neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

Upload: sayako87

Post on 13-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    1/11

    Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656

    Contents lists available atScienceDirect

    Brain Research Bulletin

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / b r a i n r e s b u l l

    Research report

    Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    Antonietta Manna a,b,,1, Antonino Raffone c,e,1, Mauro Gianni Perrucci a,b, Davide Nardo c,d,Antonio Ferretti a,b, Armando Tartaro a,b, Alessandro Londei c,d, Cosimo Del Gratta a,b,Marta Olivetti Belardinelli c,d, Gian Luca Romani a,b

    a ITAB, Institute for Advanced Biomedical Technologies, G. DAnnunzio University Foundation, Chieti, Italyb Department of Clinical Sciences and Bioimaging, University of Chieti, Chieti, Italyc Department of Psychology, La Sapienza University, Rome, Italyd ECONA (Interuniversity Center for Cognitive Processing in Natural and Artificial Systems), Rome, Italye Perceptual Dynamics Laboratory, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Wako-shi, Saitama, Japan

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:

    Received 4 September 2009

    Received in revised form 1 March 2010

    Accepted 2 March 2010

    Available online 16 March 2010

    Keywords:

    Meditation

    Attention

    Consciousness

    Prefrontal cortex

    Cognitive control

    a b s t r a c t

    Meditation refers to a family of complex emotional and attentional regulatory practices, which can be

    classified into two main styles focused attention (FA) and open monitoring (OM) involving different

    attentional, cognitive monitoringand awareness processes. In a functionalmagnetic resonance study we

    originally characterized and contrasted FA and OM meditation forms withinthe same experiment, by an

    integrated FAOM design. Theravada Buddhist monks, expert in both FA and OM meditation forms, and

    lay novices with 10 days of meditation practice, participated in the experiment. Our evidence suggests

    thatexpert meditatorscontrol cognitive engagement in conscious processing of sensory-related, thought

    and emotion contents, by massive self-regulation of fronto-parietal and insular areas in the left hemi-

    sphere, in a meditation state-dependent fashion. We also found that anterior cingulate and dorsolateral

    prefrontal cortices play antagonist roles in the executive control of the attention setting in meditation

    tasks. Our findings resolve the controversy between the hypothesis that meditative states areassociated

    to transient hypofrontality or deactivation of executive brain areas, and evidence about the activation of

    executive brain areas in meditation. Finally, our study suggests that a functional reorganization of brain

    activity patterns for focused attention and cognitive monitoring takes place with mental practice, and

    that meditation-related neuroplasticity is crucially associated to a functional reorganization of activity

    patterns in prefrontal cortex and in the insula.

    2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    Meditation can be conceptualized as a family of complex emo-

    tional and attentional regulatory practices, involving different

    attentional, cognitive monitoring and awareness processes. Many

    recent behavioral, electroencephalographic and neuroimaging

    studies have revealed the importance of investigating meditation

    states andtraits to achieve an increasedunderstanding of cognitive

    and affective neuroplasticity, attention and self-awareness, as wellas for relevant clinical implications[7,28].

    Given that regulation of attention is the central commonality

    across the many different meditation methods [14], medita-

    Correspondingauthorat: Departmentof Clinical Sciencesand Bioimaging, ITAB,

    Institute of Advanced Biomedical Technologies, G. DAnnunzio University, via Dei

    Vestini, Campus Universitario, 66100 Chieti, Italy, Tel.: +39 0871 3556952;

    fax: +39 0871 3556930.

    E-mail address:[email protected](A. Manna).1 These authors have contributed equally to this work.

    tion practices can be usefully classified into two main styles

    focused attention (FA) and open monitoring (OM) depend-

    ing on how the attentional processes are directed [7,28]. In the

    FA (concentrative) style, attention is focused on an intended

    object in a sustained fashion. The second style, OM (mindfulness-

    based) meditation, involves the non-reactive monitoring of the

    content of experience from moment to moment, primarily as

    a means to recognize the nature of emotional and cognitive

    patterns.FA meditation entails the capacities of monitoring the focus

    of attention and detecting distraction, disengaging attention from

    the source of distraction, and (re)directing and engaging atten-

    tion to the intended object[28].These attentional and monitoring

    functions have been related to dissociable systems in the brain

    involved in conflict monitoring, selective and sustained attention

    [12,28,32,40]. A study with a binocular rivalry paradigm showed

    that Tibetan Buddhist monks were able to perceive a stable, super-

    imposed percept of two dissimilar, competing images presented to

    separate eyes for a longer duration both during and after FA med-

    itation, but not during and after a form of compassion (emotional

    0361-9230/$ see front matter 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03619230http://www.elsevier.com/locate/brainresbullmailto:[email protected]://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_7/dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001http://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_7/dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001mailto:[email protected]://www.elsevier.com/locate/brainresbullhttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03619230
  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    2/11

    A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656 47

    OM) meditation[10].These extreme increases in perceptual dom-

    inance durations suggest that extensive training in FA meditation

    mightimprovethe abilitiesto sustain attentionfocus on a particular

    object andto control the flowof items being attended forconscious

    access. A recent fMRI study investigated the neural correlates of

    FA meditation in experts (following Tibetan Buddhist traditions)

    and novices, with meditation focus on an external visual point

    [4].FA meditation compared with a rest condition, was associated

    with activation in multiple brain regions involved in monitoring,

    such as dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), attentional orient-

    ing (e.g., the superior frontal sulcus and intraparietal sulcus) and

    engaging attention (visual cortex). The meditation-related activa-

    tion patterns depended on the level of expertise of the meditation

    practitioners.

    OM meditation involves no explicit attentional focus, and there-

    fore does notseemassociated tobrainareas implicated in sustained

    or focused attention, but to brain regions involved in vigilance,

    monitoring and disengagement of attention from sources of dis-

    traction from the ongoing stream of experience[28].OM practices

    are based on an attentive set that is characterized by an open

    presence and a nonjudgemental awareness of sensory, cognitive

    and affective fields of experience in the present moment, and

    involves a higher-order awareness or observation of the ongoing

    mental processes[7].The cultivation of this reflexive awarenessin OM meditation is associated to a more vivid conscious access

    to the rich features of each experience and enhanced metacogni-

    tive and self-regulation skills[28].Behavioral studies have shown

    a more distributed attentional focus[39],enhanced conflict moni-

    toring [37] and reduced attentional blink or more efficient resource

    allocation to serially presented targets [34] in OM meditation prac-

    titioners.

    Despite the increasing number of studies on neuralcorrelates of

    meditation states and traits, the differential brain activity patterns

    in focused attention and open monitoring meditation forms have

    not been contrasted yet in a neuroimaging experiment. Therefore,

    in an fMRI experiment we studied the FA and OM meditation-

    related brain activity patterns of Buddhist monks whoare expertin

    Samatha (FA) and Vipassana (OM) meditation forms, and follow theoldest (Theravada) currently active Buddhist tradition. Vipassana

    (insight) meditation is central in mindfulness-based clinical inter-

    ventions and studies [8,38]. Although lay practitioners ofVipassana

    have participated in recent research (e.g., [17,34]),to our knowl-

    edge this is the first study in which Theravada Buddhist monks are

    involved.

    Our integrated FAOM experimental design allows testing of

    whether FA and OM meditation styles enhance or, by contrast,

    reduce brain activations in frontal and other executive areas, given

    controversial evidence and theoretical stances. Indeed, it has been

    recently argued that meditative states are associated to transient

    hypofrontality or deactivation in executive networks[15,26]. In

    contrast, other authors have emphasized the activation of exec-

    utive areas in meditation [7,28]. We hypothesize that the brainregions associatedwith conflict monitoring, suchas the dorsal ante-

    rior cingulate cortex (ACC) and DLPFC [9,40],selective attention,

    such as the temporalparietal junction, ventrolateral prefrontal

    cortex, intraparietal sulcus and frontal eye fields [12] and sus-

    tained attention, such as right frontal and parietal areas, and the

    thalamus[13,32],were more involved in inducing and maintain-

    ing the state of FA meditation as compared to the conditions of

    OM meditation and non-meditative rest[28].Given neuropsycho-

    logical [22] and psychophysical [18] evidence of dominance of

    the left cerebral hemisphere in conscious access, and theoretical

    bases to hypothesize a leading role of this hemisphere in conscious

    experiences [2,22,27], we predict a leftward bias of activation in

    fronto-parietal areas in OM meditation as compared to the other

    conditions.

    2. Materials and methods

    2.1. Participants

    Participants included 8 Theravada Buddhist monks (males, meanage 37.9years,

    range 2553 years, SD 9.4years),with 15,750h on averageof balancedSamatha (FA)

    andVipassana(OM) meditation practice in Theravada monasteries (SD 9900 h). The

    monks were from the Santacittarama monastery, in central Italy, following a Thai

    Forest Tradition (the order was funded by Ajahn Chah, one of the most influential

    Buddhist teachers in the 20th century). In this tradition, monks experience reg-

    ular intensive meditation retreats, with a balanced practice of FA (Samatha) andOM (Vipassana) meditation forms, including an about 3-month long winter retreat.

    Outside the retreat period, the monks typically practice SamathaVipassanamed-

    itation, with a balance of FA and OM meditation, 2 h per day with the monastery

    community. Individual meditation practice, with a balance of FA and OM medita-

    tion forms is also emphasized. Thus, on average the expertise of the monks in the

    studied group can be estimated in 15,750h of balanced FAOM meditation prac-

    tice. Participants also included a group of 8 novice meditators (males, mean age

    32 years, ages 2636 years, SD 3 years), recruited from the local community. All

    novice subjects were interested in meditation but had no prior meditation experi-

    ence. The novice participants were given oral and written instructions on how to

    performSamathaand Vipassanameditation styles, and during the 10 days before

    the fMRI scan session practiced each of the two meditation styles 30 min per day.

    The meditation instructions were written by Ajahn Chandapalo, the abbot of the

    Santacittarama monastery, expertSamathaVipassanameditation teacher. All par-

    ticipantswere right-handed. Subjectsgave theirwritten informedconsent according

    to the Declaration of Helsinki[41].

    2.2. Task and protocol

    The FAOM experimental paradigm consisted of 6min FA (Samatha) and 6min

    OM (Vipassana) meditation blocks, each preceded and followed by a 3 min non-

    meditative resting state block (Rest), for three times (seeFig. 1).The total duration

    of the experiment was 57min. The condition switch was instructed by an auditory

    word-signal during the experiment.

    To perform FA meditation, participants were given the following instruction:

    gently engage in sustaining the focus of your attention on breath sensations, such

    as at the nostrils, noticing with acceptance and tolerance any arising distraction,

    as toward stimuli or thoughts, and return gently to focus attention on the breath

    sensations after having noticed the distraction source. In OM meditation, partici-

    pants weregiventhe followinginstruction:observe andrecognize anyexperiential

    or mental content as it arises from moment to moment, without restrictions and

    judgement,including breath and body sensations,percepts of external stimuli, aris-

    ing thoughts and feelings. The instruction for Rest was the following: rest in a

    relaxed awakestate. In FA and OMmeditation aswell asin theResttaskcondition,

    the subjects did not employ any discursive strategy, recitation, breath manipula-tion or visualization technique. During all the conditions, the participants kepteyes

    closed. At the end of the experiment, all participants reported they could perform

    the FA, OM and Rest task conditions according to the given instructions, with no

    differences in the experienced difficulty to perform FA and OM meditation condi-

    tions.

    2.3. Functional MRI recording

    Functional MRI scans were acquired on a Siemens Magnetom Vision scanner

    at 1.5T, equipped with a standard receiver head coil. BOLD contrast functional

    imaging was performed using a T2-weighted echo planar (EPI) free induction decay

    (FID) sequence with: TR= 4 s, 28 slices, voxel size 4 mm4 mm4 mm, 860 func-

    tional volumes for each run. A high-resolution T 1-weighted whole-brain image

    was also acquired at the end of each session via a 3D-MPRAGE sequence (sagit-

    Fig. 1. Sequence of the experimental conditions during the measurements.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    3/11

    48 A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656

    tal matrix= 256256, FOV = 256mm, slice thickness=1 mm, no gap, in-plane voxel

    size=1mm1 mm, flip angle= 12, TR/TE = 9.7/4.0ms).

    2.4. Physiological measures

    Respiration rateand ECGwere recorded throughout eachscanning session in all

    subjects. EEG was also recorded, with data to be analyzed for a subsequent report.

    2.5. Data analysis

    Rawdata wereanalyzed using Brain VoyagerQX 1.7software (Brain Innovation,TheNetherlands).Thefirstfive scansof each runwerediscardedto avoidthe T1sat-

    uration effect. Preprocessing consisted in a 3D motion correction and in a temporal

    filtering of voxel time series. The data set of one of the monks was discarded from

    further analysis due to excessive motion. Preprocessed functional volumes were

    co-registered with the corresponding structural data set. Co-registration transfor-

    mation wasdetermined using theslice position parametersof thefunctional images

    and the position parameters of the structural volume. Temporal filtering included

    linear andnon-linear (high-pass filter of twocycles pertime course)trendsremoval.

    Structural and functional volumes were than transformed into the Talairach space

    [36]. No spatial or temporal smoothing was applied.

    Statistical analysis was carried out for individual subjects and condition using

    the General Linear Model [21]. To account for the hemodynamic delay, the box-

    car waveform of each task condition was convolved with the Boynton empirically

    founded hemodynamic response function[3]. In order to search for activated areas

    common to the entire group of subjects, a voxel-wise random effect group analysis

    was performed, distinguishing between monks and novice meditators (within-

    group analysis). To this purpose, all the subjects time series were z-normalized

    and individualbetascomputedby specifying subject-specific regressors in theGLM.

    Moreover, to quantitatively evaluate differences between the brain activity pat-

    terns of monks and novices, and to includegroupas a factor in our statistical model

    of the fMRI data, between-group inferences were also computed (between-group

    analysis).

    Group statistical maps were thresholded at an overall significance level (the

    probabilityof a false detectionfor theentirefunctional volume)ofp < 0.01,corrected

    for multiple comparisons. The correction for multiple comparisons was performed

    using a cluster-size thresholding algorithm[19]based on Monte Carlo simulations

    and implemented in the BrainVoyager QX software. A threshold ofp < 0.01 at the

    voxel level, a FWHM = 1 voxel as Gaussian kernel of the spatial correlation among

    voxels and 5000 iterations were used as input in the simulations, yielding a mini-

    mum cluster size of 8 voxels (< 0.01).

    2.6. Activations

    Clusters of activation were obtained from the group activation maps consider-

    ing voxels showing a significant response (p < 0.01, corrected) to any experimentalcondition. The Talairach coordinates ofTable 1are referred to the most significant

    voxelin eachactivated cluster.A correlation analysisbetween these activations was

    performed. Specifically, the correlation between the time course of the mean BOLD

    response (in a certain experimental condition) recorded from voxels belonging to

    two given clusters was evaluated. In order to assess the significance level for the

    r-Pearson coefficient, the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons among

    clusters was applied to thep-threshold.

    Moreover, an additional correlation betweenBOLD estimatedparameters(beta-

    weightsresulting from both contrasts, FA > Rest and OM> Rest) and both expertise

    levelandagewas evaluated, with reference to the ROIs emerging from the within-

    group analysis for the monks.

    2.7. Respiration rate correlation

    Since all subjects underwent physiological monitoring during scanning, a

    correlation analysis between the average respiration rates recorded in different

    meditation/rest conditionswas performed; in addition, t-test across conditionswasevaluated for bothmonks and novices,to probe condition-related breathing behav-

    ior differences between expert and novice meditators.

    3. Results

    3.1. Within-group analysis

    In order to test the involvement of differential brain activations

    in FA and OM meditation styles, with reference to Rest, the FA

    meditation vs. Rest condition (FA > Rest) and OM meditation vs.

    Rest condition(OM > Rest)contrastswere considered.Complemen-

    tarily, we analyzed brain activations in the OM meditation vs. FA

    meditation (OM > FA) contrast. The results revealed by these con-

    trasts for monks and novices are summarized inTable 1.

    3.1.1. Contrasts in the monk group

    The contrast FA > Rest (Fig. 2)showed a wide pattern of deacti-

    vations in the left hemisphere, comprising multiple clusters in the

    middle frontal gyrus (MFG), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)

    (BAs9/46), lateral anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) (BA10), pre-

    cuneus(BA7), transversetemporalgyrus (TTG)(BA41), anterior and

    posterior insula (BA13). The deactivations of the inferior frontal

    gyrus (IFG) (BA44 andBA46) andthe superior temporal gyrus (STG)

    (BA22) were found in therighthemisphere. Moreover,threemedial

    frontal areas exhibited an increased activity as compared to Rest,

    located in the left and right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)

    (BA24), and in the right medial aPFC (BA10).

    The contrast OM>Rest (Fig. 3) revealed three activations in

    the left hemisphere: medial aPFC (BA10), superior temporal gyrus

    (STG) (BA22) and superior parietal lobule (SPL)/precuneus (BA7;

    the activation cluster was centered in the SPL but extended medi-

    ally in the precuneus). The contrast OM> FA (Fig. 4) showed

    a large pattern of activations in the left hemisphere, including

    DLPFC (BAs9/46, superior frontal gyrusSFG and middle frontal

    gyrusMFG), lateral aPFC (BA10, MFG), medial frontal gyrus

    (MeFG) (BA9), precuneus (BA7), superior parietal lobule (SPL)

    (BA7), and anterior insula (BA13). In the right hemisphere, the SFG

    in lateral aPFC (BA10), the IFG (BA46) and the TTG (BA41) were

    activated. It is due to make the reader notice that a large part of theactivations are actually due to the fact that those areas are deac-

    tivated during FA, rather than activated during OM. A deactivation

    of the dorsal ACC (BA24) and the medial aPFC (BA10) was found in

    the right hemisphere.

    3.1.2. Contrasts in the novice group

    As regards the novices, the contrast FA > Rest (Fig. 5)showed a

    single activation in the left posterior cingulate(BA31). The contrast

    OM> Rest (Fig. 5)showed activations in the left dorsal ACC (BA32),

    the right rostral ACC (BA32), the right lateral orbitofrontal cortex

    (IFG, BA47) and the right medial aPFC (BA10). The contrast OM> FA

    yielded no significant activation.

    3.1.3. Cluster correlationsIn order to explore the differential state-dependent signal cor-

    relations between areas activated in the FA > Rest and OM> Rest

    contrasts, as well as between these areas and other hypothesis-

    relevant regions, we conducted a correlation analysis with clusters

    of activation derived from the contrasts, in the monks and con-

    trol subject groups. We evaluated the correlation between time

    courses of the mean BOLD response recorded from voxels belong-

    ingto twogivenclusters, in FA,OM or Rest conditions. In themonks,

    the correlations between clusters which were(positively) activated

    in the FA > Rest contrast were considered, with the correlations

    between such clusters and a subset of hypothesis-relevant clus-

    ters deactivated in the same contrast, including left lateral DLPFC

    and right IFG. In the same way, the correlations between the clus-

    ters which were (positively) activated in the OM > Rest contrastwere considered, with thecorrelations between such clusters anda

    subsetof hypothesis-relevant clusters activatedin theOM > FAcon-

    trast, including anterior insula, lateral aPFC and SPL. Correlations

    were also computed between clusters activatedin theFA > Rest and

    OM > Rest contrasts in the novices.

    In particular, we hypothesized positive correlations within the

    set of the three clusters activated in FA > Rest, and within the set

    of the three clusters activated in OM > Rest, to reflect two inter-

    area brain circuitries for FA and OM meditation: a medial frontal

    (prevalently right) circuitry for attentional focusing and monitor-

    ing in FA meditation; a left fronto-parieto-temporal circuitry for

    open awareness in OM meditation, We also hypothesized nega-

    tive correlations between the three activated clusters and a set of

    deactivated executive clusters in the FA> Rest contrast, including

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    4/11

    A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656 49

    Table 1

    Contrast results for monks and novices.

    Group/contrast/area x y z k T p

    Monks

    FA meditation> Rest

    Left SFG, BA10 10 66 19 81 5.579 ***

    Left dorsal ACC, BA24 9 26 16 270 6.832 ***

    Left MFG, BA46 48 39 13 618 5.706 ***

    Left MFG, BA9 47 32 28 243 6.338 ***

    Right MeFG, BA10 12 50 13 351 8.821 ****Right dorsal ACC, BA24 12 32 14 432 6.639 ***

    Right IFG, BA44 54 17 13 135 5.174 **

    Right IFG, BA46 48 41 14 270 8.225 ****

    Left precuneus, BA7 0 70 49 270 3.806 *

    Left TTG, BA41 41 25 10 314 8.552 ****

    Right STG, BA22 57 51 13 54 6.439 ***

    Left anterior insula, BA13 42 8 3 270 5.627 ***

    Left anterior insula, BA13 42 19 1 162 3.581 *

    Left posterior insula, BA13 41 10 7 1458 8.714 ****

    OM meditation > FA meditation

    Left MFG, BA46 41 44 25 108 7.319 ***

    Left MeFG, BA9 3 38 29 162 8.993 ****

    Left SFG, BA9 27 48 31 54 4.333 **

    Left MFG, BA10 26 59 13 57 5.201 **

    Left MFG, BA46 48 32 13 81 4.311 **

    Right SFG, BA10 24 41 22 54 5.386 **

    Right dorsal ACC, BA24 12 30 14 270 9.525 ****Right MeFG, BA10 15 48 10 351 4.726 **

    Right IFG, BA46 51 29 14 108 4.683 **

    Left STG, BA22 48 31 5 297 4.157 **

    Left TTG, BA41 39 31 13 513 5.183 **

    Right TTG, BA41 54 20 13 189 8.367 ****

    Left precuneus, BA7 6 73 46 297 6.957 ***

    Left SPL, BA7 30 55 61 108 3.69 *

    Left anterior insula, BA13 32 23 5 513 7.76 ***

    OM meditation> Rest

    Left MeFG, BA10 3 53 10 216 7.867 ***

    Left SPL/precuneus, BA7 18 64 43 108 4.503 **

    Left STG, BA22 54 37 7 297 5.63 ***

    Novices

    FA meditation> Rest

    Left posterior cingulate, BA31 23 25 37 216 8.889 ****

    OM meditation> Rest

    Left dorsal ACC, BA32 12 20 22 117 4.809 **

    Right rostral ACC, BA32 12 39 4 378 7.892 ****

    Right MeFG, BA10 15 56 14 243 6.812 ****

    Right IFG, BA47 21 23 5 2125 8.035 ****

    Contrast results for monks and novices. Single-voxel uncorrectedp-value are denoted by *(p < 0.01), **(p < 0.005), ***(p < 0.001), ****(p < 0.0001).

    lateral frontal areas in theleft andrighthemispheres. Finally, in this

    correlation analysis we considered activated clusters in the insula,

    given the observed massive deactivation of it in the FA > Rest con-

    dition, and its implication in meditation from previous studies (see

    [7,28]).

    Average positive and negative significant correlations in each

    of the groups, were computed. We considered clusters as corre-

    lated if the r-value was larger than 0.4 in at least four subjectsin the group, for positive correlations, and lower than -0.4 in at

    least four subjects, for negative correlations. The positively or neg-

    atively correlated clusters, according to such criteria, are shown in

    Table 2.

    3.1.4. Correlations in the monk group

    As regards clusters resulting from FA> Rest contrast, we found a

    highpositivecorrelation between theactivatedmedialfrontalareas

    during FA,OM andRest conditions (with theleft dorsalACC andthe

    right medialaPFC correlatingonly in theRest condition). A negative

    correlation, during the Rest condition, was found between these

    activated right and left dorsal ACC clusters, and a deactivated left

    DLPFC cluster,as well asbetween the leftdorsalACC and a right IFG

    activation in the same contrast. A negative correlation between the

    (activated) right medial aPFC cluster and a (deactivate) left DLPFC

    cluster, was also found in Rest.

    With reference to clusters activated in the OM > Rest and

    OM > FA contrasts, we found that left medial aPFC positively cor-

    related with lateral aPFC, STG and anterior insula in the left

    hemisphere. Theleft STGalso correlated with theleft superior pari-

    etal lobule and the left anterior insula. These correlations were

    foundin all experimental conditions. Finally, the left SPL/precuneuscorrelated with the left STG, in the Rest condition.

    3.1.5. Correlation with expertise level

    The probed correlation between beta-weights recorded in

    both contrasts (FA> Rest and OM > Rest) and meditation expertise

    revealed no significant outcomes for the OM condition, whereas

    during FA meditation both deactivations of right IFG (BA46) and

    of the posterior insula (BA13) positively correlate with meditation

    expertise (Fig. 7). Finally, no significant correlation with age was

    observed.

    3.1.6. Correlations in the novice group

    Correlations between the four activations in the FA > Rest

    or OM> Rest contrasts, in the novices group, were computed.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    5/11

    50 A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656

    Fig. 2. Activations and deactivations revealed by the FA > Rest contrast, in the monks group. Note the deactivation of insula (BA13), MFG (BA46), TTG (BA41) and precuneus

    (BA7) in the left hemisphere, and of IFG (BAs44/46) in the right hemisphere. Note also the activation of left/right dorsal ACC (BA24) and right medial aPFC (BA10). In this and

    in all the figures the right hemisphere is on the left side (radiological convention).

    Fig. 3. Activations revealed by the OM > Rest contrast, in the monks group, including medial aPFC (BA10), SPL/precuneus (BA7) and STG (BA22), in the left hemisphere.

    Table 2

    Correlationsbetweenactivationsresulting fromthe FA> Rest, OM> Restand OM> FA contrasts, for bothmonks and novices.The percentage of subjectsfor which significance

    was displayed, is indicated.

    Group Contrast Condition Areas Averager-v alue S ubje cts pe rcentage

    Monks FA > Rest Rest Dorsal ACC, L-BA24/MeFG, R-BA10 0.46 88%

    Monks FA > Rest All Dorsal ACC, L-BA24/Dorsal ACC, R-BA24 0.59 100%

    Monks FA > Rest All MeFG, R-BA10/Dorsal ACC, R-BA24 0.58 100%

    Monks FA >Rest Rest Dorsal ACC, L-BA24/MFG, L-BA9 0.44 62.5%

    Monks FA >Rest Rest Dorsal ACC, L-BA24/IFG, R-BA44 0.44 62.5%

    Monks FA > Rest Rest MeFG, R-BA10/MFG, L-BA9 0.43 62.5%

    Monks FA >Rest Rest Dorsal ACC, R-BA24/MFG, L-BA9 0.44 88%

    Monks OM > Rest All MeFG, L-BA10/MFG, L-BA10 0.47 62.5%

    Monks OM > Rest All MeFG, L-BA10/STG, L-BA22 0.40 62.5%

    Monks OM > Rest All MeFG, L-BA10/Anterior insula, L-BA13 0.43 62.5%

    Monks OM > Rest Rest Precuneus, L-BA7/STG, L-BA22 0.44 62.5%

    Monks OM > Rest All STG, L-BA22/SPL, L-BA7 0.43 62.5%

    Monks OM > Rest All STG, L-BA22/Anterior insula, L-BA13 0.53 75%

    Controls OM > Rest All Dorsal ACC, L-BA32/MeFG, R-BA10 0.41 63%

    Controls OM > Rest All Rostral ACC, R-BA32/MFG, R-BA11 0.53 75%

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    6/11

    A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656 51

    Fig.4. Activations anddeactivationsrevealed by theOM > FA contrast,in themonks group. Notethe activation of anteriorinsula(BA13),STG (BA22),TTG (BA41),MFG (BA46),

    MeFG (BA9) and SPL (BA7) in the left hemisphere. Note the activation of IFG (BA46) in the right hemisphere as well. The deactivation of right dorsal ACC (BA24) and medial

    aPFC (BA10), are also shown.

    We found a positive correlation between left dorsal ACC andright medial aPFC, and between right rostral ACC and the right

    orbitofrontal cluster, in all conditions.

    3.2. Between-group analysis

    Quantitative differences emerged from the between-group

    analysis (seeFig. 6).In order to evaluate if these differences were

    significant and determine their direction, t-tests (between the

    BOLD of monks andcontrols) were computed on theresulting clus-

    ters. This analysis confirms that the monks, as compared to the

    novices, increased dorsal ACC (BA24) and right MeFG (BA6 and

    BA10) activity bilaterally to focus their attention (FA meditation).

    In OM meditation, the monks engaged more than novices the leftprecuneus/SPL (BA7), the right dorsal ACC (BA32) and the right

    parahippocampal gyrus. By contrast, novice participants showed

    a higher engagement of the right rostral ACC (BA32), bilateral IFG

    (BA47), right orbitofrontal (BA11) and right medial aPFC in open

    monitoring.

    3.2.1. Respiration rate correlation

    The resulting correlation matrix (Fig. 8)showed no significant

    differences between OM, FA and Rest conditions for the monks,

    i.e. individual differences in basal rates of respiration (e.g., during

    rest) are preserved in the other conditions (FA, OM). In contrast,

    the novices differentiatedtheir respiration rates, aboveall between

    Fig. 5. Activations and deactivations revealedby theFA >Rest andOM >Rest contrasts, in thenovice group. Onthe left, thedeactivation of theleftposterior cingulate (BA31)

    in FA > Rest is shown. The other sections show the activation of the orbitofrontal IFG (BA47), medial aPFC (BA10) and rostral ACC (BA32), in the right hemisphere, and dorsal

    ACC (BA32) in the left hemisphere. The orientation of the sections follows the radiological convention.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    7/11

    52 A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656

    Fig. 6. Quantitative differences between monks and novices emerging from the between-group analysis during FA (left) and OM (right) meditations. The letterk indicates

    the number of voxels, andpthe significance of the peak voxel.

    OM and rest condition (Fig. 8(a) and (b),t-test across conditions).

    This result attests a peculiar condition-related breathing behavior

    of each group, and indicates that novice participants might reduce

    their focus on respiration when they are asked to emphasize the

    monitoring faculty.

    4. Discussion

    For the first time brain activity patterns in FA and OM med-

    itation were contrasted in a neuroimaging (fMRI) experiment,

    in expert (Buddhist monks) and lay novices, with an integrated

    FAOM paradigm. Overall, we found striking differences between

    the patterns of brain activity of monks and novices, in OM and

    FA meditation styles. The brain activity patterns of the monksin OM meditation resembled their ordinary brain resting state,

    whereas their brain activity in focused attention meditation

    sharply contrasted with both these states. In the monks, the

    larger differentiation between brain activity patterns in FA and

    OM meditation conditions, as compared to the OM > Rest con-

    trast, indeed suggests that open monitoring (mindfulness) is also

    reflected and thus practiced in ordinary non-meditative condi-

    tions.

    It hasbeen recently arguedthat meditative states areassociated

    to transient hypofrontality or deactivation in executive networks

    [15,26]. In contrast, other authors have emphasized the activa-

    tion of executive areas in meditation [7,28]. As expected, the

    results with our experimental design resolve this controversy: we

    conclude that FA meditation is associated to an enhanced (predom-

    inantly right) medial frontal and a reduced (predominantly left)

    lateral prefrontal activation, and OM meditation to an increased

    (predominantly left) medial frontal activation, as compared to

    rest. We also conclude that OM meditation, as compared with FA

    meditation, is characterized by a lateral prefrontal activation in

    both hemispheres, with a more subtle differentiation in medial

    frontal brain activations associated to these fundamental medita-

    tion styles.

    Unlike brain activations, signal correlations between areas

    activated in the contrasts were mostly not sensitive to the

    meditation/rest conditions, against our expectations. The result-

    ing correlation patterns suggest that networks or large-scale

    multiregional assemblies of neurons, plausibly emerging by neu-

    roplasticity, are recruited throughout different rest and meditationconditions. However, the activation level of their components

    located in different brain areas would depend on the meditation or

    rest conditions, as shown by our contrasts. As discussed below, the

    resulting positive and negative correlation patterns contribute to

    shed light on functional connections between subsets of activated

    brainareas,and suggest thata functionalreorganizationof thebrain

    resting state takes place in mental practice experts, with spatially

    distributed neural activations modulated by different meditation

    states.

    We will first consider meditation state effects in the monks and

    then in the novices, and subsequently discuss these two groups

    comparatively. The implications of our findings for the cognitive

    neuroscience of attention and awareness, will be thoroughly con-

    sidered through the discussion.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    8/11

    A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656 53

    Fig. 7. Significant correlation results between clusters emerging from the within-

    group analysis in the monk group. The correlation between beta-weights recorded

    in each condition (FA and OM) and both meditation expertiseand age reveals that

    during the FA meditation the right IFG (BA46) and the left posterior insula (BA13)

    positively correlate with meditation expertise. No significant correlation with age

    was observed.

    4.1. Self-regulation of cognitive engagement in the monks

    At a macroscopic level, a relative left-lateralization of brain

    activity patterns resulted in our experiment. Most of the deactiva-

    tions in FA> Rest and activations in OM> FA, and all the activations

    in OM > Rest, were in the left hemisphere. These patterns include

    a massive deactivation of left anterior and posterior insula in

    FA> Rest, and a consistent activation of the left anterior insula

    in OM > FA. We also found that the precuneus, which has been

    associated to self-referential activations[11,26], was involved in

    the left hemisphere in all contrasts. Specifically, it was deacti-

    vated in FA>Rest, and activated in OM>Rest and OM>FA, as

    hypothesized. The left precuneus was the only neural site to show

    a similar pattern in our contrasts. This evidence suggests that

    the left precuneus might plausibly act as a key brain region in

    the self-induced transitions between brain resting states asso-

    ciated to meditative and non-meditative attentional sets, in the

    monks.The OM > Rest and OM > FA contrasts, with related correlation

    analyses, also point out the relevance of the left SPL in BA7, which

    might act in an inter-area circuitry with the left STG activated in

    the OM > Rest contrast.In asymmetrywith the left hemisphere, in the right hemisphere

    we did not find activation or deactivation of (medial and lateral)

    BA9, insula and posterior parietal areas, in any of the contrasts.

    This evidence could not be predicted in light of the findings of

    a set of thicker cortical areas (including anterior insula) in the

    right hemisphere of insight meditators[25].This set of right hemi-sphere areas found in previous MRI structural studies might be

    involved in an ongoing phenomenal awareness of the fields of

    experience,independent on meditation-relatedattention focusand

    open monitoring. Indeed, during FA and OM meditations (as well

    as in rest), Buddhist monks experience an ongoing phenomenal

    awareness of sensory fields, even though items in these fields may

    not be intentionally accessed and investigated [28]. Conscious

    access to selected contents of experience might instead take place

    in fronto-parietalareas of the left hemisphere, consistent with psy-

    chophysical evidence about dominance of the left hemisphere in

    perceptual awareness[18,29,31].

    Finally, the activations found in the OM > Rest contrast in the

    monks concern three main regions typically associated to self-

    referential processing[32],with a peculiar left-lateralization. This

    Fig. 8. Correlationsbetweenrespirationfrequencies recordedin differentmeditation/rest conditions: correlation matrix (left) andt-testevaluated for monks (a)and novices

    (b). No significant differences emerged in monks, i.e. individual differences in basal rates of respiration (e.g., during rest) are preserved in the other conditions (FA, OM); in

    contrast,novicesdifferentiatedtheir respirationrates,aboveall betweenOM andRest condition(a andb: t-test across conditions). Thisfurther result revealscondition-related

    breathing behavior differences between expert and novice meditators.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    9/11

    54 A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656

    left-lateralized network of cortical midline and superior temporal

    areas might reflect a neuroplasticity-based cognitive reorganiza-

    tion of themonk brains, with neuronal populations in brain regions

    ordinarily linked to self-referential processing reallocated to a

    metacognitive observation of phenomenal experience and of the

    experiencing subject [33,35]. This refers to a crucial aspect of

    Buddhist practice and meditation, i.e. the cultivation of whole-

    some mental states going beyond the cognition of a separated self

    [1].

    As we did not find activation (or deactivation) of the left IFG,

    hypothesized to play an important role in self-awareness as an

    inner speech-related region [30],it can be inferred that the left-

    lateralized activity patterns we have observed are likely not to be

    associated with a narrative self function theorized to account for

    the dominance of the left hemisphere in conscious access [2,22].

    Moreover, given that it has been recently shown that the left IFG

    is characterized by a higher regional grey matter concentration in

    Vipassanameditators[23], the activation of this area could have

    been revealed in OM > Rest and OM > FA. Our negative evidence,

    however, indicate thatthe monksdid notperforma language-based

    access to present moment experiences.

    Our evidence suggests that the monks might control cognitive

    engagement and broadcasting in brain networks for conscious

    access to sensory-related, thought and emotion contents, by mas-sive self-regulation of fronto-parietal and insular areas in the left

    hemisphere, in a meditation state-dependent fashion. Specifically,

    this self-regulation appears mostly controlled by alternated acti-

    vations of (right) dorsal ACC and a set of neural sites in (left)

    MFG(right) IFG, andreflected in massive changes in activity levels

    of (left) insula and parietal posterior cortex, as well as in a shift of

    medial aPFC activation.

    4.2. Control of focused attention in the monks

    The observed dorsal ACC (left and right BA24) activations

    showed by the FA > Rest contrast are consistent with their pre-

    dicted involvement in conflict monitoring during FA meditation.

    The right medial aPFC (BA10) area activated in the same contrastis plausibly involved in focused awareness during FA medita-

    tion. The observed high positive correlations between these three

    frontal medial areas suggest that these midfrontal areas interact

    in a unitary circuitry, with a higher activation during FA medi-

    tation. The hypothesized activations in FA meditation associated

    to selective attention (temporalparietal junction, ventrolateral

    prefrontal cortex, intraparietal sulcus and frontal eye fields) and

    sustaining attention (right frontal and parietal areas, andthe thala-

    mus) were not observed. As an unpredicted result, in FA > Rest we

    observed a remarkable deactivation of DLPFC, more pronounced in

    the left hemisphere. The deactivation of the left DLPFC was even

    more widespread in OM > FA, with the additional involvement of

    the dorsolateral SFG. This evidence suggests that sustaining the

    attentional focus in FA meditation implies a deactivation of DLPFCareas.

    Interestingly, a negative correlation (in Rest) between both the

    right and left dorsal ACC clusters activated in FA > Rest, and a left

    DLPFC cluster deactivatedin FA> Rest,was found.Thisnegativecor-

    relation suggests that, especially when distraction is more likely

    (Rest), dorsal ACCand leftDLPFC playa contrasting rolein maintain-

    ing cognitive focus and opening the field of cognitive monitoring.

    We also found evidence about a role of the right IFG (BA44 and

    BA46) and the left DLPFC, as shown in contrasts and correlations

    (see Section3).

    It can therefore be concluded that dorsal ACC and lateral (in

    particular dorsolateral) prefrontal cortex play antagonist roles in

    executive control of the attentionsetting,as observed in meditation

    tasks without goal-related actions.

    4.3. Meditation state effects in the novices and comparison with

    the monks

    As regards thenovices,we only found thedeactivation of theleft

    posterior cingulate cortex in FA > Rest. Considering this result and

    the evidence about the precuneus in the monks, consistent with

    the recent proposal that the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex

    plays a pivotal role in the default mode network [20], it can be

    hypothesized that the left precuneus/posterior cingulate region is

    the component of the default mode network which can be more

    sensitively affected by a goal-independent task, such as FA med-

    itation. We also conclude that unlike FA meditation on a visual

    point [4], breath-centered FA meditation may demand a longer

    (more intensive) practice to observe more differentiated fMRI acti-

    vations, at least with a paradigm characterized by short-duration

    meditation blocks as in our experiment. It can also be concluded

    that the monks performed a demanding FA meditation task in our

    experiment.

    Four activated clusters in the OM > Rest contrast were found in

    the novice group. Overall, it seems that in novices open monitoring

    mostly involved right prefrontal areas. The activation of the left

    dorsal ACC might be explained by the executive demandto novices

    in OM meditation performance. The positive correlation between

    left dorsal ACC and right medial aPFC in the novice group, suggestthat these two areas co-operate in enabling cognitive focus.

    The activations in novices of (right) rostral ACC and (right) lat-

    eral orbitofrontal cortex (IFG), which were not found in the monk

    group, suggest that in novices open monitoring may reflect an

    evaluation-based stance. Indeed, there is evidence that rostral ACC

    and lateral orbitofrontal cortex are involved in affective and cogni-

    tive evaluation processes[6,16,24].These two areas were found to

    be positively correlated in the novice group.

    The key involvement of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (BA24)

    andright medialfrontalgyrus forfocusedattentionin monks,andof

    the right rostral anterior cingulate cortex (BA32) and orbitofrontal

    cortex/inferior frontal gyrus (BA11/BA47) for open monitoring in

    novices, is confirmed by the between-group analysis.

    4.4. Correlates of monk expertise

    The probed correlation between beta-weights recorded in both

    conditions and meditation expertise of the monks revealed that

    during FA meditation both the deactivations of theright IFG(BA46)

    and of the left (posterior) insula (BA13) positively correlate with

    meditation expertise, i.e. larger deactivations are observed in more

    expert practitioners. This evidence suggests that the expertise-

    dependent sustained focused attention implies the deactivation

    of such regions, likely to be involved in a more transient aware-

    ness of experience contents [28]. The disengagement of these

    areas might also be plausibly related to a more effortless main-

    tenance of attentional (cognitive) focus in expert meditators[28],

    in terms of a reduced background of neural activations which canpotentially reorient the allocation of limited attention-relatedbrain

    resources.

    4.5. Potential caveats and further investigations

    It remains to be seen how the differential brain activity patterns

    we have found correlate with hours of practice in a larger group

    of FA/OM meditation practitioners. Although all the participants in

    our study reported that they could perform FA and OM meditation

    forms according to the given instructions and with no differences

    in experienced difficulty, in a further study with a larger group

    of participants, a quantitative measure of effort (e.g., self-ratings

    on a Likert-scale), performed block by block, can provide a more

    accurate information about effort for FA and OM meditation forms.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    10/11

    A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656 55

    Indeed, effort is an important aspect of meditation performance

    [4,28]. Consideration of other aspects, such as balancing for subjec-

    tive efficacy (how well FA andOM were performed), intensity (how

    intense the experience was) and stability (for how long the med-

    itative state persisted) within and between groups, also appears

    important. As suggested by previous research[4],controlling for

    subjective motivation while meditating and structural differences

    in brain anatomy due to ethnicity, are further relevant aspects to

    keep into account.

    In further studies it may be insightful to compare brain activity

    patterns in OM meditation conditions with differential awareness

    of fields of experience, such as body sensory field, external sensory

    fields andinternal thoughts andfeelings. There is indeedevidence

    with a related theoretical account [5], to differentiate prefrontal

    regions involved in stimulus-oriented and stimulus-independent

    processing. Moreover, it appears useful to design an experiment

    comparing brain activity patterns in FA meditation with focus on

    breath-related sensations(as in our study) and on an external visual

    point (as in[4]). FA meditation might involve a different level of

    engagement with external world processing and modulation of

    self-referential (default mode) brain networks, depending on the

    focused object.

    To conclude, the present study appears to shed light on fun-

    damental aspects of meditation practice and the related set ofcognitive functions, beyond and complementarily to the findings

    in previous studies, as related to the unique participants in it and

    its design. The present findings might open the way to deeper

    investigations of meditation-based awareness, with important

    implications for the domain of neural correlates of consciousness

    [33].

    Conflict of interest

    Theauthors declare that they have no competingfinancialinter-

    ests.

    Acknowledgements

    First of all, we thank themonksof theSantacittaramamonastery

    for their kind participation in the study, as well as for useful feed-

    back at different stages of the work. We thank Antoine Lutz for

    important suggestions to improve the design of the experiment.

    We also thank Alessandro DAusilioand Valerio Santangelo foruse-

    ful comments, and Luca Simione for kind assistance. Finally, we

    have benefited from helpful and critical discussions about statisti-

    cal data analysis from two of our close colleagues, Gianna Sepede

    and NicolettaCera andwe wish to sincerely thank their continuous

    support in this study. Finally, we would like to thank two anony-

    mous reviewers for important remarks and comments which have

    conducted to a significantly improved manuscript.

    References

    [1] A. Sumedho, Intuitive Awareness, Amaravati Buddhist Monastery, HemelHempstead, UK, 2004.

    [2] B.J.Baars, In the theatre of consciousness, J. Conscious. Stud. 4 (1997) 292309.[3] G.M. Boynton, S.A. Engel, G.H. Glover, D.J. Heeger, Linear systems analysis of

    functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1, J. Neurosci. 16. (1996)42074241.

    [4] J.A.Brefczynski-Lewis,A. Lutz, H.S.Schaefer,D.B.Levinson,R.J. Davidson, Neuralcorrelates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners, Proc.Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 (2007) 1148311488.

    [5] P.W. Burgess, I. Dumontheil, S.J. Gilbert, The gateway hypothesis of rostral PFC(area 10) function, Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 (2007) 290298.

    [6] G. Bush, P. Luu, M.I. Posner, Cognitive and emotional influences in anteriorcingulate cortex, Trends Cogn. Sci. 4 (2000) 215222.

    [7] B.R. Cahn, J. Polich, Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging

    studies, Psychol. Bull. 132 (2006) 180211.

    [8] L.E. Carlson, M. Speca, P. Faris, K.D. Patel, One year pre-post interventionfollow-up of psychological, immune, endocrine and blood pressure outcomesof mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in breast and prostate canceroutpatients, Brain Behav. Immun. 21 (2007) 10381049.

    [9] C.S. Carter, S.T. Braver, D.M. Barch, M.M. Botvinick, D. Noll, J.D. Cohen, Anteriorcingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance,Science 280 (1998) 747749.

    [10] O.L.Carter,D.E. Presti, C. Callistemon, Y. Ungerer,G.B. Liu,J.D. Pettigrew, Medi-tation alters perceptual rivalry in TibetanBuddhistmonks,Curr. Biol.15 (2005)R412R413.

    [11] A.E. Cavanna, M.R. Trimble, The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy

    and behavioural correlates, Brain 129 (2006) 564583.[12] M. Corbetta, G.L.Shulman, Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven atten-

    tion in the brain, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 (2002) 201215.[13] J.T. Coull, Neural correlates of attention andarousal: insight fromelectrophysi-

    ology functional neuroimagingand psychopharmacology, Progr. Neurobiol. 55(1998) 343361.

    [14] R.J.Davidson, D.J.Goleman, The roleof attention in meditation and hypnosis: apsychobiological perspective on transformations of consciousness, Int. J. Clin.Exp. Hypn. 25 (1977) 291308.

    [15] A. Dietrich, Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: thetransient hypofrontality hypothesis, Conscious. Cogn. 12 (2003) 231256.

    [16] R. Elliott, R.J. Dolan, C.D. Frith, Dissociable functions in the medial and lat-eral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies, Cereb.Cortex 10 (2000) 308317.

    [17] N.A.S. Farb, Z.V. Segal, H. Mayberg, J. Bean, D. McKeon, Z. Fatima, A.K. Ander-son, Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neuralmodes of self-reference, Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2 (2007) 313322.

    [18] J.H. Fecteau, A. Kingstone, J.T. Enns, Hemisphere differences in explicit andimplicit word reading, Conscious. Cogn. 13 (2004) 550564.

    [19] S.D. Forman, J.D. Cohen, M. Fitzgerald, W.F. Eddy, M.A. Mintun, D.C. Noll,Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic reso-nance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold, Magn. Reson. Med. 33(1995) 636647.

    [20] P. Fransson, G. Marrelec, The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a piv-otal role in the default mode network: evidence from a partial correlationnetwork analysis, Neuroimage 42 (2008) 11781184.

    [21] K.J. Friston, A.P. Holmes, K.J. Worsley, J.P. Poline, C.D. Frith, R.S.J. Frackowiak,Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear modelapproach, Hum. Brain Mapp. 2 (1995) 173181.

    [22] M.S.Gazzaniga,Principlesof human brain organizationderived fromsplit-brainstudies, Neuron 14 (1995) 217228.

    [23] B.K. Hlzel, U. Ott, T. Gard, H. Hempel, M. Weygandt, K. Morgen, D. Vait, Differ-ential engagement of anterior cingulated and adjacentmedial frontal cortex inadept meditators and nonmeditators, Neurosci. Lett. 421 (2007) 1621.

    [24] M.L. Kringelbach, E.T. Rolls, The functional neuroanatomy of the humanorbitofrontal cortex:evidence from neuroimagingand neuropsychology, Progr.Neurobiol. 72 (2004) 341372.

    [25] S.W. Lazar, C.E. Kerr, R.H. Wasserman, J.R. Gray, D.N. Greve, M.T. Treadway, M.McGarvey, B.T. Quinn, J.A. Dusek, H. Benson, S.L. Rauch, C.I. Moore, B. Fischl,Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness, Neu-roReport 16 (2005) 18931897.

    [26] H.C. Lou, M. Nowak, T.W. Kjaer, The mental self, Progr. Brain Res. 150 (2005)197204.

    [27] A.R. Luria, The Working Brain: An Introduction to Neuropsychology, PenguinBooks, London, UK, 1973.

    [28] A. Lutz, H.A. Slagter, J.D. Dunne, R.J. Davidson, Attention regulation and moni-toring in meditation, Trends Cogn. Sci. 12 (2008) 163169.

    [29] T.A. Mondor, M.P. Bryden, On the relation between auditory spatial atten-tion and auditory perceptual asymmetries, Percept. Psychophys. 52 (1992)393402.

    [30] A. Morin, J. Michaud, Self-awareness and the left inferior frontal gyrus: innerspeech use during self-related processing, Brain Res. Bull. 74 (2007) 387396.

    [31] M.E.R. Nicholls, C.J. Cooper, Hemispheric differences in the rates of informa-tion processing for simple non-verbal stimuli, Neuropsychologia 29 (1991)677684.

    [32] M.I. Posner, M.K. Rothbart, Research on attention networks as a model for theintegration of psychological science, Ann. Rev. Psychol. 58 (2007) 123.

    [33] A. Raffone, N. Srinivasan, An adaptive workspace model about the neural cor-relates of consciousness: insights from neuroscience and meditation studies,in:N. Srinivasan (Eds.),Progress in Brain Research: Attention, vol.176, Elsevier,Amsterdam, 2009, pp. 161180.

    [34] H.A. Slagter, A. Lutz, L.L. Greischar, A.D. Francis, S. Nieuwenhuis, J.M. Davis, R.J.Davidson, Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources, PLoSBiol. 5 (2007) e138.

    [35] A. Tagini, A. Raffone, The I and the Me in self-referential awareness: a neu-rocognitive hypothesis, Cogn. Process. 11 (1) (2010) 920.

    [36] J. Talairach, P. Tournoux, Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain,Thieme, New York, 1998.

    [37] Y.Y. Tang, Y.H. Ma, J. Wang, Y.X. Fan, S.G. Feng, Q.L. Lu, Short-term meditationtrainingimproves attentionand self-regulation, Proc.Natl. Acad. Sci.U.S.A.104(2007) 1715217156.

    [38] J.D. Teasdale, J.M.G. Williams, J.M. Soulsby, Z.V. Segal, V.A. Ridgeway, M.A. Lau,Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-basedcognitive therapy, J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68 (2000) 615623.

  • 7/26/2019 Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation

    11/11

    56 A. Manna et al. / Brain Research Bulletin 82 (2010) 4656

    [39] E.R. Valentine, P.L.G. Sweet, Meditation attention: a comparison of the effectsof concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention, MentalHealth Relig. Cult. 2 (1999) 5970.

    [40] D.H. Weissman, K.C. Roberts, K.M. Visscher, M.G. Woldorff, The neural corre-lates of momentary lapses in attention, Nat. Neurosci. 9 (2006) 971978.

    [41] World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki,World Medical AssociationDeclaration of Helsinki. Recommendations guiding physicians in biomedi-cal research involving human subjects, J. Am. Med. Assoc. 277 (11) (1997)925926.