a model of moral emotional reactions to injustice - implications for psychological well-being and...

Upload: tehnic97

Post on 01-Jun-2018

229 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    1/147

    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA

    Los Angeles

    A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice:

    Implications for Psychological Well-Being and Prosocial Action

    A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the

    requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy

    in Psychology

    by

    Sabrina Joy Pagano

    2007

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    2/147

    UMI Number: 3295730

    INFORMATION TO USERS

    The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy

    submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and

    photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper

    alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

    In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript

    and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized

    copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

    UMIUMI Microform 3295730

    Copyright 2008 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.

    All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against

    unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

    ProQuest Information and Learning Company

    300 North Zeeb RoadP.O. Box 1346

    Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    3/147

    The dissertation of Sabrina Joy Pagano is approved.

    ^ A A [ A A A ^ Ia a \~Ij a /

    Peter Bentler

    Shelly Gable Nayak

    Maia Young

    W i Q,$

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    4/147

    DEDICATION

    This dissertation is dedicated to the authors grandparents, Mr. Vincent and Josephine

    Pagano, to her father, Mr. John Pagano, and to her aunt, Ms. Anna Marie Pagano.

    iii

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    5/147

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    I. Introduction 1

    A. Emotions and their Relationship to Prosocial Action 6

    B. Origins of Moral Emotions 9C. Moral Emotions as Mediators between Focus of Attention

    and Prosocial Action 14

    1. Guilt and Reparative Action 16

    2. Empathy and Humanitarian Action 17

    3. Moral Outrage and Preventative/Retributive Action 19

    D. Psychological Consequences of Moral Emotions 21

    1. Guilt: Directed at the Self 21

    2. Empathy and Moral Outrage: Directed at the Other 23E. Potential Limits of Moral Emotions as Motivators of

    Prosocial Action 24

    F. Present Research 26

    II. Study 1: Does Helping the Victims Harm the Observers? 27

    A. Predictions 28

    1. Prediction 1 28

    2. Prediction 2 28

    3. Prediction 3 294. Prediction 4 30

    B. Method 30

    1. Participants 30

    2. Materials and Procedure 31

    a. Manipulating Focus of Attention 32

    b. Measuring Moral Emotional Reactions 34

    c. Measuring Psychological Well-Being 35

    d. Measuring Support for Prosocial Action 35

    iv

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    6/147

    e. Manipulation Checks for Responsibility,

    Perceived Discrepancy, Identification, and Perceived

    Injustice 36

    f. Manipulation Check for Focus of Attention 37

    C. Results 371. Factor Analyses on Emotion Items 38

    a. Exploratory factor analysis on emotion items 38

    b. Confirmatory factor model for emotions 38

    2. Effectiveness of the Focus of Attention Manipulation 39

    3. Does Changing Ones Focus of Attention Produce

    Different Moral Emotions? 41

    4. Moral Emotions and Support for Prosocial Action 42

    a. Predicting Humanitarian Action 42b. Predicting Reparative Action 43

    c. Predicting Preventative Action 43

    d. Predicting Retributive Action 44

    5. Moral Emotions and Observer Psychological Well-

    Being 44

    a. Predicting State Self-Esteem 47

    b. Predicting State Anxiety 47

    6. Structural Equation Model o f Moral EmotionalReactions to Injustice 48

    a. Factor Model for Prosocial Actions 50

    b. Factor Models for Measures of Psychological

    Well-Being (Pre- and Post-) 51

    7. Modeling Emotions and Prosocial Action 52

    a. Do Perceptions o f Responsibility (Blame) Predict

    Peoples Moral Emotional Reactions? 54

    b. Predicting Guilt 55

    c. Predicting Empathy 56

    d. Predicting Moral Outrage 57

    v

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    7/147

    D. Discussion 57

    III. Study 2: Injustice and Emotions in the Laboratory 63

    A. Predictions 64

    1. Prediction 1 64

    2. Prediction 2 64

    B. Method 65

    1. Participants 65

    2. Materials and Procedure 65

    a. Manipulation Check for Comprehension of

    Allocations Made for the Cash Prize 68

    b. Manipulating Focus of Attention 69

    c. Measuring Emotional Reactions 71

    d. Island Survival Task and Reactions Questionnaire 72

    e. Measuring Support for Prosocial Action 73

    f. Manipulation Checks for Perceived Injustice and

    Focus of Attention / Demographic Items 74

    C. Results 76

    1. Perceptions of Injustice for Victim and Self 76

    2. Reliability and Means of Moral Emotion Scales 77

    3. Effectiveness of the Focus of Attention Manipulation 78

    4. Does Changing Ones Focus of Attention Produce

    Different Moral Emotions? 79

    5. Effects of Emotions on Support for Prosocial Action 80

    a. Predicting Humanitarian Action 81

    b. Predicting Reparative Action and Preventative

    Action 82

    c. Predicting Retributive Action 82

    6. Perceptions of Injustice and Support for Prosocial

    Action 82

    D. Discussion 83

    vi

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    8/147

    IV. General Discussion 85

    V. Summary 95

    VI. Appendix A 98

    VII. Appendix B 109

    VIII. Appendix C 117

    IX. Appendix D 118

    X. Appendix E 123

    XI. References 124

    vii

    produced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    9/147

    LIST OF FIGURES

    Figure 1: A Model of Emotional Reactions to Injustice 6

    Figure 2: Conceptual Diagram of the Hypothesized Relationships

    between Three Types of Focus o f Attention and Corresponding

    Moral Emotions 29

    Figure 3: Conceptual Diagram o f the Hypothesized Relationships

    between Three Types of Moral Emotion and Four Types o f

    Prosocial Action 29

    Figure 4: Conceptual Diagram of Modeled Relationships Between

    Moral Emotions and Prosocial Action 54

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    10/147

    LIST OF TABLES

    Table 1. Exploratory Factor Analysis on Emotion Items 40

    Table 2. Effect of Focus of Attention on Moral Emotion 45

    Table 3. Regressions and Zero-Order Correlations Between MoralEmotions and Prosocial Actions 46

    Table 4. Regressions and Zero-Order Correlations Between Moral

    Emotions and Psychological Well-Being 49

    Table 5: Summary of Findings from Structural Equation Model of

    Moral Emotions and Prosocial Action 56

    Table 6: Regressions and Zero-Order Correlations between

    Attributions of Blame and Moral Emotions 58

    ix

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    11/147

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    This research was supported by the following: Oderberg Dissertation Year Fellowship.

    The author would like to thank her advisor, David Sears, as well as the following

    individuals for their comments, proofreading, and other input on this project at various

    stages: Peter Bentler, Karen Cheng, Shelly Gable Nayak, Matthew Hays, and Maia

    Young.

    x

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    12/147

    February 2,1978

    2000

    2001-2002

    2003

    2004-2006

    2005

    2006

    2006-2007

    2006,2007

    2007

    VITA

    Bom, Mount Vemon, New York

    B.A., Psychology, highest honors

    B.A., Political ScienceGraduated with University Distinction

    University of North Carolina

    Chapel Hill, North Carolina

    Political Psychology Fellowship

    University of California, Los Angeles

    M.A., Psychology

    University of California

    Los Angeles, California

    PROPS Graduate Mentor

    Department of Psychology

    University of California, Los Angeles

    Shepherd Ivory Franz Distinguished Teaching Award

    Department of Psychology

    University of California, Los Angeles

    Teacher Training Award

    Department of Psychology

    University of California, Los Angeles

    Oderberg Dissertation Year Fellowship

    Department of Psychology

    University of California, Los Angeles

    Instructor of Record

    Department of Psychology

    University of California, Los Angeles

    Collegium of University Teaching Fellowship

    Department of PsychologyUniversity of California, Los Angeles

    produced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    13/147

    PUBLICATIONS AND PRESENTATIONS

    Pagano, S. J. (January, 2007). Moral Emotions and Prosocial Action: Does helping the

    victims harm the observers?Poster presented at the annual meeting o f the Society

    fo r Personality and Social Psychology.Memphis, TN.

    Pagano, S. J. (February, 2003). Guilt and moral outrage: Distinct emotions with distinct

    implications for helping behavior.Paper presented at the annual meeting o f the

    Society fo r Personality and Social Psychology Justice Pre-Conference.Universal

    City, CA.

    Pagano, S. J., & Huo, Y. J. (2007). The role o f moral emotions in predicting political

    attitudes about post-war Iraq.Political Psychology, 28 (2),227-255.

    Pagano, S. J., & Huo, Y. J. (January, 2007). The role of moral emotions in predicting

    political attitudes about post-war Iraq.Paper presented at the annual meeting o f

    the Society fo r Personality and Social Psychology Justice Pre-Conference.Memphis, TN.

    Pagano, S. J., & Huo, Y. J. (January, 2005). The role of moral emotions in predicting

    policy attitudes about post-war Iraq.Poster presented at the annual meeting o f

    the Society fo r Personality and Social Psychology.New Orleans, LA.

    Pagano, S. J., & Huo, Y. J. (January, 2004). Moral outrage and guilt as predictors of

    differential helping behavior on behalf of the disadvantaged.Poster presented at

    the annual meeting o f the Society fo r Personality and Social Psychology.Austin,

    TX.

    Pagano, S. J., & Montoya, R. M. (January, 2006). Emotional aspects of dignity: Toward a

    preliminary understanding of self-worth.Poster presented at the annual meeting

    o f the Society fo r Personality and Social Psychology Emotion Pre-Conference.

    Palm Springs, CA.

    Sears, D. O., Molina, L. E., & Pagano, S. J. (July, 2004). External attacks and internal

    cohesion: Impact of September 11 on domestic interethnic relations.Paper

    presented at the International Society o fPolitical Psychology.Lund, Sweden.

    Sears, D. O., Molina, L. E, & Pagano, S. J. (April, 2003). External attacks and internal

    cohesion: The effects of September 11 on domestic interethnic relations.Paperpresented at the annual meeting o f the Midwestern Political Science Association.

    Chicago, IL.

    xii

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    14/147

    ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION

    A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice:

    Implications for Psychological Well-Being and Prosocial Action

    by

    Sabrina Joy Pagano

    Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology

    University of California, Los Angeles, 2007

    Professor David Sears, Chair

    Two studies tested a model of moral emotional reactions to injustice. Three types of

    focus of attention (self, victim, perpetrator) were examined as elicitors of guilt, empathy,

    and moral outrage, respectively. These emotions were posited to give rise to support for

    distinct forms of prosocial action (e.g., humanitarian and preventative action) and to have

    distinct implications for observers psychological well-being (i.e., state anxiety and state

    self-esteem). Results across both studies indicated weak support for the role of focus of

    attention in eliciting distinct moral emotions. In Study 1, participants moral emotional

    reactions to a written scenario about the working poor were shown to predict their

    xiii

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    15/147

    support for different types of prosocial action and to have unique patterns of association

    with psychological well-being, supporting several hypothesized relationships. Among

    these were positive associations between guilt and reparative action, between empathy

    and preventative action, and between moral outrage and both preventative action and

    retributive action. Unexpected relationships included a positive association between guilt

    and humanitarian action, a positive relationship between empathy and retributive action,

    and positive associations between moral outrage and both humanitarian action and

    reparative action. As hypothesized, participants who felt guilt reported lower state self

    esteem prior to, but not after, endorsement of prosocial action. Interestingly, participants

    feeling moral outrage (but not guilt, as hypothesized) reported higher state anxiety both

    before and after endorsement of prosocial action. Study 2 was a laboratory study that

    tested the effects of focus of attention on moral emotions and in turn the effects of moral

    emotions on support for prosocial action. Study 2 did not produce support for these

    relationships; the focus of attention manipulation was unsuccessful. Although Study 2

    participants perceived injustice in the experimental situation, this perception did not

    result in moral emotional reactions. Nevertheless, some of the overall results suggest that

    distinguishing among different types of moral emotional reactions to injustice may be

    important in predicting what kind o f prosocial actions people will support. Moreover,

    observers psychological well-being may be impacted in different ways when people

    experience different emotional reactions to injustice.

    xiv

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    16/147

    Introduction

    Whenever theys a fight so hungry people can eat, Ill be there. Whenever theys a cop

    heatin up a guy, Ill be ther e. . . . Ill be in the way guys yell when theyre mad anIll

    be in the way kids laugh when theyre hungry an they know suppers ready. An when

    our folks eat the stuff they raise an live in the houses they buildwhy, Ill be there.

    Tom Joad, Chapter 28, Grapes o f Wrath

    As the experience of Tom Joad suggests, the recognition of injustice can promote

    a powerful desire to be there and helpa desire so powerful that Tom believes it will

    extend beyond his mortal life. Powerful reactions to injustice are not limited to the

    characters in novels, however, but may occur in real life whenever injustice strikes a

    chord in its observers. What chord is being struck? Imagine the anger you might feel, for

    example, toward a boss unfairly chastising and humiliating his hardworking employee.

    Or the guilt you may experience when confronted with someone who is starving as you

    walk from the grocery store with your arms full of bags. While not everyone will live a

    life like Tom Joads, few among us are unfamiliar with the emotionsso often resulting

    when we confront injustice.

    What our experience tells us also finds support in psychological research.

    Emotional reactions to suffering and injustice (often in the form o f empathy) are a driving

    force behind willingness to enact prosocial action aimed at assisting victims (for reviews,

    see Batson, 1998, and Eisenberg & Fabes, 1991). Emotions such as guilt and moral

    outrage (i.e., anger on behalf of victims directed at perpetrators) also have come under

    examination. Of course, emotional reactions to injustice are not limited to these. When

    1

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    17/147

    injustice takes the form o f inequality, emotions like pride, disdain, pity, and moral

    indignation (e.g., in response to reverse discrimination) all are potential reactions

    (Leach, Snider, & Iyer, 2002).

    In part because guilt, empathy, and moral outrage motivate support for actions

    aimed at protecting the welfare and interest of other individuals or o f society as a whole,

    they fall into a special class called moral emotions(Haidt, 2003). Haidt presents a

    preliminary definition o f moral emotions as those that are linked to the interests or

    welfare either of society as a whole or at least of persons other than the judge or agent

    (p. 853). Moral emotions have two defining features. First, they have disinterested

    elicitors,meaning that they can be triggered even when the self (or those who are close

    to, similar to, or otherwise valued by the self) has no involvement with the eliciting event.

    Haidt proposes that the more an emotion can be prompted by a disinterested elicitor, the

    more prototypical of a moral emotion it is. Second, moral emotions have associated

    prosocial action tendencies,meaning that they motivate one to perform some goal-related

    action in response to the eliciting event. Although the action may not in fact be taken,

    experiencing a moral emotion should place one into a cognitive and motivational state

    that readies one to perform actions related to the experienced emotion; these potentiated

    actions can be described as prosocial action tendencies.

    A set of examples may be instructive in clarifying what a moral emotion is and

    what a moral emotion is not. Anger often is felt in response to direct insults or threats to

    the self. This anger may in turn prompt actions designed to correct any wrongs that have

    2

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    18/147

    been done. On other occasions, we may feel anger at someone because o f the harm she

    has brought to another. This anger may in turn motivate us to take action on the wronged

    persons behalf that also is intended at righting the wrong that has been done. Only in the

    second example can we consider the anger being experienced to be moral. In part for

    heuristic purposes, Haidt (2003) goes on to provide an alternative definition of moral

    emotions as capturing the difference between the emotional life ofHomo Sapiensand

    the emotional life ofHomo Economicus a fictitious and purely self-interested creature

    largely resembling a psychopath (pg. 855).

    Weiner (2006) instead defines moral emotions as a subset of social emotions

    (whose antecedents are found at the social leveloutside the individual) that involve a

    consideration of right or wrong, good and bad, and ought and should (p. 87). In

    Weiners view, controllability, volition, and responsibility are central features o f whether

    an experienced emotion is moral. Hence, an emotion such as sympathy may most likely

    be experienced in response to someone if he could not control his situation, did not aim

    for it to happen, and was not responsible for the situation coming about. Weiner identifies

    an exhaustive list o f 12 emotions that he considers to be moral emotions. Among these

    are emotions such as Schadenfreude (joy at the suffering of others). The inclusion o f such

    an emotion on a list of moral emotions seems in opposition to Haidts (2003) definition;

    even if the disinterested elicitor criterion were met, it is difficult to imagine that

    Schadenfreude would prompt emotions that truly serve the interests of another individual

    3

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    19/147

    or of society (although Weiner argues that Schadenfreude may serve as a signal to others

    to discontinue what they are doingan action that may not always serve others).

    Both Haidts (2003) and Weiners (2006) definitions appear to have intuitive

    appeal; the study o f moral emotions remains at a nascent enough stage that only time will

    tell which of these definitions will hold in terms o f their explanatory value and overall

    utility. For two reasons, Haidts definition is preferred for the purposes o f the present

    paper. First, the present work aims to examine emotions in response to injustice that are

    likely to prompt action intended to help the victim of this injustice in one of several

    possible ways. This aim seems to resonate most closely with Haidts approach. Second,

    although Haidts definition is not free of complications, its greater flexibility (e.g., for the

    same emotion to be understood as moral or not depending on its specific instantiation) is

    a desirable characteristic, given the labile nature of human interactions and emotional

    experience in response to injustice.

    The set o f moral emotions composed of guilt, empathy, and moral outrage have

    the benefit of addressing the main actors in situations where injustice occurs: the

    observer, victim, and perpetrator. Other emotions that lead observers to celebrate the self

    (e.g., pride) or denigrate the victim (e.g., disdain) in the face of injustice are not

    considered to be moral emotions; they are less likely than are guilt, empathy, or moral

    outrage to give rise to a willingness to help the disadvantaged. The moral emotions of

    guilt, empathy, and moral outrage perhaps can be understood best if we consider first the

    more general relationship between emotion and prosocial action and then move on to

    4

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    20/147

    consider the potential elicitors of these specific emotions. The present paper then will

    give a more detailed description of these emotions and their proposed consequences,

    wherein formal definitions will be given.

    Among moral emotions, differences in the nature and experience of these

    emotions may distinguish them from one another. For example, while prompting actions

    that improve victims welfare, some emotions may come with costs to the well-being of

    those witnessing the injustice. Emotional reactions to injustice characterized by a focus

    on the self may be associated with powerful feelings of dysphoria. Imagine once again

    our grocery store scene. I f you recognize the discrepancy between your own welfare and

    that of the other, and feel that the situation is unjust, chances are that you will not only

    feel guilty, but that your self-directed attention will lead you to feel uncomfortable in

    other ways. Other emotional reactions that direct attention away from the self also may

    be aversive to experience. Nonetheless, while you may be upset, the negativity o f your

    experience likely will be directed not at yourself but instead outward (e.g., your anger

    toward the patronizing boss). In sum, emotions that direct attention outward and away

    from the observeremotions that focus instead on the victims or the perpetrators of

    injusticemay be associated with less detrimental outcomes for observer well-being.

    The goal of the present work is to examine these potential relationships by

    presenting and testing a model o f moral emotional reactions to injustice (see Figure 1

    below). In doing so, this paper will examine both the elicitors of distinct moral emotions

    and their consequences both for observers and victims o f injustice. The present paper will

    5

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    21/147

    begin with a brief discussion o f the historical and current view o f emotions and a succinct

    overview of emotions and prosocial action. Next, the paper will move on to detail the

    various parts of the proposed model. After presenting two studies designed to test this

    model, results will be detailed and the implications o f these findings for the model, future

    work, and possible applications will be discussed.

    Figure 1: A Model o f Emotional Reactions to Injustice

    Prosocial

    Actions

    Focus Of

    Attention

    Emotions

    Psychological

    Well-Being

    Emotions and their Relationship to Prosocial Action

    Philosophers and other scholars have long debated the merits and pitfalls of

    emotions. Early analyses o f emotion appear to date back at least to Platonic times and

    later received much attention from Platos student, Aristotle, in the context o f ethics.

    Emotions or passions often were thought to arise from illogical and biological

    precursors to thought and actiona view that finds continued representation in

    psychological scholarship (for a discussion, see Zajonc, 1998). Bom from illogic,

    emotions were seen as giving rise to bias and misjudgment, and thereby were controlled

    best by reason and intellect. Emotions, then, were thought to play little role in prosocial

    or moral behavior, which was instead engendered by rational processes (Kant,

    1788/1949).

    6

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    22/147

    In stark contrast to this view, David Hume famously claimed that, reason is, and

    ought only to be the slave of the passions, and can never pretend to any other office than

    to serve and obey them (1739/1969, p.462). He later argued that emotional responses

    such as sympathy and benevolence are in fact primary motives underlying prosocial

    action (Hume, 1777/1966). Although it is the less historically popular position, this view

    finds contemporary support in the work of scholars in both social and political

    psychology (e.g., Batson, 1998; Haidt, 2003; Marcus, 2003).

    Emotions gain force as impetuses for prosocial action in part because abstract

    moral principles resulting only from cool cognitive reasoning lack the motivational force

    necessary to promote action (De Rivera, Gerstmann, & Maisels, 2002; Hoffman, 2000).

    Recent evidence from studies in neuroscience provides compelling support for this view.

    In these studies, patients suffering from bilateral damage to their amygdala suffer an

    impairment of their ability to experience emotional responses but retain complete

    cognitive functioning. Of interest is the finding that these patients, lacking an emotional

    impetus, are substantially less inclined to enact the behaviors that their reasoning tells

    them are appropriate and desired (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997; Bechara

    etal., 1995).

    If emotion is in fact necessary for the promotion of prosocial action, then it should

    form a core component of models that examine reactions to injustice. This premise gains

    some support from the following: when allowed to report their open-ended reactions to

    injustice, people often describe these reactions as being hot and emotionally laden (e.g.,

    7

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    23/147

    Bies & Tripp, 2002). Some authors have even argued that justice itself can be thought of

    as an affective event (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). A large portion of the justice

    literature nonetheless has emphasized attitudinal and behavioral, instead o f emotional,

    reactions to injustice.

    Nonetheless, when examining the vast literature on prosocial behavior and the

    more recent justice literature, it is clear that emotion can and does promote prosocial

    action aimed at alleviating injustice or other disadvantage. Emotion has been shown to

    generate helping behavior for those in need (Dovidio, Allen, & Schroeder, 1990; for a

    review, see Batson, 1998), is an important mediator of support for political actions

    helping the disadvantaged (e.g., Montada & Schneider, 1989; Pagano & Huo, 2007), and

    can even prompt reallocation o f goods from the advantaged to the disadvantaged (e.g.,

    Batson, Chang, Orr, & Rowland, 2001). Emotion appears to be associated not only with

    action, but also with the recognition of injustice and attitude change toward the person or

    group in question that often precedes this prosocial action. Both early (e.g., Homans,

    1961) and recent (e.g., Barclay, Skarlicki, & Pugh, 2005; Weiss, Suckow, & Cropanzano,

    1999) work indicates that emotions are powerfully associated with perceptions of

    injustice and can help generate more positive attitudes toward the stigmatized (e.g.,

    Batson, Polycarpou, et al., 1997; Dovidio et al.).

    As Haidt (2003) argues, distinct moral emotions should have distinct

    psychological bases and should prompt distinct action tendencies (i.e., motivations).

    Rather than producing a diffuse willingness to help in any possible way, some emotions

    8

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    24/147

    may be more strongly associated with certain forms of prosocial action than with others.

    Because of the specificity of these relationships, an emphasis on discrete emotions (rather

    than overall affective valence) may increase the likelihood o f predicting specific

    behaviors (Weiss et al., 1999). Recently, attempts have been made to specify how these

    distinct emotional responses to injustice may determine in what way people will choose

    to assist disadvantaged others (e.g., Gault & Sabini, 2000; Haidt; Iyer, Leach, &

    Pedersen, 2004; Montada & Schneider, 1989; Pagano & Huo, 2007). At the grocery store,

    you may divert your eyes, rush away, or even distract yourself with other thoughts. But

    perhaps you will give some money or food to the starving individual. Compare this

    response with the one you may have when witnessing the employee being unfairly

    castigated. Will you confront the boss? Report him to his own supervisor? Write the

    company in charge, asking that they change the way their employees are treated?

    The strength of an emphasis on studying discrete emotions is that it allows for

    clear predictions both about the types of outcomes expected and about the specific

    elicitors o f each emotion. Below, I present the proposed components of a model o f moral

    emotional reactions to injustice, identifying several hypotheses regarding the elicitors and

    outcomes of three moral emotions: guilt, empathy, and moral outrage.

    Origins o fMoral Emotions

    What might prompt these moral emotions? A preliminary suggestion is offered in

    work by Stotland (1969). He was perhaps the first to discover that two different forms of

    perspective-taking, one focused on imagining another persons experience with suffering

    9

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    25/147

    and one focused on imagining how the self would experience the others situation,

    produce distinct physiological reactions and self-reported emotion.

    Batson and his colleagues (e.g., Batson, Early, & Salvarani, 1997) furthered

    Stotlands insight by distinguishing the emotion produced by an other-focused

    perspective (i.e., thinking of how another person feels) as empathy and the emotion

    produced by a self-focused perspective (i.e., thinking of how one might feel i f one were

    in the others situation) as personal distress. There is now much evidence that these

    different emotions, following from different focuses of attention, motivate an observer to

    perform different types of prosocial action. For example, while empathy leads to a

    motivation to relieve the suffering of the other (i.e., an altruistic motivation), personal

    distress leads to a motivation to relieve ones own distress (i.e., egoistic motivation; for a

    review, see Batson, 1991). Personal distress therefore motivates and ultimately results in

    helping only when situational escape is not possible. Outside of Batson and his

    colleagues line of work, however, the role of perspective-taking as a causal determinant

    of distinctmoral emotions and subsequent prosocial behavior has been underexplored.

    Varying peoples focus of attention may differ somewhat from varying the type of

    perspective-taking they use. In both cases, it appears that the kind of information made

    most salient changes as a function o f the type of perspective or type of focus taken. For

    example, both self-focused perspective-taking and self-focused attention should increase

    availability of information relevant to the self. The precise nature of the difference

    between focus of attention and perspective-taking has not yet been specified; nonetheless,

    10

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    26/147

    both perspective-taking and focusing peoples attention may increase peoples likelihood

    of helping (Batson, 1998).

    When another persons suffering is brought about by injustice (i.e., when the

    victims treatment or outcome are unfair), moral emotions may be elicited as a function

    of ones focus o f attention. One can direct attention to the self as observer, to the victim,

    or to the third-party perpetrator (i.e., the actor harming the victim). People that attend to

    the same situation but focus on these different targets may experience distinct emotional

    responses (Leach et al., 2002; Montada & Schneider, 1989). Some researchers (e.g.,

    Haidt, 2003) even use focus o f attention to distinguish among moral emotion families

    (i.e., variations on a common emotional theme). Based on Haidts (2003)

    conceptualization, the emotions selected for the present studies fall into the following

    categories: self-conscious family (guilt), other-suffering family (empathy), and other-

    condemning family (moral outrage).

    Work by Iyer, Leach, and Crosby (2003) provides a preliminary experimental

    demonstration of the relationship between moral emotions and focus of attention. In two

    studies that examined support for different forms of affirmative action policy, Iyer et al.

    found that guilt was elicited by a self-focused orientation to the problem of racial

    inequality, while sympathy was elicited by an other-focused orientation toward racial

    inequality. Work by Pagano and Huo (2007) corroborates this finding by demonstrating

    that guilt, empathy, and moral outrage were associated with self-focused, other (or

    victim)-focused, and perpetrator-focused attention, respectively.

    11

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    27/147

    The general conception that consideration of different types o f information (as

    when focus o f attention is varied) may result in different emotional reactions is supported

    by cognitive appraisal theories o f emotion. Cognitive appraisal theories hold that it is

    secondary appraisals o f context, such as attributions of agency (self versus others) that

    form the basis of differentiation among different emotions (Smith & Pope, 1992, as cited

    in Weiss, Suckow, & Cropanzano, 1999). The idea that emotions can be elicited by

    considering situation-relevant information also is consistent with the cognition-affect

    sequence posited in Weiners model of attribution (Weiner, 1986).

    Based on these findings, some arguments about the relationships between

    different focuses of attention and the emotions of guilt, empathy, and moral outrage can

    be made. First, a focus on the role played by oneself or ones ingroup regarding harm

    done to the victim should elicit guilt. As discussed in the present paper, guilt can be

    understood as a dysphoric feeling arising from a consideration of ones real or imagined

    social or moral transgression. Interestingly, although guilt involves a belief that one has

    caused harm, loss, or distress to another(Hoffman, 1982), its defining feature o f self

    focus ultimately directs attention inward (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988; Salovey &

    Rosenhan, 1989; Tangney, 2003; Weiner, 1982). In other words, guilts self-focus leads

    those experiencing it to attend less to the other who has been wronged and more to their

    own feelings about the transgression (Baumeister, Reis, & Delespaul, 1995). This internal

    focus brings about a state in which the self is seen as the object of experience, thereby

    making environmental effects on the self of primary importance and consideration, and

    12

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    28/147

    making self-preservation concerns most salient (Cohen, Dowling, Bishop, & Maney,

    1985; Hoffman, 1998; Iyer et al., 2003; Tobey-Klass, 1978; for a discussion relating self

    focus to personal distress, see Batson, Early, et al., 1997).

    Although feelings of guilt typically arise in recognition of personal responsibility

    for a wrongdoing or inequity, they also can arise in the absence o f responsibility, based

    on association with an ingroup (for a discussion o f guilt by association, see Doosje,

    Branscombe, Spears, & Manstead, 1998), or even through virtual or existential means

    (Montada & Schneider, 1989; for a discussion, see Hoffman, 2000). Hoffman, for

    example, details how guilt may arise over achievement, affluence, or even relative

    advantage. A unifying element among different experiences of guilt is the recognition

    that the victim has been unjustly underbenefited, persecuted, or otherwise harmed,

    particularly when compared to the status enjoyed by those experiencing guilt.

    Focusing attention instead on the victims suffering should elicit empathy (e.g.,

    Hoffman, 2000; Batson, 1991). The word empathy was first coined by Titchener in 1909

    as a translation of the German Einfuhlung, which was used in perceptual contexts in

    reference to the process whereby an event is seen from the inside (Batson). Hence, the

    term empathy itself implies taking on the victims perspective when considering his or

    her plight. This link has been well established in prior research, primarily by Batson and

    his colleagues (e.g., Batson, Early, et al., 1997). The link between empathy and other-

    focus also is suggested by evidence outside the context of the laboratory. For example,

    13

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    29/147

    rescuers in Nazi Germany, for whom empathy was a principal motive, reported taking

    others, rather than themselves, as their primary focus (Oliner & Oliner, 1988).

    Finally, focusing ones attention on the role of a third-party perpetrator should

    elicit moral outrage. While moral outrage is similar to guilt in its recognition of and

    attributions of blame for injustice, it differs from guilt in its assignment of blame to an

    external or third-partyperpetratorresponsible for the victims harm (Iyer et al., 2004).

    The perpetrator can be identified as a person or regime or can be symbolically

    represented by the political or other system as a whole in which the perpetrator is acting.

    Moral outrage shares with empathy an outward-directed focus that moves the focus of

    attention away from the self. Consistent with this conceptualization, Hoffman (2000)

    notes that, if someone else is the cause of the victims plight, attention may be directed

    away from the victim and toward the perpetrator. There also is some indication that Nazi

    rescuers motivated by moral outrage reserved their strong emotions for those violating

    justice and were less focused on victims, having relatively impersonal relationships with

    them (Oliner & Oliner, 1988). Although no experimental tests of the link between

    perpetrator focus and moral outrage have been made to date, survey data provide some

    preliminary support for the link between them (Pagano & Huo, 2007).

    Moral Emotions as Mediators between Focus o fAttention and Prosocial Action

    Guilt, empathy, and moral outrage are not solely the outcome o f different types of

    focus, but may also serve as important mediators between focus of attention and support

    for different forms of prosocial action.

    14

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    30/147

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    31/147

    Nuremberg Trials are one famous example of bringing retribution to those that

    perpetrated injustice. Despite the unique importance o f each of these four forms o f

    prosocial action, the distinctions between and among them and their predictors have been

    largely unexamined (for one exception, see Pagano & Huo, 2007).

    Guilt and Reparative Action

    Guilt can be defined as the dysphoric feeling resulting from the recognition that

    one has violated a moral or social standard that is personally relevant (Kugler & Jones,

    1992). Characteristic of guilt is an inward-focus, which incorporates internal attributions

    of responsibility (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). These attributions o f responsibility can be

    directed at the self or at a group to which one belongs when this group is responsible for

    an immoral act against another group (Iyer et al., 2004; Leach et al., 2002). This so-called

    guilt by association(Doosje et al., 1998) can be contrasted with theguilt over relative

    advantageoften felt when ones advantage over another seems unjustified or arbitrary

    (Hoffman, 2000). Returning once again to our grocery store example: we might feel guilt

    by association i f a close relative or friend mocked the starving individual. We instead

    might feel guilt over relative advantage simply thinking o f the discrepancy between our

    own situation and the situation of the other. What ties the two forms of guilt together is

    their common focus on the self and on acknowledged transgression.

    Because guilt arises from the acknowledgement of wrongdoing, it may assist in

    the regulation of positive social relations between the self and others (e.g., De Rivera,

    1984). Guilt thereby helps ensure that one fits in and does not draw the enmity of

    16

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    32/147

    others. In part because of the potential damage to ones social standing that feelings of

    guilt over ones real or imagined transgression imply, it is highly uncomfortable to view

    oneself as responsible for an immoral act (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1994).

    A motivation to engage in prosocial action may arise as one means of relieving

    the aversive state associated with feelings of guilt. Because guilt is posited to follow from

    a se lf focus and to produce a desire to eliminate negative feelings quickly, the prosocial

    motivation is likely to involve action thatspecificallymakes reparation for one's real or

    virtual transgression (Doosje et al., 1998; Haidt, 2003; Hoffman, 2000; Iyer et al., 2004;

    Pagano & Huo, 2007). Prosocial actions prompted by guilt therefore are designed to

    restore the moral value of both the victim and the person in whom guilt has been

    generated through the use of apology, confession, or other forms o f compensation (Iyer et

    al.; Minow, 1998). For example, Doosje et al. found that Dutch students in whom guilt

    was induced when they were reminded of their nations past colonial behavior were most

    willing to support national compensation (i.e., reparative action) for the formerly

    colonized country. Similar findings were obtained by Iyer et al. (2003) in a study

    examining the role o f White guilt in promoting support for compensatory affirmative

    action programs for African Americans. They also found that guilt did not predict support

    for non-compensatory forms o f affirmative actioneven low-cost programs aimed solely

    at providing equal opportunity.

    Empathy and Humanitarian Action

    17

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    33/147

    Empathy can be defined as an emotional response congruent with the perceived

    welfareo f another person (but not necessarily congruent with how the other person feels).

    The category of potential emotional responses therefore is quite broad, changing based on

    the other persons situation. An empathic response to anothers suffering, for example,

    may be characterized by feelings such as sympathy, compassion, and tenderness (Batson,

    1991). Empathy (as an emotion) is distinct from the cognitive process of perspective-

    taking that frequently gives rise to this congruent emotional response (Batson, 1991).

    Although the sensitivity to others suffering that follows from empathy has been called a

    basic feature of human nature (e.g., Smith, 1759/1956; Hume, 1739/1969; Piaget,

    1932/1965, as cited in Haidt, 2003), feelings of empathy may be strongest when a

    victims disadvantage is seen as illegitimate, stable, and uncontrollable (Leach et al.,

    2002).

    Because of these feelings of compassion and tenderness, empathy prompts an

    altruistic motivation that has as its ultimate goal the relief of the others distress and the

    provision o f comfort (for a review, see Batson, 1991). However, while empathy has been

    associated with a wide array o f prosocial actions aimed at accomplishing this broad goal

    (Batson, 1998), it may be particularly effective at motivating helping behaviors aimed at

    relieving the specific need for which it is felt (Blader & Tyler, 2002; Dovidio et al.,

    1990). For example, empathys focus on the victims plight is likely to prompt actions

    designed to provide immediate and effective relief of this suffering, such as humanitarian

    action (Pagano & Huo, 2007).

    18

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    34/147

    Empathy may be less strongly associated with other forms of prosocial action,

    such as support for the implementation of political and other reforms aimed at longer-

    term and therefore less direct assistance (Pagano and Huo, 2007). Indeed, in both survey

    (Montada & Schneider, 1989) and experimental (De Rivera et al., 2002) studies, empathy

    was unassociated with support for political action aimed at aiding victims of injustice.

    Empathys focus on the victim also should reduce the likelihood of support for retributive

    actions, which instead are directed at external forces contributing to a victims plight

    (e.g., third-party perpetrators) (e.g., Pagano & Huo, 2007; for a review o f empathys role

    in aggression, see Miller & Eisenberg, 1988). Despite its limitations, empathys strength

    nonetheless lies in its other-directed focus, which frees it o f the accompanying personal

    distress associated with a self-focused perspective o f the victims situation (Batson,

    Early, et al., 1997).

    Moral Outrage and Preventative/Retributive Action

    Moral outrage can be defined as anger directed at a third-party perpetrator that is

    felt on behalf of a victim who has experienced illegitimate harm or insult (Haidt, 2003;

    Hoffman, 2000; Leach et al., 2002; Montada & Schneider, 1989; Vidmar, 2000). For

    example, moral outrage may be felt when a greedy CEO takes advantage of his or her

    employees or a political tyrant brings about the suffering of his or her people (Pagano &

    Huo, 2007). The insult or violation also can be an affront to community values (Miller,

    2001). In situations that prompt people to experience moral outrage, attributions of

    responsibility for injustice are made to an individual other than the self (Tangney &

    19

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    35/147

    Dearing, 2002). For example, swastika graffiti left by neo-nazi groups may result in

    moral outrage when its symbolism conflicts with a communitys belief in tolerance.

    Moral outrage is perhaps the least understood of the moral emotions, as it often may be

    linked incorrectly with more destructive and egoistically motivated forms of anger, such

    as those resulting from frustration and goal blockage (Haidt, 2003). As Aristotle first

    noted, moral outrage instead arises as an emotional response to a conspicuous slight

    directed without justification''and can be felt either for oneself or on behalf of others

    (Haidt, 2003).

    Moral outrage is not simply a response to wounded pride, but instead is a specific

    response to injustice that results in a desire to punish the third-party perpetrator (Haidt,

    2003; Vidmar 2000). When a perpetrator violates moral codes o f conduct, prosocial

    actions such as victim compensation may be insufficient to restore a sense o f justice;

    additional punishment may be necessary in order to re-establish shared social norms and

    values (Averill, 1982; Montada & Schneider, 1989; Tyler, Boeckmann, Smith, & Huo,

    1997; Weiner, 1985). Retributive action therefore may be sought in order to rectify

    injustice and to uphold desired social norms (Gault & Sabini, 2000; Pagano & Huo, 2007;

    Vitaglione & Barnett, 2003). It is perhaps for this reason that philosopher John Stuart

    Mill described moral outrage as the guardian of justice (as cited in Hoffman, 2000, p.

    96).

    Moral outrage also may be associated with support for actions designed to prevent

    perpetrators possiblefuture offenses. Because moral outrage does not involve a focus on

    20

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    36/147

    the sel f s role in the injustice, those experiencing moral outrage can more easily call for a

    change in the existing societal or political structure causing harm (Iyer et al., 2004). One

    way of calling for change is through support for preventative action or politic or societal

    reformation. Findings from at least three separate studies (Montada & Schneider, 1989;

    Pagano & Huo, 2007; Pedersen, Iyer, & Leach, 2002) provide support for the association

    between moral outrage and political action.

    Given their lack of focus on righting moral wrongs perpetrated by a third-party,

    humanitarian and reparative actions aimed at direct assistance to the victims should be

    unrelated to moral outrage (Pagano & Huo, 2007). It should also be noted that moral

    outrage is conceptually different from other forms of anger (e.g., self-reproachful anger)

    that are based on assuming personal responsibility for injustice, which would arguably

    have different consequences for prosocial action.

    Psychological Consequences o fMoral Emotions

    Guilt: Directed at the Sel f

    Despite the considerable benefits to those disadvantaged by injustice, some moral

    emotional reactions to injustice may come with consequences for the psychological well

    being of the person experiencing the moral emotion. For example, a rich research

    tradition in self-awareness (which can be conceptualized as self-focused attention)

    supports the idea that attentional focus on the self can be aversive, particularly when the

    self is subject to negative evaluation. This discomfort may be increased further when

    ones attention is directed at the self not as a relatively benign actor, but instead as a party

    21

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    37/147

    responsible for an immoral act, thereby resulting in guilt (Baumeister, Stillwell, &

    Heatherton, 1994). Like other states characterized by self-awareness, feelings of guilt

    (and the concomitant cognition that one has caused injustice to befall another) can in

    some cases create a painful sense of dysphoria or unease. This dysphoria has been

    described as a sinking feeling (Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Roseman, Wiest, &

    Schwartz, 1994). Dysphoria, which is in fact considered by some researchers to be a core

    component of guilt, has been described as a subclinical form of psychological distress;

    feelings of dysphoria ultimately may become manifest in high scores on depression and

    anxiety measures (Bibb, 1996).

    Other work also supports the idea that guilt should be associated with decrements

    in psychological well-being. For example, Derakshan and Eysenck (2001) found that

    self-focused attention (a large component o f guilt) led to increased feelings o f anxiety,

    which is widely considered to be detrimental to psychological well-being in the long

    term. Guilt also has been associated more directly with decreased well-being in a number

    of different contexts (e.g., Atkins, Martin, & Poon, 1996; Joseph, Hodgkinson, Yule, &

    Williams, 1993; O'Connor, Berry, & Weiss, 1999; Saravanan, 2002). In other work,

    subjectively negative states such as regret, concern about misdeeds, and even

    dissatisfaction with one's self also have been associated with feelings of guilt (Institute

    for Personality & Ability Testing, 1974, as cited in Atkins, Martin, & Poon, 1996). It

    seems clear, then, that guilt is associated with a wide range of arguably undesirable

    psychological outcomes.

    22

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    38/147

    This discussion of the possible effects of guilt on psychological well-being is

    limited to two different measures of well-being: state anxiety (on which the effects of

    guilt are more general) and state self-esteem (on which the effects of guilt are more

    specific). First, state anxiety is predicated on an appraisal of threatening demands or

    dangers, such as the threats to self-worth inherent in guilts attributions of personal

    responsibility (Lazarus, 1991). At the most general level, guilt should be associated with

    unpleasant emotional arousal, such as that experienced in state anxiety. While frequently

    examined as dependent variables in the same study, however, the direct relationship

    between state anxiety and guilt has been surprisingly unexplored.

    The other measure of well-being that should be negatively influenced (at a more

    specific level) by guilt is state self-esteem; the self-focused attention associated with guilt

    may intensify attributions o f personal responsibility for harm committed (Federoff &

    Harvey, 1976). Findings from a recent study provide support for this claim,

    demonstrating that self-focused attention affected multiple indicators of state self-esteem

    (Jimenez, 2001), although the direct relationship between state self-esteem and guilt in

    response to injustice has not been examined.

    Empathy and Moral Outrage: Directed at the Other

    Empathy, which results in feelings of warmth and tenderness toward another (and

    directs attention away from the self; e.g., Worthington & Scherer, 2004), may in some

    cases and in some ways even be pleasant to experience. Even if the actual experience of

    empathy is unpleasant, however, people nonetheless may be pleased that they are

    23

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    39/147

    experiencing it (Batson, 1991). Moral outrage also is considered an outward-focused

    emotion (Barclay et al., 2005). Moral outrage, like other forms o f anger, typically may be

    aversive to experience. In contrast to guilts focus on the self, the outward-directed (i.e.,

    on the victim or third-party perpetrator) focus o f other emotions, such as empathy and

    moral outrage, should insulate the observer from dysphoria. In the case o f empathy, this

    insulation may be complete. Moral outrage instead may be associated with levels of

    psychological well-being intermediate between guilt and empathy.

    Potential Limits o fMoral Emotions as Motivators o f Prosocial Action

    Of course, the ultimate judgments about when and how to act do not exist in a

    vacuum. The potential costs associated with the experience of moral emotions may

    inhibit prosocial action. For example, aware of the costs that sometimes result from

    feeling empathy for others, people may try to avoid situations (and a focus on the victim)

    that would elicit this emotion even before encountering appeals for helping and explicit

    considerations of cost (Batson, 1998; Hodges & Wegner, 1997). The pedestrian averting

    her eyes from a homeless person on the street or the woman who looks at her newspaper

    rather than attend to the suffering of the employee being berated by his boss both may be

    demonstrating an implicit understanding that the experience of empathy comes with the

    cost of prosocial acts such as humanitarian action. People may (not infrequently) expose

    themselves to empathy-eliciting information (e.g., immersing themselves in news

    broadcasts after an atrocity), enjoying the experience o f this emotion. Eventually,

    24

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    40/147

    however, when the costs o f their empathy (e.g., a compunction to act) become clearer,

    they may discontinue their viewing.

    As a form o f anger, moral outrage should be associated with a high degree of

    arousal (e.g., Ramirez & Andreu, 2006), and is theorized to prompt seemingly high-cost

    forms of helping (e.g., preventative action). People aware of the association between their

    feelings of moral outrage and their resulting desire to provide high-cost help also may

    limit their exposure to situations or people eliciting this emotion. Despite these limits,

    however, the outward-focused nature o f both empathy and moral outrage should insulate

    those experiencing these emotions from the more direct costs to self that are incurred by

    self-focused emotions such as guilt.

    Because o f the inherent aversiveness in the experience of guilt, the effectiveness

    of appeals based on guilt may be limited even without considerations of cost.

    Experiencing guilt or other self-focused emotions often may be avoided (for reviews, see

    Carver & Scheier, 1981, and Wicklund, 1975) and feelings of guilt therefore are rare

    (Iyer et al., 2004). One way in which guilt can be evaded is through cognitive

    justification (e.g., hes probably not really starving anyway). Another form of cognitive

    justification is victim derogation, whereby victims are dehumanized (Baumeister et al.,

    1994) or blamed for their plight (Branscombe, Owen, Garstka, & Coleman, 1996). People

    of course also may avoid guilt by distancing themselves physically or psychologically

    from the victims (Batson, 1998). For example, in a study by Iyer et al., participants in a

    self-focus (i.e., guilt-inducing) condition were more likely than those in an other-focus

    25

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    41/147

    (i.e., empathy-inducing) condition to refuse responsibility for and deny the presence of

    racial discrimination, thereby avoiding guilt.

    When feelings o f guilt cannot be avoided, people will be motivated to provide

    help in the form o f reparative action to those harmed as a way o f alleviating guilt-related

    distress (e.g., Salovey, Mayer, & Rosenhan, 1991; Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow,

    1996). When this negative state or dysphoria can otherwise be relieved, helping should

    no longer occur (Cialdini, Darby, & Vincent, 1973). Hence, even guilts limited effects in

    the promotion of reparative action also may dissipate in the face of easier and more

    attractive alternatives to making one feel better.

    Present Research

    Two studies sought to add to our knowledge of the elicitors of three moral

    emotional reactions to injustice (guilt, empathy, and moral outrage) and o f their

    consequences for prosocial action and observer psychological well-being by testing a

    model of moral emotional reactions to injustice. The first study, a web-based experiment,

    examined the effects o f a focus-of-attention manipulation on moral emotional reactions to

    a member of a disadvantaged group. Study 1 also examined the relationships between

    different moral emotional reactions and two forms of psychological well-being, both

    before and after participants endorsement of several forms of prosocial action. The

    second study was intended to be a high-impact laboratory experiment designed to

    examine more directly the causal mechanisms behind these moral emotions through the

    use of an alternative manipulation of focus of attention. Study 2 also aimed to provide a

    26

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    42/147

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    43/147

    reactions to the working poor and several distinct types of prosocial action (e.g.,

    humanitarian aid) aimed at assisting this group. The third study goal was to offer a

    preliminary examination o f the relationship between these moral emotions and the

    psychological well-being o f observers both before and after ratings o f support for various

    forms of prosocial action. The final study goal was to examine whether participants

    feeling guilt will report decreases in psychological well-being before, but not after,

    endorsement of prosocial action. The relationship between specific focuses of attention

    and specific moral emotions, and in turn between specific moral emotions and specific

    prosocial action are represented in the conceptual diagrams below.

    Predictions

    Prediction 1

    The first prediction for this study was that varying the type o f information on

    which participants were focused would produce different moral emotional reactions to

    injustice. Specifically, a self-focus would produce guilt, a victim-focus would produce

    empathy, and a perpetrator-focus would produce moral outrage.

    Prediction 2

    The second prediction concerned the relationship between each of the moral

    emotions and support for four distinct types of prosocial action. Specifically, guilt would

    be associated with support for prosocial action aimed only at reparative action. Moreover,

    empathy would be associated with support for humanitarian, reparative, and preventative

    28

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    44/147

    Figure 2: Conceptual Diagram of the Hypothesized Relationships between Three

    Types of Focus o f Attention and Corresponding Moral Emotions

    Self-Focus Guilt

    Victim-Focus Empathy

    Perpetrator- Moral

    Focus Outrage

    Figure 3: Conceptual Diagram of the Hypothesized Relationships between Three

    Types of Moral Emotion and Four Types of Prosocial Action

    Reparative Action

    Humanitarian Action

    Reparative Action

    Preventative Action

    Retributive Action

    + + + and

    Moral Outrage

    Guilt

    Empathy

    Moral

    Outrage

    forms of prosocial action, and with opposition to retributive action (i.e., punishment).

    Finally, moral outrage would be associated with support for both preventative and

    retributive action.

    Prediction 3

    29

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    45/147

    The third prediction for this study was that different moral emotional reactions to

    injustice would have different effects on observer psychological well-being. Compared

    with empathy and moral outrage, guilt would result in greater detriment to observers

    psychological well-being. Specifically, guilt would produce increased scores on state-

    anxiety and decreased scores on state self-esteem. In contrast, empathy would not

    produce changes in peoples scores on either state anxiety or state self-esteem. Finally,

    moral outrage (an emotion more aversive than empathy but less aversive than guilt)

    would result in scores on the two measures of psychological well-being that would be

    located somewhere in between the effects produced by experiences of empathy and guilt.

    Prediction 4

    The fourth and final prediction for this study was that participants experiencing

    guilt as a result of induced self-focus would be motivated to alleviate any associated

    reductions in their psychological well-being by endorsing a prosocial action. Once they

    had the opportunity to endorse prosocial action, they would no longer experience reduced

    psychological well-being. Participants experiencing guilt would not report increased

    scores on state-anxiety and decreased scores on state self-esteem after they had the

    opportunity to endorse prosocial action.

    Method

    Participants

    Participants were 200 introductory psychology students at the University of

    California, Los Angeles (125 women, 74 men; 1 did not report) who received partial

    30

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    46/147

    credit toward the fulfillment o f an optional course requirement. Participants were

    recruited via email based on their responses to a family income question during mass

    testing at the beginning o f the academic term. Participants indicating a family income of

    $80,000 or higher were selected for the study in order to increase the likelihood that the

    discrepancy between the students own situation and the situation o f the disadvantaged

    person about whom they read was a salient one. The sample o f participants reflected the

    ethnic diversity of the general student body, with 1.5% African Americans, 35% Asians,

    4.5% Latinos, 48.5% Whites, and 10% who indicated their ethnicity as other. The

    sample tended to be liberal, with a mean o f 3.08 (SD=1.33) on a 7-point scale of

    ideology, with lower scores representing greater liberalism. Using a randomized block

    procedure, participants were assigned to a control condition or one o f three experimental

    conditions: self-focus, victim-focus, or perpetrator-focus.

    Materials and Procedure

    Participants completed the study online, which helped ensure participant

    anonymity and reduce social desirability concerns and on average did not appear to

    negatively impact participants conscientiousness (e.g., in terms o f number o f items or

    response consistency). After reading an informed consent that presented this study as a

    study concerned with peoples emotional reactions to a group of people presented in a

    written passage and with their opinions about several ways of assisting the people

    described, and with how peoples self-perceptions may relate to these emotional

    31

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    47/147

    reactions and opinions on assistance, participants read the study introduction (modified

    from Batsons work; e.g., Batson, Sager, et al., 1997):

    Today, you will read one of several test articles written for a new column

    called News from the Personal Side, which is designed to capture human-interest stories.

    The goal of this series of articles is to go beyond the facts of news events

    to report how these events affect the lives of the individuals involved,

    thereby taking a more personal approach to news reporting. In todays

    article, you will read about a group of people called the working poor. By

    working poor, we mean that these people are regularly employed, but

    are nonetheless living below the poverty line.

    Prior research has shown that an important factor affecting peoples

    reactions to news media is the way in which they approach this media.Along those lines, we ask that you give your complete attention to reading

    the news article, really immersing yourself in the story presented to you.

    This will help us to understand better your reactions to it and to the people

    being described in this case, the working poor. When you are ready to

    read the article, move on to the next page.

    Manipulatingfocu s o fattention. In fact, all participants read a similar fictitious

    article on the plight o f the working poor in the United States that differed only on the

    manipulation of focus of attention. Each version of the article (see Appendix A)

    contained general information about the working poor and about the plight o f one

    woman, Lynn Townsend, in particular. This basic information comprised the victim-

    focus condition, and thereby contributed to feelings of empathy.

    The other two conditions additionally included information designed to focus

    participants attention on information relevant to the self (self-focus) or to a third-party

    perpetrator (perpetrator-focus). Specifically, the self-focus condition contained

    32

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    48/147

    information about how the relatively well-off (with whom the participants ostensibly

    would identify) had contributed to the plight of the working poor. For example,

    participants were provided with information about the percentage o f income typically

    taken up by rent among the working poor, thereby allowing participants to make a

    comparison with their own situation and in turn to experience guilt. A manipulation

    check verified the accuracy of the assumption that participants would perceive a

    discrepancy between their own situation and the situation o f the working poor.

    Finally, information regarding the role of corporate abusers was included in the

    perpetrator-focus condition in order to provide an identifiable third-party perpetrator of

    the victims suffering, thereby contributing to feelings of moral outrage. For example,

    participants read about indignities imposed on the working poor by the corporations that

    employ them. ..including constant surveillance, random searches o f personal belongings

    and random drug tests possibly including protocols requiring workers to strip down to

    their undergarments and void their bladders in front of aides or technicians.

    The control condition was designed to disperse participants attention across the

    various types o f information and thereby result in a more diffuse emotional response to

    which the other conditions could be compared. This objective was accomplished by

    adding to the basic information all o f the additional information provided in the guilt and

    moral outrage versions of the article.

    The different types of information (self, victim, third-party perpetrator) presented

    in the different versions o f the stimulus article thereby established the basis for the

    33

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    49/147

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    50/147

    Measuring psychological well-being.At two points during the study (both before

    and after the measure of support for prosocial action), participants rated the degree to

    which they agreed with several statements assessing their state psychological well-being.

    This was one o f two major dependent measures. A modified version o f the state anxiety

    subscale of the well-validated State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger,

    Gorsuch, & Luschene, 1983) was used to measure state anxiety. Participants were asked

    to indicate the extent to which 20 different emotional states described how they are

    feeling right now on a four-point scale (1 = not at all, 4 = very much). Self-esteem was

    measured on a 6-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 6 = strongly agree) with ten items

    from the well-validated Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale (Rosenberg, 1979). This scale was

    adapted to reflect state self-esteem by asking participants to think about their responses to

    each of the statements right now. Both scales were split into subscales comprising

    equal numbers of items, which then were used in analyses as pre- and post-measures of

    each construct (for a list of pre- and post- items, see Appendix B). The full-scale alpha

    for state self-esteem was .90, while the alpha for the pre-measure was .82, and the alpha

    for the post-measure was .84. The full-scale alpha for state anxiety was .93, while the

    alpha for the pre-measure was .89 and the alpha for the post-measure was .88.

    Measuring support fo r prosocial action.Next, participants read, We are

    interested in your honest opinions about the experiences of the working poor about whom

    you have just read, and your views about the appropriateness of different types of actions

    aimed at helping to improve their situation. Please share your opinion by selecting for

    35

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    51/147

    each question a number based on the scale given below. Participants then completed

    items assessing their willingness to endorse each o f four types of prosocial action (all

    measured on a 9-point labeled scale with anchors at 1 = strongly disagree, 9 = strongly

    agree). This scale, which was modeled on work by Pagano and Huo (2007), comprised

    the second major dependent measure. Humanitarian action(5 items) assessed

    participants willingness to provide basic support (e.g., we should offer basic aid when

    possible to alleviate the immediate suffering o f the working poor), while reparative

    action(7 items) assessed participants willingness to compensate the working poor (e.g.,

    the well-off should support the replenishment of programs aimed at helping the working

    poor).Preventative action(7 items) assessed participants willingness to support system

    change (e.g., Laws against unfair labor practices should be developed in order to help

    the working poor), while the six items on retributive actionassessed participants

    willingness to punish the victims third-party perpetrator (e.g., We should blow the

    whistle on corporate leaders placing monetary gain over human welfare"). Alphas were

    computed for each prosocial action: a) humanitarian action (a = .86); b) reparative action

    (a =.84); c) preventative action (a = .83), and d) retributive action (a = .86). (For a list of

    prosocial action items, see Appendix B.)

    Manipulation checks fo r responsibility, perceived discrepancy, identification, and

    perceived injustice.After completing the dependent measures, participants completed

    several manipulation checks before being debriefed and thanked for their participation.

    The first of the dependent measures was a question assessing the degree to which

    36

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    52/147

    participants thought distinct actors (the well-off, the working poor, and corporations)

    were responsible for the plight of the working poor. Participants also completed questions

    assessing the salience o f the discrepancy between their situation and that o f the working

    poor, their degree of identification with the well-off and with corporations, and their

    perception o f injustice.

    Manipulation checkfo r focus o f attention. Finally, four items were designed to

    assess the degree to which participants took each of three focuses of attention or

    concentrated about equally on all possible actors. Participants were asked to indicate the

    extent to which they concentrated on themselves, corporations, the working poor, or

    about equally on all actors when reading the passage about the working poor.

    Results

    Prior to computing any o f the internal or substantive analyses, frequency analyses

    first were computed on several items in order to determine whether data from some

    participants should be excluded from analysis. Participants were excluded under the

    following conditions: 1) they did not perceive the situation of the working poor to be

    unfair (9 participants); 2) they did not identify with the well-off (5 participants); 3) they

    identified strongly with corporations (2 participants), or 4) they felt that they were

    disadvantaged compared to the working poor (1 participant). It is noteworthy that over

    50% of the sample believed that the working poor were at least partially to blame for

    their situation. In light o f this finding, no participants were excluded based on their

    37

    roduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    53/147

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    54/147

    used as indicators of latent variables of the same names. The conceptually and

    empirically related emotion factors were allowed to correlate since they should all

    motivate prosocial action. The results were consistent with the EFA, demonstrating that

    the three emotions represent three distinct constructs. Although the Chi-square value was

    significant,^2 (167, N = 171) = 283.64,p

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    55/147

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    56/147

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    57/147

    participants experienced more guilt in the self-focus condition than in the other

    conditions (M= 2.80 vs.Ms= 2.19,2.30, and 2.57 for the victim-focus, perpetrator-

    focus, and control conditions, respectively; /(179) = 2.20,p

  • 8/9/2019 A Model of Moral Emotional Reactions to Injustice - Implications for Psychological Well-being and Prosocial Action

    58/147

    action. Surprisingly, empathy did not emerge as a significant predictor o f humanitarian

    action (fi= .10, ns).Also contrary to predictions, increases in guilt were associated with

    corresponding increases in support for humanitarian action (ft= .18,p< .05). Finally,

    moral outrage surprisingly also was the strongest predictor of humanitarian action (fi =