heterogeneous peer e ects: a study of education in the

39
Heterogeneous Peer Effects: A Study of Education in the Third World * Ashvin Gandhi Pomona College Current Version: May 6, 2013 * I thank Gary Smith, Samuel Antill, and the participants of Gary Smith’s Senior Exercise for their helpful comments. 1

Upload: others

Post on 09-Dec-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Heterogeneous Peer Effects: A Study of Education in

the Third World∗

Ashvin Gandhi

Pomona College

Current Version: May 6, 2013

∗I thank Gary Smith, Samuel Antill, and the participants of Gary Smith’s Senior Exercise for their helpfulcomments.

1

Introduction

Many factors contribute to educational outcomes. Undoubtedly, student-specific traits such

as intelligence, socioeconomic status, and parental education play a significant role in student

performance. Additionally, school level resources—textbooks, computers, teachers, etc.—

also affect educational attainment. Student educational outcomes may also be, at least in

part, a function of peer quality. High achieving peers may act as positive role models, while

low achieving ones may act as negative ones.1 A wealth of literature exists that examines

such educational peer effects and their policy implications. Epple et al. (2002), for example,

argues that if high achievers produce positive achievement externalities, then tracking high

achievers with other high achievers stands to harm low achievers in both an absolute and

relative sense: low achievers will perform worse than they would have without tracking, and

the achievement gap between high and low achievers will widen. Peer effects are not strictly

unidirectional, however. Zimmerman (2003), for example, finds that low-achieving college

students may impose a negative externality on their roommates.

Peer effects are not the sole consideration in determining whether to employ tracking.

Duflo et al. (2011) examines a randomized treatment of tracking in Kenyan schools and finds

that in spite of the effects described by Epple et al., tracking benefitted all students because

it allowed teachers to better target their teaching methods to the level of the class. Another

interesting framework through which to examine peer-driven variation is that of competition

with peers. Gneezy et al. (2003), for example, examines how the performance of men and

women differ in competitive environments.

In this paper, I extend previous analyses of peer effects by examining whether these effects

are different for men and women. Strong evidence already exists of positive externalities

from female role models in developing countries. Beaman et al. (2012) found the presence of

female role models in leadership positions in Indian villages substantially shrank the male-

1Additionally, if low achievement imposes a negative externality on other students (e.g. in group projects),there may be a peer-pressure effect as seen in Mas and Morretti (2009).

2

female aspiration gap for both adults and adolescents, erased the educational attainment

gap, and decreased the amount of time that women spent on educational attainment.2 Such

examinations, however, have focused on women in role-model positions rather than in peer

positions. One paper that does examine the role of gender in peer effects is Hoxby (2000),

which examines the role of classroom gender composition in peer effects.

I use two datasets to test the hypothesis that positive educational peer effects are different

for men and women. The first dataset is the Learning and Educational Attainment in Punjab

Schools (LEAPS) dataset, which has been used in a number of educational studies including

Andrabi et al. (2011) and Andrabi et al. (forthcoming). The dataset contains a panel

tracking 34,013 Punjab students’ scores in Urdu, English, and mathematics over a period of

four years. The panel is very unbalanced, however, so I am only able to find a subsample of

5849 students present for testing in the first two years. All of the students examined were in

third grade during the first year of the survey, and almost all of them graduated to fourth

grade in the second year. A small fraction of students—less than 1%—maintained their

status as third graders in the second year but were surveyed because they were still in the

same classroom as their fourth grade peers.3 This is because in some rural Punjab schools,

teachers teach multiple grade levels in the same classroom. I chose to include these students

because their presence in the same classroom may contribute to peer effects, however all of

my results are robust to their exclusion.

The second dataset I use contains student test scores from Western Kenya’s experimental

Extra Teacher Program (ETP).4 Through the ETP, 121 Kenyan primary schools that only

had one first grade teacher were able to hire a second contract teacher for the second term

of the 2005 school year. Of these 121 schools, 60 were asked to assign students into sections

by achievement level while 61 were asked to randomize assignment. Duflo et al. (2011)

2The positive externalizes of higher achieving females do not only affect girls. Andrabi et al. (2012)finds that even minimal maternal education has a substantial positive effect on the educational outcomes forchildren of both genders.

3Both third and fourth grade students took the same test in the second year.4These data can be downloaded from the Jameel Poverty Action Lab (J-PAL) website.

3

compares tracking and non-tracking schools to study the costs and benefits of achievement

tracking. Because I am interested only in peer effects, I restrict my analysis to those 61

schools with random assignment.

In order to test my hypothesis, I use regressions on both datasets to exploit variation

in class composition. In particular, I test the relationship between a student’s score and

the mean and semi standard deviation of his or her peers’ scores, as well as whether or not

the student’s class has a “high achiever.”5 By interacting class-level peer-quality variables

with dummy variables for student sex, I am able to separate the peer effects on men and on

women.6

One major challenge to any study of peer effects is stated well by Caroline Hoxby (2000):

The central problem with estimating peer effects in schools is that vast majority of

cross-sectional variation in students’ peers is generated by selection. Families self-

select into schools based on their incomes, job locations, residential preferences,

and educational preferences. A family may even self-select into a school based

on the ability of an individual child.

Thus, I must be careful not to confuse selection effects for peer effects. Using a panel of

data certainly helps avoid this problem. It should be relatively infrequent that students

change schools within the panel based on their ability. However, it is still possible that

variation in class composition was not randomized but was deliberately selected, which may

lead to incorrect inference. For example, a school may track high achievers with other high

achievers, which might lead one to falsely infer that high achieving peers are a cause—rather

than coincidence—of high achieving students. Furthermore, it may be that high quality

teachers improve student quality across the class, which might lead to the false inference

that one student’s high score is caused by the other students’ high scores, when in reality

5See the section on methodology for the definition of semi standard deviation used in this paper.6The control variables I use in my regressions are borrowed partially from Andrabi et al. (2011) and

Duflo et al. (2011).

4

the two are caused by the same thing. In order to lessen such problems of endogeneity, I

employ an instrumental variables approach.

Data

In my analysis I use two educational datasets: the first is from the Learning and Educa-

tional Attainment in Punjab Schools (LEAPS) survey, and the second is from Kenya’s Extra

Teacher Program (ETP).

Learning and Educational Attainment in Punjab Schools

The following quotation from the Learning and Educational Attainment in Punjab Schools

(LEAPS) Project website describes the data composition:

The LEAPS Survey consists of data from 823 schools in 112 villages in 3 districts

of Punjab-Attock, Faisalabad and Rahim Yar Khan. These districts represent an

accepted stratification of the province into North (Attock), Central (Faisalabad)

and South (Rahim Yar Khan). The 112 villages in these districts were chosen

randomly from the list of all villages with an existing private school.

These data have been used before to study educational achievement in Pakistan (Andrabi

et al. forthcoming) and teacher value-added metrics (Andrabi et al. 2009).

The LEAPS dataset includes an unbalanced panel of test scores for approximately 34,000

students in rural Punjab schools over four years. Tests were administered near the end of the

school year, allowing sufficient time for students to be affected by their classmates should

peer effects be present.7 Each year, students were given extensive standardized tests in

three key areas: mathematics, English, and Urdu. The questions on these tests were not

7For a smaller sub-sample, highly detailed information on household and demographic information isavailable. I do not make use of this information, however, because it would require me to greatly restrict mysample size.

5

all weighted identically, and the LEAPS Project used a weighting mechanism to develop

test scores in each area that best reflect student performance. I use these test scores in my

analysis and do not modify them in any way, such as by normalizing them.

I restricted the dataset to students who are present in the sample for the first and second

years of the panel, and whose class in the second year had at least fifteen people. I impose the

first restriction because I use statistics from both the first and second years in the regressions.

I impose the second restriction so that the classes are large enough to avoid small-sample

problems with the class-level statistics, especially the class semi standard deviation. After

restricting, there are 5849 students left in the dataset. All of these students were in third

grade in the first year of the panel, and nearly all of these students were in fourth grade

in the second year of the panel. Both third and fourth grade students were given the same

test in the second year. In my exposition, I use the term year to refer to years in the panel

and grade to refer to the child’s school grade. In general, I will refer to the year 1 scores as

baselines cores and the year 2 scores as endline scores.

Table 1 gives student-level summary statistics of test scores, and Table 2 gives other

student-level summary statistics relevant to the analysis. Table 3 gives a summary of

classroom-level statistics. Of particular note is that most students were in either all-male

or all-female classes. Only 1394 of the 5849 students attended coeducational classes. Unfor-

tunately, this prevents me from doing more thorough analyses of co-ed classroom dynamics

using the LEAPS data. For example, it might have been interesting to ask whether girls are

more affected by their female than their male peers.

Extra Teacher Program

With funding from the World Bank, ICS Africa was able to hire an extra first grade teacher

for 140 primary schools in Western Kenya.8 Of these schools, 121 originally only had one

first grade teacher and were able to split that single section into two at the start of the

8ICS stands for Investing in Children and their Societies.

6

second term. Of those 121 schools, 61 were required to randomly assign students into the

two sections. I focus my analysis on these 61 schools, exploiting the randomized variation

in peer-quality caused by the random assignment of students to different teachers.

Before classes were split for the second term, each school assigned its students a first

term score. The material tested in assigning this first term score was not standardized

across schools and are therefore normalized at the school level in the dataset. Still, these

baseline scores provide good proxies for student achievement before the second term and

highly correlate with their performance on standardized tests in mathematics and literature

given at the end of the second term. These second term tests were identical for all schools

participating in the ETP program. In analyzing the ETP dataset, I will usually refer to the

second term standardized test scores as endline scores and the first term scores as baselines

scores. Table 10 gives the summary statistics for student test scores and other variables of

interest.

One key advantage to the ETP dataset over the LEAPS dataset is that the ETP data

come from an experiment and therefore have truly randomized assignment of students to

classes. This controls for a number of factors—such as tracking—that might simultaneously

affect one’s performance and peer quality. Notably, one source of endogeneity that we must

still be careful of is teacher quality.

On the other hand, there a few key disadvantages of the ETP data. First, the ETP data

have been normalized by the Jameel Poverty Action Lab (J-PAL), which hosts the data.

Student test scores were normalized to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. The

classroom level statistics, however, were normalized using a more complex mechanism that

cannot be reversed and is not entirely transparent in the code from Duflo et al. (2011).

Therefore, one must interpret the results in context of these normalized variables. That the

normalization cannot be reversed prevents determining separate means for men and women

that are consistent with the classroom means present in the J-PAL data. To prevent misuse

of these normalized variables, I closely follow the methodology used by Duflo et al. (2011)

7

in the main analysis of this paper. In Appendix B, however, I analyze within and across

gender peer effects using alternately calculated means.

Methodology

In testing for the effects of high or low achieving students on their peers, I would ideally

like to employ a methodology similar to that in Mas and Moretti (2009). Mas and Moretti

study the effect of high-productivity workers being present on the productivity of generally

low-productivity workers. However, Mas and Moretti have a long panel, while the LEAPS

and ETP panels are very short. The principle, though, remains the same: the regressions

need to control for other factors that may contribute to changes in performance. To do this,

I closely follow the methodology of Duflo et al. (2011) in employing the following regressions

on student endline test scores yij (where i indexes individuals and j indexes class):

yij = α0 + αxij + βbij + λy−ij + εij (1)

yij = α0 + αxij + βbij + λy−ij + λffemaleij × y−ij + εij (2)

yij = α0 + αxij + βbij + λy−ij + γSemiSD(y−ij) + εij (3)

yij = α0 + αxij + βbij + λy−ij + γSemiSD(y−ij) + γffemaleij × SemiSD(y−ij) + εij (4)

yij = α0 + αxij + βbij + λy−ij + ϕHA−ij + εij (5)

8

yij = α0 + αxij + βbij + λy−ij + ϕHA−ij + ϕffemaleij ×HA−ij + εij (6)

In the above equations, x gives a matrix of control variables. In the analysis of the LEAPS

dataset, x includes age, school type (public or private), indicator variables for student and

teacher gender, indicator variables for all-male and all-female classes, and village-level fixed

effects. In the analysis of the ETP dataset, x includes age, an indicator for student gender,

an indicator for whether the teacher was a new ETP hire, and school-level fixed effects. My

choices of controls derive from Andrabi et al. (2009) and Duflo et al. (2011), respectively.

The variable bij gives a student’s baseline test score. In the LEAPS dataset, a student’s

baseline score is taken to be their subject-specific standardized test score in the previous

year. In the ETP dataset, a student’s baseline score is their score from the first term before

the classes were split. It is worth noting that the baseline scores in the ETP dataset are not

standardized across schools, and as such are normalized within each school.

The variables y−ij and SemiSD(y−ij) give the mean and semi standard deviation of

scores received by student i’s classmates. Formally, I define the semi standard deviation as

follows:

SemiSD(yij) =

√√√√ 1

|Classj|∑

ykj>y−ij ,k 6=i

(ykj − y−ij)2.

In other words, I define the semi standard deviation to be the square root of the average

squared distance from the mean for above-average students in student i’s class. This gives a

metric of the dispersion of above average students in student i’s class. A high semi standard

deviation might indicate the presence of outlier students.

Importantly, I exclude student i when calculating statistics with a subscript of −i because

to not do so would bias the coefficients.9 In each of these regressions, I instrument for y−ij

and SemiSD(y−ij) with b−ij and SemiSD(b−ij), respectively, in a two-stage least squares

9For example, if I did not exclude yij from the mean, an increase in yij would actually cause the meanscore to increase. This could lead one to falsely infer that high class means cause high scores when theopposite is true.

9

regression. I employ an instrumental variables approach because y−ij and SemiSD(y−ij) are

likely endogenous, and to simply apply OLS might bias the coefficients and lead to incorrect

inference. For example, good teachers might cause both y−ij and yij to increase. Without

an instrumental variables approach, this would bias the coefficient of y−ij upwards and yield

evidence of peer effects even when none exist.

The variable HA−ij is a dummy variable for at least one of student i’s classmates being

a high achieving student. In the analysis of the LEAPS dataset, I define a high achieving

student to be one whose test score is above the 95th percentile in the sample. Approximately

50% of students in the LEAPS dataset have a high achieving classmate. In the analysis of

the ETP dataset, I define high achievers to be the top performing first grade student at the

school.10 Since each school has two first grade classes that are approximately the same size,

approximately 50% of students in the ETP dataset are in a class with a high achiever. As

with y−ij and SemiSD(y−ij), I instrument for the high achieving variable using a baseline

high achieving variable.

In Equation (1), I test whether peer effects can be detected in correlation achievement

and average peer achievement. This equation is taken directly from Duflo, et al. (2011), who

find evidence of non-gender-specific peer effects in the ETP data. In Equation (2), I try to

differentiate whether average peer score affects males and females differently. In Equation

(3), I allow for the semi standard deviation of peer scores to affect student scores. One

might expect this for a number of reasons. For example, a high semi standard deviation

might indicate the presence of very high performing students, and these strong positive

outliers might be the primary drivers of peer effects. If this is the case, the coefficients in

Equation (3) will have significant implications for how to group students to optimize positive

peer effects. Alternatively, it may be that teacher efficacy is a function of the variance of

student achievement—e.g. Duflo et al. (2011) suggests that teachers are more effective at

teaching classes with a lower variance of achievement. Equation (4) tests whether the semi

10High achievers cannot be defined using percentiles in the ETP dataset because the baseline scores arenot standardized across schools.

10

standard deviation of classmate scores affects males and females differently. Equation (5)

tests whether high achievers drive peer effects, and Equation (6) tests whether high achiever

effects are different for men and women.

I run each of these regressions three times for each dataset—once for each dependent

variable of interest. In the LEAPS analysis, the dependent variables are math score, English

score, and Urdu score. In the ETP analysis, the dependent variables are math score, liter-

ature score, and total score. In all regressions using the LEAPS data, standard errors are

clustered at the village level, and in all regressions using the ETP data, standard errors are

clustered at the school level.

Results

Learning and Educational Attainment in Punjab Schools

Tables 4, 5, and 6 give the results of the LEAPS regressions without using instrumental

variables. These regressions consistently show large statistically significant coefficients for

the class mean variable but not for the same variable interacted with the female dummy

variable. Notably these coefficients are largest for math, second largest for English, and

smallest for Urdu. Still, even the smallest coefficients are quite large. In general these

results indicate that a 1 point increase in the average peer score correlates with a .4 to .7

point increase in a student’s scores. It is likely that these very high coefficients are the result

of achievement tracking, self-selection, and other sources of endogeneity. Interestingly, the

coefficient on the female interacted class mean variable is not statistically significant in any

of the regressions.

The coefficients of the semi standard deviation variables are consistently negative with

very large coefficients. As with the coefficients on the class mean variable, this result should

be interpreted cautiously. The coefficient on the semi standard deviation variable may be

significant because having a greater spread of above average students reduces peer effects,

11

but it may simply be evidence of other patterns—e.g. that students of better teachers have

lower variance outcomes. Notably, the negative relationship between test scores and the peer

semi standard deviation appears much stronger for men than for women.

The coefficients of the dummy for having a high achiever in the classroom is always

negative with large magnitudes and statistical significance. This might indicate that high

achievers do not produce positive peer effects in proportion to how much they increase

the class mean—i.e., fixing mean classmate score, high achievers have a negative effect on

their peers. As with the mean and semi standard deviation, the coefficient may be driven

by endogenous factors. For example, good teachers might produce high achievers while

simultaneously improving the scores of all students. The female-interacted high achievement

variable is only statistically significant in the case of math scores, in which case it is large

and positive.

Tables 7, 8, and 9 give the results of the LEAPS regressions when a two-stage least

squares, instrumental variables approach is applied.11 While the application of instrumental

variables should mitigate many sources of endogeneity in our variables of interest, the large

standard errors in these regressions prevent inference about peer effects. All of the coefficients

regarding peer effects become statistically insignificant in these regressions, and the signs of

the coefficient are not always consistent with the regressions without instruments—e.g. the

coefficients for the class mean are generally negative in the two stage least squares regression,

while they were consistently positive when no instruments were used.

These results shed doubts on the reliability of the coefficients seen in the analysis without

instruments. They should not be taken as positive evidence that peer effects do not exist.

The standard errors of the coefficients in the two stage least squares regressions are very

large, and therefore, it may be that peer effects exist, but the standard errors are simply too

large to detect these effects. Little can be done to correct this here, and I hope to perform

11I do not include the first stages, however, I did a joint-significance test on the instruments in eachfirst-stage regression and found that the instruments were quite strong (the p-value associated with theF-statistics were all less than .0001).

12

similar analyses with larger datasets in the future. We can say with confidence, however,

that peer effects in these data are not strong enough to be discernable when applying an

instrumental variables approach.

While these regressions do not provide any conclusive evidence of peer effects—let alone

heterogeneity in peer effects—I do find that some of the control variables are consistently

significant in the regressions. Most notably, baseline scores consistently have a strong and

statistically significant positive relationship with endline scores. Since the baseline scores

were previous year scores, this likely reflects that prior ability strongly correlates with current

ability. That the coefficient of one’s baseline score is less than 1 indicates a regression to the

mean of student test scores. This phenomenon is commonly known—see for example Mee

and Chua (1991)—and does not cause any further problems for inference. Additionally, I

find age to consistently have a negative relationship with test scores. This may be because

students who are young for their grade and skipped a grade are likely high-ability, high

achieving students, while those who are old for their grade are likely low-ability, low achieving

students.

Extra Teacher Program

Tables 11, 12, and 13 give the results of the ETP regressions without using instrumental

variables. The results in these regressions can be interpreted less cautiously than those in

the LEAPS regressions without instruments because the ETP enforced random assignment

of students to teachers. Therefore, self-selection cannot bias the coefficients. Still, there is

some cause for concern because the effects of teacher quality might still yield problems of

endogeneity in the model.

In these regressions, I find large, statistically significant positive coefficients for the class

mean variable. Because the class mean variable has been normalized in a complex manner,

it is important to interpret its regression coefficient in context of the variable mean and stan-

dard deviation: .00009 and .1056, respectively. Therefore, for example, we should interpret

13

a regression coefficient of .5 as meaning that a one standard deviation increase in average

classmate quality leads to a .05 standard deviation increase in a student’s achievement.

Therefore, while the coefficients are very large, the peer effects they describe are actually

much smaller: the coefficients generally indicate that a one standard deviation increase in

the average peer score yields a .02 to .04 standard deviation increase in a student’s score.

These weak peer effects generally appear stronger for math scores than for literature scores.

There is little evidence from these regressions that these small peer effects differ between

women and men.

I find the class semi standard deviation variable to be consistently negative and statis-

tically significant.12 As before, while this may suggest that greater spread of above average

students reduces peer effects, it may also be the result of students of better teachers having

lower variance outcomes. In the case of math scores, there is some evidence that the negative

effect of the semi standard deviation of classmates is larger for women.

Regression (5) in both the math and total score regressions indicates that a high achieving

student may have positive peer effects more than in proportion to how much they increase

the class mean—i.e. fixing mean classmate score, high achievers improve their peers’ perfor-

mances by about .05 standard deviations. If these effects are believed, then the peer effects

of a high achiever on his peers’ math scores is greater than the peer effects produced by

a one standard deviation increase in the average peer math score. These effects, however,

are not statistically significant in regression (6), when a gender-interacted high achievement

variable is included.

Tables 14, 15, and 16 give the results of the LEAPS regressions when a two-stage least

squares, instrumental variables approach is applied.13 While the application of instrumental

variables mitigates problems of endogeneity, it does also reduce the efficiency of the coefficient

estimators, increasing the standard errors. Noticeably, many of the coefficients regarding peer

12While the coefficient of this variable are large, the effects they imply are much more reasonable in contextof the means and standard deviations of the semi standard deviation.

13I do not include the first stages, however they all show that the instruments are very strong (p valuesless than .0001).

14

effects become statistically insignificant in these regressions. The sole exception is that the

class mean variable consistently shows coefficients that are consistently positive and generally

statistically significant. As before, while these coefficients are large, they generally imply

relatively small peer effects: a one standard deviation increase in the average peer score

generally yields a .04 to .06 standard deviation increase in a student’s score. The result is

most robust in math scores, where it is at least statistically significant at the 10 percent level

in all of the regressions. Where the class mean is found insignificant, it is usually due to very

large standard errors. Notably, the regressions do not discern any statistically significant

difference in how average peer achievement affects males and females. However, this may

be driven by the fact that the standard errors are large while the peer effects in general are

very small, making it difficult to discern a difference should one exist.

The class semi standard deviation variable, though generally insignificant, is consistently

positive and even statistically significant at the 10 percent level in regression (3) of Table 15.

Its coefficient of nearly .6 is seemingly large, but taken in context of the standard deviation

of the class semi standard deviation, which is .19, the coefficient actually describes quite a

small effect. These results cast doubt on the large negative coefficients seen in Tables 11, 12,

and 13. The regressions do not discern any statistically significant difference in the way that

males and females are affected by the semi standard deviation of their classmates, but this

may be the result of large standard errors. None of the regressions find statistically significant

evidence that high achievers drive peer effects. This also, may be driven by relatively small

effects and large standard errors.

As in the LEAPS regression we find that some control variables are consistently signif-

icant. Most notably, baseline score consistently has a strong and statistically significant

positive relationship with student scores. This again likely reflects the persistence of innate

abilities from the baseline period into the testing period. Notably, the baseline score co-

efficients tend to be lower in the ETP regressions than in the LEAPS regressions. This is

likely because LEAPS baseline tests were standardized, subject-specific, and highly similar

15

to the endline tests, while the ETP tests were not. Therefore, the LEAPS baseline test

scores likely better correlate with aptitude for the endline tests than do the ETP baseline

scores. We also once again find age to consistently have a negative relationship with test

scores. This likely holds for the same reason in the ETP data as it did in the LEAPS data.

We also find evidence that the new ETP hires tend to improve student performance. This

is consistent with the findings of Duflo et al., who argue that the finding evidences poor

incentive structures for Kenyan teachers who are not new hires.

Conclusion

My results from analyzing the LEAPS data are generally inconclusive: I do not find any

evidence of peer effects that are robust to the use of an instrumental variables approach, but

this may simply be the result of large standard errors. The results from analyzing the ETP

data are more promising: I find some robust evidence of weak peer effects that positively

correlate with mean peer achievement. Namely, a 1 standard deviation increase in mean

peer quality yields approximately a .04 to .06 increase in student achievement through peer

effects. In my instrumental variable analysis, I do not find any evidence of robust differences

in how peer quality affects men and women. If gender heterogeneity in peer effects exist,

they are not large enough to be discernable from the ETP dataset. Even when instrumental

variables are not used, there is little to no evidence of different effects for men and women.

While this indicates that such heterogeneity likely does not exist or is too small to detect,

further analysis using larger datasets and yielding smaller standard errors would help to

determine this more conclusively. If my results are to be believed, then peer effects are likely

too small to be a major factor in education policy, and the differences between peer effects

for men and women certainly are.

While my analysis does not conclusively answer the questions about heterogeneity in

peer effects, it does provide testament to three important things. The first is the importance

16

of instrumental variables where endogeneity is suspected, for many of my results were not

robust to the use of instruments. The second is the importance of very large datasets when

analyzing such questions, for frequently my inference was hindered by large standard errors.

The third is the value of experiments in isolating desired variation. I am better able to detect

peer effects in the ETP dataset, even though it was smaller than the LEAPS dataset, likely

because the ETP data derived from a randomized experiment while the LEAPS data did

not.

References

Andrabi, Tahir, Jishnu Das, Asim Ijaz Khwaja, and Tristan Zajonc. 2011. “Do Value-Added

Estimates Add Value? Accounting for Learning Dynamics.” American Economic Journal:

Applied Economics, 3(3): 29-54.

Andrabi, Tahir, Jishnu Das, and Asim Ijaz Khwaja. 2012. “What Did You Do All Day?:

Maternal Education and Child Outcomes.” Journal of Human Resources 47(4): 873-912.

Beaman, L., Duflo, E., Pande, R., and Topalova, P. (2012). “Female leadership raises aspi-

rations and educational attainment for girls: A policy experiment in India.” Science, 335,

582586.

Duflo, Esther, Pascaline Dupas, and Michael Kremer. 2011. “Peer Effects, Teacher Incen-

tives, and the Impact of Tracking: Evidence from a Randomized Evaluation in Kenya,”

American Economic Review, 101(5): 1739-74.

Epple, Dennis, Elizabeth Newlon, and Richard Romano. 2002. “Ability Tracking, School

Competition, and the Distribution of Educational Benefits.” Journal of Public Economics,

17

83(1): 148.

Gneezy, Uri, Muriel Niederle, and Aldo Rustichini. 2003. “Performance in Competitive

Environments: Gender Differences.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(3): 1049-1074.

Hoxby, Caroline. 2000. “Peer Effects in the Classroom: Learning from Gender and Race

Variation.” National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper 7867.

Learning and Educational Achievement in Punjab Schools (LEAPS):Insights to Inform the

Policy Debate. Forthcoming, Oxford University Press: (Joint with Jishnu Das, Asim Ijaz

Khwaja, Tara Vishwanath and Tristan Zajonc.)

Mas, Alexander and Enrico Moretti. 2009. “Peers at Work.” American Economic Review,

99(1): 112-145.

Mee, Robert W. and Tin Chiu Chua. 1991. ”Regression toward the mean and the paired

sample t test.” The American Statistician 45(1): 39-42.

Zimmerman, David. 2003. “Peer Effects in Academic Outcomes: Evidence form a Natural

Experiment.” Review of Economics and Statistics, 85(1), 9-23.

18

Appendix A

Table 1: Student-level Score Summary Statistics

All Students, N=5849Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMath Score (Year 2) 541.52 144.58 24.47 941.99Math Score (Year 1) 519.74 128.45 6.16 903.70English Score (Year 2) 535.89 136.49 38.53 962.86English Score (Year 1) 505.59 135.70 0.49 905.08Urdu Score (Year 2) 549.90 142.79 22.26 1000.00Urdu Score (Year 1) 513.80 134.40 20.41 922.97

At Boys’ Schools, N=2501Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMath Score (Year 2) 538.74 150.15 24.47 867.65Math Score (Year 1) 516.28 128.12 6.16 854.12English Score (Year 2) 484.43 146.20 38.53 877.10English Score (Year 1) 460.44 138.17 0.49 815.93Urdu Score (Year 2) 514.46 154.29 22.26 896.38Urdu Score (Year 1) 482.75 141.65 20.41 922.97

At Girls’ Schools, N = 1954Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMath Score (Year 2) 498.71 142.17 24.47 852.81Math Score (Year 1) 494.11 137.48 6.16 903.70English Score (Year 2) 530.74 105.79 38.53 839.98English Score (Year 1) 492.42 115.62 54.88 870.61Urdu Score (Year 2) 543.46 130.23 22.26 975.40Urdu Score (Year 1) 511.80 130.82 20.41 888.32

At Coed Schools, N = 1394Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMath Score (Year 2) 606.51 110.21 62.69 941.99Math Score (Year 1) 561.90 102.90 58.10 792.30English Score (Year 2) 635.44 97.24 38.53 962.86English Score (Year 1) 605.06 102.44 76.51 905.08Urdu Score (Year 2) 622.52 107.22 22.26 1000.00Urdu Score (Year 1) 572.30 103.30 35.47 865.12

19

Table 2: Other Student-level Summary Statistics, N = 5849

Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxFemale 0.44 0.50 0 1Female Teacher 0.51 0.50 0 1Private School 0.19 0.39 0 1Age (Year 2) 10.44 1.40 7 17.83Grade (Year 2) 3.99 0.07 3 4Class Size 27.68 13.05 15 92Girls’ School 0.33 0.47 0 1Boys’ School 0.43 0.49 0 1

20

Table 3: Classroom Summary Statistics

All Schools, N = 243Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMean Math (Year 2) 542.96 88.06 324.28 806.32Mean Math (Year 1) 520.31 83.00 278.59 735.66Mean English (Year 2) 537.21 98.85 195.17 776.99Mean English (Year 1) 505.61 106.25 89.15 732.37Mean Urdu (Year 2) 550.17 84.69 233.33 832.48Mean Urdu (Year 1) 512.90 86.25 90.54 693.02SD Math (Year 2) 542.96 88.06 324.28 806.32SD Math (Year 1) 95.57 28.90 30.91 188.19SD English (Year 2) 92.39 32.79 41.43 200.84SD English (Year 1) 88.35 26.91 37.37 163.36SD Urdu (Year 2) 111.85 35.71 35.11 221.96SD Urdu (Year 1) 101.80 34.83 46.10 216.92Semi SD Math (Year 2) 101.95 31.25 38.33 185.90Semi SD Math (Year 1) 87.17 27.08 23.88 170.47Semi SD English (Year 2) 82.95 29.99 33.45 227.21Semi SD English (Year 1) 81.25 26.33 35.44 194.48Semi SD Urdu (Year 2) 101.17 30.27 35.52 190.34Semi SD Urdu (Year 1) 92.38 33.61 36.24 284.24

Boys’ Schools, N = 103Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMean Math (Year 2) 538.87 89.72 324.28 716.05Mean Math (Year 1) 514.70 78.23 309.74 735.66Mean English (Year 2) 483.95 98.35 195.17 705.91Mean English (Year 1) 455.19 104.15 89.15 715.91Mean Urdu (Year 2) 513.98 85.77 233.33 729.27Mean Urdu (Year 1) 479.59 87.87 90.54 693.02SD Math (Year 2) 538.87 89.72 324.28 716.05SD Math (Year 1) 100.28 27.37 43.44 188.19SD English (Year 2) 108.56 36.05 41.43 200.84SD English (Year 1) 95.59 28.69 43.51 163.36SD Urdu (Year 2) 127.68 36.85 65.38 221.96SD Urdu (Year 1) 112.31 32.55 57.25 196.34Semi SD Math (Year 2) 111.77 31.49 57.79 185.90Semi SD Math (Year 1) 88.80 25.56 35.86 170.47Semi SD English (Year 2) 96.59 35.88 44.88 227.21Semi SD English (Year 1) 88.08 29.41 39.28 194.48Semi SD Urdu (Year 2) 111.87 30.63 51.62 190.34Semi SD Urdu (Year 1) 101.88 37.62 48.77 284.24

21

Table 3 ContinuedGirls’ Schools, N = 77

Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMean Math (Year 2) 499.47 76.81 334.18 676.17Mean Math (Year 1) 493.13 90.88 278.59 709.38Mean English (Year 2) 530.81 55.78 427.98 653.04Mean English (Year 1) 492.49 75.39 334.82 695.55Mean Urdu (Year 2) 541.98 64.84 379.39 675.37Mean Urdu (Year 1) 509.89 79.44 325.59 666.28SD Math (Year 2) 499.47 76.81 334.18 676.17SD Math (Year 1) 102.81 31.22 30.91 178.19SD English (Year 2) 88.28 25.05 48.61 145.18SD English (Year 1) 90.23 23.76 38.42 157.19SD Urdu (Year 2) 110.02 32.17 40.18 192.67SD Urdu (Year 1) 102.34 38.80 47.22 216.92Semi SD Math (Year 2) 105.29 28.60 45.48 180.09Semi SD Math (Year 1) 94.70 30.04 23.88 159.62Semi SD English (Year 2) 78.93 19.31 44.89 123.37Semi SD English (Year 1) 82.40 23.23 38.78 148.46Semi SD Urdu (Year 2) 100.84 29.58 43.57 186.25Semi SD Urdu (Year 1) 92.18 32.18 36.24 182.38

Coed Schools, N = 63Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxMean Math (Year 2) 602.79 61.48 480.98 806.32Mean Math (Year 1) 562.69 62.49 403.09 691.25Mean English (Year 2) 632.10 67.08 379.99 776.99Mean English (Year 1) 604.07 70.46 388.88 732.37Mean Urdu (Year 2) 619.33 60.18 485.37 832.48Mean Urdu (Year 1) 571.02 57.54 451.03 668.25SD Math (Year 2) 602.79 61.48 480.98 806.32SD Math (Year 1) 79.00 21.25 41.14 138.17SD English (Year 2) 70.95 19.19 44.50 127.34SD English (Year 1) 74.22 22.06 37.37 128.87SD Urdu (Year 2) 88.18 22.03 35.11 155.00SD Urdu (Year 1) 83.97 25.32 46.10 162.07Semi SD Math (Year 2) 81.81 24.33 38.33 157.76Semi SD Math (Year 1) 75.30 21.54 37.08 135.72Semi SD English (Year 2) 65.54 17.32 33.45 129.66Semi SD English (Year 1) 68.87 19.87 35.44 119.14Semi SD Urdu (Year 2) 84.08 21.74 35.52 149.16Semi SD Urdu (Year 1) 77.11 20.63 38.92 134.95

22

Tab

le4:

LE

AP

SM

ath

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(N

oIn

stru

men

ts)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Mat

hSco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.58

4***

0.59

9***

0.47

7***

0.49

6***

0.68

3***

0.67

9***

(0.0

59)

(0.0

67)

(0.0

80)

(0.0

82)

(0.0

63)

(0.0

64)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n-0

.035

(0.0

80)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-0.5

25**

*-0

.683

***

(0.1

44)

(0.1

52)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.52

2***

(0.1

91)

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

-7.5

31**

*-9

.108

***

(1.1

66)

(1.2

11)

Fem

ale

XH

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss4.

779*

**(1

.626

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

532*

**0.

533*

**0.

526*

**0.

525*

**0.

531*

**0.

532*

**(0

.031

)(0

.031

)(0

.031

)(0

.030

)(0

.030

)(0

.030

)A

ge-6

.293

***

-6.2

83**

*-6

.200

***

-6.0

84**

*-6

.286

***

-6.2

72**

*(1

.399

)(1

.398

)(1

.422

)(1

.396

)(1

.385

)(1

.380

)F

emal

e-3

1.96

9*-1

0.49

2-3

1.02

8*-7

6.85

8***

-29.

364

-38.

064*

*(1

7.77

6)(5

4.60

2)(1

7.67

5)(2

6.03

7)(1

8.97

3)(1

9.04

3)F

emal

eT

each

er-2

4.92

5-2

4.98

9-2

4.16

4-2

3.52

0-2

2.22

3-2

0.89

1(1

5.33

7)(1

5.30

7)(1

7.01

2)(1

6.17

6)(1

6.55

5)(1

7.01

0)F

emal

eX

Fem

ale

Tea

cher

33.7

1033

.540

33.0

3832

.995

29.0

2831

.105

(20.

600)

(20.

777)

(20.

483)

(21.

060)

(21.

568)

(20.

792)

Pri

vate

Sch

ool

12.8

5212

.315

4.67

92.

764

6.66

58.

690

(9.1

97)

(9.1

81)

(10.

931)

(10.

712)

(10.

818)

(10.

905)

Con

stan

t-2

9.22

1-3

6.98

588

.033

*85

.547

*-7

2.91

8**

-71.

653*

*(2

8.86

0)(3

1.32

9)(4

5.94

2)(4

7.65

1)(3

0.85

8)(3

0.65

0)

Obse

rvat

ions

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

R-s

quar

ed0.

483

0.48

40.

488

0.49

00.

488

0.48

9R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

23

Tab

le5:

LE

AP

SE

ngl

ish

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(N

oIn

stru

men

ts)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Engl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.54

6***

0.54

3***

0.42

6***

0.43

4***

0.60

8***

0.60

8***

(0.0

70)

(0.0

86)

(0.0

59)

(0.0

58)

(0.0

75)

(0.0

75)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n0.

013

(0.0

93)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-0.6

01**

*-0

.715

***

(0.1

67)

(0.1

83)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.63

2**

(0.2

91)

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

-4.2

90**

*-4

.343

***

(0.8

44)

(0.9

07)

Fem

ale

XH

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

171

(0.8

32)

Bas

elin

eSco

re0.

459*

**0.

459*

**0.

456*

**0.

457*

**0.

458*

**0.

458*

**(0

.032

)(0

.032

)(0

.030

)(0

.027

)(0

.031

)(0

.031

)A

ge-4

.839

***

-4.8

43**

*-5

.124

***

-5.0

61**

*-4

.952

***

-4.9

51**

*(1

.080

)(1

.069

)(1

.128

)(1

.092

)(1

.072

)(1

.072

)F

emal

e0.

093

-7.8

631.

964

-42.

855

-3.1

57-3

.491

(15.

337)

(59.

333)

(15.

370)

(25.

844)

(14.

936)

(15.

536)

Fem

ale

Tea

cher

0.64

70.

835

11.2

2113

.293

2.13

32.

157

(10.

402)

(10.

953)

(11.

512)

(11.

318)

(11.

653)

(11.

686)

Fem

ale

XF

emal

eT

each

er9.

425

8.99

86.

208

7.27

78.

197

8.13

2(1

7.01

1)(1

7.18

1)(1

6.96

0)(1

9.20

5)(1

6.44

1)(1

6.33

3)P

riva

teSch

ool

-5.6

34-5

.595

-5.9

09-6

.269

-12.

358

-12.

320

(6.9

98)

(7.0

81)

(8.4

18)

(8.3

97)

(7.6

03)

(7.6

40)

Con

stan

t-3

2.80

1*-3

1.06

810

5.39

4**

96.2

07**

-46.

129*

*-4

6.02

6**

(18.

894)

(22.

880)

(43.

717)

(40.

507)

(18.

584)

(18.

648)

Obse

rvat

ions

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

R-s

quar

ed0.

580

0.58

00.

585

0.58

70.

583

0.58

3R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

24

Tab

le6:

LE

AP

SU

rdu

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(N

oIn

stru

men

ts)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Urd

uSco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.49

4***

0.49

2***

0.42

4***

0.44

1***

0.60

0***

0.59

9***

(0.0

80)

(0.1

03)

(0.0

88)

(0.0

87)

(0.0

88)

(0.0

88)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n0.

004

(0.0

98)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-0.4

35**

*-0

.578

***

(0.1

51)

(0.1

88)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.46

6**

(0.2

21)

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

-8.2

40**

*-7

.821

***

(1.4

63)

(1.4

88)

Fem

ale

XH

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss-0

.862

(1.6

37)

Bas

elin

eSco

re0.

544*

**0.

544*

**0.

538*

**0.

538*

**0.

544*

**0.

544*

**(0

.032

)(0

.032

)(0

.032

)(0

.031

)(0

.031

)(0

.031

)A

ge-6

.612

***

-6.6

13**

*-6

.404

***

-6.2

80**

*-6

.634

***

-6.6

17**

*(1

.260

)(1

.247

)(1

.292

)(1

.271

)(1

.212

)(1

.213

)F

emal

e14

.918

12.5

4515

.495

-25.

150

15.8

3317

.452

(12.

441)

(60.

122)

(12.

441)

(24.

805)

(12.

485)

(12.

542)

Fem

ale

Tea

cher

8.62

78.

661

14.2

9216

.056

11.1

0510

.871

(13.

862)

(14.

245)

(13.

301)

(12.

906)

(13.

746)

(13.

879)

Fem

ale

XF

emal

eT

each

er-2

.524

-2.5

86-2

.829

-2.6

82-4

.086

-4.1

53(1

4.33

8)(1

4.56

5)(1

4.48

1)(1

5.40

2)(1

4.31

9)(1

4.46

2)P

riva

teSch

ool

1.24

31.

301

0.13

9-1

.844

-6.1

94-6

.357

(9.8

05)

(10.

173)

(10.

614)

(10.

428)

(12.

637)

(12.

609)

Con

stan

t-3

5.10

6-3

4.46

550

.544

50.6

32-7

9.09

6**

-79.

649*

*(3

1.96

2)(3

8.78

3)(4

6.43

6)(4

6.46

8)(3

5.22

3)(3

5.54

3)

Obse

rvat

ions

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

R-s

quar

ed0.

495

0.49

50.

498

0.50

00.

501

0.50

1R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

25

Tab

le7:

LE

AP

SM

ath

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Mat

hSco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

-0.3

93-0

.248

-0.2

15-0

.198

-0.6

13-0

.658

(0.3

18)

(0.3

02)

(0.3

21)

(0.3

18)

(0.5

86)

(0.6

11)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n-0

.309

(0.4

08)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

0.90

50.

762

(1.1

65)

(1.0

61)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.63

7(1

.694

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss13

.008

27.0

97(2

1.16

0)(2

5.22

3)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

-34.

696

(34.

661)

Bas

elin

eSco

re0.

611*

**0.

610*

**0.

622*

**0.

624*

**0.

616*

**0.

619*

**(0

.025

)(0

.025

)(0

.026

)(0

.027

)(0

.025

)(0

.026

)A

ge-7

.883

***

-7.7

63**

*-8

.055

***

-8.0

44**

*-7

.977

***

-8.1

47**

*(2

.181

)(2

.130

)(2

.241

)(2

.246

)(2

.387

)(2

.539

)F

emal

e-1

.403

188.

263

-2.8

14-5

4.84

5-4

.338

59.1

65(2

5.15

4)(2

52.7

19)

(24.

486)

(140

.854

)(2

1.37

1)(7

6.41

7)F

emal

eT

each

er19

.363

17.9

3918

.358

17.7

9916

.964

8.15

8(4

1.87

6)(4

0.52

1)(4

0.81

0)(3

9.99

7)(3

8.95

6)(3

5.87

3)F

emal

eX

Fem

ale

Tea

cher

-2.0

90-2

.898

-1.1

80-0

.020

4.16

3-1

0.74

1(2

7.80

0)(2

8.50

6)(2

7.63

1)(2

8.58

2)(2

4.71

7)(4

2.35

3)P

riva

teSch

ool

57.4

6051

.838

71.8

6071

.636

70.4

3059

.716

(40.

684)

(37.

431)

(54.

039)

(55.

236)

(44.

854)

(43.

050)

Con

stan

t37

2.69

5**

296.

138*

173.

355

168.

656

468.

740*

491.

313*

(159

.699

)(1

55.4

53)

(248

.882

)(2

48.7

52)

(276

.963

)(2

89.8

87)

Obse

rvat

ions

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

R-s

quar

ed0.

364

0.36

50.

335

0.32

50.

321

0.26

2R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

26

Tab

le8:

LE

AP

SE

ngl

ish

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Engl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

reE

ngl

ish

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.10

70.

130

-0.0

09-0

.004

0.08

80.

089

(0.1

32)

(0.1

51)

(0.2

03)

(0.2

15)

(0.1

54)

(0.1

54)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n-0

.088

(0.1

85)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-0.6

68-0

.507

(0.7

54)

(0.8

59)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

-0.5

18(0

.857

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

964

-0.0

60(1

.802

)(1

.537

)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

3.36

5(4

.362

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

528*

**0.

528*

**0.

523*

**0.

523*

**0.

529*

**0.

529*

**(0

.020

)(0

.020

)(0

.022

)(0

.023

)(0

.020

)(0

.020

)A

ge-5

.307

***

-5.2

78**

*-5

.664

***

-5.7

76**

*-5

.286

***

-5.2

80**

*(1

.451

)(1

.462

)(1

.598

)(1

.652

)(1

.447

)(1

.445

)F

emal

e11

.537

65.2

0513

.084

46.0

2112

.388

5.84

1(1

6.20

9)(1

12.7

83)

(16.

392)

(59.

440)

(16.

599)

(17.

561)

Fem

ale

Tea

cher

32.2

0130

.718

42.5

6343

.057

32.2

0132

.651

(24.

222)

(25.

304)

(27.

631)

(28.

137)

(23.

747)

(24.

381)

Fem

ale

XF

emal

eT

each

er-8

.270

-5.2

66-1

1.06

5-1

0.38

8-8

.182

-9.4

57(1

7.74

7)(2

0.74

1)(1

7.95

6)(1

7.94

2)(1

7.81

9)(1

7.58

8)P

riva

teSch

ool

22.2

3721

.791

20.6

2619

.975

24.0

4324

.834

(24.

092)

(24.

366)

(23.

177)

(22.

713)

(24.

499)

(25.

238)

Con

stan

t10

0.70

788

.111

248.

543

234.

129

105.

114

107.

262

(64.

828)

(74.

136)

(197

.029

)(2

09.0

80)

(70.

228)

(71.

184)

Obse

rvat

ions

5,84

95,

849

5,82

15,

821

5,84

95,

849

R-s

quar

ed0.

554

0.55

40.

558

0.55

50.

552

0.55

1R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

27

Tab

le9:

LE

AP

SU

rdu

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Urd

uSco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

reU

rdu

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

-0.2

36-0

.208

-0.3

38-0

.320

-0.1

73-0

.169

(0.1

92)

(0.2

13)

(0.2

76)

(0.2

59)

(0.1

72)

(0.1

67)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n-0

.087

(0.3

58)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-0.6

27-0

.779

(0.6

75)

(0.6

83)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.88

6(1

.009

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss-3

.493

-4.3

82(5

.584

)(6

.457

)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

1.45

3(8

.066

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

612*

**0.

612*

**0.

603*

**0.

606*

**0.

610*

**0.

610*

**(0

.026

)(0

.026

)(0

.029

)(0

.028

)(0

.026

)(0

.027

)A

ge-7

.910

***

-7.8

77**

*-7

.612

***

-7.5

88**

*-7

.886

***

-7.9

13**

*(1

.979

)(1

.987

)(1

.918

)(1

.852

)(1

.912

)(1

.953

)F

emal

e44

.419

***

97.7

5145

.277

***

-27.

613

44.0

60**

41.3

07*

(16.

720)

(222

.262

)(1

6.91

2)(8

3.90

5)(1

6.78

1)(2

3.81

1)F

emal

eT

each

er52

.372

51.7

2460

.571

60.3

5352

.315

52.7

02(3

8.58

9)(3

8.34

3)(4

5.06

8)(4

2.60

2)(3

8.93

8)(3

8.57

3)F

emal

eX

Fem

ale

Tea

cher

-36.

723*

*-3

5.42

7*-3

7.19

3**

-38.

770*

*-3

6.52

0**

-36.

391*

*(1

7.56

6)(1

9.04

1)(1

7.52

6)(1

8.63

1)(1

7.67

5)(1

7.40

8)P

riva

teSch

ool

39.0

3437

.848

37.4

7836

.059

34.9

2534

.981

(36.

590)

(36.

374)

(36.

034)

(37.

209)

(37.

202)

(37.

470)

Con

stan

t24

1.22

1**

227.

571*

*36

4.83

4*36

5.60

4*21

5.58

0**

215.

146*

*(1

01.1

54)

(111

.976

)(2

05.7

07)

(195

.873

)(9

5.15

2)(9

4.53

6)

Obse

rvat

ions

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

5,84

95,

849

R-s

quar

ed0.

429

0.42

80.

431

0.43

30.

436

0.43

6R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

28

Table 10: Kenya: Summary Statistics, N = 2188

Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxTotal Score 0.01 1.00 -1.36 3.01Math Score -0.01 0.99 -1.56 2.96Literature Score 0.03 1.01 -0.91 3.40Baseline Score 0.05 0.98 -2.84 3.85Semi SD Total Score 0.88 0.24 0.38 1.48Semi SD Math Score 0.80 0.19 0.40 1.37Semi SD Lit Score 0.98 0.31 0.24 1.59Semi SD Baseline Score 1.06 0.22 0.62 1.78ETP Teacher 0.52 0.50 0 1Female 0.48 0.50 0 1Age 9.19 1.47 6.00 18.00

29

Tab

le11

:E

TP

Tot

alSco

reR

egre

ssio

n(N

oIn

stru

men

ts)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Tot

alSco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.32

6***

0.28

8***

0.41

3***

0.40

7***

0.27

1***

0.26

8***

(0.0

92)

(0.1

02)

(0.0

92)

(0.0

92)

(0.0

89)

(0.0

88)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n0.

075

(0.1

74)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-0.9

23**

*-0

.823

***

(0.1

29)

(0.1

48)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

-0.2

02(0

.185

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

063*

*0.

012

(0.0

25)

(0.0

49)

Fem

ale

XH

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

108

(0.0

84)

Bas

elin

eSco

re0.

504*

**0.

504*

**0.

496*

**0.

498*

**0.

503*

**0.

502*

**(0

.026

)(0

.026

)(0

.026

)(0

.025

)(0

.026

)(0

.026

)E

TP

Tea

cher

0.08

8**

0.08

8***

0.12

8***

0.12

9***

0.08

0**

0.08

0**

(0.0

34)

(0.0

33)

(0.0

40)

(0.0

39)

(0.0

34)

(0.0

34)

Fem

ale

0.03

70.

037

0.03

40.

211

0.03

8-0

.016

(0.0

39)

(0.0

39)

(0.0

38)

(0.1

59)

(0.0

39)

(0.0

60)

Age

-0.0

30*

-0.0

30*

-0.0

28*

-0.0

29*

-0.0

29*

-0.0

28*

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

Con

stan

t0.

263

0.26

11.

197*

**1.

109*

**0.

233

0.25

1(0

.162

)(0

.161

)(0

.196

)(0

.225

)(0

.154

)(0

.154

)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

R-s

quar

ed0.

391

0.39

10.

406

0.40

70.

392

0.39

2R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

30

Tab

le12

:E

TP

Mat

hSco

reR

egre

ssio

n(N

oIn

stru

men

ts)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Mat

hSco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.37

1***

0.34

7***

0.15

70.

153

0.29

8***

0.29

7***

(0.0

92)

(0.1

26)

(0.1

09)

(0.1

08)

(0.0

99)

(0.0

98)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n0.

047

(0.1

97)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

-1.2

14**

*-1

.021

***

(0.2

35)

(0.2

49)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

-0.4

03*

(0.2

15)

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

0.05

1***

0.01

7(0

.019

)(0

.026

)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

0.07

6(0

.047

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

494*

**0.

494*

**0.

481*

**0.

483*

**0.

492*

**0.

492*

**(0

.022

)(0

.022

)(0

.022

)(0

.021

)(0

.022

)(0

.022

)E

TP

Tea

cher

0.05

0*0.

050*

0.09

5**

0.09

4**

0.04

70.

048

(0.0

27)

(0.0

27)

(0.0

36)

(0.0

36)

(0.0

29)

(0.0

29)

Fem

ale

-0.0

30-0

.030

-0.0

290.

291*

-0.0

29-0

.081

(0.0

40)

(0.0

39)

(0.0

39)

(0.1

67)

(0.0

39)

(0.0

53)

Age

0.00

70.

007

0.01

00.

011

0.00

60.

007

(0.0

16)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

15)

(0.0

15)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

16)

Con

stan

t-0

.140

-0.1

420.

898*

*0.

739*

*-0

.157

-0.1

38(0

.162

)(0

.162

)(0

.355

)(0

.353

)(0

.157

)(0

.156

)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

R-s

quar

ed0.

361

0.36

10.

379

0.38

00.

362

0.36

3R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

31

Tab

le13

:E

TP

Lit

erat

ure

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(N

oIn

stru

men

ts)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Lit

Sco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.23

0**

0.18

0*0.

466*

**0.

466*

**0.

213*

0.21

4*(0

.100

)(0

.107

)(0

.105

)(0

.105

)(0

.109

)(0

.111

)F

emal

eX

Cla

ssM

ean

0.10

2(0

.198

)C

lass

Sem

iSD

-0.9

21**

*-0

.922

***

(0.1

19)

(0.1

31)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.00

3(0

.134

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

029

-0.0

31(0

.029

)(0

.051

)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

0.12

5(0

.088

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

411*

**0.

411*

**0.

403*

**0.

403*

**0.

411*

**0.

410*

**(0

.030

)(0

.030

)(0

.029

)(0

.029

)(0

.030

)(0

.030

)E

TP

Tea

cher

0.11

1***

0.11

1***

0.16

4***

0.16

4***

0.11

3***

0.11

2***

(0.0

36)

(0.0

36)

(0.0

44)

(0.0

44)

(0.0

37)

(0.0

38)

Fem

ale

0.08

6**

0.08

6**

0.08

5**

0.08

20.

086*

*0.

021

(0.0

42)

(0.0

42)

(0.0

41)

(0.1

18)

(0.0

42)

(0.0

60)

Age

-0.0

55**

*-0

.055

***

-0.0

54**

*-0

.054

***

-0.0

55**

*-0

.054

***

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

17)

Con

stan

t0.

550*

**0.

546*

**1.

728*

**1.

729*

**0.

532*

**0.

558*

**(0

.153

)(0

.152

)(0

.181

)(0

.199

)(0

.154

)(0

.153

)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

92,

189

2,18

92,

189

2,18

92,

189

R-s

quar

ed0.

330

0.33

00.

353

0.35

30.

330

0.33

1R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

32

Tab

le14

:E

TP

Tot

alSco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Tot

alSco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.44

4***

0.35

20.

388*

0.36

20.

415*

**0.

416*

**(0

.132

)(0

.289

)(0

.211

)(0

.260

)(0

.149

)(0

.148

)F

emal

eX

Cla

ssM

ean

0.18

5(0

.486

)C

lass

Sem

iSD

0.72

01.

762

(0.7

27)

(2.2

52)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

-1.3

46(2

.387

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

027

0.01

8(0

.046

)(0

.112

)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

0.01

8(0

.199

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

504*

**0.

504*

**0.

510*

**0.

522*

**0.

504*

**0.

503*

**(0

.026

)(0

.026

)(0

.026

)(0

.030

)(0

.026

)(0

.026

)E

TP

Tea

cher

0.07

0**

0.07

0**

0.03

60.

020

0.06

7*0.

067*

*(0

.035

)(0

.035

)(0

.054

)(0

.076

)(0

.034

)(0

.034

)F

emal

e0.

036

0.03

70.

039

1.21

40.

037

0.02

8(0

.039

)(0

.039

)(0

.040

)(2

.088

)(0

.039

)(0

.107

)A

ge-0

.030

*-0

.029

*-0

.031

*-0

.032

*-0

.029

*-0

.029

*(0

.017

)(0

.017

)(0

.017

)(0

.017

)(0

.017

)(0

.017

)C

onst

ant

0.27

3*0.

266*

-0.4

54-1

.408

0.25

90.

263

(0.1

55)

(0.1

55)

(0.7

88)

(2.1

33)

(0.1

61)

(0.1

66)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

R-s

quar

ed0.

390

0.39

00.

357

0.31

30.

391

0.39

1R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

33

Tab

le15

:E

TP

Mat

hSco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Mat

hSco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.46

9***

0.59

7*0.

643*

**0.

634*

**0.

403*

*0.

411*

*(0

.129

)(0

.314

)(0

.183

)(0

.185

)(0

.202

)(0

.203

)F

emal

eX

Cla

ssM

ean

-0.2

54(0

.564

)C

lass

Sem

iSD

0.59

5*0.

483

(0.3

27)

(0.4

67)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

0.22

1(0

.817

)H

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss0.

049

-0.1

40(0

.087

)(0

.236

)F

emal

eX

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

0.33

7(0

.375

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

494*

**0.

494*

**0.

500*

**0.

499*

**0.

492*

**0.

490*

**(0

.022

)(0

.022

)(0

.023

)(0

.023

)(0

.022

)(0

.021

)E

TP

Tea

cher

0.04

00.

038

0.01

10.

013

0.03

70.

045

(0.0

28)

(0.0

28)

(0.0

35)

(0.0

36)

(0.0

29)

(0.0

30)

Fem

ale

-0.0

31-0

.031

-0.0

32-0

.208

-0.0

30-0

.257

(0.0

40)

(0.0

40)

(0.0

40)

(0.6

52)

(0.0

40)

(0.2

62)

Age

0.00

70.

006

0.00

50.

005

0.00

60.

009

(0.0

16)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

15)

Con

stan

t-0

.134

-0.1

27-0

.639

**-0

.547

-0.1

50-0

.061

(0.1

55)

(0.1

55)

(0.3

21)

(0.4

50)

(0.1

56)

(0.1

91)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

R-s

quar

ed0.

361

0.36

00.

339

0.33

70.

362

0.35

3R

obust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

34

Tab

le16

:E

TP

Lit

erat

ure

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

VA

RIA

BL

ES

Lit

Sco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

0.42

2***

0.14

80.

402*

*0.

624

0.40

1**

0.40

9***

(0.1

45)

(0.3

30)

(0.1

95)

(2.4

71)

(0.1

59)

(0.1

54)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Mea

n0.

561

(0.5

94)

Cla

ssSem

iSD

0.44

7-1

5.56

3(1

.061

)(1

18.9

08)

Fem

ale

XC

lass

Sem

iSD

13.3

08(1

01.0

50)

Hig

hA

chie

ver

inC

lass

0.03

70.

184

(0.0

87)

(0.2

60)

Fem

ale

XH

igh

Ach

ieve

rin

Cla

ss-0

.275

(0.4

34)

Bas

elin

eSco

re0.

411*

**0.

410*

**0.

415*

**0.

275

0.41

1***

0.41

2***

(0.0

30)

(0.0

30)

(0.0

29)

(1.0

35)

(0.0

30)

(0.0

30)

ET

PT

each

er0.

078*

*0.

078*

*0.

036

0.90

40.

079*

0.07

8**

(0.0

38)

(0.0

38)

(0.1

09)

(6.3

93)

(0.0

40)

(0.0

39)

Fem

ale

0.08

6**

0.08

7**

0.08

7**

-12.

814

0.08

6**

0.23

1(0

.042

)(0

.043

)(0

.043

)(9

7.99

5)(0

.042

)(0

.234

)A

ge-0

.055

***

-0.0

53**

*-0

.055

***

-0.0

13-0

.055

***

-0.0

56**

*(0

.017

)(0

.017

)(0

.017

)(0

.313

)(0

.017

)(0

.017

)C

onst

ant

0.56

6***

0.54

9***

0.00

218

.072

0.54

4***

0.47

9**

(0.1

49)

(0.1

50)

(1.3

96)

(134

.029

)(0

.174

)(0

.217

)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

92,

189

2,18

92,

189

2,18

92,

189

R-s

quar

ed0.

329

0.32

70.

300

0.32

90.

320

Rob

ust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

35

Appendix B

In this section, I test whether peer gender plays a role in peer effects. To do this, I employ

the following regression on test scores in the ETP dataset:14

yij = α0 +αxij +βbij +λy−ij,f +λffemaleij×y−ij,f +γy−ij,m +γffemaleij×y−ij,m + εij (7)

In Equation (7), xij and bij are defined as before, and y−ij,f and y−ij,m give the class average

scores (excluding student i) for the females and males, respectively. Thus, the coefficients of

Equation (7) should give some information about whether students are affected differently

by students of the same gender than they are by students of the opposite gender. As with

the regressions in the main text, I instrument for the four regressors of interest using their

baseline analogs in a two-stage least squares regression.

One key challenge to performing this analysis is that I do not have the raw ETP data

and therefore cannot normalize the gender-specific class means in the same way as Duflo et

al. (2011). Instead, I take means of the each classmate’s normalized test scores. While these

means do not perfectly replicate the normalized means used by Duflo et al., the two are

highly correlated. Figure 1 gives a scatter plot of the classroom baseline means calculated

using our methodology against the classroom baseline means included in the J-PAL dataset

that were used in Duflo et al. (2011).

In fact, the correlation between the two types of means is .69 for baseline scores. Thus,

while the gender-specific means used in our analysis are not perfect, their regression co-

efficients should provide the desired insight into whether peer effects induced by men and

women differ. Table 17 gives the summary statistics for our calculated gender-specific means.

Table 18 gives the results of the regressions both with and without instrumental variables.

As with my other regressions, I consistently find baseline score to have a strong, statistically

14I cannot run these regressions using the LEAPS dataset because most students in the LEAPS datasetare not not in coeducational schools.

36

Figure 1: Normalized Means and Means Normalized

−.2

−.1

0.1

.2.3

Cla

ss M

ean

of N

orm

aliz

ed B

asel

ine

Sco

res

−.2 −.1 0 .1 .2 .3Normalized Class Mean of Baseline Scores

Table 17: Kenya: Summary Statistics, N = 2188

Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min MaxClass Female Mean Total 0.06 0.54 -1.00 1.61Class Male Mean Total -0.02 0.42 -1.04 1.87Class Female Mean Math 0.00 0.46 -1.17 1.33Class Male Mean Math -0.01 0.41 -1.15 1.67Class Female Mean Literature 0.11 0.57 -0.87 1.78Class Male Mean Literature -0.03 0.44 -0.78 1.76Class Female Mean Baseline 0.040372 0.264597 -0.6066 1.089602Class Male Mean Baseline 0.00834 0.248194 -0.82874 0.73065

37

significant positive relationship with current scores, as well as that students of new ETP

hires tend to perform better. Notably, the regressions without instruments indicate that

peer effects primarily exist between female students. Furthermore, these female-to-female

peer effects are quite large: a one standard deviation increase in the average score of female

classmates yields a .13 standard deviation increase in the score of female students. This is a

much larger effect than is present in Tables 14, 15, and 16.15

Importantly, this result is not robust to the application of two-stage least squares. While

the coefficient is still positive and very large in the total score and math score regressions,

its standard errors are too large for inference. Therefore, it is unclear whether the apparent

female-to-female effects are truly peer effects or the result of something else—e.g. the exis-

tence of some teachers who are able to more effectively teach math to females. Either result

would be very fascinating, and this question certainly warrants further investigation. In the

future, I hope to continue this line of analysis with larger datasets to determine whether

peer effects are truly a primarily female-to-female phenomenon or whether other factors are

biasing the coefficients in the regression without instruments.

15It is possible that women are the sole drivers of peer effects and that Tables 14, 15, and 16 show smallereffects because they are diluted by males.

38

Tab

le18

:E

TP

Lit

erat

ure

Sco

reR

egre

ssio

n(S

econ

dSta

ge)

No

Inst

rum

ents

Sec

ond

Sta

geN

oIn

stru

men

tSec

ond

Sta

geN

oIn

stru

men

tSec

ond

Sta

geV

AR

IAB

LE

ST

otal

Sco

reT

otal

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reM

ath

Sco

reL

itSco

reL

itSco

re

Cla

ssM

ean

Mal

eSco

re0.

024

-1.0

510.

047

-0.8

40-0

.101

-0.9

18(0

.097

)(2

.460

)(0

.108

)(2

.134

)(0

.150

)(4

.349

)F

emal

eX

Cla

ssM

ean

Mal

eSco

re-0

.113

1.76

5-0

.098

1.91

8-0

.167

0.38

6(0

.125

)(4

.954

)(0

.127

)(4

.418

)(0

.160

)(8

.836

)C

lass

Mea

nF

emal

eSco

re-0

.082

0.07

2-0

.103

-0.7

49-0

.151

0.44

3(0

.131

)(1

.058

)(0

.131

)(3

.214

)(0

.136

)(0

.782

)F

emal

eX

Cla

ssM

ean

Fem

ale

Sco

re0.

245*

**0.

539

0.18

3*1.

832

0.29

6***

-0.0

17(0

.091

)(2

.342

)(0

.095

)(6

.597

)(0

.098

)(1

.214

)B

asel

ine

Sco

re0.

504*

**0.

494*

**0.

495*

**0.

488*

**0.

407*

**0.

405*

**(0

.026

)(0

.051

)(0

.022

)(0

.035

)(0

.029

)(0

.056

)E

TP

Tea

cher

0.13

6***

0.11

6**

0.09

1*0.

072

0.17

7***

0.20

7(0

.050

)(0

.059

)(0

.048

)(0

.046

)(0

.053

)(0

.192

)F

emal

e0.

021

0.05

1-0

.028

0.00

80.

049

0.10

2(0

.033

)(0

.103

)(0

.034

)(0

.187

)(0

.037

)(0

.175

)A

ge-0

.029

*-0

.021

0.00

70.

022

-0.0

54**

*-0

.051

**(0

.017

)(0

.032

)(0

.015

)(0

.047

)(0

.017

)(0

.025

)C

onst

ant

0.24

40.

270

-0.1

75-0

.297

0.56

7***

0.66

4*(0

.185

)(0

.314

)(0

.204

)(0

.613

)(0

.165

)(0

.385

)

Obse

rvat

ions

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

2,18

82,

188

R-s

quar

ed0.

390

0.18

70.

357

0.33

40.

301

Rob

ust

stan

dar

der

rors

inpar

enth

eses

***

p<

0.01

,**

p<

0.05

,*

p<

0.1

39