behavioral manipulation of retrieval in a spatial...

10
Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophilamelanogaster Oerold Wustmann and Martin Heisenbergl The0d0r-B0veri-Institut ftir Bi0wissenschaften (Bi0zentrum) Lehtstuhl ffir Genetik Am Hubland D-97074 Wfirzburg, Germany Abstract A paradigm for operant conditioning of freely walking single Drosophila flies has been described previously. A fly can be conditioned to avoid one side of a small test chamber if the chamber is heated whenever the fly enters this side. In a subsequent memory test without heat the fly continues to avoid the previously heat-associated side. In this experimental design one cannot exclude that flies mark the heated side by an odor that they subsequently avoid during the test. As a final proof for associative learning in the present experiment, flies are trained in one chamber and tested for learning in another, similar one. Handling in the transfer experiment interferes with memory display, even if the fly is returned to the old chamber instead of a new one. Memory can be reactivated, however, by subjecting the fly to an additional brief training (priming), which is too short to establish significant learning in naive flies. For efficient priming, heat has to be applied to the same side as during training in the old chamber. Only then the fly subsequently shows a side preference and avoids the side of the new chamber, which in the old one had been associated with heat. The two chambers are similar but not identical. The transfer experiment therefore raises the question as to what the flies use as spatial reference during training and test. In the light, they can be shown to orientate according to visual landmarks associated with the chamber. In complete darkness, 1Corresponding author. where training and memory scores do not differ from those in the light, they are assumed to use a combination of tactile and idiothetic information for orienting. Introduction Progress in the genetics of learning and memory in Drosophila melanogaster has been considerable in the last two decades (summarized, e.g., in Dudai 1988; Heisenberg 1989; DeZazzo and Tully 1995; Spatz 1995; Davis 1996). However, the field suffers from an important bias: Learning/ memory mutants have been screened only in para- digms for olfactory learning (Dudai et al. 1976; Quinn et al. 1979; Aceves-Pina et al. 1983; Dura et al. 1993), and most published mutant studies of learning and memory were carried out with classi- cal olfactory conditioning (e.g., Tully and Quinn 1985; Dura et al. 1993).Knowledge concerning the molecular underpinnings of other types of associa- tive learning, such as visual classical conditioning, or operant conditioning is still limited. A genetic dissection of different types of learning may broaden our understanding of its molecular, cellu- lar, and behavioral basis. A prerequisite for a genetic dissection is an efficient mutant screen. Although many learning paradigms have been developed for D. melanogas- ter, most of them are time-consuming and require considerable experience. Moreover, no appropiate screening procedure for operant conditioning is available so far. To overcome these shortcomings, Wustmann et al. (1996) developed a new paradigm for operant conditioning of freely walking flies. In- dividual animals are trained to avoid one side of a small test chamber. Whenever they enter this side, the chamber is heated. In a subsequent memory test without heat, the flies continue to avoid the LEARNING & MEMORY 4:328-336 9 1997 by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press ISSN1072-0502/97 $5.00 & 328 L E A R N / N G M E M O R Y Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.org Downloaded from

Upload: others

Post on 13-Mar-2020

9 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster Oerold Wustmann and Martin Heisenbergl The0d0r-B0veri-Institut ftir Bi0wissenschaften (Bi0zentrum)

Lehtstuhl ffir Genetik Am Hubland

D-97074 Wfirzburg, Germany

A b s t r a c t

A p a r a d i g m for o p e r a n t c o n d i t i o n i n g o f f r ee ly w a l k i n g s ing le Drosophila flies has b e e n d e s c r i b e d p r e v i ous l y . A fly c a n be c o n d i t i o n e d to avo id o n e s ide o f a sma l l tes t c h a m b e r if t h e c h a m b e r is h e a t e d w h e n e v e r t h e f ly e n t e r s th is side. In a s u b s e q u e n t m e m o r y tes t w i t h o u t h e a t t he f ly c o n t i n u e s to avo id t h e p r e v i o u s l y hea t - a s soc ia t ed side. In th i s e x p e r i m e n t a l de s ign o n e c a n n o t e x c l u d e t h a t fl ies m a r k t h e h e a t e d s ide b y a n o d o r t h a t t h e y s u b s e q u e n t l y avoid d u r i n g t h e test . As a f ina l p r o o f fo r assoc ia t ive l e a r n i n g in t he p r e s e n t e x p e r i m e n t , flies a re t r a i n e d in o n e c h a m b e r a n d t es ted for l e a r n i n g in a n o t h e r , s i m i l a r one . H a n d l i n g in t h e t r a n s f e r e x p e r i m e n t i n t e r f e r e s w i t h m e m o r y d i sp lay , e v e n if t he f ly is r e t u r n e d to t he o ld c h a m b e r i n s t e a d o f a n e w one . M e m o r y c a n be reac t iva ted , h o w e v e r , b y sub jec t ing t he f ly to a n a d d i t i o n a l b r i e f t r a i n i n g ( p r i m i n g ) , w h i c h is too s h o r t to e s t ab l i sh s ign i f i can t l e a r n i n g in na ive flies. For e f f ic ient p r i m i n g , h e a t ha s to be a p p l i e d to t h e s a m e s ide as d u r i n g t r a i n i n g in t h e o ld c h a m b e r . O n l y t h e n the f ly s u b s e q u e n t l y s h o w s a s ide p r e f e r e n c e a n d avoids t he s ide o f t h e n e w c h a m b e r , w h i c h in t h e o ld o n e h a d b e e n a s soc ia t ed w i t h hea t . The two c h a m b e r s a re s i m i l a r b u t n o t ident ica l . The t r a n s f e r e x p e r i m e n t t h e r e f o r e ra ises t h e q u e s t i o n as to w h a t t h e flies use as spa t ia l r e f e r e n c e d u r i n g t r a i n i n g a n d test. In t he l ight , t h e y c a n be s h o w n to o r i e n t a t e a c c o r d i n g to v i sua l l a n d m a r k s a s soc ia ted w i t h t h e c h a m b e r . In c o m p l e t e d a r k n e s s ,

1Corresponding author.

w h e r e t r a i n i n g a n d m e m o r y sco res do n o t d i f fer f r o m t h o s e in t he l ight, t h e y a re a s s u m e d to use a c o m b i n a t i o n o f tact i le a n d id io the t ic i n f o r m a t i o n for o r i en t i ng .

I n t r o d u c t i o n

Progress in the genetics of learning and memory in Drosophila melanogaster has been considerable in the last two decades (summarized, e.g., in Dudai 1988; Heisenberg 1989; DeZazzo and Tully 1995; Spatz 1995; Davis 1996). However, the field suffers from an important bias: Learning/ memory mutants have been screened only in para- digms for olfactory learning (Dudai et al. 1976; Quinn et al. 1979; Aceves-Pina et al. 1983; Dura et al. 1993), and most published mutant studies of learning and memory were carried out with classi- cal olfactory conditioning (e.g., Tully and Quinn 1985; Dura et al. 1993).Knowledge concerning the molecular underpinnings of other types of associa- tive learning, such as visual classical conditioning, or operant conditioning is still limited. A genetic dissection of different types of learning may broaden our understanding of its molecular, cellu- lar, and behavioral basis.

A prerequisite for a genetic dissection is an efficient mutant screen. Although many learning paradigms have been developed for D. melanogas- ter, most of them are time-consuming and require considerable experience. Moreover, no appropiate screening procedure for operant conditioning is available so far. To overcome these shortcomings, Wustmann et al. (1996) developed a new paradigm for operant conditioning of freely walking flies. In- dividual animals are trained to avoid one side of a small test chamber. Whenever they enter this side, the chamber is heated. In a subsequent memory test without heat, the flies continue to avoid the

LEARNING & MEMORY 4:328-336 �9 1997 by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press ISSN1072-0502/97 $5.00

& 328

L E A R N / N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 2: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

PRIMING AS A PREREQUISITE

side previously associated wi th heat. The learning mutants dunce, rutabaga, and amnesiac avoid the heat-associated side but show no corresponding after effect (for dunce and rutabaga, Wustmann et al. 1996; for amnesiac, G. Wustmann, unpubl.) . These and other observations suggest that the ap- paratus measures learning and memory. However, definite proof that the heat ing chamber scores as- sociative learning is still missing. As an alternative interpretation, flies might deposi te a repellant on one side of the c h a m b e r in response to heat and later avoid that odor wi thout r emember ing any- thing.

In the present study we provide this proof. In addition, we de termine the relevant cues that the fly associates wi th heat. The exper iments reveal that the fly uses primari ly visual landmarks in the light and tactile and idiothetic (Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt 1973) information in comple te dark- ness. References outside the chamber , such as the e lectromagnet ic field are ignored.

Figure 1: Heating chamber for operant conditioning. For explanations, see text. Adapted from Wustmann et al. (1996).

M a t e r i a l s a n d M e t h o d s

FHES

Canton-S (CS) and Berlin wild-type strains were used. All flies tested were b e t w e e n 2 and 6 days old and were mainta ined at 25~ on standard cornmeal /molasses m e d i u m in a 16-hr l ight /8-hr dark cycle at 60% humidity. Females as well as males were studied (-50% each). No differences in the behavior of the two genders in the apparatus were detected, and the respect ive data were pooled.

CONDITIONING APPARATUS

The original apparatus deve loped by Wust- m a n n et al. (1996) was used wi th slight modifica- tions. The test apparatus (Fig. 1) is a rectangular chamber (40 x 4 x 2.5 mm). The uppe r and lower surfaces consist of Peltier elements, the sides of t ransparent acryl glass plates. A control circuit and a the rmosensor in the c h a m b e r keep the Peltier e lements at a permissive cold or restrictive hot temperature. The chamber is virtually subdivided perpendicu la r to its long axis into two halves (left and right) by a directionally selective light gate that informs the compute r about w h e t h e r the fly is in the left or right half. The light gate consists of two infrared (IR)-emitting diodes and two IR-sensitive photodetectors . The detectors are 3 m m apart

(center to center). The c h a m b e r is i l luminated by four small lamps close to the corners of the alley; two are yellow, and two are blue. Looking from the posi t ion of the light gate down the alley in ei ther direction, the b lue lamps are always on the right side, and the yel low ones are on the left of the chamber ' s long axis. W h e n e v e r the fly is on the right side of the light gate only the two blue lamps are swi tched on, if the fly is on the left side, only the two yel low lights i l luminate the chamber . A rack wi th 15 similar chambers was used for the exper iments . Therefore, 15 flies were tested in par- allel in the standard exper imen t (Figs. 2 and 9, below). In exper iments involving transfer f rom one c h a m b e r to another, only two chambers were used in parallel in order to keep t ime b e t w e e n training and test short.

PROCEDURES

The exper iments consist of three phases: (1) a p re fe rence test; (2) training, and (3) m e m o r y test. A compute r controls the exper iments in the fol- lowing manner: During all three phases it continu- ously monitors the t ime and directions of transi- tions at the light gate. During training, in addition, it turns heat on w h e n the fly enters one side and turns it off w h e n the fly leaves it again. Note that the Peltier e lements cover the whole length of the chamber . This ensures that the fly does not expe-

& 329

L E A R N / N G M E M 0 R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 3: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

W u s t m a n n a n d H e i s e n b e r g

r ience a spatial but only a temporal t empera tu re gradient, hot is at 37~ (+2~ and cold at 25~ (_+2~ During the p re fe rence and m e m o r y tests the t empera tu re is f ixed at 25~ (cold), indepen- dently of the posi t ion of the fly. Between consecu- tive trims the sides for the permissive and restric- tive condit ions are switched. The chambers are used wi thou t cleaning for several hundred trials. After training, the m e m o r y test does not begin until the fly enters the o the r side; therefore, inactive flies are excluded. The activity of the flies is esti- ma ted by count ing the n u m b e r of passages at the light gate.

ANALYSIS OF DATA

The pe r fo rmance of individual flies is calcu- lated as Per formance Index (PI) = (A - B)/(A + B), wi th A indicating the t ime during the training that the fly spends on the side associated wi th permis- sive t empera tu re (cold) and B indicating the t ime on the hot side. The PI can vary b e t w e e n - 1 and + 1. A PI of zero indicates that the fly spends 50% of the t ime on the hot side. In the p re fe rence test, PI measures the fly's spontaneous preference for one side of the chamber ; during training it indicates heat avoidance, and during the m e m o r y test it is a m e m o r y score. In the exper iments half of the flies are condi t ioned to avoid left and the o ther half to avoid right. In this w a y the effects of spontaneous pre fe rences for a distinct side and of slight assym- metries in the appara tus are largely eliminated f rom the data.

behavior. An alternative hypothesis is that the ani- mal deposits olfactory landmarks during the train- ing and subsequent ly uses them to avoid the pre- viously hea ted side during the so-called m e m o r y test.

To distinguish b e t w e e n these possibilities, w e carried out a transfer exper iment . Flies are trained in one c h a m b e r and tested in similar one in w h i c h sides could not be marked by odor landmarks. Af- ter a 30-sec preference test and a subsequent 3-min training, flies are gently aspirated into a p ipet te and t ransferred to a new, identical c h a m b e r w h e r e the 60-see m e m o r y test is carried out. The results are p resen ted in Figure 3. Flies do not avoid the cor- responding side in the n e w chamber . There are several explanat ions for this failure: They might be disorientated because they are missing the odor cues that guided them in the original exper iment ; the n e w chamber might not be sufficiently similar to the first one; or the transfer process might in- terfere wi th the m e m o r y test.

To examine the influence of the t ransfer on the behavior of the flies, individuals are taken out of the chamber after training and re in t roduced into the same chamber for the m e m o r y test. Again, no m e m o r y score is obtained (Fig. 4). This suggests that handling disturbs the m e m o r y display. On the o ther hand, this does not rule out the possibility that the flies rely on deposi ted olfactory landmarks;

STATISTICS

Wilcoxon tests for significance of differences f rom zero ( the r andom choice level) are carried out. The results are p resen ted toge ther wi th the s tandard error of the mean (S.E.M.).

Results

PROOF FOR ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

A fly can be condi t ioned to avoid one side of

the chamber if it is hea ted w h e n e v e r the fly enters that side. In a subsequen t m e m o r y test wi thout heat, wild-type Drosophila cont inues to avoid the side associated previously wi th heat (Fig. 2). The quest ion arises w h e t h e r learning is involved in this

Figure 2: Average PI before, during, and after avoid- ance training. The noxious stimulus is heat (37 _+2~ PI = (A - B)/(A + B), where A is the time the fly spends on the side associated with no heat (cold) and B the time on the heat-associated side (hot). A PI of zero indicates the random choice level. Flies are trained to avoid the left or the right side in an alternating sequence. (Wild- type CS flies; n = 51 .)

& 330

L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 4: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

PRIMING AS A PREREQUISITE

Figure 3: No memory in a transfer experiment. After a preference test flies are trained for 3 min. Subsequently the animals are removed from the chamber and trans- ferred to a similar one. (Wild-type Berlin flies; n - 32.)

it s imply shows that the t ransfer expe r imen t is not suited to allow the fly to decide b e t w e e n the alter- natives.

Assume that the animals fail to display the m e m o r y because they do not recognize the situa- tion after t ransfer to be the same as that before. In this case, one could try to focus the fly's at tention on the similarities b e t w e e n the old and n e w situa- tion. To this end, flies are again t ransferred to a n e w chamber after the training phase. But, now, a 10-sec training session (priming) p recedes the m e m o r y test. The heat-associated side is the same in bo th chambers . To assure that all flies experi-

ence some heat at all during the lO-sec period, pr iming is always started wi th the flies on the heat- associated side. The results are shown in Figure 5. After pr iming flies show a preference for the pre- viously cold side. If flies are placed back into their old c h a m b e r and pr imed there, m e m o r y scores are not significantly greater (data not shown). As a fur- ther control , naive animals are t reated for 10 sec in the same m a n n e r (Fig. 6). This short training pe-

riod alone does not establish a significant m e m o r y score in the subsequent test.

The exper iments of Figures 5 and 6 suggest that the handling during the transfer to the n e w chamber does not destroy the m e m o r y trace but, rather, masks it. Yet, an alternative explanat ion still remains. The flies might be sensitized during train- ing in the first chamber to quickly mark one side wi th olfactory landmarks during priming. In the second chamber , then, only the sensitized flies would be able to mark sufficiently the appropr ia te side during the 10-see pr iming period. To test this hypothesis a so-called reverse-priming expe r imen t is performed. Flies are trained for 3 min in one chamber . After t ransfer to the n e w chamber the alternative side of the c h a m b e r is pun ished during pr iming ( compared to training in the first one). Flies trained to avoid the left side in the first cham- ber are p r imed to avoid the right side in the second one, and vice versa. If, in contrast to the naive flies,

Figure 4: Transfer to the old chamber. After training flies (wild-type CS; n = 61 ) are taken out of the chamber and reintroduced back into the same chamber for the memory test. The animals do not show any memory after handling between training and test.

Figure 5: Priming experiment. PIs of preference test (30 sec), training (180 sec), priming (10 sec) and memory test (60 sec). A fly (wild-type CS; n = 72) is trained for 3 min in one chamber and is subsequently transferred to a new chamber. There, the priming (e.g., training) starts when the fly enters the punished side. Hence, each fly gets heated about 6 sec during priming. After priming the memory test starts immediately. The PI of the learn- ing test is significantly different from zero (P < 0.003).

& 331

L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 5: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

Wustmann and Heisenberg

ther avoid the p r imed side nor the side correspond- ing to the one hea ted in the first chamber . To- gether, the two exper iments indicate that the priming reactivates the m e m o r y of the training in the first chamber . Odor cues are not used as essen- tial landmarks.

Figure 6: No learning is observed with priming alone. A fly (wild-type CS; n = 84) is placed into a chamber and trained for 10 sec with heat. Training begins when the fly enters the punished side. This is why the PI of the train- ing is slightly negative. Immediately after training the test (60 sec) starts.

these flies develop a side p re fe rence according to the polari ty of priming, then during training in the old c h a m b e r they must have learned (non-associa- tively) to deposi t during the short priming per iod olfactory landmarks for orientat ion in the m e m o r y test. The data (Fig. 7) reveal no such effect. Re- verse pr iming seems to confuse the flies. They nei-

SIDE OF ENTRY

If the priming alone does not provide the ref- erence for the choice b e t w e e n the two sides in the test, wha t are the cues telling the fly w h i c h side in the second chamber cor responds to w h i c h in the first one? In the exper iments descr ibed so far flies are p laced into the chambers always f rom the same side. It is possible, therefore, that flies use the side of entry as reference. To address this question, in the next transfer expe r imen t flies are placed into and taken out of the training c h a m b e r f rom the left but are p laced into the test chamber f rom the right. The results of Figure 8 s h o w clearly that the animals avoid the previously punished side as well as they did w h e n enter ing the two chambers f rom the same side. Obviously, they do not use the side of entry as a reference for orientation.

Figure 7" Priming of the alternative side. A fly (wild- type CS; n = 48) is trained to avoid the left side in a chamber. Immediately after training the animal is trans- ferred to another chamber. In the new one priming starts when the fly enters the punished side. But in the new chamber the fly is heated on the right side (the alterna- tive side of the training in the former chamber). Half of the flies are trained right and primed left, the other half in the opposite way. No positive memory score is ob- served in the subsequent test.

Figure 8: Transfer experiment with alternative en- trances. A fly (CS; n = 108) is placed into a chamber from the/eft and trained to avoid one particular side (left or right). Immediately after training the fly is removed through the same left entrance and put into a new cham- ber through the other (right) entrance. The training in chamber 1 and priming in chamber 2 take place at the same side of the chambers. The PI of the memory test is significantly different from zero (P < 0.001 ).The flies do not use the entrance as a reference for orientation in the chambers.

& 332

L E A R N / N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 6: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

PRIMING AS A PREREQUISITE

LEARNING IN DARKNESS

The previous expe r imen t suggests that in the test chamber the flies can identify the sides corre- sponding to the hea ted and nonhea ted sides of the training chamber , i rrespective of the side of entry and the side of priming. The chambers are illumi- nated th rough the lateral acryl glass plates by two blue or two yel low lights that are alternatively swi tched on depending on the fly's posi t ion in the chamber . In principle, the fly can distinguish its posi t ion wi th respect to the light gate by the di- rect ion and color of illumination. It is therefore conceivable that the flies use visual stimuli for their orientat ion in the chamber . To examine this hy- pothesis, the next exper iments are conduc ted in comple te darkness. After placing a fly into each chamber all lights are swi tched off. Only the two IR-emitting diodes of the directional light gate are still in operation, but flies are blind at this spectral wave leng th ( X - 950 nm) (Heisenberg and Wolf 1984). In Figure 9 the PIs in darkness are compared to those obtained under normal illumination con- ditions. There are no significant differences be- tween the two groups. The data show clearly that no vision is required for this type of learning in the

chamber . In the presen t paradigm locomotor activity is a

prerequisi te for conditioning. Flies in darkness might still be less or more active than in the light.

1 V " - - - - con t ro l - - d a r k n e s s a, TEST . . . .

n=51 n=86 �9 TRAINING

0.8h . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Z S.E. M. ....

0.6 ,.'-" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

-

&. 0.4 . . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . . . .

0.2 .... / ~ - . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

0 ~ / /

-0.2 i i I i / . . /~ E ,

30 60 90 120 150 30 60

t ime [s]

Figure 9: Learning in darkness. For one group of flies (CS; control; n = 51; same data as Fig. 2) during the whole experiment the chambers and the laboratory are illuminated as during all others experiments described in Materials and Methods. For the other group of flies (CS; darkness; n = 86) lights are switched off, i.e., flies are in complete darkness. No significant differences of the PIs between the two groups are seen.

. . . . . . . . . . . . . ,, rEsT �9 TRAINING

. . . . . . . . ~ . . . ~ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . - S.E.M. .. . .

2 . . . . . . . . .

. . . . con t ro l n=51

0

da rkness n=86

/ . . . /

I I I I 4 t r I I l /

30 60 90 120 150 30 60

t ime [sl

Figure 10: Average number of passages at the light gate per minute during the 240-sec experimental session of the two groups of flies in the experiment of Fig. 9.

This would make the compar i son of the two con- ditions more difficult. Activity scores are shown in Figure 10. During the first minute of the experi- ment no difference in locomotor activity (mea- sured by count ing the transitions at the light gate) is observed. In bo th groups a slight increase is seen after the beginning of the training period. Thereaf- ter, the activity of the flies in darkness decreases during the rest of the training period, whereas it stays high in the light. Interestingly, during the transit ion b e t w e e n training and m e m o r y tests ac- tivity in the light decreases sharply to the level that bo th groups s h o w during the m e m o r y tests. The same levels of activity during the m e m o r y tests in light and darkness suppor t the above conclusion that light is not required for learning in the present exper iment . The fly must use tactile and idiothetic htformation (Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt 1973) to distinguish b e t w e e n the two halves of the cham- ber.

CUES FROM OUTSIDE THE CHAMBER

In the t ransfer exper iment , however , tactile and idiothetic information cannot provide the ref- e rence needed to match the two sides in the old and n e w chamber . If vision is not required, odor cues are not used and nei ther the side of entry nor the side of pr iming provides the reference in the test chamber ; one wonde r s w h e t h e r the electro- magnet ic field or o ther (unidentified) cues outside the chamber might serve that purpose . This hy- pothesis can be tested by a simple procedure : As above, flies are trained in one chamber and trans-

& 333

L E A R N / N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 7: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

Wustmann and Heisenberg

ferred for the test to another similar one. In the n e w c h a m b e r they are p r imed for 10 sec and tested for 60 sec. In this case, however , the old and n e w chambers are or ienta ted at right angles to each other. The positive m e m o r y score (Fig. 11) shows that the flies do not need any external reference such as the e lec t romagnet ic field to orientate cor- rectly in the test chamber .

WHAT IS THE REFERENCE?

Have all conceivable references n o w been ex- cluded? Do we have to postulate a new, yet uni- dentified sense for orientat ion in flies? No. The reader may be aware that w e have demons t ra ted independence of vision only for the s tandard ex- pe r iment wi thou t transfer. It is still possible that visual cues are used as re ference in the test cham- ber after transfer. To test this last opt ion flies are trained in one chamber in comple te darkness and tested in similar chamber in darkness as well. Only during t ransfer the chambers are i l luminated by the room light to facilitate the manipulations. In the two chambers opposi te sides are heated, that is, the left side in chamber 1 and the right side for

Figure 12: Learning in darkness: Priming of the alter- native side. Flies (CS; n = 50) are trained to avoid one particular side of chamber 1. Immediately after training flies are transferred to chamber 2. In this chamber the flies are punished (priming) on the opposite side (com- pared to chamber 1). In the test, animals keep avoiding the primed side (P < 0.006).

priming in chamber 2, and vice versa. The results are shown in Figure 12. In contrast to the result in the light (Fig. 7), the flies do not show confusion but avoid the pr imed side. As in the light (Fig. 6), in darkness a lO-sec training session does not elicit a m e m o r y score in naive flies (Fig. 13). In darkness, the flies use the side of pr iming as reference. The different ou tcome of the expe r imen t in the light (Fig. 7) shows that the flies do rely on visual cues in the transfer exper imen t if they are available.

Figure 11 : Test for outside references. PIs of preference test (30 sec), training (180 sec), priming (10 sec), and memory test (60 sec). A fly (wild-type Berlin; n = 83) is trained for 3 min in one chamber and is subsequently transferred to a similar one. The chambers are orientated at right angles to each other. Because of the 90 ~ rotation of any outside references between training and test, the flies should be unable to use them for avoiding the pre- viously heated side. The PI of the memory test is signifi- cantly different from zero (P < 0.0001) and not different from that of the transfer experiment without rotation (P > 0.54).

D i s c u s s i o n

The heat ing chamber descr ibed by Wus tmann et al. (1996) introduces a fast, simple, and robust single-fly m e m o r y test for Drosophila wild type and mutants. In the original presenta t ion of the chamber the claim that it assesses learning and m e m o r y was based on circumstantial evidence. It could still be argued that the flies might deposi t a chemical marker during training on one side of the chamber and later use it for p r o p e r orientat ion in the test, wi thou t any learning being involved. The transfer exper imen t in the presen t s tudy firmly

rules out this explanation. Of part icular interest is the finding that the

simple transfer exper imen t does not work. A zero m e m o r y score is measured after the handling, irre- spective of w h e t h e r the fly is t ransferred to a n e w chamber or back into the old one. This result alone

& 334

L E A R N / N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 8: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

PRIMING AS A PREREQUISITE

Figure 13: Learning in darkness: No learning is ob- served with priming alone. A fly (CS; n = 81) is placed into a chamber and trained for 10 sec with heat. Imme- diately after training the test (60 sec) starts. No side pref- erence is seen after the training.

might be interpreted to indicate that the handl ing erases the m e m o r y trace. However, wi th a br ief training after the handl ing the PI is up again.

The pr iming has two independen t effects: (1) It provides information about the polarity of the n e w chamber wi th respect to heat and lack of heat; and (2) it seems to channel the fly's "a t tent ion" to this part icular task. The precise nature of the chan- nel ing is not understood. Attention has been de- scr ibed for Drosophila earlier in a visual orienta- tion task in w h i c h animals confine their course control responses to certain regions of the visual field (Wolf and Heisenberg 1980; Heisenberg and Wolf 1984). It may be similar to the pr iming effect observed here in that the animal can selectively direct its behavior to certain aspects of the situa- tion. In the present case, the m e m o r y of the con- dit ioning process that is suppressed because of handl ing during the transfer is restored by a brief repet i t ion of the original training procedure. That is, not the m e m o r y of the lesson but its retrieval is impaired in the present example . Impai rment of retrieval as distinct f rom m e m o r y loss has also been repor ted for the Drosophila mutant amnesiac (Quinn et al. 1979) and in honeybees (Menzel et al. 1991, Hammer and Menzel 1995). In the honey bees, inject ion of dopamine into the region of the central complex transiently interferes wi th m e m o r y retrieval. What is n e w in the present ac- count is that retrieval is manipula ted purely by be- havioral means.

The argument inferring that the test PI in the

transfer exper iments reflects the reactivated m e m o r y from the training in the first chamber is based on two additional controls: (1) The pr iming in the test c h a m b e r by itself, that is, wi thout the training in the first chamber , does not generate a positive PI (Fig. 6); and (2) wi th the normal illumi- nat ion of the chambers , pr iming restores PIs only if heat is appl ied wi th the same polarity as during the training in the first chambers (Fig. 5). With the opposi te polarity PIs are close to zero (Fig. 7) sug- gesting that the information about the polari ty de- rived from the pr iming is in conflict wi th some other information about polarity stored in memory, w h i c h as we n o w know is visual (see below). This ou tcome implies that the side preference ex- pressed by the test PI is at least partly based on information gathered in the first c h a m b e r and, con- sequently, cannot be expla ined entirely as an un- condit ional chemosensory response.

An impor tant part of the conclusion is based on a compar ison of the exper imen t of Figure 12 wi th that of Figure 7. In both cases, flies are trained in c h a m b e r 1 to avoid one particular side and p r imed in c h a m b e r 2 to avoid the opposi te side. The only difference is the absence of light in the exper imen t of Figure 12. In this case, the animals rely entirely on the pr iming in their side prefer- ence. They avoid the p r imed side, a l though a l O- sec training alone is not sufficient to induce a con- sistent side preference. Formally, the training in the first c h a m b e r could be said to increase the salience of the reinforcer (heat) during the subse- quent priming. However, one should keep in mind that the fly, primarily, has to learn to treat the tem- poral sequence of heat and lack of heat as a spatial problem. Only after it has acquired this ability can it manage to get the polarity right during the lO-sec pr iming period.

The expe r imen t of Figure 7 indicates that the animals use visual cues, if present, as wel l as idio- thetic and tactile informat ion for their side prefer- ence. In darkness, they rely entirely on the latter. On the other hand, the additional visual cues do not increase the m e m o r y score if they have the same polarity as the priming. Little is k n o w n about what limits the PI during the test.

The transfer exper iment should work only if the two chambers are recognized as similar by the flies. In fact, the flies do not indicate to us by their behavior that they distinguish them. After transfer to the n e w and to the old c h a m b e r they show the same m e m o r y and activity scores. This suggests that the two sides of the chamber also appear simi-

& 335

L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 9: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

Wustmann and Heisenberg

lar to the flies. It is impor tant to r e m e m b e r (see Materials and Methods) that the only visual cues the fly can exploit during the test are the color, subjective intensity, and direction of illumination, w h i c h change abrupt ly b e t w e e n two alternating states w h e n e v e r the fly passes in front of the light gate. It is quite remarkable that the fly attr ibutes any significance to this entirely artificial cue at all and that it relies on it about as much as on the lO-sec pr iming to de termine the hea t / l ack of heat

polarity in the test chamber . These exper iments demons t ra te that D. mela-

nogaster is sufficiently well or ientated in comple te

darkness to pass our avoidance and m e m o r y tests. This basic sense of space, w h i c h relies on idiot- hetic orientat ion and thus in turn only on tactile and propr iocept ive information, is little under- s tood in Drosophila as well as in humans.

Acknowledgments The apparatus was built in the workshops of K. Ochsner

and H. Kaderschabeck. Their expertise substantially improved the electronic and mechanical design. The excellent technical assistance of S. Flursch0tz is gratefully acknowledged. We also thank C. Hayward and J. Dickinson for working on the English of an earlier version of the manuscript, R. Wolf for redesigning Figure 1, and H. Spanheimer for final corrections on spelling.

The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 USC section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

References Aceves-Pina, E.O., R. Booker, J.S. Duerr, M.S. Livingstone, W.G. Quinn, R.F. Smith, P.P. Sziber, B.L. Tempel, and T.P. Tully. 1983. Learning and memory in Drosophila, studies with mutants. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 48: 831-839.

Davis, R.L. 1996. Physiology and biochemistry of Drosophila learning mutants. Physiol. Rev. 76:299-31 7.

DeZazzo, J. and T. Tully. 1995. Dissection of memory formation: From behavioral pharmacology to molecular genetics. Trends Neurosci. 18:212-218.

Dudai, Y. 1988. Neurogenetic dissection of learning and short-term memory in Drosophila. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11 : 537-563.

Dudai, Y., Y. Jan, D. Byers, W.G. Quinn, and S. Benzer. 1976. dunce, a mutant of Drosophila deficient in learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 73" 1684-1688.

Dura, J.-M., T. Preat, and T. Tully. 1993. Identification of

linotte, a new gene affecting learning and memory in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurogenet. 9:1-14.

Hammer, M. and R. Menzel. 1995. Learning and memory in the honeybee. J. Neurosci. 15:1617-1630.

Heisenberg, M. 1989. Genetic approach to learning and memory (mnemogenetics) in Drosophila melanogaster. In Fundamentals of memory formation: Neural plasticity and brain function (ed. H. Rahmann), pp. 3-45. Gustav Fischer, Stuttgart, Germany.

Heisenberg, M. and R. Wolf. 1984. Vision in Drosophila. In Studies of brain function (ed. V. Braitenberg) Vol. 12. Springer, Berlin, Germany.

Menzel, R., M. Hammer, G. Braun, J. Mauelshagen, and M. Sugawa. 1991. Neurobiology of learning and memory in honeybees. In The behavior and physiology of bees (ed. L.J. Goodman, and R.C. Fisher), pp. 323-353. CAB International, Wallingford, UK.

Mittelstaedt, H. and M.-L. Mittelstaedt. 1973. Mechanismen der Orientierung ohne richtende Aussenreize. Fortschr. Zool. 21 : 45-58.

Quinn, W.G., P.P. Sziber, and R. Booker. 1979. The Drosophila memory mutant amnesiac. Nature 277:212-214.

Spatz, H.-Ch. 1995. Posttranslational modification of protein kinase A. The link between short-term and long-term memory. Behav. Brain Res. 66: 79-84.

Tully, T. and W.G. Quinn. 1985. Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A 156: 263-277.

Wolf, R. and M. Heisenberg. 1980. On the fine structure of yaw torque in visual flight orientation of Drosophila melanogaster. II. A temporally and spatially variable weighting function for the visual field ("visual attention"). J. Comp. Physiol. A 140: 69-80.

Wustmann, G., K. Rein, R. Wolf, and M. Heisenberg. 1996. A new paradigm for operant conditioning of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A 179: 429-436.

Received June 23, 1997; accepted in revised form September 26, 1997.

& 336

L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from

Page 10: Behavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial …learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.pdfBehavioral Manipulation of Retrieval in a Spatial Memory Task for Drosophila melanogaster

10.1101/lm.4.4.328Access the most recent version at doi: 4:1997, Learn. Mem. 

  G Wustmann and M Heisenberg  Drosophila melanogaster.Behavioral manipulation of retrieval in a spatial memory task for

  References

  http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/4/4/328.full.html#ref-list-1

This article cites 13 articles, 3 of which can be accessed free at:

  License

ServiceEmail Alerting

  click here.top right corner of the article or

Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the

Copyright © Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press on March 14, 2020 - Published by learnmem.cshlp.orgDownloaded from