variation in an iron metabolism gene moderates the association

13
Psychological Science 2016, Vol. 27(2) 257–269 © The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797615618365 pss.sagepub.com Research Article Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental condition with complex etiology that includes effects of genetics, early environment, and their interplay. Because of the extensive literature on the neural effects of lead (Bellinger, 2011; Hu et al., 2011; Senut et al., 2014) and its established epidemiological associations with ADHD and other behavioral and cog- nitive outcomes, lead exposure provides an attractive “case study” of potential toxicological influences on neurodevelopmental conditions. Lead easily crosses the blood-brain barrier, and among the affected brain regions and tissues are those of long-standing interest in ADHD research, including prefrontal cortex, hippocam- pus, basal ganglia, cerebellum, and white matter in gen- eral (Costa, Aschner, Vitalone, Syversen, & Soldin, 2004). Its effects therefore provide a biologically plausible route to ADHD via subtle environmental perturbation of neurodevelopment. Regulation of lead in the 1970s resulted in substantial reductions in average body burden in the United States. However, lead remains ubiquitous in the environment, and children continue to ingest it in small amounts through normal hand-to-mouth contact from house and yard dust (i.e., residuals from prior use of leaded paint and gasoline). It leaches into tap water from aging water pipes, is found in some toys and jewelry, and occurs in industrial air pollution (Holstege, Huff, Rowden, & O’Malley, 2013; World Health Organization, 2015). In recent years, it has become apparent that even at the current historically low levels typical in the United 618365PSS XX X 10.1177/0956797615618365Nigg et al.Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead research-article 2015 Corresponding Author: Joel T. Nigg, Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University, 3550 SW U.S. Veterans Hospital Rd., DC7P, Portland, OR 97239 E-mail: [email protected] Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association Between Blood Lead Levels and Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder in Children Joel T. Nigg 1,2 , Alexis L. Elmore 3 , Neil Natarajan 1 , Karen H. Friderici 4 , and Molly A. Nikolas 4 1 Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University; 2 Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University; 3 Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of Iowa; and 4 Department of Microbiology and Molecular Genetics, Michigan State University Abstract Although attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a heritable neurodevelopmental condition, there is also considerable scientific and public interest in environmental modulators of its etiology. Exposure to neurotoxins is one potential source of perturbation of neural, and hence psychological, development. Exposure to lead in particular has been widely investigated and is correlated with neurodevelopmental outcomes, including ADHD. To investigate whether this effect is likely to be causal, we used a Mendelian randomization design with a functional gene variant. In a case-control study, we examined the association between ADHD symptoms in children and blood lead level as moderated by variants in the hemochromatosis (HFE) gene. The HFE gene regulates iron uptake and secondarily modulates lead metabolism. Statistical moderation was observed: The magnitude of the association of blood lead with symptoms of ADHD was altered by functional HFE genotype, which is consistent with a causal hypothesis. Keywords attention, childhood development, psychopathology Received 3/26/15; Revision accepted 10/29/15 at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016 pss.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Upload: lethuy

Post on 01-Jan-2017

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Psychological Science2016, Vol. 27(2) 257 –269© The Author(s) 2015Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0956797615618365pss.sagepub.com

Research Article

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental condition with complex etiology that includes effects of genetics, early environment, and their interplay. Because of the extensive literature on the neural effects of lead (Bellinger, 2011; Hu et al., 2011; Senut et  al., 2014) and its established epidemiological associations with ADHD and other behavioral and cog-nitive outcomes, lead exposure provides an attractive “case study” of potential toxicological influences on neurodevelopmental conditions. Lead easily crosses the blood-brain barrier, and among the affected brain regions and tissues are those of long-standing interest in ADHD research, including prefrontal cortex, hippocam-pus, basal ganglia, cerebellum, and white matter in gen-eral (Costa, Aschner, Vitalone, Syversen, & Soldin, 2004). Its effects therefore provide a biologically plausible route to ADHD via subtle environmental perturbation of neurodevelopment.

Regulation of lead in the 1970s resulted in substantial reductions in average body burden in the United States. However, lead remains ubiquitous in the environment, and children continue to ingest it in small amounts through normal hand-to-mouth contact from house and yard dust (i.e., residuals from prior use of leaded paint and gasoline). It leaches into tap water from aging water pipes, is found in some toys and jewelry, and occurs in industrial air pollution (Holstege, Huff, Rowden, & O’Malley, 2013; World Health Organization, 2015).

In recent years, it has become apparent that even at the current historically low levels typical in the United

618365 PSSXXX10.1177/0956797615618365Nigg et al.Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Leadresearch-article2015

Corresponding Author:Joel T. Nigg, Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University, 3550 SW U.S. Veterans Hospital Rd., DC7P, Portland, OR 97239 E-mail: [email protected]

Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association Between Blood Lead Levels and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children

Joel T. Nigg1,2, Alexis L. Elmore3, Neil Natarajan1, Karen H. Friderici4, and Molly A. Nikolas4

1Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health & Science University; 2Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health & Science University; 3Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of Iowa; and 4Department of Microbiology and Molecular Genetics, Michigan State University

AbstractAlthough attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a heritable neurodevelopmental condition, there is also considerable scientific and public interest in environmental modulators of its etiology. Exposure to neurotoxins is one potential source of perturbation of neural, and hence psychological, development. Exposure to lead in particular has been widely investigated and is correlated with neurodevelopmental outcomes, including ADHD. To investigate whether this effect is likely to be causal, we used a Mendelian randomization design with a functional gene variant. In a case-control study, we examined the association between ADHD symptoms in children and blood lead level as moderated by variants in the hemochromatosis (HFE) gene. The HFE gene regulates iron uptake and secondarily modulates lead metabolism. Statistical moderation was observed: The magnitude of the association of blood lead with symptoms of ADHD was altered by functional HFE genotype, which is consistent with a causal hypothesis.

Keywordsattention, childhood development, psychopathology

Received 3/26/15; Revision accepted 10/29/15

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

258 Nigg et al.

States, lead burden in the body is correlated with alter-ations in human neural development (Brubaker et al., 2009; Cecil et al., 2011) and with behavioral and cogni-tive outcomes in children, including ADHD, conduct problems, reductions in executive functioning, reduc-tions in IQ, and learning problems (Canfield, Kreher, Cornwell, & Henderson, 2003; Goodlad, Marcus, & Fulton, 2013; Lanphear, 2015; Marcus, Fulton, & Clarke, 2010; Nigg et al., 2008). The effect sizes after control-ling for covariates are medium to small (on the order of r = .15) but are similar for ADHD, IQ, and conduct problems (a developmental outgrowth of some cases of ADHD). These correlations appear to be nonlinear, such that even at low doses, lead has clinically signifi-cant associations with outcomes (Lanphear, 2015). Although these effects are statistically modest, they may have substantial population importance because of the near-universal exposure of children to lead (see Lanphear, 2015).

Although many potential confounders have been sta-tistically covaried in this research, and animal studies have demonstrated a causal effect of lead exposure on activity level (Luo et al., 2014), what appear to be causal effects of lead on ADHD in humans could still be due to unmeasured confounders. One approach to clarify cau-sality is Mendelian randomization (Lewis, Relton, Zammit, & Smith, 2013), which is a type of natural experiment. This approach entails dividing groups of people by geno-type for a functional gene that metabolically or physio-logically influences the presumed causal input (e.g., lead). The genotype must vary in the population at ran-dom and must be independent of both measured con-founders (i.e., lower socioeconomic status or parental IQ); therefore, it can be assumed to be independent of unmeasured confounders. In particular, the absence of a correlation between the genotype and the phenotype (in this case, ADHD) sharply reduces the likelihood of a reverse causality (i.e., the probability that increased hyperactivity is causing more lead exposure). Therefore, if this change in functional metabolism moderates the clinical endpoint, a causal pathway is supported.

Mendelian randomization has been a valuable strategy for other disorders, including alcoholism (Irons, McGue, Iacono, & Oetting, 2007; Ritchie et al., 2014); however, it is underused in research concerning most psychiatric conditions and has not been previously used to study ADHD in children. Figure 1 schematizes the predictive logic of this design as implemented in the current study.

Among the most widely studied functional gene vari-ants thought to influence lead metabolism is the human hemochromatosis gene (HFE) located at 6p21.3. Two somewhat common missense mutations (i.e., mutations in which a change in one nucleotide results in a different amino acid) within the HFE gene—cysteine to tyrosine at

position 82 (C282Y) and histidine to aspartic acid at posi-tion 63 (H63D)—cause a shortage of the HFE protein, resulting in up-regulation of transferrin receptors and divalent metal transporters and increased iron uptake in the gut. Homozygous mutation can cause adult-onset hemochromatosis, an autosomal recessive disease of iron overload (Hanson, Imperatore, & Burke, 2001; Santos, Krieger, & Pereira, 2012).

Because lead interacts with iron metabolically, these mutations are believed to alter lead’s effects (Gundacker, Gencik, & Hengstschlager, 2010). There is no consensus on the mechanism of HFE ’s effects on lead metabolism, but one hypothesis is that HFE variants alter lead’s health effects via increased oxidative stress (Park et al., 2009). That hypothesis is supported by laboratory work demon-strating increased iron-related lipid oxidation in the pres-ence of lead (Adonaylo & Oteiza, 1999). Under that model, the HFE variants are not required to alter blood lead level per se; rather, the variants alter the biochemical reactions involving lead and iron at a given blood lead level and thus change the phenotype.

Accordingly, HFE variants appear to modulate lead’s association with several phenotypic outcomes. For exam-ple, in a series of studies of older men, Wright and his colleagues reported that the association between lead level and cognitive decline was stronger for men who were carriers of HFE mutations (Wang et al., 2007) and that the association between blood lead level and dis-turbed cardiac function was greater for those with the C282Y variant of the HFE gene (Park et al., 2009). The C282Y variant also altered the association between lead and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Eum et al., 2014) and altered the rate of placental lead transfer (Karwowski et al., 2014); H63D variants altered lead’s association with low birth weight (Cantonwine et al., 2010). These find-ings are quite consistent, even though the strength of association among HFE variants and absolute blood lead level is modest and inconsistent in these studies.

Our governing hypothesis, therefore, was that carriers of specific HFE mutations (C282Y and H63D) are more susceptible than noncarriers to developmental damage as a result of low-level lead body burden. Thus, if lead influ-ences ADHD symptoms, HFE variants will statistically moderate the magnitude of the association between blood lead level and ADHD.

Method

Participants

We recruited physically healthy children with normative lead exposure for a study with a case-control design. To ensure that effects on ADHD symptoms within the clinical range would be adequately represented, we

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead 259

overselected youths with ADHD. Our participants were from Michigan, a U.S. region in which generalizability of allele frequencies could be compared with those found in a randomized population study published previously (Barry, Derhammer, & Elsea, 2005). All children were recruited via mailings to parents in regional school dis-tricts, public advertisements, and outreach to local clinics. Parents provided written informed consent, and children provided written informed assent. All procedures were approved by the university’s institutional review board and complied with all applicable guidelines for protec-tion of human participants. Data collection proceeded until funding was exhausted and sample size was suffi-cient to meet power targets.

Families entered a multistage screening process that established diagnostic groupings for the case-control design. For each child, a parent and a teacher completed standardized clinical rating scales, and a trained clinician completed a semistructured clinical interview with the parent about the child (Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–epidemiologic version, or K-SADS-E; Puig-Antich & Ryan, 1986). All interviewers had master’s degrees in clinical psychology or social work. To establish reliability of the coder KSAD diagno-ses and fidelity to procedures, we video-recorded 20 interviews conducted by each interviewer. These inter-views were then double-coded by the primary inter-viewer and by an expert criterion coder (for all disorders mentioned in this report, κs > .80).

Using all available data, two experienced clinicians (a board-certified child psychiatrist and a licensed clinical child psychologist) blind to the study’s hypotheses and to the children’s blood lead levels arrived independently at a best-estimate diagnostic decision for ADHD and comor-bid disorders, for each child. Impairment was required, and so the two clinicians quantified their impairment rat-ings using the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000), interrater agree-ment r > .80.

The experienced clinicians’ agreement rates for diag-nostic assignment were acceptable for ADHD, conduct disorder (CD), and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), all κs > .80, as well as for all other disorders with a base rate of greater than 5% in the sample, all κs > .75. Disagreements were resolved by discussion. The clini-cians assigned a primary diagnosis of ADHD when they saw evidence of impairment, cross-situational symptoms, and onset by the age of 7 years and concluded that a diagnosis of ADHD best accounted for symptoms; this practice was consistent with ADHD criteria from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; American Psychiatric Association, 2000), which was current at the time of data collection. Youths with situational ADHD symptoms (i.e., symptoms noticeable only at home or school) were coded as “not otherwise specified.” Interviewers coded youths judged to have five symptoms of one dimension (i.e., either inat-tention or hyperactivity) but fewer than five of the other dimension as subthreshold. This step was intended to minimize diagnostic assignment errors around the six-symptom cutoff. These youths (n = 24) were excluded from ADHD-control group comparisons but included in regression analysis of symptom severity effects. Sibling pairs were allowed into the study if they met our inclu-sion criteria, because we were also conducting ancillary studies of sibling effects in ADHD; nonindependence of sibling data was handled statistically because sample size was not large enough to use sibling differences as a causal lever.

Children were excluded at the intake stage if they were taking long-acting psychotropic medication (includ-ing antidepressants or long-acting ADHD-related medica-tion, such as atomoxetine) because it was unlikely that they could complete a medication washout for the

Lead

Wild-TypeHFE

HFE Mutation

Normal Effect ofLead on Iron

Oxidation

AcceleratedEffect of Lead on

Iron Oxidation

Relatively SmallEffect on ADHD

Symptoms

Relatively LargeEffect on ADHD

Symptoms

ExposureGeneticVariation

Putative BiologicalChange Outcome

Fig. 1. Diagram of the hypothesized interplay between blood lead level and hemochromato-sis (HFE) genotype in influencing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms.

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 4: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

260 Nigg et al.

ancillary studies of neurocognitive function in ADHD (Martel, Nikolas, Jernigan, Friderici, & Nigg, 2012; Nikolas & Nigg, 2015). Children whose parents reported a history of seizure (except a single febrile seizure), neurological impairments, prior diagnosis of intellectual disability or autism spectrum disorder, head injury with loss of con-sciousness beyond 30 s, sensorimotor handicap, or other major medical conditions were excluded. If the research diagnostic evaluation revealed substance addiction, autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, history of psychosis, sleep disorder, a medical or neurological con-dition, or an estimated IQ of less than 75 (evaluated by a short form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–4th Edition; Wechsler, 2003), children were excluded. These criteria were intended to minimize diagnostic assignment errors for ADHD. After exclusion, the sample comprised 386 children, ages 6 to 17 years, from 267 families (148 singletons, 119 sibling pairs).

Measures

Blood lead. Researchers drew 2 ml of whole blood from each child’s arm into a 2-ml Vacutainer tube (BD Diagnostics, Franklin Lakes, NJ) containing ethylenedi-aminetetraacetic acid (tubes were lot checked for lead by lab before use). Blood samples were labeled with a study number, frozen, and stored at −20 °C before analysis. Samples were assayed using the process of inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry. This method’s detec-tion limit for lead is 0.3 µg/dl; interrun precision was 5.8% (coefficient of variation) at a lead value of 2.9 µg/dl. The process began with bringing each whole-blood sam-ple to room temperature and vortexing it so that no par-ticulate matter remained at the bottom. Samples were diluted 1:50 with a diluent composed of 1.0% tetrameth-ylammonium hydroxide, internal standard (iridium), 1.0% isopropyl alcohol, 0.01% ammonium pyrrolidine dithiocarbamate, and 0.05% wetting solution (Triton X-100). Samples were then mixed by inverting them three or four times. The analysis then entailed quantitat-ing total lead by summing the signal intensities for three isotopic masses of lead (206, 207, and 208), using three replicates per sample on an Elan DRC Plus inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometer (PerkinElmer, Waltham, MA).

Genotyping. Samples were obtained from buccal swabs  (43%) and salivary DNA (57%). Buccal samples were purified using a method described previously (Meulenbelt, Droog, Trommelen, Boomsma, & Slagboom, 1995). Oragene samples were processed according to the  manufacturer’s instructions (DNAgenotek, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada). Polymerase chain reaction (PCR) was  performed according to the method used by

Barry et  al. (2005), but the number of cycles was increased from 30 to 35 for a more robust amplification product. Genomic DNA (40–60 ng) was amplified using 0.5 units of Taq DNA polymerase (Invitrogen Corp., Carlsbad, CA) in standard PCR buffer consisting of 20 mmol/L tris(hydroxymethyl)aminomethane hydrochlo-ride, 50 mmol/L potassium chloride, 1.5 mmol/L magne-sium chloride, 0.2 mmol/L deoxynucleoside triphos phates, and 0.7 µmol/L concentrations of each primer—for  C282Y: 5′-TCCCAAGGGTAAACAGATCC-3′ (forward) and  5′-TACCTCCTCAGGCACTCCTC-3′ (reverse); for H63D: 5′-CTTCATGGGTGCCTCAGAGC-3′ (forward) and 5′-CCCTTGCTGTGGTTGTGATT-3′ (reverse).

Reaction conditions consisted of an initial denaturing step at 94 °C for 3 min, followed by 35 cycles at 94 °C for 30 s each, annealing at 63 °C for 30 s, and an extension at 72 °C for 30 s, followed by a final extension step at 72 °C for 5 min. Expected product sizes for C282Y and H63D were 393 and 243 base pairs, respectively. Digestion of the C282Y PCR product with the restriction endonuclease RsaI (New England Biolabs, Ipswich, MA) for 1 to 2 hr yielded the fragment sizes of 251 and 142 base pairs (bp) for the wild type and 251, 113, and 29 bp for the mutation. Digestion of the H63D PCR product with the restriction endonuclease DpnII (New England Biolabs) yielded frag-ment sizes of 100, 85, and 58 bp for the wild type and 185 and 58 bp for the mutation. Both were analyzed on a 3.0% agarose gel. Genotyping failed for 3 children, meaning that 383 were included in the final analyses.

Measures of ADHD and comorbid externalizing symptoms. For data-analysis purposes, ADHD symp-toms were assessed by parental and teacher ratings on three widely used rating scales: (a) the ADHD Rating Scale (DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos, & Reid, 1998) inat-tention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptom scores; (b) Conners’ Rating Scale–Revised (Conners, 1997), spe-cifically the subscales for cognitive problems (i.e., inat-tention) and hyperactivity problems; and (c) the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behav-ior Scale (SWAN; Swanson et al., 2012) inattention and hyperactivity symptom scores (from parents but not teachers), on which ADHD symptoms are listed on a nor-malized scale and worded to reflect normal variation, to avoid floor effects often seen on symptom scales. Teach-ers also completed ratings of ODD and CD behaviors as part of the ADHD Rating Scale; parents’ ratings of ODD and CD symptoms were derived from their reports on the K-SADS-E. In this sample, all scales exhibited adequate internal consistency (αs from .83 to .94), and their manu-als provide adequate test-retest reliabilities. For children taking prescribed medication, raters were asked to rate, to the extent possible, the children’s behavior as observed when they were not taking medication.

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead 261

Data analysis

Statistical power. A review of prior studies of Mende-lian designs, studies of Gene × Environment in ADHD, and HFE interaction studies suggested that we needed to be able to detect a small to medium interaction effect. Power analysis was conducted in G*Power (Faul, Erd-felder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) using assumptions regard-ing allele frequencies and correlational structure. With more than 350 participants, as we were able to achieve, power was .80 to detect an interaction effect size (ΔR2) of .037 at an α level of .05. When α was adjusted to .0125, power was .80 for an effect size of .04. These effect sizes are between small, R2 = .01, and medium, R2 = .09, by Cohen’s conventions.

Handling of covariates. Total gross annual income in the child’s primary household was reported by parents as a proxy for socioeconomic status and was included as a covariate. Because of the relatively wide age range of the children, we included age as a covariate even though it was not reliably correlated with blood lead level.

HFE mutations are more common among White par-ticipants (Hanson et  al., 2001). In our sample, White youths were more likely to have one or two copies of the H63D mutation, p = .004, whereas the C282Y mutation was not significantly higher among White youths relative to non-White youths (p = .29).

Race is an important potential confound in any genetic study. Table 1 reports in detail the racial makeup of the sample. We controlled for race in two ways, following the method of Keller (2014). For our main results, we simply used a single race code (White vs. non-White) as a covari-ate. We then conducted secondary checks in which we entered all the interactions (one for each group listed in Table 1) simultaneously, so that all effects of race were controlled. Mixed-race individuals were coded separately as biracial and treated as one group.

To rule out artifact, we created a comparison variable that was not expected to yield an interaction with HFE genotype. Specifically, we obtained measures of parenting behavior using the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (Shelton, Frick, & Wooton, 1996). Because there is no evi-dence or theory suggesting that HFE genotype moderates the association of parenting behavior with ADHD symp-toms, we used this check to rule out the possibility that our analyses overidentified interactions with genotype.

Testing for ODD and CD is important to clarify whether any effects found are specific to ADHD or are related to externalizing problems. We report secondary data checks involving a composite ODD-CD outcome variable (based on parental reports on the K-SADS-E and teacher reports on the ODD and CD items added to their ADHD Rating Scale).

Iron hemoglobin level was assayed by standard meth-ods from blood obtained by venipuncture and was a covariate in all analyses to protect against the possibility of attributing to lead an effect that was actually attribut-ing to iron. The reference range for iron hemoglobin in children is 11 to 13 g/dl; in adolescents, the reference range is 12 to 16 g/dl for girls and 14 to 18 g/dl for boys. Values for the current sample were within the reference range (11.0–15.6 g/dl).

IQ was not covaried because reduction in IQ may be part of ADHD as a neurodevelopmental condition or may even be one of its consequences (Dennis et al., 2009; Miller & Chapman, 2001). Medication and treat-ment history were not covaried because they were substantively correlated with symptom severity; greater symptom severity was positively correlated with past or current medication or other treatment, rs ranging from .49 for lifetime treatment to .71 for current treat-ment, ps < .001.

A substantial literature suggests that boys are more vulnerable than girls to early-life neurodevelopmental perturbations ( Jacquemont et al., 2014), which perhaps accounts for the fact that nearly all neurodevelopmental conditions affect more males than females (Martel, 2013). We therefore handled the sex variable in two ways. First, our primary analyses included sex as a covariate to pre-serve statistical power for detecting our primary interac-tion of interest (Blood Lead × Genotype). Because of boys’ greater vulnerability, we also conducted secondary analyses to explore the lower-powered but potentially important interactions between sex and lead exposure in predicting ADHD symptoms as reported by parents and teachers.

Data reduction

ADHD symptom composite scores as dependent vari-ables. To maximize the reliability and validity of our out-come measures while minimizing the number of statistical tests required, we used structural equation modeling to create latent factors for inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity for each informant (i.e., each parent and teacher). Thus, two separate two-factor models were fitted to the data for each informant. The models were based on reports from each informant for (a) composite inattention-disorganization, and (b) composite hyperactivity-impulsiv-ity. Each composite was created from the relevant scales on the ADHD Rating Scale, the Conners’ Rating Scale–Revised, and the parent-reported SWAN. Each model pro-vided a good fit to the data—parental reports: χ2(7, N = 383) = 51.3, comparative fit index = .97, Tucker-Lewis index = .94, root-mean-square error of approximation = 0.04; teacher reports: χ2(3, N = 369) = 12.5, comparative fit index = .98, Tucker-Lewis index = .97, root-mean-

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

262 Nigg et al.

square error of approximation = 0.08. To ease replication by other researchers, we created composite scores from the factor indicators for further analysis (results were unchanged using factor scores). These composite scores had satisfactory internal reliability—inattention: α = .89 for parental reports, α = .92 for teacher reports; hyperactivity-impulsivity: α = .91 for parental reports, α = .93 for teacher reports.

ODD and CD. A sum total of ODD symptoms reported by parents was computed for each child. To create a par-allel sum score for teacher ratings, we counted the num-ber of teacher-rated ODD and CD symptoms present (i.e., items rated as occurring “often” or “very often”). These totals were transformed (to correct skewness) and mean-centered.

Lead nondetection. Three children’s lead levels were below the limit of detection of 0.3 µg/dl. Following con-vention, those levels were scored as 0.2 (i.e., 0.3/ 2 ). Also following a frequent convention in the literature, we log10-transformed the blood lead score to reduce the influence of outliers (posttransformation skew < 1.0).

Handling gene-environment correlation effects. To account for potential gene-environment correlation effects, we regressed blood lead level on HFE genotype to remove variance in lead scores associated with geno-type. These residuals were standardized and used for all analyses.

Multiple testing. We provide 95% confidence intervals (CIs), but we also provide modified Bonferroni-corrected

Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Differences Between the Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Non-ADHD Groups

CharacteristicNon-ADHD

group (n = 147)ADHD group

(n = 122)Difference

between groups: p

Male (%) 49.7 68.4 < .001Age (years) 12.5 (3.1) 11.5 (2.8) .003Race White (non-Hispanic) 80.3%, n = 118 75.0%, n = 159 .51 African American (non-White) 8.2%, n = 14 8.5%, n = 18 .91 Latino-Hispanic (non-White) 1.4%, n = 2 3.8%, n = 8 .17 Asian American (non-White) 1.3%, n = 2 0%, n = 0 .28 Native American (non-White) 1.4%, n = 2 1.9%, n = 4 .66 Mixed race 5.4%, n = 8 10.3%, n = 22 .44 Pacific Islander 0.68%, n = 1 0.0%, n = 0 .49 Middle Eastern 0%, n = 0 0.47%, n = 1 .51Total household income (U.S. $) 87,300 (45,800) 68,000 (40,100) < .001Global Assessment Functioning score 81.2 (9.4) 67.5 (9.1) < .001Estimated Full Scale IQ 111.5 (12.6) 103.8 (14.6) < .001Inattention symptoms 1.7 (2.0) 8.5 (1.4) < .001Hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms 1.0 (1.5) 5.9 (3.1) < .001Oppositional defiant disorder (%) 2.7 26.5 < .001Conduct disorder (%) 0.0 4.7 .008Blood lead level (µg/dl) 0.74 (0.35) 0.94 (0.52) < .001Iron hemoglobin level (g/dl) 14.1 (1.2) 14.0 (1.2) .23HFE C282Y genotype (%) 11.0 11.4 .89HFE H63D genotype (%) 24.0 31.0 .15Conners’ Rating Scale–Parent Inattention (cognitive) T score 46.8 (6.7) 71.1 (10.3) < .001 Hyperactivity T score 48.2 (6.9) 66.5 (15.1) < .001Conners’ Rating Scale–Teacher Inattention (cognitive) T score 49.3 (9.0) 57.7 (13.0) < .001 Hyperactivity T score 48.0 (7.1) 59.1 (10.2) < .001

Note: Values in parentheses are standard deviations. Twenty-four children with a diagnosis of ADHD not otherwise specified were excluded from these group comparisons. Parental and teacher ratings were collected using the Conners’ Rating Scale–Revised (Conners, 1997), and Full Scale IQ was estimated with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–4th edition (Wechsler, 2003). HFE = hemochromatosis gene.

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead 263

p values for the tests of interaction reliability, using an α level of .0125 for both parental and teacher data.

Data-analytic strategy

Unless otherwise noted, analyses were conducted in Mplus Version 7.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 2015). The CLUSTER option was used to account for nonindepen-dence issues stemming from sibling data. Missing data were handled using full-information maximum-likelihood procedures in Mplus; missing data were minimal except for income (7% missing). Linear regression was the pri-mary method of examining Gene × Environment effects. All models included the main effects of the respective HFE genotypes (examined separately), blood lead level, and their interaction. All continuous variables were stan-dardized so they would be centered on zero. Effect cod-ing was used to designate genotype groups, centered on 0 (wild type = −1, one or two mutation copies = 1). Standardized parameter estimates were computed and are presented along with their 95% CIs.

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations

Table 1 presents demographic and clinical data and the percentage of children in our sample who were homozy-gous and heterozygous for the mutation. As we reported previously for an almost identical sample, blood lead level was higher in the ADHD group than in the non-ADHD group, d = 0.45, 95% CI = [0.24, 0.66]. Iron hemoglobin levels did not differ reliably by group. In a random popu-lation sample of more than 3,500 White individuals in Michigan, Barry et al. (2005) found an HFE C282Y allele frequency of 5.7% (compared with 5.6% for our overall sample), and an HFE H63D allele frequency of 14.0% (compared with 14.6% for our overall sample). This means that 11.4% of the Barry et al. sample were either homozy-gous or heterozygous for the C282Y mutation, compared with 10.9% of our sample (10.8% in the non-ADHD group and 11.4% in the ADHD group). In their study, by exten-sion 28.0% were either homozygous or heterozygous for the HFE H63D mutation, compared with our 28.5% (24.4% non-ADHD group, 31.1% ADHD group).

The average blood lead level in our sample was almost identical to the national average reported by the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention at the time of data collection (for discussion, see Nigg et al., 2008; Nigg, Nikolas, Mark Knottnerus, Cavanagh, & Friderici, 2010). Thus, as intended, our sample provided a reasonable representation of allele frequency and lead exposure and allowed us to take advantage of a case-control design. Table 2 shows the bivariate correlations among blood

lead and hemoglobin levels, HFE genotype, teacher and parental ratings of ADHD symptoms, and selected demo-graphic indicators. Sex was associated with ADHD symp-toms (i.e., boys were overrepresented in the ADHD group) and was also associated with lead level, such that males had higher mean levels of both, further justifying our decision to covary sex and to secondarily examine interactions with sex.

As reported previously (Nigg et al., 2008), blood lead level correlated with scores calculated from both parental and teacher reports of the inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity factor, even at these low, population-typical blood lead levels. Contrary to expectations, blood lead level was only weakly positively correlated with iron hemoglobin level. However, as expected, higher blood lead level was associated with lower income, lower full-scale IQ, and the HFE C282Y variant genotype, whereas the association between blood lead level and the H63D variant genotype was not statistically reliable.

Primary tests of Gene × Environment effects

Parental reports. The interaction between HFE C282Y genotype and blood lead level was a statistically reliable predictor of parental reports of hyperactivity-impulsivity, β = 0.22, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.40], ΔR2 = .02, total R2 = .28,

Table 2. Correlations Among Blood Lead Level, Genotype, and Demographic Indicators

CharacteristicBlood lead

C282Y genotype

H63D genotype

Sex –.24** .07 .06Age .08 –.08 –.04Household income –.22** .01 –.11*Estimated Full Scale IQ –.11* .02 –.05Global assessment functioning –.09 –.03 –.09Parental ratings (composite) Inattention .29** .04 .05 Hyperactivity-impulsivity .34** –.02 .06Teacher ratings (composite) Inattention .22** .03 –.01 Hyperactivity-impulsivity .26** .02 –.04Blood lead level — –.12* –.06HFE C282Y genotype –.12* — –.11*HFE H63D genotype –.05 –.11* —Iron hemoglobin .11* –.02 .08

Note: Parental and teacher inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity scores are factor scores based on the two-factor latent-variable model constructed to capture variance across the parents’ and teachers’ reports on the ADHD measures, as described in the Method section. N = 386, including all children with a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) not otherwise specified. HFE = hemochromatosis gene.*p < .05. **p < .01.

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

264 Nigg et al.

but not parental reports of inattention, β = 0.16, 95% CI = [−0.004, 0.32], ΔR2 = .005, total R2 = .14. The association between blood lead level and hyperactivity was signifi-cantly stronger for youths with the HFE C282Y mutation, β = 0.74, 95% CI = [0.52, 0.96], than for those with the wild-type genotype, β = 0.28, 95% CI = [0.15, 0.41]. Fig-ure 2 illustrates this effect, which passed our modified Bonferroni threshold. The interactions between the HFE H63D genotype and blood lead level as a predictor of parental and teacher reports of ADHD symptoms were very small and not statistically reliable, ΔR2s < .01, ps > .3.

Teacher reports. HFE C282Y genotype and blood lead level also interacted in predicting teacher reports of hyperactivity-impulsivity, although the 95% CI did include zero, β = 0.19, 95% CI = [–0.002, 0.41], ΔR2 = .015, total R2 = .22. The association between blood lead and hyperactivity was qualitatively stronger for youths with the HFE C282Y mutation, β = 0.47, 95% CI = [0.22, 0.72], than for those with the wild-type genotype, β = 0.29, 95% CI = [0.18, 0.40]. The interaction of HFE C282Y genotype and blood lead level was not a statistically reli-able predictor of teacher-reported inattention, β = 0.06, 95% CI = [–0.04, 0.12], ΔR2 = .002, total R2 = .11, just as it was not for parent-reported inattention. As with parental reports, the interaction effects of HFE H63D genotype and blood lead level were small and not reliably differ-ent from zero. For secondary analyses, we focused on the C282Y genotype.

Secondary analyses

Sex. Males and females differed significantly in mean level of ADHD symptoms and blood lead level, and there is a substantial theoretical literature on biological sex as a moderator of ADHD vulnerability (Martel, 2013). There-fore, we conducted secondary analyses to determine whether sex moderated the impact of lead exposure on ADHD symptoms. Interaction terms were not reliably dif-ferent from zero for analyses of either parental or teacher reports of inattention symptoms. The Sex × Blood Lead interaction term was reliably different from zero in the  model predicting teacher reports of hyperactivity- impulsivity, β = 0.26, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.41], but not the model predicting parental reports of hyperactivity- impulsivity, β = 0.10, 95% CI = [–0.04, 0.24]. The direction of the effect for teacher reports indicated that the effect was larger in boys than in girls. We then examined effects separately within sex. For completeness, we report results for both teacher and parental reports.

In parental reports for boys, the Blood Lead × C282Y Genotype interaction was associated with hyperactivity-impulsivity, β = 0.31, 95% CI = [0.14, 0.48], ΔR2 = .033, total R2 = .24. The association between blood lead level and

hyperactivity-impulsivity was stronger for boys with the HFE C282Y mutation, β = 0.50, 95% CI = [0.10, 0.90], than for boys with the wild-type genotype, β = 0.21, 95% CI = [0.06, 0.27]. In the smaller sample of females, the interac-tion between HFE C282Y mutation and blood lead level was not reliable, β = 0.09, 95% CI = [–0.16, 0.34], and the association between blood lead level and hyperactivity did not differ much between girls with the HFE C282Y mutation, β = 0.22, 95% CI = [–0.02, 0.46], and those with the wild-type genotype, β = 0.17, 95% CI = [–0.10, 0.54].

In teacher reports for boys, the interaction between HFE C282Y genotype and blood lead level was a reliable predictor of reports of hyperactivity-impulsivity, β = 0.19, 95% CI = [0.07, 0.31], ΔR2 = .016, total R2 = .15. Once again, the association between blood lead level and ADHD symptoms was stronger for boys with the HFE C282Y mutation, β = 0.46, 95% CI = [0.23, 0.69], than for boys with the wild-type genotype, β = 0.15, 95% CI = [0.02, 0.28]. In the smaller sample of girls, again, the interaction was not reliable, β = 0.11, 95% CI = [–0.03, 0.25]. The associations between blood lead level and hyperactivity-impulsivity were similar for girls with the HFE C282Y mutation, β = 0.20, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.36], and those with the wild-type genotype, β = 0.16, 95% CI = [0.07, 0.25].

Race. Following the method of Keller (2014), we also created a model that included an interaction term for HFE C282Y genotype and each of the seven racial groups shown in Table 1 to determine whether these interactions accounted for the interactive effect of C282Y genotype and blood lead level on ADHD symptoms. This was repeated for H63D. The results for this model were not discernibly different from the results already reported.

Other interactions. Also following the method of Keller (2014), we created models with blood lead level and terms for the interaction between HFE genotype and all other covariates (i.e., age, ethnicity, income, iron hemoglobin). Again, the results for this model were essentially the same as those reported earlier.

Comorbidity. When ODD-CD score was included as a covariate, results were essentially the same. The interac-tion between C282Y genotype and blood lead level was still a significant predictor of parental reports of hyperac-tivity on the K-SADS-E, β = 0.20, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.36], ΔR2 = .020, total R2 = .28; the interaction between C282Y genotype and blood lead level was not a reliable predic-tor of parental reports of inattention, β = 0.15, 95% CI = [–0.003, 0.31], ΔR2 = .005, total R2 = .14. When teacher-reported ODD-CD sum score was included as a covari-ate, the interaction between C282Y genotype and blood lead level was not reliably different from zero for either hyperactivity, β = 0.17, 95% CI = [–0.003, 0.43], ΔR2 = .018,

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 9: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead 265

total R2 = .22, or for inattention, β = 0.07, 95% CI = [–0.05, 0.19], ΔR2 = .003, total R2 = .11. Conversely, in a model predicting ODD-CD score (controlling for ADHD symp-toms), the interaction of C282Y genotype and blood lead level was small and not reliably different from zero, β = 0.08, 95% CI = [–0.07, 0.23], ΔR2 = .006, total R2 = .17.

Specificity. As a cross-check on specificity of effects, we conducted analyses examining HFE C282Y genotype as a moderator of the impact of parental monitoring, an environment not expected to be related to ADHD or to interact with HFE genotype (Ellis & Nigg, 2009). Parental monitoring was evaluated by parental self-report using the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (Shelton et  al., 1996). As expected, there was no hint that the interaction between parental monitoring and genotype was a reli-able predictor of parental or teacher reports of hyperac-tivity or inattention, all R2s < .01, all ps > .50.

Discussion

We used Mendelian randomization to evaluate effects of blood lead level in childhood ADHD. Consistent with the proposition that lead plays a causal role in the develop-ment of some cases of ADHD, the HFE C282Y mutation heightened the association between lead and ADHD, principally for hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms. The reliability of effects for hyperactivity but not for inatten-tion echoes the literature, which suggests that the neuro-biology and etiology of the two ADHD symptom domains

is partially distinct (Sonuga-Barke, 2005; Willcutt et  al., 2012). Lead may preferentially act in DA-related reward circuits, for example, and thus preferentially affect the hyperactivity-impulsivity symptom domain.

In this school-age sample, the association between the HFE C282Y mutation and lead level itself was rather weak. Although similar associations were seen in prior studies of U.S. adults (Wright et al., 2004), the direction of association was reversed in a study of Mexican preschool-ers (Hopkins et al., 2008). Aside from markedly greater lead exposure and different allele frequencies in the Mexican preschoolers compared with the U.S. adults, the functional effect of HFE mutations on lead level may vary across development in relation to developmental changes in iron demand in the body (Delatycki, Powell, & Allen, 2004; Gundacker et al., 2010). Park et al. (2009) proposed that if the C282Y effect primarily alters lead’s influence on secondary metabolic activity (rather than lead uptake per se), then C282Y could alter lead’s effect on hyperac-tivity without markedly altering blood lead readings. Although their specific hypothesis remains unproven, it would be consistent with the effects seen here.

The same may hold for iron level. In this sample, HFE variants were not associated with increased iron hemo-globin level. Perhaps a different measure, such as serum iron, would have given different results (Zimmermann, 2008). More likely, this result is related to greater iron demand during development, which is known to attenu-ate the HFE association with absolute iron stores in the body. Nonetheless, it is clear that confirmation of the

–3

–2

–1

0

1

2

3

–3 –2 –1 0 1 2 3

Hype

ract

ivity

-Impu

lsiv

ity P

aren

tal C

ompo

site

Blood Lead Level (z score)

Children With HFE C282Y Mutation

Children With Wild-Type HFE Genotype

β = 0.74, p < .001

β = 0.28, p = .006

Fig. 2. Parental reports of hyperactive-impulsive symptoms as a function of blood lead level (z-scored), plot-ted separately for youths with the hemochromatosis (HFE) C282Y mutation and youths with the HFE wild-type genotype.

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 10: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

266 Nigg et al.

hypothesis proposed here will require a better under-standing of how absolute iron and lead levels contribute to altered metabolism or cell damage in the presence of particular genotypes, particularly in children. We reported previously that lead’s association with ADHD was medi-ated by effects on cognitive control (Nigg et  al., 2008; Nigg et al., 2010). To better evaluate this theory and its pathophysiological meaning, future researchers could consider intermediate phenotypes, such as cognitive con-trol and, in particular, reward processing related to hyperactivity-impulsivity.

Effects were reliable for C282Y but not for H63D. Although some past work has indicated moderation of lead’s clinical effects by H63D, effects have been more consistent for C282Y. Evidence suggests that C282Y has a more powerful effect on iron-related metabolism than that of H63D. The HFE C282Y mutation causes a greater loss of HFE protein function than does the H63D muta-tion and confers a greater risk for development of hemo-chromatosis and iron overload in adulthood (Hanson et al., 2001).

Findings were most robust for boys. Boys are more vulnerable than girls are to early neurodevelopmental insult, and ADHD is more common in boys, possibly because of hormone-mediated effects (Martel, 2013). It may be that additional biochemical pathways interact with lead and iron handling in boys. The boys in this sample also had higher lead levels than the girls, so it is also possible that C282Y’s moderating effects are stronger at slightly higher lead levels.

The timing of the exposure to lead in this cross-sec-tional sample is unknown; obviously, if lead exposure is causal, it should precede development of ADHD. In keeping with this idea, results from broader population studies have shown that peak lead exposure in children occurs in the toddler years, whereas ADHD is typically not observed or diagnosed until later in preschool or school age. Even so, it is unclear whether early exposure or sustained exposure drove the effects we observed.

The size of the Blood Lead Level × HFE interaction effect on ADHD was modest. Although this effect passed multiple-testing correction and tests of specificity, and therefore is reliably different from zero and indicates a causal association, it is also likely that HFE genotype is only one of the elements moderating the strength of association between blood lead level and ADHD.

Lead is associated with conduct problems and delin-quency as well as ADHD. In the current study, effects were specific to ADHD. Many youths with ODD will not develop CD or delinquency, and few youths with CD were included. In addition, many of these youths were not old enough to display conduct problems. ADHD is a typical precursor to CD, particularly for boys. Thus, as these youths mature, an effect with CD might emerge.

This study highlights lead as an exemplar environ-mental exposure for understanding the interplay among environmental, genetic, and (potentially) epigenetic inputs to the development of psychopathology in the case of ADHD. The results enable speculation on the larger question of Gene × Environment etiology of ADHD, although this larger Gene × Environment ques-tion was not our focus. Twin studies demonstrate sub-stantial heritability for ADHD (Nikolas & Burt, 2010). Genetic factors have been shown to influence the uptake of toxic elements, including lead (Whitfield et al., 2007). Widespread and common environmental exposures (such as that to lead) likely function as shared environ-ments (Nigg, 2006), which means that lead exposure is correlated among siblings within the same family and serves to increase their similarity. However, given that shared environmental effects may influence ADHD within the context of genetic risk, gene-by-environment interac-tions have been proposed (Nigg, 2006; Nigg et al., 2010). Shared environment is of specific interest in the current study, because the Gene × Shared Environment interac-tion is subsumed within the additive genetic variance in twin studies, inflating the typical heritability estimate (Purcell, 2002). Thus, Nigg (2006) proposed that studies of environment and ADHD should emphasize common environmental exposures, among which low-level lead exposure is salient.

Our results add to the growing picture of how ADHD and related disorders involve dynamic, genetically modi-fied responses to common environments, and they should stimulate development of appropriate theoretical conceptualizations. At the same time, this work should encourage further research on other neurotoxicants and neurodevelopmental disorders. For example, even low-level lead exposure may result in epigenetic changes dur-ing development (e.g., histone modification), and such changes can mediate increased hyperactive behavior (Luo et  al., 2014). Thus, future work could specifically examine changes in expression in ADHD-relevant genes related to lead, as well as other toxicants.

Studies of specific genes in specific environments move the field toward biology-based nosology and per-sonalized treatments for developmental psychopathol-ogy. Although achievement of those goals remains some distance away, the present findings suggest that further investigation of environmental toxicants may provide clues regarding causal pathways for neurodevelopmental disorders.

Author Contributions

J. T. Nigg and K. H. Friderici developed the study concept and design. Data collection, including genotyping of samples ana-lyzing blood lead levels, was overseen by J. T. Nigg, K. H. Friderici, and M. A. Nikolas. M. A. Nikolas, A. L. Elmore, and N. Natarajan performed the data analysis with supervision and

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 11: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead 267

guidance by J. T. Nigg. J. T. Nigg, A. L. Elmore, and M. A. Nikolas drafted the manuscript, and all authors provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final manuscript before submission.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Funding

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants R01-MH070004, R01-MH099064, and R37-MH59015.

References

Adonaylo, V. N., & Oteiza, P. I. (1999). Pb2+ promotes lipid oxidation and alterations in membrane physical properties. Toxicology, 132, 19–32. doi:10.1016/S0300-483X(98)00134-6

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and sta-tistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Barry, E., Derhammer, T., & Elsea, S. H. (2005). Prevalence of three hereditary hemochromatosis mutant alleles in the Michigan Caucasian population. Community Genetics, 8, 173–179. doi:10.1159/000086760

Bellinger, D. C. (2011). The protean toxicities of lead: New chapters in a familiar story. International Journal of Envi-ronmental Research and Public Health, 8, 2593–2628. doi:10.3390/ijerph8072593

Brubaker, C. J., Schmithorst, V. J., Haynes, E. N., Dietrich, K. N., Egelhoff, J. C., Lindquist, D. M., . . . Cecil, K. M. (2009). Altered myelination and axonal integrity in adults with childhood lead exposure: A diffusion tensor imag-ing study. NeuroToxicology, 30, 867–875. doi:10.1016/ j.neuro.2009.07.007

Burns, J. M., Baghurst, P. A., Sawyer, M. G., McMichael, A. J., & Tong, S. L. (1999). Lifetime low-level exposure to environ-mental lead and children’s emotional and behavioral devel-opment at ages 11–13 years. The Port Pirie Cohort Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 149, 740–749. Retrieved from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/content/149/8/740.long

Canfield, R. L., Kreher, D. A., Cornwell, C., & Henderson, C. R., Jr. (2003). Low-level lead exposure, executive functioning, and learning in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 35–53. doi:10.1076/chin.9.1.35.14496

Cantonwine, D., Hu, H., Tellez-Rojo, M. M., Sanchez, B. N., Lamadrid-Figueroa, H., Ettinger, A. S., . . . Wright, R. O. (2010). HFE gene variants modify the association between maternal lead burden and infant birthweight: A prospective birth cohort study in Mexico City, Mexico. Environmental Health, 9, Article 43. doi:10.1186/1476-069X-9-43

Cecil, K. M., Dietrich, K. N., Altaye, M., Egelhoff, J. C., Lindquist, D. M., Brubaker, C. J., & Lanphear, B. P. (2011). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in adults with childhood lead exposure. Environmental Health Perspectives, 119, 403–408. doi:10.1289/ehp.1002176

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners Rating Scales–Revised: Techni-cal manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

Costa, L. G., Aschner, M., Vitalone, A., Syversen, T., & Soldin, O. P. (2004). Developmental neuropathology of envi-ronmental agents. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 44, 87–110. doi:10.1146/annurev.pharmtox .44.101802.121424

Delatycki, M. B., Powell, L. W., & Allen, K. J. (2004). Hereditary hemochromatosis genetic testing of at-risk children: What is the appropriate age? Genetic Testing, 8, 98–103. doi:10.1089/1090657041797248

Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covari-ate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 331–343. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090481

DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, norms, and clini-cal interpretation. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Ellis, B., & Nigg, J. (2009). Parenting practices and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: New findings suggest partial specificity of effects. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 146–154. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819176d0

Eum, K. D., Seals, R. M., Taylor, K. M., Grespin, M., Umbach, D. M., Hu, H., . . . Weisskopf, M. G. (2014). Modification of the association between lead exposure and amyo-trophic lateral sclerosis by iron and oxidative stress rela-ted gene polymorphisms. Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis and  Frontotemporal Degeneration, 16, 1–8. doi:10.3109/ 21678421.2014.964259

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

Goodlad, J. K., Marcus, D. K., & Fulton, J. J. (2013). Lead and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 417–425. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.01.009

Gundacker, C., Gencik, M., & Hengstschlager, M. (2010). The relevance of the individual genetic background for the toxicokinetics of two significant neurodevelopmental toxi-cants: Mercury and lead. Mutation Research, 705, 130–140. doi:10.1016/j.mrrev.2010.06.003

Hanson, E. H., Imperatore, G., & Burke, W. (2001). HFE gene and hereditary hemochromatosis: A HuGE review. American Journal of Epidemiology, 154, 193–206.

Holstege, C. P., Huff, J. S., Rowden, A. K., & O’Malley, R. N. (2013). Pathophysiology and etiology of lead toxic-ity. Retrieved from Medscape Web site: http://emedicine. medscape.com/article/2060369-overview#a3

Hopkins, M. R., Ettinger, A. S., Hernandez-Avila, M., Schwartz, J., Tellez-Rojo, M. M., Lamadrid-Figueroa, H., . . . Wright, R.  O. (2008). Variants in iron metabolism genes predict higher blood lead levels in young children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 116, 1261–1266. doi:10.1289/ehp.11233

Hu, Q., Fu, H., Song, H., Ren, T., Li, L., Ye, L., . . . Dong, S. (2011). Low-level lead exposure attenuates the expres-sion of three major isoforms of neural cell adhesion molecule. NeuroToxicology, 32, 255–260. doi:10.1016/ j.neuro.2010.12.007

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 12: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

268 Nigg et al.

Irons, D. E., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Oetting, W. S. (2007). Mendelian randomization: a novel test of the gate-way hypothesis and models of gene-environment inter-play. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 1181–1195. doi:10.1017/S0954579407000612

Jacquemont, S., Coe, B. P., Hersch, M., Duyzend, M. H., Krumm, N., Bergmann, S., . . . Eichler, E. E. (2014). A higher mutational burden in females supports a “female protec-tive model” in neurodevelopmental disorders. American Journal of Human Genetics, 94, 415–425. doi:10.1016/ j.ajhg.2014.02.001

Karwowski, M. P., Just, A. C., Bellinger, D. C., Jim, R., Hatley, E. L., Ettinger, A. S., . . . Wright, R. O. (2014). Maternal iron metabolism gene variants modify umbilical cord blood lead levels by gene-environment interaction: A birth cohort study. Environmental Health, 13, Article 77. doi:10.1186/1476-069X-13-77

Keller, M. C. (2014). Gene × environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: The problem and the (simple) solution. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 18–24. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.006

Lanphear, B. P. (2015). The impact of toxins on the develop-ing brain. Annual Review of Public Health, 36, 211–230. doi:10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114413

Lewis, S. J., Relton, C., Zammit, S., & Smith, G. D. (2013). Approaches for strengthening causal inference regarding prenatal risk factors for childhood behavioural and psy-chiatric disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-atry and Allied Disciplines, 54, 1095–1108. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12127

Luo, M., Xu, Y., Cai, R., Tang, Y., Ge, M. M., Liu, Z. H., . . . Wang, H. L. (2014). Epigenetic histone modification regu-lates developmental lead exposure induced hyperactivity in rats. Toxicology Letters, 225, 78–85. doi:10.1016/j.tox-let.2013.11.025

Marcus, D. K., Fulton, J. J., & Clarke, E. J. (2010). Lead and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clini-cal Child & Adolescent Psychology, 39, 234–241. doi:10 .1080/15374411003591455

Martel, M. M. (2013). Sexual selection and sex differences in the prevalence of childhood externalizing and adolescent inter-nalizing disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1221–1259. doi:10.1037/a0032247

Martel, M. M., Nikolas, M., Jernigan, K., Friderici, K., & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Diversity in pathways to common childhood disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 1223–1236. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9646-3

Meulenbelt, I., Droog, S., Trommelen, G. J., Boomsma, D. I., & Slagboom, P. E. (1995). High-yield noninvasive human genomic DNA isolation method for genetic studies in geo-graphically dispersed families and populations. American Journal of Human Genetics, 57, 1252–1254. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1801361/

Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding anal-ysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.40

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.

Nigg, J. T. (2006). What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Nigg, J. T., Knottnerus, G. M., Martel, M. M., Nikolas, M., Cavanagh, K., Karmaus, W., & Rappley, M. D. (2008). Low blood lead levels associated with clinically diagnosed attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and mediated by weak cognitive control. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 325–331. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.013

Nigg, J. T., Nikolas, M., Mark Knottnerus, G., Cavanagh, K., & Friderici, K. (2010). Confirmation and extension of association of blood lead with attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder (ADHD) and ADHD symptom domains at population-typical exposure levels. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 51, 58–65. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02135.x

Nikolas, M. A., & Burt, S. A. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 1–17. doi:10.1037/a0018010

Nikolas, M. A., & Nigg, J. T. (2015). Moderators of neuropsy-chological mechanism in attention-deficit hyperactivity dis-order. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 271–281. doi:10.1007/s10802-014-9904-7

Park, S. K., Hu, H., Wright, R. O., Schwartz, J., Cheng, Y., Sparrow, D., . . . Weisskopf, M. G. (2009). Iron metabo-lism genes, low-level lead exposure, and QT interval. Environmental Health Perspectives, 117, 80–85. doi:10.1289/ ehp.11559

Puig-Antich, J., & Ryan, N. (1986). The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (Kiddie-SADS). Pittsburgh, PA: Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic.

Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554–571.

Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C., Corley, J., McNeill, G., Davies, G., Liewald, D. C., . . . Deary, I. J. (2014). Alcohol consumption and lifetime change in cognitive ability: A gene × environ-ment interaction study. Age, 36, Article 9638. doi:10.1007/s11357-014-9638-z

Santos, P. C., Krieger, J. E., & Pereira, A. C. (2012). Molecular diagnostic and pathogenesis of hereditary hemochromato-sis. International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 13, 1497–1511. doi:10.3390/ijms13021497

Senut, M. C., Sen, A., Cingolani, P., Shaik, A., Land, S. J., & Ruden, D. M. (2014). Lead exposure disrupts global DNA methylation in human embryonic stem cells and alters their neuronal differentiation. Toxicological Sciences, 139, 142–161. doi:10.1093/toxsci/kfu028

Shelton, K. K., Frick, P. J., & Wooton, J. M. (1996). Assessment of parenting practices in families of elementary school-aged children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25, 317–329.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: From common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1231–1238.

Swanson, J., Schuck, S., Porter, M. M., Carlson, C., Hartman, C. A., Sergeant, J. A., . . . Wigal, T. (2012). Categorical and

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 13: Variation in an Iron Metabolism Gene Moderates the Association

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Blood Lead 269

dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: History of the SNAP and the SWAN Rating Scales. International Journal of Educational and Psychological Assessment, 10, 51–69.

Wang, F. T., Hu, H., Schwartz, J., Weuve, J., Spiro, A. S., Sparrow, D., . . . Wright, R. O. (2007). Modifying effects of the HFE polymorphisms on the association between lead burden and cognitive decline. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115, 1210–1215. doi:10.1289/ehp.9855

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.

Whitfield, J. B., Dy, V., McQuilty, R., Zhu, G., Montgomery, G.  W., Ferreira, M. A., . . . Martin, N. G. (2007). Evidence of genetic effects on blood lead concentra-tion. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115, 1224–1230. doi:10.1289/ehp.8847

Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., . . . Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 991–1010. doi:10.1037/a0027347

World Health Organization. (2015). Lead poisoning and health (Fact Sheet No. 379). Retrieved from http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs379/en/

Wright, R. O., Silverman, E. K., Schwartz, J., Tsaih, S. W., Senter, J., Sparrow, D., . . . Hu, H. (2004). Association between hemochromatosis genotype and lead exposure among elderly men: The normative aging study. Environmental Health Perspectives, 112, 746–750.

Zimmermann, M. B. (2008). Methods to assess iron and iodine status. British Journal of Nutrition, 99(Suppl. 3), S2–S9. doi:10.1017/s000711450800679x

at The University of Iowa Libraries on March 21, 2016pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from