understanding the determinants of aging and …

188
UNDERSTANDING THE DETERMINANTS OF AGING AND LONGEVITY: THE INFLUENCE OF THE SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT, BIOLOGY, AND HERITABILITY THROUGHOUT THE LIFE COURSE by Heidi Anne Hanson A dissertation submitted to the faculty of The University of Utah in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Sociology The University of Utah December 2013

Upload: others

Post on 26-Mar-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

UNDERSTANDING THE DETERMINANTS OF AGING AND

LONGEVITY: THE INFLUENCE OF THE SOCIAL

ENVIRONMENT, BIOLOGY, AND

HERITABILITY THROUGHOUT

THE LIFE COURSE

by

Heidi Anne Hanson

A dissertation submitted to the faculty of

The University of Utah

in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Sociology

The University of Utah

December 2013

Copyright © Heidi Anne Hanson 2013

All Rights Reserved

T h e U n i v e r s i t y o f U t a h G r a d u a t e S c h o o l

STATEMENT OF DISSERTATION APPROVAL

The dissertation of Heidi Anne Hanson

has been approved by the following supervisory committee members:

Ken R. Smith , Chair 09/06/2013

Date Approved

Rebecca Utz , Member 09/06/2013

Date Approved

Ming Wen , Member 09/09/2013

Date Approved

Sandra J. Hasstedt , Member 09/09/2013

Date Approved

Antoinette M. Stroup , Member

Date Approved

and by Kim Korinek , Chair/Dean of

the Department/College/School of Sociology

and by David B. Kieda, Dean of The Graduate School.

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this dissertation is to investigate the heterogeneity in patterns of

aging and the factors throughout the life course that shape them. By focusing on

variability within the population we are able to advance our knowledge of how

circumstances throughout the life course affect the way individuals age. We find that the

paths to disease and longevity are diverse and that the social environment plays an

important role in shaping these patterns. Our results support a wide body of literature

showing that morbidity is not an inevitable consequence of aging, even in the oldest old

population. Health status and longevity are shaped by the historical circumstances and

social environments that we live in. This study offers three innovative and significant

contributions to the understanding of biological and environmental determinants of aging

by (1) disentangling the biological and temporal sources of trends in cancer incidence

among the elderly, (2) investigating the possible social and physiological effects of

fertility history on comorbidity trajectories after age 65, and (3) studying heterogeneity in

the heritable contributions to variation in longevity across early life family and social

environments.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT………………………………………………………………………. iii

LIST OF TABLES..………………………………………………………………. vi

LIST OF FIGURES……………………………………………………………... vii

Chapters

1. AGING AND LONGEVITY: THE PAST AND FUTURE TRENDS..…….. 1

Introduction………………………………………………………………….. 1

The Biodemographic Perspective of Aging…………………………………. 3

Biology, the Life Course, and Aging………………………………………… 7

The Determinants of Aging and Longevity………………………………….. 13

References……………………………………………………………………. 17

2. AN AGE-PERIOD-COHORT ANALYSIS OF CANCER INCIDENCE

AMONG THE OLDEST OLD…………………..………………………….. 24

Abstract……………………………………………………………………… 24

Introduction…………………………………………………………………. 25

Background…………………………………………………………………. 27

Methods…………………………………………………………………….. 34

Results………………………………………………………………………. 38

Discussion…………………………………………………………………… 41

References…………………………………………………………………... 49

3. REPRODUCTIVE HISTORY AND LATER LIFE COMORBIDITY

TRAJECTORIES…………………………………………………………… 59

Abstract…………………………………………………………………….. 59

Introduction………………………………………………………………… 60

Background………………………………………………………………… 61

Methods……………………………………………………………………. 70

Results……………………………………………………………………… 84

Discussion………………………………………………………………….. 93

Conclusion…………………………………………………………………. 97

References……………………………………………………………….….…. 98

4. HERITABILITY OF LONGEVITY AND THE ROLE OF EARLY

AND MIDLIFE ENVIRONMENTS………………………………….…….… 125

Introduction……………………………………………………………...….… 125

Background…………………………………………………………………… 126

Methods………………………………………………………………………. 134

Results………………………………………………………………………… 146

Discussion…………………………………………………………………….. 151

References……………………………………………………………………. 156

5. CONCLUSION……………………………………………………………….. 172

Future Research……………………………………………………………… 174

Conclusions……………………………………………………………………. 177

References……………………………………………………………………. 179

v

LIST OF TABLES

Table

1.1 Biological Evolution Theories………………………………………………... 23

3.1 Hypothesized Relationship between Fertility and Later-life Comorbidity…. 107

3.2 Description of Sample Selection by Sex and Age………………………….. 108

3.3 Descriptive Statistics by Gender and Age Group………………………….. 109

3.4 Sample Selection Means by Gender and Age………………………..……. 110

3.5 Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership vs. Robust Group: Women Ages 66 – 74 in

1992...……………….……………………………………………………… 111

3.6 Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership vs. Robust Group: Women Ages 75 – 84 in

1992...……………….……………………………………………………… 112

3.7 Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership vs. Robust Group: Men Ages 66 – 74 in

1992...……………….……………………………………………………… 113

3.8 Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership vs. Robust Group: Men Ages 75 – 84 in

1992...……………….……………………………………………………… 114

4.1 Pedigree Selection…………………………………....…………………….. 161

4.2 Descriptive Statistics for Individuals from 802 Utah Families...…….…..… 162

4.3 Summary of the Results Obtained for Polygenic Models of LS……..…….. 163

4.4 Summary of the Results Obtained for Polygenic Models of EL……..……. 164

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure

2.1 All-site cancer incidence rates by age and period…………..………..……... 54

2.2 All-site cancer incidence by birth cohort………………..…………...…..….. 55

2.3 APC IE estimated trends of all-site cancer incidence rates for ages

65 – 99 in the state of Utah ….…………………………………………....... 56

2.4 APC IE estimates of female breast and male prostate cancer incidence

rates for ages 65 to 99 in the state of Utah…..………………………...……. 57

2.5 Possion log-linear estimates of age and period effects on colon cancer

incidence……………………………………………………….……….…… 58

3.1 Comorbidity trajectories for females ages 66 – 74 in 1992…………..…….. 115

3.2 Comorbidity trajectories for females ages 75 – 84 in 1992……………...…. 116

3.3 Comorbidity trajectories for males ages 66 – 74 in 1992………...………… 117

3.4 Comorbidity trajectories for males ages 75 – 84 in 1992….…...…...……… 118

3.5 Morbidity trajectories for females ages 66 – 74 in 1992…….…...………… 119

3.6 Morbidity trajectories for females ages 75 – 84 in 1992……...…………… 120

3.7 Morbidity trajectories for males ages 66 – 74 in 1992…..…...……………. 121

3.8 Morbidity trajectories for males ages 75 – 84 in 1992…….………………. 122

3.9 Female birth certificate results: Ages 66 – 74 in 1992.…...……….………. 123

3.10 Male birth certificate results: Ages 66 – 74 in 1992.……..………….…... 124

4.1 Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Expected………….…………….……… 165

4.2 Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Triggering..……….…………….……… 166

4.3 Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Compensation...….…………….……… 167

4.4 Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Enhancement.…….…………….……… 168

4.5 Predicted values of survival to the 50th

and 90th

percentiles by gender

and birth year………………………………….…………………………. 169

4.6 Distribution of calculated longevity for individuals born between 1850

and 1927 and surviving to age 30……….….……………………………. 170

4.7 Distribution of longevity by environment………………………………… 171

viii

CHAPTER 1

AGING AND LONGEVITY: THE PAST AND FUTURE TRENDS

Introduction

Demographers, biologists, social scientists, geneticists, historians, and other

scientists have long embarked on the quest of uncovering the secrets of healthy aging and

longevity. While the fascination with longevity is not unique to this time period, the

rapid changes in life expectancy and population structure over the past century have

elevated the importance of understanding determinants of healthy aging and longevity.

The mortality profiles of the developed countries have especially undergone fundamental

transformations over the past century. Life-expectancy in these populations has increased

linearly by approximately 3 months per year for the past 160 years (Oeppen & Vaupel,

2002). Historically, these improvements have been largely due to improvements in

survival in infancy and childhood. While less recognized, death rates at older ages have

also greatly improved over the last half of the 20th

century (Vaupel et al., 1998).

Significant declines in fertility during the demographic transition combined with

gains in life expectancy past age 65 have led to population aging (rising proportions of

the population age 65 and older) and increased levels of old-age dependency. These

changes have had profound policy implications. It is estimated that the proportion of the

2

population age 65 years and older will increase from 12.3% in 2000 to 21.1% in 2050 in

the United States (Uhlenberg, 2005). The oldest old population (ages 85+) is projected to

more than triple from its current estimate of 5.7 million to 24 million by 2050 (Vincent,

Velkoff, & Bureau, 2010), making it the fastest growing segment of the population. The

rising proportions of the population above the age of 65, combined with increases in life

expectancy and current trends in mortality decline in the oldest age categories, have made

the determinants of longevity and healthy aging critical to understanding population

health. Aging research is an extremely important domain of population health, and its

significance will increase as the proportion of the population age 65 and older continues

to rise.

The biological and social factors that determine healthy aging and longevity, and

their interaction, are still not well understood. In the past, misconceptions about the

limits of life-span have led demographers to underestimate the rate of decline in old-age

mortality (Uhlenberg, 2005). Current projections suggest that if the present gains in life

expectancy continue, more than half of individuals born after 2000 will live to see their

100th

birthday (Christensen, Doblhammer, Rau, & Vaupel, 2009). Unfortunately, such

projections ignore the complex interactions of social and biological factors that determine

mortality.

This dissertation improves upon previous research by investigating the influence

of the social environment, biology, and heritability throughout the life course on healthy

aging and longevity. The studies presented in this manuscript seek to disentangle the

biological and temporal sources of trends in cancer incidence, investigate the possible

social and physiological effects of fertility history on comorbidity trajectories after age

65, and explore the heterogeneity in heritable components of total phenotypic variation in

3

longevity across early life family and social environments. A fuller understanding of

heterogeneity in patterns of aging and the factors throughout the life course that shape

them will lead to more accurate population prediction, identify at risk population that

may benefit from more effective public health interventions, and characterize the process

of aging in a diverse population.

A rigorous investigation into biological and social causes of healthy aging and

longevity at advanced ages requires a theoretical framework capable of assimilating

theories from multiple disciplines. Biodemography provides a multidisciplinary

synthesis of biological, evolutionary, social science, ecological, life history and

demographic theories and is primed to answer a range of questions including those about

both how and why humans age (Vasunilashorn & Crimmins, 2008). Over the past few

decades, demographers have broadened the focus of work in the demography of aging

from a population aging perspective (i.e., measures of change in population age

structure), to include a perspective that integrates health and biological explanations with

traditional demographic and social theories of aging to explain variations in health and

mortality within and between populations (Olshansky, Carnes, & Brody, 2002; Siegel,

2011; Vasunilashorn & Crimmins, 2008).

The Biodemographic Perspective of Aging

As developed countries began to recognize most of the longevity gains to be

secured were achieved by improving infant and childhood mortality, questions began to

surface about how much improvement can be made in mortality rates at the other end of

the spectrum, what proportion of mortality at these ages is biologically determined, and

whether we are approaching a maximum life expectancy or linear gains can continued to

4

be realized (Carnes & Olshansky, 2007; Vaupel et al., 1998). “Aging, Natural Death, and

the Compression of Morbidity,” an article published by James Fries (1980), resulted in a

lively debate about the limits of life-span within the field of demography, with some

arguing that physiological decay was innately programmed (Fries, 1980), others

suggesting that old age is not biologically determined but there are practical limits that

will make steady improvements difficult (Carnes & Olshansky, 2007), and a third group

projecting linear increases in life expectancy for the foreseeable future (Vaupel et al.,

1998).

This debate is centered on a pivotal question; are we biologically programmed to

die? Even under ideal conditions, there is a progressive increase in age-specific death

rates and senescence (Carey & Judge, 2001). Theories aimed at answering why we

senesce and inevitably die can be classified into two broad categories: thermodynamic

and biological evolutionary theories of aging (Austad, 2001). Thermodynamic theories

implicitly or explicitly claim that aging is the inescapable consequence of the physical

nature of matter. These theories arrive at the conclusion that senescence is a genetically

programmed rate of decay, the natural consequence of approaching one’s maximum

possible life-span (Fries, 1980). Biological evolution theories explain senescence in

terms of selection forces acting on life history traits (Kirkwood & Rose, 1991).

Reliability theories of aging, optimization models, and nonadaptive mutation models (see

Table 1.1 for a more detailed description of these theories) all describe senescence as a

byproduct of evolution and not an innately programmed switch that is common to all

organisms.

The compression of morbidity hypothesis (Fries, 1980) argues that morbidity and

disability can be compressed to shorter periods toward the end of the life-span through

5

primary prevention such as maintaining a healthy lifestyle. Principally, he argued that

the rectangularization of the survival curve would be accompanied by an increase in age

at onset for chronic disease and disability which, in turn, compresses the time spent in a

diseased or disabled state. Others argued the failure of success hypothesis, which

suggests that improved survival of frail individuals will lead to increases in disease later

in life (Gruenberg, 1977; Kramer, 1980). Not only did these hypotheses spark interest in

determinants of life-span, but they also led to debate about heterogeneity in patterns of

aging, whether increased life expectancy indicated increased healthy life expectancy, and

whether centenarians escaped major age-related diseases.

The evidence consistent with morbidity compression is still uncertain. Most

evidence for individuals younger than 85 suggests there has been a postponement in

disease and disability over time, but little is known about trends in the population age

85+. This is largely because health data for this group of the population is not as readily

available (Boscoe, 2008). Although there are some studies of disease in centenarians that

suggest that a proportion of these exceptionally long-lived individuals delay or escape

disease, there is still considerable variation in disease experience (Andersen, Sebastiani,

Dworkis, Feldman, & Perls, 2012; Evert, Lawler, Bogan, & Perls, 2003). Uncertainty of

the expected trends in morbidity with age coupled with the fiscal demands of the

Medicare program have made the question of morbidity patterns above age 65 a central

biodemographic question. While the association between morbidity and mortality is

complex and varies across populations and environments (Siegel, 2011), most

classifications of morbidity (for example, heart disease and dementia) lead to higher rates

of death (Vaupel, 2010). Therefore, a more complete understanding of the determinates

6

of major morbid conditions and how these factors change over time can yield better

predictions of morbidity and mortality trends at advanced ages.

In his 1980 paper, Fries made a prediction: life expectancy would not exceed 85

years. This prediction was quashed in 2007, when the average life-expectancy for

Japanese women reached 86 years (Christensen et al., 2009). The continued steady rise

in life expectancy suggests that if there is a fixed limit, we have not yet reached it. While

limits in life expectancy suggested by those supporting a biological limit to life-span have

been surpassed, Jean Calment’s documented life-span of 122 years has yet to be broken.

Therefore two questions still remain: are we biologically programmed to die, and what

patterns of disease can we expect to see if life expectancy continues to rise? While these

questions have important implications for future population projections, we cannot arrive

at a suitable answer unless we consider another component that has been largely ignored

up to this point in the discussion: the relationship between social context, healthy aging,

and longevity.

Aging does not take place in isolation. It is heavily influenced by our

environments. Understanding the interplay between social context and biological factors

is imperative to understanding and predicting future trends in aging and mortality. Social

and historical context must be considered when determining morbidity and mortality

trends within a population. For example, studies have suggested that age, period, and

cohort factors are all important factors that affect population trends in mortality (Preston

& Wang, 2006; Yang, 2008). Mortality at ages 80 years and above has fallen at an

unprecedented pace since the 1950s (Kannisto, 1996), but old-age mortality in the United

States has stagnated since 1980 (Rau, Soroko, Jasilionis, & Vaupel, 2008). Other authors

have also noted the potential flaws in predicting future trends in longevity without

7

considering the social and historical context which shapes it, and have suggested that the

rapid increase in obesity may lead to declines in life expectancy in the near future (Jay

Olshansky et al., 2005; Reither, Olshansky, & Yang, 2011). Accurate predictions of

health and mortality of the aged population requires an approach that crosses disciplinary

boundaries and integrates biological and sociological concepts.

Biodemographers embrace the view that sociological context affects healthy

aging and longevity, for not only do social theories explain the demographic transition

but the central idea that health and longevity is socially patterned is deeply rooted in the

sociological tradition (Berkman & Syme, 1979; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988; Link

& Phelan, 1995; Wen, Browning, & Cagney, 2003; Wise, 2003). But by integrating

biological theories and measures with sociological theories, the field of biodemography

has great potential for making contributions that will improve public health by

considering how social, economic, behavioral, and psychological conditions “get under

the skin” to cause health problems (Crimmins & Seeman, 2004; Robine, 2006;

Vasunilashorn & Crimmins, 2008). It has also become evident that proximate social

circumstances alone cannot explain heterogeneity in aging and the experiences across the

life course play an important role.

Biology, the Life Course, and Aging

Healthy aging and longevity cannot be understood by restricting analysis to a

single life stage because aging is a lifelong process. The life course perspective places

importance on both the historical and demographic parameters related to aging and

longevity, as well as the biological, social and psychological factors that influence aging

and longevity through direct (e.g., biological imprinting) and indirect (e.g., cumulative

8

and pathway) mechanisms throughout the life course (S. H. Preston, Hill, & Drevenstedt,

1998; Settersten, 2003). Simply put, it requires the researcher to consider how risk is

shaped throughout the life course, beginning with biological development in utero.

Genetic influences have been cited as perhaps the earliest biological factor

contributing to later life morbidity and mortality (Smith, Hanson, & Zimmer, 2012). The

two types of longevity genes, gerontogenes and longevity-assurance genes, can be used to

describe the effects of genes on longevity (Christensen, Johnson, & Vaupel, 2006;

Sebastiani et al., 2012). Gerontogenes negatively affect longevity, thus life-span

increases when their expression is blocked. Longevity assurance genes lead to a

phenotypic expression of longer life-span and therefore longevity decreases when their

expression is blocked. Thus, genetic endowments may either be protective, as in the case

of familial excess longevity (Smith, Mineau, Garibotti, & Kerber, 2009), or detrimental,

as in the case of certain apolipoprotein E (APOE) alleles (Ewbank, 2004).

Genes are fixed at birth, but is their expression? To answer this question,

comparisons of monozygotic and dizygotic twins have been made to compare life spans

while holding the childhood environment constant. These studies estimate heritability of

life-expectancy to be 25% (Herskind et al., 1996; Skytthe et al., 2003). Twin studies

have also revealed the variable nature of gene expression with age (Fraga et al., 2005;

Petronis et al., 2003) and it has been suggested that epigenetic mechanisms cause

individuals with the same genotype to have increasingly divergent phenotypes with age.

An individual’s genotype is inherited at birth and can be considered immutable.

However, gene expression is malleable because it is influenced by the environment

through the epigenome. Epigenetic modifications can be defined as “the sum of heritable

changes…that affect gene expression without changing DNA sequence” (Montesanto,

9

Dato, Bellizzi, Rose, & Passarino, 2012). Epigenetics is a bridge between genetics and

environment and may explain a portion of the variation in the rate of aging and longevity.

It is one of several possible biological mechanisms that allow social circumstances to get

“under the skin,” and epigenetic modifications have the propensity to persist across

subsequent generations (Feinberg, 2007). Differences in community and family

environments may affect the epigenetic regulation of gene expression and lead to

variation in the longevity phenotype. Recent studies suggest a relationship between

strength of genetic correlations and the quality and variability of an environment

(Charmantier & Garant, 2005). There may also be epigenetic changes in response to

individual social experiences throughout the life course (Champagne, 2010). Does the

social environment throughout the life course shape later life health and mortality?

Events throughout the life course can alter physiological functioning and affect

later life health and longevity. Early life conditions have been shown to be significantly

correlated with adult mortality for individuals and cohorts (Abel & Kruger, 2010; Barker,

1995; Doblhammer & Vaupel, 2001; Eriksson, Forsén, Tuomilehto, Osmond, & Barker,

2001). The fetal origins hypothesis and inflammation hypothesis are two theories that

have been used to explain the biological programming of an individual early in life.

According to the fetal origins hypothesis, individuals exposed to adverse conditions in

utero may have altered morbidity and mortality trajectories due to altered development of

key organ systems or epigenetic modifications during gestation. The inflammation

hypothesis argues that exposures to infectious disease during infancy and childhood

result in altered morbidity and mortality trajectories in adulthood (Crimmins & Finch,

2006; Finch & Crimmins, 2004). McDade, Rutherford, Adair, and Kuzawa (2010) have

proposed a related but alternative hypothesis predicting a negative relationship between

10

exposure to infectious disease and inflammation in adulthood, arguing that exposure to

infectious diseases are necessary for healthy development of the immune system.

Early life conditions may also be indirectly associated with morbidity and

mortality outcomes through correlated environments, cumulative processes, health

selection, and mortality selection. Indirect associations through correlated environments

are based on the principle of continuity of the life course and that one’s environment

during childhood is the same or similar to one’s adult environment. Selection

mechanisms may also lead to an indirect association between early life circumstances and

later life health outcomes. The health selection hypothesis argues that illness has social

consequences that may lead to poor socioeconomic status (SES) later in life and that it

may be the more proximate exposure to poor SES that is responsible for the observed

association between early life conditions and later life health (Montez & Hayward, 2011).

This continuity may lead to erroneously attributing the observed outcome to early life

conditions, when it is the proximate environment that is leading to adverse health

outcomes. Alternatively, genetic heterogeneity in the population may lead to differential

mortality selection; with those at the highest risk, the frail, being culled from the

population early leading to a population with a disproportionate representation of robust

individuals at older ages (Elo & Preston, 1992; Hawkes, Smith, & Blevins, 2012).

Related to this argument is the cumulative advantage/disadvantage hypothesis,

which argues that early life events can set into motion a trajectory where

advantage/disadvantage is accumulated throughout the life course (O'Rand & Hamil-

Luker, 2005). Sequential exposures to adverse environments may lead to excess stress or

exposure to chronic stress that leads to increased risk for disease later in life.

Conceptualizing aging and longevity as a lifelong processes allows for the study of how

11

inequalities are created through the accumulation of advantage/disadvantage across the

life course (Elder & Giele, 2009). Cumulative disadvantage can be set into motion by

early life events or situations that lead to structural constraints throughout the life course

(O'Rand & Hamil-Luker, 2005).

Physiological changes to the body in response to social conditions are not

constrained to critical or sensitive periods of development. These changes can occur in

response to prolonged exposure to stress throughout the life course. Allostatis, the ability

to achieve stability through change, is maintained in the body through the autonomic

nervous system, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, and the cardiovascular,

metabolic, and immune systems (McEwen, 1998). Allostatic load describes a process

through which exposure to chronic stress throughout the life course can lead to wear and

tear in these systems and lead to poor health in adulthood (Geronimus, 1992; McEwen,

1998), and these effects can be attenuated or accentuated by an individual’s access to

economic, social, or personal resources (Elder & Giele, 2009). Under this hypothesis,

individuals that are continually exposed to stress may experience physiological

deterioration of key systems that lead to chronic disease later in life. Recent epigenetic

research has also shown that epigenetic modifications occur across the life span

(Champagne, 2010; Montesanto et al., 2012; Shanahan & Hofer, 2011). While more

research needs to be done, it has been suggested that epigenetic changes during the aging

process may directly contribute to malignant transformation of cells (Fraga, 2009).

Placing human lives in context is fundamental to life course research. The life

course perspective promotes the view that social and physical environments vary by time

and space and underscores the multiple layers of human experience. It also highlights the

importance of historical change in determining health. These temporal changes in the

12

social or ecological environment are dependent upon the age of an individual and are

unique to a group of people born during the same time period or birth cohort.

Biodemography adds to this concept by recognizing that we live in a very different

environment from the one in which our life history evolved. Genetic, social, and

economic history and environments play an important role in shaping health and disease

patterns across populations and communities.

Birth cohorts are a measure of the social forces that shape health throughout the

life course (Keyes, Utz, Robinson, & Li, 2010). They vary in size, demographics, social

norms, prevalence of infectious disease, food availability, level of medical knowledge,

education, occupation, urbanization, etc., making each cohort unique. For example,

changes in smoking patterns or other environmental exposures over time may lead to

cohort specific trends in cancer incidence. They have been regarded as fundamental units

of social organization (Easterlin, 1998; Elder, 1999). Finch and Crimmins’ cohort

morbidity phenotype hypothesis suggests that differential exposure to infectious diseases

during childhood will lead to cohort differences in old age morbidity and mortality

(Crimmins & Finch, 2006; Finch & Crimmins, 2004). While cohort effects have proven

important for a number of different outcomes related to aging and longevity (Chen, Yang,

& Liu, 2010; Yang, 2008), some researchers are skeptical of the life course researcher’s

fascination with historical time (Fry, 2003), and others have argued that period factors

play a more important role in determining mortality rates in old age (Gagnon & Mazan,

2009; Kannisto, 1996).

The life course perspective facilitates questions about possible pathways to later

life health and potential confounding factors. The integration of biodemographic

principles and the life course framework could lend important insight into how the

13

heritability of longevity may be altered by events throughout the life course. Biological,

social, and psychological theories of development will be integrated in an attempt to

create a more complete view of how later life health is shaped by a lifetime of past

exposures.

Population aging is one of the greatest societal challenges of the next 50 years

(Kalache, Barreto, & Keller, 2005; Schoeni & Ofstedal, 2010). There are both social and

economic consequences of population aging. Accurate projections of how the elderly

population ages has policy implications for forecasting Social Security and Medicare

expenditures and predicting the costs of aging nationally and globally. The increase in

the proportion of the population over the age of 65 will also change the types of illnesses

and prevalent diseases in the population, affecting the types of medical services needed.

The projected increase in the proportion of the population at advanced ages has grave

implications for public pension programs, health care, and old age dependency. A greater

understanding of the sociological, biological, and heritable determinants of aging and

longevity is essential to maintaining a healthy population and economy.

The Determinants of Aging and Longevity

Perhaps the best way to elucidate mechanisms of aging and longevity is to study

the heterogeneity in morbidity and longevity and determine what factors contributed to

observed differences. This research contributes to the scientific understanding of aging

and longevity patterns and the factors throughout the life course that influence them.

Understanding the sources of variation in patterns of aging and longevity is important for

creating accurate population predictions, identifying at-risk populations that may benefit

14

from public health interventions, and characterizing the process of aging in a diverse

population.

Cancer was the second leading cause of death for individuals aged 65 and older in

the United States in 2010 (Miniño & Murphy, 2011), making it an essential component to

the study of aging and morbidity. Studies that do not account for changes in the

environment, diet, health behaviors, and screening and diagnostic practices are ignoring

the multifaceted determinants of cancer and may be inadvertently attributing temporal

determinates of observed trends to biological mechanisms. Failing to account for cohort

and period specific trends may confound the true age trajectory of cancer in this

population. Chapter 2 disentangles these trends for individuals age 65 to 99 using Utah

cancer incidence rates from 1963 to 2002, which lend better understanding to the true

age-specific trends in cancer incidence, including the previously understudied oldest old

age group (85+). It is important to account for cohort variations in aging and longevity in

order to avoid misattributing patterns caused by historical circumstances to biological

changes associated with age. Disentangling age, period, and cohort effects for major

health conditions in the oldest old categories will allow for more definitive assertions

about the possible causes of mortality deceleration and increased accuracy in forecasting

of future trends used to predict the fiscal burdens of an aging population.

The pathology of chronic disease is multifaceted, determined by genetic profiles,

biological and physiological development, and the social environment, with the strength

and relative importance of each of these factors varying throughout the life course.

Understanding longitudinal patterns of morbidity after age 65 is important to

understanding the mechanisms of aging and longevity. Equally important is what

15

predicts the observed patterns. Are measures of fertility and reproductive health

associated with morbidity profiles later in life?

Biological, evolutionary, and social theories all predict a relationship between

fertility and later life morbidity trajectories. Chapter 3 examines the role of parity, young

age at first birth, age at last birth, interbirth intervals, infant death, multiple births (twins),

marital status at time of birth, birth weight of offspring, and preterm births for both men

and women on disease progression after age 65. This study utilizes Centers for Medicare

(CMS) data spanning from 1992 – 2009 linked to the Utah Population Database, which is

a rich source of longitudinal data. Studying the effects of fertility history on men and

women at several stages in the aging process will lend clues to biological, evolutionary,

and social mechanisms that may lead to the observed outcomes.

For a more complete understanding of population heterogeneity in life-span and

the forces behind it, one must not only understand the average contribution of genes and

environment within a population toward explaining variation in adult mortality, but

uncover the factors that influence patterns of variation within the population. While there

is strong evidence supporting a genetic component to longevity, surprisingly, its size and

relative importance is poorly understood. Longevity is a complex trait, determined by a

multiplicity of genetic and environmental factors, with each factor contributing a

potentially small amount to phenotypic variation. This phenotypic variation can be

partitioned into genetic and environmental variation. Chapter 4 tests for heterogeneity in

the heritability of longevity across several early and midlife environments and explores

the possibility of gene-environment interactions (GxE). By examining sources of

variation in heritability estimates, we can illuminate factors that modify the expression of

genetic predisposition in a population. Understanding the role of biological and

16

environmental determinants of aging and mortality, and how they interact, can allow for

the identification of sources of variation in morbidity and mortality and improve

predictions of morbidity and mortality for future generations.

The final chapter provides a short summary of the findings from the studies

presented in Chapters 2-3. These studies provide insight into patterns and processes of

aging and highlight important factors to consider as the proportion of the population aged

65 years and older continues to grow. This chapter also gives direction for future

research and policy implications.

17

References

Abel, E. L., & Kruger, M. L. (2010). Birth month affects longevity. Death Studies, 34(8),

757-763.

Andersen, S. L., Sebastiani, P., Dworkis, D. A., Feldman, L., & Perls, T. T. (2012).

Health span approximates life span among many supercentenarians: Compression

of morbidity at the approximate limit of life span. The Journals of Gerontology

Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67A(4), 395-405. doi:

10.1093/gerona/glr223

Austad, S. N. (2001). Concepts and theories of aging. In E. J. Masaro & S. N. Austad

(Eds.), Handbook of the biology of aging (Fifth ed., Vol. 1, pp. 3 - 18). United

States: Academic Press.

Barker, D. (1995). Fetal origins of coronary heart disease. BMJ, 311(6998), 171-174.

Berkman, L. F., & Syme, S. L. (1979). Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: A

nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. American Journal of

Epidemiology, 109(2), 186-204.

Boscoe, F. P. (2008). Subdividing the age group of 85 years and older to improve US

disease reporting. American Journal of Public Health, 98(7), 1167.

Carey, J. R., & Judge, D. S. (2001). Principles of biodemography with special reference

to human longevity. Population: An English Selection, 13, 9-40.

Carnes, B. A., & Olshansky, S. J. (2007). A realist view of aging, mortality, and future

longevity. Population and Development Review, 33(2), 367-381.

Champagne, F. A. (2010). Epigenetic influence of social experiences across the lifespan.

Developmental Psychobiology, 52(4), 299-311. doi: 10.1002/dev.20436

Charmantier, A., & Garant, D. (2005). Environmental quality and evolutionary potential:

Lessons from wild populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 272(1571), 1415-1425. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3117

Chen, F., Yang, Y., & Liu, G. (2010). Social change and socioeconomic disparities in

health over the life course in china. American Sociological Review, 75(1), 126-

150. doi: 10.1177/0003122409359165

Christensen, K., Doblhammer, G., Rau, R., & Vaupel, J. W. (2009). Ageing populations:

The challenges ahead. The Lancet, 374(9696), 1196-1208. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61460-4

18

Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., & Vaupel, J. W. (2006). The quest for genetic

determinants of human longevity: Challenges and insights. Nat Rev Genet, 7(6),

436-448.

Crimmins, E. M., & Finch, C. E. (2006). Infection, inflammation, height, and longevity.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,

103(2), 498-503. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501470103

Crimmins, E. M., & Seeman, T. E. (2004). Integrating biology into the study of health

disparities. Population and Development Review, 30, 89-107.

Doblhammer, G., & Vaupel, J. W. (2001). Lifespan depends on month of birth. Proc Natl

Acad Sci U S A, 98(5), 2934-2939.

Easterlin, R. A. (1998). Growth triumphant: The twenty-first century in historical

perspective. Ann Arbor: Univ of Michigan Press.

Elder, G. H., & Giele, J. Z. (2009). The craft of life course research. New York: The

Guilford Press.

Elder, G. H. (1999). Children of the Great Depression: Social change in life experience:

Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Elo, I. T., & Preston, S. H. (1992). Effects of early-life conditions on adult mortality: A

review. Population Index, 58(2), 186-212.

Eriksson, J., Forsén, T., Tuomilehto, J., Osmond, C., & Barker, D. (2001). Early growth

and coronary heart disease in later life: Longitudinal study. BMJ, 322(7292), 949-

953.

Evert, J., Lawler, E., Bogan, H., & Perls, T. (2003). Morbidity profiles of centenarians:

Survivors, delayers, and escapers. The Journals of Gerontology Series A:

Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 58(3), M232-M237. doi:

10.1093/gerona/58.3.M232

Ewbank, D. C. (2004). The APOE gene and differences in life expectancy in europe. The

Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences,

59(1), B16-B20. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.1.B16

Feinberg, A. P. (2007). Phenotypic plasticity and the epigenetics of human disease.

Nature, 447(7143), 433-440.

Finch, C. E., & Crimmins, E. M. (2004). Inflammatory exposure and historical changes

in human life-spans. Science, 305(5691), 1736-1739. doi:

10.1126/science.1092556

19

Fraga, M. F. (2009). Genetic and epigenetic regulation of aging. Current Opinion in

Immunology, 21(4), 446-453. doi: 10.1016/j.coi.2009.04.003

Fraga, M. F., Ballestar, E., Paz, M. F., Ropero, S., Setien, F., Ballestar, M. L., Heine-

Suner, D. et al. (2005). Epigenetic differences arise during the lifetime of

monozygotic twins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America, 102(30), 10604-10609. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500398102

Fries, J. F. (1980). Aging, natural death, and the compression of morbidity. New England

Journal of Medicine, 303(3), 130-135.

Fry, C. L. (2003). The life course as a cultural construct. Invitation to the life course:

Toward new understandings of later life (pp. 269-294). New York: Baywood

Publising Company.

Gagnon, A., & Mazan, R. (2009). Does exposure to infectious diseases in infancy affect

old-age mortality? Evidence from a pre-industrial population. Social Science &

Medicine, 68(9), 1609-1616. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.008

Geronimus, A. T. (1992). The weathering hypothesis and the health of African-American

women and infants: Evidence and speculations. Ethnicity & Disease, 2(3), 207.

Gruenberg, E. M. (1977). The failures of success. The Milbank Memorial Fund

Quarterly. Health and Society, 55(1), 3-24. doi: 10.2307/3349592

Hawkes, K., Smith, K. R., & Blevins, J. K. (2012). Human actuarial aging increases

faster when background death rates are lower: A consequence of differential

heterogeneity? Evolution, 66(1), s103-114.

Herskind, A., McGue, M., Holm, N., Sørensen, T., Harvald, B., & Vaupel, J. (1996). The

heritability of human longevity: A population-based study of 2872 Danish twin

pairs born 1870–1900. Human Genetics, 97(3), 319-323. doi:

10.1007/bf02185763

House, J., Landis, K., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science,

241(4865), 540-545. doi: 10.1126/science.3399889

Kalache, A., Barreto, S. M., & Keller, I. (2005). Global ageing: The demographic

revolution in all cultures and societies. The Cambridge Handbook of Age and

Ageing (pp.30, 606). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kannisto, V. (1996). The advancing frontier of survival: Life tables for old age (Vol. 3).

Odense, Denmark: Univ Press of Southern Denmark.

Keyes, K. M., Utz, R. L., Robinson, W., & Li, G. (2010). What is a cohort effect?

Comparison of three statistical methods for modeling cohort effects in obesity

20

prevalence in the United States, 1971–2006. Social Science & Medicine, 70(7),

1100-1108. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.12.018

Kirkwood, T. B. L., & Rose, M. R. (1991). Evolution of senescence: Late survival

sacrificed for reproduction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of

London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 332(1262), 15-24. doi:

10.1098/rstb.1991.0028

Kramer, M. (1980). The rising pandemic of mental disorders and associated chronic

diseases and disabilities. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 62(S285), 382-397. doi:

10.1111/j.1600-0447.1980.tb07714.x

Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. (1995). Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease.

Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 80-94. doi: 10.2307/2626958

McDade, T. W., Rutherford, J., Adair, L., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2010). Early origins of

inflammation: Microbial exposures in infancy predict lower levels of C-reactive

protein in adulthood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,

277(1684), 1129-1137. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1795

McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New

England journal of medicine, 338(3), 171-179. doi:

doi:10.1056/NEJM199801153380307

Miniño, A. M., & Murphy, S. L. (2011). Death in the United States, 2010. NCHS Data

Brief, 99 (pp. 1-8). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. 2012

Montesanto, A., Dato, S., Bellizzi, D., Rose, G., & Passarino, G. (2012).

Epidemiological, genetic and epigenetic aspects of the research on healthy ageing

and longevity. Immun Ageing, 9(1), 6.

Montez, J. K., & Hayward, M. D. (2011). Early life conditions and later life mortality.

International handbook of adult mortality (pp. 187-206). New York: Springer.

O'Rand, A. M., & Hamil-Luker, J. (2005). Processes of cumulative adversity: Childhood

disadvantage and increased risk of heart attack across the life course. The

Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,

60(Special Issue 2), S117-S124.

Oeppen, J., & Vaupel, J. W. (2002). Broken limits to life expectancy. Science, 296(5570),

1029-1031. doi: 10.1126/science.1069675

Olshansky, S. J., Carnes, B. A., & Brody, J. (2002). A biodemographic interpretation of

life span. Population and Development Review, 28(3), 501-513.

Olshansky, S. J., Passaro, D. J., Hershow, R. C., Layden, J., Carnes, B. A., Brody, J.,

Hayflick, L., et. al. (2005). A potential decline in life expectancy in the United

21

States in the 21st century. New England Journal of Medicine, 352(11), 1138-

1145. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsr043743

Petronis, A., Gottesman, I. I., Kan, P., Kennedy, J. L., Basile, V. S., Paterson, A. D., &

Popendikyte, V. (2003). Monozygotic twins exhibit numerous epigenetic

differences: Clues to twin discordance? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(1), 169-178.

Preston, S. H., & Wang, H. (2006). Sex mortality differences in The United States: The

role of cohort smoking patterns. Demography, 43(4), 631-646. doi:

10.1353/dem.2006.0037

Preston, S. H., Hill, M. E., & Drevenstedt, G. L. (1998). Childhood conditions that

predict survival to advanced ages among African-Americans. Social Science &

Medicine (1982), 47(9), 1231.

Rau, R., Soroko, E., Jasilionis, D., & Vaupel, J. W. (2008). Continued reductions in

mortality at advanced ages. Population and Development Review, 34(4), 747-768.

doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2008.00249.x

Reither, E. N., Olshansky, S. J., & Yang, Y. (2011). New forecasting methodology

indicates more disease and earlier mortality ahead for today’s younger Americans.

Health Affairs, 30(8), 1562-1568. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0092

Robine, J. M. (2006). Research issues on human longevity. Human longevity, individual

life duration, and the growth of the oldest old population (pp. 7-42). Dordrecht:

Springer Netherlands.

Schoeni, R. F., & Ofstedal, M. B. (2010). Key themes in research on the demography of

aging. Demography, 47(1), S5-S15.

Sebastiani, P., Solovieff, N., DeWan, A. T., Walsh, K. M., Puca, A., Hartley, S. W.,

Melista, E., et. al. (2012). Genetic signatures of exceptional longevity in humans.

PLoS ONE, 7(1), e29848.

Settersten, R. A. (2003). Invitation to the life course: Toward new understandings of later

life. Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing Company.

Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2011). Molecular genetics, aging, and well-being:

Sensitive period, accumulation, and pathway models. Handbook of aging and the

social sciences (pp. 135-148). New York: Elsevier.

Siegel, J. S. (2011). The demography and epidemiology of human health and aging. New

York: Springer.

Skytthe, A., Pedersen, N. L., Kaprio, J., Stazi, M. A., Hjelmborg, J. v. B., Iachine, I.,

Vaupel, J. W.. et. al. (2003). Longevity studies in GenomEUtwin. Twin Research,

6(5), 448-454. doi: 10.1375/136905203770326457

22

Smith, K. R., Hanson, H. A., & Zimmer, Z. (2012). Early life conditions and later life

(65+) co-morbidity trajectories: The Utah Population Database linked to Medicare

claims data. Paper presented at the Population Association of America 2012, San

Fransico, CA.

Smith, K. R., Mineau, G. P., Garibotti, G., & Kerber, R. (2009). Effects of childhood and

middle-adulthood family conditions on later-life mortality: Evidence from the

Utah Population Database, 1850–2002. Social Science & Medicine, 68(9), 1649-

1658. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.010

Uhlenberg, P. (2005). Demography of aging Handbook of population (pp. 143-167).

New York: Springer.

Vasunilashorn, S., & Crimmins, E. (2008). Biodemography: Integrating disciplines to

explain aging. Handbook of theories of aging (pp. 63-85). New York: Springer.

Vaupel, J. W. (2010). Biodemography of human ageing. Nature, 464(7288), 536-542.

Vaupel, J. W., Carey, J. R., Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., Yashin, A. I., Holm, N. V.,

Iachine, I. A., et. al. (1998). Biodemographic trajectories of longevity. Science,

280(5365), 855-860. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5365.855

Vincent, G. K., Velkoff, V. A., & Bureau, U. C. (2010). The next four decades: The older

population in the United States: 2010 to 2050. US Dept. of Commerce,

Economics and Statistics Administration, US Census Bureau.

Wen, M., Browning, C. R., & Cagney, K. A. (2003). Poverty, affluence, and income

inequality: Neighborhood economic structure and its implications for health.

Social Science & Medicine (1982), 57(5), 843-860. doi: 10.1016/s0277-

9536(02)00457-4

Wise, P. H. (2003). The anatomy of a disparity in infant mortality. Annual Review of

Public Health, 24, 341-362. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.24.100901.140816

Yang, Y. (2008). Trends in U.S. adult chronic disease mortality, 1960–1999: Age, period,

and cohort variations. Demography, 45(2), 387-416. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0000

Table 1.1 Biological Evolution Theories

(Adapted from Kirkwood 1991)

23

CHAPTER 2

AN AGE-PERIOD-COHORT ANALYSIS OF CANCER INCIDENCE

AMONG THE OLDEST OLD1

Abstract

Disentangling age, period, and cohort effects for major health conditions in the

oldest old categories will lead to better population projections of morbidity and mortality.

Data from the Utah Cancer Registry (UCR), the U.S. Census, the National Center for

Health Statistics (NCHS) and the National Cancer Institute’s Surveillence Epidemiology

and End Results (SEER) program are used to generate age-specific estimates of cancer

incidence for ages 65–99 from 1973–2002 for Utah. Age-period-cohort (APC) analyses

are used to describe the simultaneous effects of age, period and cohort on cancer

incidence rates in an attempt to understand the population dynamics underlying their

patterns. Our results show increasing cancer incidence rates up to the 85–89 age group

1 Accepted for publication in Population Studies. Coauthored by Ken R. Smith,

Antoinette M. Stroup, and C. Janna Harrell. Research was supported by the Utah Cancer

Registry (Contract No. HHSN261201000026C from the National Cancer Institute's

SEER Program) and NIA grant “Early Life Conditions, Survival, and Health”

(RO1AG022095, Smith PI) with additional support from the Utah State Department of

Health and the University of Utah. We would also like to acknowledge Ruldoph Rull and

Karim Al-Khafaji for their invaluable assistance with this project.

25

followed by declines for ages 90–99 net of period and cohort effects. We find significant

period and cohort effects, suggesting the role of environmental mechanisms in cancer

incidence trends between the ages of 85 and 100.

Introduction

The demographic profile of the United States is changing, with proportionately

more individuals surviving to very old ages. The oldest old population (ages 85+) is

projected to more than triple from its current estimate of 5.7 million to 24 million by

2050 (Vincent, Velkoff, & Bureau, 2010), making it the fastest growing segment of the

population. This substantial growth makes the study of morbidity and mortality for this

age group increasingly important.

The deceleration of all-site mortality at advanced ages is a commonly observed

phenomenon in both humans and animal species (Horiuchi & Wilmoth, 1998; Vaupel et

al., 1998) andcan be explained at the macrolevel due to changes in population

composition (e.g., heterogeneity hypothesis) or at the microlevel attributable to

physiological changes related to aging, (e.g., individual risk hypothesis). Alternatively,

this observed trend may be the result of age misreporting in the oldest age categories,

heterogeneous birth cohorts, and inaccurate measures of mortality in the oldest age

categories (Gavrilov & Gavrilova, 2011). While the association between morbidity and

mortality is complex (Siegel, 2011), studying patterns of human morbidity gives insight

into the age related changes in morbidity and mortality (Svetlana V. Ukraintseva &

Yashin, 2001), particularly for prevalent diseases such as cancer. A more complete

understanding of the determinates of cancer and how these factors change over time can

yield better predictions of morbidity and mortality trends at advanced ages.

26

Disentangling age, period, and cohort effects for major health conditions in the oldest old

categories will allow for more definitive assertions about the possible causes of mortality

deceleration and increased accuracy in forecasting of future trends used to predict the

fiscal burdens of an aging population.

Little is known about age-specific disease incidence and prevalence among the

oldest old, including cancer (Boscoe, 2008). In 2000, the oldest old age group accounted

for 8% of all incident cancer cases, and this number is projected to rise to 17% by 2050

assuming current incidence rates continue (Hayat, Howlader, Reichman, & Edwards,

2007). Unfortunately, traditional surveillance methods limit our ability to examine age-

specific cancer incidence in this subpopulation. The National Cancer Institute’s

Surveillance Epidemiology and End Results (SEER) program aggregates cancer

incidence information for the 85+ age group, making it difficult to study cancer trends in

the oldest old.

The few studies that examine cancer incidence trends after age 85 present

evidence of a deceleration in cancer incidence, prevalence, and mortality at the oldest

ages. (C. Harding, Pompei, Lee, & Wilson, 2008; Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005; Saltzstein,

Behling, & Baergen, 1998). While patterns for different time periods are presented for

the oldest old population, the literature is limited with regard to analyzing change in the

trends over time. Time is a dimension of context, or the structure of the physical and

social environment related to a specific period or historical experiences unique to a birth

cohort, that influences health (Suzuki, 2012). Recent studies have shown the importance

of considering not only the effect of age, but also the role of period and cohort

experiences when studying health outcomes (Reither, Hauser, & Yang, 2009; Yang,

2008). Sex-specific trends in both all-site and site-specific cancer also need to be

27

considered because there may be different biological and social determinants of cancer

for men and women that vary by site (Yancik, 2005). This study aims to contribute to the

current literature by examining age, period, and cohort trends in cancer incidence from

1973 to 2002 for ages 65 to 99 using data from the Utah Cancer Registry (UCR), the

National Cancer Institute’s Surveillance, Epidemiology and End-Results Program

(SEER), the decennial Census, and the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS).

Background

Disentangling Age, Period, and Cohort Effects

Age effects are generally understood to represent the biological characteristics of

an individual. Cross-sectional studies of all-site cancer incidence and death rates show

that rates generally increase with age, peaking between ages 75 and 85, and then

plateauing before declining in advanced ages (Andersen et al., 2005; Arbeev,

Ukraintseva, Arbeeva, & Yashin, 2005a; C. Harding et al., 2008; Saltzstein et al., 1998;

Stanta, 1997). However, many of these studies can only offer limited conclusions about

cancer trends in the oldest old because they aggregated ages 85+, examined a single

period, or failed to consider period and cohort influences.

Period effects can be described as the social and environmental context that

modifies risk for all individuals in a population at a specific point in time. Changes in

cancer screening technology may affect cancer incidence rates at all ages.

Mammography screening became widespread during the 1980s, leading to an increase in

incident female breast cancer diagnoses over the age of 65 (Edwards et al., 2002); colon

cancer cases increased during the 1990s as a result of changes in colorectal screening

(Edwards et al., 2002); and, there was a steep increase in incident prostate cancer cases

28

for males over the age of 65 between 1988 and 1992 due to the introduction of the

Prostate-Specific Antigen (PSA) screening test for prostate cancer (Edwards et al., 2002).

Changes in health care policy may also create period effects in cancer incidence. For

example, Medicare began covering mammographies in 1991(Kelaher & Stellman, 2000)

and colon cancer screening in 2001 (Berkowitz, Hawkins, Peipins, White, & Nadel,

2008). Thus, the introduction of new diagnostic tools into the health care market, and

changes in screening policies and medical practices have an impact on incidence rates

over time.

Cohort effects describe the social or ecological environment unique to individuals

born in the same group of years. Epidemiologists often describe cohort effects as the

interaction between age and period, while sociologists conceptualize them as a measure

of social forces that shape health throughout the life course (Keyes, Utz, Robinson, & Li,

2010). For example, changes in smoking patterns or other environmental exposures over

time may lead to cohort specific trends in cancer incidence. Improvement in cancer

screening technology may also have differential effects by birth cohort because the

benefit is not equally shared amongst all ages. For example, the Agency for Healthcare

Research and Quality does not recommend routine colonoscopies after the age of 75

(National Guideline), and questions about the efficacy of cancer screening for the oldest

old have also been raised (Østbye, Greenberg, Taylor, & Lee, 2003). In addition to the

age-based bias created by cancer screening recommendations, cancer incidence rates for

these ages may be subject to detection bias because screening is difficult for frail

individuals (Ukraintseva & Yashin, 2003). A more comprehensive understanding of the

age, period, and cohort factors contributing to cancer incidence rates in the oldest old is

29

essential for developing effective screening and treatment recommendations for this

population.

Cross-sectional analyses show a decline in cancer incidence for the oldest old that

is similar for men and women. However, results from previous studies indicate that the

shape, height, and peak of age-specific incidence curves are sensitive to both historical

period, cancer site, and study (C. Harding et al., 2008; Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005;

Saltzstein et al., 1998). If cancer incidence rates in this age group were based strictly on

the biological factors contributing to aging, one would expect to see consistency in age-

specific rates over multiple periods of study. However, the fluctuation in rates is

evidence of the influence of external factors, related to period and birth cohort,

contributing to cancer incidence. Treating the pattern of decline as an effect of aging

neglects evidence of a social and ecological context that may alter age-specific trends and

ignores the multifaceted determinates of cancer risk (Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005; Stanta,

1997). Using a comprehensive approach that studies cancer trends over time and

accounts for period and cohort effects will allow for a more accurate depiction of the age-

specific trends in cancer incidence.

Aging and Cancer

Disagreement exists among theories explaining the relationship between cancer

and aging and the observed decline in cancer incidence in the oldest old (Anisimov,

2003; Ukraintseva & Yashin, 2003). These controversies are similar to those surrounding

mortality deceleration and may prove useful for understanding the mechanisms driving

mortality deceleration.

30

There are three prevailing hypothesis explaining mortality deceleration (Gavrilov

& Gavrilova, 2011; Horiuchi & Wilmoth, 1998). The first hypothesis asserts that the

observed patterns of mortality deceleration are the result of age-misreporting and/or

model misspecification (Gavrilov & Gavrilova, 2011), thereby suggesting that mortality

does not decelerate with age, but is a statistical artifact. The other two hypotheses are the

heterogeneity hypothesis and the individual risk hypothesis (Horiuchi & Wilmoth, 1998).

These hypotheses lead to similar predictions about age related changes in cancer

incidence.

Theories Predicting that an Individual’s Cancer Risk Increases

with Age (Deceleration is an Artifact)

The multistage theory predicts that cancer incidence rates should increase with

age because the neoplastic transformation of cells occurs through several successive steps

(Anisimov, 2003; Armitage & Doll, 1954). This framework describes cancer incidence

as a power function of exposure time rather than age because cancer is caused by the dose

and duration of carcinogenic exposure over a person’s lifetime (Anisimov, 2003;

Ukraintseva & Yashin, 2003). Under this scheme, the path to cancer is step-wise and

irreversible, with each step leading to an increased probability of malignant

transformation with exposure time and therefore age. However, exposure risks between

cohorts may vary, giving rise to different patterns of age related incidence between birth

cohorts.

Physiological mechanisms may also explain increases of cancer incidence with

age. The cancer-longevity tradeoff hypothesis suggests that the cost of living a long life

is cancer. Physiological changes in the tissue microenvironment, telomere dysfunction,

31

a decline in immune surveillance, loss in tumor suppressor function, and mutation

accumulation are additional factors that have been cited as possible mechanisms leading

to the increasing rates of cancer incidence with age (Anisimov, 2003; Campisi, 2003;

Ukraintseva & Yashin, 2003). Many of these factors may be modified by environmental

exposures and therefore the context of time. Factors such as diet, smoking, exposure to

infectious disease (Ukraintseva & Yashin, 2003); and environmental interventions such

as exercise, social support, and screening practices, may make age specific trends

sensitive to temporal context.

Theories Predicting that an Individual’s Cancer Risk Declines

with Age (Individual Risk Hypothesis)

The individual risk hypothesis argues that the deceleration in morbidity and

mortality rates at older ages can be explained in terms of physiology, evolution, and

health behaviors (Horiuchi & Wilmoth, 1998; Vaupel et al., 1998). Although

physiological mechanisms have been used to explain increasing cancer incidence with

age, they may also predict the opposite—that cancer incidence in the oldest old age

categories decelerate and decline. Physiological changes can contribute to the decline in

cancer incidence in the oldest old through age-related declines in rates of cellular

metabolism, suppression of tumor generation, and increased cellular doubling time

(Ukraintseva & Yashin, 2003).

Natural selection may also affect age related declines in cancer incidence.

Mutation accumulation theory argues that age-related declines in the force of natural

selection may result in an accumulation of mutations that result in an increase in

mortality beginning near the end of reproductive ages followed by mortality deceleration

32

in the oldest age categories (Horiuchi & Wilmoth, 1998). In addition, mechanisms which

may protect against cancer may increase longevity, suggesting that individuals in the

oldest old age groups may be less susceptible to cancer (Campisi, 2003).

Variation in age-related health behaviors could also explain a decrease in cancer

incidence in the oldest old age groups. Cancer trends periodically shift due to changes in

screening procedures and recommendations, but these period effects may not be equal

across all ages. Routine cancer screening has increased in the general population

(Edwards et al., 2002); however, studies have suggested that there is a decrease in

surveillance for the oldest old and an increase in misdiagnosed or unreported tumors

(Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005; Stanta, 1997). These factors may lead to cohort specific

trends in cancer incidence.

Population Heterogeneity Leads to Decreased Rates of Cancer

Incidence in the Oldest Old (Heterogeneity Hypothesis).

Population heterogeneity, differential risk patterns within a population, can be the

result of both within and between cohort differences, making the context of cohort an

important consideration. Within a single cohort heterogeneity can occur because the

force of mortality may decrease at advanced ages (Horiuchi & Wilmoth, 1998), pointing

researchers to a selection hypothesis to explain the decline. According to these

hypotheses, there is differential selection in a heterogeneous population with the frail

being selected out of the population at earlier ages (Hawkes, Smith, & Blevins, 2012).

Individuals culled from the population may have a genetic or environmental

predisposition to cancer, leaving their robust counterparts to survive to the oldest ages

with a survival advantage that protects them from cancer. For example, individuals with

33

deleterious mutations, such as the BRCA1 mutation, have elevated cancer mortality rates

as compared to the general population, making them less likely to survive to advanced

ages (K. R. Smith, Hanson, Mineau, & Buys, 2011).

Population heterogeneity can also arise because different cohorts have

experienced different mortality schedules, environmental exposures, public health

initiatives (such as antismoking campaigns), and cancer screening recommendations. It

has been suggested that the multistage theory is correct, and a plateau or decline in cancer

incidence rates at old ages may reflect period and cohort trends (Yang, 2008). If

exposure to different carcinogens such as tobacco smoke, changes in diet, or other

environmental carcinogens, fluctuates over time the deceleration in incidence rates at the

oldest ages may reflect these changes rather than somatic aging per se. A decline or

deceleration in cancer trends in old ages may be a function of cohort experiences such as

screening practices or health behaviors for this age group.

Understanding the relationship between cancer incidence and age will not only

improve future predictions of cancer incidence, it will help U.S. understand the

mechanisms leading to mortality deceleration in the oldest old population. Cancer trends

for the oldest old population are understudied because cancer incidence rates are

historically aggregated for all 85+ individuals (Boscoe, 2008). This study aims to

improve upon current literature by examining temporal trends of cancer incidence from

1973 to 2002 for ages 65 to 99.

34

Methods

Data

This study uses data for the state of Utah from 1973 to 2002 collected from the

Utah Cancer Registry (UCR), the National Cancer Institute’s Surveillance, Epidemiology

and End-Results Program (SEER), the decennial Census, and the National Center for

Health Statistics (NCHS). Cancer incidence cases and the U.S. Census Bureau’s

Population Estimates for the state of Utah for ages 65 to 84 were obtained from

SEER*Stat software (2010; 2012). Statewide cancer data are collected by the UCR as

part of routine cancer surveillance for the Utah Department of Health and the National

Cancer Institute’s SEER Program. Cancer cases are reported to the UCR through health

service providers and death certificates on which cancer is listed as a cause of death. Site

and histology are coded according to the International Classification of Diseases for

Oncology (ICD-O) at the time of diagnosis (Stroup, Dibble, & Harrell, 2008).

Age-specific incidence counts for ages 85 to 99 are not reported by the SEER

program. At these ages, age misstatement is a widely recognized problem, making

population estimates less reliable (Boscoe, 2008). Tabulated incidence data by year and

age were provided by the UCR. Intercensal population estimates were calculated via the

cohort-component and extinct-cohort methods using decennial data from the U.S. Census

Bureau and mortality data from the National Center for Health Statistics (Shryock,

Siegel, & Larmon, 1980). The cohort-component method starts with the cohort

populations reported in the decennial census and then subtracts deaths to estimate

population sizes. The use of census and death certificate data has been criticized because

upward age-misstatement can lead to a downward bias of incidence rates for this

population. However, age misstatement has been shown to be a relatively rare

35

occurrence with error rates improving over time (Boscoe, 2008; Hill, Preston, &

Rosenwaike, 2000; SH Preston, Stewart, & Elo, 1999). Data collection issues have also

caused errors in population estimates for the oldest old (Siegel & Passel, 1976). The

extinct-cohort method is an alternative method of calculating population counts. It relies

on death certificate data and is thought to be more reliable when cohorts are close to

extinction because it is less subject to bias caused by age misreporting. Rates from both

methods were compared and we found that when cohorts are farther from extinction

estimates using the extinct-cohort method become less reliable. The cohort component

method was selected as the basis for the final models. A detailed description of the

methods and comparison between rates will be presented in an article by Rudy et al. and

can be provided upon request. The final data set consisted of population level cancer

incidence counts (numerators) and cohort-component population estimates

(denominators) for the 65 to 99 year old Utah population from 1973 to 2002 by sex.

Statistical Methods

Sex- and site-specific cancer incidence trends from 1973-2002 for ages 65 to 99

for the state of Utah were selected for analysis. There were no incidence cases above age

100 from 1973 – 1982 and 1988 – 1997 for males and 1973 – 1977 for females; therefore

we did not include this age category in the analysis. Cancer incidence rates were

calculated as the ratio of incident cases to person years of exposure. Age-specific

incidence rates were tabulated in age a by calendar year period p arrays with diagonal

elements of the matrix corresponding to the birth cohorts c (c = p + a -1), where the

oldest cohort is observed for the oldest age interval during the earliest calendar period

and the youngest cohort is observed for the youngest age interval during the latest

36

calendar period. Seven 5-year age groups, ranging from 65 – 69 to 95 – 99, and six 5-

year time periods, from 1973 – 1977 to 1998 – 2002 were used in the analysis. There is

some ambiguity in the measurement of cohorts because data are tabulated into 5-year age

and period groupings. For example, an individual age 69 in 1973 would have a birth year

of 1904 while an individual age 65 in 1978 would have a birth year of 1913. This yielded

12 successive ten year birth cohorts with midpoints ranging from 1878 – 1933, which

were used for the age, period, and cohort (APC) analyses.

Traditional APC analyses suffer from an “identification problem” resulting from

the linear dependency between age, period and cohort (c=a +p), precluding a unique

solution. This problem can be solved by imposing constraints to the model to allow for

an identifiable solution (Arbeev et al., 2005a; Arbeev, Ukraintseva, Arbeeva, & Yashin,

2005b; Carstensen, 2007; Yang, Fu, & Land, 2004). However, selection of the

constraint requires some a priori knowledge of the disease under investigation and

models are sensitive to the constraint selected. Other authors have suggested using a

proxy characteristic for cohort (O'Brien, 1989, 2000; O'Brien, Stockard, & Isaacson,

1999). However, cohort characteristics may not entirely explain cohort effects and the

residuals may still be confounded in the model estimates with age and period effects.

The Intrinsic Estimator (IE) proposed by Yang et al. (Yang et al., 2004; Yang,

Schulhofer‐Wohl, Fu, & Land, 2008) can also be viewed as a constrained approach;

however, it does not require a priori assumptions about the constraints. Other studies

have shown that the IE produces substantively meaningful and empirically valid results

(Yang et al., 2008), and the effects can be interpreted like conventional regression

coefficients (D. J. Harding, 2009). The limitations to this approach include the lack of a

simple explanation of its assumptions and the lack of a full investigation of its properties

37

(D. J. Harding, 2009; H. L. Smith, 2004). After initial estimations of a series of Poisson

log-linear models, we selected the IE to estimate the APC effects of cancer incidence

based on evidence of distinct age, period, and cohort effects and model fit.

Descriptive plots were produced by age group and sex for all-site, breast, colon,

and prostate cancers to assess the age, period, and cohort cancer incidence trends. Trends

in lung cancer incidence rates were not assessed as part of this analysis because the

incidence rates in Utah are very low (Jemal et al., 2008) and the case counts above age 85

were sparse. A similar approach to that used by Yang et al. (Yang et al., 2004; Yang et

al., 2008) was used to identify an appropriate model to analyze the temporal trends in all-

site and site-specific cancer incidence rates. A series of Poisson log-linear models were

estimated for each site and sex:

ln(mijk) = ln(cijk/nijk) = μ + αi + βj + γk (eq. 2.1)

where rateijk indexes the expected cancer incidence rate in cell (i, j, k); cijk indexes the

observed number of cancer incidence cases; nijk indexes the number of person years; µ

indexes the intercept of age adjusted mean rate; αi indexes the ith row age effect for i = 1,

. . . , a age groups; βj represents the jth column period effect for j =1, . . ., p periods; and

γk represents the kth diagonal cohort effect for k =1, . . ., (a + p -1) cohorts.

One-way models (a, p, or c), two factor models (ap, pc, ca) and IE models were

compared. Both descriptive analyses and model fit, based on the Akaike Information

Criterion (AIC), were used to select the final models. Full analyses are available upon

request. The log-linear regression coefficients, standard errors, and model fit were

computed using Stata 11. 2. Estimates of the full APC models using the IE approach and

the apc_ie.ado downloaded from the Stata command line. To test whether our findings

38

were sensitive to method of denominator construction, the final models were replicated

using denominators constructed using the extinct cohort method.

Results

To assess the variation in age-specific incidence by period a series of age-period

plots were created. Figure 2.1 shows the sex-specific age and period all-site cancer

incidence rate plots. Each panel displays the age-specific incidence trends by calendar

period, ranging from 1973 - 1978 to 1998 - 2002. The solid lines present the full 85+

age-specific rates and the dashed lines display the trend when cancer rates are top-coded

at age 85. These plots show that when rates are aggregated for the 85 plus age group,

specious conclusions about the age at which incidence rates peak may be drawn. These

plots indicate that for the vast majority of the periods, the age group with the highest

incidence rates can be found in the 85 to 89 age groups. The peak is then followed by a

leveling off or decline for the 90 to 94 and 95 to 99 age groups. These plots also show

that the trends are not stable over time. If the only factor influencing cancer incidence

trends were age, one would expect the age-specific curves for all periods to follow

similar trajectories. However, the plots show variation in the intercept (depicted for age

65 - 69), slope, and shape of the curve. There has been a steady increase in the level of

cancer incidence over time, with the most recent periods having the highest rates for a

majority of the age groups.

Age-cohort plots were created to assess the variation in the age trends by birth

cohort and sex. Figure 2.2 displays the variation in shape, slope, and peak in all-site

cancer incidence age trends by cohort for both males and females. We are unable to

observe a single cohort for the entire age range because we have data for a 30 year

39

window only, from 1973 - 2002. Age trends for the 1903 and 1908 cohorts have the

longest follow-up, with incidence calculated for ages 70 to 99 and 65 to 94, respectively.

There is less variation in the trend in age specific incidence rates between cohorts for

females as compared to males. The plots suggest that for men the peak in incidence is

moving to younger ages for more recent birth cohorts; however, these peaks coincide

with the expected rise in incidence that resulted from the PSA testing for prostate cancer.

There is a general increase in cancer incidence rates at younger ages for more recent

cohorts for both males and females. For both sexes, the largest differences in cancer

incidence occur at ages 90 – 99. The imprecision at advanced ages is partially a function

of decreased sample sizes. The age-period and age-cohort plots indicate period and

cohort factors may confound observed trends in age specific cancer incidence.

A series of Poisson log-linear and IE models were used to further investigate age,

period, and cohort effects. The goodness-of-fit statistics for the log linear models are

displayed in Table S1 in the online supplement. The IE model provided the best fit for

both male and female all-site cancer, female breast cancer, and prostate cancer incidence.

The fit statistics suggest weak cohort effects for women, with the IE model only

providing a slightly better fit than the age-period models for both all-site and breast

cancer incidence. The age-period model provided the best fit for both male and female

colon cancer incidence models.

Figure 2.3 shows the IE results for all-site cancer incidence by sex. The figure

shows an increase across ages in cancer incidence up to age 85. All-site cancer incidence

rates are the highest for the 85 to 89 age group net period and cohort effects for both

males and females. Female all-site incidence rates for ages 90 and above level off and

the estimated coefficients are not statistically significant. There is a steeper decline in the

40

all-site incidence rate for males after age 85; however, it is noteworthy that the estimated

coefficient for rates at age 90 (p=0.06) is still higher than the estimated rates for both the

60 to 64 and 65 to 69 age groups.

The period specific trends show that there has been a gradual increase in cancer

incidence over time. While this gradual increase in cancer incidence with time may be

indicative of changes in screening behaviors or environmental exposures, it may also be

an artifact of changes in cancer surveillance methods, with more complete identification

of cases recorded over time. The period effects for males are as expected based on the

descriptive analyses and the known increase in cancer incidence between 1988 and 1992,

which is attributable to shifts in prostate screening practices. The rates drop to pre-1988

levels in the next period and continue their decline into the 1998 – 2002 period.

Figure 2.3 also shows that there are moderate cohort effects for females. All-site

cancer incidence was significantly higher for the 1888 and 1893 birth cohorts and lower

for the 1928 birth cohort. The cohort effects for males resemble a trough, with slightly

elevated risk (albeit insignificant) for early cohorts, followed by a decline and leveling

off for the 1903 to 1917 birth cohorts, and ending with an increase that almost reaches the

height of the 1883 birth cohort.

Figure 2.4 displays the IE estimates for breast and prostate cancer. The age

effects for the site specific cancers are somewhat different than the all-sites trends. For

females, the highest level of breast cancer incidence and the only estimate significantly

different from zero, is found between the ages of 75 to 79 (p=0.03). The period effects

are similar to those observed in the all-site rates as there is a gradual increase in female

breast cancer incidence over time. Cohort effects play a small role in determining female

breast cancer incidence over age 65. The decline in breast cancer incidence for the 1928

41

birth cohort is somewhat consistent with previous studies that show a decline in breast

cancer incidence for the 1924 to 1938 birth cohorts (Lacey, Devesa, & Brinton, 2002).

The age effect for males steadily increases up to age 75 where it reaches a plateau

followed by a decline at age 90. Male prostate cancer incidence steadily increases up to

1988 and then sharply declines over time, again for reasons of PSA testing. Male cohorts

between 1898 and 1918 have slightly lower rates of prostate cancer incidence. A steady

rise in prostate cancer rates is seen in subsequent cohorts.

Figure 2.5 shows the estimated coefficients for the log-linear AP models of colon

cancer incidence. Fit statistics showed that the two-factor model provided the best fit for

colon cancer incidence, meaning that cohort effects can be constrained to zero. Fig. 2.5

indicates that there is a steady increase in colon cancer incidence with age up to age 85,

followed by a slight decline (albeit still significantly higher than the referent category of

65 – 69) at the advanced ages. The period trends show a slight elevation in colon cancer

incidence between 1983 and 1987 for females and between 1983 and 1992 for males as

compared to the sex specific incidence rates in 1973 – 1977.

To test the sensitivity of our findings, the final models were estimated using

denominators created using the extinct cohort method. The results did not substantively

change the findings presented and are not shown here.

Discussion

This study found evidence supporting hypotheses of an increase in all-site cancer

incidence up to ages 85-89 net period and cohort effects, followed by a modest decline up

to age 99. Although incidence appears to drop after age 90, the rates up to age 99 are still

higher than rates for individuals aged 65-74. This finding is supported by other studies

42

and highlights the importance of disaggregating cancer incidence rates for the oldest old

(C. Harding et al., 2008; D. W. E. Smith, 1996). We found evidence of period and cohort

effects influencing cancer trends, which highlights the importance of considering the

social factors that influence biology when studying cancer trends in this population. The

benefits of disaggregated estimates for the oldest old far outweigh the potential

challenges related to age misreporting. As more people reach these advance ages, it will

become increasingly important to understand the biological and social mechanisms

affecting cancer trends in the oldest old. These results answer Boscoe’s call for greater

specificity in age-specific data for the oldest old (Boscoe, 2008).

We conclude that the age, period, and cohort effects of site specific cancer

incidence varied by site and sex. Physiological mechanisms have been the primary

mechanisms used to explain the decline in incidence by other authors. Harding et al.

propose a simple senescence theory, claiming that increasing senescence reduces the

ability of cells to divide and limits cancer incidence in the oldest old population (C.

Harding et al., 2008; C. Harding, Pompei, & Wilson, 2012). Our results suggest that age

is not the only factor contributing to the decline in cancer incidence in the oldest old age

group. We are not arguing against a biological model of cancer decline, but we do

advocate a more inclusive theory that considers socio-environmental factors and

mortality selection, which may influence the age-specific trends.

For women, the gradual increase in all-site and breast cancer incidence over time

may be partially due to increased detection by mammographic screening (Edwards et al.,

2002) and improvements in data collection and classification. These trends in breast

cancer incidence are also consistent with previously reported trends in breast cancer (C.

Harding et al., 2008; Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005; Saltzstein et al., 1998). For men, the

43

dramatic increase in cancer incidence up to the 1988 – 1992 period is consistent with

other studies of period trends in prostate cancer incidence (Edwards et al., 2002) and the

introduction of the PSA screening test. The clear period trends observed for both men

and women bring attention to the importance of understanding how period factors

influence cancer incidence.

In our study, cohort effects played a larger role for males than females and did not

affect colon cancer incidence rates. Our results show variations in cohort trends of

cancer incidence over time that coincide with changes in environmental exposure to

tobacco products. Birth cohort did not explain variation in incidence trends for all sites in

this study. The absence of a cohort trend in colon cancer incidence is inconsistent with

other studies of colon cancer trends (Chu, Tarone, Chow, Hankey, & Ries, 1994);

however, cancer mortality trends may reflect improvements in detection and treatment

that prevent colon cancer mortality but do not necessarily modify the risk of colon cancer

incidence (which was not reported in this study). Failing to account for heterogeneity

between cohorts may lead to erroneous conclusions about deceleration in trends where

none exists (Gavrilov & Gavrilova, 2011). We have shown that there is a difference in

cancer susceptibility between cohorts, but controlling for these differences does not alter

the conclusion that cancer incidence rates do not exponentially increase with age.

Heterogeneity within a birth cohort may also be an important mechanism driving

the deceleration and decline in cancer incidence in the oldest old. Declines in the force of

mortality at all ages may lead to increased population heterogeneity at old ages, thus

possibly cohorts of individuals with more susceptibility to cancer (Hawkes et al., 2012).

However, it is also possible that there is less within-cohort heterogeneity in more recent

birth cohorts. Lynch and Brown (2001), have found evidence of less variation in

44

population frailty in birth cohorts in more contemporary cohorts, suggesting that the

population is becoming more homogeneously robust (Lynch & Brown, 2001). If this is

true, then it is possible that cancer incidence rates will decline in the future. The gradual

increase in cancer incidence for males during more recent birth cohorts, reported here,

suggests changing cohort susceptibility to cancer, a position inconsistent with the

argument that populations shift to become more homogenously robust. While it is

difficult to separate the environmental factors from the changes in population

heterogeneity, this finding emphasizes the importance of both improved surveillance in

cancer trends for the oldest old and consideration of cohort effects when studying oldest

old cancer trends.

Variation in health and cancer screening behaviors with age may also explain the

decline in incidence and sex differences in period and cohort effects. Sex differences in

cohort experiences may reflect sex differences in the timing, prevalence, and frequency

of smoking; sex differences in environmental exposures to carcinogens in the work place;

or sex differences in other risky behaviors that are patterned by generational experiences.

For example, Preston and Wang (2006) have demonstrated the close relationship between

a cohort’s mortality trajectory and its history of cigarette smoking as well as sex

differences in smoking prevalence within the cohort. It is unlikely that between-cohort

variability or period specific shifts in health behaviors are strong determinants of the age-

specific trend in cancer incidence because incidence rates decline above age 90 when

controlling for cohort and period trends.

Screening bias is another individual level component that may reduce cancer

incidence for the oldest old. For example, there were strong period effects associated

with the introduction of PSA testing for males. The birth cohorts that would have been in

45

the oldest age categories at the time PSA testing was introduced have decreased cancer

incidence rates relative to those cohorts that would have been under the age of 85. This

finding emphasizes the importance of both improved surveillance in cancer trends for the

oldest as well as further investigation into the causes of cohort differences in cancer

incidence. Kaplan and Satlzstein note parallel downward sloping trends between breast

cancer screening and prevalence in the aged population (Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005).

However, other studies not subject to screening bias show a similar trend (Stanta, 1997).

This study also suggests that screening bias may not be the reason for decreases in cancer

incidence with age. SEER data collection processes include checking death certificate

information. Any cancer occurrence contributing to the cause of death of an individual is

reported to the SEER registries. Only cancers that are not an underlying factor in the

death would be missed by the system, suggesting that detection bias is not a likely cause

in the observed decrease in cancer at advanced ages.

Our results differ slightly from previously reported trends in cancer incidence.

The estimated peak in incidence in the 85 – 89 age category for males and females is

higher than the Harding et al. (2008) estimates of 80. However, our results are consistent

with the peak observed by Saltzstein et al. (1998). There are three possible explanations

for the variation in the estimated peaks in age-specific incidence rates. First, previous

studies of age-specific cancer incidence in the oldest old age group have not considered

the role of period and cohort effects. Ignoring exogenous factors that may contribute to

cancer incidence oversimplifies the problem and leads to the age-specific incidence rates

that are confounded by period and cohort differences in cancer incidence. APC analyses

of cancer incidence may provide less biased estimates of the true relationship between

cancer and aging.

46

Second, the differences may be caused by error in the estimated denominators.

Preston et al. (1999) found that the difference between the correct population distribution

and one estimated with age overstatement increased with time (Preston et al., 1999). Any

difference between the estimated peak cancer incidence and the true peak cancer

incidence should be negative. If age-misstatement led U.S. to overestimate the size of the

oldest old population, then our estimate of a peak in incidence between the ages of 85 and

89 is conservative. Furthermore, because other studies have also used decennial census

data to construct their denominators (C. Harding et al., 2008; C. Harding et al., 2012), we

argue that this is not the reason for the observed differences in peak age-specific

incidence rates.

Third, while not directly assessed, our findings are suggestive of geographic

variation in cancer incidence trends given the difference in the estimated peak of cancer

incidence between our study and other U.S. studies of overlapping time periods (Harding

et al., 2008; Harding et al., 2012; Saltzstein et al., 1998). Regional differences in cancer

incidence trends may be attributable to differences in sociodemographic characteristics,

health beliefs, access to resources, reproductive characteristics, and exposure to

environmental carcinogens. Utah consistently has one of the lowest cancer incidence and

mortality rates in the U.S. for both males and females and the lowest rates of lung cancer

incidence and mortality (Jemal et al., 2008). However, Utah does not have low incidence

rates for all cancer sites. Age-adjusted prostate cancer incidence rates are higher in Utah

than the national averages (Stroup et al., 2008). More research should be done to

quantify geographic differences as it will provide valuable information about the social

forces shaping cancer incidence rates in these age groups.

47

There are several structural limitations to studying cancer trends in the oldest old

age groups. Use of clinical and death certificate diagnoses may lead to an underreporting

of trends in the oldest old (Kaplan & Saltzstein, 2005; Stanta, 1997). Trends calculated

using cancer registry and census data are subject to error because the reliability of age

estimates for individuals over the age of 85 may be questionable (Edwards et al., 2002;

Vincent et al., 2010). However, we used several different methods to create measures of

population size and found no substantive differences in the age, period, and cohort trends

in all-site cancer incidence.

There is not widespread consensus in the cause of the decline in cancer rates at

advanced ages because it has been largely understudied. This study contributes to the

current literature by providing estimates of cancer incidence for the 85+ population

within the broader context of period and cohort effects. This study supports the

individual risk hypothesis and mortality selection arguments that predict a deceleration in

incidence at advanced ages. Our findings do not support the position that deceleration is

an artifact of variability in morbidity profiles between cohorts, nor do they support

arguments that cancer incidence trends are strictly a function of biological mechanisms.

Studies utilizing an APC approach to the analysis of cancer trends may provide less

biased estimates of the relationship between cancer and aging and improve knowledge

about the role of biological and social influences that modify trends. The existence of

cohort and period effects also justifies the use of direct measures of the exogenous factors

contributing to cancer incidence. Future studies should evaluate the proportion of

variation in cancer incidence explained by direct measures of period influences and

cohort characteristics. Future studies should also investigate morbidity trends in other

major causes of death and explore the relationship between these trends and mortality

48

deceleration. We also show that there is variation in cancer incidence trends in the oldest

old population and reiterate the importance of treating this population as heterogeneous.

In order to gain a more comprehensive understanding of morbidity and mortality patterns

for this rapidly growing segment of the population, cancer incidence and U.S. Census

population estimates need to be disaggregated for the oldest old population.

49

References

Andersen, S. L., Terry, D. F., Wilcox, M. A., Babineau, T., Malek, K., & Perls, T. T.

(2005). Cancer in the oldest old. Mechanisms Of Ageing And Development,

126(2), 263-267.

Anisimov, V. N. (2003). The relationship between aging and carcinogenesis: A critical

appraisal. Critical Reviews In Oncology/Hematology, 45(3), 277-304.

Arbeev, K. G., Ukraintseva, S. V., Arbeeva, L. S., & Yashin, A. I. (2005a). Decline in

human cancer incidence rates at old ages: Age-period-cohort considerations.

Demographic Research, 12(11), 273-300.

Arbeev, K. G., Ukraintseva, S. V., Arbeeva, L. S., & Yashin, A. I. (2005b). Mathematical

models for human cancer incidence rates. Demographic Research, 12(10), 237-

260.

Armitage, P., & Doll, R. (1954). The age distribution of cancer and a multi-stage theory

of carcinogenesis. British Journal of Cancer, 8(1), 1.

Berkowitz, Z., Hawkins, N. A., Peipins, L. A., White, M. C., & Nadel, M. R. (2008).

Beliefs, risk perceptions, and gaps in knowledge as barriers to colorectal cancer

screening in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 56(2), 307-

314. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01547.x

Boscoe, F. P. (2008). Subdividing the age group of 85 years and older to improve U.S.

disease reporting. American Journal of Public Health, 98(7), 1167-1170. doi:

10.2105/ajph.2008.133900

Campisi, J. (2003). Cancer and ageing: Rival demons? Nature Reviews Cancer, 3(5),

339-349.

Carstensen, B. (2007). Age-period-cohort models for the Lexis diagram. Statistics in

Medicine, 26(15), 3018-3045.

Chu, K. C., Tarone, R. E., Chow, W.-H., Hankey, B. F., & Ries, L. A. G. (1994).

Temporal patterns in colorectal cancer incidence, survival, and mortality from

1950 through 1990. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 86(13), 997-1006.

doi: 10.1093/jnci/86.13.997

Edwards, B. K., Howe, H. L., Ries, L. A. G., Thun, M. J., Rosenberg, H. M., Yancik, R.,

Wingo, P., et. al. (2002). Annual report to the nation on the status of cancer,

1973–1999, featuring implications of age and aging on U.S. cancer burden.

Cancer, 94(10), 2766-2792.

50

Gavrilov, L. A., & Gavrilova, N. S. (2011). Mortality measurement at advanced ages: A

study of the Social Security Administration Death Master File. North American

Actuarial Journal: NAAJ, 15(3), 432.

Harding, C., Pompei, F., Lee, E. E., & Wilson, R. (2008). Cancer suppression at old age.

Cancer Research, 68(11), 4465.

Harding, C., Pompei, F., & Wilson, R. (2012). Peak and decline in cancer incidence,

mortality, and prevalence at old ages. Cancer, 118(5), 1371-1386.

Harding, D. J. (2009). Recent advances in age-period-cohort analysis. A commentary on

Dregan and Armstrong, and on Reither, Hauser and Yang. Social Science &

Medicine, 69(10), 1449-1451. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.034

Hawkes, K., Smith, K. R., & Blevins, J. K. (2012). Human actuarial aging increases

faster when background death rates are lower: A consequence of differential

heterogeneity? Evolution, 66(1), s103-114.

Hayat, M. J., Howlader, N., Reichman, M. E., & Edwards, B. K. (2007). Cancer statistics,

trends, and multiple primary cancer analyses from the Surveillance,

Epidemiology, and End Results (SEER) program. The Oncologist, 12(1), 20-37.

doi: 10.1634/theoncologist.12-1-20

Hill, M., Preston, S., & Rosenwaike, I. (2000). Age reporting among white Americans

aged 85+: Results of a record linkage study. Demography, 37(2), 175-186. doi:

10.2307/2648119

Horiuchi, S., & Wilmoth, J. (1998). Deceleration in the age pattern of mortality at

olderages. Demography, 35(4), 391-412. doi: 10.2307/3004009

Jemal, A., Thun, M. J., Ries, L. A. G., Howe, H. L., Weir, H. K., Center, M. M., . . .

Edwards, B. K. (2008). Annual report to the nation on the status of cancer, 1975–

2005, featuring trends in lung cancer, tobacco use, and tobacco control. Journal of

the National Cancer Institute, 100(23), 1672-1694. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djn389

Kaplan, R. M., & Saltzstein, S. L. (2005). Reduced mammographic screening may

explain declines in breast carcinoma in older women. Journal of the American

Geriatrics Society, 53(5), 862-866.

Kelaher, M., & Stellman, J. M. (2000). The impact of medicare funding on the use of

mammography among older women: implications for improving access to

screening. Preventive Medicine, 31(6), 658-664. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0759

Keyes, K. M., Utz, R. L., Robinson, W., & Li, G. (2010). What is a cohort effect?

Comparison of three statistical methods for modeling cohort effects in obesity

51

prevalence in the United States, 1971–2006. Social Science & Medicine, 70(7),

1100-1108. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.12.018

Lacey, J. V., Devesa, S. S., & Brinton, L. A. (2002). Recent trends in breast cancer

incidence and mortality. Environmental and Molecular Mutagenesis, 39(2-3), 82-

88. doi: 10.1002/em.10062

Lynch, S. M., & Brown, J. S. (2001). Reconsidering mortality compression and

deceleration: An alternative model of mortality rates. Demography, 38(1), 79-95.

National Guideline, C. Colorectal cancer screening clinical practice guideline. Retrieved

11/30/2011, from http://www.guideline.gov

O'Brien, R. M. (1989). Relative cohort size and age-specific crime rates: An age-period-

relative-cohort-size model. Criminology, 27(1), 57-78.

O'Brien, R. M. (2000). Age period cohort characteristic models. Social Science Research,

29(1), 123-139.

O'Brien, R. M., Stockard, J., & Isaacson, L. (1999). The enduring effects of cohort

characteristics on age-specific homicide rates, 1960-1995 1. American Journal of

Sociology, 104(4).

Østbye, T., Greenberg, G. N., Taylor, D. H., & Lee, A. M. M. (2003). Screening

mammography and pap tests among older american women 1996–2000: Results

from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and Asset and Health Dynamics

Among the Oldest Old (AHEAD). The Annals of Family Medicine, 1(4), 209-217.

doi: 10.1370/afm.54

Preston, S., Stewart, Q., & Elo, I. (1999). Effects of age misreporting on mortality

estimates at older ages. Population Studies, 53(2), 165-177.

Preston, S., & Wang, H. (2006). Sex mortality differences in the United States: The role

of cohort smoking patterns. Demography, 43(4), 631-646. doi:

10.1353/dem.2006.0037

Reither, E. N., Hauser, R. M., & Yang, Y. (2009). Do birth cohorts matter? Age-period-

cohort analyses of the obesity epidemic in the United States. Social Science &

Medicine, 69(10), 1439-1448. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.040

Saltzstein, S. L., Behling, C. A., & Baergen, R. N. (1998). Features of cancer in

nonagenarians and centenarians. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,

46(8), 994.

SEER*Stat Database: Incidence - SEER 9 Regs Research Data, Nov 2010 Sub (1973-

2008) <Katrina/Rita Population Adjustment> - Linked To County Attributes -

Total U.S., 1969-2009 Counties, National Cancer Institute, DCCPS, Surveillance

52

Research Program, Cancer Statistics Branch. (2010). Retrieved from:

www.seer.cancer.gov

SEER*Stat Database: Populations - Total U.S. (1969-2009) Single Ages to 85+,

Katrina/Rita Adjustment - Linked To County Attributes - Total U.S., 1969-2009

Counties, National Cancer Institute, DCCPS, Surveillance Research Program,

Cancer Statistics Branch, released April 2011. (2012). Retrieved from:

www.seer.cancer.gov

Shryock, H. S., Siegel, J. S., & Larmon, E. A. (1980). The methods and materials of

demography (Vol. 2): U.S. Dept. of Commerce, Bureau of the Census: For sale by

the Supt. of Docs. U.S. Govt. Print. Off.

Siegel, J. S. (2011). The demography and epidemiology of human health and aging: New

York: Springer.

Siegel, J. S., & Passel, J. S. (1976). New estimates of the number of centenarians in the

United States. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 559-566.

Smith, D. W. E. (1996). Cancer mortality at very old ages. Cancer, 77(7), 1367-1372.

Smith, H. L. (2004). Response: Cohort analysis redux. Sociological Methodology, 34(1),

111-119. doi: 10.1111/j.0081-1750.2004.00149.x

Smith, K. R., Hanson, H. A., Mineau, G. P., & Buys, S. S. (2011). Effects of BRCA1 and

BRCA2 mutations on female fertility. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:

Biological Sciences, 279(1732), 1389-1395.

Stanta, G. (1997). Cancer of the oldest old. What we have learned from autopsy studies.

Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 13(1), 55.

Stroup, A. M., Dibble, R., & Harrell, C. T. R. C. J. (2008). Cancer incidence and

mortality trends in Utah: 1973-2004. UH Review 2008, 25.

Suzuki, E. (2012). Time changes, so do people. Social Science & Medicine, 75(3), 452-

456. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.03.036

Ukraintseva, S. V., & Yashin, A. I. (2001). How individual age-associated changes may

influence human morbidity and mortality patterns. Mechanisms of Ageing and

Development, 122(13), 1447-1460. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0047-

6374(01)00277-9

Ukraintseva, S. V., & Yashin, A. I. (2003). Individual aging and cancer risk: How are

they related. Demographic Research, 9(8), 163-196.

53

Vaupel, J. W., Carey, J. R., Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., Yashin, A. I., Holm, N. V.,

Iachine, I. A. et. al. Curtsinger, J. W. (1998). Biodemographic trajectories of

longevity. Science, 280(5365), 855-860. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5365.855

Vincent, G. K., Velkoff, V. A., & Bureau, U. C. (2010). The next four decades: The older

population in the United States: 2010 to 2050: U.S. Dept. of Commerce,

Economics and Statistics Administration, U.S. Census Bureau.

Yancik, R. (2005). Population aging and cancer: A cross-national concern. The Cancer

Journal, 11(6), 437.

Yang, Y. (2008). Trends in U.S. adult chronic disease mortality, 1960-1999: Age, period,

and cohort variations. Demography, 45(2), 387-416.

Yang, Y., Fu, W. J., & Land, K. C. (2004). A methodological comparison of age‐period‐cohort models: The intrinsic estimator and conventional generalized linear

models. Sociological Methodology, 34(1), 75-110.

Yang, Y., Schulhofer‐Wohl, S., Fu, W. J., & Land, K. C. (2008). The intrinsic estimator

for age‐period‐cohort analysis: What it is and how to use it. American Journal of

Sociology, 113(6), 1697-1736.

54

Figure 2.1. All-site cancer incidence rates by age and period. Panels A and B show Utah

females and males, respectively. Solid lines show trends up to age 99 and dashed lines

show the trend when cancer incidence is aggregated for individuals 85 years and above.

55

Figure 2.2. All-site cancer incidence by birth cohort. Panels A and B show Utah females

and males, respectively. The scale of the y-axis is smaller than the scale for the male

graphs to allow for the visibility of the variation. Female incidence rates are lower

than male incidence rates and increase with age at a slower rate.

56

Figure 2.3. APC IE estimated trends of all-site cancer incidence rates for ages 65 to 99 in

the state of Utah. Dotted lines represent 95% confidence intervals. Panel A: Age effects

net period and cohort. Panel B: Period effects net age and cohort. Panel C: Cohort

effects net age and period.

57

Figure 2.4. APC IE estimates of female breast and male prostate cancer incidence rates

for ages 65 to 99 in the state of Utah. Dotted lines represent 95% confidence intervals.

Panel A: Age effects net period and cohort. Panel B: Period effects net age and cohort.

Panel C: Cohort effects net age and period.

58

Figure 2.5. Poisson log-linear estimates of age and period effects on colon cancer

incidence. Dotted lines represent 95% confidence intervals. Panel A: Age

effects net period. Panel B: Period effects net age.

CHAPTER 3

REPRODUCTIVE HISTORY AND LATER-LIFE COMORBIDITY

TRAJECTORIES2

Abstract

The reproductive lives of men and women may provide significant insight into

later-life health and mortality. Sociological, biological, and evolutionary theories predict

a relationship between reproductive history and later-life health and mortality, however,

current research is lacking consensus on the direction of the relationship. In this study,

the relationship between reproductive history and later-life health is examined using data

based on linkages between the Utah Population Database, a rich source of longitudinal

data, and 18 years of Medicare Claims data. Later-life health is measured using the

Charlson Comorbidity Index, a construct that summarizes nearly all serious illnesses

afflicting older individuals. Single year comorbidity scores are constructed by year from

1992 to 2009. We used group based trajectory modeling that accounts for nonrandom

attrition due to death to identify the number and types of morbidity trajectories by sex

and age group for 52,924 individuals aged 65-84 in 1992. For both males and females,

2 Coauthored by Ken R. Smith and Zachary Zimmer. We wish to thank the Pedigree and

Population Resource of the Huntsman Cancer Institute, University of Utah for providing

the data and valuable computing support. This work was also supported by NIH grant

AG022095 (Early-life Conditions, Survival and Health; Smith PI).

60

trajectory groups ranged from a robust group with little to no comorbid conditions during

the period of observation to a frail group with a consistently high comorbidity. Parity,

age at first birth, age at last birth, birth weight of offspring, having a child die as an

infant, and having a preterm birth predicted trajectory group membership for women but

had little association with trajectory group membership for men.

Introduction

Understanding how individuals experience disease after age 65 is important to

understanding the mechanisms of aging and longevity. Equally important is what

predicts the observed patterns. The etiological model of chronic disease has shifted its

focus from adult risk factors to considering factors throughout the life course (Kuh &

Ben-Shlomo, 2004). Central to the life course approach is the idea that there are certain

periods of plasticity, where individuals may experience physiological or social change

that alter their future health trajectories. The reproductive period is a sensitive period for

both men and women, in which the timing of births, number of births, and birth outcomes

might have adverse or protective effects on later-life health. It also presents a critical

period for women as physiological changes related to pregnancy may have lifelong

effects on the structure or function systems in the body (Kuh & Ben-Shlomo, 2004; Kuh

& Hardy, 2002). Therefore, the reproductive lives of men and women may provide

significant insight into later-life health and mortality, but current research is lacking

consensus on the direction of the relationship.

This study will examine the role of parity, age at first birth, age at last birth,

interbirth intervals, infant death, multiple births (twins), birth weight of offspring, and

preterm births for both men and women on disease progression after age 65. This study

61

utilizes Centers for Medicare Services (CMS) data spanning from 1992 – 2009 linked to

the Utah Population Database, which is a rich source of longitudinal data. The goals of

this analysis are threefold: 1- identify distinct trajectories of comorbidity from 1992 -

2009 for individuals aged 66 – 84 in 1992 by sex and birth cohort; 2- estimate the

association between measures of fertility and later-life comorbidity trajectories while

controlling for early-life circumstances using information from a longitudinal, familial

health database; 3- determine if the observed effects are part of a trajectory set in motion

earlier during infancy and childhood (i.e., does fertility mediate known relationships

between early-life circumstances and later-life comorbidity trajectories (K. R. Smith,

Hanson, & Zimmer, 2012)).

Background

There is a substantial amount of variation in the morbidity profile of older adults,

suggesting that morbidity is not an inevitable consequence of aging (Rowe & Kahn,

1987; Rowe & Kahn, 1997). Understanding the sources of variation in patterns of aging

is important for creating accurate population predictions, identifying at risk populations

that may benefit from public health interventions, and characterizing the process of aging

in a diverse population. Sources of heterogeneity in patterns of aging cannot be

understood by restricting analyses to a single life stage, nor can its intricacies be

understood without simultaneously considering biological and social mechanisms. The

pathology of chronic disease is multifaceted, determined by genetic profiles, biological

and physiological development, and the social environment, with the strength and relative

importance of each of these factors varying throughout the life course.

62

The majority of studies assessing the relationship between reproductive history

and health have focused on mortality with mixed results. Numerous studies have also

demonstrated the relationship between reproductive history and later-life health as

measured by activities of daily living (ADL), depressive symptoms, type 2 diabetes,

cardiovascular disease, self-rated health, self-reported limiting chronic illness, cancer,

and mental health (D. Smith, Sterne, Tynelius, & Rasmussen, 2004; Grundy &

Tomassini, 2005; Henretta, 2008; Kravdal, 1995; Lawlor et al., 2003; Myklestad et al.,

2012; Spence, 2008; Yi & Vaupel, 2004). Yet none has looked at the relationship

between reproductive history and comorbidity. These studies also often fail to account

for early-life conditions that may influence reproductive history and later-life health.

Comorbidity is one of the major components of health aging, and its presence

increases with age (L. P. Fried, Ferrucci, Darer, Williamson, & Anderson, 2004;

Guralnik, 1996). However, there is variation in the rate at which a transition into a

comorbid state occurs and the trajectory of disease once it has occurred. We assume that

heterogeneity within a population follows a specific distribution, with robust individuals

on one tail and frail individuals on the other. Therefore, unlike the geriatric definition of

frailty which refers to a variable state of physiological decline, we intend to invoke the

demographic meaning of the term. For example, individuals exhibiting a robust

phenotype may delay or evade chronic disease completely, while individuals exhibiting

the frail phenotype experience multiple morbid conditions (Andersen, Sebastiani,

Dworkis, Feldman, & Perls, 2012; Evert, Lawler, Bogan, & Perls, 2003; Ken R. Smith et

al., 2012). Identifying sources of this phenotypic variation is necessary for targeting

periods of the life course that affect later-life health and to more fully understand the

process of aging. Preston, Hill, and Drevenstedt (1998) have proposed a widely used

63

model explaining the direct and indirect effects of childhood circumstances on later-life

health. A similar model can be used to classify the theories that predict a relationship

between fertility history and later-life health. Evolutionary, biological, and social

theories predict that parity, age at first birth, age at last birth, interbirth intervals,

twinning, birth weight of offspring, and giving birth prematurely may all be associated

with later-life morbidity and mortality.

Evolutionary and Genetic Theories Linking

Reproductive Health to Aging

Evolutionary theories predict a close relationship between fertility and mortality.

Optimization hypotheses suppose that the forces of evolution select for traits that

maximize the reproductive success of an organism. Two such hypotheses, disposable

soma and antagonistic pleiotrophy, predict a positive association between parity and

comorbidity at advanced ages (Kirkwood & Rose, 1991). The disposable soma theory

argues that the two physiological costly functions of reproduction and somatic

maintenance are in direct competition for a limited amount of resources. This “trade-off”

yields optimal reproductive success at the cost of longevity for females (Kirkwood &

Rose, 1991). Similarly, antagonistic pleiotrophy suggests that genetic mutations that

increase postreproductive mortality may escape the force of natural selection because

they increase fitness early on (Kirkwood & Rose, 1991; G. C. Williams, 1957). A recent

study of fertility in carriers of the breast cancer genes BRCA1 and BRCA2 suggests that,

although these mutations significantly increase the risk of mortality, they may increase

reproductive fitness and therefore have not been selected out of the population (K. R.

64

Smith, Hanson, Mineau, & Buys, 2012). These theories predict that for females, young

age at first birth and high parity are associated with increased comorbidity later in life.

High parity, late age at last birth, multiple births, and short birth intervals may

also be associated with decreased comorbidity later in life. For example, it has been

suggested that genetic variants influence both late female fertility and slowed rates of

somatic aging (K. R. Smith et al., 2009). Fertility success may also be an indication of

health status and robustness. Women with higher fertility, shorter birth intervals, twins,

and later ages at last birth may have increased longevity because their fertility success is

a marker of a robust phenotype (Hawkes, 2010; Robson & Smith, 2011, 2012). While

evolutionary theories are an important factor in the relationship between fertility and

later-life morbidity and mortality, it is necessary to consider the direct biological and

indirect social effects of fertility history.

Direct Biological Effects of Reproductive Health and

Biological Indicators of Later-Life Health

In the life course literature, physiological scarring has been used to define an

event that permanently alters the physiological functioning of an organism. For women,

pregnancy may trigger physiological changes that may favorably or adversely affect

later-life health. Increased parity and early age at first birth have been shown to lower

the risk of postmenopausal reproductive cancer and it has been posited that biological

factors are responsible for this link. Pregnancy is one of several factors that determine

life time exposure to endogenous hormones. Several hypotheses relate the level of

endogenous hormones throughout the life course, such as androgen, insulin,

progesterone, and estrogen, to cancer risk later in life (Kelsey, Gammon, & John, 1993;

65

Kobayashi et al., 2012; Lukanova & Kaaks, 2005). There is an inverse association

between parity and cancer incidence in tissues sensitive to hormone levels, such as breast,

endometrial and ovarian (Kelsey et al., 1993; Kobayashi et al., 2012; Kvale, Heuch, &

Nilssen, 1994; Permuth-Wey & Sellers, 2009). Age at first birth has also a known risk

factor for breast cancer, with younger age at first birth being a protective factor

(MacMahon, Cole, & Brown, 1973).

Reproductive history may also lead to physiological changes that adversely affect

a woman’s health. Pregnancy related biological responses may lead to increased risk for

coronary heart disease and obesity later in life (Bastian, West, Corcoran, & Munger,

2005; Lawlor et al., 2003). A study of men and women aged 60 to 79 in Britain found a

positive association between number of children and adverse lipid profiles and diabetes

for women but not men, suggesting possible biological mechanisms (Lawlor et al., 2003).

However, life style factors were also found to play a role in the association. Birth

spacing and having multiple births (twins) may also leave a physiological imprint on the

mother. The maternal depletion hypothesis argues that the physiological demands of

pregnancy diminish physical resources and short birth intervals do not give the mother

ample time to recover from the stresses of the previous pregnancy (Jelliffe & Jelliffe,

1978; Kirkwood & Rose, 1991). These theories predict a positive relationship between

parity, short birth intervals, and multiple births and later-life comorbidity.

Characteristics of a mother’s offspring at time of birth can be used to gauge the

woman’s health during her reproductive period and may predict her health status as she

ages. Birth weight of her child and giving birth prematurely are examples of markers of

the health and vitality of the mother (G. D. Smith, Whitley, Gissler, & Hemminki, 2000).

Giving birth to an infant that is considered large for its gestation age (LGA) may be an

66

indication of pregnancy complications that affect the mother’s health, such as gestational

diabetes (Casey, Lucas, McIntire, & Leveno, 1997), which is a known risk factor for

diabetes and cardiovascular disease later in life (Bellamy, Casas, Hingorani, & Williams,

2009; Carr et al., 2006). Studies have shown that there is a positive association between

birth weight of offspring and the mother’s longevity (G. D. Smith et al., 1997; G. D.

Smith et al., 2000). However, the studies do not test whether there is a threshold to this

effect. There are several reasons for these associations. First, it may be an indicator of

the social and physical environment of the mother, with conditions affecting the

development and health of both mother and fetus. Second, it may be indicative of genetic

variants carried by the mother or father that predispose them for cardiovascular disease

later in life (Myklestad et al., 2012). These studies suggest that high birth weight, low

birth weight, and preterm babies are positively associated with adverse health outcomes

later in life.

Social Mechanisms Indirectly Linking Reproductive

History to Later-Life Health

Social theories predict both positive and negative relationships between parity and

later-life morbidity. The social benefits of adult children may negate any adverse

physiological effects of having children by providing social support, social engagement,

and receipt of instrumental help. Strong social support may foster feelings of meaning,

reduce feelings of stress, and minimize risky behavior. Individuals with more social

support and intimate ties have better health and lower levels of mortality (Berkman &

Syme, 1979; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988). Children may provide a support

network later in life, and having more children may increase the chance of having regular

67

contact with at least one child (Uhlenberg & Cooney, 1990) and receipt of help from

children (Grundy & Read, 2012). Later ages at first birth may allow for accumulation of

wealth and resources and prevent adverse health outcomes later in life.

Psychological, social, and economic impacts of children may also lead to a

negative relationship between parity and later-life morbidity. Increasing parity is

associated with obesity and coronary heart disease for both men and women, suggesting

that lifestyle factors associated with high parity may lead to increased risk of morbidity

later in life (Lawlor et al., 2003). Increased parity may not translate to increased social

support. Smith (2002) suggests that high parity children may have fewer resources to

devote to their parents and, due to the intergenerational transmission of fertility, high

parity may lead to decreased social support from children later in life. These results

suggest that the effect of fertility may be positive or negative for males and females.

Early parenthood may lead to decreased opportunities for education and

employment (Ross & Huber, 1985; Waldron, Weiss, & Hughes, 1998), which has been

shown to lead to adverse health consequences later in life (Mirowsky, 2005; Phelan,

Link, & Tehranifar, 2010). Mirowsky (2005) suggests that the optimal period for

childbirth in relation to health is the mid-thirties, but this finding may be unique to the

historical and social environment. The current literature presents conflicting findings

related to the benefits of age at last birth after age 39, with some studies suggesting that

later ages at last birth are protective (K. Smith, G. Mineau, G. Garibotti, & R. Kerber,

2009; K. R. Smith et al., 2002) while others find adverse effects (Mirowsky, 2005).

68

Considering Events Throughout the Life Course that

Affect Fertility and Morbidity

Failure to look at the relationship between fertility and morbidity using a life

course perspective and controlling for early-life circumstances can lead to an

overstatement of their association. Early-life factors may affect the reproductive health

and behaviors of men and women (Doblhammer & Oeppen, 2003; Rich-Edwards, 2002).

Therefore, part of the observed association between fertility and later-life morbidity may

be merely a reflection of genetic makeup or physiological changes during childhood.

For example, adverse childhood and adolescent circumstances are also related to early

motherhood (Geronimus & Korenman, 1992) and later-life health (Galobardes, Lynch, &

Smith, 2008; Preston et al., 1998; K. R. Smith, G. P. Mineau, G. Garibotti, & R. Kerber,

2009). These are important confounders that must be controlled for when studying the

effects of fertility on later-life morbidity. For this reason, we will include measures of

early-life circumstances that may affect fertility and later-life health outcomes.

Hypotheses

The research question addressed in this paper is whether measures of fertility and

reproductive health are associated with morbidity profiles later in life. We will examine

the role of parity, young age at first birth, age at last birth, interbirth intervals, infant

death, multiple births (twins), birth weight of offspring, and preterm births for both men

and women. We are able to control for a range of early-life circumstances, including a

familial predisposition to longevity, childhood socioeconomic status, and death of a

parent during childhood. The specific hypothesis generated by suggested evolutionary,

social, and biological mechanisms summarized in Table 3.1 are as follows:

69

H1: The optimization of life history traits leads to a “trade-off” between

fertility and somatic maintenance. According to this hypothesis, young

age at first birth, increased parity and shorter birth intervals should be

positively associated with comorbidity after age 65 for females, and age at

last birth should be negatively associated with comorbidity after age 65.

H2: Childbirth leads to physiological changes that affect later-life health.

According to this hypothesis, increased parity, shorter birth intervals, later

age at last birth, and unhealthy (high/low) birth weight of offspring should

be adversely associated with later-life comorbidity after age 65 for females

but not males. Early age at first birth and parity may also have protective

effects for females but not males.

H3: Reproductive history is a marker of a robust phenotype. According to

this hypothesis, increased parity, shorter birth intervals, later age at last

birth, and twinning should be negatively associated with comorbidity after

age 65 for both sexes and is possibly stronger for females.

H4: Social mechanisms are responsible for the observed association

between reproductive history and comorbidity after 65. According to this

theory, the observed effects of fertility history should be similar for males

and females, but there are competing hypotheses about the direction of the

effect.

H4a: Increased parity is negatively associated with comorbidity

after age 65 because children provide material, instrumental, and

social support for both men and women.

70

H4b: Increased parity is positively associated with comorbidity

after age 65 because the psychological, social, and economic

effects of having children outweigh the social benefit. Children

may also be unable to provide support because they are caring for

their own large families.

H4c: Early age at first birth is positively associated with

comorbidity after age 65 because it leads to constrained economic

and educational opportunities. Late age at first birth is negatively

associated with comorbidity after age 65 because it leads to

increased educational and economic opportunities.

Methods

Data

The majority of life-span epidemiological studies examine health influences of

early and adult life conditions with relatively modest sample sizes. This study utilizes

data drawn from the Utah Population Database (UPDB). The UPDB is one of the world’s

richest sources of linked population-based information for demographic, genetic, and

epidemiological studies. UPDB has supported biodemographic studies as well numerous

important epidemiological and genetic studies in large part because of its size, pedigree

complexity, and linkages to numerous data sources. The full UPDB now contains data on

nearly 7 million individuals due to longstanding and on-going efforts to add new sources

of data and update records as they become available (e.g., including all statewide death

certificate records (1904-present) and all Medicare claims (1992-2009). We have

identified thousands of members of birth cohorts from the first half of the 20th century,

71

individuals for whom early and midlife conditions are measured and who are linked to

their adult medical records generated decades later. These complex data links provide

unparalleled data quality and depth that focus on families (nuclear, multigenerational, full

pedigrees) and health outcomes that span entire life spans of individuals and their

relatives.

Given the large sample sizes and the quickly changing morbidity risks by age and

sex, we will conduct all analysis by sex and 10 year age categories (66-74 and 75-84).

The first age category begins at age 66 to eliminate the problems of prorating the partial

year coverage of individuals who become age eligible part way into a year when they

turn age 65. Ages are considered in 1992, the first year in which we have Medicare data.

Separating samples by age effectively holds the cohort constant and allows us to analyze

the trends by birth cohort for an 18 year period. Individuals aged 66 – 74 and 75 – 84 in

1992 are considered members of the young-old cohort (born between 1918 and 1926) and

old-old cohort (born between 1908 and 1917), respectively.

We selected once married parous individuals. Once married individuals were

selected to limit complications related to fertility spanning more than one marriage

partner. We excluded individuals with a spouse deceased before the individual reached

age 50 because they would not have completed childbearing (Gagnon et al., 2009). The

CMS data requires an individual to survive to the age of 66, therefore, by definition, the

remaining individuals would have completed their childbearing. Selecting parous

individuals helps identify the multivariate effects of both the intensity and timing of

fertility on comorbidity trajectories. It is also a necessary restriction because the UPDB

is derived from descendent genealogies in which identification of nulliparous women is

not reliable (Moorad, Promislow, Smith, & Wade, 2011). Accordingly, this analysis is

72

not intended to account for the impact of childlessness (Gagnon et al., 2009; K. R. Smith

et al., 2002). Individuals were also required to have sufficient information about parents

in the database, which allowed for the inclusion of early-life circumstances in the model.

Centers for Medicare Services (CMS) provides files that allow us to assess

whether individuals are sufficiently represented in the Medicare claims data so that they

can contribute to the construction of the morbidity trajectories. Our goal is to avoid

characterizing someone as being disease-free when in fact their health events are simply

not well represented in the Medicare data. CMS provides a monthly HMO indicator

variable that describes when a beneficiary was enrolled in a managed care plan. As

expected, few claims exist in the file for individuals during the time they are enrolled in a

managed care plan. For the purposes of this analysis, we exclude persons who have at

least 1 month of enrollment in a managed care plan. We also required all individuals to

have at least 1 full year of data and, therefore, all individuals deceased in 1992 were

excluded from the sample.

Subjects who met our data requirements (e.g., once married parous individuals for

whom we have family data (parental death dates and full fertility history)) and had

sufficient Medicare claims data are shown in Table 3.2. Individuals were then followed

for a maximum of 18 years (to 2009), our last year of Medicare data, or until death. The

total sample size is N=41,158; age specific sample sizes are shown in Table 3.2.

A secondary analysis of individuals aged 66 to 74 in 1992 was done using the

information from birth certificates. Birth certificate information in the UPDB is available

from 1915 to 1921 and 1943 to the present; however, birth weight was not recorded until

1947. Because we are interested in birth weight and prematurity, all individuals used in

this analysis were required to have their first birth in 1947 or later and all births in Utah

73

(giving us complete fertility information). Approximately 40% (n=4,142) of women and

55% (n=6,192) of men in the young-old cohort have birth certificate records for all births.

Individuals aged 66 and 74 in 1992 would have had to have their first birth at age 21 if

they were the youngest members of the cohort and at age 29 if they were the oldest. For

females, the average age at first birth in the birth certificate sample is 2.5 years greater

than those excluded (25.8 vs. 23.3, p<0.01) and for males it is 1.4 years greater than those

excluded (26.9 vs. 25.5, p<0.01). These requirements make this a select cohort, but the

benefits of linking birth outcomes to later-life health trajectories makes this a valuable

analysis.

Key Measures

Comorbidity

We are able to observe morbidity episodes from Medicare claims collected over

time for each individual. Health experience over time is measured by the Charlson

Comorbidity Index (CCI) (Charlson, Pompei, Ales, & MacKenzie, 1987). The CCI was

adapted for use with ICD-9 codes by Deyo et al. (Deyo, Cherkin, & Ciol, 1992) and

Romano et al. (Romano, Roos, & Jollis, 1993). Deyo et al. adapted the index for use with

ICD-9 diagnosis and procedure codes. Romano et al. included some diagnoses that were

not in the original Charlson index. Both modifications were intended for use with the

Medicare Part A records (Klabunde, Potosky, Legler, & Warren, 2000). Klabunde and

colleagues (Klabunde, Warren, & Legler, 2002) created two indices, one for Medicare

Part A records and one for Medicare Part B records. Introducing information from

physician claims data significantly enhanced the index’s predictive value for the risk of

mortality. In the present study, we have adopted this variant of the CCI based on the

74

SEER-Medicare Comorbidity macros

(http://healthservices.cancer.gov/seermedicare/program/comorbidity.html). We classified

individuals into similar trajectory groups with respect to their morbidity patterns

identified by their shared health experiences over time.

The SEER-Medicare macro calculates the CCI with respect to cancer based on the

Deyo adaptation of the index. Given that cancer originally was the index disease, it was

not included as a comorbid condition in this SEER-Medicare program. Accordingly, we

have added cancer as a comorbid disease. We identified specific episodes of the

following 17 major morbidities conditions occurring during the interval 1992-2009 on a

per annum basis that form the basis of the CCI. Items are coded as “1” if they occur at

any time during the year or “0” if they do not, and then weighted based upon their ability

to predict mortality:

1. Myocardial Infarction

2. Congestive Heart Failure

3. Peripheral Vascular Disease

4. Cerebrovascular disease

5. Dementia

6. Chronic pulmonary disease

7. Rheumatologic disease

8. Peptic Ulcer Disease

9. Mild Liver Disease

10. Diabetes (mild to moderate)

11. Diabetes with chronic complications

12. Hemiplegia or paraplegia

75

13. Renal (kidney) disease

14. Any malignancy

15. Moderate or severe liver disease

16. Metastatic Solid Tumor

17. AIDS

The independent variables used in the analysis can be partitioned into three domains:

demographic, early-life conditions (ELCs), and fertility.

Demographic Characteristics

All models controlled for age in 1992 centered on the mean for each sex and age

group. Widowhood is a frequent occurrence among individuals in this age range and

may be linked to changes in health status (K. Williams & Umberson, 2004). Time-

varying covariates are used to allow for altered shape of the trajectories due to loss of a

spouse. An indicator variable for each year was created for each year of observation and

defined equal to “0” during all periods where the spouse is still alive and “1” during all

periods where the spouse was deceased.

Measures of Early-life Conditions

Measures of age at parental death, childhood socioeconomic status, familial

excess longevity (FEL), and religious participation are generated from the data within the

UPDB. Death of a parent during childhood may have adverse effects on health later in

life (Andersson, Hogberg, & Åkerman, 1996; Norton et al., 2011; Umberson & Chen,

1994), and disruption of the family may affect the transition into adulthood, including

timing of childbirth. Birth, marriage and death dates are recorded comprehensively in the

76

UPDB and were used to construct eight categories of parental death. The gender of the

deceased parent may have different social and economic implications. Therefore, two

categories were created (one mother and one father) for each of the following

circumstances related to parental death: mother/father died when child was under age 18,

parent deceased after child was age 18 (reference category), and both parents deceased

when child was under 18 (orphan).

Childhood socioeconomic status may directly and indirectly influence marriage

and reproductive success, timing of childbirth, and later-life comorbidity (Doblhammer &

Oeppen, 2003; Geronimus & Korenman, 1992; D. Kuh & Ben-Shlomo, 2004).

Childhood socioeconomic status is measured using usual occupation and industry

information reported on a father’s death certificate for fathers who died in Utah and for

whom we have a death certificate (deaths occurring from 1904 forward). Occupational

strings were converted to Nam-Powers socioeconomic (NP SES) scores, a measure of

income and education based on occupational categories and range from 1 to 99, with

higher scores being associated with higher socioeconomic status (Nam & Powers, 1983).

NP SES scores were unavailable for approximately 20% of the sample. Values for these

individuals were imputed by substituting the mean plus a random number multiplied by

the distribution of nonmissing values and an additional variable indicating missing

values. A large percentage of fathers from this era, a little over 30%, have the occupation

“farmer,” resulting in a large heaping at the NP SES score of 40. Farming may also

confer a survival advantage related to life style factors (Gavrilov & Gavrilova, 2012), and

a separate category was created for the occupation of farmer.

To control for unobservable genetic and shared environmental effects, we used a

measure of family history of longevity, Familial Excess Longevity (FEL). FEL is a

77

statistic developed using deep genealogical data of multigenerational pedigrees drawn

from the UPDB. We have published the development of this statistic (Kerber, O'Brien,

Smith, & Cawthon, 2001) and have applied it to other life-span studies using UPDB

(Garibotti, Smith, Kerber, & Boucher, 2006; Kerber, O'Brien, Boucher, Smith, &

Cawthon, 2012; K. Smith et al., 2009). At its foundation, the FEL is based on the

assumption that family history of longevity follows Mendelian patterns of inheritance. To

construct familial excess longevity, we first measure individual level excess longevity,

defined as the difference between an individual’s attained age and the age to which that

individual was expected to live according to a model that incorporates basic life-span

predictors (sex, birth year). Expected longevity is estimated from an accelerated failure

time (AFT) model, and excess longevity is simply the difference between expected and

attained age. Expected longevity is based on the lognormal distribution and the AFT

model was used because it provides a simple point estimate for duration that fits the

observed data. Excess longevity is then extended to blood relatives who reached the age

of 65 for each individual, a restriction that focuses on years less affected by external

causes of death. The kinship coefficient, the probability that an individual shares a

particular allele with another individual, is used as a weight in calculating familial excess

longevity. Averaging the excess longevities of all blood kin over 65 for each ego, with

the appropriate weighting scheme, generates a point estimate of familial excess longevity.

We have found that individuals with high FEL live longer and experience more healthful

disease trajectories as they age (K. R. Smith et al., 2012; K. R. Smith et al., 2009).

Active affiliation with The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (LDS or

Mormon) church is associated with increased life expectancy (Enstrom & Breslow, 2008)

and high fertility rates (Arland, 1979). Individuals actively affiliated with the LDS

78

church are more likely to abstain from alcohol and tobacco use, fast once a month, and

participate in church related social activities (Mineau, Smith, & Bean, 2002). The UPDB

contains information on baptism and endowment dates from family history records.

These were used to classify individuals as active followers, inactive, or nonmembers.

Individuals with an endowment date have agreed to live their lives following the doctrine

of the Church and are considered active Church followers if endowed before age 40.

Individuals with a baptism but no endowment date are considered inactive, and

individuals with no baptism or endowment date are considered nonmembers (reference

category).

Measures of Fertility

Fertility information in UPDB comes from a combination of information collected

from Family Group Sheets obtained from the Utah Family History Library and linked

vital records, including birth certificate data from 1915 – 1921 and 1943 to the present.

All women in the sample have completed fertility by definition because they are required

to survive to at least age 65 to be visible in the Medicare Claims data.

Parity was measured with a set of dummy variables to indicate whether a woman

had 1-2, 3-5, 6-8, or 9+ children. On average, women in this sample had 4 children, and

the category for 3-5 children was used as the reference category. To measure the effects

of early and late childbirth, we created dummy variables for the following categories: age

at first birth before the age of 18, between ages 18 and 24, and after age 25, with 18-24

used as the reference category. There were very few men under the age of 18, and

therefore this category was combined with the 18-24 category for men. For age at last

birth we constructed three categories: under age 35 (reference group), 35 – 39, and 40 or

79

older. A dummy variable is used to identify parents of multiples (twins). Short birth

intervals are defined as interbirth intervals less than 18 months, and long birth intervals

are defined as interbirth intervals > 60 months (Conde-Agudelo, Rosas-Bermudez, &

Kafury-Goeta, 2007). Separate variables were created to identify individuals with one or

more short or long interbirth interval.

Infant mortality may be a marker for maternal health and adverse environments

(McCormick, Shapiro, & Starfield, 1984). Environments that lead to adverse health

outcomes for the infant may also be risky for the parents. Individuals losing one or more

children during the first year of life will also be identified with a dummy variable.

Birth certificate information in the UPDB is available from 1915 to 1921 and

1943 to the present. Birth certificates contain information on a mother’s marital status,

prematurity, and birth weight (starting in 1947). Using the information from the birth

certificates, individuals are categoriezed as ever having a high birth weight baby (> 4,000

grams) or low birth weight baby (<2,500 and carried 37+ weeks), which reflects the WIC

Nutrition Risk Criteria (Medicine, 1996). Preterm birth was defined as the birth of an

infant before 37 weeks of gestation. Table 3.3 presents the descriptive statistics of all the

measures by sex and age group.

Constructing Morbidity Trajectories

We seek to determine how reproductive history and health affect the likelihood of

having a particular later-life comorbidity trajectory. Assessment of comorbidity

trajectories is accomplished through the application of a finite mixture modeling

approach that is currently available as a SAS procedure called PROC TRAJ through the

work of Dr. Daniel Nagin and his colleagues (Haviland, Nagin, Rosenbaum, & Tremblay,

80

2008; Jones & Nagin, 2007; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001). The group-based modeling

approach allows for identification of distinct clusters of individual trajectories.

Given the quickly changing health landscape of an aging population, all models

are estimated within age-sex categories. The excess mortality risks of men and the

generally higher rates of morbidity of women necessitate that we use sex-specific models.

As age profoundly affects the risks of morbidity as encompassed in the CCI, as noted

above, we divide the sample into two birth cohorts determined by their age at baseline.

Because the response variable in this analysis is a weighted count of the number of

comorbid conditions, a zero-inflated Poisson (ZIP) based model was used. The ZIP

model is an expansion of the Poisson model that corrects for overdispersion by

accounting for more zeros than would be expected under a Poisson process. Both the

Poisson and censored normal distributions were also considered, but the ZIP model

provided the best fit for our data.

Trajectories were modeled for two to six groups as a quadratic function of time.

Model fit was assessed using the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), the log likelihood

plus a penalty for the number of parameters in the model. There are situations where BIC

score continues to increase as more groups are added, but the additional groups are not

necessary to summarize the distinct features of the data in a parsimonious way (Nagin,

2005). Therefore, average posterior probability of assignment, odds of correct

classification, and estimated group probabilities versus the proportion of the sample

assigned to the group (Nagin, 2005) were also used to assess the selected model’s

correspondence with the data.

Nonrandom attrition leads to altered characteristics of the population over time

and can lead to biased estimates. Because we excluded individuals who were enrolled in

81

a managed care plan during any period of a given year, our only source of truncation is

death. PROC TRAJ is used to simultaneously model the comorbidity trajectories and the

probability of death, allowing the modeled probability of death to vary across trajectory

groups. Individuals in the analysis were required to be alive at time “1,” and therefore

the probability of dropout during this period is zero for all trajectory groups. All models

accounted for nonrandom attrition due to death using the extension created by Haviland

(Haviland, Jones, & Nagin, 2011; Zimmer, Martin, Nagin, & Jones, 2012). This

extension jointly models the trajectories with a model of the logit of the dropout

probability, in this case death, by group that includes dependence on the prior period

response until dropout and age.

Trajectory group membership probabilities can vary as a function of time stable

characteristics, or characteristics established before the observation periods (Jones &

Nagin, 2007). This third component jointly estimates a multinomial logit model that

captures the effects of time stable characteristics on the probability of group membership.

This makes it possible to test the effect of early-life conditions and fertility on the

probability of membership in each group (Daniel S. Nagin & Odgers, 2010). PROC

TRAJ also allows for the inclusion of time-varying covariates measured during the

observation time that may alter the shape of the observed trajectories, such as

widowhood.

A series of mediation analysis were conducted to test the hypothesis that fertility

history mediated the relationship between early-life conditions and later-life health.

Mediation tests used the Clogg test of differences in coefficients produced when fertility

variables were added to the model (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets,

2002).

82

For each group, the following analyses were conducted. First, we derived the

basic trajectory groups in which comorbidity is a function of time only in order to select

the best model. Second, we fit a model with covariates from the demographic and early

life conditions (ELCs). Third, we fit a model with the covariates from the demographic,

ELCs, and fertility domains. All models accounted for nonrandom attrition due to death.

In addition, we examined how ELCs in the probability of trajectory membership are

mediated by timing of childbirth (age at first and last birth), preterm birth, and parity.

Addressing Sample Selection

The data requirements for selection into the sample, once married parous

individuals who survived to age 66, had a spouse survive until they were age 50, and

never enrolled in a managed care plan during the observation period, could lead to biased

estimates because the data are not representative of the population. To correct for the

potential problem of selection, we use a Heckman two-stage modeling strategy

(Heckman, 1979) performed using Stata 11.

In the first stage, a probit model assesses factors leading to selection into the

sample among all individuals (see Table 3.2). The dependent variable is a dichotomous

indicator of selection into the sample. This equation is used to generate the inverse Mills

ratio (IMR), which is a nonlinear transformation of the probit index and a decreasing

function of the probability of selection (Fu, Winship, & Mare, 2004). The IMR can be

interpreted as the hazard of not being selected into the sample on which the comorbidity

trajectories are based. The independent variables used in the selection equation are

displayed in Table 3.4. For the model to be correctly specified, the selection equation

83

must include variables that are more closely related to sample selection than the

dependent variable in the substantive equation.

Our sample selection is largely based on having complete information in UPDB

and nonenrollment in a managed care organization (MCO). Therefore, we selected

independent variables for the selection equation that are closely related to these factors.

Age in 1992 is derived from the CMS records. The longitudinal information within the

UPDB is more complete for individuals with a longer length of residence in the state of

Utah. The UPDB holds information on the birth place of individuals, and this was used

to create an indicator variable for place of birth, Utah versus outside of Utah.

Approximately 85% of the individuals selected into the sample were born in Utah,

compared to 40% of those not selected into the sample. Area level characteristics of an

individual’s current place of residence may also predict selection into the sample.

Information about an individual’s county of residence in 1992 was pulled from the 1990

US Decennial Census.

Table 3.4 shows that there are large differences in county level population and

median family income between those selected and not selected into the final sample. Of

those selected into the final sample, 61% resided in the Wasatch Front region of Utah,

compared to 48% of the nonselected individuals. This contributes to the large difference

in county level population between the groups because a larger proportion of individuals

in the nonselected group resides in populous counties in California (such as LA county;

population of 8.8 million in 1992). In the second stage, trajectory models are estimated

with the IMR from the first stage added as a covariate in the model, the goal being to

account for possible sample selection bias in the final models. The far right column in

Table 3.4 shows all of the variables included in the probit models significantly predicted

84

selection into the sample, with the exception of age in the equation for males aged 75 –

84. Age and being born in Utah were positively associated with selection into the

sample, while median family income and population were negatively associated with

selection into the sample for all genders and cohorts (with the age exception mentioned

above). A similar sample selection strategy was used by Gagnon et al. (2009) when

examining the relationship between fertility and postreproductive survival.

Results

Trajectories of Comorbidity and Morbidity

The best fitting models for both males and females ages 66 – 74 (the young-old)

in 1992 revealed six distinct groups of trajectory groups, while those for ages 75 – 84 (the

old-old) showed five distinct groups. Figures 3.1- 3.4 show the predicted comorbidity

trajectories by sex and age group. The figures show the diversity of comorbidity

experience over the 18 year period of follow-up (the youngest individuals are 84 years

old at the end of the follow-up period). To aid in the interpretation of results, trajectory

groups have been labeled as follows: “robust”- characterized by the absence of comorbid

conditions; “slow initiates”- individuals in this group begin the observation period with

no comorbid conditions, but the number gradually increases over time; “accelerated

initiates”- individuals in this group begin the observation period with no comorbid

conditions but the number of conditions quickly increases over time and then decelerates

during the last 2 years of the 18 year period; “chronic low”- characterized by the steady

level of comorbidity over time; “ailing”- this group of individuals has moderate levels of

comorbidity at baseline which steadily increase over time; “frail”- these individuals have

the highest level of comorbidity at baseline which remains high over time.

85

In addition to using BIC as model selection criteria, several other measures were

used to assess the correspondence of our models with the data (D. Nagin, 2005; Zimmer

et al., 2012). First, we calculated the average posterior probability (APP) of membership

in group j for all individuals who are most likely to belong to that group. The

recommended criterion is that the APP for each group should exceed 0.70. All selected

models met this criterion, with APP ranging from 0.79 to 0.92. Second, we compared

estimated proportions of group membership generated by the maximum likelihood

procedure to the actual proportion of the sample assigned to each group based on

maximum posterior probability of group membership. For this criterion, our models were

satisfactory, with no more than a 4 point difference in any of the selected models.

The shape of the trajectories is similar between males and females in their

respective age groups, but the intercepts differ. Individuals surviving the full 18 year

period range in age from 83 to 91 in 2009. For females in the young-old age category,

trajectory membership is fairly evenly distributed among the robust (19.1%), slow initiate

(18.8%), chronic (19.7%), and ailing (21.8%). Compared to females, males have lower

percentages of individuals in the robust (15.7%) and slow initiate (16.4%) groups, and

higher percentages of individuals in the ailing (26.1%), accelerated initiate (15.3%), and

frail groups (8%). The frail category constitutes the lowest proportion of group

membership for both males and females, 8% and 7.3%, respectively. These findings are

somewhat unexpected given the health-survival paradox, where females have worse

health and males have higher mortality. However, recently reported prevalence estimates

support our findings. The 2011 summary statistics for US adults reports higher

prevalence rates of heart disease, hypertension, and diabetes for men (Schiller, Lucas, &

Peregoy, 2012). A separate report using National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) from

86

2009 estimated that a higher percentage of men (49%) than women (42.5%) had two or

more chronic conditions (conditions considered included hypertension, heart disease,

diabetes, cancer, stroke, chronic bronchitis, emphysema, current asthma, and kidney

disease; V. Fried, Bernstein, & Bush, 2012).

Individuals in the old-old cohort surviving the full 18 year period range in age

from 92 to 101 in 2009. For both sexes in this cohort, five distinct trajectory groups were

identified. The ailing category, with a CCI of approximately “1” at baseline that

gradually increases over time, has the highest trajectory membership for both sexes, with

29.7% of females and 35.3% of males falling into this category. Compared to the young-

old, a smaller proportion of the old-old fall into the robust category (15.7% vs. 14.7% for

males and 19.1% vs. 18.2% for females). As expected, the robust in the old-old cohort

do not maintain a disease free trajectory over the period of 18 years, with a predicted CCI

of 0.74 for females and 1.5 for males in 2009. While the pattern of this robust trajectory

in the old-old cohort is similar to the pattern of the slow initiates in the young-old cohort,

individuals in the old-old robust category have a slower rate of increase over time and

end the period with a lower predicted CCI (the difference in CCI2009 is 0.82 for females

and 0.44 for males). Compared to the young-old, there is a near doubling in the

proportion of frail females and a 50% increase for the males. Another notable difference

between the young-old and old-old frail trajectories is the maximum predicted CCI,

which is higher in the young-old category for both sexes. While the two cohorts are not

directly comparable, the results suggest that there may be a decrease in the heterogeneity

of morbidity patterns with age, with fewer categories in the older age groups. The lower

number of categories that fit the data may also be a function of the decreased sample size

in the old-old cohort.

87

We found six distinct trajectory groups for the male and female birth certificate

samples. The parameter estimates and estimated trajectory memberships were similar

across samples (results not shown) with a slightly higher percentage in the robust group

for both males and females compared to the full sample. This is not unexpected given the

younger age distribution of these subsamples. The following trajectories were identified:

robust (male=16.7%, female=20.3%); slow initiate (male=16.9%, female=19.2%);

accelerated initiate (male=18.0%, female=14.3%); chronic low (male=16.1%,

female=18.0%); ailing (male=25.2%, female=21.3%); and frail (male=6.9%,

female=6.9%).

Figures 3.5 – 3.8 display the probability of death for each sex and age-group. The

probability of dropout due to death is modeled as a function of age and the comorbidity

measurement in the previous year and is allowed to vary by trajectory group. Mortality

trajectories follow a similar hierarchy as the comorbidity trajectories, with the robust

group generally having the lowest levels of mortality and the frail group having the

highest. Mortality in all groups rises with time, with the accelerated initiates having the

fastest rate of increase in mortality over the 18 year period for the young-old cohort, and

the ailing and initiates having the fastest rates of increase for females and males

respectively in the old-old cohort. Females have lower probabilities of death than males

in their respective cohorts and comorbidity groups. The young-old have lower

probabilities of death than the old-old. Both of these patterns are consistent with

expected patterns of mortality in these age groups.

Widowhood altered the shape of the trajectory for some, but not all, trajectory

groups. In general, experiencing the death of a spouse led to an increase in the level of

comorbidity. Individuals in the frail categories and males in the old-old cohort were the

88

most impervious to the effects of widowhood, with few of the effects reaching

significance. Because these results are central to the hypothesis, they are not shown here

but are available upon request.

Fertility History and Later-Life Comorbidity

Once the best fitting trajectory models were selected, we jointly modeled

multinomial logit models by sex and cohort, relating individual-level covariates to

posterior probabilities to estimate the effects of ELCs and fertility on probability of group

membership. The first set of nested models included only ELCs and the results are not

displayed in this paper, but they are available upon request. Tables 3.5 – 3.8 display the

odds ratios and 95% confident intervals for the full models that include demographic,

ELCs, and fertility measures. Comorbidity is the existence of multiple diseases. Our

results show that there are two groups in all models that escape transition into a comorbid

state (two or more simultaneous conditions), the robust and chronic low. However, for

ease of interpretation, the chronic low group will be referred to as a group with comorbid

conditions. The contrast group in all tables is the robust category, meaning that we are

comparing the probability of membership in trajectory groups with comorbid conditions

with the probability of membership in the group with no comorbid conditions. All results

discussed below are controlling for early-life events and demographic measures and,

therefore, all results presented below are ceteris paribus. Also, unless otherwise noted,

the results highlighted below are significant at the 0.05 level.

For females in the young-old cohort, we do not find a significant association

between parity and trajectory membership. We do find a relationship between age at first

birth and trajectory group. Table 3.5 shows that, compared to women having their first

89

birth between the ages of 19 and 24, young age at first birth (<18) nearly doubles the

odds of being in the frail versus robust trajectory. The results also suggest that young age

at first birth increases the odds of being in the other categories with increased

comorbidity, but the differences are merely suggestive. Age at last birth confers a

protective effect, with women having a last birth at age 35 or later having a decrease in

the odds of being in a comorbid trajectory. Females who have their last birth between the

ages of 35 and 39 and after age 40 have a 24% and 25% (p=0.07) respective decrease in

the odds of being in the frail versus robust group compared to females ending

childbearing earlier. There is a 33% decrease in the odds of being in the accelerated

initiate group versus the robust trajectory for women having one or more infant deaths,

but this pattern is not evident for other categories. There is no evidence of an association

between twinning, short birth intervals, long birth intervals, and later-life comorbidity

trajectories for females in the young-old cohort.

Table 3.6 shows the results for females in the old-old cohort. We find little

association between parity and trajectory membership ceteris paribus for females in the

old-old cohort with the exception of the frail group, where females having nine or more

children are nearly twice as likely to be in the frail versus robust group. The relationship

between age at first birth and trajectory membership are similar to the patterns observed

in the young-old cohort. Females having their first birth during the teenage years are

more likely to be in the chronic low, ailing, and frail versus robust groups. Females in

this cohort having their first birth after age 25 are less likely to be in the chronic low and

frail categories. Compared to females having their last birth before the age of 35, females

having their last child after the age of 35 are more likely to be in the chronic low group

versus the robust group. However, females having their last birth after the age of 40 have

90

a 24% decrease in the odds of being in the frail versus robust group. As with the female

young-old cohort, we find no association between trajectory membership and twinning,

infant death, and short birth intervals ceteris paribus. Having one or more long birth

interval reduces the odds of being in any of the groups with more comorbid conditions

versus the robust, with an 24%, 34%, 18%, and 27% respective decrease in the risk of

being in the initiate, chronic low, ailing, and frail versus robust group (the difference in

the magnitude of the effect across groups is not significant).

We find no association between parity and trajectory membership for males in the

young-old and old-old cohorts ceteris paribus. Having a later age at first birth (over the

age of 25 vs. less than 25) is protective for men in both cohorts. Table 3.7 shows that for

males in the young-old cohort, having their first birth at the age of 25 or older reduces the

odds of being in the accelerated initiates, chronic low, ailing, and frail groups by a little

over 20% for each group compared to the robust. Table 3.8 shows that for males in the

old-old cohort, having an older age at first birth is associated with an 18% and 29%

reduction in the risk of being in the ailing and frail groups, respectively, compared to the

robust group. Males in the old-old cohort having their last child after the age of 40 have

a 26% decrease in the odds of being in the ailing versus robust group. We do not find an

association between twinning, age at last birth, short birth intervals, long birth intervals,

and infant deaths and group membership for males in these cohorts.

The results from of the birth certificate analysis are presented in Figures 3.9 and

3.10. Individuals included in these models were required to have birth certificate records

for all births and, therefore, the results are based on a subsample of individuals in the

young-old cohorts, with a higher percentage of the males from the full sample

represented in the subsample than females (NFemale=4,124 and NMale= 6,192). This is

91

because, on average, men have an older age at first birth and are therefore more likely to

meet the selection criteria. All models controlled for early-life conditions and the fertility

covariates presented used in the earlier models. As with the previous analyses, the

multinomial logit model relates individual-level covariates to posterior probabilities of

trajectory group membership. Models were run simultaneously with the trajectories, and

the reference group is the robust, or group with the lowest number of comorbidities. All

models control for early-life conditions and demographic variables and were jointly

modeled with mortality trajectories.

We find that for females in the young-old cohort, having one or more high birth

weight (defined as >4,000 g) children increases the odds of being in the chronic low,

ailing, and frail versus the robust ceteris paribus by 39%, 60%, and 76%, respectively.

Females having one or more preterm births (defined as <37 weeks gestation) have a 54%

increase in the odds of being in the frail group versus the robust group. We do not find

an association between ever having a low birth weight (carried to term) baby and later-

life comorbidity trajectories for females. For males in the birth certificate analysis, we

find no association between premature offspring and group membership. We do find that

males having one or more high birth weight children have a 30% increase in the odds of

being in the accelerated initiate versus the robust group. Fathers of low birth weight

babies are also more likely to be in the chronic low and ailing groups compared to the

robust group.

To account for sample selection bias, the inverse Mills’ ratio (IMR) estimated

from the probit model predicting selection into the sample was included in the analyses.

Overall, the sample bias correction term (the IMR) has little effect on probability of

group membership. We do find that an increased hazard of nonselection is associated

92

with an increase in likelihood of being in the frail group for females in the young-old

cohort (OR=1.3, 95% CI= 1.02, 1.67) and the ailing group for males in the old-old cohort

(OR=1.38, 95% CI=1.07, 1.78). Simply, individuals not predicted to be in the sample

had a 30% and 38% increase in the odds of being in the frail and ailing groups for the

young-old females and old-old males, respectively. The beauty of the IMR term is that it

simultaneously tests for and corrects selection bias. Therefore, the selection bias present

in models with significant terms has been corrected for by including the IMR in our

models. However, this should be interpreted with some caution because selection models

are sensitive to the choice of covariates in the selection equation (Fu et al., 2004).

The Mediating Effects of Fertility

Mediation analyses were performed to test the mediating effects of fertility on

early-life conditions. Fertility variables were considered as possible intervening variables

if they were significantly related to comorbidity group membership. While there were

few significant differences in coefficients, the percent change in the effects of the ELCs

was small (ranging from 0.5% to 8%) and inconsistent. Therefore, we concluded that

fertility history did not significantly mediate the relationship between early-life

conditions and later-life health (results not shown here but available upon request).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was threefold. First, we sought to identify distinct

trajectories of comorbidity by sex for individuals in two age categories, the young-old

and old-old. Second, we tested specific hypotheses relating fertility to trajectory group

93

membership. Third, we tested the mediating role of fertility on the relationship between

early-life conditions and comorbidity. We found that there are distinct heterogeneous

patterns of comorbidity that range from a robust group, escaping major morbid conditions

for the majority of the observation period, to a frail group characterized by high

comorbidity throughout the entire period of observation. Fertility history is associated

with comorbidity trajectories after the age of 65 for both females and males when

controlling for early-life circumstances, although it is clear that the fertility history has a

greater impact on females. These results provide some evidence that evolutionary “trade-

off” (H1), biological (H2), and social mechanisms (H4b, H4c) may all be associated with

the observed relationship between fertility and later-life health. While we found

independent effects of early-life conditions on later-life comorbidity trajectories, we did

not find robust evidence that fertility history is on the causal pathway between early-life

conditions and later-life comorbidity.

The observed relationships between parity, age at last birth and comorbidity group

membership present evidence of a “trade-off” between fertility and aging for females in

the young-old cohort. Our finding of adverse effects at 9+ births is higher than the 5+

births reported in other studies of contemporary populations (Doblhammer, 2000; Grundy

& Tomassini, 2005). However, both studies top coded fertility at 5+ births. Our findings

support other studies suggesting that high levels of fertility are needed for a trade-off

mechanism to operate (Gagnon et al., 2009; Kitagawa & Hauser, 1973). We also find a

consistent protective relationship between age at last birth and comorbidity group

membership in the young-old cohort, with females having their last birth after age 35

more likely to be in the robust group. This is consistent with the prediction that older

ages of reproduction are a marker for slowed rates of aging (Perls, Alpert, & Fretts,

94

1997). However, we do not see the same strong protective effect for females in the old-

old cohort.

Related to the evolutionary theories discussed above are the biological

mechanisms through which fertility is linked to later-life comorbidity for women. We

did find some support for biological consequences to childbirth for women (H2), but we

did not find evidence supporting the maternal depletion hypothesis or the link between

low birth weight and comorbidity for women (Davey Smith et al., 2004; G. D. Smith et

al., 1997). High parity (9+ births) had an adverse effect on later-life health for females

but not males, suggesting the costs of increased parity are biological. Having at least one

long birth interval had a protective effect on comorbidity later in life for females in the

old-old cohort. Long birth intervals have been linked to complications during

reproductive years (Conde-Agudelo et al., 2007). However, this study suggests that there

are not long term consequences to widely spaced births for women in the old-old cohort.

We do not find strong evidence in favor of the robust phenotype hypothesis (H3),

which argues that fertility success is a marker for female health and vitality. We find no

association between short birth intervals and later-life comorbidity. While the negative

relationship between late age at last birth and comorbidity may be a marker for

robustness, the hypothesized negative relationship between increased parity, shorter birth

intervals, and twinning were not significant across trajectory groups and cohorts. Other

studies using this data from the UPDB have found evidence supporting the robust

phenotype hypothesis (Robson & Smith, 2011, 2012). However, that study uses a

historical cohort of women that survive to age 50. It is possible that twinning served as a

selection filter, with only the most robust women giving birth to multiples during that

historical time period surviving to age 50. The same effect is not observed in a

95

contemporary sample with women giving birth during a period where medical

intervention could increase the rate of survival for mothers of multiples. Therefore, we

are rejecting this hypothesis.

We do find evidence that social mechanisms explain some of the relationship

between fertility and later-life health. We do not find strong evidence of decreased

comorbidity for individuals with more children, and therefore reject the social support

hypothesis (H4a). We find strong evidence supporting the association between age at

first birth and comorbidity after age 65. Young motherhood is related to adverse health

outcomes for both cohorts in this study, and postponing parenthood is protective for

males and females when controlling for early-life circumstances including childhood

socioeconomic status. This suggests that policy aimed at reducing teenage pregnancy

may have significant effects on later-life health outcomes. The adverse effect of low

birth weight for males but not females and high birth weight of offspring for males and

females suggests that adverse birth outcomes may be a marker of risky environments

(Kramer, Séguin, Lydon, & Goulet, 2000). We did not control for social environment at

time of birth or current socioeconomic circumstance. There are large socioeconomic

disparities in perinatal and infant mortality, low birth weight, and preterm birth and, in

the United States, these disparities are often related to racial/ethnic disparities (Kramer,

1987; Kramer et al., 2000). Future research should not only consider the early-life social

environment, but the social environment throughout the life course.

Fertility decisions and outcomes are heavily influenced by social and historical

circumstances, making it important to consider the historical context of these individuals’

lives. The oldest members of the old-old cohort would have been born in 1908 and

entered childbearing age (assuming it is 15) in 1923, with the childbearing years

96

extending to 1965 for the youngest members of the cohort (assuming age at last birth is

50). The members of the young-old cohort would have initiated childbearing in 1933 and

ended in 1974. Infertility drugs would have been available for some individuals in these

cohorts. This means that parity and late age at last birth may not be completely

biologically determined. Individuals in both cohorts may have served during WWII,

which may affect the timing of fertility. Individuals in both cohorts would have been

parous during the baby boom, when fertility rates peaked at 3.8 (Westoff, 1986).

Members of the old-old cohort would also have been children during the 1918 influenza

pandemic, an exposure that may have left them physiologically scarred, affecting both

fertility and mortality. A study by Smith et al. suggests that individuals exposed to

influenza or pneumonia as children during the pandemic have lower ages at last birth and

increased mortality (Ken R. Smith, Reed, Hanson, Mineau, & Fraser, 2012).

This study has several limitations that should be addressed in future studies of the

relationship between fertility and later-life health. First, these results are based on once

married parous individuals. Future studies should consider the relationship between

nulliparity and later-life comorbidity for both men and women. Second, we were unable

to consider the role of all early-life and reproductive outcomes (including the number of

sibling, number of sons and daughters, the proportion of offspring born prematurely, and

the proportion of offspring born high/low birth weight) and later-life health. Third, while

we controlled for childhood SES, we were unable to control for SES at the time of birth

and baseline. We are currently in the process of creating files to begin these analyses.

Fourth, sibling and spouse designs may improve understanding of the mechanisms

linking reproductive history and health.

97

Conclusion

The paths to aging are heterogeneous and more research needs to be done to both

characterize these different phenotypes and the factors that influence them. Approaching

this problem using the life course lens allows us to explore events that can directly or

indirectly affect later-life health. Understanding heterogeneity in the patterns of aging

using a single period is an impossible endeavor because the biological functioning of an

individual is dependent upon an array of circumstances from birth to death. While early-

life conditions explain a portion of the heterogeneity in aging, midlife circumstances may

also alter the trajectories of disease. Parity, timing of childbearing, and birth outcomes of

offspring are significantly related to later-life health outcomes. The differences in risk

factors between men and women suggest that evolutionary, biological, and social

mechanisms must all be considered when studying these heterogeneous aging processes.

98

References

Andersen, S. L., Sebastiani, P., Dworkis, D. A., Feldman, L., & Perls, T. T. (2012).

Health span approximates life span among many supercentenarians: Compression

of morbidity at the approximate limit of life span. The Journals of Gerontology

Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67A(4), 395-405. doi:

10.1093/gerona/glr223

Andersson, T., Hogberg, U., & Åkerman, S. (1996). Survival of orphans in 19th century

Sweden—the importance of remarriages. Acta Pædiatrica, 85(8), 981-985. doi:

10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14198.x

Arland, T. (1979). Religion and fertility: The case of Mormonism. Journal of Marriage

and Family, 41(1), 131-142. doi: 10.2307/351738

Bastian, L. A., West, N. A., Corcoran, C., & Munger, R. G. (2005). Number of children

and the risk of obesity in older women. Preventive Medicine, 40(1), 99-104. doi:

10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.05.007

Bellamy, L., Casas, J.-P., Hingorani, A. D., & Williams, D. (2009). Type 2 diabetes

mellitus after gestational diabetes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The

Lancet, 373(9677), 1773-1779. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-

6736(09)60731-5

Berkman, L. F., & Syme, S. L. (1979). Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: A

nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. American Journal of

Epidemiology, 109(2), 186-204.

Carr, D. B., Utzschneider, K. M., Hull, R. L., Tong, J., Wallace, T. M., Kodama, K.,

Shofer, J. et. al. (2006). Gestational diabetes mellitus increases the risk of

cardiovascular disease in women with a family history of type 2 diabetes.

Diabetes Care, 29(9), 2078-2083. doi: 10.2337/dc05-2482

Casey, B. M., Lucas, M. J., McIntire, D. D., & Leveno, K. J. (1997). Pregnancy outcomes

in women with gestational diabetes compared with the general obstetric

population. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 90(6), 869-873. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0029-7844(97)00542-5

Charlson, M. E., Pompei, P., Ales, K. L., & MacKenzie, C. R. (1987). A new method of

classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: Development and

validation. J Chronic Dis, 40(5), 373-383.

Conde-Agudelo, A., Rosas-Bermudez, A., & Kafury-Goeta, A. C. (2007). Effects of birth

spacing on maternal health: A systematic review. Am J Obstet Gynecol, 196(4),

297-308. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2006.05.055

99

Deyo, R. A., Cherkin, D. C., & Ciol, M. A. (1992). Adapting a clinical comorbidity index

for use with ICD-9-CM administrative databases. J Clin Epidemiol, 45(6), 613-

619.

Doblhammer, G. (2000). Reproductive history and mortality later in life: A comparative

study of England and Wales and Austria. Population Studies, 54(2), 169-176. doi:

10.1080/713779087

Doblhammer, G., & Oeppen, J. (2003). Reproduction and longevity among the British

peerage: The effect of frailty and health selection. Proceedings of the Royal

Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 270(1524), 1541-1547. doi:

10.1098/rspb.2003.2400

Enstrom, J. E., & Breslow, L. (2008). Lifestyle and reduced mortality among active

California Mormons, 1980–2004. Preventive Medicine, 46(2), 133-136. doi:

10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.07.030

Evert, J., Lawler, E., Bogan, H., & Perls, T. (2003). Morbidity profiles of centenarians:

Survivors, delayers, and escapers. The Journals of Gerontology Series A:

Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 58(3), M232-M237. doi:

10.1093/gerona/58.3.M232

Fried, L. P., Ferrucci, L., Darer, J., Williamson, J. D., & Anderson, G. (2004). Untangling

the concepts of disability, frailty, and comorbidity: Implications for improved

targeting and care. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and

Medical Sciences, 59(3), M255-M263. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.M255

Fried, V., Bernstein, A., & Bush, M. (2012). Multiple chronic conditions among adults

aged 45 and over: Trends over the past 10 years. NCHS Stat Brief, 100 (pp. 1 – 7).

Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Fu, V. K., Winship, C., & Mare, R. D. (2004). Sample selection bias models. Handbook

of data analysis, 409-430. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Gagnon, A., Smith, K. R., Tremblay, M., Vézina, H., Paré, P. P., & Desjardins, B.

(2009). Is there a trade‐off between fertility and longevity? A comparative study

of women from three large historical databases accounting for mortality selection.

American Journal of Human Biology, 21(4), 533-540.

Galobardes, B., Lynch, J. W., & Smith, G. D. (2008). Is the association between

childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality established?

Update of a systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health,

62(5), 387-390. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.065508

Garibotti, G., Smith, K. R., Kerber, R. A., & Boucher, K. M. (2006). Longevity and

correlated frailty in multigenerational families. The Journals of Gerontology

Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 61(12), 1253-1261.

100

Gavrilov, L. A., & Gavrilova, N. S. (2012). Biodemography of exceptional longevity:

Early-life and mid-life predictors of human longevity. Biodemography and Social

Biology, 58(1), 14-39.

Geronimus, A. T., & Korenman, S. (1992). The socioeconomic consequences of teen

childbearing reconsidered. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(4), 1187-

1214. doi: 10.2307/2118385

Grundy, E., & Read, S. (2012). Social contacts and receipt of help among older people in

England: Are there benefits of having more children? The Journals of

Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(6), 742-

754. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs082

Grundy, E., & Tomassini, C. (2005). Fertility history and health in later-life: A record

linkage study in England and Wales. Social Science & Medicine, 61(1), 217-228.

doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.046

Guralnik, J. M. (1996). Assessing the impact of comorbidity in the older population.

Annals of Epidemiology, 6(5), 376-380. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1047-

2797(96)00060-9

Haviland, A. M., Nagin, D. S., Rosenbaum, P. R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Combining

group-based trajectory modeling and propensity score matching for causal

inferences in nonexperimental longitudinal data. Dev Psychol, 44(2), 422-436.

doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.422

Haviland, A. M., Jones, B. L., & Nagin, D. S. (2011). Group-based trajectory modeling

extended to account for nonrandom participant attrition. Sociological Methods &

Research, 40(2), 367-390. doi: 10.1177/0049124111400041

Hawkes, K. (2010). How grandmother effects plus individual variation in frailty shape

fertility and mortality: Guidance from human–chimpanzee comparisons.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(Supplement 2), 8977.

Heckman, J. J. (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica,

47(1), 153-161. doi: 10.2307/1912352

Henretta, J. C., Grundy, E. M., Okell, L. C., Wadsworth, M. E. (2008). Early motherhood

and mental health in midlife: A study of British and American cohorts. Aging &

Mental Health, 12(5), 605-614. doi: 10.1080/13607860802343084

House, J., Landis, K., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science,

241(4865), 540-545. doi: 10.1126/science.3399889

101

Institute of Medicine. ( 1996). WIC nutrition risk criteria: A scientific assessment.

Washington DC: National Academy Press.

Jelliffe, D., & Jelliffe, E. (1978). Human milk in the modern world: Psychological,

nutritional, and economic significance. Oxford: Oxford Univeristy Press/ELBS.

Jones, B. L., & Nagin, D. S. (2007). Advances in group-based trajectory modeling and an

SAS procedure for estimating them. Sociological Methods & Research, 35(4),

542-571. doi: 10.1177/0049124106292364

Kelsey, J. L., Gammon, M. D., & John, E. M. (1993). Reproductive factors and breast

cancer. Epidemiol Rev, 15(1), 36-47.

Kerber, R. A., O'Brien, E., Boucher, K. M., Smith, K. R., & Cawthon, R. M. (2012). A

Genome-wide study replicates linkage of 3p22-24 to extreme longevity in humans

and identifies possible additional loci. PLoS ONE, 7(4), e34746. doi:

10.1371/journal.pone.0034746

Kerber, R. A., O'Brien, E., Smith, K. R., & Cawthon, R. M. (2001). Familial excess

longevity in Utah genealogies. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological

Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56(3), B130-B139. doi:

10.1093/gerona/56.3.B130

Kirkwood, T. B. L., & Rose, M. R. (1991). Evolution of senescence: Late survival

sacrificed for reproduction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of

London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 332(1262), 15-24. doi:

10.1098/rstb.1991.0028

Kitagawa, E., & Hauser, P. (1973). Differential mortality in the United States: A study in

socioeconomic epidemiology (Vol. 35). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Klabunde, C. N., Potosky, A. L., Legler, J. M., & Warren, J. L. (2000). Development of a

comorbidity index using physician claims data. J Clin Epidemiol, 53(12), 1258-

1267.

Klabunde, C. N., Warren, J. L., & Legler, J. M. (2002). Assessing comorbidity using

claims data: An overview. Med Care, 40(8 Suppl), IV-26-35. doi:

10.1097/01.mlr.0000020936.03651.2d

Kobayashi, S., Sugiura, H., Ando, Y., Shiraki, N., Yanagi, T., Yamashita, H., & Toyama,

T. (2012). Reproductive history and breast cancer risk. Breast Cancer, 19(4), 302-

308. doi: 10.1007/s12282-012-0384-8

Kramer, M. S. (1987). Determinants of low birth weight: Methodological assessment and

meta-analysis. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 65(5), 663.

102

Kramer, M. S., Séguin, L., Lydon, J., & Goulet, L. (2000). Socio-economic disparities in

pregnancy outcome: Why do the poor fare so poorly? Paediatric and Perinatal

Epidemiology, 14(3), 194-210. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2000.00266.x

Kravdal, Ø. (1995). Relationship between childbearing and cancer incidence due to

biology or lifestyle? Examples of the importance of using data on men.

International Journal of Epidemiology, 24(3), 477-484. doi: 10.1093/ije/24.3.477

Kuh, D., & Ben-Shlomo, Y. (2004). A life course approach to chronic disease

epidemiology (Vol. 2). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kuh, D., & Hardy, R. (2002). A life course approach to women's health. Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Kvale, G., Heuch, I., & Nilssen, S. (1994). Parity in relation to mortality and cancer

incidence: A prospective study of norwegian women. International Journal of

Epidemiology, 23(4), 691-699. doi: 10.1093/ije/23.4.691

Lawlor, D. A., Emberson, J. R., Ebrahim, S., Whincup, P. H., Wannamethee, S. G.,

Walker, M., & Smith, G. D. (2003). Is the association between parity and

coronary heart disease due to biological effects of pregnancy or adverse lifestyle

risk factors associated with child-rearing? Circulation, 107(9), 1260-1264. doi:

10.1161/01.cir.0000053441.43495.1a

Lukanova, A., & Kaaks, R. (2005). Endogenous hormones and ovarian cancer:

Epidemiology and current hypotheses. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers &

Prevention, 14(1), 98-107.

MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002).

A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects.

Psychol Methods, 7(1), 83.

MacMahon, B., Cole, P., & Brown, J. (1973). Etiology of human breast cancer: A review.

Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 50(1), 21-42. doi: 10.1093/jnci/50.1.21

McCormick, M. C., Shapiro, S., & Starfield, B. (1984). High-risk young mothers: Infant

mortality and morbidity in four areas in the United States, 1973-1978. American

Journal of Public Health, 74(1), 18-23. doi: 10.2105/ajph.74.1.18

Mineau, G. P., Smith, K. R., & Bean, L. L. (2002). Historical trends of survival among

widows and widowers. Social Science & Medicine, 54(2), 245-254. doi:

10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00024-7

Mirowsky, J. (2005). Age at first birth, health, and mortality. Journal of Health and

Social Behavior, 46(1), 32-50. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600104

103

Moorad, J. A., Promislow, D. E. L., Smith, K. R., & Wade, M. J. (2011). Mating system

change reduces the strength of sexual selection in an American frontier population

of the 19th century. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(2), 147-155. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.10.004

Myklestad, K., Vatten, L. J., Magnussen, E. B., Salvesen, K. Å., Smith, G. D., &

Romundstad, P. R. (2012). Offspring birth weight and cardiovascular risk in

parents—A population-based HUNT 2 study. American Journal of Epidemiology,

175(6), 546-555. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwr347

Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge: Harvard

University Press.

Nagin, D. S., & Odgers, C. L. (2010). Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical

research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 109-138. doi:

doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131413

Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Analyzing developmental trajectories of distinct

but related behaviors: A group-based method. Psychol Methods, 6(1), 18-34.

Nam, C. B., & Powers, M. G. (1983). The socioeconomic approach to status

measurement: With a guide to occupational and socioeconomic status scores.

Houston: Cap and Gown Press.

Norton, M. C., Smith, K. R., Østbye, T., Tschanz, J. T., Schwartz, S., Corcoran, C.,

Breitner, J. C., et. al. (2011). Early parental death and remarriage of widowed

parents as risk factors for Alzheimer disease: The Cache County study. The

American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry : Official Journal of the American

Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, 19(9), 814-824.

Perls, T. T., Alpert, L., & Fretts, R. C. (1997). Middle-aged mothers live longer. Nature,

389(6647), 133-133.

Permuth-Wey, J., & Sellers, T. (2009). Epidemiology of ovarian cancer. In M. Verma

(Ed.), Cancer Epidemiology (Vol. 472, pp. 413-437): Totowa: Humana Press.

Phelan, J. C., Link, B. G., & Tehranifar, P. (2010). Social conditions as fundamental

causes of health inequalities: Theory, evidence, and policy implications. Journal

of Health and Social Behavior, 51(1 suppl), S28-S40. doi:

10.1177/0022146510383498

Preston, S. H., Hill, M. E., & Drevenstedt, G. L. (1998). Childhood conditions that

predict survival to advanced ages among African-Americans. Social Science &

Medicine (1982), 47(9), 1231.

Rich-Edwards, J. (2002). A life course approach to women’s reproductive health. A Life

Course Approach to Women’s Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 23-43.

104

Robson, S. L., & Smith, K. R. (2011). Twinning in humans: Maternal heterogeneity in

reproduction and survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 278(1725), 3755-3761. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0573

Robson, S. L., & Smith, K. R. (2012). Parity progression ratios confirm higher lifetime

fertility in women who bear twins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 279(1738), 2512-2514. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.0436

Romano, P. S., Roos, L. L., & Jollis, J. G. (1993). Adapting a clinical comorbidity index

for use with ICD-9-CM administrative data: Differing perspectives. J Clin

Epidemiol, 46(10), 1075-1079; discussion 1081-1090.

Ross, C. E., & Huber, J. (1985). Hardship and depression. Journal of Health and Social

Behavior, 26(4), 312-327. doi: 10.2307/2136655

Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. (1987). Human aging: Usual and successful. Science,

237(4811), 143-149. doi: 10.1126/science.3299702

Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Gerontologist, 37(4), 433-440.

doi: 10.1093/geront/37.4.433

Schiller, J., Lucas, J., & Peregoy, J. (2012). Summary health statistics for U.S. adults:

National Health Interview Survey, 2011. National Center for Health Statistics,

Vital Halth Stat 10(256).

Smith, G. D., Hart, C., Ferrell, C., Upton, M., Hole, D., Hawthorne, V., & Watt, G.

(1997). Birth weight of offspring and mortality in the renfrew and paisley study:

Prospective observational study. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 315(7117), 1189-

1193. doi: 10.2307/25176161

Smith, G. D., Whitley, E., Gissler, M., & Hemminki, E. (2000). Birth dimensions of

offspring, premature birth, and the mortality of mothers. The Lancet, 356(9247),

2066-2067. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)03406-1

Smith, G. D., Sterne, J. A. C., Tynelius, P., & Rasmussen, F. (2004). Birth characteristics

of offspring and parental diabetes: Evidence for the fetal insulin hypothesis.

Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 58(2), 126-128. doi:

10.1136/jech.58.2.126

Smith, K. R., Mineau, G., Garibotti, G., & Kerber, R. (2009). Effects of childhood and

middle-adulthood family conditions on later-life mortality: Evidence from the

Utah Population Database, 1850-2002. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 68(9),

1649.

Smith, K. R., Gagnon, A., Cawthon, R. M., Mineau, G. P., Mazan, R., & Desjardins, B.

(2009). Familial aggregation of survival and late female reproduction. The

105

Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences,

64A(7), 740-744. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp055

Smith, K. R., Hanson, H. A., Mineau, G. P., & Buys, S. S. (2012). Effects of BRCA1 and

BRCA2 mutations on female fertility. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:

Biological Sciences, 279(1732), 1389-1395. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1697

Smith, K. R., Hanson, H. A., & Zimmer, Z. (2012). Early-life conditions and later-life

(65+) comorbidity trajectories: The Utah Population Database linked to Medicare

claims data. Paper presented at the Population Association of America 2012, San

Fransico, CA.

Smith, K. R., Mineau, G. P., & Bean, L. L. (2002). Fertility and postreproductive

longevity. Biodemography and Social Biology, 49(3), 185-205.

Smith, K. R., Mineau, G. P., Garibotti, G., & Kerber, R. (2009). Effects of childhood and

middle-adulthood family conditions on later-life mortality: Evidence from the

Utah Population Database, 1850-2002. Social Science & Medicine, 68(9), 1649-

1658.

Smith, K. R., Reed, D. L., Hanson, H. A., Mineau, G. P., & Fraser, A. (2012). All in the

family: Fertility and Mortality consequences for child survivors of the 1918

influenza pandemic. Social Science History Association 2012 Annual Meeting.

Spence, N. J. (2008). The Long-term consequences of childbearing: Physical and

psychological well-being of mothers in later-life. Research on Aging, 30(6), 722-

751. doi: 10.1177/0164027508322575

Uhlenberg, P., & Cooney, T. M. (1990). Family size and mother-child relations in later-

life. The Gerontologist, 30(5), 618-625. doi: 10.1093/geront/30.5.618

Umberson, D., & Chen, M. D. (1994). Effects of a parent's death on adult children:

Relationship salience and reaction to loss. American Sociological Review, 59(1),

152-168. doi: 10.2307/2096138

Waldron, I., Weiss, C. C., & Hughes, M. E. (1998). Interacting effects of multiple roles

on women's health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 39(3), 216-236. doi:

10.2307/2676314

Westoff, C. F. (1986). Fertility in the United States. Science, 234(4776), 554-559. doi:

10.2307/1697852

Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natrual selection, and teh evolution of senescence.

Evolution, 11, 298-411.

106

Williams, K., & Umberson, D. (2004). Marital status, marital transitions, and health: A

gendered life course perspective. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45(1),

81-98. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500106

Yi, Z., & Vaupel, J. W. (2004). Association of late childbearing with healthy longevity

among the oldest old in China. Population Studies, 58(1), 37-53. doi:

10.1080/0032472032000175437

Zimmer, Z., Martin, L., Nagin, D., & Jones, B. (2012). Modeling disability trajectories

and mortality of the oldest old in China. Demography, 49(1), 291-314. doi:

10.1007/s13524-011-0075-7

Table 3.1. Hypothesized Relationship between Fertility and Later-Life Comorbidity

107

108

109

Table 3.3. Descriptive Statistics by Gender and Age Group

Female 66-74 in

1992 Female 75 - 84

in 1992 Male 66-74 in

1992 Male 75 - 84 in

1992

N=12,190 N=10,099 N=11,349 N=7,520

Early Life Conditions N(% of total sample) or Mean (SD) Father's Nam-Powers SES Score 49.37 (19.54) 47.4 (18.19) 49.0 (19.14) 47.09 (17.96)

Father Farmer 3556 (29.2%) 3699 (36.6%) 3434 (30.3%) 2786 (37.1%) Father Missing SES 2982 (24.5%) 2416 (23.9%) 2620 (23.1%) 1754 (23.3%)

Active LDS 6452 (52.9%) 5525 (54.7%) 5876 (51.8%) 4219 (56.1%) InActive LDS 2886 (23.7%) 2883 (28.6%) 2643 (23.3%) 1843 (24.5%)

Non-LDS 2852 (23.4%) 1691 (16.7%) 2830 (24.9%) 1458 (19.4%) FEL Bottom 25% 2841 (23.3%) 2335 (23.1%) 2693 (23.7%) 1640 (21.8%) FEL Middle 50% 5321 (43.7%) 4525 (44.8%) 4869 (42.9%) 3648 (48.5%)

FEL Top 25% 2662 (21.8%) 2653 (26.3%) 2554 (22.5%) 1757 (23.4%) FEL Missing 1366 (11.2%) 586 (5.8%) 1233 (10.9%) 475 (6.3%)

Mother Died when Ego <18 721 (5.9%) 746 (7.4%) 682 (6.0%) 555 (7.4%) Mother Died when Ego 18+ 11379 (93.4%) 9255 (91.6%) 10583 (93.3%) 6906 (91.8%) Father Died when Ego <18 1060 (8.7%) 943 (9.3%) 951 (8.4%) 714 (9.5%) Father Died when Ego 18+ 11040 (90.6%) 9058 (89.7%) 10314 (90.9%) 6747 (89.7%)

Both Parents Died Before 18 90 (0.7%) 98 (1%) 84 (0.7%) 59 (0.8%) Fertility History

Children 1-2 3004 (24.6%) 3138 (31.1%) 2533 (22.3%) 2104 (28.0%) Children 3-5 6798 (55.8%) 5287 (52.4%) 6605 (58.2%) 4087 (54.4%) Children 6-8 2061 (16.9%) 1410 (14.0%) 1934 (17.0%) 1135 (15.1%) Children 9+ 327 (2.7%) 264 (2.6%) 277 (2.4%) 194 (2.6%)

Infant Death (Y/N) 730 (6.0%) 757 (7.5%) 637 (5.6%) 527 (7.0%) Short Birth Interval (Y/N) 3380 (27.7%) 2238 (22.2) 3489 (30.7%) 1707 (22.7%) Long Birth Interval (Y/N) 5201 (42.7%) 4684 (46.4%) 4568 (40.3%) 3513 (46.7%)

Mother/Father of Twin 580 (4.8%) 489 (4.8%) 486 (4.3%) 370 (4.9%) Age at First Bith Less than 18 391 (3.2%) 316 (3.1%) n/a n/a

Age at First Birth 18 - 24 8590 (70.5%) 5871 (58.1%) 5815 (51.2%) 2687 (35.7%) Age at First Birth 25+ 3209 (26.3%) 3912 (38.7%) 5534 (48.8%) 4833 (34.3%)

Age at Last Birth 35 - 39 1848 (15.2%) 3105 (30.8%) 2864 (25.2%) 2188 (29.1%) Age at Last Birth >=40 3827 (31.4%) 2369 (23.5%) 2864 (25.2%) 2995 (39.8%)

Age 70.09 (2.57) 79.00 (4.76) 70.0 (2.56) 78.6 (2.78) Information from Utah Birth Certificates*

At least 1 High Birth Weight Baby 668 (16.1%) 1126 (18.8%) At least 1 Low Birth Weight Baby 283 (6.8%)

434 (7.0%)

At Least One Preterm Birth 572 (13.8%)

942 (15.2%) Adult Measures

Spouse Alive at Baseline 9523 (78.1%) 5023 (49.7%) 10913 (96.2%) 6662 (88.6%) *Female N=4142; Male N=6192

Note: Age 66-74 in 1992 with birth certificate data on all births, making this a select

sample. Females are younger (avg age=69, range 66 - 74) and had to have their first birth

after age 20 (1947 is the first year BC available). Males are younger and had to have

their first birth after age 20.

110

Table 3.4. Sample Selection Means by Gender and Age

Mean (StdDev) Mean (StdDev)

Selection

Model

Female Age 66 - 74

Excluded

(N=48,519)

Included

(N=12,190) Probit P-value

Age in 1992 69.81(2.55) 70.09(70.09) 0.024 <0.001

Born in Utah 0.39(0.49) 0.85(0.85) 1.186 <0.001

1990 County Med Family Income

(Unit=10,000) 3.35(0.53) 3.28(3.28) -0.085 <0.001

1990 County Population (Unit=100,000) 5.87(12.4) 3.63(3.63) -0.018 <0.001

Missing 1990 Census Info 1% 0.30% -0.444 <0.001

Constant

-2.911 <0.001

Female Age 75 - 84

Excluded

(N=34,225)

Included

(N=10,099) Probit P-value

Age in 1992 78.95(2.79) 79(2.84) 0.01 0.01

Born in Utah 0.41(0.49) 0.83(0.38) 1.10 <0.001

1990 County Med Family Income

(Unit=10,000) 3.33(0.55) 3.26(0.42) -0.12 <0.001

1990 County Population (Unit=100,000) 6.07(13.26) 3.65(6.34) -0.01 <0.001

Missing 1990 Census Info 1% 0.10% -0.81 <0.001

Constant

-1.48 <0.001

Male Age 66 - 74

Excluded

(N=48,866)

Included

(N=11,349) Probit P-value

Age in 1992 69.79(2.56) 69.97(2.56) 0.02 <0.001

Born in Utah 0.4(0.49) 0.86(0.34) 1.24 <0.001

1990 County Med Family Income

(Unit=10,000) 3.34(0.57) 3.28(0.43) -0.10 <0.001

1990 County Population (Unit=100,000) 6.09(13.52) 3.77(6.56) 0.00 <0.001

Missing 1990 Census Info 1% 0.30% -0.55 <0.001

Constant

-2.61 <0.001

Male Age 75 - 84

Excluded

(N=29,211) Included (N=7,520) Probit P-value

Age in 1992 78.67(2.75) 78.63(2.78) 0.00 0.61

Born in Utah 0.4(0.49) 0.84(0.37) 1.16 <0.001

1990 County Med Family Income

(Unit=10,000) 3.28(0.57) 3.26(0.43) -0.08 <0.001

1990 County Population (Unit=100,000) 5.63(13.1) 3.71(7.08) -0.01 <0.001

Missing 1990 Census Info 1% 0.30% -0.59 <0.001

Constant -1.08 <0.001

111

Table 3.5. Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership versus Robust Group (19.1%): Women Ages 66 – 74 in 1992

Slow Initiates

Accelerated

Initiates Chronic Low Ailing Frail

18.8% 13.3% 19.7% 21.8% 7.3%

Early Life Conditions Odd Ratio (95% CI)

Age in 1992 2.47(1.88,3.26) 2.54(1.89,3.41) 3.52(2.71,4.58) 3.01(2.34,3.87) 3.16(2.24,4.45)

Active Member of LDS Church 0.76(0.61,0.94) 0.55(0.44,0.68) 0.72(0.58,0.88) 0.45(0.37,0.54) 0.47(0.37,0.61)

Inactive Member of LDS

Church 0.84(0.66,1.06) 0.57(0.45,0.73) 0.78(0.62,0.98) 0.59(0.48,0.73) 0.56(0.42,0.73)

Non-Member (reference) 1 1 1 1 1

Father's NP SES (unit=10) 1.02(0.98,1.05) 0.98(0.94,1.02) 1.01(0.97,1.04) 0.99(0.96,1.03) 0.96(0.91,1)

Father Farmer 1.01(0.85,1.2) 0.8(0.67,0.96) 1.1(0.93,1.29) 0.79(0.68,0.92) 0.74(0.6,0.93)

Missing SES 0.91(0.76,1.09) 0.91(0.76,1.1) 1.08(0.91,1.28) 0.85(0.73,1.01) 1.08(0.87,1.34)

FEL in Bottom Quartile 1.19(0.99,1.44) 1.38(1.14,1.67) 1.24(1.04,1.48) 1.44(1.22,1.69) 1.27(1.02,1.59)

FEL in Mid 50% (reference) 1 1 1 1 1

FEL in Top Quartile 0.79(0.67,0.94) 0.75(0.62,0.91) 0.71(0.61,0.84) 0.58(0.49,0.68) 0.7(0.56,0.87)

FEL Missing 0.97(0.74,1.27) 0.75(0.57,1) 0.84(0.64,1.09) 0.56(0.44,0.72) 0.45(0.32,0.63)

Orphaned before Age 18 2.39(0.91,6.29) 1.78(0.62,5.14) 1.46(0.53,4.05) 2.62(1.08,6.36) 3.87(1.44,10.4)

Mother Died before Child 18 1.1(0.82,1.49) 1.28(0.94,1.73) 1.11(0.83,1.48) 1.24(0.95,1.63) 1.48(1.05,2.1)

Father Died before Child 18 1.12(0.87,1.45) 1.38(1.07,1.78) 1.12(0.88,1.43) 1.24(0.99,1.56) 1.32(0.98,1.78)

Both Parents Alive at 18

(reference) 1 1 1 1 1

Fertility

1-2 Children 0.87(0.72,1.06) 1.07(0.88,1.31) 0.89(0.74,1.06) 0.95(0.8,1.13) 1.13(0.9,1.42)

3-5 Children (reference) 1 1 1 1 1

6-8 Children 1.09(0.87,1.35) 1.04(0.82,1.31) 1.1(0.9,1.36) 1.16(0.95,1.42) 1.1(0.83,1.45)

9+ Children 0.86(0.52,1.43) 1.06(0.65,1.74) 1.01(0.65,1.58) 1.2(0.79,1.84) 0.96(0.51,1.77)

Age at First Birth < 18 1.48(0.97,2.25) 1.48(0.96,2.3) 1.33(0.88,2.01) 1.44(0.98,2.12) 1.94(1.2,3.13)

Age at First Birth 18 - 24 (ref) 1 1 1 1 1

Age at first Birth >= 25 1.04(0.88,1.24) 0.86(0.71,1.03) 0.93(0.79,1.1) 0.86(0.74,1.02) 1.06(0.86,1.31)

Age at Last Birth 35 - 39 0.85(0.71,1.02) 0.83(0.68,1.01) 0.81(0.68,0.97) 0.82(0.69,0.97) 0.76(0.61,0.96)

Age at Last Birth >= 40 0.65(0.5,0.84) 0.8(0.61,1.05) 0.82(0.65,1.04) 0.66(0.52,0.83) 0.75(0.54,1.03)

Mother of Twins 1.17(0.84,1.62) 0.95(0.66,1.38) 0.96(0.69,1.32) 1.12(0.83,1.51) 1.01(0.67,1.54)

One or More Short Birth

Intervals 1.01(0.85,1.19) 1.11(0.93,1.33) 0.92(0.78,1.08) 1.03(0.88,1.21) 1.06(0.85,1.31)

One or More Long Birth

Intervals 1.01(0.87,1.19) 1.06(0.9,1.26) 0.93(0.8,1.09) 1.02(0.88,1.17) 0.97(0.79,1.19)

One or More Infant Deaths 0.93(0.69,1.24) 0.67(0.47,0.95) 0.83(0.62,1.1) 1.02(0.79,1.32) 1.13(0.8,1.6)

Sample Selection Bias IMR 1.13(0.92,1.39) 1.03(0.82,1.28) 1.06(0.86,1.29) 1.17(0.96,1.41) 1.3(1.02,1.67)

Note: Results shown in Table 3.5 are controlling for widowhood and jointly modeled

with mortality trajectories.

112

Table 3.6. Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership versus Robust Group (18.2%): Women Ages 75 – 84 in 1992

Initiates Chronic Low Ailing Frail

21.6% 29.7% 16.9% 13.6%

Early Life Conditions Odd Ratio (95% CI)

Age in 1992 1.84(1.38,2.44) 3.89(2.89,5.24) 2.77(2.16,3.55) 2.95(2.21,3.95)

Active Member of LDS Church 1.05(0.82,1.33) 0.83(0.65,1.07) 0.89(0.72,1.1) 0.93(0.72,1.19)

Inactive Member of LDS Church 1.12(0.87,1.45) 0.79(0.6,1.03) 0.97(0.78,1.22) 1.05(0.81,1.37)

Non-Member (reference) 1 1 1 1

Father's NP SES (unit=10) 1.02(0.98,1.07) 1.02(0.98,1.07) 1.02(0.98,1.06) 1(0.95,1.04)

Father Farmer 0.93(0.78,1.12) 1.07(0.87,1.31) 1.14(0.96,1.34) 0.93(0.77,1.13)

Missing SES 1.01(0.82,1.23) 1.07(0.86,1.34) 1.11(0.92,1.33) 1.08(0.87,1.34)

FEL in Bottom Quartile 1.44(1.17,1.77) 1.41(1.12,1.76) 1.59(1.33,1.91) 1.6(1.3,1.97)

FEL in Mid 50% (reference) 1 1 1 1

FEL in Top Quartile 0.77(0.64,0.91) 0.83(0.69,1.01) 0.65(0.56,0.77) 0.61(0.51,0.75)

FEL Missing 0.91(0.63,1.3) 1.11(0.77,1.6) 0.81(0.58,1.11) 0.78(0.53,1.14)

Orphaned before Age 18 0.88(0.42,1.85) 0.97(0.42,2.23) 1.29(0.68,2.43) 0.78(0.32,1.91)

Mother Died before Child 18 1.16(0.86,1.56) 1.31(0.96,1.78) 0.99(0.75,1.3) 1.35(1,1.81)

Father Died before Child 18 1.19(0.91,1.57) 1.15(0.86,1.54) 1.27(1,1.61) 1.24(0.94,1.63)

Both Parents Alive at 18 (reference) 1 1 1 1

Fertility

1-2 Children 0.98(0.8,1.21) 0.96(0.77,1.2) 0.9(0.75,1.08) 0.89(0.72,1.11)

3-5 Children (reference) 1 1 1 1

6-8 Children 1.01(0.79,1.31) 0.93(0.7,1.23) 1.22(0.97,1.53) 1.15(0.88,1.52)

9+ Children 0.77(0.43,1.38) 0.68(0.36,1.29) 1.17(0.73,1.89) 1.98(1.18,3.32)

Age at First Birth < 18 1.45(0.84,2.49) 1.85(1.05,3.24) 1.97(1.24,3.13) 2.03(1.23,3.36)

Age at First Birth 18 - 24 (ref) 1 1 1 1

Age at first Birth >= 25 0.86(0.72,1.03) 0.77(0.63,0.94) 0.9(0.77,1.06) 0.78(0.64,0.95)

Age at Last Birth 35 - 39 1.07(0.87,1.31) 1.38(1.11,1.72) 1.01(0.84,1.21) 1.11(0.9,1.37)

Age at Last Birth >= 40 1.13(0.89,1.45) 1.34(1.03,1.75) 0.89(0.71,1.11) 0.76(0.58,0.99)

Mother of Twins 1.08(0.76,1.54) 0.96(0.64,1.44) 1.02(0.74,1.4) 1.19(0.83,1.71)

One or More Short Birth Intervals 1.04(0.85,1.26) 0.83(0.66,1.04) 1.03(0.87,1.23) 0.89(0.72,1.1)

One or More Long Birth Intervals 0.76(0.63,0.91) 0.66(0.54,0.8) 0.82(0.7,0.96) 0.73(0.61,0.88)

One or More Infant Deaths 0.78(0.57,1.06) 0.84(0.6,1.17) 1(0.78,1.29) 1.07(0.8,1.44)

Sample Selection Bias IMR 0.95(0.75,1.21) 1.13(0.87,1.45) 1.12(0.9,1.39) 1.12(0.87,1.44)

Note: Results shown in Table 3.6 are controlling for widowhood and jointly modeled

with mortality trajectories.

113

Table 3.7. Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership versus Robust Group (15.7%): Men Ages 66 – 74 in 1992

Slow Initiates

Accelerated

Initiates Chronic Low Ailing Frail

16.4% 15.3% 18.5% 26.1% 8%

Early Life Conditions Odd Ratio (95% CI)

Age in 1992 1.4(1.01,1.94) 1.64(1.2,2.23) 2.33(1.73,3.15) 3.14(2.39,4.12) 3.41(2.36,4.91)

Active Member of LDS Church 0.76(0.59,0.98) 0.55(0.44,0.7) 0.82(0.65,1.03) 0.48(0.39,0.59) 0.4(0.31,0.52)

Inactive Member of LDS Church 0.66(0.5,0.86) 0.7(0.55,0.91) 0.75(0.58,0.97) 0.59(0.47,0.74) 0.45(0.34,0.6)

Non-Member (reference) 1 1 1 1 1

Father's NP SES (unit=10) 0.98(0.94,1.02) 1.01(0.97,1.05) 0.99(0.95,1.03) 1.02(0.98,1.05) 1.02(0.97,1.07)

Father Farmer 0.99(0.81,1.2) 1.05(0.87,1.27) 0.9(0.75,1.07) 1.13(0.96,1.33) 1.06(0.84,1.32)

Missing SES 1.05(0.85,1.3) 1.1(0.9,1.35) 0.96(0.79,1.18) 1.05(0.88,1.26) 1.14(0.9,1.44)

FEL in Bottom Quartile 1.2(0.96,1.49) 1.19(0.97,1.47) 1.28(1.05,1.57) 1.37(1.14,1.64) 1.3(1.03,1.65)

FEL in Mid 50% (reference) 1 1 1 1 1

FEL in Top Quartile 0.73(0.6,0.88) 0.71(0.59,0.86) 0.68(0.57,0.82) 0.66(0.56,0.78) 0.6(0.48,0.76)

FEL Missing 0.86(0.63,1.17) 0.88(0.65,1.18) 0.87(0.65,1.17) 0.55(0.42,0.72) 0.5(0.35,0.71)

Orphaned before Age 18 0.27(0.09,0.79) 0.57(0.24,1.34) 0.58(0.25,1.35) 0.72(0.37,1.41) 0.73(0.29,1.84)

Mother Died before Child 18 1.21(0.86,1.7) 1.18(0.85,1.64) 1.14(0.82,1.58) 1.21(0.9,1.62) 1.12(0.76,1.65)

Father Died before Child 18 1.14(0.85,1.54) 1.17(0.88,1.55) 1.08(0.82,1.43) 1.25(0.97,1.6) 1.07(0.77,1.5)

Both Parents Alive at 18

(reference) 1 1 1 1 1

Fertility

1-2 Children 1.04(0.82,1.3) 1.17(0.94,1.46) 1.14(0.92,1.41) 1.01(0.83,1.23) 1.25(0.96,1.61)

3-5 Children (reference) 1 1 1 1 1

6-8 Children 0.84(0.65,1.08) 1.04(0.82,1.32) 0.91(0.72,1.15) 0.91(0.74,1.13) 1.19(0.9,1.58)

9+ Children 0.63(0.37,1.08) 0.87(0.53,1.42) 0.63(0.37,1.05) 0.71(0.45,1.1) 0.77(0.41,1.47)

Age at First Birth <25 (ref) 1 1 1 1 1

Age at first Birth >= 25 0.88(0.74,1.05) 0.79(0.66,0.93) 0.78(0.66,0.92) 0.74(0.63,0.86) 0.78(0.63,0.95)

Age at Last Birth 35 - 39 0.99(0.8,1.23) 1.15(0.93,1.42) 0.98(0.8,1.2) 1.02(0.85,1.22) 1.12(0.88,1.43)

Age at Last Birth >= 40 1.04(0.79,1.37) 1.21(0.93,1.57) 0.88(0.68,1.14) 1.09(0.87,1.38) 0.94(0.69,1.29)

Father of Twins 0.94(0.64,1.4) 0.95(0.65,1.39) 1.2(0.85,1.71) 1.01(0.72,1.4) 1.13(0.73,1.75)

One or More Short Birth

Intervals 1.11(0.92,1.33) 0.94(0.78,1.13) 1.17(0.98,1.4) 1.01(0.86,1.18) 1.13(0.91,1.41)

One or More Long Birth

Intervals 1.08(0.9,1.29) 0.98(0.82,1.17) 1.17(0.98,1.39) 0.97(0.83,1.13) 1.06(0.86,1.31)

One or More Infant Deaths 0.83(0.59,1.18) 1.07(0.78,1.47) 0.83(0.6,1.14) 0.85(0.64,1.14) 0.85(0.57,1.25)

Sample Selection Bias IMR 0.89(0.7,1.14) 0.9(0.71,1.13) 0.91(0.73,1.13) 1.06(0.87,1.29) 0.98(0.75,1.28)

Note: Results shown in Table 3.7 are controlling for widowhood and jointly modeled

with mortality trajectories.

114

Table 3.8. Effects of Early Life Conditions and Fertility on Comorbidity Trajectory

Group Membership versus Robust Group (14.7%): Men Ages 75 – 84 in 1992

Initiates Ailing Chronic Low Frail

17.4% 35.3% 20.2% 12.5%

Early Life Conditions Odd Ratio (95% CI)

Age in 1992 1.6(1.09,2.34) 2.24(1.65,3.05) 2.12(1.5,3) 2.82(1.94,4.1)

Active Member of LDS Church 0.81(0.6,1.07) 0.78(0.62,1) 0.8(0.61,1.05) 0.72(0.53,0.96)

Inactive Member of LDS Church 1.01(0.73,1.4) 0.98(0.75,1.28) 0.97(0.71,1.31) 0.97(0.7,1.34)

Non-Member (reference) 1 1 1 1

Father's NP SES (unit=10) 1(0.94,1.06) 1(0.95,1.04) 1.01(0.95,1.06) 0.96(0.9,1.02)

Father Farmer 0.95(0.75,1.21) 0.94(0.77,1.14) 0.95(0.76,1.19) 1.09(0.85,1.38)

Missing SES 1.06(0.81,1.38) 0.85(0.68,1.06) 0.9(0.7,1.16) 0.98(0.74,1.29)

FEL in Bottom Quartile 1.38(1.06,1.81) 1.7(1.36,2.13) 1.28(0.99,1.66) 1.89(1.46,2.46)

FEL in Mid 50% (reference) 1 1 1 1

FEL in Top Quartile 0.8(0.63,1.01) 0.77(0.64,0.94) 0.91(0.73,1.14) 0.67(0.52,0.87)

FEL Missing 0.71(0.45,1.11) 0.76(0.53,1.1) 0.99(0.66,1.49) 0.84(0.53,1.33)

Orphaned before Age 18 1.16(0.34,3.94) 1.28(0.5,3.24) 0.67(0.18,2.45) 0.43(0.1,1.84)

Mother Died before Child 18 1.14(0.78,1.68) 1.36(0.99,1.87) 1.11(0.77,1.61) 1(0.66,1.51)

Father Died before Child 18 1.01(0.73,1.39) 0.97(0.75,1.27) 0.79(0.57,1.09) 1(0.72,1.39)

Both Parents Alive at 18 (reference) 1 1 1 1

Fertility

1-2 Children 1.01(0.76,1.34) 0.95(0.75,1.2) 1.07(0.82,1.39) 0.97(0.73,1.29)

3-5 Children (reference) 1 1 1 1

6-8 Children 0.81(0.59,1.12) 1.14(0.88,1.47) 0.89(0.66,1.19) 0.77(0.55,1.07)

9+ Children 0.82(0.43,1.57) 0.81(0.47,1.4) 0.76(0.41,1.4) 0.61(0.31,1.2)

Age at First Birth <25 (ref) 1 1 1 1

Age at first Birth >= 25 0.97(0.77,1.23) 0.82(0.68,1) 0.86(0.69,1.07) 0.71(0.56,0.9)

Age at Last Birth 35 - 39 0.96(0.72,1.28) 0.85(0.67,1.08) 0.79(0.6,1.04) 0.73(0.54,0.98)

Age at Last Birth >= 40 0.94(0.67,1.31) 0.74(0.56,0.97) 0.94(0.69,1.28) 0.81(0.58,1.12)

Father of Twins 0.86(0.54,1.37) 0.71(0.48,1.05) 1.25(0.84,1.86) 1.12(0.71,1.74)

One or More Short Birth Intervals 1.16(0.9,1.51) 0.92(0.74,1.14) 0.98(0.76,1.25) 1.15(0.88,1.49)

One or More Long Birth Intervals 1.11(0.88,1.39) 1.05(0.87,1.26) 1.12(0.9,1.38) 1.1(0.87,1.39)

One or More Infant Deaths 1.03(0.69,1.55) 1.27(0.92,1.76) 1.01(0.69,1.49) 0.96(0.64,1.46)

Sample Selection Bias IMR 1.21(0.89,1.64) 1.38(1.07,1.78) 1.15(0.86,1.54) 0.98(0.71,1.36)

Note: Results shown in Table 3.8 are controlling for widowhood and jointly modeled

with mortality trajectories.

115

Figure 3.1. Comorbidity trajectories for females ages 66 – 74 in 1992.

116

Figure 3.2. Comorbidity trajectories for females ages 75 – 84 in 1992.

117

Figure 3.3. Comorbidity trajectories for males ages 66 – 74 in 1992.

** **

118

Figure 3.4. Comorbidity trajectories for males ages 75 – 84 in 1992.

119

Figure 3.5. Morbidity trajectories for females ages 66 – 74 in 1992.

120

Figure 3.6. Morbidity trajectories for females ages 75 – 84 in 1992.

121

Figure 3.7. Morbidity trajectories for males ages 66 – 74 in 1992.

122

Figure 3.8. Morbidity trajectories for males ages 75 – 84 in 1992.

123

Figure 3.9. Female birth certificate results: Ages 66 – 74 in 1992. Controlling for early-

life conditions, fertility variables, demographic characteristics, and jointly modeled with

mortality trajectories. Birth certificate sample (N=4,214)

0.00

0.50

1.00

1.50

2.00

2.50

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

Slow Initiates(19.2%)

AcceleratedInitiates (14.3%)

Chronic Low(18.0%)

Ailing (21.3%) Frail (6.9%)

Od

ds

Rat

io

124

Figure 3.10. Male birth certificate results: Ages 66 – 74 in 1992. Controlling for early-

life conditions, fertility variables, demographic characteristics, and jointly modeled with

mortality trajectories. Birth certificate sample (N=6,192)

0.00

0.50

1.00

1.50

2.00

2.50

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

HB

W O

ffsp

rin

g

LBW

Off

spri

ng

Pre

term

Off

spri

ng

Slow Initiates(19.2%)

AcceleratedInitiates (14.3%)

Chronic Low(18.0%)

Ailing (21.3%) Frail (6.9%)

Od

ds

Rat

io

CHAPTER 4

HERITABILITY OF LONGEVITY AND THE ROLE OF EARLY

AND MIDLIFE ENVIRONMENTS3

Introduction

Mortality is the quintessential measure of the health of a population, and large

gains in human life expectancy over the past 150 years are evidence of the important

relationship between social context and health. While there is certainly variability in

longevity between populations, there is also wide variation in longevity within

populations. Understanding the determinants of this heterogeneity is essential to

understanding the processes of aging and health of a population. But how much of this

variation is determined by genetic factors and how much is determined by the

environment? While the question of heritability of longevity is not new, with heritability

estimates of longevity ranging from 0 to 0.3 (Kerber, O'Brien, Smith, & Cawthon, 2001),

we seek to determine if heritability estimates vary between subpopulations and explore

the possibility of gene-environment interactions (GxE). By examining sources of

3 Coauthored by Ken R. Smith and Sandra Hasstedt. We wish to thank the Pedigree and

Population Resource of the Huntsman Cancer Institute, University of Utah for providing

the data and valuable computing support. This work was also supported by NIH grant

AG022095 (Early-life Conditions, Survival and Health; Smith PI).

126

variation in heritability estimates, we can illuminate factors that modify the expression of

genetic predisposition in a population.

We will investigate heterogeneity in the genetic basis of longevity by assessing

the phenotypic correlation between relatives. Variance components and heritability

values will be generated using a large genealogical database with information on family

structure as well as measures of the broader environment. This study will examine the

relationship between social context and the amount of additive genetic variance in adult

life-span and exceptional longevity using data from the Utah Population Database

(UPDB), a rich source of linked population-based information for demographic, genetic,

and epidemiological studies. The sample used in this study consists of 20,120 individuals

from 802 three generation pedigrees. This analysis has two goals: 1) estimate the

heritability of longevity after age 30 as well as exceptional longevity in a population

using methods designed for use in multigenerational pedigree information; 2) test for

differences in heritability estimates of life-span in populations stratified by environmental

exposure.

Background

Heritability of Longevity

Over the past few decades, demographers have broadened the focus of work in the

demography of aging from a population aging perspective (i.e., measures of change in

population age structure) to include a perspective that integrates health and biological

explanations with traditional demographic and social theories of aging to explain

heterogeneity in health and mortality within and between populations (Olshansky,

Carnes, & Brody, 2002; Siegel, 2011; Vasunilashorn & Crimmins, 2008). While it is

127

widely accepted that life-span is determined by a combination of genetic, social and

physical environment, and stochastic factors, the interdependent and dynamic role of

genes and environment is still not well understood. This may be partially due to fears of

genetic determinism within the field of sociology (Shostak & Freese, 2010), the divergent

paths of genetics and demography (Adams, 1990), and the difficulty of assessing the role

of genes and environment biomarker data.

Longevity is a complex trait, determined by a multiplicity of genetic and

environmental factors, each of which contributes to a potentially small amount of

phenotypic variation. The genetic variation that is the natural background is a shortened

life-span (relative to exceptional longevity) and exceptional longevity is the result of

mutations. Genes affecting longevity have been parsed into two categories described as

gerontogenes: genes that have a negative effect on longevity and longevity-assurance

genes that promote longevity (Christensen, Johnson, & Vaupel, 2006). Findings from the

New England Centenarian Study (NECS) have suggested that supercentenarians do not

lack gerontogenes, but have longevity assurance genes that can counter the deleterious

effects of genes and environment as well as slow the rate of aging and lead to delayed

onset of age-related disease (Sebastiani et al., 2012). It is also believed that longevity

mutations increase the ability to handle stress and robustness (Christensen et al., 2006).

The proportion of variation in life-span due to genes is moderate, which can be illustrated

by the fact that there is variation in life-span between monozygotic twins (Herskind et al.,

1996).

In summary, longevity is determined by a complex relationship between both

genes and environment. For a more complete understanding of population heterogeneity

in life-span and the forces behind it, one must not only understand the average

128

contribution of genes and environment within a population toward explaining variation in

adult mortality, but uncover the factors that influence patterns of variation within the

population.

At the most basic level, phenotypic variation can be partitioned into additive

genetic variance and general environmental variance. Additive genetic variance is the

deviation from the average phenotype that is due to the inheritance of a particular allele

and that allele’s effect on the phenotype. General environmental variance can then be

described as the remaining variance that cannot be attributed to genes. The proportion of

variation due to inheritance of a particular allele is not fixed across all environments

because the relationship between genotype and phenotype may vary by environment, a

phenomenon known as phenotypic plasticity. Narrow sense heritability is a population

level statistic that describes the amount of total phenotypic variation (VT) that can be

attributed to additive genetic variation (VA) in the population (h2

= VA/VT). The

polygenic model can be used to partition variation into genetic and residual

environmental effects. Because it is a population level statistic, it is important to keep in

mind that it not a property of individual traits. When h2equals zero, it indicates that all

phenotypic variation within a population can be explained by individual differences,

while an h2

of 1 indicates that all the phenotypic variation is explained by genetic

differences. This is not to say that high heritability suggests little environmental effect on

the phenotype. When h2 is elevated the environment may uniformly contribute to the

expression of the trait and therefore contribute little to differences between people. It has

been shown that heritability of traits can vary across subpopulations (Boardman, 2009;

Boardman et al., 2012; Rowe, Almeida, & Jacobson, 1999). But how much of the

population heterogeneity in life-span is determined by genetic factors?

129

There is evidence of the presence of familial clustering of longevity over many

generations and across diverse populations, suggesting that there is a genetic or familial

component to successful aging and longevity (Christensen et al., 2006; Finch & Tanzi,

1997; Herskind et al., 1996; Kerber et al., 2001; Perls, Kunkel, & Puca, 2002). The

longevity literature has described the genetic and environmental contribution to mortality

as being divided into one-third and two-third proportions, respectively (C. E. Finch &

Tanzi, 1997; Siegel, 2011). It has also been suggested that 50% of the variation in life-

span after age 30 can be ascribed to attributes (genetic and nongenetic) that are fixed

prior to that age (Yashin & Iachine, 1997), and that genetics plays a stronger role with

advancing age (Hjelmborg et al., 2006; Montesanto, Dato, Bellizzi, Rose, & Passarino,

2012; Vaupel et al., 1998; see J. W. Rowe and Kahn (1997) for a dissent from this view).

If the proportion of variation in life-span that can be explained by genetic factors varies

by age, is it also conditioned by social context? And if so, does this conditioning vary by

age?

Conceptualizing the Relationship between Social Environment

and Heritability of Longevity

Attempting to understand the genetic component of longevity without considering

how it may be modified by specific environmental factors may not be a fruitful approach

to gaining insights into the heritability of this complex trait (Petronis, 2010). The

heritability of certain phenotypes may vary throughout the life course (Turkheimer,

Haley, Waldron, D'Onofrio, & Gottesman, 2003) and by gender (Visscher, Hill, & Wray,

2008). Given that humans are constantly interacting with the environment and the

environment has the ability to alter gene expression, we must also understand how

130

environmental influences might modify the heritability of longevity. Accordingly, in this

analysis, comparisons of heritability will be made between three subgroups of the

population based on: 1) religious involvement; 2) early disease and nutritional

environment; and 3) family environment during childhood.

This study assumes that the same genes affect longevity across environments

within a population, but certain attributes of the environment serve to moderate the effect

of genes on phenotypic variation. Shanahan and Hofer (2005) have presented a

framework for gene and social context interactions that has been used to explain the

relationship between the social environment and health behaviors (Boardman, 2009;

Boardman et al., 2012). We present a slightly modified version that also utilizes concepts

presented by Hoffmann and Merilä (1999) as well as new modifications to help formulate

our hypotheses.

Under Shanahan and Hofer’s framework for gene-environment (GxE)

interactions, the environment is conceptualized as social context (Shanahan & Hofer,

2005). Four perspectives, described in detail below, can be used to depict how the social

environment might affect heritable variation: triggering, compensation, social control,

and enhancement. Figures 4.1 – 4.4 show a modified version of a schema presented by

Sebastiani et al. (2012) describing the genetic components of aging. Sebastiani et al.

have hypothesized that individuals living to exceptional ages have gerontogenes, but the

longevity assurance genes counter the deleterious effects of genetic and environmental

factors. Figure 4.1 shows the proportion of total phenotypic variance (VT) that is

attributable to additive genetic variance (VA) and environmental variance (VE), where the

phenotype is longevity after age 30. We show that in a normal environment where there

is no GxE interaction (panel A), individuals with shorter life-spans have higher

131

heritability of gerontogenes and individuals with exceptional life-spans have higher

heritability of longevity assurance genes.

A triggering effect refers to an environment that interacts with personal

predispositions to a diseased state and shortened life-span through, for example,

environmental stressors or other factors that induce a biological change. Figure 4.2

shows the hypothesized triggering GxE interaction in which an adverse environment

directly affects the phenotype. When triggering mechanisms are responsible for

environmental differences in heritability, we expect to see a decrease in average

longevity in adverse environments and an increase in additive genetic variance. This is

because the environment leads to phenotypic expression that would otherwise be

dormant. This relationship may change with exceptional longevity because selection

mechanisms may change the heritability of a trait over time. If the genetically frail

individuals are selected out of this population at an earlier age, the surviving population

may be comprised of more robust individuals with a genetic predisposition for

exceptional longevity (i.e., longevity assurance genes; Hawkes, Smith, & Blevins, 2012),

leading to higher levels of heritability of exceptional longevity in environments

detrimental to health. Therefore, under this formulation, we expect that individuals

exposed to an unhealthy environment during childhood will have higher heritability of

longevity, but a shorter life-span compared to those living in more advantageous

circumstances. This may also translate into higher heritability of exceptional longevity in

unhealthy environments because only the robust in an unhealthy environment survive to

exceptional ages.

The second type of GxE interaction is compensation. According to this

perspective, in normal and adverse environments the predisposition to a diseased state

132

and shortened life-span is realized but not in enriched settings. The expected change in

additive genetic variance in an enriched environment is presented in Figure 4.3. The

compensation GxE perspective assumes that the continuous exposure to a healthy

environment prevents the expression of a genetic diathesis that predisposes an individual

to premature death. Unlike the triggering mechanism, the relationship between

environment and phenotype is not causal, but due to environmental variation. Therefore,

we would expect to see an increase in average life expectancy in an enriched environment

with lower additive genetic variance for the longevity phenotype.

Social control is the third GxE model. This interaction is not presented in Figures

4.1 – 4.4 because the expected outcomes are similar to those presented in Figure 4.3.

Heritability of longevity may be attenuated in environments with high social control

because social norms and structural constraints place limits on choices, and, therefore the

environment suppresses phenotypic variance. This is similar to the evolutionary

argument of canalization, which argues that selection favors suppression of quantitative

traits in constant and structured environments, but the genotype maintains a potential for

expressing certain phenotypes under particular environmental conditions (Hoffmann &

Merilä, 1999). Thus, involvement with a religious institution that maintains strong social

norms for health related behaviors such as alcohol consumption, smoking, social support,

and dietary restrictions may lead to increased longevity and exceptional longevity for all

members of the group and suppress genetic predispositions for disease. In this situation,

we expect to see increased longevity and exceptional longevity for active religious

participants with lower levels of heritability compared to nonparticipants.

The enhancement model of GxE is presented Figure 4.4. This is similar to the

social control mechanism, but rather than suppressing a predisposition to a shortened life-

133

span, social context can serve to enhance genetic predispositions for longevity.

Individuals in advantaged and organized social settings may be more apt to realize their

genetic potential for longevity, while disadvantaged environments lead to unrealized

potential. For example, an environment of undernutrition or high levels of exposure to

infectious agents may lead to physiological changes that alter an individual’s ability to

reach their genetic potential (Barker, 1995; Barker et al., 1993; Crimmins & Finch,

2006). Here, we would expect to see mean differences in survival between environments

and higher heritability of life-span and exceptional longevity in environments more

advantageous for health and longevity.

In this paper, we build on a body of literature that examines the heritability of

longevity by comparing heritability of longevity and exceptional longevity between sub-

populations exposed to different environments that are known to affect adult mortality

risks. Using the GxE perspectives discussed above, we compare the heterogeneity of

genetic effects by environment. We expect to see differences in the heritability of

longevity between environments characterized as salubrious or unhealthy. The GxE

categories of triggering, compensation, and social control predict higher levels of

heritability of longevity in environments less beneficial to health, while the enhancement

typology predicts increased heritability of longevity in healthy environments. We can

make generalizations about what type of GxE interaction leads to the observed patterns,

but the exact mechanism is not testable under this formulation. Comparing the

components of variance between environments will add to the understanding of the

relative importance of both genes and environment in determining longevity.

134

Methods

Data

The majority of life-span epidemiological studies examine health influences of

early and adult life conditions with relatively modest sample sizes, particularly given the

complexity of the phenomena and the manifold exposures and outcomes. This study

utilizes data drawn from the Utah Population Database (UPDB). The UPDB is one of the

world’s richest sources of linked population-based information for demographic, genetic,

and epidemiological studies. UPDB has supported biodemographic studies as well

numerous important epidemiological and genetic studies in large part because of its size,

pedigree complexity, and linkages to numerous data sources. In the mid-1970s, over

185,000 three-generation families were identified on Family Group Sheets from the

archives at the Utah Family History Library. These families have been linked into

multigenerational families and the full UPDB now contains data on nearly 7 million

individuals due to longstanding and on-going efforts to add new sources of data and

update records as they become available.

Mortality data are fundamental to the study of exceptional longevity. Information

on deaths prior to 1904 comes from genealogical records obtained from the Utah Family

History Library and linked to other records within the UPDB. All Utah death certificates

are available from 1904 to the present. The UPDB also links to the U.S. Social Security

Death Index (SSDI) for the years 1936 – 2011. The SSDI records provide information on

deaths based on Social Security records regardless of place of death and are linked to the

UPDB. The unique combination of genealogy, death certificates, and SSDI data provide

wide spatial and temporal coverage for both the fact and date of death.

135

The sample used to construct measures of longevity comprises all individuals in

the UPDB born between 1850 and 1927. We selected 1927 as the maximum birth year to

allow us to observe mortality to at least age 85 for the youngest members of the cohort.

To minimize variability in survival unrelated to aging and based on other evidence of the

fixed attributes related to life-span after age 30 (Yashin & Iachine, 1997), we will model

mortality beginning at age 30 (Hawkes et al., 2012). We identified 685,949 individuals

who met the criteria listed above. Of those, approximately 9% (n=64,258) were right

censored and 91% (N=621,961) had vital status follow-up information from family

history group sheets, Utah death certificates, or linked Social Security Death Index

(SSDI) information. The gender distribution of the sample was 52.5% male and 47.5%

female.

Using individuals from the baseline survival analysis, we selected 111,324 three-

generation families. Table 4.1 shows the restrictions imposed at the family level. We

attempted to select families with the highest data quality and the most complete

information. As a result, 31,322 families were excluded from the analysis because at least

one grandparent had no information in the UPDB. All founding pedigree members were

required to have a birth year greater than 1850 (Utah was settled in 1847) and all

members of the family were required to be born before 1928, which allowed us to

observe the youngest members of the cohort to age 84. On average, these families had 4

individuals in the first generation (by definition), 13 individuals in the second generation,

and 19 individuals in the third generation (range = 1 to 83). Pedigree size ranged from 7

to 109 members. The final sample consisted of 802 three-generation families with 20,120

members with a calculated longevity measure and information on family of origin. To

study exceptional longevity, a nearly deceased cohort is needed. Therefore, individuals

136

born between 1914 and 1927 were excluded from the exceptional longevity sample,

yielding a sample of 14,618 individuals for these analyses.

Measuring Early and Midlife Environments

Both early and midlife conditions will be considered as possible social context

that may modify phenotypic expression. To simplify both the measurement and

conceptualization of the environment, each environment is treated as a simple dichotomy,

comparing salubrious environments to those that are less advantageous to health. We

will compare the heritability of longevity by religious participation, infant mortality rate

(IMR) in the family of origin, childhood mortality rate (CMR) of the family of origin,

and number of siblings. Justifications for each as the basis for deleterious and beneficial

environments are described in turn below.

Religious involvement in general is associated with increased life expectancy

(Hummer, Rogers, Nam, & Ellison, 1999). It is not surprising that active affiliation with

the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (LDS or Mormon) church is also

associated with increased life expectancy (Enstrom & Breslow, 2008). Individuals

actively affiliated with the LDS church are more likely to abstain from alcohol and

tobacco use, fast once a month, and participate in church related social activities (Mineau,

Smith, & Bean, 2002). Therefore, affiliation with the LDS church will be treated as a

social environment with defined healthy lifestyle norms for men and women. The UPDB

contains information on baptism dates from family history records, which were used to

classify individuals as followers of the LDS church. Individuals baptized as members of

the LDS church before the age of 30 are considered followers of the LDS Church.

137

Individuals will be parsed into two environments: 1) LDS church involvement; and 2) no

LDS church involvement.

Early life health can have long-term consequences on later life health and

mortality (Elo & Preston, 1992; Smith, Mineau, Garibotti, & Kerber, 2009). While it is

difficult to obtain a measure of early life exposure to disease and other adverse

circumstances, we can use mortality outcomes of siblings as a sentinel for early life

circumstances. Postneonatal mortality (the first year of life excluding the first 28 days)

for our cohorts of study (1850 – 1927) is closely related to viral and bacterial disease,

malnutrition, and income (B. K. Finch, 2003; McKeown, 1976; Preston & Haines, 1991).

A similar argument has been made for childhood mortality by Crimmins and Finch

(2006), who argue that birth cohorts with lower childhood mortality have increased

longevity. As such, we use the death of a sibling during the first year of life (IMR in

family of origin) or between ages 1 and 5 (CMR in family of origin) as indicators of an

adverse childhood environment. Neonatal deaths, deaths within the first 28 days, and

stillbirths are not included in our final measure of IMR because these deaths are likely

due to endogenous causes and may not represent a family environment marked by

disease, an assumption most likely to be true for the years considered here. We consider

infant and childhood mortality as distinct environments because it has been suggested

that the determinants of infant and childhood mortality decline over time differed during

this period (Wolleswinkel-van den Bosch, van Poppel, Looman, & Mackenbach, 2000).

Individuals with one or more infant or childhood death in their family of origin (i.e.,

death of a sibling) were considered to be in an environment of high infant or childhood

mortality, respectively.

138

Sibship size (number of siblings) has been shown to be positively associated with

lower educational achievement and unhealthy lifestyle choices (Downey, 1995; Hart &

Davey Smith, 2003). Sibship size may also be related to exposure to infectious diseases,

with children from large sibships having a greater risk of contracting an infectious

disease (Hart & Davey Smith, 2003). However, a strong association between sibship size

and adult mortality has not been demonstrated in all studies assessing this relationship

(Smith et al., 2009). The definition of large sibship was derived empirically as having 7

or more siblings (75th

percentile for the sample).

While sex is inherently a biological trait, sex differences in life expectancy are

determined by both social and biological factors (Crimmins & Saito, 2001; Lindahl-

Jacobsen et al., 2013; Rieker, Bird, & Lang, 2010). The effects of early life conditions on

later life health may differ by sex. Male fetuses have higher mortality rates than female

fetuses, a disadvantage that continues throughout the life course (Kraemer, 2000). Earlier

studies have found slight differences in the heritability of longevity between males and

females, with males having higher heritability than females (Herskind et al., 1996).

Accordingly, we test for environmental differences in the heritability of longevity by sex.

Definition of Longevity

The mortality schedule for individuals born between 1850 and 1927 has changed

considerably. Longevity, therefore, defined simply as years lived after age 30, is not

appropriate because it is not directly comparable across birth cohorts. While much of the

improvement in life expectancy seen during this period was due to improvements in

infant and child mortality, there were also gains in adult mortality. Cohort life tables for

Utah show that individuals born in 1850 and surviving to age 30 could expect to live an

139

additional 40.5 and 39.5 years for females and males, respectively, compared to 53.1 and

48.5 years of additional life after 30 for individuals born in 1920 (Lindahl-Jacobsen et al.,

2013). Therefore, to de-trend the data, we define longevity as the difference between an

individual’s attained age (y) and the age to which that individual was expected to live

(median predicted age of death conditioned on surviving to 30, ̂) according to a model

that incorporates two basic determinants of life-span: gender and birth year. Therefore, a

longevity score (LS) is simply the difference between these two values, y - ̂. The

baseline survival models used to determine ̂ are described below. This approach is

similar to one taken by Kerber et al. (2001) in calculating a measure of familial excess

longevity using Utah genealogies.

Previous studies have suggested that the heritability of longevity increases with

age (Hjelmborg et al., 2006; Yashin & Iachine, 1997), and may perhaps be the strongest

for those surviving to the latest ages (Atzmon et al., 2004; Gudmundsson, Gudbjartsson,

Frigge, Gulcher, & Stefánsson, 2000; T. T. Perls, Bubrick, Wager, Vijg, & Kruglyak,

1998). Exceptional longevity can be defined as an exceptionally long life-span compared

to other individuals experiencing the same historical influences (birth cohort; Michael

Anson et al., 2012). As done in previous studies, we will define the exceptional

longevity (EL) phenotype as living to exceptional age, and explore differences in

heritability of survival to the 90th

and 95th

percentile based on the baseline hazard models

(Kerber, O'Brien, Boucher, Smith, & Cawthon, 2012).

Constructing Baseline Survival Models

We assume a parametric form for the survival distribution and a generalized class

of accelerated failure time (AFT) models, the extended family of generalized gamma

140

models. Unlike proportional hazard models, AFT models assume that the effect of

covariates is multiplicative with respect to survival time. We test the fit of the

exponential, Weibull , log-normal, log-logistic, and gamma models. These models were

selected because they provide a simple point estimate for duration that generally fits the

observed data for adult mortality. While the gompertz model is appropriate for modeling

human mortality between 30 and 85 (Olshansky & Carnes, 1997), this study is concerned

with exceptional longevity (past the age of 85) and therefore this model was not

considered. The nested structure of the family of generalized gamma models

(exponential, Weibull, log-logistic, and gamma) allows for use of the likelihood ratio test

to assess model fit. The Akaike information criterion, or AIC, can be used to test the fit

of nonnested models. Final models were selected for the construction of the longevity

measures based on model fit.

The full sample of 685,949 individuals born between 1850 and 1927 meeting the

sample criteria described in the data section above were used to estimate survival time for

individuals surviving to age 30. Models were stratified by gender and included two

covariates, birth year, and birth year squared. All models showed a significant positive

relationship between birth year and survival. The generalized gamma model proved to be

the best fit based on likelihood ratio test (p<0.001) and AIC. The shape and scale

parameters in the generalized gamma model are also significantly different than “0” and

“1,” implying that the fitted distribution is different from the Weibull, log-normal, and

exponential models. The change in the predicted 50th

and 90th

percentile by model and

year is displayed in Figure 4.5. Panels A and B show the trend in predicted median and

90th

percentile longevity respectively for men and panels C and D display the estimates

for women. The exponential model does not fit the data well and therefore the results are

141

not shown. These figures show that the log-normal and log-logistic models, which

provided the worst fit based on the AIC statistic, also predict out of range values for the

90th

percentile. Both the Weibull and generalized gamma model provide sensible

estimates. Therefore, the generalized gamma model was used to estimate ̂ and

consequently LS.

LS was defined as the observed, minus the expected, life-span for all deceased

individuals. The UPDB contains multiple sources of linked records that can be used to

create a last observed date. Therefore, we know that individuals without a death record

were alive until their last observed date in UPDB. The observed life-span for individuals

born after 1905, not deceased, and with a known follow-up date that exceeded the median

predicted survival time for their gender and birth cohort is calculated by subtracting the

birth year from the last observed date in UPBD. Therefore, censored individuals that are

likely still living were used in the LS analyses and have a positive LS score by definition.

To test for biased results created by this specification, we ran sensitivity analysis using

the nearly deceased cohort (N=14,618).

Heritability Estimates

Several forms of analysis of variance (ANOVA) are available to measure

heritability of a phenotypic trait, such as parent-offspring regressions and sibling

analyses. While these models have useful features, they are limited because they do not

use information from multigenerational relationships and they require that sample sizes

be well-balanced. Unlike other forms of analysis of variance (ANOVA), maximum

likelihood (ML) estimators do not place special demands on the design or balance of the

data, providing a powerful approach to estimating variance components using large

142

pedigrees (Lynch & Walsh, 1998) and minimizing the inflation of estimates of additive

genetic variance due to shared environments between relatives. To allow for use of

information on multigenerational relationships, heritability is estimated with a polygenic

model using PAP v. 7.1 (Hasstedt, 2005).

Genotypic variance can be decomposed into additive , dominance , and

epistatic . and are, however, extremely difficult to estimate in non-

experimental settings (Kruuk, 2004). The polygenic model specifies the expected genetic

relationship between relatives as a function of the coefficient of relationship, allowing for

the estimation of variation due to genetic and residual environmental effects. The

coefficient of relationship is (1/2)p, where p is the degree of relationships (it is also

commonly described as two times the probability that two individuals will share a

common gene by descent (IBD)). For example, for a parent-child relationship the

coefficient of relationship is 0.5, which equals 2 x 0.25, where 0.25 is the probability that

parent and child share a common allele. The polygenic model allows us to partition the

total phenotypic variance ( ) into the following components:

(eq. 4.1)

where is the additive genetic variance and

is the residual variance, which includes

environmental, dominance, and epistatic effects. These components are used to

calculate heritability, with narrow sense heritability (h2) being defined as the proportion

of phenotypic variance,

, that can be attributed to the additive genetic effects, :

h2=

/ (eq. 4.2)

The polygenic model is similar to a mixed model with fixed and random effects. The

general model in matrix form is:

(eq.4.3)

143

where y is a column vector containing the phenotypic values for a trait measured in n

individuals; β is a vector of fixed effects; u is a vector of random effects; X and Z are

known incidence matrices; and e is a column vector of random residual effects. We

assume that u follows a multivariate normal (MVN) distribution with mean zero and

variance G, and that e also follows a MVN distribution with mean zero and variance R.

Note that G = A, where A is an n x n matrix of kinship coefficients describing the

genetic correlation between all individuals in the sample, and R= I, where I is the

identity matrix. This general model can be used to estimate the variance components for

a single trait (univariate model) and has been extended to allow for joint modeling of

multiple traits (bivariate for two traits and multivariate for multiple traits). The univariate

model was used to estimate the heritability of LS and exceptional longevity in the

population. We then use the multivariate model to estimate h2 by environment.

The multivariate model provides a means for estimating covariance and,

therefore, correlation between traits. Falconer (1952) suggested that traits measured in

two environments can be treated as two different traits. This allows comparisons

between discrete environments of different types, where the bivariate model defines a

trait as being expressed in environment one or two. For example, if individual i is in

environment one, they have a value for trait one and are missing trait two. Conversely, if

individual j is in environment two, they are missing trait one and have a value for trait

two. This approach is more appropriate than stratification, because it allows for the joint

estimation of heritability in two subpopulations. It is also slightly different than a normal

bivariate trait model, which jointly models two phenotypes measured on the same

individual because no individual expresses a trait in both environments. In this approach,

144

k traits (in our case k=2) are combined to form a vector [

]= (y11, … , y1n, …, y2n)

with mean µz and variance G. The model in matrix notation is,

[

] [

] [

] [

] [

] (eq. 4.4)

where y1 and y2 are vectors of phenotypic values in environment one and two,

respectively; β1 and β2 are the vectors of the fixed effects in environment one and two,

respectively; a1 and a2 are the vectors of the random additive genetic effects in

environment one and two, respectively; e1 and e2 are the vectors of random residual

effects for environment one and two, respectively; X1 and X2 are the known incidence

matrices relating the observations to the respective fixed effects in environments one and

two; and W1 and W2 relate the observations to the random effects in environments one

and two.

The variance-covariance matrix for Z can be expressed as V = G + R

, where G is the Kronecker product of C and A . C is the k x k matrix of

additive genetic covariances, and E is the k x k residual covariance matrix. A and I are

respectively the n x n kinship coefficient and identity matrices, with cij=σA(i,j) being the

additive genetic covariance between characters i and j within an individual and cross-

covariance cijAlm being the additive genetic value of character i in individual l and the

additive genetic value of character j in individual m (Lynch & Walsh, 1998, p. 777). In a

bivariate analysis, C is a 2 x 2 matrix of the form:

[

] (eq. 4.5)

where and

are the additive genetic variances for traits 1 and 2, respectively,

and is the additive genetic cross covariance.

145

Defining the environment at the individual level and estimating heritability using

the multivariate model without defining the genetic correlation between traits leads to

biased estimates of heritability because heritability estimates from an environment only

include information about family members in the same environment. To correct for this

problem, we assume perfect genetic correlation between the trait values. A bivariate

analysis that explicitly models genetic correlations exploits more information content of

the data (Amos, de Andrade, & Zhu, 2001). The genetic correlation between traits can be

defined as:

(eq. 4.6)

where and

are all components of variance mentioned above and

. By constraining to 1, we are requiring the covariance

between traits to equal the square-root of the product of the variances and forcing the

model to include information from both environments. Constraining the genetic

correlation to unity allows for heritability and additive genetic variance to vary in both

environments, but requires them to be dependent. In a bivariate trait analysis, where both

phenotypes are measured for an individual, the genetic correlation is often estimated and

used to describe the pleiotropic nature of the traits. However, estimating the genetic

correlation across environments would be erroneous in our situation because when

, we are only using partial information from the pedigree because the covariance is

weighted by the correlation coefficient ( ). Algebraically, this solves the bias

problem because it forces the measure of additive genetic variance for each environment

to include information about family members from both environments. It is also

146

conceptually plausible because a genetic correlation of 1 indicates the effect of the same

polygenes on the trait in both environments.

LS was Box-Cox transformed and standardized (µ=0, σ=1) to improve

computational performance and abide by distributional assumptions of the variance

components models. The transformation was performed using Proc transreg in SAS,

which uses a maximum likelihood approach to find the optimal transformation, which in

this cases was λ=1.75. This transformation reduced the skewness coefficient from -0.85

to -0.26. The simple correlation between the transformed variable and the original

measure of LS was 0.98.

To test the hypothesis of heterogeneity in heritability, the likelihood ratio statistic

was used. Models were estimated, allowing heterogeneity in heritability estimates

between environments, and compared to models where the heritability estimates were

constrained to be equal across environments. Sex and birth year were not considered as

covariates in the model because they were controlled for when creating the measures of

longevity.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Figure 4.6 shows the distribution of LS for the baseline survival cohort and the

sample selected for the heritability estimates. Both distributions are slightly skewed with

means of -1.2 and -1.7 for the full cohort and the heritability cohort, respectively. The

skewed distribution reflects the change in the rate of mortality between ages 30 and the

median predicted survival time for an individual’s sex and cohort. Cohort life tables for

Utah show that the qx for females at age 30 in the 1900 birth cohort is 0.02, compared to

147

0.05 at age 60 and 0.29 at age 80 (Lindahl-Jacobsen et al., 2013). Therefore, it is not

unexpected to see the long left tail in the LS distribution. The distributional skew is due

to a combination of factors including model fit (the fit provides a good approximation of

the survival curve, but does not fit the data exactly) and censoring of the youngest cohort.

Table 4.2 shows the descriptive statistics for individuals in the heritability

samples. The longevity sample includes all 20,120 individuals with calculated longevity,

LS, from the 802 selected pedigrees. Individuals in the exceptional longevity sample

were required to be born before 1914 so that we could observe survival in the UPDB to

age 99. Approximately 8% of males and females in this sample survive to the 90th

percentile for their cohort and sex. This number is slightly smaller than 10% because the

cut point for the 90th

percentile is derived from the baseline survival models. Forty-eight

percent of the sample is female and approximately three-fourths of the sample was

affiliated with the LDS church. All members of a family with a sibling that died during

infancy or childhood are counted as having an infant death in their family of origin and in

historical cohorts. Children from large families experience excess infant and childhood

mortality rates (Bean, Mineau, & Anderton, 1990; Knodel & Hermalin, 1984), so this

percentage is slightly higher than the 17.4% and 18.5% percent of nuclear families with

an infant or childhood death, respectively. There is not a substantial amount of overlap in

these measures, with 6.4% of nuclear families having both an infant and childhood death.

Figure 4.7 shows the effect of environment on LS without considering family

structure. Significant differences in LS exist in all environments. Panel A shows the

distribution of LS by religious status, with individuals not affiliated with the LDS church

on average having a 2 point reduction in longevity score (p<0.001). The distribution of

LS by infant and childhood mortality in family of origin is displayed in panels B and C,

148

respectively, with individuals having one or more sibling die during the postneonatal

period having a 2 point reduction in LS (p<0.001), and individuals with one or more

sibling deaths during childhood having a 1.5 reduction in LS (p<0.001). Panel D shows

the distribution of LS by sibship size and illustrates the nearly 2-point reduction of LS for

individuals having seven or more siblings.

Heritability Estimates

The overall heritability of LS in the sample is 0.18, which is within the range of

previously reported estimates. We find that in the four environments considered when

not conditioned on sex, the mean LS is lower in unhealthy environments but there are no

significant differences in h2. The pattern of heritability of LS by environment is

somewhat mixed, with higher heritability of LS in environments with low IMR and

CMR, but lower heritability of LS in the other two healthy environments. It is important

to note that heritability is a population statistic, thus we are comparing subpopulations

defined by an environment and not average individual differences in phenotype. The

addition of environment-specific means and variances significantly improve model fit for

all environments, with lower means and environmental variances in environments that are

considered beneficial to longevity.

To further investigate sex differences in heritability and GxE interactions, we

considered models separately by sex. In a bivariate model, considering only sex

differences, we find that heritability of LS is significantly lower for females compared to

males, h2

LSf= 0.14 and h2

LSm=0.22 (LR χ2= 9.03, p=0.003), and there is little difference

between the mean and environmental variances by gender. The lack of difference in the

mean LS is by design. LS was constructed as a gender specific measure (i.e., the baseline

149

survival models were stratified by sex), and therefore one would not expect to see gender

differences in the average LS.

Multivariate models were used to calculate the heritability estimates for LS by

environment and sex (results in Table 4.3). When considering the differences in

heritability of LS by sex and environment, the mean differences in LS by environment

are similar, with lower mean LS in environments considered unhealthy. We find no

significant differences in the heritability of LS by environment with the exception of

female environments classified by CMR, which show a 9 point difference in h2

LS between

the healthful and unhealthful environments. The heritability of LS is lower in female

environments with high CMR when compared to female environments with low sibling

CMR (LR χ2=5.88, p=0.015). This is in contrast to the higher heritability of LS clustered

about a lower mean LS in the male environment with high CMR compared to an

environment with low CMR, although these differences are not significant. For females,

there is little difference in total phenotypic variance between the two CMR

subpopulations ( is approximately 1.30 and 1.29 in high CMR and low CMR

subpopulations, respectively). This is supportive of the enhancement hypothesis, which

suggests that individuals are unable to realize there genetic potential in adverse

environments.

Sensitivity analyses using the nearly deceased cohort (n=14,618) were run for the

LS models. We found that heritability estimates were slightly smaller (0.17 vs. 0.18 in

the larger sample), but the observed differences by gender and environment were all in

the same direction. The differences in heritability by CMR environment remained

significant.

150

We considered defining EL as survival to the 90th

or 95th

percentile conditioned

on birth year and sex. The sample for these analyses is smaller than the sample used to

obtain estimates of heritability of LS because observing EL requires a nearly extinct

cohort (NLS=20,120, NEL= 14,618). Heritability estimates for the two phenotypes were

very similar, with h2

EL=0.352 when EL is defined as survival to the 90th

percentile

(shown in Table 3.4), and h2

EL=0.345 (95% CI= 0.244, 0.447) when EL is defined as

survival to the 95th

percentile (results not shown). The small decline in heritability

between the 90th

and 95th

percentiles suggests that heritability does not increase linearly

with age, and that perhaps there is an upper limit to increases in heritability of longevity.

However, the differences are negligible and not relevant to the main hypotheses of this

paper. Therefore, we show results for survival to the 90th

percentile conditioned on age

and sex.

Table 4.4 shows the heritability estimates for EL by environment and gender. We

find that heritability for EL is nearly twice the heritability of LS (0.18 vs. 0.35).

Bivariate models were used to test for environmental differences in the heritability of EL.

We find that allowing the prevalence to vary by environment significantly improves

model fit, with higher prevalence of EL in healthful environments. We find no difference

in heritability of EL by environment when not conditioned on gender. There are also no

gender differences in the heritability of EL (LR χ2= 0.552, p=0.46), which differs from

the LS findings.

When using the multivariate model to test for environmental differences in

heritability of EL by gender and environment, we do not find evidence of significant

differences with the exception of the male CMR environment. The heritability of EL is

31 points higher in male environments with high CMR compared to male environments

151

with low CMR (LR χ2=4.25, p=0.04), and there is no difference in the prevalence of

exceptional longevity between environments. This suggests that a triggering GxE

interaction may be operating through selection mechanisms, where the frail are selected

out of the adverse populations at faster rates and only the genetically robust individuals

with longevity assurance genes that are able to thwart the effects of gerontogenes survive

to exceptional ages.

Discussion

Our analysis of longevity is based on information from 20,120 individuals from

802 three-generation families used to examine the heritability of longevity, defined as

survival after age 30. We also estimated the heritability of exceptional longevity using

information from a subset of that sample (n=14,618 ) that is nearly extinct. Our findings

support previous studies suggesting a moderate heritable component to longevity that

increases with age (Herskind et al., 1996; Hjelmborg et al., 2006; Kerber et al., 2001),

although the adult ages at which this assessment is made varies across analyses. We find

little difference between the heritability of survival to the 90th

and 95th

percentiles,

suggesting that the increase in proportion of variance due to genetic factors may not be a

constant linear increase as suggested by other studies (Hjelmborg et al., 2006). We find

that sex differences in the heritability of longevity after age 30 support other studies

showing higher heritability of longevity for males (Herskind et al., 1996), but no sex

differences in the heritability of exceptional longevity. We investigated the heterogeneity

of longevity and exceptional longevity by early and midlife social environments. We

find some evidence that the heritability of longevity varies between environments, but

152

overall there is not strong support of a gene-environment interaction for the selected

environments.

We find evidence that childhood environments marked by high child mortality,

indicative of exposure to infectious disease and undernutrition for the surviving members,

may affect the proportion of phenotypic variation attributable to genetic factors. The sex

and age differences of the effects suggest an enhancement GxE interaction because

adverse childhood circumstances limit the genetic potential of individuals to survive to

older ages. Conceptually, CMR is used to identify environments with excess exposure to

infectious disease and undernutrition. For females, genetic factors contribute little to the

total variance in longevity in such environments, which suggests that genetic potential is

not reached in such environments. While a similar pattern exists for EL, the difference in

heritability between environments is not significant.

We see the opposite effect for male environments, although the observed patterns

do not necessarily conflict with the female results. Males have a mortality disadvantage

relative to females throughout the life course that is partially due to biological factors

(Kraemer, 2000). Therefore, they may be more susceptible to environmental conditions

that trigger genetic predispositions for disease and lead to higher mortality selection

compared to females reared in the same environment. The difference in the direction of

the effect for males suggests that the adverse environment may actually trigger genetic

diatheses, with higher heritability clustered about a lower mean longevity in deleterious

environments, but these differences are not significant. This results in higher heritability

of exceptional longevity because individuals surviving to this age have some

predisposition or genetic robustness that prevented them from being selected out of the

population at earlier ages.

153

It is interesting that we find heterogeneity in CMR environments, but not in

environments characterized by IMR. This may be partially due to differences in specific

causes of death for the two groups, as suggested by Wolleswinkel-van den Bosch et al.

(2000). We did consider variations of our definition of IMR, which included neonatal

deaths, although this did not change the substantive conclusion that heritability of

longevity does not vary between subpopulations with different rates of infant mortality.

While we find some evidence of heterogeneity in the heritability of longevity

between environments, heritability estimates seem to be fairly impervious to early and

midlife circumstances. Herskind et al. (1996) reported stability of heritability estimates

over sex and cohort during periods of rapid change in living conditions. However, the

birth cohorts selected for that study would still be children during periods with higher

childhood mortality (1870 – 1900) than experienced during modern times. Our results

suggest that improvements in social and health conditions that have caused declines in

childhood mortality may lead to a higher proportion of variability in longevity

attributable to genetic factors. More research needs to be done to test for other

environmental differences in the heritability of longevity, including socioeconomic status

and fertility history.

The nearly twofold increase in heritability of exceptional longevity compared to

the heritability of longevity after age 30 suggests that selection mechanisms may affect

the heritability of longevity throughout the life course. Individuals without longevity

assurance genes may be selected out of the population at early ages, leaving a subset of

the population that is made up of a higher proportion of robust individuals. While the

heritability of longevity increases with age, exceptional longevity is still only moderately

heritable, and the environment explains the largest amount of phenotypic variation. It is

154

also remarkable that there are gender differences in heritability of longevity after age 30,

but not with respect to exceptional survival. This suggests that individuals surviving to

exceptional ages have survived mortality selection because they have a genetic variant

that increases the ability to handle stress and/or counteract deleterious effects of the

environment or generontogenes. This is further supported by other research suggesting

the buffering role of longevity genes (Bergman, Atzmon, Ye, MacCarthy, & Barzilai,

2007; Sebastiani et al., 2012).

Epigenetics is one of several possible biological mechanisms that allow social

circumstances to get “under the skin,” and it recently has been suggested that epigenetic

changes have the propensity to persist across subsequent generations (Feinberg, 2007).

This is a provocative idea that lends support to mutligenerational transmission of social

disparities. More research needs to be done to uncover the possible mechanisms leading

to phenotypic variation across social environments and the possibility of transmitting the

adverse effects to subsequent generations. We suggest further study into the possibility

of GxE interactions and health and longevity outcomes. While we did not find an

association between all environments, there is a suggestion that the social environment

may play an important role in modifying the heritability of longevity.

In this paper, we assessed variation in heritability estimates of longevity after age

30. However, other cutoffs, such as postreproductive aging, should also be considered.

Further modeling of heterogeneity of variance and the variance of longevity across other

environments could be valuable in understanding how the social environment moderates

the genetic component to aging. Care should be taken when interpreting polygenic

heritability when the genetic correlation has been fixed to unity, because it is assumed

that the same genes affect longevity across environments. While we feel this is a valid

155

assumption for subgroups of a single population, the reader should be aware of this

constraint.

To our knowledge, this is the first study using multigenerational pedigree

information to investigate heterogeneity in heritability of longevity across multiple early

and midlife environments. Studies in other fields have examined heterogeneity in

variance components by gender and age using a similar method (Giolo, Pereira, de

Andrade, Krieger, & Soler, 2010; Pilia et al., 2006), lending validity to this approach.

.

156

References

Adams, J. (1990). Convergent issues in genetics and demography. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Amos, C. I., de Andrade, M., & Zhu, D. K. (2001). Comparison of multivariate tests for

genetic linkage. Human Heredity, 51(3), 133-144.

Atzmon, G., Schechter, C., Greiner, W., Davidson, D., Rennert, G., & Barzilai, N.

(2004). Clinical phenotype of families with longevity. Journal of the American

Geriatrics Society, 52(2), 274-277. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52068.x

Barker, D. J., (1995). Fetal origins of coronary heart disease. BMJ, 311(6998), 171-174.

Barker, D. J., Godfrey, K. M., Gluckman, P. D., Harding, J. E., Owens, J. A., &

Robinson, J. S. (1993). Fetal nutrition and cardiovascular disease in adult life. The

Lancet, 341(8850), 938-941. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)91224-a

Bean, L. L., Mineau, G. P., & Anderton, D. L. (1990). Fertility change on the American

frontier: Adaptation and innovation (Vol. 4). Berkely and Los Angeles: Univ of

California Press.

Bergman, A., Atzmon, G., Ye, K., MacCarthy, T., & Barzilai, N. (2007). Buffering

mechanisms in aging: A systems approach toward uncovering the genetic

component of aging. PLoS Comput Biol, 3(8), e170. doi:

10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030170

Boardman, J. D. (2009). State-level moderation of genetic tendencies to smoke. American

Journal of Public Health, 99(3), 480-486. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.134932

Boardman, J. D., Roettger, M. E., Domingue, B. W., McQueen, M. B., Haberstick, B. C.,

& Harris, K. M. (2012). Gene–environment interactions related to body mass:

School policies and social context as environmental moderators. Journal of

Theoretical Politics, 24(3), 370-388. doi: 10.1177/0951629812437751

Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., & Vaupel, J. W. (2006). The quest for genetic

determinants of human longevity: Challenges and insights. Nat Rev Genet, 7(6),

436-448.

Crimmins, E. M., & Finch, C. E. (2006). Infection, inflammation, height, and longevity.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,

103(2), 498-503. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501470103

Crimmins, E. M., & Saito, Y. (2001). Trends in healthy life expectancy in the United

States, 1970-1990: Gender, racial, and educational differences. Social Science and

Medicine, 52(11), 1629-1642.

157

Downey, D. B. (1995). When bigger is not better: Family size, parental resources, and

children's educational performance. American Sociological Review, 60(5), 746-

761. doi: 10.2307/2096320

Elo, I. T., & Preston, S. H. (1992). Effects of early-life conditions on adult mortality: A

review. Population Index, 58(2), 186-212. doi: 10.2307/3644718

Enstrom, J. E., & Breslow, L. (2008). Lifestyle and reduced mortality among active

California Mormons, 1980–2004. Preventive Medicine, 46(2), 133-136. doi:

10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.07.030

Falconer, D. (1952). The problem of environment and selection. American Naturalist,

293-298.

Feinberg, A. P. (2007). Phenotypic plasticity and the epigenetics of human disease.

Nature, 447(7143), 433-440.

Finch, B. K. (2003). Early origins of the gradient: The relationship between

socioeconomic status and infant mortality in the United States. Demography,

40(4), 675-699. doi: 10.2307/1515203

Finch, C. E., & Tanzi, R. E. (1997). Genetics of aging. Science, 278(5337), 407-411.

Giolo, S., Pereira, A., de Andrade, M., Krieger, J., & Soler, J. (2010). Evaluating gene by

sex and age interactions on cardiovascular risk factors in Brazilian families. BMC

Medical Genetics, 11(1), 132.

Gudmundsson, H., Gudbjartsson, D. F., Frigge, M., Gulcher, J. R., & Stefánsson, K.

(2000). Inheritance of human longevity in Iceland. European Journal of Human

Genetics : EJHG, 8(10), 743-749.

Hart, C. L., & Davey Smith, G. (2003). Relation between number of siblings and adult

mortality and stroke risk: 25 year follow up of men in the Collaborative study.

Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57(5), 385-391. doi:

10.1136/jech.57.5.385

Hasstedt, S. (2005). jPAP: document-driven software for genetic analysis. Genet

Epidemiol, 29(3), 255.

Hawkes, K., Smith, K. R., & Blevins, J. K. (2012). Human actuarial aging increases

faster when background death rates are lower: a consequence of differential

heterogeneity? Evolution, 66(1), s103-114.

Herskind, A., McGue, M., Holm, N., Sørensen, T., Harvald, B., & Vaupel, J. (1996). The

heritability of human longevity: A population-based study of 2872 Danish twin

pairs born 1870–1900. Human Genetics, 97(3), 319-323. doi:

10.1007/bf02185763

158

Hjelmborg, J. B., Iachine, I., Skytthe, A., Vaupel, J. W., McGue, M., Koskenvuo, M.,

Christensen, K. (2006). Genetic influence on human lifespan and longevity.

Human Genetics, 119(3), 312-321.

Hoffmann, A. A., & Merilä, J. (1999). Heritable variation and evolution under favourable

and unfavourable conditions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 14(3), 96-101. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01595-5

Hummer, R., Rogers, R., Nam, C., & Ellison, C. (1999). Religious involvement and U.S.

adult mortality. Demography, 36(2), 273-285. doi: 10.2307/2648114

Kerber, R. A., O'Brien, E., Boucher, K. M., Smith, K. R., & Cawthon, R. M. (2012). A

genome-wide study replicates linkage of 3p22-24 to extreme longevity in humans

and identifies possible additional loci. PLoS ONE, 7(4), e34746. doi:

10.1371/journal.pone.0034746

Kerber, R. A., O'Brien, E., Smith, K. R., & Cawthon, R. M. (2001). Familial excess

longevity in Utah genealogies. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological

Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56(3), B130-B139. doi:

10.1093/gerona/56.3.B130

Knodel, J., & Hermalin, A. I. (1984). Effects of birth rank, maternal age, birth interval,

and sibship size on infant and child mortality: Evidence from 18th and 19th

century reproductive histories. American Journal of Public Health, 74(10), 1098-

1106. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.74.10.1098

Kraemer, S. (2000). The fragile male. Clinical Medicine NetPrints, 1.

Kruuk, L. E. B. (2004). Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the

‘animal model’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1446), 873-890. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1437

Lindahl-Jacobsen, R., Hanson, H. A., Oksuzyan, A., Mineau, G. P., Christensen, K., &

Smith, K. R. (2013). The male–female health-survival paradox and sex

differences in cohort life expectancy in Utah, Denmark, and Sweden 1850–1910.

Annals of Epidemiology, 23(4), 161-166. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2013.02.001

Lynch, M., & Walsh, B. (1998). Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sunderlan:

Sinauer.

McKeown, T. (1976). The modern rise of population (Vol. 11). London: Edward Arnold.

Mineau, G. P., Smith, K. R., & Bean, L. L. (2002). Historical trends of survival among

widows and widowers. Social Science & Medicine, 54(2), 245-254. doi:

10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00024-7

159

Montesanto, A., Dato, S., Bellizzi, D., Rose, G., & Passarino, G. (2012).

Epidemiological, genetic and epigenetic aspects of the research on healthy ageing

and longevity. Immun Ageing, 9(1), 6.

Olshansky, S. J., Carnes, B. A., & Brody, J. (2002). A biodemographic interpretation of

life span. Population and Development Review, 28(3), 501-513.

Perls, T., Kunkel, L. M., & Puca, A. A. (2002). The genetics of exceptional human

longevity. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50(2), 359-368. doi:

10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.49283.x

Perls, T. T., Bubrick, E., Wager, C. G., Vijg, J., & Kruglyak, L. (1998). Siblings of

centenarians live longer. The Lancet, 351(9115), 1560.

Petronis, A. (2010). Epigenetics as a unifying principle in the aetiology of complex traits

and diseases. Nature, 465(7299), 721-727.

Pilia, G., Chen, W.-M., Scuteri, A., Orrú, M., Albai, G., Dei, M., Lai, S., et. al. (2006).

Heritability of cardiovascular and personality traits in 6,148 Sardinians. PLoS

Genet, 2(8), e132. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020132

Preston, S. H., & Haines, M. R. (1991). Fatal years: Child mortality in late nineteenth-

century America. Princeton: National Bureau of Economic Research, Inc.

Rieker, P. P., Bird, C., & Lang, M. (2010). Understanding gender and health: Old

patterns, new trends and future directions. Handbook of medical sociology.

Nashville: Vanderbilt University Press.

Rowe, D. C., Almeida, D. M., & Jacobson, K. C. (1999). School context and genetic

influences on aggression in adolescence. Psychological Science, 10(3), 277-280.

doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00150

Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Gerontologist, 37(4), 433-440.

doi: 10.1093/geront/37.4.433

Sebastiani, P., Solovieff, N., DeWan, A. T., Walsh, K. M., Puca, A., Hartley, S. W.,

Melista, E., et. al. (2012). Genetic signatures of exceptional longevity in humans.

PLoS ONE, 7(1), e29848.

Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment interactions:

Retrospect and prospect. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological

Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(Special Issue 1), 65-76. doi:

10.1093/geronb/60.Special_Issue_1.65

Shostak, S., & Freese, J. (2010). Gene-environment interaction and medical sociology.

Handbook of Medical Sociology. Nashville: Vanderbilt University Press, 418-434.

160

Siegel, J. S. (2011). The demography and epidemiology of human health and aging. New

York: Springer.

Smith, K. R., Mineau, G. P., Garibotti, G., & Kerber, R. (2009). Effects of childhood and

middle-adulthood family conditions on later-life mortality: Evidence from the

Utah Population Database, 1850–2002. Social Science & Medicine, 68(9), 1649-

1658. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.010

Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003).

Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological

Science, 14(6), 623-628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x

Vasunilashorn, S., & Crimmins, E. (2008). Biodemography: Integrating disciplines to

explain aging. Handbook of theories of aging. New York: Springer Publishing

Company, 63-85.

Vaupel, J. W., Carey, J. R., Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., Yashin, A. I., Holm, N. V.,

Iachine, I. A., et. al. (1998). Biodemographic trajectories of longevity. Science,

280(5365), 855-860. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5365.855

Visscher, P. M., Hill, W. G., & Wray, N. R. (2008). Heritability in the genomics era—

Concepts and misconceptions. Nat Rev Genet, 9(4), 255-266.

Wolleswinkel-van den Bosch, J. H., van Poppel, F. W., Looman, C. W., & Mackenbach,

J. P. (2000). Determinants of infant and early childhood mortality levels and their

decline in The Netherlands in the late nineteenth century. International Journal of

Epidemiology, 29(6), 1031-1040. doi: 10.1093/ije/29.6.1031

Yashin, A., & Iachine, I. (1997). How frailty models can be used for evaluating longevity

limits: Taking advantage of an interdisciplinary approach. Demography, 34(1),

31-48. doi: 10.2307/2061658

161

Table 4.1. Pedigree Selection

3 Generation Families from

1850 - 1927 Cohort* 111,324

Exclusions

Families missing information

on at least one grandparent

(These people have

placeholder genealogy

records)

31,322

At least one of the

grandparents is born before

1850

52,780

A member of G3 born after

1927 26,420

Total Number of 3-

Generation Families for

Analysis 802

*This is calculated by taking any member of the BC and ascending 3

generations. The result is 111,324 distinct treetops (defined by the unique

combination of maternal and paternal grandparents). Families that did not

meet our selection criteria were then excluded.

128

Table 4.2. Descriptive Statistics for Individuals from 802 Utah families

Longevity

(N=20,120)

Exceptional Longevity

(N=14,618)

Male

(N=10,393)

Female

(N=9,727)

Male

(N=7,532)

Female

(N=7,086)

Birth Year 1897 (21.1)

1897

(20.3)

1889

(18.8)

1889

(18.0)

Longevity Score (LS) -1.7 (15.2) -1.9 (15.5)

-2.3 (15.4) -2.7 (16.5)

Survived to the 90th Percentile (EL)

8.4% 8.0%

Survived to the 95th Percentile

4.2% 3.9%

Baptized Latter-Day Saint 72.5% 74.4%

75.6% 77.7%

One or more Postneonatal Infant

Deaths in Family of Origin 19.5% 19.7%

23.9% 23.1%

One or more Childhood Deaths in

Family of Origin 19.5% 19.9%

23.1% 24.2%

Large number of Siblings 29.6% 29.2% 35.6% 35.6%

162

129

Table 4.3. Summary of the Results Obtained for Polygenic Models of LS (N=20,120)

163

128

Table 4.4. Summary of the Results Obtained for Polygenic Models of EL

164

165

Figure 4.1. Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Expected. The expected phenotypic

variation in a normal environment.

166

Figure 4.2. Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Triggering. A triggering GxE interaction in

an adverse environment.

167

Figure 4.3. Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Compensation. A compensation GxE

interaction in an enriched environment.

168

Figure 4.4. Hypotheses for GxE interactions: Enhancement. An enhancement interaction

in an enriched environment.

169

Figure 4.5. Predicted values of survival to the 50th and 90th percentiles by gender and

birth year. Panels A and B show the estimates for male 50th

and 90th

percentile estimates,

respectively. Panels C and D show estimates for female 50th

and

90th

percentiles, respectively.

170

Figure 4.6. Distribution of calculated longevity for individuals born between 1850 and

1927 and surviving to age 30. Panel A shows the distribution for the cohort used in the

baseline survival analysis (N=685,949). Panel B shows the distribution for the

heritability sample (N=20,120).

171

Figure 4.7. Distribution of longevity by environment. Empirical densities of longevity

are plotted by environment. Panel A shows the distribution of longevity by LDS status.

Panel B shows the distribution of longevity by infant mortality in family of origin. Panel

C shows the distribution of longevity by childhood mortality in family of origin. Panel D

shows the distribution of longevity by number of siblings.

CHAPTER 5

CONCLUSION

This dissertation investigated heterogeneity in patterns of aging and the factors

throughout the life course that shape them. By focusing on variability within the

population we are able to provide a clearer picture of how circumstances throughout the

life course affect the way individuals age. We found that the paths to disease and

longevity are diverse and that early and midlife factors play an important role in

determining later life health and longevity. Our results support a wide body of literature

showing that morbidity is not an inevitable consequence of aging, even in the oldest old

population, but is shaped by the historical circumstances and social environments that we

live in. This study offered innovative and significant contributions to the understanding

of biological and socioenvironmental determinants of aging. Our research sought to

disentangle the biological and temporal sources of trends in cancer incidence,

investigating the possible social and physiological effects of fertility history on

comorbidity trajectories after age 65, and studying the heterogeneity in the heritable

components of the total phenotypic variation in longevity across early life family and

social environments.

173

Fully understanding the sources of heterogeneity in the patterns of aging and

longevity using only measures of the early life or proximate environment is an impossible

endeavor because the biological functioning of an individual is dependent upon a vast

array of circumstances from birth to death. Healthy aging and longevity phenotypes

should be characterized as plastic and not one fixed at or near the time of birth. Trends in

cancer incidence in the oldest old are sensitive to period and cohort influences. Fertility

history affects disease progression later in life and these effects are independent of early

life circumstances, including a family history of longevity. These findings suggest that

childhood is not the only malleable period in the life course; midlife circumstances may

also alter the trajectories of age related degeneration. The moderate heritability of

exceptional longevity is evidence that genes are not the only factor contributing to this

phenotype. There is also some evidence that heritability of longevity is sensitive to

childhood environments. Therefore, it is essential to consider how events throughout the

life course and their interrelationships influence aging and longevity.

Little is known about age-specific disease incidence and prevalence among the

oldest old (Boscoe, 2008; Christensen, Johnson, & Vaupel, 2006). This study highlighted

the heterogeneity of disease patterns in this population by analyzing age, period, and

cohort (APC) effects on cancer incidence in the oldest old and individual trajectories of

disease for two cohorts over an 18 year period. We found significant evidence of

variance in disease patterns for this population and evidence that social context and

events throughout the life course influence patterns of disease even for the exceptionally

long lived. The APC analyses provided evidence that the decline in cancer incidence for

this age group is not strictly related to biological phenomenon. Characterizing

trajectories of disease up to age 91 in the young-old cohort and 101 in the old-old cohort

174

provides evidence that even into these advance ages the patterns of disease are diverse.

We found that there are distinct heterogeneous patterns of comorbidity that range from a

robust group, escaping major morbid conditions for the majority of the observation

period, to a frail group characterized by high comorbidity throughout the entire period of

observation. These findings underscore the importance of more rigorous and

interdisciplinary research into the biological and social underpinnings of disease for this

rapidly increasing segment of the population.

It is important to consider how mortality selection shapes age-related patterns of

disease. While it is clear that longevity is only moderately heritable at extreme ages,

heritability also increases with age. This suggests that there may be genetic variants that

are protective against deleterious genetic and environmental effects. The buffering

mechanisms in aging hypothesis suggests that longevity genes buffer against the harmful

effects of deleterious genotypes (Huffman et al., 2012). The decline in cancer incidence

above age 90 also suggests that individuals reaching the extreme ends of longevity may

be less susceptible to disease. However, the diverse patterns of comorbidity experience

provide evidence that longevity is not synonymous with disease free living.

Future Research

Sociological and demographic studies of aging and longevity should inform and

be informed by the fields of genetics, epidemiology, and biology. More attention should

be given to uncovering genetic and biological pathways to health and their interaction

with the social environment. The integration of theories of aging from multiple

disciplines is essential to unraveling the secrets of this multifactorial process. It is

difficult to identify the mechanisms that separate the exceptionally longevous, salubrious

175

individuals from those with multiple morbidities and a shortened life-span; however, it is

clear that these differences cannot be wholly explained by biological or social

mechanisms. Therefore, future demographic work should continue to improve upon the

specification of biological and social paths to health outcomes.

The studies presented in this dissertation utilized a range of tools to describe

aging and longevity trends in the populations. Each of these tools has unique aspects that

can be leveraged to further advance the field of aging and longevity. Age, period, and

cohort (APC) analyses can be considered descriptive. It is not possible to make causal

statement as to what factors led to the observed trends. However, this is not sufficient

reason for demographers to abandon APC analyses. It is the ability to describe the

multidimensionality of morbidity and mortality trends that give these analyses so much

power and point to domains that may hold some of the answers to fundamental questions

about the origins of longevity.

Making definitive statements about age-related trends using cross-sectional data

or longitudinal data from a single cohort is a dangerous practice. The observed trends in

cross-sectional data may be confounded by cohort differences in exposure, while

longitudinal data from a small number of cohorts are not generalizable because the

observed patterns may be specific to these cohorts. Therefore, we advocate a

decomposition approach to understanding the underlying factors of disease by first

defining age, period, and cohort effects of morbidity and cause-specific mortality. This

first step does not provide a complete explanation for the trends, but it helps to elucidate

what needs to be explained. The second step then involves further investigating the

components and interrelationships in order to make more definitive causal statements

about the disease process. Investigating population trends using this strategy can shed

176

considerable light on the ways that social environments intersect with biological factors

determining disease.

The use of group-based trajectory modeling to construct heterogeneous patterns of

aging is extremely informative to the studies of aging and longevity. It has become clear

that there is not a linear pattern of decline in physiological function determined by

chronological age at the population or individual level. Assessing unique patterns of

disease and disability experience give us a more realistic picture of how people age. We

also have much to learn about which experiences throughout the life course contribute to

specific patterns of aging. Other sources of early life information, such as linked

Decennial Censuses and military service records, will be available as part of the UPDB

infrastructure in the near future. This will allow for further investigation into the

association between early and midlife events and later life morbidity. We also suggest

that study is warranted for investigating specific diseases or groups of disease (rather than

composite measures only) with similar biological underpinnings to further understand the

association between fertility and later life health.

Exploring gene-environment (GxE) interactions using a multigenerational

database with information on early and midlife conditions is a fruitful approach to

understanding how the social environment affects later life health. Not only can the

study presented here be expanded to include other environments, but it can also be

expanded to examine variation in heritability of other aging phenotypes. For example,

we identified a group of robust individuals that experienced low levels of disease over an

18 year period in the trajectory analysis. It would be interesting to see if there is evidence

of heritability of a “robust” phenotype. Other ways of investigating GxE interactions

should also be explored. There is potential for using biomarker data, including APO-E

177

and telomere length, to investigate the possibility of environmentally altered phenotypic

expression.

Population projections that assume future gains in healthy life expectancy and life

expectancy in general will remain on a fixed path should be viewed with caution because

life expectancy is sensitive to both historical and current sociological context. This is not

to say that future improvements in healthy life expectancy and life expectancy in general

are impossible, but they are dependent upon factors that are still not well understood.

Also, much more research needs to be conducted in order to understand disease

trajectories specific to the oldest old population. We cannot plan for proper care of this

rapidly increasing population when we know so little about their healthcare needs.

Conclusions

This research contributes to a growing body of literature that draws attention to

effects of early life circumstances on later life health. Health policies should be aimed at

promoting the well-being of individuals throughout the life course. This research

specifically highlights the importance of maternal well-being during childbearing years.

The future health of women is not only affected while they are in their reproductive

years, but it has been shown to affect the health of her offspring (Gluckman & Hanson,

2005). Therefore special attention should be given to this sensitive period that may alter

the health of multiple generations. This research also highlights the importance of social

context throughout the life course in determining later life health. Health risks are

created and maintained by social structures and more work must be done to understand

the social disparities that lead to disparities in health later in life and possibly across

generations. As a more individuals advance into the oldest old ages, we will need to

178

review health and cancer screening recommendations of the past. The view that this age

group is too frail or has too many comorbid conditions should be reconsidered (Østbye,

Greenberg, Taylor, & Lee, 2003) based on trends in cancer incidence for this population.

Understanding the sources of variation in patterns of aging is important for

creating accurate population predictions, identifying at risk populations that may benefit

from public health interventions, and characterizing the process of aging in a diverse

population. Rather than focusing on the average life expectancy or healthy life

expectancy of the population and their trends over time, we should be focusing on the

variability of these measures within a population and changes in the sources of variation

over time. This is a subtle but important difference. By elucidating mechanisms that

lead to heterogeneous patterns of aging, we can not only gain more insight into the

determinants of aging, but focus on the factors that have the largest impact. Health

policy should be focused on not only curing ailments once they present themselves, but

more importantly, preventing them throughout the life course.

179

References

Boscoe, F. P. (2008). Subdividing the age group of 85 years and older to improve US

disease reporting. American Journal of Public Health, 98(7), 1167-1170. doi:

10.2105/ajph.2008.133900

Christensen, K., Johnson, T. E., & Vaupel, J. W. (2006). The quest for genetic

determinants of human longevity: Challenges and insights. Nat Rev Genet, 7(6),

436-448.

Gluckman, P. D., & Hanson, M. (2005). The fetal matrix: Evolution, development, and

disease. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press.

Huffman, D. M., Deelen, J., Ye, K., Bergman, A., Slagboom, E. P., Barzilai, N., &

Atzmon, G. (2012). Distinguishing between longevity and buffered-deleterious

genotypes for exceptional human longevity: The case of the MTP gene. The

Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences,

67(11), 1153-1160. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls103

Østbye, T., Greenberg, G. N., Taylor, D. H., & Lee, A. M. M. (2003). Screening

mammography and pap tests among older American women 1996–2000: Results

from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and Asset and Health Dynamics

Among the Oldest Old (AHEAD). The Annals of Family Medicine, 1(4), 209-217.

doi: 10.1370/afm.54