddhist and modern approach is that Buddhist focuses on how the mind reacts to such sensory

input while modern practices includes focusing on features of the sensory objects themselves

(Keng, Smoski & Robin, 2011). This difference is likely due to the fact that modern practices are

based on more tangible problems and issues such as events that leads to emotional reactions and/or

depressive relapses rather than a more abstract spiritual cultivation from which final liberation of

suffering can be reached.

2.06 Future of Mindfulness Research

Although few will doubt that research in mindfulness has passed its infancy stage, it is also

important to acknowledge that there is still a long way to go before our understanding of

mindfulness based treatments reaches maturity. Although mindfulness based interventions have

enjoyed a certain level of success and a number of different models of the phenomena of

mindfulness have been developed, there are still matters of what variables can affect the models that

has been laid out. By looking at the variables in which modern psychology have come to perceive

as being relevant to the operation to the mind, the phenomena of mindfulness can be integration into

our paradigm of knowledge of the mind in a more holistic manner. Not only will this yield greater

Page 76: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

70 levels of intellectual understanding, but more importantly, we can identify specifically what can

limit the efficacy of mindfulness interventions, giving rise to the possibility of further refinement

and customization of this approach.

CHAPTER 3

Rationale for Present Study

3.01 Theoretical Approach

Given the theoretical account of vulnerability to depression, mindfulness interventions have

been put forward as a treatment of depression. The rationale for this is that the practice of

mindfulness meditation impedes the cognitive processes that maintain rumination. However, it can

be speculated that mindfulness operates to reduce vulnerability to depression by redirecting

attention away from negative thinking and rumination. That is, mindfulness increases an

individual’s control over what they attend. No study thus far has looked into how mindfulness, as a

trait, affects their likelihood to ruminate through their AC capacity.

Page 77: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

71

Having an understanding of the connection between emotions and attention allows for the

introduction of an additional variable in the rumination cycle – attention allocation and control. The

original DAH model included depressive mood and dysfunctional attitude. Evidence has shown that

an individual’s emotions are intricately linked with AC capacity, with emotions being regarded as

one of the prominent factors in the prioritization of attention allocation (Compton, 2003). What this

means is that when a person’s emotion is aroused, the individual’s attention will be focused onto the

event that triggered the arousal. It has been found that people who suffer depression have difficulty

with AC. Indeed as noted previously, one of the diagnostic items of depressive disorder is the

impaired ability to concentrate or indecisiveness. Among adolescents, as much as 70% of those with

major depression experienced impaired concentration and/or attention (Goodyer & Cooper, 1993).

Rumination occurs when an individual focuses attention on a particular subject in their inner world

and can’t shift attention. This is most likely when depressive mood is high. Studies have supported

the theory that ruminators exhibit attentional inflexibility (Davies & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000) and

have trouble with switching attention away from one way of thinking to another (Grant, Thase &

Sweeney, 2001) as they are shown to perseverate in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. Indeed,

Watkins and Brown (2002) have demonstrated that rumination may worsen general executive

performance while other studies have shown ruminators to have an inability to consciously inhibit

their ongoing tasks (Whitmer & Banichm 2007). In addition, evidence points to depressed or FD

people having a preference to negative information and stimuli. For example, Ingram and Ritter

(2000) employed audio tasks in which participants were exposed to a story in one ear while an

emotionally charged distracter in the other. While ND people did not show any biases towards any

of the distracters, FD participants displayed a clear bias towards the negative adjectives after

negative moods were induced. Rinck and Becker (2005) also demonstrated attentional biases to

negative stimuli. Such findings suggest that people suffering from depression have trouble

controlling their thoughts as well as redirecting their attention elsewhere. In fact, one theory

regarding why ruminative thinking styles can contribute to the maintenance of clinical depression is

because the patient is unable to switch attention to a less dysfunctional thinking pattern (Wilkinson

& Goodyer, 2006).

While research to date has been implied that attention and (more specifically) AC is critical

to the rumination process, no empirical evidence has thus far surfaced that links an individual’s AC

capacity to both these variables as well as their rumination levels. It is possible that the ability to

control attention is critical for moving attention away from negative thought contents, impeding the

activation of DA, and thus lowering the tendency to ruminate.

Page 78: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

72

How mindfulness influences this process is unknown. Although it has been proposed that

the efficacy of mindfulness lies in its ability to decouple the link between depressive mood and DA,

the relationship between mindfulness and AC capacity and how this relationship affects the overall

rumination process, has not been explored. Mindfulness includes the capacity to disengage attention

from the ongoing stream of consciousness and redirect attention as well as the capacity to be aware

of the person’s own thoughts. Thus it seems likely that people high in trait mindfulness would be

better at noticing that their attention is being directed towards negative thinking and better able to

redirect their attention towards more positive thinking.

Figure 6. Sequential Meditation of Negative Affect and Rumination

This study is based on the theoretical development of the DAH but contributing to it by

including the variable of AC capacity. However, although theoretically the model is cyclic, the

DAH was originally conceived with the premise of negative thoughts leading to negative emotions

being an empirically established relationship rather than a theoretical one. As such, the cyclic nature

of that approach will not be examined but rather only the relationships between depressive mood,

AC, DA and mindfulness will be investigated. The model that is being tested here will shed light to

a more detailed understanding of how and why people are vulnerable to depression on a conceptual

level as well as what role mindfulness may play in the overall picture.

3.02 Hypothesis

Hypothesis 1: Depression and rumination levels will be positively correlated

with DA and NA, while negatively correlated with AC levels.

Hypothesis 2: Trait mindfulness scores will be negatively correlated with

depression, Rumination, DA, and Sadness while positively correlated with AC.

Hypothesis 3: The relationship between Rumination and Sadness will be

sequentially mediated by AC and DA.

Hypothesis 4: The relationship between Sadness and AC will be mediated by

one or all facets of mindfulness.

Attention Control

Negative Affect

Dysfunctional Attitude

Rumination

Page 79: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

73

CHAPTER 4

Methodology

4.01 Participants

A sample of 392 adults were recruited for the study. The majority (75%) were female and

ranged in age from 16 to 48 (n = 19.95, S.D. = 5.12). Cultural background of the participants were

primarily Anglo (n = 179), Oceanic (n = 66), Confucian (n = 49), South Asian (n = 41), Eastern

European (n = 18), and Germanic European (n = 11) with no other cultural group having more than

10 participants. Religious affiliation of subjects were mainly to Christianity (n = 204), Atheism (n =

114), Buddhism (n = 27) and Agnostic (14) with no other groups having more than 10 participants.

4.02 Procedure

Participants for this study were recruited from three sources.

Page 80: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

74

Undergraduate students from the Schools of Psychology at the University of Adelaide and

Queensland were recruited via the institution’s respective online research participant platform

websites. After logging on to a website hosting the online survey, a link appeared that directed the

participants to the start page of the online survey. Two online survey packages were used to conduct

the online survey, specifically researchmonkey.net for the Adelaide participants and Qualtrics for

the UQ and social media participants.

The link was spread virally through social networks starting from the researcher’s own

personal contacts. Social network sites such as Facebook were employed to reach participants that

the researcher did not personally know. Participants were encouraged to send the link to other

contacts. Participants were able to log on at any time to complete the surveys. The undergraduate

student participants received allotted course credits for their participation. Completion of all

questionnaires in this study took approximately 1 hour.

4.03 Measures

Depression. The Depression & Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-42; Lovibond & Lovibond,

1995) is a 42 item self report questionnaire consisting of three sub-scales measuring depression,

anxiety and stress. Items are rated on a 4-level Likert scale. Responses to each item are scored, 0

(did not apply to me at all), 1 (applied to me to some degree, or some of the time), 2 (applied to me

to a considerable degree, or a good part of time) and 3 (applied to me very much, or most of the

time). It was designed for examining three subscale, The DASS-42 has been tested with a large non-

clinical sample (N=2914) which found that reliability, (Cronbach’s alpha), was acceptable for all

three subscale (0.91 for depression, 0.84 for anxiety and 0.90 for stress). A clinical sample yielded

similar results (Antony et al, 1998It was found that the three-factor model was the optimal fit.

Interpretation of the data is based primarily on the use of cut-off, scores which range from ‘normal’

to ‘extremely severe’ on the basis of percentile scores. 0–78 is classified as ‘normal’, 78–87 as

‘mild’, 87–95 as ‘moderate’, 95–98 as ‘severe’, and 98–100 as ‘extremely severe’. The shortened

DASS-21 version will be used for this study. The DASS-21 halves the number of questions of the

DASS-42 but totals for each scale are doubled, so that score are comparable to those for the larger

instrument. The internal reliability of the DASS-21 is good with Cronbach's alphas of 0.94 for

Depression, 0.87 for Anxiety, and 0.91 for Stress (Antony et al., 1998).

Rumination. The Rumination on Sadness Scale (RSS; Conway et al, 2000) is a 13 item self

report questionnaire that measures depressive rumination levels. Items are rated on a 5-level Likert

scale, 1 (not at all), 2 (rarely), 3 (sometimes), 4 (quite a bit) and 5 (very much). Total scores on the

Page 81: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

75 RSS ranged from 13 to 60. The internal reliability of the RSS has been reported to be good with a

Cronbach’s alpha of 0.91 (Conway et al, 2000). Test–retest reliability over a 2-to 3-week period

was 0.70. Later studies such as Roelof et al (2006) also replicated supportive results for the RSS in

these areas. The convergent and discriminant validity of the scale was also supported. There has

been mixed findings regarding gender differences of the RSS. While Mezulis, Abramson, & Hyde,

(2002) reported that there was differences, the study conducted by Roelof et al (2006) did not.

Dysfunctional Attitude. Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS) is a 40 item self report

questionnaire that measures dysfunctional attitude levels. Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale.

It was designed to measure cognitive distortions especially those in relation to the aetiology of

depression. The theoretical foundation of the DAS was Beck’s cognitive therapy model in which is

based on 7 major value systems including Approval, Love, Achievement, Perfectionism,

Entitlement, Omnipotence, and Autonomy. Responses to each item are given in the form of score

including 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree). Total scores on the DAH ranged from 40 to 280 if

no questions are omitted. If there are any items that are not answered, a score of 0 is given. Early

reports on its internal consistency coefficients gave the range of 0.79 to 0.93 for a university

population (Weissman & Beck, 1978; Weissman, 1980; Dobson & Breiter, 1983). Early reliability

testing for its test-retest reliabilities ranged from 0.84 for a period of two month (Weissman, 1980)

and 0.74 for period of three month (Kuiper, Olinger & Air 1985) among nonclinical university

student population. People who are depressed individuals were found to have higher scores in the

DAS compared to the non-depressed population (Gotlib, 1984). In addition, the predictive

capabilities of the DAS with future depressive episode have also been with acceptable parameter

(Rholes, Riskind, & Neville, 1985).

Depressive Mood (Sadness) & Attention Control. Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ-

Short form; Evans & Rothbart, 2007) that measures the temperament of adults. Items are rated on a

7-level Likert scale with an option of not applicable for each item. Reponses ranges from 1

(extremely untrue of you) to 7 (extremely true for you). Discounting any ‘non-applicable’

responses, total score ranges from 77 to 539. Based on the temperament Rothbart model of 4

temperament dimensions and 13 subdimensions, it demonstrates strong reliability with all

dimensions having a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.80 or higher, and only one scale was lower than 0.70

(inhibitory control at 0.66). The negative affect factor score was highly correlated with Big Five

Neuroticism (rD.74), orienting sensitivity with Big Five Intellect/Openness (rD.65), temperamental

Extraversion/Surgency with Big Five Extraversion (rD.67), and Affiliativeness with Big Five

Agreeableness (rD.69). The Effortful Control factor score was highly correlated with Big Five

Page 82: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

76 conscientiousness (rD.64), while also having a substantial negative correlation (rD¡.41) with Big

Five Neuroticism. For the purposes of this study, not the entire dimension of Negative Affectivity

will be employed. Only the Attention Control subscale of the Effortful Control and Sadness from

the Negative Affect dimension will be included in this study. As this is a relatively new instrument

in the discipline, there has yet to be independent studies looking specifically at the psychometric

properties of the ATQ.

Mindfulness. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al, 2006) was

developed to measure five dimensions of mindfulness within an individual. Items are rated on a 39

items and employ a 5-item Likert scale measuring from 1 (Never or very rarely true) to 5 (Always

true). Scores ranges from 39 to 195 with higher scores representing higher level of trait

mindfulness. FFMQ can be subdivided into five different facets of mindfulness including

Nonreactivity, Observing, Acting with Awareness, Describing and Nonjudging. Items can also be

calculated as a whole as a Global scale score. The FFMQ has acceptable internal consistency, with

Cronbach’s alphas values between 0.75 and 0.91 for the various facets with the Global scale having

a Cronbach’s alphas of 0.87. Test-retest reliability is also strong as differences between tests were

not significant.

4.03 Ethical Consideration

Participants were provided with information on the front-page of the survey before

proceeding to questionnaires. Participants were informed that they were free to withdraw from the

study at any point without penalty.

4.04 Analysis & Reporting of Results

All data analysis was performed using SPSS 17 with the add-in statistics program called

Process (Hayes, 2013).

H1 was analysed using one-tail bivarate correlations including Sadness; along with

Attention Control, Dysfunctional Attitude, Overall Depression and Rumination.

H2 was analysed using one-tail bivarate correlations including Sadness, Attention Control,

Dysfunctional Attitude, and the subscales of FFMQ including Observing, Describing, Acting with

Awareness, Nonjudging and Nonreactivity. In addition, an overall score of Trait Mindfulness was

taken by averaging the other subscales together and also used for the calculations.

Page 83: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

77

H3 was analysed using sequential mediation analysis. To minimize indirect effect, a

bootstrap approach was employed and with the data re-sampled 5000 times. Sequential mediation

(model 6) of the SPSS add-in program Process was used to perform the analysis.

Figure 7. Conceptual Diagram of Model 6

The sequential mediation analysis was conducted with Sadness as the IV and Rumination

as the DV. The output of interest includes the regression of the IV and DV with and without the

mediating variables, as well as the regression level between the IV and MV1, MV1 and MV2, as

well as MV2 and DV. In order for the mediation effect to be considered valid, three criteria must

be satisfied.

1. The regression between the IV and DV decrease in predictive power after the mediating

variables has been included.

2. Each of the ‘links’ of the sequential mediation are significant.

3. The regression between the IV and DV is shown to be mediated by the both of the added

variables. In order to determine this, all values between the lower and upper bound bias

confidence interval must not include 0.

H4 was analysed using mediation analysis. Only IVs that satisfied all criteria were included

in the analysis to test H4. Sequential mediation (model 4) of the SPSS add-in program Process

(Hayes, 2013) was used to perform the analysis.

Mediating Variable 1

Independent Variable

Mediating Variable 2

Dependent Variable

Mediating Variable 1

Independent Variable

Dependent Variable

Page 84: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

78

Figure 8. Conceptual Diagram of Model 4

Analyses were conducted only if results from the previous analysis satisfy all three stated

criteria. MV1 will be set as the four subscales of mindfulness including, Observing, Acting with

Awareness, Describing and Nonjudging, Nonreactivity; as well as the Overall Trait Mindfulness

variable. Attention Control will be set as the DV. In order for the mediation effect to be considered

valid, the same three criteria of H3 must be satisfied.

Page 85: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

79

CHAPTER 5

Results

5.01 Hypothesis 1 (H1). Depression and rumination levels will be

positively correlated with DA and Sadness (negative affect) , and negatively correlated with

AC levels.

The descriptive statistics and correlations between the measured variables related to

rumination in this study are presented in Table 1. Rumination scores were comparable to those

found in undergraduate samples by Conway, Csank, Holm and Blake (2000). Sadness

and Attention Control scores were similar to

non-clinical samples of Evan and Rothbart

(2007). Table 1 Descriptive Statistics & Correlations of Rumination-Related Variables

Overall

Depression (DASS-21)

Rumination (RSS)

Sadness (ATQ)

Attention Control (ATQ)

Dysfunctional Attitude

(DAS) Mean 14.51 34.67 4.33 3.80 123.95 S.D 10.14 11.74 .90 .93 32.69 Depression (DASS21) .533** .278** -.169** .314** Rumination (RSS) .225** -.176** .198** Sadness (ATQ) -.166** .059 Attention Control (ATQ) -.145** **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (1-tailed).

In support of Hypothesis 1, Depression as measured by the DASS-21 was positively

correlated with Sadness (r = .28, p < .001) of the ATQ as well as Dysfunctional Attitude scores (r =

.31, p < .001). In addition, the Depression score of the DASS-21 was negatively correlated with the

Attention Control (r = -.17, p < .001) subscale of the Effortful Control dimension of the ATQ.

Page 86: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

80

Also in support of Hypothesis 1, Rumination was positively correlated with the Sadness

subscale of the ATQ (r = .23, p < .001) as well as the Dysfunctional Attitude score (r = .20, p <

.001). In addition, the Rumination score was negatively correlated to the Attention Control (r = -.18,

p < .001) subscale of the ATQ.

5.02 Hypothesis 2 (H2). Trait mindfulness scores will be negatively

correlated with depression, rumination, DA, and NA, and positively correlated with AC

Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics of each of the subscales of the FFMQ as well as their

correlation with depression, rumination, DA, Sadness and AC. All facets of mindfulness scores

were comparable to the mean scores of undergraduate samples found in the study conducted by

Baer, Carmody and Hunsinger (2012).

With the exception of Observing, all subscales of FFMQ, along with Overall Trait

Mindfulness, were significantly correlated with the variables. Of the significant correlations found,

the strongest was between Attention Control and Acting with Awareness (r = .52, p < .001) and the

weakest is between Overall Depression score with Nonreactivity (r = -.10, p = .023)

Table 2

Descriptive statistics of Mindfulness Facets and correlations with Negative Affects, Attention

Control, Dysfunctional Attitude, Depression and Rumination

Observing Describing Acting with Awareness

Nonjudging Nonreactivity Overall Trait Mindfulness

Mean 22.35 24.05 27.38 26.22 16.69 23.34

S.D 5.74 6.48 5.97 7.26 4.79 5.43

Overall Depression

.086 -.107* -.317** -.366** -.102* -.316**

Rumination .117* -.036 -.322** -.409** -.118* -.303**

Sadness .116* -.085 -.164** -.355** -.234** -.274**

Attention Control

-.077 .276** .523** .239** .192** .430**

Dysfunctional Attitude

-.012 -.070 -.237** -.213** .01 -.210**

*. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Page 87: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

81 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

5.03 Hypothesis 3 (H3). The relationship between Sadness (negative

affectivity) and rumination levels is mediated by two variables of attention control capacity

and dysfunctional attitude.

Hypothesis 3 was tested with a mediation model using estimates based on 5,000 bootstrap

samples (Preacher & Hayes, 2008).

When Attention Control and Dysfunctional Attitude were entered as mediating variables of

the relationship between Sadness (negative affectivity) and Rumination, Sadness was found to be a

significant predictor of Attention Control (b = -.17, p < .001), Attention Control was found to be a

significant predictor of Dysfunctional Attitude (b = -3.58, p = .043) and Dysfunctional Attitude was

found to be significant predictor of Rumination (b = .07, p < .001).

The point estimate for the direct effect of the Sadness equated to 2.5 (SE =.63, p < .001);

indicating that Sadness continues to exert a direct effect on Attention Control in spite of the

mediating variables’ effect. The point estimate for the sequential indirect effect via AC and DA was

.04 (SE =.03).

The 95% Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect of the model

that included both mediating variables did not include zero [04, .14], indicating that the indirect

effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .05. Thus, as predicted, the effect of Sadness on

Rumination was mediated by both Attention Control and Dysfunctional Attitude.

Page 88: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

82

5.04 Hypothesis 4 (H4). Trait Mindfulness will mediate the

relationship between depressive mood and attention control capacity.

Hypothesis 4 was tested with a mediation model using estimates based on 5,000 bootstrap

samples (Preacher & Hayes, 2008).

5.04.1 Overall Trait Mindfulness. When Overall Trait Mindfulness was

entered as a mediator for the relationship between Sadness and Attention Control, Sadness was not

found to be a significant predictor of Overall Trait Mindfulness (b = -.05, p = .274). Although

Overall Trait Mindfulness (b = .02, p < .001) was found to be significant predictor of Attention

Control.

The point estimate for the indirect effect of Overall Trait Mindfulness was -.12 (SE =.03).

The 95% Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect did not include zero

[-.18, -.07], indicating the indirect effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .05. As such, the

effect of Sadness on Attention Control Capacity was mediated by Overall Trait Mindfulness.

5.04.2 Observing. When Observing was entered as a mediator for the relationship

between Sadness and Attention Control, Sadness was found to be significant predictor for

Observing (b = .74, p = .021) but Observing was not found to be a significant predictor of Attention

Control (b = -.01, p = .245).

The point estimate for the indirect effect of Observing was -.07 (SE =.01). The 95% Bias

Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect did include zero [-.03, .00],

indicating the indirect effect was not significantly different from 0 at p = .05. As such, the effect of

Page 89: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

83 Sadness on Attention Control Capacity was not mediated by Observing.

5.04.3 Describing. When Describing was entered as a mediator for the

relationship between Sadness and Attention Control, Sadness was not found to be a significant

predictor of Describing (b = -.29, p = .093) but Describing was found to be a significant predictor of

Attention Control (b = -.04, p < .001).

The point estimate for the indirect effect of Describing was -.02 (SE =.01). The 95% Bias

Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect did include zero [-.058, .002],

indicating the indirect effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .05. As such, the effect of

Sadness on Attention Control Capacity was not mediated by Describing.

5.04.4 Acting with Awareness. When Acting with Awareness was entered

as a mediator for the relationship between Sadness and Attention Control, Sadness was found to be

a significant predictor of Acting with Awareness (b = -1.08, p < .001) and Acting with Awareness

was found to be a significant predictor of Attention Control (b = .08, p < .001).

The point estimate for the direct effect of Sadness was -.08 (SE =.04, p = .059). This

suggests Sadness continues to exert a direct effect on Attention Control in spite of the mediating

variable’s effect. The point estimate for the indirect effect of Acting with Awareness equated to -.09

(SE =.03). The 95% Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect did not

include zero [-.15, -.03], indicating the indirect effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .050.

As such, the effect of Sadness on Attention Control Capacity was mediated by Acting with

Awareness.

5.04.5 Nonjudging of Inner Experiences. When Nonjudging was

entered as a mediator for the relationship between Sadness and Attention Control, Sadness was

found to be a significant predictor of Nonjudging (b = -2.81, p < .001) and Nonjudging was found

to be a significant predictor of Attention Control (b = .03, p < .001).

The point estimate for the direct effect of Sadness was -.09 (SE =.05, p = .077). Meaning

Sadness continues to exert a direct effect on Attention Control in spite of the mediating variable’s

effect. The point estimate for the indirect effect of Nonjudging equated to -.07 (SE =.02). The 95%

Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect did not include zero [-.13, -

.04], indicating the indirect effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .05. As such, the effect

of Sadness on Attention Control Capacity was mediated by Nonjudging.

Page 90: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

84

5.04.6 Nonreacting of Inner Experiences. When Nonreacting was

entered as a mediator for the relationship between Sadness and Attention Control, Sadness was

found to be a significant predictor of Nonreacting (b = -1.21, p < .001) and Nonreacting was found

to be a significant predictor of Attention Control (b = .03, p = .001).

The point estimate for the direct effect of Sadness was -.13 (SE =.05, p = .012), suggesting

that Sadness no longer had a significant direct effect on Attention Control after the mediating

variable was added. The point estimate for the indirect effect of Nonreacting equated to -.03 (SE

=.02). The 95% Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect did not

include zero [-.08, -.01], indicating the indirect effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .05.

As such, the effect of Sadness on Attention Control Capacity was mediated by Nonreacting.

5.05 Post-hoc Analysis. Independent mediating effect of Attention Control and Dysfunction

Attitude of the relationship between Sadness (negative affect) and Rumination

Given that the sequential mediating effect of Attention Control

and Dysfunction Attitude was only .04, post-hoc analyses were

performed to assess the mediating effect of each variable

individual to determine which had a greater effect.

5.05.01 Attention Control. When AC was entered as a mediator for the

relationship between Sadness and Rumination, Sadness was found to be a significant predictor of

AC (b = 2.62, p < .001) and AC was found to be a significant predictor of Rumination (b = 1.80, p

= .004).

The point estimate for the direct effect of Sadness equated to 2.62 (SE =.65, p < .000). This

means that Sadness continues to exert a direct effect on Rumination in spite of the mediating

variable’s effect. The point estimate for the indirect effect of AC equated to .31 (SE =.16). The 95%

Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect (mediated) effect of the model that included AC

as a mediating variable did not include zero [03, .84], indicating the indirect effect was significantly

different from 0 at p = .05, indicating that the effect of Sadness on Rumination was mediated by

Attention Control.

5.05.02 Dysfunctional Attitude. When DA was entered as a mediator of

the relationship between Sadness and Rumination, Sadness was not found to be a significant

Page 91: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

85 predictor of DA (b = 2.12, p = .243) but DA was found to be a significant predictor of Rumination

(b =.07, p < .001).

The point estimate for the direct effect of Sadness equated to -.09 (SE = 2.79, p = .063),

indicating Sadness does not exert a direct effect on Rumination. The point estimate for the indirect

effect of DA was .14 (SE =.14). The 95% Bias Corrected confidence intervals for the indirect

(mediated) effect of the model that included AC as a mediating variable did include zero [-.08, .45],

indicating the indirect effect was significantly different from 0 at p = .05. As such, the effect of

Sadness on Rumination was not mediated by Dysfunctional Attitude.

Overall, the pattern of results supported the hypotheses that an individual’s trait mindfulness

mediated the relationship between their emotions and AC capacity. Not all facets of mindfulness

however had this mediating effect. Only the facets Nonjudgment and Nonreactivity to Inner

Experiences would have a significant effect.

CHAPTER 6

Discussion

6.01 Discussion

The aim of the current research was to investigate the validity of a theoretical framework for

understanding the operation of mindfulness by combining several known related variables including

AC and DA into a single model. Specifically, the study hoped to replicate previous findings with

regards to the relationship between depression, Sadness, Rumination, DA, AC and mindfulness. In

keeping with previous research, depression was found to be positively correlated with rumination,

DA and Sadness; and negatively correlated with AC and overall mindfulness levels.

The second aim of this study was to investigate whether attention control capacity would

mediate the relationship between negative Affect and Rumination. Some level of support was found

for this as sequential mediating effect of the AC and DA were found for the relationship between

Sadness and Rumination. An unexpected finding was that the strength of the effect was low. A

possible reason for this is possibly the community-based population sample that participated in this

study, which will be discussed further in a later section. Lastly, this study looked into how different

dimensions of mindfulness mediate the relationship between Sadness and Attention Control

capacity. Results show that not all traits of mindfulness mediated the relationship between an

Page 92: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

86 individual’s degree of Sadness and their AC capacity. Specifically, it is adopting a Nonjudgmental

attitude and the capacity of Nonreactivity to one’s own inner experience that have the effect were

the only significant mediators.

The relationship between depressive mood and DA as two primary components of

rumination has been firmly established in previous literature (see chapter 1). However, this study

contributes to existing knowledge by looking at the possibility of a rumination system that

incorporates AC capacity as a mediating variable. In support of the primary hypothesis of this study

(H3), Attention Control and Dysfunctional Attitude mediated the relationship between Sadness and

Rumination. This is a significant finding as it implies that unlike the rumination model established

by the ICS which includes only depressive mood and DA, an individual’s innate attention control

capacity is also an intricate part of the rumination process. Of interest was the degree of the

correlation decrease between Sadness and Rumination after the mediating variables were included.

The overall effect of the sequential mediation was surprisingly small (b = 0.04) and thus although

the mediating effect was significant, the question why the effect was small needs explaining. After

looking at the post-hoc analysis, DA did not mediate the relationship between Sadness and

Rumination. This is inconsistent with previous studies of DA being an intrinsic part of the

connection between depressive mood and Rumination. It is possible that this outcome is due to the

sampling method employed, a point discussed in more detail below.

The cyclic nature of the rumination cycle implies that rather than just DA being the direct

predictor of rumination, others variables are also important in predicting rumination. It is the

operation of the cycle that results in what we conceptualize as being rumination. According to the

DAH, rumination is a process involving the components of DA, negative thought content and

depressive mood supporting each other. However, this approach fails to take into account the effect

of an individual’s capacity to control how attention is directed. Although it is commonly accepted

that depressive mood is related to DA, a person’s ability to control their attention would regulate

how strongly DA influences the person. If a person has greater ability to direct their attention

towards or away from their feelings, then they will be less likely to engage in DA. This in turn

would lessen the chances of more negative feelings being generated by those thoughts. Because this

process operates in a self-perpetuating cycle, affecting any part of this cycle would affect all the

variables that are related to rumination.

.

Page 93: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

87

Figure 9. Schematic representation rumination model

Taking into account the role emotions play on attention allocation that is supported by

previous studies both conceptually and empirically, we can infer that the rumination cycle contains

at least four components. The original relationship includes DA, the negative thought content that

arises from DA, as well the negative and depressive feelings that arise from these cognitions.

However, the results of this study indicate than rather than the depressive mood giving rise to DA

directly, this relationship is actually mediated by the individual’s AC Capacity.

Through this model, it is logical to understand why attentional inflexibility is a symptom in

ruminators. As each element of the depressive rumination cycle continues to reinforce each other,

attention is held by an intricate component of that cycle, namely Sadness. Therefore, the process of

rumination in itself is feeding the maintenance of attention on the ruminated topic. Due to the

presence of the DA, the content will always be negative, generating more depressive emotion and

again feeding the attentional hold.

There are clinical implications of this study with regard to how treatment can be

approached. One way of looking at the inability to draw attention away from a particular line of

thinking is focusing on the AC deficits within the ruminating individual rather than the frequency

and intensity of negative emotions. The clinical implication of this is that developing higher levels

of AC capacity may decrease an individual’s tendency to ruminate. Traditionally CBT has been

limited to teaching strategies that aim to reducing the DA and the negative thought content of

people who ruminate. While decreasing the severity of the components of the cycle no doubt works

during acute treatment, leaving the associative links intact means that if any one component is

triggered this will set the whole cycle back into motion. Indeed, this may mean that the slowed

ability to switch attention can undermine the therapeutic process of CBT during the acute

Page 94: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

88 depression phase (March, et al., 2004). The key therefore, is to disrupt the cycle itself through

weakening the associative links between the components, and one way to do this is through learning

how to control attention in order that the person is better able to direct attention away from negative

thoughts when experiencing negative affect.

Although there has been considerable theoretical development with regard to the conceptual

understand of mindfulness, there are still a number of questions that have yet to be answered. One

such question is with regard to the phenomenology of mindfulness. Specifically, in what way does

mindfulness affect and change an individual’s disposition to ruminate. In theory, mindfulness is a

practice that targets the rumination process described by the Differential Activation Hypothesis

(DAH) as proposed by Teasdale (1988). On the level of the specific mental mechanisms, the theory

suggests emotional arousal, or affective reactivity, triggers an individual’s mind to engage in

rumination. However, the specific connection between affect and emotion, with the mind’s

tendency to elaborate, dramatize and analyse a given topic has never been satisfactorily explained.

The critical connection relevant to this study is the empirically supported relationship

between attention and emotion. As outlined in chapter 1, regardless of the type of emotions, any

form of affective reactivity to both external and internal stimuli activates the focusing of a person’s

attention to those stimuli. The more personal-stake one has to the stimuli, event or topic, the more

likely that attention will be allocated to it. Therefore, rather than saying that depressive mood leads

to rumination, it may be better to imply that negative emotions activate the allocation of attention to

that topic, and through the process of mental contents being influenced by DA, more depressive

mood are generated, leading to the attention being held in that place. The result of this process is

conventionally called ‘rumination’. Once an individual is locked into the rumination cycle, it is

unlikely the person can extract themselves from it unless some form of distraction occurs.

Of special interest is that the three key variables measured in this study, Sadness, DA and

Rumination, were not significantly correlated to Mindful Describing. This lack of correlation may

be due to their conceptual heterogeneity. Describing, the process of verbalizing experiences, is one

that has little relation to these constructs. Neither Sadness (an emotional reaction one experiences as

a result of environmental triggers), or Dysfunction Attitude (the cognitive process in which

associates a sensory stimuli or thought to a negative possibility or outcome), overlaps conceptually

with one ability to verbalize physical sensation. As noted previously in chapter 1, these two are the

key components of the rumination cycle. It is therefore reasonable to deduce that the lack of

correlation between them and the facet of Describing logically leads to the lack of correlation

between Describing and Rumination.

Page 95: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

89

Having established that the mindfulness facet of Describing is unrelated to the Sadness

subscale in the results of the second hypothesis, the lack of correlation here was unsurprising. What

may need addressed is the fact that neither the facet Nonjudging to Inner Experience, nor Acting

with Awareness were correlated with Sadness when used to mediate its relationship to AC. Both

variables being uncorrelated to Sadness as a mediating variable is unexpected. Firstly, neither one

truly overlaps heavily with an individual’s emotions on a conceptual level. Acting with Awareness

is a facet of mindfulness dealing with an individual’s focus when acting without being distracted.

Although this is both conceptually, and shown in this study, to be statistically related to the actual

attention itself, it is not in itself related to emotions that gave rise to the increase or decrease of the

individual’s AC. In addition, the questions within the FFMQ were not tailored to action that are

performed when one is experiencing sadness specifically, so the possible overlap with positive

affect based attention may have affected the results. Nonjudgemental is a construct that deals with

the self-referencing attitude of an individual. Therefore, it is likely that it this particular construct is

more related to the generated DA rather than experienced of emotion itself.

Nonreacting to Inner Experiences has been shown here to be a significant mediator of the

Sadness and AC. This supports the hypothesis of this study while at the same time also provides

important insights as to the how mindfulness operates with regards to attention. Within this model,

three components are needed in order to sustain or even exacerbate rumination; depressive mood,

(lack of) AC, and DA. As a psychological mechanism, depressive mood is not something that can

be altered in itself. Rather, what is altered are the triggers that causes depressive mood to arise.

These triggers are the negative thought content that, at least initially, are triggered by environmental

cues. What this study suggests is that people who score high on Nonreactivity do not react as much

to their emotional state, and thus attention (switching and focusing on the negative event) is not

activated, impeding the activation of DA.

Nonjudgement to Inner Experience was also found to be a significant mediator of Sadness

and AC. This is important as it implies that mindfulness’ effect on rumination is more than just a

matter of attention. Nonjudgement, as one of the pillars of mindfulness, is an attitude in one holds

when addressing either external environment or internal reactions. Given that emotions directly

influenced the allocation of attention, it is possible that holding a nonjudgemental attitude might

decrease the intensity of the experienced emotion and thus facilitate an individual’s ability to

control their attention.

Page 96: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

90

Figure 10. Schematic representation of the rumination model

with mindfulness effect

There are important implications about this with regards to the relationship between

mindfulness and attention. The conventional conceptualization of mindfulness is that mindfulness

allows the individual to shift to a different mode of the mind such as the Mindful

Experiencing/Being of the Interacting Cognitive Subsystem (ICS) by Teasdale (1999) or the Wise

Mind utilized by the Dialectical Behaviour Therapy. This shift often requires one to direct their

attention inwards so that the Sadness becomes the object of observation bringing about greater

understanding and the reprieving of a person’s own experiences. There is however, one problem

within this theory. Specifically, this act of shifting attention from external environmental issues to

internal mental events is effectively an attention switching task. However as mentioned in chapter

3.04.2, Chambers, Lo and Allen (2008) found evidence suggesting that as far as attention switching

capabilities are concerned, mindfulness meditation did not improve performance in attention-

switching tasks compared to those who did not take part in an 10-day retreat. Although the findings

of the study did not refer to this form of external-internal shift, it is the assumption of this study that

the two are, while may not equate to each other, is similar in nature. The present study seems to

support this theory that the attention portion of mindfulness is related but not identical to an

individual’s attention switching.

Attention switching is in effect an act that is composed of two parts: first the inhibition of

attention from what one was originally focused on and second, the reengagement of attention on the

Attention Depressive Mood

Dysfunctional Attitude

Negative Thought Content

Rumination Cycle

Mindfulness

Page 97: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

91 new object. It is the proposal of this thesis that rather than a complete switch in attention,

mindfulness increases the attention inhibition/disengagement capacity of the individual when faced

with emotionally arousing stimuli. It blocks the path to engaging in rumination as it does not stay

on that topic long enough for DAs to arise and affect the outcome of the thought process. In

addition to the evidence, the rationale behind this is that, although mindfulness practices do allocate

attention on an anchor or foundation of some sort, this is fundamentally a different form of

attention. Attention engagement, in this context, is seen as the type that is influenced and based on

affect arousal. These affects are the fundamental, pre-conscious, and immediate affect that are

normally either positive, negative, or neutral. As detailed in chapter 1.07, such feelings have self-

centred goal as its foundation. In contrast, a mindful state is one that is characterized as having a

perception where the ego-self are decoupled from whatever it is being perceived. As such, they are

not influenced by the same mental dynamics. This fundamental difference in the nature of engaged

attention and attention disengagement, is assumed here to be one of the phenomenological variables

of mindfulness and in turn, a component as to why mindfulness practice has enjoyed such success.

6.02 Methodological Issues

Although this study has provided concrete results from which supports most of the original

hypothesis, it nonetheless suffers from a number of mythological limitation. Primary of which is the

population sample that was selected for this study. The participants of this study were primarily

either first year undergraduate psychology students, or community sample that was recruited from

the researcher’s own social network. In both situation, the same already predisposed certain biases

to the results. For example, it is likely the majority of these participants were middle-class, well-

educated people who have a certain level of emotional and attention regulation capability. As such,

the data that was collected could have been skewed in this manner. The majority of the sample was

taken from undergraduate students who participated as a part of their studies. Indeed, all students

were given one course credit that they required in order to complete the course. The motivation for

taking part in the study may also have affected the data received. Similarly, the sample is

unrepresentative of both real generalized population, and has even less meaning for the clinical

samples that are actually suffering from depression. Due to this, it is possible that it affected the

connection between DA and Rumination in the form of lacking in significant correlation lack of

mediating effect on the relationship between Sadness and Rumination.

Survey based study is also an obstacle that should be addressed. Without actual experiment-

based data, the question of the validity of the results comes into question. Subjects may have,

Page 98: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

92 intentionally or not, altered their answers due to self-esteem issues, social identity, among other

possible reasons.

6.03 Future Research

This study has opened up many possible researches for the future. Firstly, this project has

provided support for the reconceptualization of rumination. It therefore sets the foundation from

which studies of the validation of this model can be conducted. Secondly, to address the

mythological problem of sampling, similar study should be conducted on a wider population

encompassing a greater range of demographics. Similarly, the sample can be more focused and

employed to a clinical sample so that the findings, and theories developed from these findings, can

be used to understand the operation of rumination of people who are dysfunctional in their daily

lives due to rumination. Although the theoretical framework of depressive rumination developed in

this study may be logically sound, and that the various links are supported by research findings, it

nonetheless looks at depression in it isolated form. As noted previously (section 1.04.01), there is a

relatively high chance that depression comes with other comorbid psychological disorders. An

interesting question here is, how do additional variables that are brought into play by these

comorbid disorders affect this process? Of the different disorders, personality disorders are of

primary interest in this study. In doing so, validity of the theories proposed as a result of this study

can have stronger empirical support.

Second, this study indicates that the role of attention disengagement during emotional

arousal is the cognitive mechanism in which mindfulness practice operates. As such, pro-post

mindfulness intervention time reaction experiments could be conducted as a mean to validate

whether an individual’s attention disengagement indeed increased as a result of mindfulness

practice. This can also involve the comparison of the data found in this study so to analyse what

variable can benefit or impede the efficacy of mindfulness interventions. From this, mindfulness

programs could be refined so to allow for specialization from which could achieve greater level of

effectiveness based on the specific mental composition of the patient.

6.04 Summery & Conclusion

Depression is among the most serious and urgent problems faced by the modern world. Key

reasons of this is both its debilitating effect on the day-to-day life of individuals leading to the

immense amount of society resources lost to it, as well as its ability to relapse even after treatment

has been considered successful in conventional therapy. The reason why it has this innate ability to

relapse in people is because depression fundamentally involves various mental characteristics that

Page 99: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

93 make the victim prune to ruminate. The key to managing, if not solving, the problem of depression

is knowing how rumination can be disrupted.

Early theories of rumination saw rumination as a single concept involving various

components such as maladaptive self-referencing thoughts that make a person feel worse. While the

idea of having negative thoughts will increase the sadness the individual may seem to be common

sense to many, the Differential Activation Hypothesis developed by Teasdale (1988) was among the

first to propose that sad emotions actually triggers negative thoughts through a process of selective

memory retrieval and filtered by DA, instigated by the emotion itself. Logical development of this

theory regards rumination as an interactive, self-perpetuating process involving multiple cognitive

mechanisms. Each component feeds each other in a cyclic loop where the victim mind is locked

within. Through running through this cycle, the symptoms of depression become gradually more

severe which lead to much greater difficulty in treatment and more discouraging prognosis.

However, the DAH does not address the issue of attentional control deficits of people with mood

disorders and how the connection between attention control and emotion is the key to holding the

focus of an individual on the thought process that drives the cycle of rumination. Results of this

study indicate that rumination is seen here as a cycle of Sadness, AC and dysfunctional thought

processes. With depression being a disorder in which negative mood is ever-present in the mind of

the individual, this study proposes that the two phenomena are linked. Further, AC deficit is an

inability to direct attention away from emotionally-charged mental states and thoughts. In

rumination, the subtle depressive mood generated by the thought process holds the individual’s

attention as opposed to emotionally neutral stimuli which the individual may otherwise be engaging

in. Therefore, even the slightest increase in stress level or experience of frustration or failure

through environmental factors may propel the individual down the rumination spiral.

This study also provides support to the idea that an individual’s trait mindfulness, which

could be enhanced through mindfulness practice, can mediate the effect of depressive mood on the

AC of the rumination process. When one increases their trait mindfulness, the AC development

comes in the form of attention disengagement that can result in the Nonreactivity to emotions.

Depression, rumination and mindfulness intervention has had a long and respected line of

research and empirical support. This study has contributed to ongoing efforts through further

refinement of the conceptualization of rumination and possible mechanism from which mindfulness

operates. This provides grounds in which later studies can build on so as to tackle various

theoretical issues that has yet been address, opening doors to possible cognitive experiments that

Page 100: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

94 allow us to understand mindfulness on a deeper level, as well as possible refinement of

contemporary mindfulness programs to tailor to the specific mental composition of the recipient.

References

Abramson, L.Y., Metalsky, G.I., & Alloy, L.B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-

based subtype of depression. Psychological Review. Vol.96

Ahadi, S.A., & Rothbart, M.K. (1994). Temperament, development, and the

Big Five. In C.F. Halverson., G.A. Kohnstamm, & R.P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure

of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc

Alloy, L.B., Abramson, L.Y., Whitehouse, W.G., Hogan, M.E., Panzarella, C., & Rose, D.T.

(2006). Prospective incidence of first onsets and recurrences of depression in individuals at high

and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.115(1)

Alpert, J.E., Fava, M., Uebelacker, L.A., Nierenberg, A.A., Pava, J.A., Worthington III, J.J.,

& Rosenbaum, J.F. (1999). Patterns of axis I comorbidity in early-onset versus late-onset major

depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. Vol.46

Page 101: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

95

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical manual for mental

disorders. 4th Ed. Washington.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Practice guideline for the treatment of patients

with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.157(4).

American Psychiatric Association. (2008). Practice guidelines for the treatment of

psychiatric disorders: compendium 2008. Washington, DC: Author.

Anastasi, A. (1990). Psychological testing. (6th ed.). New York: Macmillan.

Ansseau, M. (1997). The obsessive–compulsive personality; diagnostic aspects and

treatment possibilities. In J.A. den Boer, & H.G.M. Westenberg (Eds.), Obsessive–

compulsive spectrum disorders. Amsterdam: Syn-Thesis.

Antony, M.M., Bieling, P.J., Cox, B.J., Enns, M.W., & Swinson, R.P. (1998). Psychometric

Properties of the 42-Item and 21-Item Versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in Clinical

Groups and a Community Sample. Psychological Assessment. Vol.10(2).

Anti-Depression Association of Australia. (2010). ADAA. Retrieved 27th, June 2011, from

http://adaa.org.au/

Arntz, A. (1994). Treatment of Borderline Personality disorder: a challenger for cognitive-

behavioral therapy. Behavioral Research Therapy. Vol.32

Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2011). 27th, June 2011, from http://www.abs.gov.au/

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW). (2003). Australian hospital statistics

2001-02. Health Services Series no. 20. Cat. no. HSE. 25. Canberra: AIHW

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (AIHW). (2010). Mental health services in

Australia 2007-08. Mental Health Series. no. 12. Cat. no. HSE. 88. Canberra: AIHW

Avants, S.K., & Margolin, A. (2004). Development of spiritual self–schema therapy for the

treatment of addictive and HIV risk behaviour: A convergence of cognitive and Buddhist

psychology. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration. Vol.14(3)

Page 102: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

96

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self -

report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. Vol.13(1)

Ball, S.A. (2001). Reconceptualizing Personality Disorder Categories Using Personality

Trait Dimensions Introduction to Special Section. Journal of Personality. Vol.69(2)

Bargh, J.A., & Tota, M.E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic processing in depression:

accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology. Vol.54.

Bartak, A., Spreeuwenberg., D.I., Andrea, H., Busschbach, J.J.V., Croon, R.A., Verheul, R.,

Emmelkamp, P.M.G., & Stijnen, T. (2009). The use of propensity score methods in psychotherapy

research: A practical application. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. Vol.78.

Baxter, L.R., Schwartz, J.M., Mazziotta, J.C., Phelps, M.E., Pahl, J.J., Guze, B.H., &

Fairbanks, L. (1988). Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in nondepressed patients with obsessive-

compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.145

Beck, A.T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical aspects. New York:

Harper.

Beck, A.T., Freeman, A & Davis, D.D (Eds.) (2004) Cognitive Therapy for Personality

Disorder – Second Edition. The Guilford Press. New York. London.

Beevers, C.G., & Meyer, B. (2002). Lack of positive experiences and positive expectancies

mediate the relationship between BAS responsiveness and depression. Cognition and Emotion

Vol.16

Binks, C.A., Fenton, M., McCarthy, L., Lee, T., Adams, C.E., & Duggan, C. (2006)

Psychological therapies for people with borderline personality disorder. Cochrane Database

Systematic Review. Vol.25

Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N.D. Carmody, J., Segal, Z,V.,

Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004) Mindfulness: A proposed operational

definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. Vol.11(3).

Black, D.W., Bell, S., Hulbert, J., & Nasrallah, A. (1998) The importance of Axis II in

patients with major depression. Journal of Affective. Disorder. Vol.14.

Page 103: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

97

Blatt, S.J., & Shahar, G. (2004). Psychoanalysis: with whom, for what, and how?

comparisons with psychotherapy. Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association;

Bockting, C.L.H., Ten Doesschate, M.C., Spijker, J., Spinhoven, P., Koeter, M.W.J.,

Schene, A.H., & the DELTA Study Group. (2008). Continuation and maintenance antidepressant

use in recurrent depression. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. Vol.77.

Bradley, B.P. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety:

manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition & Emotion. Vol.12(6).

Brieger, P., Ehrt, U., & Marneros, A. (2003). Frequency of comorbid personality disorders

in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry. Vol.44.

Brieger, P., Röttig, S., Röttig, D., Marneros A., & Priebe, S. (2007). Dimensions underlying

outcome criteria in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Affective Disorder. Vol. 99.

Brodaty, H., Luscombe, G., Peisah, C., Anstey, K., & Andreas G. (2001). A 25-year

lognitudina, comparison study of outcome of depression. Psychological Medicine. Vol.31.

Broderick P. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: Contrasts with

rumination and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Research Vol.29

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its

role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.84.

Brugman, T., Ferguson, S. (2002). Physical Exercise and Improvements in Mental Health.

Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services. Vol.40.

Burke, K.C., Burke, J., Rae, D.S., Regier, D.A., (1991). Comparing age at onset of major

depression and other psychiatric disorders by birth cohorts in five US community populations.

Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.48.

Buchheld, N., Grossman, P., & Walach, H. (2001). Measuring mindfulness in insight

meditation (vipassana) and meditation-based psychotherapy: The development of the freiburg

mindfulness inventory (FMI). Journal for Meditation and Meditation Research. Vol.1(1),

Buss, A., & Plonin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of personality development. Wiley-

Interscience, London.

Page 104: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

98

Bylsma, L.M. (2008). Examining emotional reactivity to daily events in major and minor

depression. (Masters Thesis) Retrieved from http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd /155.

Carey, W.B. (1999). Problems in diagnosing attention and activity. Paediatrics. Vol.103.

Carlson, N.R., Heth, C.D., Miller, H., Donahoe, J.W., Buskist, W., Martin, G.N., Schmaltz,

R.M. (2010) Psychology: Science of Behavior - 4th Canadian Edition. Pearson Canada.

Caspi, A., Harrington, O., Milne, B., Amell, J.W., Theodore, R.F., & Moffitt, T.E. (2003).

Children’s behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. Journal

of Personality. Vol.71.

Cavedini, P., Erzegovesi, S., Ronchi, P., & Bellodi, L. (1996). Predictive value of obsessive-

compulsive personality disorder in antiobsessinal pharmacological treatment. European

Neuropsychopharmacology. Vol.7.

Chambers, R., Lo, B.C., & Allen, N.B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training

on attentional control, cognitive style and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research. Vol 32(3).

Charney, D.S., Nelson, J.C., & Quinlan, D.M. (1981). Personality traits and disorder in

depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.138.

Chew, R.H., Hales, R.E., & Yudofsky, S.C., (2009) What Your Patients Need to Know

About Psychiatric Medications. 2nd ed. Washington, DC.: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2009.

Ciesla J.A., & Roberts, J.E., (2002) Rumination, Negative Cognition, and Their Interactive

Effects on Depressed Mood. Emotion. Vol.7(3)

Clark, L.A. (1993). Manual for the schedule for nonadaptive and adaptive personality.

Minneapolis, MN, University of Minnesota Press.

Clark, L.A. (2005). Temperament as unifying basis for personality and psychopathology.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.114(4).

Clark, L.A. (2007). Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: perennial issues and

emerging reconceptualization. Annual Review in Psychology. Vol.58

Page 105: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

99

Clark, L.A., Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric

evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal Abnormal Psychology Vol.100

Clark, L.A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and the mood and

anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Special Issue: Personality and

Psychopathology. Vol.103.

Clark, L.A. Livesley, W.J., & Morey, L. (1997) Personality disorder assessment: The

challenge of contract validity. Journal of Personality Disorder. Vol.11

Clarkin, J. F., Foelsch, P. A., Levy, K. N., Hull, J. W., Delany, J. C., Kernbery, O. F. (2001).

The development of a psychodynamic treatment for patients with borderline personality disorder: A

preliminary study of behavioral change. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol.15

Clarkin, J.F., Levy, K.N., & Dammann, G.W. (2002) An Object-Relations Approach to the

Treatment of Borderline Patients. In F.W. Kaslow, & J.J. Magnavita, (Eds.) Comprehensive

Handbook of Psychotherapy. Volume 1. Psychodynamic/Object Relations. John Wiley & Sons, New

York.

Clarkin, J.F., Levy, K.N., Lenzenweger, M.F., & Kernberg, O.F. (2007). Evaluating Three

Treatments for Borderline Personality Disorder: A Multiwave Study. American Journal of

Psychiatry. Vol.164.

Cloninger, C.R., (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the

development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments. Vol.4(3):

Cloninger, C.R. (1998). The genetics and psychobiology of the seven-factor model of

personality. Silk, K.R (Ed.). Biology of personality disorders. Washington, DC, US: American

Psychiatric Association.

Cloninger, C.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Prezbeck, T.R. (1993). Integrative psychobiological

approach to psychiatric assessment and treatment. Archive of General Psychiatry. Vol.50

Cloninger, C.R., Przybeck, T.R, Svrakic, D.M., & Wetzel, R.D. (1994). The temperament

and character inventory. (TCI).: a guide to its development and use. St Louis, MO: Centre of

Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University.

Page 106: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

100

Coccaro, E.F., & Kavoussi, R.J. (1997) Fluoxetine and impulsive aggressive behavior in

personality-disordered subjects. Archive of General Psychiatry. Vol.54

Coffey, K. A., & Hartman, H. (2008). Mechanisms of action in the inverse relationship

between mindfulness and psychological distress. Complementary Health Practice Review. Vol.13

Compton, R. (2003). The Interface Between Emotion and Attention: A Review of Evidence

from Psychology and Neuroscience. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience review. Vol.2(2).

Conway, M., Csank, P., Holm, S., Blake, C., & Mendelson, M. (2000). A ruminative

response style to sadness mediates the impact of victimization on depression. Journal of Personality

and Assessment. Vol.75

Cornelius, J.R., Soloff, P.H., Perel, J.M., & Ulrich, R.F. (1991). A preliminary trial of

fluoxetine in refractory borderline patients. Journal of Clinical psychopharmacology. Vol.11.

Coryell, W., Endicott, J., Winokur, G., Akiskal, H., Solomon, D., Leon, A., Mueller, T., &

Shea, T. (1995). Characteristics and significance of untreated major depressive disorder. American

Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.152.

Coryell, W., Solomon, D., Leon, A., Fiedorowicz, J.G., Schettler, P., Judd, L., & Keller, M.

(2009). Does major depressive disorder change with age? Psychological Medicine. Vol.39.

Costa, P.T., Jr. & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R)

and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment

Resources

Costa, P.T., & Widiger, T.A. (Eds). (2002) Introduction: Personality disorders and the five

factor model of personality. In P Costa, P.T., & Widiger, T.A (Eds) Personality disorders and the

five factor model of personality. (2nd ed.) Washington, DC: American Psychological Association

Cottraux, J., & Blackburn, I-M (2001). Cognitive Therapy. In W.J. Livesley (Ed.) Handbook

of personality disorders – Theory, research and treatments. The Guildford Press.

Crane, R. (2009). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy - Distinctive Features. Routledge.

Page 107: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

101

Dalgleish, T. (1995). Performance on the emotional Stroop task in groups of anxious, expert

and control subjects: A comparison of computer and card presentation formats. Cognition and

Emotion. Vol.9(4).

Davis, R.N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive Inflexibility among ruminators and

nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research. Vol.24

Davison, S., Leese, M., & Taylor, P.J. (2001). Examination of the screening properties of

the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire 4+ (PDQ-4+). in a prison population. Journal of

Personality Disorders. Vol.15(2).

Derry, P.A., & Kuiper, N.A. (1981). Schematic processing and self reference in clinical

depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.90.

Dhammananda, K.S. Ven. (1993) What Buddhists Believe. Corporate Body of the Buddhist

Educational Foundation.

Diguer, L., Barber, J.P., & Luborsky, L. (1993). Three Concomitants. Psychiatric Severity

and Outocme of Dynamic Psychotherapy of Major Depression. American Journal of Psychiatry.

Vol.150

DiLalla L.F., & Jones, S. (2000) Genetic and environmental influences on temperament in

preschoolers. In: Molfese V.J., & Molfese D.L., (eds). Temperament and personality development

across the life span. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.

DiMaio, S., Grizenko, N., & Joober, R. (2003) Dopamine genes and attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: a review. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience Vol.28.

Dimaggio, G., Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Nicolo, G., & Procacci, M. (2007).

Psychotherapy of personality disorders – Metacognitionm states of mind and interpersonal cycles.

Rouledge.

Dimaggio, G., Procacci, M., Nicolò, G., Popolo, R., Semerari, A., Carcione, A, & Lysaker,

P.H. (2007). Poor Metacognition in Narcissistic and Avoidant Personality Disorders: Analysis of

four psychotherapy patients. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy. Vol.14.

Dobson, K. S., & Breiter, H. J. (1983). Cognitive assessment of depression: Reliability and

validity of three measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.92.

Page 108: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

102

Dobson, K.S., & Pusch D. (1993). Towards a definition for conceptual and empirical

boundaries of cognitive therapy. Australian Psychologist. Vol.28.

Dolan, B.M., Warren, F., Norton, K., (1997). Change in borderline symptoms one year after

therapeutic community treatment for severe personality disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry

Vol.171

Driessen, M., Herrmann, J., Stahl, K., de Zwaan, M., Meier, S., Hill, A., Osterheider, M., &

Petersen, D. (2000). Magnetic resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in

women with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Archives of General

Psychiatry. Vol.57.

Durbin, E., Klein, D.N., Hayden E.P., Buckley, M.E., & Moerk, K.C. (2005).

Temperamental emotionality in preschoolers and parental mood disorder. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. Vol.114.

Dykman, B.M. (1997). A test of whether negative emotional priming facilitates access to

latent dysfunctional attitudes. Cognition and Emotion. Vol.11.

Eich, E. (1995). Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychological Science. Vol.6.

Eisenberg, N., & Morris A.S. (2002) Children’s emotional-related regulation. Advance

Children Development Behavior. Vol.30.

Ellsworth, P.C., & Scherer, K.R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In R.J. Davidson,

K.R. Scherer, & H.H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford: Oxford University

Press.

Ekselius, L., & von Knorring, L. (1998). Personality disorder comorbidity with major

depression and response to treatment with sertraline or citalopram. International Clinical

Psychopharmacology. Vol.13

Emmelkamp, P.M.G., & Kamphuis, J.H. (2007). Personality disorders. Psychology Press.

Evans D.D., & Craighead, W.E. (1995). Relationship among cognitive distortions and

personality disorders in severely depressed patients. Paper presented at the 29th annual meetings of

the Association for Advancement of Behaviour Therapy, Washington, DC.

Page 109: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

103

Evans, D.E., & Rothbart, M.K. (2007). Development of a model for adult temperament.

Journal of Research in Personality. Vol.41

Farrant, J., & Perex, M. (1989). Immunity and depression. In J.G Howells (Ed.). Modern

perceptive in the Psychiatry of Affective Disorder. Modern Perspective in Psychiatry. Vol.13

Fava, M., Alpert, J.E., Borus, J.S., Nierenberg, A.A., Pava, J.A., Rosenbaum, J.F., (1996).

Patterns of personality disorder comorbidity in early-onset versus late-onset major depression. The

American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.153

Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.P. (2007). Mindfulness

and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the cognitive and affective

mindfulness scale-revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,

Vol.29(3)

Fenske, M.J., & Eastwood, J.D. (2003). Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing

emotion: Evidence from flanker tasks. Emotions. Vol.3

Flickstein, M. (2007) The Meditator's Atlas: A Roadmap to the Inner World. Wisdom

Publications

Frances, A., Fryer, M., & Clarkin, J. (1986) Personality and suicide. Annals of New York

Academy of Sciences. Vol.487

Frances, A.J., Clarkin, J., Gilmore, M., Hurt, S., & Brown, S. (1984). Reliability of criteria

for borderline personality disorder: A comparison of DSM-III and the diagnostic interview for

borderline patients. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.42

Freud, A. (1937). The ego and mechanisms of defense. New York: International Universities

Press.

Fresco, D.M., Alloy L.B., & Reilly-Harrington, N. (2006). Association of attribution stule

for native and positive events and occurrence of life events with depression and anxiety. Journal of

Social and Clinical Psychology. Vol.25

Fresco, D.M., Flynn, J.J., Mennin, D.S., & Haigh, E.A.P (2011). Mindfulness-Based

Cognitive Therapy. In Herbert, J.D. & Forman E.M. (Ed.) Acceptance and mindfulness in

Page 110: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

104 cognitive behavior therapy : understanding and applying the new therapies. John Wiley & Sons,

Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey

Fredrickson, B.L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology.

Vol.2

Friedman R.C., Aronoff, M.S., Clarkin, J.F., Corn, R., & Hurt, S.W. (1983). History of

suicidal behavior in depressed borderline inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.140

Fossati, A., Maffei, C, Bagnato, M, Donati, D., Donini, M., Fiorilli, M., Novella, L., &

Ansoldi, M. (1998). Brief communication: Validity of the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire-4+

(PDQ-4+). in a mixed psychiatric sample. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol.12

Furmark, T., Tillfors, M., Marteinsdottir, I., Fischer, H., Pissiota, A., Långström, B., &

Fredrikson, M. (2002). Common changes in cerebral blood flow in patients with social phobia

treated with citalopram or cognitive-behavioral therapy. Archive of General Psychiatry. Vol.59.

Gabbard, G.O. (1990). Psychodynamic psychiatry in clinical practice. Washington, DC:

American Psychiatric Press.

Gabbard, G.O. (2003) Psychoanalysis and Psychoanalytic psychotherapy. In W.J. Livesley

(Ed.). Handbook of personality disorders – Theory, research and treatments. The Guildford Press.

Gallagher, N.G., South, S.C., & Oltmanns, T.F. (2003). Attentional coping style in

obsessive-compulsive personality disorder: A test of the intolerance of uncertainty hypothesis.

Personality and Individual Differences. Vol.34.

Gelfand, D.M., & Teti, D.M. (1990). The effect of maternal depression on children. Clincial

Psychology Review. Vol.10.

Gemar, M.C., Segal, Z.V., Sagrati, S., & Kennedy, S.J. (2001). Mood-induced changes on

the Implicit Association Test in recovered depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

Vol.110.

Gilbert, B.D., & Christopher, M.S. (2010). Mindfulness-based attention as a moderator of

the relationship between depressed affect and negative cognitions. Cognitive Therapy and

Research. Vol.34(6)

Page 111: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

105

Gilson, M., Freeman, A., Yates, M.J., & Freeman, S.M., (2009) Overcoming depression. A

cognitive therapy approach – Therapist Guide. Oxford University Press.

Gjone, H., & Stevenson, J.A. (1997). Longitudinal twin study of temperament and behavior

problems: common genetic or environmental influences? Journal of the American Academy of

Child., & Adolescent Psychiatry. Vol.36

Glomb, T.M., Duffy, M.K., Bono J.E., & Yang, T. (2011) Mindfulness at work. Research in

Personnel and Human Resources Management, Vo.30.

Goldberg, J.F., & Kocsis, J.H. (1996). Relapse During SSRI Treatment for Depression.

Medscape Psychiatry., & Mental Health eJournal. Vol.1(5). Retrieved 30th, June 2011, from

http://www.medscape.com

Golin, S. Hartman, S.A., Klatt, E.N., Munz, K., & Wolfgang, G.L. (1977). Effects of

selfesteem manipulation on arousal and reactions to sad models in depressed and nondepressed

college students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.86.

Goodyer, I.M., & Cooper, P.J. (1993). A community study of depression in adolescent girls.

II: The clinical features of identified disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.163

Gortner, E.T., Gollan, J.K., Dobson, K.S & Jacobson, N.S., (1988) Cognitive–behavioral

treatment for depression: Relapse prevention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.

Vol.66(2)

Gotlib, I.H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in university students. Journal

of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.93

Gotlib, I.H., & MacLeod, C. (1997). Information processing in anxiety and depression: a

cognitive developmental perspective. In J. Burack, & J. Enns (Eds.), Attention, development, and

psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press

Goyer, P.F., Andreason, P.J., Semple, W.E., Clayton, A.H., King, A.C., Compton-Toth,

B.A., Schulz, S.C., Cohen, R.M. (1994). Positron-emission tomography and personality disorders.

Neuropsychopharmacology, Vol.10(1).

Page 112: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

106

Goyer, P.F., Konicki, P.E., & Schulz, S.C. (1994). Brain imaging in personality disorders.

In K.R. Silk (Ed.), Biological and neurobehavioral studies of borderline personality disorder.

Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Grant, M.M., Thase M.E., & Sweeney, J.A. (2001). Cognitive disturbance in outpatient

depressed younger adults: Evidence of modest impairment. Biological Psychiatry. Vol.50

Grilo, C.M., Sanislow, C.A., Shea, M.T., Skodol, A.E., Stout, R.L., Gunderson, J.G., Yen,

S., Bender, D.S. Pagano, M.E., Zanarini, M.C., Morey L.C., & McGlashan, T.H. (2005). Two-Year

Prospective Naturalistic Study of Remission From Major Depressive Disorder as a Function of

Personality Disorder Comorbidity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Vol.73(1).

Gross, J.J., & Thompson, R.A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J.J.

Gross (Ed.)., Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press

Grossman, R. (2004) Pharmacotherapy of Personality Disorders In W.J. Livesley (Ed.).

Handbook of personality disorders – Theory, research and treatments. The Guildford Press.

Gunaratana, H (1980) A critical analysis of the Jhanas in Theravada Buddhist meditation.

Buddha Dharma Education Association Inc.

Gunderson, J.G. (1992) Severe Personality disorders: Diagnostic controversies. In. Tasman,

A., & Ribas M.B (Eds). Review of Psychiatry. Vol.11. Washington. DC: American Psychiatric

Press.

Gunderson, J.G., Ronningstem, E., & Smith, L.E. (1991). Narcissistic personality disorder.

A review of data on DSM-III-R descriptions. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol.5.

Gunderson, J.G., Zanarini, M.C., & Kisiel, C.L.E. (1991). Borderline personality Disorders:

A review of data on DSM-III-R descriptions. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol.5.

Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (1998) Preschool temperament and environmental factors

related to the five-factor model of personality in middle childhood. Merill-Palmer Quarterly. Vol.44.

Hanh T.N. (1998). The heart of Buddha’s Teaching. Berkeley, CA: Parallax Press.

Page 113: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

107

Hansen, R.A. Gaynes, B.N., Gartlehner, G., Moore, C.G., Tiwari, R., & Lohr, K.N. (2008)

Efficacy and Tolerability of Second-Generation Antidepressants in Social Anxiety Disorder.

International Clinical Pharmacology. Vol.23(3)

Hammen, C., Brennan, P.A., Keenan-Miller, D., Herr, N.R., (2008). Early onset recurrent

subtype of adolescent depression: clinical and psychosocial correlates. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. Vol.49

Hardy, G., Barkham, M., Shapiro, D., Reynolds, S., & Rees, A. (1995). Credibility and

outcome of cognitive-behavioural and psychodynamic-interpersonal therapy. British Journal of

Clinical Psychology. Vol.34

Hare, D., Driscoll, A., & Stewart, A. (2009). beyondblue. In Screening and Management of depression in cardiac settings: An examination of clinical practice and a national study of prevalence of screening for depression by cardiologists and their beliefs regarding screening and management. Retrieved 27th, June 2011, from

http://www.beyondblue.org.au

Hart, A.B., Craighead W.E., & Craighead L.W. (2001). Predicting recurrence of major

depressive disorder in young adults: A prospective study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

Vol.110

Hartmann, H. (1939). Ego psychology and the problem of adaptation. New York:

International Universities Press.

Hautzinger, M., de Jong-Meyer, R., Treiber, R., Rudolf, G. A., & Thien, U. (1995). The

efficacy of cognitive behavior therapy and pharmacotherapy, alone or in combination, in

nonendogenous unipolar depression. Zeitschrift fur Klinische Psychologie. Forschung und Praxis.

Vol.25

Henry, J.D., & Crawford, J.R. (2005). The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety

Stress Scales. (DASS-21).: construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample.

British Journal of Clinical Psychology. Vol.44

Page 114: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

108

Herpertz, S., Gretzer, A., Steinmeyer, E.M., Muehlbauer, V., Schuerkens, A., & Sass, H.

(1997). Affective instability and impulsivity in personality disorder: Results of an experimental

study. Journal of Affective Disorders Vol.44(1).

Hickie, I.B., Pirkis, J.E., Blashki, G.A., Groom, G.L., & Davenport, T.A. General

practitioners’ response to depression and anxiety in the Australian community: a preliminary

analysis. Medical Journal of Australia Vol.181. (4).

Hill, C. V., Oei, T.P., & Hill, M.A. (1989). An empirical investigation of the specificity and

sensitivity of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire and Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. Journal of

Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. Vol 11(4).

Hilton, M. (2004). Assessing the financial return on investment of good management

strategies and the WORC Project. The University of Queensland.

Hirschfeld, R.M.A., Keller, M.B., Panico, S., Arons, B.S., Barlow, D., Davidoff, F.,

Endicott, J., Froom, J., Goldstein, M., Gorman, J.M., Guthrie, D., Marek, R.G. Maurer, T.A.,

Meyer, R., Phillips, K., Ross, J., Schwenk, T.L., Sharfstein, S.S., Thase, M.E & Wyatt, R.J. (1997).

The National Depressive and Manic-Depressive Association Consensus Statement on

the Undertreatment of Depression. Journal of the American Medical Association.

Vol.277(4)

Hogg, B., Jackson, H.J., Judd, R.P., & Edwards, J. (1990). Diagnosing personality disorders

in recent-onset schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. Vol.178.

Hoglend, P. (1993). Personality disorders and long-term outcome after brief psychodynamic

therapy. Journal of Personality Disorder. Vol.7(2)

Hollon, S.D., DeRubeis, R.J., Shelton, R.C., Amsterdam, J.D., Salomon, R.M., O’Reardon,

J.P., Lovett, M.L., Young, P.R., Haman, K.L., Freeman, B.B., & Gallop, R. (2005). Prevention of

relapse following cognitive therapy vs. medications in moderate to severe depression. Archives of

General Psychiatry. Vol.62

Huxter, M.J. (2007). Mindfulness as therapy from a Buddhist perspective. Australian

Academic Press.

Hyler, S.E. (1994). The Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire. New York: New York State

Psychiatric Institute

Page 115: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

109

Hyler, S.E., Rieder, R.O., Williams, J.B.W., & Spitzer, R.I. (1988). The Personality

Diagnostic Questionnaire: development and preliminary results. Journal of Personality Disorders.

Vol.2

Hyler, S.E., Rieder, R.O.. Williams. J.B.W., Spitzer, R.L., Lyons. M., & Handler, J. (1989).

A comparison of clinical and self-report diagnoses of DSM-III personality disorders in 552

patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry. Vol.30

Hawthorne, G., Goldney, R., & Taylor, A.W. (2008) Depression Prevalence: Is it Really

Increasing? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.46(4)

Ilardi, S.S., Craighead, W.E., & Evan D.D. (1997). Modeling Relapse in Unipolar

Depression The effects of Dysfunctional Cognition and Personality disorder. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology. Vol.65(3).

Ingram, R.E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z.V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New

York: Guilford Press.

Ingram, R.E., & Ritter, J. (2000). Vulnerability to depression: cognitive reactivity and

parental bonding in high-risk individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.109.

Jablensky A, & Kendell, R.E. (2002). Criteria for assessing a classification in psychiatry. In

Maj, M., Gaebel, W., Lo’pez-Ibor, J., & Sartorius, N. The Future of Psychiatric Diagnosis and

Classification (edi.). Wiley: Chichester.

Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C., Mills, P. J., Bell, I., & Schwartz, G.E.

(2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: Effects

on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral Medicine.

Vol. 33

Jarret, R.B., Eaves, G.G, Grannemann, B.D., & Jush, A.J., (1991). Clinical, Cognitive and

demographic predictors of responses to cognitive therapy for depression: a preliminary report.

Psychiatry Research. Vol.37(2)

Jarrett, R.B., Kraft, D., Schaffer, M., Witt-Browder, A., Risser, R., Atkins, D.H., & Doyle,

J. (2000). Reducing relapse in depressed outpatients with atypical features: A pilot study.

Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. Vol.69

Page 116: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

110

Johansson, R.E.A. (1985). The Dynamic Psychology of Early Buddhism. Scandinavian

Institute of Asian Studies Monograph Series. Curzon Press

Johnson, D.R. (2009). Emotional Attention Set-Shifting and Its Relationship to Anxiety and

Emotion Regulation. Emotion. Vol.9

Johnson, D.R. (2009). Attentional control capacity for emotion: An individual- difference

measure of internal controlled attention. Cognition and Emotion Vol.23(8).

Judd, L.L. (1997). The clinical course of unipolar major depressive disorders. Archives of

General Psychiatry Vol.54

Just, N., Abramson L.Y., & Alloy, L.B. (2001). Remitted depression studies as tests of the

cognitive vulnerability hypothesis of depression onset. A critique and conceptual analysis. Clinical

Psychology Review. Vol.21

Just, N., & Alloy, L.B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression: Tests and an

extension of the theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.106

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are. New York: Hyperion.

Kagan, J. (1997). In the beginning: The contribution of temperament to personality

development. Modern Psychoanalysis. Vol.22

Kagan, J. (2010). The Temperamental Thread: How Genes, Culture, Time and Luck make

Us Who We Are. Dana Press.

Karterud, S., Vaglum, S., Friis, S., Irion, T., Johns, S., & Vaglum, P. (1992) Day, hospital

therapeutic community treatment for patients with personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease. Vol.180(4)

Kauff, P. F. (1993). The contribution of analytic group therapy to the psychoanalytic

process. In A. Alonson, & H.I. Swiller (Ed.), Group therapy in clinical practice. Washington, DC:

American Psychiatric Press.

Kass, F., Skodol, A., Charles, E., Spitzer, R.L., & Williams, J.B.W. (1985). Scaled ratings

of DSM-III personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.142

Page 117: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

111

Katz, M., & Klerman, G.L. (1979). Introduction: Overview of clinical studies program.

American Journal of Psychology. Vol.136

Kaviani, H., Hatami, N., & Javaheri, F. (2010) The impact of Mindfulness-based Cognitive

Therapy (MBCT) on mental health and quality of life in a sub-clinically depressed population.

Archives of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy. Vol.1

Kay, J., & Tasman, A. (2006). Essentials of Psychiatry. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Keller, M.B., & Boland, R.J. (1998). Implications of failing to achieve successful long term

maintenance treatment of recurrent unipolar Major Depression. Biological Psychiatry.

Vol.44Keltner, D., & Gross, J.J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition & Emotion.

Vol.13

Keltner, D., & Kring, A.M. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review

of General Psychology. Vol.2(3).

Kendler, K.S., Thorton, L.M., & Gardner, C.O (2001) Genetic risk, number of previous

episodes, and stressful life events in predicting onset of depression. American Journal of Psychiatry.

Vol.158

Keng, S-L., Smoski, M.J., & Robins, C.J. (2011) Effects of mindfulness on psychological

health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review. Vol.31(6)

Kenny, M.A., & Williams, J.M.G., (2007). Treatment-resistant depressed patients show a

good response to Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Behaviour Research & Therapy. Vol.45

Kessler, R.C. (1995). Epidemiology of psychiatric comorbidity. In M. Tsung., M. Tohen, &

G. Zahner (Eds.) Textbook in psychiatric epidemiology. New York: Wiley-Liss.

Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, I., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K.R., Rush, J.,

Walters, E.E, Wong, P.S. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: Results from the

national Comorbidity Survey Replication. (NCS-R). Journal of American Medical Association.

Vol.289(23).

Kernberg, O.F. (1976). Object relation theory and clinical psychoanalysis. Northvale, NJ:

Aronson.

Page 118: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

112

Kernis, M.H., Paradise, A.W., Whitaker, D.J., Wheatman, S.R., & Goldman, B.N. (2000).

Master of one’s psychological domain? Not likely if one’s self-esteem is unstable. Personality and

Social Psychology Bulletin. Vol. 26

Kiken, L.G., & Shook, N.J. (2012). Mindfulness and emotional distress: The role of

negatively biased cognition. Personality and Individual Differences. Vol.52

Klein, M.H., Wonderlich S., & Shea, M.R. (1993). Models of relationships beween

personality and depression: Towards a framework for theory and research. In M.H. Klein, D.J.

Kupfer, & M.T. Shea (Eds.), Personality and depression. A current view. New York: Guilford

Press.

Klerman, G.L. (1990). The psychiatric patient’s right to effective treatment: implications of

Osheroff v. Chestnut Lodge. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.147.

Knaus, W.J (2006) The Cognitive Behavioral Work book for Depression. New Harbinger

Publications, Inc.

Koenigsberg, H.W., Harvey, P.D., Mitropoulou, V., New, A.S., Goodman, M., Silverman,

.J, Serby, M., Schopick, F., & Siever, L.J., (2001). Are the interpersonal and identity disturbances

in the borderline personality disorder criteria linked to the traits of affective instability and

impulsivity? Journal of Personality Disorder. Vol.15(4).

Koons, C.R., Robins, C,J., Tweed, J.L., Lynch, T.R., Gonzalez, A.M., Morse, J.Q., Bishop,

G.K., Butterfield, M.I., & Bastian, L.A. (2001). Efficacy of dialectical behaviour therapy in women

veterans with borderline personality disorder. Behavior Therapy. Vol.32

Kretschmer, W. (1953). Die Meditation als Prinzip der Lebensgestaltung. Psychologe:

Berater für Gesunde und Praktische Lebensgestaltung. Vol.5

Kruger, R.F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T.E., Silva, P.A., & McGee, R. (1996). Personality traits

are differentially linked to mental disorders: A multitrait-multidiagnosis study in adolescent birth

cohort. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.105.

Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients:

An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema’s response styles theory. Psychological Medicine. Vol.29.

Page 119: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

113

Kuiper, N. A., Olinger, L. J., & Air, P. J. (1985). Stress, coping, and vulnerability to

depression. Unpublished Manuscript. University of Western Ontario.

Kutz, I., Broysenko, J.Z., & Benson, H. (1985) Meditation and psychotherapy: A rationale

for the integration of dynamic psychotherapy, the relaxation response, and mindfulness meditation.

American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.142(1).

Kurtz, R. (1990). Body-Centred Psychotherapy: The Hakomi Method. Mendocino, LA:

LifeRhythm.

Kuyken, W., Crane, R.S., & Williams, J.M.G. (2012). The Bangor, Exeter & Oxford

mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) implementation resources.

http://mindfulnessteachersuk.org.uk/pdf/MBCTImplementationResources.pdf Universities of

Bangor, Exeter and Oxford

Lara, M.E., Klein, D.N., & Kash, K.L. (2000). Psychosocial predictors of the short-term

course and outcome of major depression: A longitudinal study of a nonclinical sample of recent-

onset episode. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.109.

Lane, R.D., Chua, P.M.L., & Dolan, R.J. (1999). Common effects of emotional valence,

arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures. Neuropsychologia,

Vol.37.

Lau, M.A., Segal, Z.V., & Williams, M.G. (2004). Teasdales’s differential activation

hypothesis: implications for mechanisms of depression relapse and suicidal behaviour. Behaviour

Research and Therapy. Vol.42.

Lavori, P.W., Keller, M.B. & Klerman, G.L. (1984). Relapse in affective disorders: a

reanalysis of the literature using life table methods. Journal of Psychiatric Research. Vol.18.

Leichsenring, F., & Leibing, E. (2003) The effectiveness of psychodynamic therapy and

cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of personality disorders: a meta-analysis. American

Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.160

Lemma, A. (2003). Introduction to the practice of psychoanalytic psychotherapy. John

Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Page 120: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

114

Lenzenweger, M.F., Lane, M.C., Armand, W., Loranger, A.W., & Kessler, R.C (2007)

DSM-IV personality disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biological

Psychiatry. Vol.62(6)

Lerner, D., Adler, D.A., Chang, H., Lapitsky, L., Hood, M.Y., Perissinotto, C., Reed, J.,

McLaughlin, T.J., Berndt, E.R., & Rogers, W.H. (2004). Unemployment, job retention, and

productivity loss among employees with depression. Psychiatric Service. Vol.55

Lesch, K.P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S.Z., Greenberg, B.D., Petri, S., Benjamin, J.,

Muller, C.R., Hamer, D.H., & Murphy, D.L. (1996) Association of anxiety- related traits with a

polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science Vol.274:

Levy, K.N & Scott, L.N. (2006) Other Personality Disorders. In M. Hersen, & J.C. Thomas

(Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of personality and psychopathology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Lewinsohn, P.M., Allen, N.B., Seeley, J.R., & Gotlib, I.H., (1999) First onset versus

recurrence of depression: differential process of psychosocial risk. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. Vol.108

Lewinsohn, P.M., Steinmetz, J.L., Larson, D.W., & Franklin, J. (1981). Depression-related

cognitions: antecedent or consequence? Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol.3

Leyton, M., Okazawa, H., Diksic, M., Paris, J., Rosa, P., Mzengeza, S., Young, S.N., Blier,

P., & Benkelfat, C. (2001). Brain regional alpha-[-sup-1-sup-1C]Methyl-l-tryptophan trapping in

impulsive subjects with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.158.

Luminet, O. (2004). Measurement of depressive rumination and associated constructs. In:

Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A (Eds.), Depressive rumination. Nature, theory and treatment.

Chichester: Wiley.

Linehan, M.M., Heard, H.L. (1992). Dialectical behavior therapy for

borderline personality disorder. In J.F. Clarkin, E. Marziali, & H. Munroe-Blum (Eds.),

Borderline personality disorder: Clinical and empirical perspectives. New York, NY, US:

Guilford Press

Page 121: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

115

Linehan, M.M., Rizci, S.L., Shaw Welch, S., & Page, B. (2000). Psychiatric aspect of

suicidal behaviour: Personality disorders. In K. Hawtonm, & K.V. Heeringen (Eds.), The

International Handbook of Suicide and Attempted Suicide. New York: Jon Wiley. & Sons.

Linehan, M.M. (1993a). Cognitive behavioural treatment for borderline personality

disorder. New York: Guilford Press.

Linehan, M.M. (1993b). Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder.

New York: Guilford Press.

Linehan, M.M., Armstrong, H.E., Surarez, A., Allmon, D., & Heard, K.L. (1991). Cognitive

behavioural treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Archive of general

psychiatry. Vol.48.

Linehan, M.M., Heard, H.L., & Armstrong, H.E (1993). Naturalistic follow-up of a

behavioural treatment for chronically parasuicidal borderline patient. Archive of General

Psychiatry. Vol.50

Livesley, W.J., & Jang, K.L., (2005). Differentiating Normal, Abnormal, and Disordered

Personality. European. Journal of Personality. Vol.19

Livesley, W.J., Jang, K.L, Vernon, P.A. (1998). Phenotypic and genetic structure of traits

delineating personality disorder. Archive of General Psychiatry. Vol.55

Livesley, W.J. (2003). Diagnostic dilemmas in classifying personality disorder. In K.A.

Phillips, M.B. First. & H.A. Pincus (Eds.), Advancing DSM: Dilemmas in psychiatric diagnosis.

Washington, DC, US: American Psychiatric Association

Lonigan, C.J., Vasey, M.W., Phillips, B.M., & Hazen, R.A. (2004). Temperament, anxiety,

and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology.

Loizzo, J. (1999) The science of enlightenment: Revolution in the moment. Trycycle. Vol.4

Lovibond, P.F., & Lovibond, S.H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states:

Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. (DASS). with the Beck Depression and Anxiety

Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy. Vol.33

Page 122: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

116

Luborsky, L., McLellan, A.T., Woody, G.E., O’Brian, C.P., & Auerbach, A. (1985).

Therapist success and its determinants. Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.42.

Lykins, E., & Baer, R.A. (2009). Psychological functioning in a sample of long-term

practitioners of mindfulness meditation. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International

Quarterly. Vol.23.

Lyons, M.J., Tyrer, P., Gunderson, J., Tohen, M. (1997). Special Feature: Heuristic Models

of Comorbidity of Axis I and Axis II Disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol.11(3).

Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric

rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.65(2).

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. M., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.95

MacLeod, C. (2005). The Stroop task in clinical research. In A. Wenzel, & D.C. Rubin,

(Eds.), Cognitive methods and their application to clinical research. Washington, DC, USA:

American Psychological Association.

Magnavita, J.J. (2002). Theories of personality: Contemporary approaches to the science of

personality. New York: Wiley.

Malamut, B.L., Saunders, R.C., & Mishkin, M. (1984). Monkeys with combined amygdalo-

hippocampal lesions succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hour intertrial intervals.

Behavioral Neuroscience. Vol.98

Manke, B., Saudino, K.J., & Grant, J.D. (2001) Extremes analyses of observed temperament

dimensions. In: Emde R.N., & Hewitt J.K. (eds) Infancy to early childhood: genetic and

environmental influences on developmental change. New York: Oxford University Press.

March, J., Silva, S., Petrycki, S., Curry, J., Wells, K., Vitiello, B., & Severe, J. (2004).

Fluoxetine, cognitive behavioural therapy, and their combination for adolescents with depression:

Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study. (TADS). randomized controlled trial. Journal of

American Medical Association. Vol.292

Markovitz, P.J., Calabrese, J.R., Schulz, S.C., & Meltzer, H.Y. (1991). Fluoxetine treatment

of borderline and schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.148.

Page 123: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

117

Markovitz, P.J., & Wagner, S. (1995). Venlafaxine in the treatment of borderline personality

disorder. Psychopharmocology Bulletin. Vol.31

Makrkoviz, P.J. (2001). Pharmacotherapy. In W.J. Livesley (Ed.) Handbook of personality

disorders – Theory, research and treatments. The Guildford Press.

Martin, R.P., Wisenbaker, J., & Huttunen, M. (1994). Review of factor analytic

studies of temperament measures based on the Thomas-Chess structural

model: Implications for the Big Five. In: Charles F. Jr., Kohnstamm, G.A., & Martin,

R.P (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to

adulthood. Halverson, Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc

Mathews, A., & Klug, F. (1993). Emotionality and interference with color-naming in

anxiety. Behaviour research and Therapy. Vol.31

McCrae, R.R & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1990). Personality In Adulthood. New York: Guildford

McCrae, R.R & Costa, P.T., Jr. (2002). Introduction: Personality disorders and the five

factor model of personality. In R.R. McCrae & P.T. Costa. (Ed.) Personality disorders and the five

factor model of personality. American Psychological Association.

McCrae, R.R., Costa, P.T., Jr., & Busch, C.M. (1986). Evaluating comprehensiveness in

personality systems: The California Q-Set and the five factor model. Journal of Personality. Vol.54.

McCown, D., Reibel, D., & Micozzi, M.S. (2010) Teaching Mindfulness A Practical Guide

for Clinicians and Educators. Springer New York

McCullough, J.P., McCune, K,J., Kaye, A.L., Braith, J.A., Friend, R., Roberts, W.C.,

Belyea-Caldwell, S., Norris, S.L., & Hampton, C. (1994). Group comparison of a community

dysthymia sample at screening with matched group of a nondepressed community controls. Journal

of Nervous and Mental Disease. Vol.182(7).

McGlashan, T.H. (1987) Recovery style from mental illness and long-term outcome. Journal

of Mental Disease. Vol.17

McIntosh, W.D. (1997). East meets west: Parallels between Zen Buddhism and social

psychology. International Journal of Psychology of Religion. Vol.7

Page 124: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

118

McKee, L., Zvolensky, M. J., Solomon, S. E., Bernstein, A., & Leen-Feldner, E. (2007).

Emotional-vulnerability and mindfulness: A preliminary test of associations among negative

affectivity, anxiety sensitivity, and mindfulness skills. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 36

Mckim, Ryan Douglas. (2008). Rumination as a mediator of the effects of

mindfulness: Mindfulness-based stress reduction. (MBSR). with a heterogeneous community sample experiencing anxiety, depression, and/or chronic pain. Dissertation

Abstract International. (B).: Physical Sciences and Engineering Vol.68. (11-b).

Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., & Hyde, J. S. (2002). Domain specificity of gender

differences in rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research. Vol.16

Miller, I.W., Norman, W.H., & Keitner, G.I. (1990). Treatment response of high cognitive

dysfunction: Depressed inpatients. Contemporary Psychiatry. Vol.30

Millon, T. (1996) Disorders of Personality: DSM IV and Beyond. John Wiley & Sons Inc.

New York.

Millon, T. (2002). Assessment is not enough: The SPA should participate in constructing a

comprehensive clinical science of personality (master lecture at the Annual Meeting of the Society

for Personality Assessment). Journal of Personality Assessment. Vol.78.

Miranda, J., Gross, J., Persons, J., & Hahn, J. (1998). Mood matters: negative mood

induction activates dysfunctional attitudes in women vulnerable to depression. Cognitive Therapy

and Research. Vol.22.

Miranda, J., & Persons, J.B. (1988). Dysfunctional attitudes are mood-state dependent.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.97.

Mogg, K., Miller, N., & Bradley, B.P. (2000). Biases in eye movements to threatening facial

expression in generalized anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. Vol.19

Moore, R.G., & Garland, A. (2003). Cognitive Therapy for Chronic and Persistent

Depression. John Wiley & Sons Ltd

Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affect cues and influence of instructions.

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Vol.11

Page 125: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

119

Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the

remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Vol.58

Munafo, M.R., Clark, T.G., Moore, L.R., Payne, E., Walton, R., & Flint, J. (2003). Genetic

polymorphisms and personality in healthy adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Molecular

Psychiatry. Vol.8(5)

Nace, E.P., Saxon, J.J., & Shore, N. (1983). A comparison of borderline and nonborderline

alcoholic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.40

Nyanapoika Thera. (1962). The Heart of Buddhist Meditation – A handbook of mental

training based on the Buddha’s way of mindfulness. Buddhist Publication Society. Inc

Nyanasamvara, S.P (2004). A guide to awareness – dhamma talks on the foundation of

mindfulness. Buddhist Publication Society. Inc

Nelson-Gray, R.O., Johnson, D., Foyle, L.W., Daniel, S.S., & Harmon, R. (1996). The

effectiveness of cognitive therapy tailored to depressives with personality disorders. Journal of

Personality Disorders. Vol.10

Newton-Howes, G., Tyrer, P., & Johnson, T. (2006). Personality disorder and the outcome

of depression: meta-analysis of published studies. British Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.188

Nigg, J.T., John, O.P., Blaskey, L.G., Huang-Pollock, C.L., Willcutt, E.G., Hinshaw, S.P., &

Pennington, B. (2002). Big Five dimensions and ADHD symptoms: Links between personality traits

and clinical symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.83

Nordahl, T.E., Benkelfat, C., Semple, W.E., Gross, M., King, A.C., Cohen, R.M. (1989).

Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology.

Vol.2.

Nolan, S.A., Roberts, J.E., & Gotlib, I.H. (1998). Neuroticism and ruminative response style

as predictors of change in depressive symptomatology. Cognitive Therapy and Research. Vol.22(5)

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory.

Psychological Bulletin. Vol.107

Page 126: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

120

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed

anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.109(3).

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and

posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.61

Nolen-Hoeksem, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B.L. (1993). Effects of rumination and

distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion. Vol.7

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed

mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.67

Noyes, R., Jr., Woodman, C.L., Holt, C.S., Reich, J.H., & Zimmerman, M.B. (1995).

Avoidant personality traits distinguish social phobic and panic disorder. Journal of Nervous and

Mental Diseases. Vol.183(3)

Nigg, J.T., John, O.P., Blaskey, L.G., Huang-Pollock, C.L., Willicut, E.G., Hinshaw, S.P., &

Pennington, B. (2002). Big five dimensions and ADHD symptoms: links between personality traits

and clinical symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.83(2).

Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions.

Cognition and Emotions. Vol.1(1).

Olfson, M., Marcus, S.C., Druss, B., Elinson, L., Tanielian, T., & Pincus, H.A., (2002)

National trends in the outpatient treatment of depression. Journal of American Medical

Association. Vol.287(2).

O’Leary, K., & Cowdry, R. (1994). Neuropsychological testing results in borderline

personality disorder. In K.R. Silk (Ed.), Biological and neurobehavioral studies of borderline

personality disorder. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Orsillo, S.M., Roemer, L., Lerner, J.B., & Tull, M.T. (2004). Acceptance, mindfulness, and

cognitive-behavioral therapy: Comparisons, contrasts, and application to anxiety. In S.C. Hayes,

V.M. Follette & M.M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding the cognitive-

behavioral tradition. New York: Guilford Press.

Page 127: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

121

Paykel, E.S., Ramana, R., Cooper, Z., Hayhurst, H., Kerr, J., & Barocka, A. (1995).

Residual symptoms after partial remission: an important outcome in depression. Psychological

Medicine. Vol.25.

Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2003). An Empirical Test of a Clinical Metacognitive Model

of Rumination and Depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol.27(3).

Pashler, H.E. (1998). The Psychology of Attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Perry, J.C. (1993). Longitudinal Studies of Personality Disorder. Journal of Personality

Disorders. Vol.7

Peselow E.D., Robin, C., Block, P., Barouche, F., & Fieve R.R. (1990). Dysfunctional

attitudes in depressed patients before and after clinical treatment and normal control subjects.

American Journal of Psychiatry Vol.147

Pilkonis, P.A., Hallquist, M.N., Morse, J.Q., & Stepp, S.D. (2011). Striking the (im)proper

balance between scientific advances and clinical utility: commentary on the DSM-5 proposal for

personality disorders. Personality Disorder. Vol.2.

Pincus A.L., & Wilson, K.R. (2001). Interpersonal variability in dependent personality.

Journal of Personality. Vol.69.

Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1999) Behavioral genetics and personality. In: Pervin L.A., &

John O.P, eds. Handbook of personality: theory and research. New York: Guilford,

Pretzer, J. L. (1998). Cognitive-behavioral approaches to the treatment of personality

disorders. In C. Perris, & P.D. McGorry (Eds.) Cognitive psychotherapy of psychotic and

personality disorders: Handbook of theory and practice. New York: Wiley.

Pretzer, J. (2004). Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders. In J.J. Magnavita (Eds.)

Handbook of Personality Disorders - Theory and Practice. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken,

New Jersey.

Pretzer, J.L., & Beck, A.T (2004) A cognitive theory of personality disorders. M.F.

Lenzenweger., & J.F. Clarkin (Eds.), Major theories of personality disorder, second edition. The

Guilford Press

Page 128: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

122

Psoner, M.I., Snyder, C.R.R., & Davidson, B.J. (1980). Attention and the detection of

signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology General. Vol.109

Psoner, M.I., Walker, J.A., Friedrich, F.J., & Rafal, R.D. (1984). Effects of parietal lobe

injury on covert orienting of visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience. Vol.4

Rait, G., Walters, K., Griffin, M., Buszewicz, M., Petersen I., & Nazareth, I. (2009) Recent

trends in incidences of recorded depression in primary care. British Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.195.

Ramel, W., Goldin, P.R., Carmona, P.E., & McQuaid J.R. (2004). The effects of mindfulness

meditation on cognitive processes and affect in patients with past depression. Cognitive Therapy

and Research. Vol.28

Rauch, S.L., Jenike, M.A., Alpert, N.M., Baer, L., Breiter, H.C., Savage, C.R., & Fischman,

A.J. (1994). Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-

compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography.

Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.51.

Rauch, S.L., Keuthen, N., Rosen, B.R., Caviness, V.S., Filipek, P.A., (1996). Cerebral

structural abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder - A quantitative morphometric magnetic

resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.53.

Reich, J. (2000). Drug treatment & personality disorder traits. Psychiatric Annuals.

Vol.32(10).

Reich, J.H., & Vasile, R.G. (1993). Effect of personality disorders on the

treatment outcome of Axis I conditions: An update. Journal of Nervous and Mental

Disease. Vol 181(8).

Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2010). The genetic epidemiology of personality disorders.

Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. Vol.12

Reynolds, F.E., & Carbine, J.A (Eds.) (2000). The life of buddhism. University of California

Press, Ltd.

Page 129: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

123

Reynolds, C.F. (2000). Psychopharmacology: antidepressants and mood stabilizers. In B.J.

Sadock, & V.A. Sadock (Eds.) Kaplan & Sadock's comprehensive textbook of psychiatry. 7th

edition. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Rettew D.C., & McKee, L. (2005). Temperament and its role in developmental

psychopathology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. Vol.13

Rholes, W., Riskind, J.H., & Neville, B. (1985). The relationship of cognitions and

hopelessness to depression and anxiety. Social Cognition. Vol.3.

Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2005). A comparison of attentional biases and memory biases

in women with social phobia and major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.114(1).

Rinne, T., van den Brink, W., Wouters, L., & Dyck, R.V. (2002). SSRI treatment of

borderline personality disorder: a randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial for female patients

with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.159.

Roberts, J.E., Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I.H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability

to episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy and

Research. Vol.22(4)

Robbins, M. (1989). Primitive personality organization as an interpersonally adaptive

modification of cognition and affect. International Journal of Psychoanalysis. Vol.70 (3)

Robinson, M.S., & Alloy, L.B. (1998). Negative inferential style, stress reactive rumination

and life stress as interactive risk factors in the onset of depression. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. Vol.109

Robinson, M.D., Wilkowski, B.M., Kirkeby, B.S., & Meier, B.P. (2006). Stuck in rut:

Perseverative tendencies and the neuroticism– distress relationship. Journal of Experimental

Psychology. Vol.135

Roelofs, J., Peters, M.L., Deutz, J., Spijker, C., & Vlaeyen, J.W.S. (2005). The fear of pain

questionnaire. (FPQ): Further psychometric examination in a non-clinical sample. Pain. Vol.116

Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of

General Psychology. Vol.2(3)

Page 130: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

124

Rossi, A., Marinangeli, M.G., Butti, G., & Scinto, A Di Cicco, Loredana., Kalyvoka., & A

Petruzzi, C. (2001). Personality disorders in bipolar and depressive disorders. Journal of Affective

Disorders. Vol.65

Rothbart, M.K., Ahadi, S.A., Hersey, K.LL., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of

temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior

Questionnaire. Child Development. Vol 72(5).

Rothbart, M.K., Ahadi, S.A., & Evans, D.E. (2000). Temperament and personality: origins

and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol.79(1).

Rothbart, M.K., Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in

temperament. In M.E, Lamb, & A.L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental Psychology.

Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum.

Rose, D.T., Abramson, L.Y., Hodulik, C.J., Halberstadt, L., & Leff, G. (1994).

Heterogeneity of cognitive style among depressed inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

Vol.103(3).

Rounsaville, B.J., Prusoff B.A., & Padian, N. (1980). The course of non-bipolar, non-

psychotic primary depression — A prospective 16 month study of ambulatory patients. Journal of

Nervous and Mental Disease. Vol.168(7).

Rubin RT, Ananth J, Villanueva-Meyer J, Trajmar PG, Mena I. (1995) Regional Xenon-133

cerebral blood flow and cerebral Tc-99m-HMPAO uptake in patients with obsessive-compulsive

disorder before and during treatment. Biological Psychiatry. Vol.38.

Saudino, K.J., & Cherny, S.S. (2001). Sources of continuity and change in observed

temperament. In: Emde R.N., & Hewitt J.K., (eds). Infancy to early childhood. New York: Oxford

University Press,

Sahdra, B.K., Shaver, P.R., Brown, K.W. (2010). A Scale to Measure Nonattachment: A

Buddhist Complement to Western Research on Attachment and Adaptive Functioning. Journal of

Personality Assessment. Vol.92(2).

Page 131: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

125

Salzman. C., Wolfson, A.N., Schatzberg. A., Looper, J., Henke, R., Albanese, M., Schwartz,

J., & Miyawaki, E. (1995). Effect of fluoxetine on anger in symptomatic volunteers with borderline

personality disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. Vol.15.

Samuel, D.B., & Widiger, T.A. (2006). Differentiating Normal and Abnormal

Personality from the Perspective of the DSM. In S. Strack (Ed.) Differentiating

normal and abnormal personality. (2nd ed.). New York, NY, US: Springer Publishing Co.

SANE Australia. (2010). Borderline Personality Disorder. Retrieved May 3, 2012 from:

http://www.sane.org/information/factsheets-podcasts/160-borderline-personalit y-disorder.

Sanislow, C.A., Grilo, C.M., Morey, L.C., Bender, D.S., Skodol, A.E., Gunderson, J.G.,

Shea, M.T., Stout, R.L., Zanarini, M.C.., & McGlashan, T.H. (2002). Confirmatory factor

analysis of DSM-IV criteria for borderline personality disorder: findings from the collaborative

longitudinal personality disorders study. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.159.

Sanislow C.A., & McGlashan, T.H. (1998). Treatment outcome of Personality disorders.

Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.43

Sarason, I.G. (1986). Test anxiety, worry, and cognitive interference. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.),

Self-related cognitions in anxiety and motivation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sarbacker, S.R. (2005). Samadhi: The numinous and cessative in Indo-Tibetan yoga. State

University of New York Press

Saxena, S., & Rauch, S. L. (2000). Functional neuroimaging and the neuroanatomy of

obsessive-compulsive disorder. Pediatric Clinics of North America. Vol.23.

Schmaling, K.B., Dimidjian, S., Katon, W., & Sullivan, M. (2002). Response styles among

patients with minor depression and dysthymia in primary care. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

Vol.111.

Schoth, D. E., & Liossi, C. (2010). Attentional bias towards pictorial representations of

pain in individuals with chronic headache. The Clinical Journal of Pain. Vol.26(3).

Segal, Z.V., & Dobson, K.S. (1992). Cognitive models of depression: report from a

consensus development conference. Psychological Inquiry. Vol.3.

Page 132: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

126

Segal, Z.V., Gemar, M.C., Truchon, C., Guirguis, M., & Horowitz, L.M. (1995). A priming

methodology for studying self-representation in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. Vol.104.

Segal, Z.V., Gemar, M., & Williams, S. (1999). Differential cognitive response to a mood

challenge following successful cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy for unipolar depression.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.108.

Segal, Z.V., Kennedy, S., Gemar, M., Hood, K., Pedersen, R., & Buis, T. (2006). Cognitive

reactivity to sad mood provocation and the prediction of depressive relapse. Archives of General

Psychiatry. Vol.63

Serretti, A., Mandelli, L., Lorenzi, C., Landoni, S., Calati, R., Insacco, C., & Cloninger, C.R

(2006) Temperament and Character in Mood Disorders: Influence of DRD4, SERTPR, TPH and

MAO-A Polymorphisms. Neuropsychobiology. Vol.53

Shapiro, S.L., Brown, K.W., Thoresen, C., Plante, T.G. (2011). The moderation of

mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait Mindfulness: results from a randomized

controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology. Vol. 67(3).

Shapiro, S.L., Carlson, L.M., Astin, J.A., & Freedman, B. (2005) Mechanisms of

Mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology. Vol. 62(3)

Shaw, S. (2006) Buddhist meditation: an anthology of texts from the Pali Canon. Routledge

Shea, M.T., Ckass, D.R., Pilkonis, P.A., Watkins, J. (1987). Frequency and implication of

personality disorders in a sample of depressed outpatients. Journal of Personality Disorder. Vol.1

Shea, M.T., Leon, A.C., Mueller, T.I., Solomon, D.A., Warshaw, M.G., & Keller, M.B.

(1996). Does major depression result in lasting personality change? American Journal of

Psychiatry. Vol.153.

Shea, M.T. Pilkonis, P.A., Beckham, E.C, Joseph F., Elkin, I., Sotsky, S.M., & Docherty,

J.P. (1990) Personality disorders and treatment outcome in the NIMH Treatment of Depression

Collaborative Research Program. The American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.147(6)

Page 133: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

127

Sibrava, N. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2006). The cognitive avoidance theory of worry. In

G.C.L. Davey., & A. Wells (Eds.) Worry and its psychological disorders: Theory, assessment, and

treatment. West Sussex, England: Wiley.

Siever, LJ., & Davis, K.L. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on personality disorders.

American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.148.

Siever, L.J., Friedman, L., Moskowitz, J., Mitropoulou, V., Keefe, R., Riotman, S.L.,

Merhige, D., Trestman, R., Silverman, J., & Mohs, R. (1994). Eye movement impairment and

schizotypal psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.151.

Siever, L.J., Kalus, O., & Keefe, R.S. (1993). The boundaries of schizophrenia. Psychiatric

Clinics of North America. Vol.16.

Silverman, J.S., Silverman, J.A., & Eardley, D.A. (1984). Do maladaptive attitudes cause

depression? Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.41.

Simons, A.D., Garfield, S.L., & Murphy, G.E. (1984). The process of change in cognitive

therapy and pharmacotherapy: changes in mood and cognition. Archives of General Psychiatry.

Vol.41.

Skodol, A.E., Siever, L., Livesley, W.J., Gunderson, J.G., Pfohl, B., & Widiger, T.A.

(2002). The borderline diagnosis II: Biology, genetics, and clinical course. Biological Psychiatry.

Vol.51.

Skodol, A.E, Oldham, J.M., Rosnick, L., Kellman, H.D & Hyler, S.E. (1991). Diagnosis of

DSM-III-R personality disorders: a comparison with two structured interviews. International

Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research.

Smith, J.M., Grandin, L.D., Alloy, L.B., & Abramson, L.Y. (2006). Cognitive vulnerability

to depression and Axis II Personality Dysfunction. Cognitive Therapy Research. Vol.30.

Spasojevic, J., & Alloy, L.B. (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relating to

depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion. Vol.1.

Sperry, L. (1995). Psychopharmacology and psychotherapy: Strategies for maximizing

treatment outcomes. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Page 134: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

128

Sperry, L. (2003). Handbook of the diagnosis and treatment of DSM-IV-TR personality

disorders. (2nd Ed.). New York: Brunner-Routledge.

Sperry, L. (2006). Psychological treatment of chronic illness: The biopsychosocial therapy

approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Stallings, M.C., Hewitt, J.K., Cloninger, C.R., Heath, A.C., & Eaves, L.J. (1996). Genetic

and environmental structure of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: three or four

temperament dimensions? Journal of Personality Social Psychology. Vol.70.

Strelau, J. (1987). The concept of temperament in personality research. European Journal of

Personality. Vol.1.

Stewart, W.F., Ricci, J.A., Chee, E., Hahn, S.R., & Morganstein, D. (2003). Cost of lost

productive work time among US workers with depression. Journal of American Medical

Association. Vol.289(23)

Stone, M. (1993). Abnormalities of personality: within and beyond the realm of treatment.

New York: Norton.

Strack, S., & Lorr, M. (1997) Invited essay: The challenage of differentiating normal and

disordered personality. Journal of Personlaity Disorder. Vol.11

Stroop, J.Ridley. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of

Experimental Psychology. Vol.18(6). Retrieved on 11th July, 2011 from

http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Stroop

Sujato, B. Ven. (2006). A history of mindfulness: How insight worsted tranquillity in the

Satipatthana Sutta. The Corporate Body of the Buddha Education Foundation

Svrakic, D.M., Przybeck, T.R., & Cloninger, C.R. (1991). Further contribution to the

conceptual validity of the unified biosocial model of personality: US and Yugoslav data.

Comprehensive Psychiatry. Vol.32.

Swedo, S.E, Pietrini, P., Leonard, H.L., Schapiro, M.B., Rettew, D.C., Goldberger, E.L.,

Rapoport, S.I., Rapoport, J.L., & Grady, C.L. (1992) Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-

onset obsessive- compulsive disorder. Revisualization during pharmacotherapy. Archives of

General Psychiatry. Vol.49.

Page 135: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

129

Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and

Emotion. Vol.2.

Teasdale, J.D., & Barnard, P. (1993). Affect, cognition and change: remodelling depressive

thought. Lawrence Elbaum Associates Lrd. Publishers. UK.

Teasdale, J.D., & Dent, J. (1987). Cognitive vulnerability to depression: an investigation of

two hypotheses. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. Vol.26.

Teasdale, J., Moore, R., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. (2002).

Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology. Vol.70(2).

Teasdale, J.D., Segal, Z.V., Williams, J.M.G., Ridgeway, V.A., Soulsby J.M., & Lau, M.A.

(2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive

therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. Vol.68(4).

Thackery, E., & Harris, M (Eds). (2003). The Gale Encyclopedia of Mental Disorders. Gale

Group. Inc.

Thase M.E., Simons, A.D., Mcgeary, J., Cahalane, J.F., Hughes, C., Harden, T., Friedman,

E. (1992). Relapse after cognitive behavior therapy of depression: Potential implication for longer

course of treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry Vol.149.

Thompson, A.H. (2008). Younger onset of depression is associated with greater suicidal

intent. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. Vol.43.

Thompson, B.L., & Waltz, J. (2007). Everyday mindfulness and mindfulness meditation:

Overlapping constructs or not? Personality and Individual Differences. Vol.43

Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., Marteinsdottir, I., Fischer, H., Pissiota, A., Långström, B., &

Fredrikson, M. (2001). Cerebral blood flow in subjects with social phobia during stressful speaking

tasks: A PET study. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.158

Tragesser, S.L., Solhan, M., Schwartz-Mette, R., & Trull, T.J. (2007). The Role of Affective

Instability and Impulsivity in Predicting Future BPD Features. Journal of Personality Disorders.

Vol.21(6).

Page 136: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

130

Trull, T.J., & Durrett, C.A. (2005). Categorical And Dimensional Models Of

Personality Disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. Vol 1(1).

Trungpa, C. Rinpoche (2001). Glimpses of Abhidharma. Boston: Shambhala

Trungpa, C. Rinpoche. (2004) The Collected Works of Chögyam Trungpa: The path is the

goal ; Training the mind and cultivating loving-kindness ; Glimpses of abhidharma ; Glimpses of

shunyata ; Glimpses of Mahayana ; Selected writings Shambhala Publications.

Turkat, I.D., & Maisto, S.A. (1985). Personality disorders: Application of the experimental

method to the formulation and modification of personality disorders. In D.H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical

handbook of psychological disorders: A step by step treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press

Tyrer, P., Gunderson, J., Lyons, M., & Tohen, M. (1997). Extent of comorbidity between

mental state and personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol 11(3).

Tyrer, P., & Johnson, T. (1996). Establishing the severity of personality disorder. American

Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.153.

Van Noorden, M.S., Minkenberg, S.E., Giltay, E.J., Den Hollander-Gijsman, M.E., Van

Rood, Y.R., Van Der Wee, N.J., & Zitman, F.G. (2011). Pre-adult versus adult onset major

depressive disorder in a naturalistic patient sample: the Leiden Routine Outcome Monitoring Study.

Psychological Medicine. Vol.41.

van der Linden, G.J., Stein, D.J., & van Balkom, A... (2000) The efficacy of the selective

serotonin reuptake inhibitors for social anxiety disorder (social phobia): a meta-analysis of

randomized controlled trials. International Clinical Psychopharmacology. Vol.15(2).

van Reekum R., Conway, C.A., Gansler, D, White, R., & Bachman, D.L. (1993).

Neurobehavioral study of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience

Vol.18.

Vittengl, J.R., Clark, L.A, Dunn, T.W., & Jarrett R.B. (2007). Reducing Relapse and

Recurrence in Unipolar Depression: A Comparative Meta-Analysis of Cognitive–Behavioral

Therapy’s Effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Vol.75(3).

Page 137: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

131

Villemarette-Pittman, N.R., Stanford, M.S., Greve, K.W., Houston, R.J., & Mathias, C.W.

(2004) Obsessive—Compulsive Personality Disorder and Behavioral Disinhibition. The Journal of

Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied. Vol.138(1)

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J.L., & Dolan, R.J. (2003). Reciprocal links between emotion and

attention. In K.L. Friston, C.D. Frith, R.J. Dolan, C. Price, J. Ashburner, W. Penny, S. Zeki, &

R.S.J. Frackowiak (Eds.), Human Brain Function. (2nd ed.). New York. Academic Press.

Wade, T.J., & Cairney, J. (2000). Major Depressive disorder and marital transition among

mothers: Results from a national panel study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. Vol.88

Waldinger, R.J., & Gunderson, J.G. (1984). Completed psychotherapies with borderline

patients. American Journal of Psychotherapy. Vol.38

Wallace, B.A., & Shapiro, S.L. (2006). Mental balance and well-being: Building bridges

between Buddhism and Western psychology. American Psychologist. Vol.61

Waller, N.G., Lilienfeld, S.O., Tellegen, A., & Lykken, D.T. (1991). The Tridimensional

Personality Questionnaire: structural validity and comparison with the Multidimensional

Personality Questionnaire. Multivariate Behavioral Research. Vol.26.

Watkins. E. (2004) Adaptive and maladaptive ruminative self-focus during emotional

processing. Behaviour Research & Therapy. Vol.42

Watkins, E., & Mason, A. (2002). Mood as input and rumination. Personality and Individual

Differences. Vol.32

Watson, D. (2000). Mood and Temperament. New York: The Guilford Press.

Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation

systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol 76(5).

Weissman, A.N., & Beck, A.T. (1978). Development and validation of the Dysfunctional

Attitude Scale: A preliminary investigation. Paper presented at the annual convention of the

American Educational Research Association, Toronto.

Page 138: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

132

Weissman, A. N. (1980). Assessing depressogenic attitudes: A validation study. Paper

presented at the 51st Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Hartford.

Weissman, M.M., & Kasl, S.V. (1976). Help-seeking in depressed out-patients following

maintenance therapy. British Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.129.

Western D., & Arkowitz-Western, L. (1998). Limitations of Axis II in diagnosing

persanolity pathology in clinical practice. American Journal of Psychiatry. Vol.155

Whitlock, F.A., & Siskind, M. (1979). Depression and cancer: a follow-up study.

Psychological Medicine. Vol.9.

Whitmer, A.J., & Banichm, M.T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different

forms of rumination. Psychological Sciences. Vol.18(6).

Widiger, T.A. (1992). Categorical vs dimensional classification: Implication from and for

research. Journal of Personality. Vol.69

Widiger, T.A. (1993). The DSM-III-R categorical personality disorder diagnosis: A

critique and an alternative. Psychological Inquiries. Vol.4

Widiger, T.A., & Costa, P.T., Jr. (2002). Five-factor model personality

disorder research In Costa, P.T Jr., & Widiger, T.A (Eds.), Personality disorders and the

five-factor model of personality. (2nd ed.). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological

Association.

Widiger, T.A., Simonsen, E., Krueger, R., Livesley, J., & Verheul, R. (2005). Personality

disorder research agenda for the DSM-V. Journal of Personality Disorders. Vol.19

Widiger, T.A., & Rogers, J.H. (1989). Prevalence of comorbidity of personality disorder.

Psychiatric Annals. Vol.19.

Widiger, T.A., & Samuel, D.B. (2005). Evidence based assessment of personality disorders.

Psychological Assessment. Vol.17

Wiggins, J.S., & Pincus, A.L. (1989) Conception of personality disorders and dimensions of

personality. Psychological Assessment: A journal of consulting and Clinical Psychology. Vol.1

Page 139: Theoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy ...344280/s43050280_mphil_submission.pdfTheoretical Framework and Variables Affecting the Efficacy of Mindfulness- Based

133

Wilkinson, P.O., & Goodyer, I.M. (2006). Attention difficulties and mood-related

ruminative response style in adolescents with unipolar depression. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry. Vol.47(12).

William, M., Teasdale, J., Segal, Z., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2007). The Mindful Way through

Depression – Freeing yourself from chronic unhappiness. The Guilford Press.

Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., Neale, J. M., Stone, A. A., & Rachmiel, T. A. (1990). Self-

focused attention, coping responses, and distressed mood in everyday life. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology. Vol.58(6).

Woody, G.E., McLellan, A.T., Luborsky, L., & O’Brian, C.P. (1985). Sociopathy and

psychotherapy outcome. Archives of General Psychiatry. Vol.4.

World Health Organization, (2008). The global burden of disease: 2004 update. World

Health Organization Press

Zajonc, R. (1984). On Primacy of Affect. American Psychologist. Vol.39.

Zimmerman, M., Coryell, W., Corethal, C., & Wilson, S. (1986). Dysfunctional attitude and

attribution style in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia, psychotic depression and

nonpsychotic depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.95(4).

Zisook, S., Lesser, I., Stewart, J.W., Wisniewski, S.R., Balasubramani, G.K., Fava, M.,

Gilmer, W.S., Dresselhaus, T.R., Thase, M.E., Nierenberg, A.A., Trivedi, M.H., A.J. Rush. (2007).

Effect of age at onset on the course of major depressive disorder. The American Journal of

Psychiatry. Vol.164.