computer-based learning of neuroanatomy: cognitive science

36
Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science applied to anatomy education Julia Chariker, Ph.D. Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences Bioinformatics Core University of Louisville

Upload: others

Post on 21-Apr-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Computer-based Learning

of Neuroanatomy:

Cognitive science applied to

anatomy education

Julia Chariker, Ph.D.

Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences

Bioinformatics Core

University of Louisville

Page 2: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

We are grateful for support from:

Grant RO1-LM008323 from the National Library of Medicine,

National Institutes of Health, John Pani, PI

Instructional Graphics Laboratory

John Pani, Director Julia Chariker Farah Naaz

Page 3: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Magnetic Resonance Image (MRI) Cryosection Microanatomical Section

Students must learn to identify whole

structures and sectioned structures.

In sectional anatomy, structures must by

identified in two-dimensional sections that

have been sampled from a three-

dimensional structure.

Page 4: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Sectional Anatomy:

A Challenge for Cognition

A single 3D structure

can appear very

different in sectional

anatomy depending on

the orientation and

depth of the section.

Two very differently

shaped 3D structures

can appear similar in

sectional anatomy.

Page 5: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Neuroanatomy

Anatomical structures are densely packed together and irregularly

shaped. There is little color or texture to distinguish them.

Page 6: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Why computer based learning?

• Computers offer unique approaches to

visualization

– View structures and relationships from many

perspectives

– Models can be repeatedly dissected

• Capability for repeated self-study

Page 7: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Our Goals

• Research based design

• Improve learning in sectional anatomy

– Test hypotheses

• Ecological validity

– Materials and procedures for real classrooms

• Comprehensive assessment

– Learning over time

– Transfer to new situations

– Retention

Page 8: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Our Hypothesis

• Organization improves learning and

memory for material – For example, see Bower, Clark, Lesgold, and

Winzenz, 1969

• Hypothesis

– Developing rich knowledge of whole anatomy

may improve learning and retention of

sectional anatomy

Page 9: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Research Design

Learn WA Learn SA

Learn SA

“Whole then Sections”

“Sections Only”

Page 10: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Our approach to learning:

Adaptive Exploration

• High quality representation of the domain

• Tools for intuitive exploration

• Cycles of study, test, and feedback

The spacing effect in learning

See for example, Cepeda, Pashler, Vul,Wixted, & Rohrer, 2006

The testing effect in learning

See for example, Karpicke, & Roediger, 2008

Page 11: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Neuroanatomical Model

Amygdala

Brainstem, Crus Cerebri, Internal Capsule

Caudate Nucleus

Cerebellum

Cortex

Fornix

Globus Pallidus

Hippocampus

Hypothalamus

Mammillary Bodies

Nucleus Accumbens

Optic Tract

Pituitary Gland

Putamen

Red Nucleus

Substantia Nigra

Subthalamic Nucleus

Thalamus

Ventricles

Images available through the Visible Human

Project of the National Library of Medicine

Page 12: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Neuroanatomical Sections

Coronal Sagittal Axial

60

serial sections

50

serial sections

46

serial sections

Page 13: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Whole Anatomy Learning Program: Study

Participant could rotate the model 360 degrees in any direction, zoom

in and out on the model, remove and restore individual structures, and

select structures to find their name.

Page 14: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Whole Anatomy Learning Program: Test

Participant were asked to find and name as many structures as possible.

Page 15: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Whole Anatomy Learning Program: Feedback

Participants had all of the tools that were available in the study phase. They were also

provided with graphical feedback on their test performance. Structures named correctly

were green. Structures named incorrectly were red.

Page 16: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Sectional Anatomy Learning Program: Study

Participants were presented with a series of sections from one of the

three standard views. A slider was available that allowed participants

to explore the sections. Participants could stop on any section and

select individual structures to learn their name.

Page 17: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Sectional Anatomy Learning Program: Test

Participant were shown a series of sections, and were asked to name

the structures indicated with arrows. The structures that were tested

varied across the learning trials.

Page 18: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Sectional Anatomy Learning Program: Feedback

Participants had all of the tools available as in the study phase, but were

provided with graphical feedback on their test performance. Again, green

indicated a correct answer, and red indicated an incorrect answer.

Page 19: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Learning:

Repeated Cycles of Study-Test-Feedback

Coronal View Axial View Sagittal View Coronal View Axial View Sagittal View

90% 90% 90%

Performance Criterion

S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F S - T - F

Interleaving (alternating) material in learning improves retention

See for example, Taylor and Rohrer, 2009

A single learning trial consisted of …

Study (3 min.) – Test (unlimited) – Feedback (3 min.)

The view of anatomy presented for learning alternated across learning blocks.

Learning was completed upon reaching 90% accuracy in three consecutive views.

Page 20: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Research Design

Transfer to

Biomedical

Images

Retention of

Anatomy

2-3 weeks

Learn WA Learn SA

Learn SA

“Whole then Sections”

“Sections Only”

Page 21: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Testing Transfer to Biomedical Images

Magnetic Resonance Images (MRI) Digital Photographs of

Cryosections

(Visible Human Images)

Question: Could our participants use the anatomical knowledge gained from our model

to identify structures in biomedical images they had not see before?

Page 22: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Transfer Test: Uncued Recognition

Participants were given an image and asked to identify as many structures as

possible in the image.

Page 23: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Transfer Test: Submit Structure

Participants were provided with the name of a structure and asked to find the

structure in the image

Page 24: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Transfer Test: Submit Name

A structure was indicated by an arrow in the image, and participants were

asked to provide the name of the structure.

Page 25: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Retention Test

Participants were given a series of sections. In each section structures were

identified with arrows. Participants named the structures under the arrows.

Page 26: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Participants

• 72 undergraduate students

• 3 visits to the lab per week, one hour each

• On average, 5 weeks in the study

• Spatial Ability scores were balanced across

the groups (ranging from 5th to 99th

percentile in each group).

• No differences in spatial ability or rate of

visits to lab between groups.

Page 27: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Is whole anatomy more efficient to learn?

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Perc

en

t C

orr

ect

Learning Block

Whole Anatomy, WtS

Sectional Anatomy, SO

Participants learning whole

anatomy began learning at

a higher level of accuracy,

learned at a faster rate,

and completed learning in

half the time of those

learning sectional anatomy.

Page 28: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Does knowledge of whole anatomy improve the

efficiency of learning sectional anatomy?

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Perc

en

t C

orr

ect

Learning Block

Sectional Anatomy, WtS

Sectional Anatomy, SO

Participants in the whole

then sections group (WtS)

had higher initial accuracy

in learning sectional

anatomy and completed

learning more quickly than

participants in the sections

only group (SO).

Page 29: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Error in Learning

Time spent learning Number of errors in learning

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

Whole plus Sections Sections Alone

Nu

mb

er

of

Blo

cks

Whole AnatomySectional Anatomy

Whole then Sections Sections Only

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

110

Whole plus Sections Sections AloneN

um

ber

of

Err

ors

Whole AnatomySectional Anatomy

Whole then Sections Sections Only

Although the whole then sections group took longer to learn two views of anatomy, they made

significantly fewer errors in learning than the sections only group.

Page 30: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

Coronal Sagittal Axial

Pe

rce

nt

Co

rre

ct

View

Transfer

Sections AloneSections Only Sections Only Whole then Sections

Sections Only

How well is sectional anatomy retained over 2-3

weeks? Does knowledge of whole anatomy

support retention?

In the sagittal view,

performance was

significantly higher for the

whole then sections group.

In subsequent research we

found a greater benefit of

whole anatomy across all

views at retention intervals

of 4-8 weeks.

Retention of sectional

anatomy was still at the

criterion for learning in

the coronal view.

It fell just below criterion

in the sagittal and axial

views.

Page 31: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

Coronal Sagittal Axial Coronal Sagittal Axial Coronal Sagittal Axial

Pe

rce

nt

Co

rre

ct

View

MRI, Transfer

MRI, Sections Alone

VH, Transfer

VH, Sections AloneSections Only

Sections Only

Uncued Recognition Submit Structure Submit Name

Does knowledge derived from our model transfer to

biomedical images? Does knowledge of whole

anatomy support transfer to complex images?

Whole then Sections

Whole then Sections

Although

performance varied

across the tests,

participants were

able to transfer

knowledge of

anatomy to complex

biomedical images.

There is no evidence

in this study that

knowledge of whole

anatomy supported

this process.

Page 32: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Summary

• Adaptive Exploration

– Rapid learning

– Transfer of knowledge to complex biomedical

images

– High levels of retention at 2-3 weeks

Page 33: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Summary

• Whole anatomy supports learning in

sectional anatomy

– Initial accuracy is higher

– Learning is accomplished more quickly

– Less error over the entire course of learning

– Supports retention of sectional anatomy

Page 34: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Use in the classroom • Collaboration with Sandy Sephton (PBS), Ben Mast

(PBS), Cynthia Corbitt (Biology), Jeff Petruska

(ASNB), Robert Lundy (ASNB)

– Undergraduate neuroscience

– Graduate clinical neuroscience

– Programs for high school students

Page 35: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Subsequent Development

• New approaches to integrating whole and

sectional anatomy

• Longer retention intervals

• Transfer to biomedical images

• Evaluation in neuroscience courses

• Updated interface

• Increasing complexity of our

neuroanatomical model

Page 36: Computer-based Learning of Neuroanatomy: Cognitive science

Publications

Chariker, J. H., Naaz, F., & Pani, J. R. (2011). Computer-Based Learning of

Neuroanatomy: A Longitudinal Study of Learning, Transfer, and Retention. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 103(1), 19-31.

Chariker, J. H., Naaz, F., & Pani, J. R. (2012). Item difficulty in the evaluation of computer-

based instruction: An example from neuroanatomy. Anatomical Sciences Education, 5(2),

63-75.

Pani, J.R., Chariker, J.H., & Naaz, F (2013). Computer based learning: Interleaving whole

and sectional representation of neuroanatomy. Anatomical Sciences Education, 6(1), 11-

18.

Papers in Progress

Chariker, J. H., Naaz, F., & Pani, J. R. The effects of spatial ability in computer-based

learning of neuroanatomy. (Manuscript in preparation).

Pani, J. R., Chariker, J. H., Naaz, F., Roberts, J., & Sephton, S. E. Computer-based

learning of neuroanatomy in the undergraduate classroom. (Manuscript in preparation).

Naaz, F., Chariker, J. H., & Pani, J. R. Learning from graphically integrated 2D and 3D

representations improves retention of neuroanatomy. (Manuscript in preparation).