a resource for eliciting student alternative conceptions: examining the adaptability of a concept...

Upload: anthony-petrosino

Post on 07-Jul-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    1/29

    Metadata of the article that will be visualized in OnlineFirst

     

    1 Article Title A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining

    the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Natural Selection at the

    Secondary School Level

    2 Article Sub- Title

    3 Article Copyright -

    Year 

    Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

    (This will be the copyright line in the final PDF)

    4 Journal Name Research in Science Education

    5

    Corresponding

    Author 

    Family Name Lucero

    6 Particle

    7 Given Name Margaret M.

    8 Suffix

    9 Organization Santa Clara University

    10 Division Department of Education

    11 Address 500 El Camino Real, Santa Clara 95053, CA, USA

    12 e-mail [email protected]

    13

    Author 

    Family Name Petrosino

    14 Particle

    15 Given Name Anthony J.

    16 Suffix

    17 Organization The University of Texas at Austin

    18 Division

    19 Address Austin, TX, USA

    20 e-mail

    21

    Schedule

    Received  

    22 Revised  

    23 Accepted

    24 Abstract The Conceptual Inventory of Natural Selection (CINS) is an example of a

    research-based instrument that assesses conceptual understanding in an area

    that contains well-documented alternative conceptions. Much of the CINS’s

    use and original validation has been relegated to undergraduate settings, but

    the information learned from student responses on the CINS can also

     potentially be a useful resource for teachers at the secondary level. Because of 

    its structure, the CINS can have a role in eliciting alternative conceptions and

    induce deeper conceptual understanding by having student ideas leveraged

    during instruction. In a first step toward this goal, the present study further 

    investigated the CINS’s internal properties by having it administered to a

     

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    2/29

    group (n = 339) of students among four different biology teachers at a

     predominantly Latino, economically disadvantaged high school. In addition,

    incidences of the concept inventory’s use among the teachers’ practices were

    collected for support of its adaptability at the secondary level. Despite the

    teachers’ initial enthusiasm for the CINS’s use as an assessment tool in the

     present study, results from a principal components analysis demonstrate

    inconsistencies between the original and present validations. Results alsoreveal how the teachers think CINS items may be revised for future use among

    secondary student populations.

    25 Keywords separated

     by ' - '

    Concept inventory - Alternative conceptions - Evolution education

    26 Foot note

    information

     

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    3/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    1

    23

    4A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions:

    5Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory6for Natural Selection at the Secondary School Level

    7Margaret M. Lucero1

    & Anthony J. Petrosino2

    8

    9# Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

    10

    11Abstract   The Conceptual Inventory of Natural Selection (CINS) is an example of a research-

    12 based instrument that assesses conceptual understanding in an area that contains well-

    13documented alternative conceptions. Much of the CINS’s use and original validation has been

    14relegated to undergraduate settings, but the information learned from student responses on the

    15CINS can also potentially be a useful resource for teachers at the secondary level. Because of 

    16its structure, the CINS can have a role in eliciting alternative conceptions and induce deeper 

    17conceptual understanding by having student ideas leveraged during instruction. In a first step

    18toward this goal, the present study further investigated the CINS’s internal properties by19having it administered to a group (n = 339) of students among four different biology teachers

    20at a predominantly Latino, economically disadvantaged high school. In addition, incidences of 

    21the concept inventory’s use among the teachers’  practices were collected for support of its

    22adaptability at the secondary level. Despite the teachers’ initial enthusiasm for the CINS’s use

    23as an assessment tool in the present study, results from a principal components analysis

    24demonstrate inconsistencies between the original and present validations. Results also reveal

    25how the teachers think CINS items may be revised for future use among secondary student 

    26 populations.

    27Keywords   Concept inventory. Alternative conceptions . Evolution education

    28

    29Among different science domains, concept inventories (CIs) (e.g., see Hestenes et al.  1992;

    30Klymkowsky et al.   2003;   Q1Smith et al. 2008) are   “research-based instruments designed to

    31measure student conceptual understanding in areas where students are known (through

    32rigorous research) to hold common misconceptions”  (Garvin-Doxas et al.  2007, p. 277). CIs

    Res Sci Educ

    DOI 10.1007/s11165-016-9524-z

    *   Margaret M. Lucero

    [email protected]

    1 Department of Education, Santa Clara University, 500 El Camino Real, Santa Clara, CA 95053, USA

    2 The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    4/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    33are traditionally relegated to undergraduate settings, but they can be a valuable resource for 

    34teachers and other educators in secondary school settings. Within a secondary school context,

    35the value of CIs can be realized by probing what students know about a topic, as opposed to

    36other forms of large-scale assessment, like traditional high-stake state tests that do not 

    37necessarily probe for deep conceptual understanding (National Research Council [NRC]382001). The distracter answer choices found on CIs are composed with students’   thoughts

    39and ideas in mind, especially since the item development is guided by students’  rationale for 

    40specific responses and analyses of written, open-ended answers to related questions

    41(Richardson  2005). Because each potential answer reveals where student understanding of 

    42the phenomena differs from accepted knowledge (Garvin-Doxas et al.   2007), secondary

    43educators can potentially use the information they gain about their students to better plan

    44lessons (e.g., instructional activities and assessments) for conceptual understanding.

    45One such CI that could potentially be used as an additional resource for formative

    46assessment that elicits student ideas is the Conceptual Inventory of Natural Selection (CINS)47(Anderson et al.  2002). Like other CIs, the CINS was initially developed within an under-

    48graduate setting in order to aid instructors in identifying alternative conceptions with a concept 

    49that often presents challenges for students to learn. Even though the CINS has been used

    50mostly in undergraduate settings with published findings from over 75 articles and conference

    51 proceedings, we believe that its adaptability and usefulness in secondary biology classrooms is

    52an underutilized formative assessment opportunity. In this manuscript, we explore this forma-

    53tive assessment possibility with findings from an empirical study in which we attempted to

    54adapt the CINS for use in a secondary school setting, specifically with a group of teachers and

    55students from a large high-minority, low-socioeconomic high school. The secondary setting is

    56all the more important because improving student understanding of natural selection is a 

    57central portion of any general life science/biology course from middle school through college.

    58Having effective research-based and classroom-tested assessment tools to monitor student 

    59understanding in this area is essential since most, if not all, students find natural selection a 

    60challenging topic to master. We present a case where four teachers administered the CINS to

    61their respective students and reported how the CINS was used in their classrooms. The

    62findings build on previous research regarding the CINS’s development (Anderson et al.

    632002) and validity and reliability (Anderson et al.  2002; Nehm and Schonfeld   2008), with

    64the additional voices and concerns about such an instrument ’s use from in-service science

    65teachers.

    66Background on CIs

    67CIs were first developed as an instructional tool in the field of undergraduate physics and had a 

    68significant impact in advancing the field of physics education research. The force concept 

    69inventory (FCI) was the first CI to be developed (Hestenes et al.  1992). The FCI is a 29-

    70question assessment that focuses on probing students’  understanding of Newtonian and non-

    71 Newtonian concepts about force. It was designed to measure six conceptual dimensions of the

    72concept of force that were considered essential for a college level understanding of physics

    73(i.e., kinematics; kinds of forces; the superposition principle; and Newton’s first, second, and

    74third laws). The FCI was credited with being the vehicle for implementing important reforms

    75in undergraduate physics education, such as the development of a model of peer instruction

    76(Mazur   1997). It was also fairly revolutionary in demonstrating that student learning gains

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    5/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    77were greater with interactive pedagogy as compared to more traditional lecture-style methods

    78(Hake 1998; Freeman et al. 2014). The FCI aided in promoting discussions about pedagogy in

    79many academic circles. Other CIs, such as the force and motion conceptual evaluation

    80(Thornton and Sokoloff  1998), were created, but few have had as much widespread influence

    81and use as the FCI (Smith and Tanner  2010).82For all their potential in offering an additional form of assessment to teachers, CIs still have

    83issues that warrant consideration. Researchers in physics education, for example, have con-

    84tinually discussed whether or not CIs actually measure the conceptual understanding they are

    85designed to assess (Smith and Tanner  2010). Among the papers that have discussed this issue

    86(i.e., Heller and Huffman 1995; Hestenes and Halloun 1995; Huffman and Heller  1995), there

    87were claims that the FCI was perhaps measuring student intuitions in physics rather than a 

    88deep conceptual understanding of the different conceptual dimensions of the force concept. In

    89fact, after undergoing a factor analysis, the FCI did not yield a robust mapping of test items

    90onto each predicted conceptual force dimension (Huffman and Heller   1995). From these91results, Huffman and Heller proposed that the FCI may be measuring student understanding

    92within contextualized scenarios and not more global conceptual understanding. For example,

    93students may have more familiarity with questions on the physics of hockey pucks, and this

    94may explain why these questions group together on a particular component during a factor 

    95analysis as opposed to being grouped according to a deep conceptual understanding of what 

    96these questions were intending to assess.

    97Other aspects of CIs may potentially limit their usefulness in assessment, including the

    98vocabulary CI use and the format they employ (Smith and Tanner  2010). Some CIs’ use of 

    99content-specific jargon may obscure the conceptual understanding that the CIs are sup-

    100 posed to reveal. Smith and Tanner describe one CI’s use of the terms  positive control  and

    101negative control  in a question that probes students’ understanding of the scientific method.

    102They argue that without a working knowledge of what these terms mean, students would be

    103unable to demonstrate their conceptual understanding of the scientific process and exper-

    104imental design. Hence, a student ’s understanding of experimental design would actually go

    105unnoticed because of a jargon-filled question, thus potentially resulting in a threat to a CI’s

    106validity and reliability.

    107The Conceptual Inventory of Natural Selection

    108The CINS consists of three reading passages and 20 closed-response (multiple-choice)

    109questions with a series of distracters derived from alternative conceptions that have been

    110researched extensively. For example, many students will equate biological fitness with

    111strength, speed, intelligence, or longevity, when, in fact, biological fitness incorporates

    112organisms’   ability to survive and reproduce. In another example, as opposed to under-

    113standing that most populations are normally stable in size except for seasonal fluctuations,

    114many students will tend to think that all populations grow in size over time or fluctuate

    115widely and randomly (Anderson et al. 2002). Additional examples have been documented,

    116including alternative conceptions dealing with how members of a population exhibit 

    117variation (students thinking that all members of a population are nearly identical or 

    118variations do not influence survival) or how traits are inherited (students having the idea 

    119that traits acquired during an organism’s lifetime will be inherited by offspring). Other 

    120related alternative conceptions are comprehensively discussed elsewhere (e.g., see Gregory

    1212009). Each reading passage on the CINS describes a brief background of a particular 

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    6/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    122 population of organisms (e.g., the Galapagos finches) and establishes the context for the

    123series of questions that follow it. Ten concepts or components (biotic potential , population

    124 stability, limited  (natural ) resources, limited survival , variation within a population, origin

    125of variation, variation is inherited , differential survival , change in population, and origin

    126of species) related to natural selection are represented on the CINS (two questions per 127concept).

    128Similar to the FCI, there have been discussions surrounding the CINS’s validity and

    129reliability (see Nehm and Schonfeld 2008, 2010; Anderson et al. 2010). A main concern with

    130the CINS surrounds findings from Nehm and Schonfeld’s (2008) principal component analysis

    131(PCA), which was conducted on a population of biology majors and examined the internal

    132structure of the CINS by seeing how different questions mapped on different components (or 

    133natural selection concepts in the CINS’s case). In contrast to Anderson et al.’s (2002) original

    134PCA sample of community college non-majors, Nehm and Schonfeld did not find strong

    135support  “for the different (PCA) components representing distinct evolutionary concepts”136(p. 1145). In fact, Nehm and Schonfeld found only one component that  “included a highly

    137correlated suite of key concepts” (p. 1145). Anderson et al. (2010) acknowledge that more

    138“PCA should be conducted with additional populations to clarify this situation so that 

    139items can be refined as needed”  (p. 356).

    140 Nehm and Schonfeld (2008, 2010) argue that for all the value the CINS’s authors claim

    141the instrument possesses, it was originally validated on just one population of students and

    142strongly suggest the CINS needs to be continually explored for its efficacy and

    143generalizability among students from different racial and ethnic groups, geographic

    144regions, socioeconomic and language backgrounds, and content preparations. In a 

    145response to Nehm and Schonfeld on this point, Anderson et al. (2010) claimed the CINS

    146has been  “appropriate for assessing the knowledge of high school students, biology non-

    147majors, and biology majors at ethnically diverse institutions” (p.356). However, Nehm and

    148Schonfeld (2010) countered this claim by asserting that none of the findings from such

    149administrations of the CINS have yet been published or peer reviewed. The current study

    150aims to fulfill this research gap. In addition, considering no study currently exists that 

    151explores how teachers make use of the instrument, we argue that supplementing the

    152CINS’s validity and reliability with findings that reveal its classroom utility among

    153secondary science teachers will add another dimension to the practical value of concept 

    154inventories as pedagogical and assessment tools.

    155Theoretical Basis of CIs

    156The premise of CI development and use is based on student ideas. According to Piaget ( 1983),

    157student ideas and alternative conceptions are the raw material of classroom learning and they

    158may be refined, shaped, revised, connected, and built upon by both teachers and students alike.

    159As opposed to being viewed as obstacles to learning that must be overcome, pre-conceived

    160student ideas can be viewed as assets. It is worth noting that the framework document for the161 Next Generation Science Standards (NRC 2012) has placed emphasis on students’ ideas to be

    162used in this manner:

    163164Some of children’s early intuitions about the world can be used as a foundation to build

    165remarkable understanding, even in the earliest grades. Indeed, both building on and

    166refining prior conceptions is important in teaching science at any grade level. (p. 30)

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    7/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    167168Student Ideas of Biological Phenomena   Secondary students’   explanations for biological

    169 phenomena originate from the body of knowledge they possess as young children. In their 

    170article reviewing young children’s naïve theory on biology, Hatano and Inagaki (1994) state

    171that there are three components that constitute a naïve biology: (1) knowledge about the living/ 

    172non-living and mind/body distinctions, (2) inference for predicting biological behavior by173making use of personification, and (3)   “a non-intentional causal explanatory framework for 

    174 behaviors needed for individual survival and bodily processes” (p. 173). This third component 

    175refers to children possessing an intermediate form of causality (i.e.,  vitalistic causality) to

    176explain biological phenomena because they cannot yet offer mechanical explanations with

    177 physiological mechanisms. When children reason with a vitalistic causality, they explain that a 

    178 biological phenomenon is caused by an organism’s internal organ(s) activating its  “agency” in

    179the form of an unidentified substance, energy, or information. Hatano and Inagaki hypothesize

    180that vitalistic causality is quite similar to a teleological-functional explanation for biological

    181 processes and most likely originates from children’s use of personification; in that, children try182to understand biological phenomena by attributing human-like characteristics to target objects.

    183As is the case with naïve ideas, children’s naïve theory on biology allows them to problem

    184solve and make sense of the biological phenomena they encounter on a daily basis. In fact,

    185children immediately access personification and vitalistic causality when they are introduced

    186to a biological concept and the easy accessibility of the second and third components of this

    187naïve theory continues to hinder the development of evolutionary ideas as children get older.

    188If students are to learn about evolution and other biological concepts in a meaningful way, a 

    189restructuring of the naïve theory is required. As students get older, their use of personification

    190and vitalistic causality should change toward more scientific explanations as they learn about 

    191inferences based on a complex biological hierarchy and physiological mechanisms (Hatano

    192and Inagaki 1994). Indeed, various researchers (e.g., Danish et al.  2011; Dickes and Sengupta 

    1932013) have investigated students’   reasoning through complex biological phenomena and

    194found conceptual growth along this dimension with elementary students while they were

    195engaged and participating in ecosystem simulations (i.e., ecosystems with honeybees and

    196 birds-butterflies, respectively). Furthermore, prior research indicates that high school and

    197college students can also obtain deeper conceptual growth with complex biological phenom-

    198ena (i.e., population dynamics) when participating in such models and simulations (Wilensky

    199and Reisman 2006).

    200Rationale for CIs   Rather than viewing alternative conceptions as problematic and un-

    201 productive, the research presented here adopts the view that these ideas are useful in

    202different contexts, especially when other novice ideas are involved (Elby   2000;   Q2Smith

    203et al. 1993). These novice ideas may also be flawed, but they may be refined and

    204developed for mature understanding (diSessa  1994). Given appropriate instruction, these

    205novice ideas may be productive in the learning process. Science teacher learning about the

    206role and value of student ideas may be described as a learning progression which has upper 

    207and lower anchors with multiple pathways between them that are possible (Duncan and

    208Hmelo-Silver   2009;   Q3 NRC 2007). The lower anchor could represent an acceptance that 

    209students’  ideas play a role in learning, and the upper anchor may be considered a more

    210sophisticated view of student ideas as the raw material of learning, with successful

    211elicitation and incorporation being integral parts of a teacher ’s practice. When considered

    212as a whole, this progression approach to viewing student ideas could represent a poten-

    213tially important shift in how teachers think, especially when one enters the teaching

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    8/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    214 profession with a conception that science knowledge is transferable from one individual to

    215another (Duncan and Hmelo-Silver  2009).

    216If used as resources during instruction, alternative conceptions can foster further growth and

    217development of ideas and ultimately lead to meaningful understanding of scientific concepts

    218(Elby 2000; diSessa  1994; Scott et al.   2007; Larkin 2012). Alternative conceptions that are219elicited and used for learning are closely tied to different formative assessment efforts of 

    220scientific concepts (Black and Wiliam 1998a , b), but these efforts may also access personal,

    221environmental, and social resources as well (Cohen et al. 2003), which then may bring about a 

    222metacognitive awareness in students about their alternative ideas (Larkin 2012). In this sense,

    223students receive opportunities to compare their alternative frameworks with other ideas when

    224they offer explanations, make arguments, and provide justifications (Beeth and Hewson 1999;

    225Hennessey 2003; Duckworth 2006).

    226The importance of being aware of students’  worldviews, beliefs, and alternative concep-

    227tions cannot be underestimated, and many methods, such as journal writing, concept maps,228student questioning, small-group work, word associations, and CIs, have been proposed as

    229instructional strategies for teachers to use in order to elicit student ideas (Mintzes et al.  2000;

    230van Zee et al. 2001; Hovardas and Korfiatis 2006; Anderson et al.  2002). Once elicited, the

    231resources present in students’   alternative conceptions can then be leveraged for conceptual

    232understanding (e.g., Rivet and Krajcik   2008) with different instructional strategies.

    233 Nevertheless, it should be noted that CIs’  utility as a resource is only valuable as instruction

    234that allows students to construct new representations of complex scientific phenomena (Duschl

    235et al. 2007; Lehrer et al. 2000; Lehrer and Schauble 2006).

    236As opposed to other formal varieties of assessment (i.e., high-stake state tests), which often

    237do not relay valuable information about student alternative conceptions to teachers, CIs have

    238the potential to stimulate discussions among teachers about student learning because of their 

    239goals in probing conceptual understanding. In using the recently developed Host Pathogen

    240Interaction Concept Inventory (HPI-CI) among undergraduates, Marbach-Ad et al. (2010)

    241discovered that the instrument became  “the best catalyst ”  (p. 415) to get instructors to begin

    242discussions about student learning. The HPI-CI results brought about internal professional

    243development opportunities with the various instructors, and Marbach-Ad et al. went on to say

    244that  “As a teaching community, we found that the HPI-CI anchored and deepened discussions

    245of student learning…Confronting our expectations of student learning with student responses

    246challenged us to think and converse in a reflective manner ” (p. 415). Nevertheless, it is the goal

    247of the education community that CIs are actually measuring what they intend to measure so

    248that reliable and accurate information can be effectively discussed and used by teachers.

    249Therefore, it is incumbent upon education researchers to investigate the various properties of 

    250CIs among different populations.

    251The present study aligns itself within this vein of research inquiry; in that, it explores the

    252adaptability of a CI, namely, the CINS, for use at the high school level by answering the

    253following research questions:

    254RQ1: From when it was originally validated on a group of undergraduate students, what 

    255comparisons can be made about the CINS’s internal validity when it is now administered

    256to a group of high school students?

    257RQ2: As observed through alignment with CINS concepts, to what extent did a group of 

    258 biology teachers use the concept inventory’s pre-assessment results to guide and inform

    259instruction on evolution by natural selection?

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    9/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    260

    261

    262Materials and Methods

    263Data collection took place with 339 students enrolled in every biology class (n =22)

    264offered in a science department at a large (approximately 1700 students) urban high school

    265in the southwest USA. Four teachers (100 % of biology teachers in department) partici-266 pated in the study. The study site serves grades 9 – 12 and is located within a predominantly

    267(81 %) Latino community. At the time of data collection, about 90 % of the study site ’s

    268student population was of Latino origin (compared with state average of 50 %), 69 % were

    269classified as at-risk (state average = 45 %), 14 % demonstrated limited English proficiency

    270(state average = 17 %), and 87 % were economically disadvantaged (state average = 60 %).

    271According to the state in which the study site is located,   “at-risk ”  is defined as someone

    272who meets any one of 13 different criteria that may place a student at risk of dropping out of 

    273school. These criteria include non-advancement from one grade to the next, previous

    274expulsion, demonstrating limited English proficiency, being pregnant or a parent, home-275lessness, failure to maintain a passing average in two or more subject areas, and under the

    276custody/care of child protective services. In addition, a student is considered economically

    277disadvantaged if he/she is eligible for free or reduced-price lunch under federal guidelines

    278(State Education Agency 2015).

    279According to the study site’s state guidelines, a student who demonstrates limited English

    280 proficiency (or is an English language learner) possesses a primary language other than

    281English and has difficulty performing ordinary class work in English. The students in the

    282 present study who received the   “English language learner ”   classification may have been

    283receiving official sheltered or bilingual support in other content areas from the study site but 

    284not in their biology instruction (other than the instructional strategies with which the teacher 

    285 participants were familiar).

    286Investigating CINS’s Validity in a High School Context

    287In order to investigate the CINS’s adaptability for high school use, the original instrument 

    288underwent slight modifications using feedback that was generated when the original version

    289was administered to a group of approximately 15 – 20 volunteer 11th grade students enrolled in

    290a general chemistry class at the study site 1 year prior to formal data collection taking place291(Authors 2012). Specifically, the modified version of the original CINS had various vocabu-

    292lary terms explained (e.g., iridescent = reflective) that may have posed difficulty to high school

    293students, included illustrations of the animals from each CINS reading passage, and removed

    294citations. See separate attached appendix for a full copy of the modified version given to

    295students. A reading passage that provides some background on the Galapagos finches and a 

    296sample question with answer choices from the original CINS are shown below.

    297298Scientists have long believed that the 14 species of finches on the Galapagos Islands

    299evolved from a single species of finch that migrated to the islands one to five million

    300years ago (Lack 1940). Recent DNA analyses support the conclusion that all of the

    301Galapagos finches evolved from the warbler finch (Grant, Grant, and Petren 2001;

    302Petren, Grant, and Grant 1999). Different species live on different islands. For example,

    303the medium-ground finch and the cactus finch live on one island. The large cactus finch

    304occupies another island. One of the major changes in the finches is in their beak sizes

    305and shapes, as shown in this figure.

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    10/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    306307

    308309What would happen if a breeding pair of finches was placed on an island under ideal

    310conditions with no predators and unlimited food so that all individuals survived? Given

    311enough time

    312

    313(a) The finch population would stay small because birds only have enough babies to replace314themselves.

    315(b) The finch population would double and then stay relatively stable.

    316(c) The finch population would increase dramatically.

    317(d) The finch population would grow slowly and then level off.

    318Here is the same reading passage and sample question with answer choices from the

    319modified version in the present study.

    320321Scientists have long believed that the 14 species of finches on the Galapagos Islands

    322evolved from a single species of bird that came to the islands one to five million years323ago. Recent DNA studies support the conclusion that all of the Galapagos finches

    324evolved from the warbler finch. Different species live on different islands. For example,

    325the medium-ground finch and the cactus finch live on one island. The large cactus finch

    326lives on another island. One of the major differences between the finches is in their beak 

    327sizes and shapes, as shown in the picture below.

    328

    329330

    331332What would happen if a breeding pair of finches was placed on an island under ideal

    333conditions with no predators and unlimited food so that all individuals survived? Given

    334enough time

    335

    336(a) The finch population would stay small because birds only have enough babies to replace

    337themselves.

    338(b) The finch population would double and then stay relatively the same.

    339(c) The finch population would increase dramatically.

    340(d) The finch population would grow slowly and then level off.

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    11/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    341Adjusting the CINS’s reading passages and answer choices with the suggested changes

    342improved its overall readability for the current study’s student population of ninth-grade

    343 biology students. The original and modified CINS versions were analyzed for readability

    344using the Flesch-Kincaid Reading Level and Flesch Reading Ease tests (Kincaid et al.  1975).

    345With the Flesch-Kincaid, a score closest to zero indicates easier readability. The exact opposite346is true with the Flesch Reading Ease score (FRES), where a score closest to 100 indicates

    347easier readability. The original version of the CINS had a Flesch-Kincaid grade level score of 

    3489.7 (indicating that an average ninth-tenth grader could understand its text) and a FRES of 53,

    349which was slightly beyond the upper limit of what an average 13- to 15-year-old student could

    350easily understand. The modified CINS had a slightly lower Flesch-Kincaid grade level score

    351(9.4), and a higher FRES of 56, indicating the text was somewhat more  “on par ” with what an

    352average ninth grader (in his/her second semester) could understand.

    353The CINS was administered to the biology students as a pre-test approximately three to four 

    354class meetings before the students’  respective teacher ’s instructional unit on evolution began355(which occurred at the mid-point of the academic year ’s second semester). In order to promote

    356thoughtful and carefully chosen answers among the students, every teacher used the pre-test as

    357a form of extra credit on various assignments of the teachers’  choosing. The teachers made

    358their students aware of this incentive through an in-class announcement before the pre-test was

    359administered. Although the students were administered a post-test as well, pre-test information

    360is reported here because of the precedent established by Anderson et al., in which the original

    361CINS was administered as an in-class pre-test before any instruction on natural selection

    362concepts had begun. After each teacher ’s students were initially assessed with the CINS, the

    363students

    ’  results were compiled and distributed to each teacher. Each teacher received an

    364overall breakdown of his/her students’  results according to each question on the CINS (see

    365example shown in Fig. 1).

    366In order to answer RQ no. 1, the modified CINS underwent a PCA with the student 

    367 participants (n = 339), who were mostly (>95 %) enrolled as ninth graders. A PCA is a data 

    368reduction procedure that helps to interpret data in a more meaningful form by reducing a 

    369number of variables to a few linear combinations of the data. Each linear combination then

    370corresponds to a principal component, and taken together, principal components can highlight 

    371similarities and differences in data (Jackson 1991). This technique is particularly useful when

    372using data with a number of dimensions (as is the case with the CINS’s 10 different conceptual

    373categories). The original CINS, which was validated on a population of undergraduate

    374community college students (n = 206) not majoring in biology, had also undergone a PCA,

    375and its results demonstrated   “strong support for the internal validity of [its] underlying

    376measurement structure” (Anderson et al. 2002, p. 968). The present PCA was conducted using

    377SPSS statistical software.

    378Investigating Teachers’ Use of the CINS

    379The teachers who participated in investigating the CINS’s adaptability for their classes all

    380taught biology to >95 % of the study site’s freshmen (ninth graders). All four teachers had

    381varying amounts of experience. Participants are referred to by the pseudonyms teachers A, B,

    382C, and D. Personal participant data is found in Table  1.

    383RQ no. 2 used a variety of data sources which mainly included (a) observations of the four 

    384 biology teachers in their classrooms during their evolutionary instructional units with their 

    385students and common planning meetings and (b) individual interviews (both before, during,

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    12/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    386and after their instructional units) with these teachers regarding their personal perspectives on

    387the CINS, evolution, science teaching, student knowledge, and their classroom strategies for 

    388teaching evolution. All interviews were audio recorded, transcribed, and member checked for 

    389accuracy. While the teachers’ views on the CINS’s classroom utility emerged as themes from

    390the different interviews, it was still necessary to track each teacher ’s participation during

    391common planning meetings and instruction during classroom events to gauge how (or if) the

    392students’ pre-test performance on each of the CINS concepts influenced the teachers’ practices.

    393The teachers officially met once a week for common planning sessions (with the exception

    394of teacher C who cited personal reasons for not attending any of the meetings). This 45-min395 period was built within their schedules and designated as a time for the teachers to share lesson

    396 plans and resources. How this time was used may have had an influence on the instructional

    397strategies and activities that were used for the evolutionary instructional unit. Therefore, it was

    398important to take note of any conversations among these teachers that centered on the students’

    Answer problems are grouped according to the natural selection concept they are designed to

    address. Answer choices with (*) are correct. All other answer choices are alternative

    conceptions students may possess.

    PER. 1N=8

    PER. 2N=21

    PER. 4N=14

    PER. 5N=20

    PER. 7N=18

    TOTALN=81

    AVG. #

    CORRECT

    ANSWERS

    5.63 6.38 7.36 6.85 7.39 6.81

    AVG. SCORE 28.13% 31.9% 36.79% 34.25% 36.94% 34.07%

    #1

    Biotic Potential

    PER. 1

    N=8

    PER. 2

    N=21

    PER. 4

    N=14

    PER.5

    N=20

    PER. 7

    N=18

    TOTAL

    N=81

    A

    Organisms only replace themselves.13% 10% 7% 20% 6% 11%

    B

    Populations level off. 0% 14% 14% 30% 6% 15%

    C*

    All species have such great potential

    fertility that their population size would

    increase exponentially if all individuals

    that are born would again reproduce

    successfully.

    63% 5% 57% 35% 89% 42%

    D

    Populations level off. 25% 71% 21% 15% 0% 28%

    Fig. 1   Example of student pre-test summary results presented to teacher B

    t1:1   Table 1   Teacher participant personal data for current study

    t1:2   Teacher Years of biology

    teaching

    experience

    Highest 

    degree

    earned

    Undergraduate major No. of biology

    classes

    teaching

     No. of 

     biology

    students

    t1:3   A 7 BS Zoology 6 94

    t1:4   B 3 BS Biology and land surveying 5 81

    t1:5   C 2 BA/BS Chemistry, biology, and biochemistry 5 82

    t1:6   D 1 semester BS Biology 6 82

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    13/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    399CINS results and how they may have informed the teachers’ lesson planning. The first author 

    400attended and observed all of these meetings recording events and topics of conversation in the

    401form of field notes. Each of these meetings was audio recorded, transcribed, and member 

    402checked for accuracy.

    403A schedule was used to consistently observe an afternoon class from each teacher during404his/her instructional unit. As a result, the following teachers’  classes were formally observed

    405and video recorded by the first author: teacher D’s fifth period, teacher C’s sixth period, teacher 

    406B’s seventh period, and teacher A’s eighth period. With the exception of teacher C’s pre-AP

    407 biology sixth period class, all observed classes were regular biology classes. All observations

    408lasted the entire length of each class. Classes were observed and video recorded only when the

    409teachers were present. Each teacher ’s instructional unit spanned 9 – 10 days. All classes were

    410approximately 45 – 50 min in length and scheduled to meet every day.

    411RQ no. 2 was analyzed largely by a review of the extent with which each teacher 

    412incorporated CINS concepts into his/her practice and how any information from the CINS413(the instrument itself or student results) was used to plan and/or revise lessons and reflect upon

    414the instructional unit on evolution. The overall presence of the CINS’s concepts in the

    415teachers’ classes was measured by reviewing their instructional activities and video transcripts

    416for these classes. Statistical significance of the concepts’  presence was determined through a 

    417chi-squared test. Further examination of the concepts’ presence was made by determining the

    418number of teacher-student interactions that occurred during random portions (up to 30 %) of 

    419each teacher ’s observed classroom instruction. That is, each time a teacher initiated a formal

    420question or made a statement that incorporated the use of a CINS concept during these

    421 portions, that particular interaction was counted as a distinct instance in which a CINS concept 

    422was used. In general, teacher or student follow-up questions were not included in the  “CINS

    423interactions”  count because they were still considered to be in the main line of conceptual

    424thought during the entire interaction. These portions were independently coded by the first 

    425author and a recent doctoral graduate in science education with a coding scheme (see Table 2)

    426that used the operationalized definitions of the previously mentioned CINS concepts from

    427Anderson et al. (2002).

    428Both coders achieved an inter-rater agreement of >95 %, and any differences were resolved

    429 by discussion. For an example of how one such interaction was coded with the coding scheme,

    430see Fig. 2. Some interactions may have had as many as 10 teacher-student exchanges or as few

    431as one based on how often the line of thought between concepts changed. Student-student 

    432interactions were not included so as to maintain focus on the teachers’  use of the CINS as a 

    433classroom tool.

    434Results

    435The primary goal of this study was to explore the CINS’s adaptability to a typical high school

    436setting, which included using a student population that was distinctly different from the

    437undergraduate community college student population with which the CINS was originally

    438validated. Whereas the original student population was described as being   “diverse”   and

    439enrolled in a semester-long biology course with a curriculum that was open to instructor 

    440design and flexibility, the current study’s students were mostly members of a traditionally

    441underserved minority group whose mandatory biology education was guided and overseen by

    442many structured state standards.

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    14/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    443Given the current study’s overall goal, student results and teacher actions had vital roles in

    444relaying information. Accordingly, the results section is divided into two major parts. Part one

    445addresses our first research question and presents the results of the current study’s PCA. This

    446PCA is then compared to Anderson et al.’s original PCA and examined for similarities and

    447differences among the two different study populations. In part two, we focus on our second

    448research question and present if and how closely teacher instruction was aligned to CINS

    449concepts as a result of the teachers interpreting their respective students’ pre-test CINS results.

    450RQ No. 1

    451Theoretically, the CINS’s final PCA would have 10 components that explain the variation

    452among the 20 test items, with each component representing a separate natural selection

    453concept. Furthermore, each set of two items that are designed to measure a single concept 

    Teacher A: So over time...say 10...15 generations later...you come back and you look at this population

     of birds [Change in a Population (CP)]  living on the island, what do their beaks look like?SS: Big.Teacher A: Big...why? [Variation in a Population (VP)]

    S1: Because of [Student L]...

    Teacher A: ...So everybody else that had larger beaks is surviving. [Differential Survival (DS)] So their

     genes are going forward and the ones that had genes [Inheritable Variation (IV)] for smaller beaks

     [VP]...not going anywhere [IV].

    Fig. 2   Example of how coding scheme for occurrences of CINS concepts was used within one of teacher A’s

    interactions on day 1 of her instructional unit 

    t2:1   Table 2   Coding scheme for occurrences of CINS concepts during instructional activities and teacher-student 

    interactions

    t2:2   Codes for occurrences

    of CINS concepts

    Criteria for CINS concepts’  presence among instructional activities and

    teacher-student interactions (from Anderson et al. 2002)

    t2:3   Differential survival Activity/interaction involves students learning about 

    t2:4   Biological fitness in that those individuals whose surviving characteristics

    fit them best to their environment are likely to leave more offspring

    than less fit individuals

    t2:5   Variation within a population How individuals of a population vary extensively in their characteristics

    t2:6   Inheritable variation Traits being inherited from parent to offspring

    t2:7   Limited survival How production of more individuals than the environment can support 

    leads to a struggle for existence among individuals of a population

    t2:8   Natural resources Natural resources necessary for organisms to live are in limited supply

    at any given time

    t2:9   Change in a population How (1) the unequal ability of individuals to survive and reproduce will

    lead to gradual change in a population, as opposed to individual members,

    and (2) learned behaviors are not inherited

    t2:10   Origin of variation How random mutations and sexual reproduction produce variations,

    and while many are harmful or of no consequence, a few are beneficial

    in some environments

    t2:11   Origin of species How an isolated population may change so much over time that it becomes

    a new species

    t2:12   Biotic potential Species having great potential fertility in that their population size would

    increase exponentially if all individuals that are born would again

    reproduce successfullyt2:13   Population stability Populations being mostly stable in size except for seasonal fluctuations

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    15/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    454should both load on the same component. The present study maintained the same criteria for 

    455determining the final PCA solution as Anderson et al.’s original study (2002). The criteria 

    456included   “(a) having a large proportion of the total matrix variation explained, (b) having a 

    457high number of items with a strong (>.40) loading on at least one component, (c) having a 

    458minimum number of complex items (items with strong loadings on more than one compo-459nent), and (d) having a component pattern that was theoretically interpretable” (p. 966).

    460In contrast to Anderson et al.’s optimal seven-component extraction, the present study

    461retained the eight-component extraction (due to the number of eigenvalues >1 rule), which

    462accounted for 55 % of the total variance (Anderson et al.’s seven components accounted for 

    46353 % of the total variance). The comparative results from both varimax-rotated component 

    464matrices are found in Table 3.

    465In Anderson et al.’s PCA, all 20 items (questions) loaded >.40 on at least one component.

    466The present study had 16 items which loaded >.40 on at least one component. No items loaded

    467>.40 on multiple components in the present study (versus Anderson et al.’s question 12 which468loaded on components 3 and 5). Striking differences can be seen when examining the specific

    469 pairs of items. In Anderson et al.’s original study,  “9 of the 10 pairs of items that represented

    470the 10 different evolutionary concepts emerged together on the same component ”   (p. 966).

    471That pattern is not readily seen in the present study, with the exception of questions 4 and 13,

    472which probed for  change in a population. In addition, the present study shows seemingly

    473unrelated questions emerging on the same component (e.g., questions 11, 12, and 9 all loading

    474on component 2). The fact that there is contrast between these and Anderson et al. ’s results

    475may indicate that the CINS is also detecting the present study’s students’ lack of expertise with

    476natural selection concepts; in that, the students could be responding to surface features of the

    477questions, as opposed to deeper conceptual understanding. At the ninth-grade level, this

    478surface-level response is most likely to be expected.

    479RQ No. 2

    480We begin the results for RQ no. 2 by providing an overview of how the current study ’s

    481students performed on the CINS before their teachers’   instructional units on evolution

    482commenced (see Table 4). As mentioned previously, the CINS assesses 10 different concepts

    483related to natural selection. On average, the current study’s students experienced less difficulty

    484with questions that assessed  biotic potential ,  variation within a population, and  inheritable

    485variation. Conversely, the students experienced most difficulty with questions that assessed

    486origin of variation  and  differential survival . For a concise description of each of these CINS

    487concepts, please refer to Table 2. The overall pre-test results from the current study’s students

    488 became a useful guide to explore which CINS concepts were more or less emphasized by these

    489teachers during instruction.

    490After reviewing her students’   pre-test results, teacher A commented that the results

    491confirmed what she already knew about her students’   alternative conceptions. Nevertheless,

    492she did appreciate the breakdown of her students’ data and some of the results helped her with

    493realizing and confirming which evolutionary concepts needed to be stressed throughout her 

    494instructional unit. For example, teacher A described how the concept of  biological fitness  (or 

    495differential survival , in CINS terminology) was particularly important;   “It ’s like…with the

    496fitness and what does that mean to them and stuff …so that was like,   ‘Uh-huh…kind of 

    497figured’…mostly in that way. It ’s like…those are the particular areas that we need to hit.”

    498(post-instruction interview)

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    16/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    499Teacher B welcomed the CINS as a form of assessment and found it to be more useful in

    500relaying student understanding as opposed to other forms of assessment, such as results from

    501the state-mandated tests. She also found value with the student results that were presented to

    502her after the pre-test administration. Teacher B admitted pleasant surprise with the way her 

    503students answered some of the questions and used some of the results as an additional guide

    504for planning instructional activities and deciding which concepts really needed to be stressed

    505and those that did not.

    t3:1   Table 3   PCA comparison between current study and Anderson et al.’s (2002) original study for the CINS

    t3:2   Component 

    t3:3   Item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

    t3:

    4   Biotic potentialt3:5   1 .624   .672

    t3:6   11   .594   .714

    t3:7   Population stability

    t3:8   3 .845   .591

    t3:9   12   .667   .455 .596

    t3:10   Natural resources

    t3:11   2 .706   .684

    t3:12   14 .502

    t3:

    13   Limited survivalt3:14   5 .569   .756

    t3:15   15 .589   .443

    t3:16   Variation within a population

    t3:17   9   .569   .737

    t3:18   16   .669   .547

    t3:19   Inherited variation

    t3:20   7 .502   .513

    t3:21   17   .743   .687

    t3:22

      Differential survivalt3:23   10 .769

    t3:24   18 .472   .562

    t3:25   Change in a population

    t3:26   4 .406

    t3:27   .636

    t3:28   13 .671

    t3:29   .722

    t3:30   Origin of variation

    t3:31   6 .501   .725

    t3:32   19 .659   .667

    t3:33   Origin of species

    t3:34   8 .418

    t3:35   20 .593

    Anderson et al.’s original study (2002) and the present study’s loading values are compared. Values for the

     present study are in boldface

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    17/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    506507What I did like about it …is it gave me an idea of what they understood and what I didn’t 

    508know that they understood. And I kind of like that because there some stuff that I chose

    509that they didn’t, and I was like,  “Oh wow…They do understand this a little more than I

    510thought they had.” So that was kind of nice. It kind of helps you with the lesson in the

    511sense that I’m like,  “Well, I don’t really have to go and talk about that because they do

    512know.” So I can kind of just skim through this. (post-instruction interview) 513

    514Teacher C stated that he did use the students’ pre-test results to help guide his instruction

    515and planning, but justification of his claim is difficult to ascertain as there exists little

    516evidence. He provided no specific examples as to how the CINS results were affecting his

    517 practice. He mentioned he paid particular attention to the CINS concepts his students found

    518difficult and made an effort to incorporate and concentrate on these concepts more than he

    519normally would. However, evidence pertaining to his claim was elusive because of the few

    520total interactions that exist with his students regarding these concepts (see Table   4). In

    521addition, there was a lack of instructional activities in which his students were engaged

    522with these concepts.

    523The novice (with regards to teaching experience) of the group, teacher D found herself still

    524 being acquainted with the various forms of assessment made available to her. She explored her 

    525different options and found the CINS to be a useful form of assessment; in that, it provided

    526 practical information about how her students understood natural selection concepts, especially

    527with   differential survival . From this information, teacher D occasionally tweaked her 

    t4:1   Table 4   Percentage of student correct responses on CINS pre-test grouped according to each teacher 

    t4:2   CINS concept Teacher A Teacher B Teacher C Teacher D Average

    t4:3   No. 1 Biotic potential 69 % 42 % 67 % 57 % 59 %

    t4:

    4   No. 11 Biotic potential 48 % 27 % 54 % 49 % 45 %t4:5   No. 2 Natural resources 51 % 62 % 53 % 60 % 57 %

    t4:6   No. 14 Natural resources 29 % 23 % 33 % 21 % 27 %

    t4:7   No. 3 Population stability 71 % 67 % 63 % 58 % 65 %

    t4:8   No. 12 Population stability 25 % 19 % 31 % 23 % 25 %

    t4:9   No. 4 Change in a population 29 % 26 % 22 % 21 % 25 %

    t4:10   No. 13 Change in a population 36 % 31 % 25 % 21 % 28 %

    t4:11   No. 5 Limited survival 39 % 26 % 40 % 38 % 36 %

    t4:12   No. 15 Limited survival 20 % 22 % 28 % 27 % 24 %

    t4:

    13   No. 6 Origin of variation 14 % 11 % 9 % 11 % 11 %t4:14   No. 19 Origin of variation 44 % 31 % 40 % 26 % 35 %

    t4:15   No. 7 Inheritable variation 68 % 62 % 56 % 51 % 59 %

    t4:16   No. 17 Inheritable variation 53 % 46 % 42 % 28 % 42 %

    t4:17   No. 8 Origin of species 35 % 46 % 37 % 29 % 37 %

    t4:18   No. 20 Origin of species 28 % 25 % 24 % 20 % 24 %

    t4:19   No. 9 Variation within a population 43 % 38 % 41 % 38 % 40 %

    t4:20   No. 16 Variation within a population 66 % 49 % 78 % 51 % 61 %

    t4:21   No. 10 Differential survival 10 % 17 % 17 % 12 % 14 %

    t4:22

      No. 18 Differential survival 14 % 15 % 19 % 15 % 16 %t4:23   Average number of student respondents on pre-test 83 81 80 81 38 %

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    18/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    528instructional unit in different places to ensure that her students received optimal engagement 

    529with natural selection concepts. Teacher D cited an example.

    530531…‘cause when I looked at that, I could see what they would be thinking for fitness. So, I

    532did give more like examples of what fitness…well, which one do you think is more fit 

    533and then we would do that. I had a mission countdown [warm-up questions], I think, too.

    534So, I added that. (post-instruction interview)535

    536As mentioned previously, 10 concepts encompass natural selection on the CINS. Figure 3

    537displays how often each CINS concept occurred across the instructional activities within each

    538teacher ’s instructional unit. Overall results did not yield any significance with certain CINS

    539concepts being more or less emphasized than others. Based on descriptive results, the CINS

    540concepts of  inheritable variation and differential survival  received slightly more mention than

    541other CINS concepts during the teachers’   instructional units. None of the teachers’   instruc-

    542tional activities placed an emphasis and built on the concept of  population stability.543Tables 5 and 6 provide an overview of the teacher-student evolution-related interactions that 

    544occurred during each teacher ’s instructional unit. While Table 5  gives an overall sense of the

    545amount and sort of classroom interactions each teacher had during instruction, Table 6 further 

    546categorizes the CINS interactions into those which had a specific focus on CINS concepts. In

    547addition, the concepts with which each teacher ’s students experienced most and least difficulty

    548are indicated, thereby enabling trends to be observed and noted between these most and least 

    549difficult concepts for students and the amount of interactions devoted to specific CINS

    550concepts during instruction.

    551An avid questioner of her students, teacher A had an approximate 181 interactions with her 

    552students throughout her instructional unit and of those interactions; approximately 51 %

    553(n = 92) were devoted to CINS concepts (see Table   5). She had her students engage with

    554almost all (with the exception of two, biotic potential  and population stability; see Table 6) of 

    555the CINS concepts in some form or another during her instructional unit. Several of her 

    556instructional activities also coincided with various CINS concepts (see Fig.  3), and some of 

    557these concepts appeared more frequently (e.g., differential survival  and  inheritable variation)

    0

    2

    4

    6

    8

    10

    12

       B   i  o   t   i  c

       P  o   t  e  n   t   i  a   l

       N  a   t  u  r  a   l   R  e  s  o  u  r  c  e  s

       P  o  p  u   l  a   t   i  o  n

       S   t  a   b   i   l   i   t  y

       C   h  a  n  g  e   i  n  a   P

      o  p  u   l  a   t   i  o  n

       L   i  m   i   t  e   d

       S  u  r  v   i  v  a   l

       O  r   i  g   i  n  o   f

       V  a  r   i  a   t   i  o  n

       I  n   h  e  r   i   t  a   b   l  e

       V  a  r   i  a   t   i  o  n

       O  r   i  g   i  n  o

       f   S  p  e  c   i  e  s

       V  a  r   i  a   t   i  o  n

       W   i   t   h   i  n  a

       P  o  p  u

       l  a   t   i  o  n

       D   i   f   f  e  r  e  n   t   i  a   l   S  u  r  v   i  v  a   l

       T  o   t  a   l   O  c  c  u  r  r  e  n  c  e  s  o   f   C   I   N   S   C  o  n  c  e  p   t  s

       D  u  r   i  n  g   I  n  s   t  r  u  c   t   i  o  n  a   l   U  n   i   t

    CINS Concept

    Tchr A

    Tchr B

    Tchr C

    Tchr D

    Fig. 3   Total number of targeted CINS concepts found within instructional activities across each teacher ’s

    evolutionary unit 

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    19/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    558than others with the selection of these activities. Not surprisingly, the frequency of these

    559concepts with her instructional activities matched the frequency of her interactions with her 560students.

    561Using a variety of instructional strategies and questions (especially during her lectures when

    562her students were taking notes), teacher B had an approximate 125 different interactions with

    563her students throughout her instructional unit. Of those interactions, approximately 40 %

    564(n = 50) were devoted to CINS concepts (see Table 5). Overall, results suggest that teacher 

    565B had her students engage with certain CINS concepts (e.g.,  origin of species  and inheritable

    566variation; see Table 6) more than other concepts during her instructional unit. The same sort of 

    567trend appears with teacher B’s instructional activities as with her interactions. The CINS

    568concepts of  origin of species and inheritable variation appear more frequently with teacher B

    ’s

    569use of instructional activities (see Fig.  3).

    570Throughout his instructional unit, teacher C had approximately 88 interactions with his

    571students on the topic of evolution. Compared with his colleagues, teacher C’s total interactions

    572occurred less frequently. Of his 88 interactions, about 34 % (n = 30) specifically dealt with

    573CINS concepts (see Table   5). When his students were engaged with specific instructional

    574activities (i.e., watching selected videos, creating word clouds, and presenting music videos),

    575there was minimal teacher-student or student-student interaction with regard to CINS concepts.

    576With the exception of  differential survival  and inheritable variation, teacher C’s students were

    577not able to explore other CINS concepts with the various instructional activities. The concepts

    578were briefly mentioned with a few isolated teacher-student exchanges (see Table  6). When his

    579students did receive opportunities to explore more CINS concepts, the opportunities came all

    580at once in a teacher-centered lecture toward the end of his instructional unit. Recall that by

    581choice, teacher C did not participate in common planning meetings with his colleagues.

    582Whether it being due to his lack of participation in these meetings, uneasiness with evolu-

    583tionary concepts, or by some other mechanism, there were substantially fewer CINS concepts

    584found with the instructional activities of his evolutionary unit (see Fig. 3) as opposed to his

    585colleagues.

    586Quite methodical and purposeful with her strategies and questioning, teacher D had an

    587approximate 128 interactions with her students throughout her instructional unit and of those

    588interactions; approximately 56 % (n = 72) were devoted to CINS concepts (see Table   5).

    589Teacher D’s overall results demonstrate that her students engaged with almost all (with the

    590exception of two, biotic potential  and population stability; see Table 6) of the CINS concepts

    591in some form or another during her instructional unit. Similar to her manner in determining

    592how to interact with her students, teacher D was also conscientious with which instructional

    t5:1   Table 5   Each teacher ’s percentage of interactions with CINS concepts and other evolution-related ideas among

    his/her total teacher-student evolutionary interactions

    t5:2   Number of total teacher-student 

    evolution-related interactions

    during instructional unit 

    Teacher-student interactions

    with CINS concepts (%)

    Other evolution-related

    teacher-student interactions

    (%)

    t5:3   Teacher A 181 51 49

    t5:4   Teacher B 125 40 60

    t5:5   Teacher C 88 56 44

    t5:6   Teacher D 128 34 66

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    20/29

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    21/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    593activities her students engaged. Several of her interactions that involved instructional activities

    594coincided with various CINS concepts (see Fig. 3) and, like teachers A and B, some of these

    595concepts appeared more frequently (e.g.,   differential survival ,   inheritable variation, and

    596variation within a population) than others with the selection of these activities. The frequency

    597of these concepts occurring with her instructional activities approximately matched the598frequency of their occurrence with her total CINS concepts classroom interactions.

    599With this group of teachers, the overall alignment of interactions and instructional activities

    600with CINS concepts that may have needed the most attention was met with mixed results.

    601According to student pre-test responses (see Table   4),   differential survival   was the most 

    602difficult concept for students to grasp. As verified with teacher responses, classroom interac-

    603tions, and implementation of instructional activities, the majority of these teachers made

    604instruction surrounding differential survival  a priority (see Table 6). Furthermore, the teachers

    605may have realized that they could spend less time with certain concepts because their students

    606seemed to have an overall grasp of these concepts (e.g., biotic potential ). However, there were607still less conceptually difficult CINS concepts that were prioritized during instruction over 

    608other concepts that were considerably more difficult for students. For example, every teacher 

    609spent significant instructional time with   inheritable variation  (see Fig.  3 and Table 6), even

    610though students demonstrated overall competency with this concept; whereas, three of four 

    611teachers placed less instructional priority with   origin of variation, a concept with which

    612students seemed to grapple.

    613Discussion

    614Overall results from the present study suggest that the CINS has potential for use in secondary

    615classes, as evidenced by the study’s teacher participants’  enthusiasm for becoming familiar 

    616with an instrument that can identify their students’  alternative conceptions. However, further 

    617research with additional secondary school populations will be required in order to refine

    618various assessment items, particularly because of the teachers’ demonstrated limited approach

    619and skepticism about the CINS’s use as an instructional tool in its current form. We believe that 

    620the core reasons for this approach and skepticism can be traced back to the CINS’s internal

    621structural properties.

    622The results of the present study’s PCA fall short of the  “strong support ” that Anderson et al.

    623(2002) originally demonstrated for the inventory. Consequently, it appears that some of the

    624concerns about the CINS that were relayed with Nehm and Schonfeld’s (2008, 2010) findings

    625apply in the present study’s context as well. Specifically, if the eight components on the present 

    626PCA represent distinct evolutionary concepts, then only one component contained a single set 

    627of question pairs that represented one concept (change in a population). This finding is similar 

    628to that of Nehm and Schonfeld (2008), in which there was only one component that  “included

    629a highly correlated suite of key concepts”  (p. 1145). Most other components revealed where

    630questions intended to measure different concepts were actually similar to each other. For 

    631example, component 1 contained questions 16 (variation within a population) and 17

    632(inherited variation). A closer examination of these two questions reveals that they are related

    633to each other in the respect that they both occur toward the end of the assessment and ask about 

    634features or traits of the Canary Islands lizard population (please refer to separate attachment to

    635this manuscript for full wording of CINS questions and answer choices). While question 16

    636was intended to ask about the variability of certain traits and question 17 was intended to ask 

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    22/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    637about how such traits were passed down from generation to generation, it is entirely reasonable

    638that a high school student sees these concepts as being very closely related, being that many

    639traits manifest themselves as physical characteristics and these traits are inherited from parents.

    640Also, some students may have resorted to guessing for the answers to these questions as these

    641questions appeared toward the end of the assessment.642The same rationale can be used for questions 2 (natural resources) and 5 (limited survival ),

    643which were both contained in component 3. Both questions had very similar wording in that 

    644they asked about the relationships between the Galapagos finches and their food supply. Many

    645of the students most likely viewed these particular questions as practically indistinguishable

    646from each other and may have been reacting to the surface-level features of these questions

    647rather than any deep conceptual understanding. Follow-up questions with a sample of students

    648would be required to corroborate this claim, but for the moment, it remains a reasonable

    649hypothesis. Anderson et al. revealed a similar finding in stating,  “This is not surprising because

    650when students understand that there is a competition for resources, they acknowledge that 651some individuals die” (p. 968).

    652In a similar vein, the current analysis showed that questions 1 (biotic potential ) and 15

    653(limited survival ) were both contained on component 5 (which seems to be related to the

    654aforementioned component 3). Question 1 was designed to assess a student ’s understanding of 

    655how populations would grow if there were ideal conditions, that is, no predators and unlimited

    656food. Question 15’s scenario of predicting what would happen to a population when the food

    657supply was limited was the exact opposite in nature. Therefore, students may have viewed

    658these two questions as being very similar to one another because of the questions’  inherent 

    659opposing realities

     — when a population has an unlimited food supply, it thrives, and when food

    660 becomes scarce, individuals begin to starve and die.

    661The other two questions which were found on a single component were questions 6 (origin

    662of variation) and 18 (differential survival ). These two questions were contained in component 

    6638, and it was initially unclear as to why they clustered together. Question 6 asked about how

    664different finch beak types may have appeared on the Galapagos Islands, whereas question 18

    665dealt with notions of biological fitness. These questions appear to be assessing two distinct 

    666ideas, but upon closer inspection of specific answer choices, there appears to be a subtle

    667relationship between these two questions for this population of students. Question 6’s answer 

    668choices (i.e., answer choices b and d) contain language that suggests the acquisition of specific

    669traits through generations, and in a similar manner, question 18’s data table also has language

    670regarding traits and offspring. It is conceivable that the present study’s students perceived these

    671two questions as being related to the acquisition of inherited traits through time.

    672There were two instances in which three different questions were contained on a single

    673component. In one case, all three questions were designed to be assessing three separate

    674natural selection concepts. Questions 9 (variation in a population), 11 (biotic potential ), and

    67512 ( population stability) were all contained in component 2. Such an instance never occurred

    676in the original study’s PCA. These three questions all used the context of the Venezuelan

    677guppies to assess the seemingly different concepts. Questions 11 and 12 are more related to

    678one another in the sense that, like the situation mentioned above with questions 1 and 15, one

    679question describes a scenario where there are ideal conditions for a population to grow

    680(question 11) and the other question describes a more realistic setting where there are predators

    681that threaten population expansion (question 12). Question 9, which describes the overall

    682features of the guppy population, is more peripherally related to questions 11 and 12.

    683 Nevertheless, students may have gleaned from question 9’s answer choices that the

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    23/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    684 population’s characteristics certainly have an influence on certain members’  survival when

    685they are faced with predators. As a whole, these three questions were tied to the entire theme of 

    686 population dynamics, and the variability of a population is a strong determinant of how a 

    687 population fluctuates or remains the same.

    688The next case which had three questions load strongly on a single component was a bit 689different from the previously mentioned case; in that, two of the questions (questions 4 and 13)

    690represented a pair that was originally designed to assess one concept  — change in a population.

    691Question 7 (inheritable variation) was the third question that was contained in component 4.

    692Questions 4 and 13 described scenarios from two different contexts (i.e., Galapagos finches

    693and Venezuelan guppies) and probed for the main changes that occur in a population over time.

    694The answer choices of these two questions used genetic-inclined language (e.g.,   “traits,”

    695“ passed on to their babies,” and  “mutations”), and this may explain why the students related

    696these questions to question 7. Question 7 also dealt with the Galapagos finches and directly

    697asked what type of variation was being passed on future finch populations. The students may698have reasoned that only genetic variation can be inherited and the main mechanism by which

    699changes can occur in populations is through inheritance.

    700There were four questions which were not contained in any component: questions 8 (origin

    701of species), 10 (differential survival ), 14 (natural resources), and 20 (origin of species).

    702Questions 8 and 20 probed for understanding of origin of species, and this may have been a 

    703case of the students truly having a limited awareness of how different species arise. Indeed,

    704when teacher C asked his students what their thoughts were about the origins of plants on an

    705island, many students were unable to offer any explanations, and those that did offer an

    706explanation claimed some

     “force

    ” or 

     “something in the soil

    ” made the plants appear. Within

    707the study site’s home state, the concept of speciation is not fully realized until high school. In

    708fact, there is minimal mention of speciation in the elementary and middle grades according to

    709the state standards. Question 14 dealt with the availability of food for the Canary Islands

    710lizards. However, when taken and read independently, the question suggests that an under-

    711standing of the food supply on the Canary Islands may be required in order to correctly answer 

    712the question. Since the students had never heard of the Canary Islands, they may have believed

    713that it was impossible to correctly answer the question if one had no familiarity with the food

    714supply dynamics of the Canary Islands. Lastly, it is unclear why question 10 was not contained

    715in any component. Question 10 probed understanding of biological fitness and was intended to

    716detect any alternative conceptions that dealt with strength, size, speed, and agility. Further 

    717exploration into this question and its counterpart (question 18) will be needed in order to see

    718how these items can be improved for future use.

    719Since the CINS was such an integral component of the current study, the teachers may have

    720had a heightened awareness of the concepts involved with natural selection, but this was not 

    721always the case. Granted, the teachers’   units involved other evolutionary topics, such as

    722evidence for evolution,   sexual selection, and   genetic drift , that were not specific to the

    723CINS’s topics, but teachers A and D demonstrated efficiency with natural selection concepts,

    724with more than half of their total interactions with students from their units dealing with CINS

    725concepts. Teachers B and C were not as efficient with their frequencies occurring below 50 %.

    726When examining the occurrence of a CINS concept, especially in the form of written questions

    727or other tasks, teachers A, B, and D’s students received opportunities to make associations with

    728these concepts two to three times more than teacher C’s students were able to do so, suggesting

    729that the three teachers who common planned together maintained a tighter adherence to natural

    730selection concepts than did the single teacher who planned in isolation.

    Res Sci Educ

    JrnlID 11165_ArtID 9524_Proof# 1 - 04/04/2016

  • 8/18/2019 A Resource for Eliciting Student Alternative Conceptions: Examining the Adaptability of a Concept Inventory for Na…

    24/29

       U   N  C  O   R   R   E  C

       T   E   D

     P   R  O

      O   F

    731As mentioned previously, certain CINS concepts received more mention than others by

    732the teachers, with an increased frequency on  differential survival  and inheritable variation.

    733As noted in the  “Results” section, the students’ pre-test results on CINS questions dealing

    734with  differential survival   were of special interest to some of the teachers and the results

    735may have guided these teachers to frequently mention this concept. These teachers often736used the phrase  “survival of the fittest ” to help explain natural selection. Once this phrase

    737was used, the teachers inevitably followed up with their students by asking them what was

    738meant by   “ being fit ”   or   “fitness.”   Inheritable variation   dealt with organisms’   different 

    739adaptations and how traits were passed from one generation to the next. The topic of 

    740genetics had been taught by all four t