what do idioms really mean

22
JOURNAL OF MEMORY AND LANGUAGE 31,485506 (1992) What Do idioms Realty Mean? RAYMOND W. GIBBS, JR. Universify of California, Santa Cruz The “dead” metaphor view of idiomaticity suggests that idioms were once metaphoric but have lost their metaphoricity over time and now are equivalent to simple literal phrases such that blow your stack = “to get very angry,” crack the whip = “to exert authority,” and spill the beans = “to reveal a secret.” The purpose of the present studies was to demon- strate that idioms are not dead metaphors but have more complex meanings that are moti- vated by conceptual metaphors linking idiom phrases with their figurative interpretations. Six experiments are reported that examine the difference between idioms and their literal paraphrases. A fust study examined people’s intuitions about different knowledge domains to illustrate that the meanings of idioms are consistent with the source-to-target domain mappings of the conceptual metaphors that motivate these phrases’ figurative meanings. The data from Experiments 2 and 3 indicate that people view idioms as having more complex meanings than do their roughly, equivalent literal paraphrases. Experiments 4 through 6 show that idioms are most appropriate to use and easiest to comprehend when they are encountered in discourse situations that are consistent with the entailments of the concep- tual metaphors that motivate these phrases’ idiomatic meanings. The findings from these studies suggest that idioms are not dead metaphors with simple figurative interpretations. Instead, idioms have complex meanings that are motivated by independently existing con- ceptual metaphors that are partly constitutive of everyday thought. D ISZ Academic press, w. One of the most persistent ideas in both “folk” and linguistic accounts of idioms is that these phrases are “dead” metaphors. It is commonly assumed that idioms were metaphorical in their origins, but have lost their metaphoricity over time and now exist as frozen, semantic units or as “dead” met- aphors. Although metaphors are lively, cre- ative, and resistant to literal paraphrase, id- ioms are dead, hackneyed expressions that are equivalent in meaning to simple literal phrases. To many scholars, classifying an utterance or phrase as “idiomatic” is tan- tamount to a theoretical explanation in it- self, since it assumes that idioms are dead metaphors and really belong to the waste- basket of formulas and phrases that are sep- This research was supported by Grant MH42980 from the National Institute of Mental Health and by a Faculty Research Grant from the University of Cali- fornia, Santa Cruz. Correspondence and reprint re- quests may be sent to Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr., Pro- gram in Experimental Psychology, Clark Kerr Hall, University of California, Santa Cruz, CA 95064. arate from the generative component of the grammar (Gibbs, 1990, in press). Most idiom dictionaries give simple deli- nitions for idioms (Boatner, Gates, & Mak- kai, 1975;Cowie, Mackin, & McCaig, 1983; Long & Summer, 1979). For instance, crack the whip is defined as “to be in con- trol,” spill the beans means “to make known a secret,” go to pieces means to “become distressed,” and blow your stack means to “become very angry.” Of course, dictionary definitions do not necessarily re- flect the complexity in people’s mental rep- resentations for words and phrases. Yet many semantic theories assume that the meanings of idioms are best represented by simple definitions because idioms are mostly dead metaphors (Cruse, 1986; Palmer, 1981). My contention, contrary to the dead metaphor view, is that idioms have complex figurative interpretations that are not arbitrarily determined but are motivated by independently existing con- ceptual metaphors that provide the founda- 485 0749-596X/92 $5.00 Copyright Q 1992 by Academic Press, Inc. AU tights of reproduction in any form reserved.

Upload: samanta-kelly-menoncin-pierozan

Post on 18-Aug-2015

239 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

idioms

TRANSCRIPT

JOURNALOFMEMORYANDLANGUAGE31,485506(1992) WhatDoidiomsRealtyMean? RAYMONDW.GIBBS,JR. Universifyof California,SantaCruz Thedeadmetaphorviewof idiomaticitysuggests thatidiomswereoncemetaphoricbut havelosttheirmetaphoricityovertimeandnowareequivalenttosimpleliteralphrasessuch thatblowyourstack=togetveryangry,crackthewhip=toexertauthority,and spillthebeans=torevealasecret.Thepurposeofthepresentstudieswastodemon- stratethatidiomsarenotdeadmetaphorsbuthavemorecomplexmeaningsthataremoti- vatedbyconceptualmetaphorslinkingidiomphraseswiththeirfigurativeinterpretations. Sixexperimentsarereportedthatexaminethedifferencebetweenidiomsandtheirliteral paraphrases.Afuststudyexaminedpeoplesintuitionsaboutdifferentknowledgedomains toillustratethatthemeaningsofidiomsareconsistentwiththesource-to-targetdomain mappingsoftheconceptualmetaphorsthatmotivatethesephrasesfigurativemeanings. ThedatafromExperiments2 and3 indicatethatpeopleviewidiomsas havingmorecomplex meaningsthandotheirroughly,equivalentliteralparaphrases.Experiments4through6 showthatidiomsaremostappropriatetouseandeasiesttocomprehendwhentheyare encounteredindiscoursesituationsthatareconsistentwiththeentailmentsof theconcep- tualmetaphorsthatmotivatethesephrasesidiomaticmeanings.Thefindingsfromthese studiessuggestthatidiomsarenotdeadmetaphorswithsimplefigurativeinterpretations. Instead,idiomshavecomplexmeaningsthataremotivatedbyindependentlyexistingcon- ceptualmetaphorsthatarepartlyconstitutiveof everydaythought.D ISZ Academicpress,w. Oneofthemostpersistentideas inboth folkand linguisticaccounts ofidiomsis thatthese phrases aredeadmetaphors. Itiscommonlyassumed thatidiomswere metaphoricalintheirorigins,buthavelost theirmetaphoricityovertime and nowexist as frozen,semantic units oras deadmet- aphors.Althoughmetaphors are lively,cre- ative,and resistant to literalparaphrase,id- ioms aredead,hackneyed expressions that areequivalentinmeaningtosimpleliteral phrases.Tomanyscholars,classifyingan utteranceorphraseas idiomaticis tan- tamounttoatheoreticalexplanationinit- self,since itassumes thatidiomsaredead metaphorsandreallybelongtothewaste- basket offormulasand phrases that are sep- ThisresearchwassupportedbyGrantMH42980 fromtheNationalInstituteofMentalHealthandbya FacultyResearchGrantfromtheUniversityofCali- fornia,SantaCruz.Correspondenceandreprintre- questsmaybesenttoRaymondW.Gibbs,Jr.,Pro- graminExperimentalPsychology,ClarkKerrHall, UniversityofCalifornia,SantaCruz,CA95064. arate fromthe generativecomponentofthe grammar(Gibbs,1990, inpress). Mostidiomdictionariesgivesimpledeli- nitionsforidioms(Boatner,Gates, &Mak- kai,1975; Cowie,Mackin,& McCaig,1983; Long&Summer,1979).Forinstance, crack thewhipis definedas tobeincon- trol,spillthebeansmeanstomake knownasecret,gotopiecesmeansto becomedistressed,andblowyourstack means tobecomeveryangry.Ofcourse, dictionarydefinitionsdonot necessarily re- flectthe complexityin peoplesmental rep- resentationsforwordsandphrases.Yet manysemantictheoriesassumethatthe meanings ofidiomsare best representedby simpledefinitionsbecauseidiomsare mostlydeadmetaphors(Cruse,1986; Palmer,1981). Mycontention,contraryto thedeadmetaphorview,isthatidioms havecomplexfigurativeinterpretations thatarenotarbitrarilydeterminedbutare motivatedbyindependentlyexistingcon- ceptualmetaphors thatprovidethe founda- 485 0749-596X/92$5.00 CopyrightQ1992byAcademicPress,Inc. AUtightsofreproductioninanyformreserved. 486 RAYMONDW.GIBBS,JR. tionformuchofoureverydaythoughtand reasoning. Therehasrecentlybeenagreatdealof researchincognitivelinguisticsandpsy- cholinguisticsthatquestionsthedeadmet- aphorviewofidiomaticity(Fillmore,Kay, &OConner,1988;Gibbs,1990;Gibbs& Nayak,1991,1989;Gibbs&OBrien,1990; Lakoff,1987;Langacker,1987;Nayak& Gibbs,1990).Thisworkgenerallysuggests thatmanyidiomsarenotsimple,dead metaphors,butactuallyretainagooddeal oftheirmetaphoricity.Forexample,the figurativemeaningsofblowyourstackand flipyourlidarespecificallymotivatedby twoindependentlyexistingconceptual mappingsinlong-termmemory-MINDIS ACONTAINERandANGERIS HEATEDFLUIDINACONTAINER. Theseconceptualmetaphorsallowspeak- erstorefertoideasaboutgettingangry throughparticularinstantiationsofthe mappingfromasourcedomain(e.g., heatedfluid)ontoatargetdomain(e.g., anger).Speakersmakesenseofidi- oms,suchasblowyourstackandflipyour lid,preciselybecausetheirmeaningscanbe motivatedbytheconceptualmappingsthat linktheindividualwordsinidiomstotheir figurativemeanings. Variousexperimentalstudieshaveinves- tigatedthepsycholinguisticconsequences ofthisconceptualviewofidiomaticity. Someresearchshowedthatpeoplestacit knowledgeofdifferentconceptualmeta- phors(e.g.,theMINDISACONTAINER, ANGERISHEATEDFLUIDINACON- TAINER)constraintheirmentalimagesfor idiomaticphrases(Gibbs&OBrien,1990). Otherstudiesdemonstratedthatpeoples metaphoricalunderstandingofemotion concepts,suchasanger,joy,sadness,and fear,facilitatesthecontext-sensitiveuse andunderstandingofidiomsindiscourse (Nayak&Gibbs,1990).Myaiminthe presentstudieswastoshowthatidiomsare notequivalentinmeaningtotheirliteral paraphrases.Numerousstudiesreportthat peopleprocessidiomsmorequicklythan theydocorrespondingliteralphrases (Gibbs,1980,1986;Gibbs&Gonzales, 1985;Gibbs,Nayak,&Cutting,1989;Or- tony,Schallert,Reynolds,&Antos,1978; Swinney&Cutler,1979).However,no workhasspecificallyexaminedhowand whyidiomsactuallydifferinmeaningfrom theirliteralparaphrases. Considerthephrasesblowyourstack, jlipyourlid,andhittheceiling.Whywould speakersuse theseexpressionstomeanto getveryangry?Myhypothesisisthat theseidiomsarenotequivalenttoasimple, literalparaphrasesuchastogetveryan- grybecausetheyhavecomplexsemantic configurationsthataremotivatedbytwo conceptualmetaphors-MINDISACON- TAINERandANGERISHEATED FLUIDINACONTAINER.TheMINDIS ACONTAINERmetaphorispartofthe moregeneralCONDUITmetaphor(Reddy, 1979),andtheANGERISHEATmetaphor comesfromthecommonfolktheorythat thephysiologicaleffectsofangerarein- creasedbodyheat,increasedinternalpres- sure,andagitation(Kovecses,1986;La- koff,1987).Thus,peoplesmetaphorical mappingofknowledgefromasourcedo- main(e.g.,heatedfluidinacontainer)onto atargetdomain(e.g.,theangeremotion) helpsthemconceptualizeinmoreconcrete termswhatisunderstoodaboutthetarget domain.Thismetaphoricalmappingpre- servesthestructuralcharacteristicsorthe cognitivetopologyofthesourcedomain (Lakoff,1990).Forexample,ourunder- standingthattoomuchheatedfluidcan causeasealedcontainertoexplodeis mappedontothetargetdomainofanger suchthatweconceptualizetheexplo- sionofsomeonesangerasbeingper- formedunintentionallyandwithgreatin- tensity.Variousspecificentailmentsresult fromthesegeneralmetaphoricalmappings, entailmentsthatprovidespecificinsight intothecauses,intentionality,manner,and consequencesoftheactivitiesdescribedby IDIOMATICMEANING487 stacksblowing,lidsflipping,andceilings beinghit. Themetaphoricalwaysinwhichwepar- tiallyconceptualizeexperiences,suchas anger,providepartofthemotivationfor whylinguisticexpressionssuchashitthe ceiling,jlipyourlid,orblowyourstack makesenseinhavingthefigurativemean- ingstheydo(Gibbs,1990;Gibbs&Nayak, 1991).Ontheotherhand,literalpara- phrasesof theseidiomssuchas togetvery angrydonotconveythesameinferences aboutthecauses,intentionality,andman- nerinwhichsomeoneexperiencesandex- presseshisorheranger.Mostliteralpara- phrasesofidiomsarenotmotivatedbythe samesetofrichconceptualmetaphorsas areidioms(Gibbs&OBrien,1990).This differencebetweenidiomsandtheirliteral paraphrasesisnotsimplyduetothefact thatidiomsarea typeoffigurativelanguage andthusmorelikelytobemotivatedby conceptualmetaphorsthanareliteralex- pressions.Indeed,manyliteralexpressions makesensetouspreciselybecausethey tooaremotivatedbyconceptualmeta- phors.Forexample,theexpressions:He attackedeveryweakpointinmyargument; Zdemolishedhisargument;andHiscriti- cismswererightontargetappeartomost speakersas beingfairlyliteral.Yeteachex- pressionismotivatedbythesameconcep- tualmetaphorwherebyargumentsareun- derstoodintermsofwars.Myclaimthat literalparaphrasesofidiomsarenotcleady motivatedbyconceptualmetaphoristhere- forenotacommentonliterallanguageper se.Rather,myhypothesisisthatsimplelit- eralparaphrasesofidioms,suchastoget veryangryortorevealthesecretare notbythemselvesmotivatedbysinglecon- ceptualmetaphorsandthereforedonot possessthekindofcomplexinterpretations as doidiomaticphrases.Thepresentexper- imentsaimedtodiscoverwhetherpeople viewedidiomsas havingdifferentmeanings fromtheirliteralparaphrases.Thesestud- iesaresignificantnotonlyforpsycholin- guistictheoriesoffigurativelanguageuse, butalsobecausetheyprovideadditional evidenceonthemetaphoricalfoundationof everydaythought. EXPERIMENT1 Thefirststudyattemptedtoprovidedata showinghowthefigurativemeaningsofid- iomsaremotivatedbyconceptualmeta- phor.Thepreviousworkonmentalimag- eryforidiomsindicatedthatpeoplehave verysimilarintuitionsabouttheactions thataredescribedbyidiomaticexpressions (Gibbs&OBrien,1990).Consideranger idiomssuchasblowyourstack,flipyour lid,andhittheceiling.Participantsinthe earlierstudiesstronglyagreedaboutthe causes,intentionality,andmannerinwhich stacksareblown,lidsareflipped,andceil- ingshitwhentheyformmentalimagesfor theseangeridioms.Thisconsistencyin peoplesintuitionsabouttheirmentalim- agesforidiomswasattributedtothecon- strainingpresenceofspecificconceptual metaphorsthatmotivatedthefigurative meaningsoftheseidioms.Fortheangerid- iomsstudied,theconceptualmetaphorAN- GERISHEATEDFLUIDINACON- TAINERprovidespartofthelinkbetween anidiomanditsfigurativemeaningandalso constrainstheinferencespeoplemake aboutwhattheseidiomsmean. Thepresentexperimentextendedthese earlierstudiestoshowthatpeoplesunder- standingofidiomaticmeaningreflectsthe particularentailmentsoftheirunderlying conceptualmetaphors.Eachconceptual metaphormapsknowledgefromaspecific sourcedomain(e.g.,HEATEDFLUIDIN ACONTAINER)ontoadissimilartarget domain(e.g.,ANGER).Myhypothesis wasthatthemetaphoricalmappingsthat motivateidiomaticmeaningspreservesthe structuralcharacteristicsofthesourcedo- main.Forexample,peoplesunderstanding ofthecauses,intentionality,andmannerof physicalevents,suchasheatingfluidin containers(i.e.,sourcedomains),shouldbe 488RAYMONDW.GIBBS,JR. similartotheirunderstandingsofthe causes,intentionality,andmannerofthe angertowhichidiomssuchasblowyour stack,jlipyourlid,andhittheceilingrefer. ThepurposeofExperiment1 wastosim- plyassesspeoplesunderstandingofthe causes,intentionality,andmannerofthe actionsindifferentsourcedomains(e.g., heatedfluidinacontainer,thebehaviorof brittleobjectsincontainers,andsoon). Theseeventscorrespondedtoparticular sourcedomainsinvariousconceptualmet- aphors(e.g.,ANGERISHEATEDFLUID INACONTAINER,IDEASAREPHYS- ICALENTITIESINCONTAINERS, THEMINDISABRITTLEOBJECT, CONTROLISPOSSESSIONOFSOME OBJECT)thathavebeenseeninprevious researchas motivatingthefigurativemean- ingsofidiomssuchas blowyourstack,spill thebeans,loseyourgrip,andlaydownthe law(Gibbs&OBrien,1990).Participants readbriefscenariosdescribingspecific sourcedomains.Thesescenariosmakeno referencetoanythingaboutidiomsortothe targetdomainstowhichidiomsrefer(e.g., anger,therevelationofsecrets,goingin- sane,etc).Afterreadingeachscenario,for example,aboutfluidinsideasealedcon- tainer,theparticipantsansweredspecific questionsregardingthecause,intentional- ity,andmannerofvariouspossibleevents, suchas whatmightcausethefluidtoescape fromasealedcontainer.Ifidiomsarepar- tiallymotivatedbyconceptualmetaphors, thenpeoplesintuitionsaboutthecausa- tion,intentionality,andmannerofaction forthesemetaphorssourcedomains shouldbeverysimilartowhatpeoplegen- erallyperceiveasbeingthefigurative meaningsoftheseidioms.Theresultsof thisstudy,therefore,providethebasisfor makingspecificpredictionsaboutwhatidi- omsmean,basedonanindependentassess- mentofpeoplesintuitionsabouttheindi- vidualsourcedomainsintheconceptual metaphorsthatmotivatethefigurative meaningsofidioms. Methods Subjects.Thirty-eightundergraduatestu- dentsfromtheUniversityofCalifornia, SantaCruzservedasparticipantsinthis study.Theyreceivedcoursecreditfortheir service.Alltheparticipantswerenative Englishspeakers. Stimulianddesign.Fourdifferentcon- ceptualmetaphorsthatmotivatedthefigu- rativemeaningsofidiomsreferringtoan- ger,insanity,exertingcontrol,andrevela- tionwereemployedas theprimarystimuli. Theseconceptualmetaphorswereprevi- ouslyanalyzedasmotivatingthemeanings ofdifferentidiomsinGibbsandOBrien (19!30)andincludedANGERISHEATED FLUIDINACONTAINER(foranger), theMINDISABRITTLEOBJECT(for insanity),CONTROLISAPOSSESSION (forexertingcontrol),andIDEASARE ENTITIESINACONTAINER(forreve- lation). Ashortscenariowaswrittentodepict thebasicelementsineachofthefour sourcedomains.Forexample,thescenario forthesourcedomainofheatedfluidina containerstatedImaginethatyouare lookingatacontainerthatisshapedlikea cylinder.Thetopofthecontainerissealed. Thecontaineriscompletelyfilledwith somesortoffluid.Followingeachsce- nariowerethreequestionsthatqueriedpar- ticipantsaboutvariouseventsrelevantto thesesourcedomains.Onequestionas- sessedpeoplesintuitionsaboutthecausa- tionofsomeevent(e.g.,Describesome- thingthatwouldcausethefluidtocome spontaneouslyoutofthecontainer).A secondquestionassessedpeoplesintui- tionsabouttheintentional@ofthatevent (e.g., Imaginethatsomethingcausedthe fluidtocomeoutofthecontainer.Doyou thinkthatthefluidcomesoutonpurposeor doesthefluidjustsomehowgetoutbyac- cident?).Afinalquestionassessedpeo- plesintuitionsaboutthemannerinwhich theeventisperformed(e.g.,Imagine IDIOMATICMEANING 489 again thatthefluidcomes outofthesealed container.Doyou thinkthe fluidcomes out inagentlemannerordoesitexplode out?).Table1 presentsthescenarios and thethreeactionquestionsforeachofthe foursource domains. Procedure.Eachparticipantwaspre- sentedwithatestbookletthatcontained the experimentalinstructionsalong withall the stimulimaterials.Theparticipantswere toldthatthepurposeofthestudywasto examinetheirintuitionsaboutsimpleob- jectsand eventsinthe realworld.Thepar- ticipants read the firstscenario and then an- sweredthethreequestionsthatfollowed. Thiswasthendoneforthesecond,third, and,finally,fourthsourcedomains.Nei- thertheexperimentinstructionsortheex- perimentersaid anythingaboutthisstudy relatingtolinguisticsor,morespecifically, tothemeanings ofidioms.Theexperiment tookapproximately15 mintocomplete. ResultsandDiscussion The participantsresponses to each ques- tion wereanalyzed in the followingmanner. First,each persons response toeach ques- tionwas analyzed forits generalcharacter- istics. Forexample,whena participantre- portedthathisorherresponse tothecau- TABLE1 SCENARIOS ANDACTIONQUESTIONS USEDINEXPERIMENT1 Sourcedomain Fluidinc1container-Imaginethatyouarelookingata containerthatisshapedlikeacylinder.Thetopof thecontainerissealed.Thecontaineriscompletelyfilledwithsomesortof fluid. Describesomethingthatwouldmakethefluidexplodeoutofthesealedcontainer.(Causation) Imaginethatthefluidisheatedtoahightemperatureandthatthefluidcomesoutofthecontainer.Do youthinkthatthefluidcomesoutonpurpose(thatisintentionally)duetoitsownwillordoesthefluidjust somehowgetoutunintentionallyorbyaccident?(Intentional&y) Imagineagainthatonceheatedtoaveryhightemperaturethatthefluidcameoutofthesealedcontainer. Doyouthinkthatthefluidcomesoutofthesealedcontainerina gentlemannerordoesitexplodeout? (Manner) Fragileobjectincontniner-Imaginethatyouhaveacontainerandinsideofitisaverybrittle,fragile object. Describehowthefragileobjectsinsidethecontainermightbreakorfallapart.(Causation) Imaginenowthatsomethinghappenstothecontainerandthatthiscausesthefragileobjectinsideto break.Doyouthinktheobjectfallsapartintentionallythroughitsownwillordoesitfallapartbyaccident? (Intentional&y) Imagineagainthatsomethinghappenstothecontainerandthatthefragileobjectbreaksorfallsapart.Do youthinktheobjectfallsapartgracefullyandslowlyordoesitfallapartallatonce?(Manner) Smallobjectsincontainer-Imaginethatyouarelookingatanothercontainer.Thecontaineris fullof many smallpiecesofsomething. Describesomethingthatwouldmakethesmallpiecesofmaterialcomeoutofthecontainer.(Causation) I fthesmallpiecesdidsomehowgetoutofcontainer,doyouthinkthiswouldhappenonpurposethrough thewillofthepiecesorwouldthishappenaccidentally?(Intentional&y) Imagineagainthatsomethinghappenstothecontainerandthatthesmallpiecesofthematerialgetoutof thecontainer.Dothesepiecesgetoutslowlyordotheysomehowgetoutofthecontainerquitequickly, perhapsallatonce?(Manner) Takingcontrolofsomeobject-Considerthesituationwhereyoutakesomeactioninordertotakesome objectoreventunderyourcontrol. Whatmakesyouexertcontrolontheobjectorsituation?(Causation) Istheactionyoudototakesomethingunderyourcontroldoneonpurposethroughyourowndesireor doesit justoccurbyaccident?(Intentional&y) Istheactionyoudototakesomethingunder,yourcontroldonegentlyandslowlyorisitperformedwith someauthorityandforce?(Manner) 490RAYMONDW.GIBBS,JR. sationquestionforthefluidinacontainer scenariowasifyouheatthefluidtoa high temperatureitmightexplodeout,thiswas scoredas referringtoheatorinternalpres- sureas themaincauseofthefluidescaping. Twoindependentjudgesexaminedthere- sponseprotocolsandinitiallyreached96% agreementastohowtheindividualpartici- pantsresponsestoquestionsshouldbe scored.Subsequentdiscussionamongthe judgesproducedcompleteagreement.In thesecondstageoftheanalysis,thediffer- entgeneralcharacteristicsforpeoplesre- sponsestoeachquestionacrosspartici- pantsweretallied.Fromthis,thesingle mostfrequentanswertoeachquestionfor eachsourcedomainwas determined.Table 2 presentstheproportionoftotalresponses acrossalltheparticipantsthatconformed tothemostfrequentlynotedresponsefor eachquestionineachsourcedomain. TheresponseslistedinTable2 generally showthatpeoplewerequiteconsistentin theirintuitionsregardingthecausation,in- tentionality,andmannerofeventsforthe fourdifferentsourcedomainsstudied.On average89%oftheparticipantsresponses wereinagreementcollapsedacrossthe threetypesofquestionsandthefoursource domains.Thisresultreflects,forexample, thefindingthatpeoplecommonlyagree thattheprobablecauseoffluidescaping outofasealedcontainerissomeinternal pressurecausedbytheincreaseintheheat ofthefluidinsidethecontainer;thatthis explosionisunintentionalbecausecontain- ersandfluidhavenointentionalagency; andthattheexplosionoccursinaviolent manner. Ananalysisoftheseintersubjectspro- portionsindicatednosignificantvariability acrossthedifferentquestions.Thepartici- pantswereinhigheragreementintheirre- sponsestothequestionsforthefragileob- jectsina containerdomainthantothesmall objectsdomain,z=2.05,p