religiousness and depression

23
Religiousness and Depression: Evidence for a Main Effect and the Moderating Influence of Stressful Life Events Timothy B. Smith Brigham Young University Michael E. McCullough University of Miami Justin Poll Brigham Young University The association between religiousness and depressive symptoms was examined with meta-analytic methods across 147 independent investigations (N 98,975). Across all studies, the correlation between religiousness and depressive symptoms was –.096, indicating that greater religiousness is mildly associated with fewer symptoms. The results were not moderated by gender, age, or ethnicity, but the religiousness– depression association was stronger in studies involving people who were undergoing stress due to recent life events. The results were also moderated by the type of measure of religiousness used in the study, with extrinsic religious orientation and negative religious coping (e.g., avoiding difficulties through religious activities, blaming God for difficulties) associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms, the opposite direction of the overall findings. Depression and depressive symptoms are among the most com- mon of all mental disorders and health complaints. Worldwide, as many as 330 million people may suffer from depression at any given time, with prevalence estimates ranging from 2%–3% for men and 5%–12% for women (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Approximately 20 million visits to physicians in 1993– 1994 involved reports of depressive symptoms (Pincus et al., 1998). Depression is costly as well as prevalent: The worldwide market for antidepressants in 1998 was approximately $7 billion (“Spirit of the Age,” 1998). In the United States alone, approxi- mately $12 billion in labor is lost to depressive symptoms each year. Moreover, depression is also a leading cause of physical disability and has even been found to be a risk factor for cardio- vascular mortality (Saz & Dewey, 2001; Wulsin, Vaillant, & Wells, 1999). Given the prevalence of depression and the burdens it creates, investigators have spent a great deal of effort trying to identify factors that may be useful for improving its detection and diagno- sis. Well-established risk factors include (a) genetic factors (Na- tional Institute of Mental Health Genetics Workgroup, 1998); (b) gender (Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson, & Grayson, 1999); (c) social isolation (Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Joiner & Coyne, 1999; Lara, Leader, & Klein, 1997); (d) personality traits such as dependency, Introversion, and Neuroticism (Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Jorm et al., 2000); and (e) stressful life events (e.g., Monroe & Simons, 1991; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991). Another variable that has recently received attention in the depression literature is religious involvement. Several recent high- profile studies (e.g., Braam et al., 2001; Koenig, George, & Peter- son, 1998; Murphy et al., 2000) indicate that certain aspects of religiousness (e.g., public religious involvement, intrinsic religious motivation) may be inversely related to depressive symptoms (with greater religious involvement associated with fewer symp- toms of depression). Of note, Braam et al. (2001) reported that public religious involvement (viz., church attendance) was in- versely related to depression among older individuals from Euro- pean countries that were included in the EURODEP collaboration. Also Koenig, George, and Peterson (1998) reported that among clinically depressed older adults, intrinsic religiousness was strongly associated with the speed with which individuals’ depres- sive episodes abated, even after controlling for a variety of poten- tial confounds. Reviewers of the overall literature on religion and depression (e.g., Koenig, McCullough, & Larson, 2001; McCul- lough & Larson, 1999) have reached similar conclusions. Studies on these issues have been accumulating for more than 100 years (see McCullough & Larson, 1999). Indeed, studies as far back as the 1880s have pointed to religion as a possible influence on the occurrence and severity of depression (Koenig et al., 2001). Given the perceived size and scope of this literature, scholars have recently recommended that investigators quantify and summarize Timothy B. Smith and Justin Poll, Department of Counseling Psychol- ogy, Brigham Young University; Michael E. McCullough, Department of Psychology, University of Miami. Work on this article was supported by grants from the John Templeton Foundation, the Campaign for Forgiveness Research, TP Industrials Inc., and the Religious Research Association. We thank Sharon Black, Rick Ingram, and Michael Lambert for their helpful comments on previous versions of this article and Brandon Dickson, Tiffany Martin, and Jim Scriber for their work as coders. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Timothy B. Smith, Department of Counseling Psychology, Brigham Young Univer- sity, 340 MCKB, Provo, Utah 84602-5093 or to Michael E. McCullough, Department of Psychology, University of Miami, P.O. Box 248185, Coral Gables, Florida 33124-2070. E-mail: [email protected] or mmccullough@ umiami.ir.miami.edu Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 129, No. 4, 614 – 636 0033-2909/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.614 614

Upload: auraspsy

Post on 01-Oct-2015

223 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

DESCRIPTION

Religie si psihologie

TRANSCRIPT

  • Religiousness and Depression: Evidence for a Main Effect and theModerating Influence of Stressful Life Events

    Timothy B. SmithBrigham Young University

    Michael E. McCulloughUniversity of Miami

    Justin PollBrigham Young University

    The association between religiousness and depressive symptoms was examined with meta-analyticmethods across 147 independent investigations (N 98,975). Across all studies, the correlation betweenreligiousness and depressive symptoms was .096, indicating that greater religiousness is mildlyassociated with fewer symptoms. The results were not moderated by gender, age, or ethnicity, but thereligiousnessdepression association was stronger in studies involving people who were undergoingstress due to recent life events. The results were also moderated by the type of measure of religiousnessused in the study, with extrinsic religious orientation and negative religious coping (e.g., avoidingdifficulties through religious activities, blaming God for difficulties) associated with higher levels ofdepressive symptoms, the opposite direction of the overall findings.

    Depression and depressive symptoms are among the most com-mon of all mental disorders and health complaints. Worldwide, asmany as 330 million people may suffer from depression at anygiven time, with prevalence estimates ranging from 2%3% formen and 5%12% for women (American Psychiatric Association,2000). Approximately 20 million visits to physicians in 19931994 involved reports of depressive symptoms (Pincus et al.,1998). Depression is costly as well as prevalent: The worldwidemarket for antidepressants in 1998 was approximately $7 billion(Spirit of the Age, 1998). In the United States alone, approxi-mately $12 billion in labor is lost to depressive symptoms eachyear. Moreover, depression is also a leading cause of physicaldisability and has even been found to be a risk factor for cardio-vascular mortality (Saz & Dewey, 2001; Wulsin, Vaillant, &Wells, 1999).

    Given the prevalence of depression and the burdens it creates,investigators have spent a great deal of effort trying to identify

    factors that may be useful for improving its detection and diagno-sis. Well-established risk factors include (a) genetic factors (Na-tional Institute of Mental Health Genetics Workgroup, 1998); (b)gender (Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson, & Grayson, 1999); (c) socialisolation (Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Joiner & Coyne, 1999; Lara,Leader, & Klein, 1997); (d) personality traits such as dependency,Introversion, and Neuroticism (Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Jorm et al.,2000); and (e) stressful life events (e.g., Monroe & Simons, 1991;Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991).

    Another variable that has recently received attention in thedepression literature is religious involvement. Several recent high-profile studies (e.g., Braam et al., 2001; Koenig, George, & Peter-son, 1998; Murphy et al., 2000) indicate that certain aspects ofreligiousness (e.g., public religious involvement, intrinsic religiousmotivation) may be inversely related to depressive symptoms(with greater religious involvement associated with fewer symp-toms of depression). Of note, Braam et al. (2001) reported thatpublic religious involvement (viz., church attendance) was in-versely related to depression among older individuals from Euro-pean countries that were included in the EURODEP collaboration.Also Koenig, George, and Peterson (1998) reported that amongclinically depressed older adults, intrinsic religiousness wasstrongly associated with the speed with which individuals depres-sive episodes abated, even after controlling for a variety of poten-tial confounds. Reviewers of the overall literature on religion anddepression (e.g., Koenig, McCullough, & Larson, 2001; McCul-lough & Larson, 1999) have reached similar conclusions.

    Studies on these issues have been accumulating for more than100 years (see McCullough & Larson, 1999). Indeed, studies as farback as the 1880s have pointed to religion as a possible influenceon the occurrence and severity of depression (Koenig et al., 2001).Given the perceived size and scope of this literature, scholars haverecently recommended that investigators quantify and summarize

    Timothy B. Smith and Justin Poll, Department of Counseling Psychol-ogy, Brigham Young University; Michael E. McCullough, Department ofPsychology, University of Miami.

    Work on this article was supported by grants from the John TempletonFoundation, the Campaign for Forgiveness Research, TP Industrials Inc.,and the Religious Research Association. We thank Sharon Black, RickIngram, and Michael Lambert for their helpful comments on previousversions of this article and Brandon Dickson, Tiffany Martin, and JimScriber for their work as coders.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to TimothyB. Smith, Department of Counseling Psychology, Brigham Young Univer-sity, 340 MCKB, Provo, Utah 84602-5093 or to Michael E. McCullough,Department of Psychology, University of Miami, P.O. Box 248185, CoralGables, Florida 33124-2070. E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected]

    Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.2003, Vol. 129, No. 4, 614636 0033-2909/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.614

    614

  • this literature through meta-analysis so that the potential utility ofreligiousness as a predictor of depressive symptoms could beassessed more objectively (McCullough & Larson, 1998), just asrecent meta-analytic work has contributed to understanding of thenature of the relationship of religiousness and longevity (McCul-lough, Hoyt, Larson, Koenig, & Thoresen, 2000). A similar meta-analysis on the relationship of religiousness and depressive symp-toms also would permit an examination of several subsidiaryquestions.

    Possible Mechanisms of Association

    If religiousness and depressive symptoms are indeed related,what factors influence that relationship? It is challenging to offera theoretical account that is both elegant and comprehensive be-cause research has yet to confirm which variables moderate therelationship, let alone which variables mediate it. Identification ofmoderating and mediating variables is important but difficult be-cause both religiousness and depressive symptoms are influencedby a host of biological, social, and psychological factors. Tocomplicate matters, the variables with which religion and depres-sion are correlated typically are not exclusively one-way relation-ships: Because of the possibility of reciprocal relationships, manyfactors that are believed to be causes of depression may also beconsequences of depression, and the same is true of religiousness.Regardless, insofar as a relationship between religiousness anddepressive symptoms does exist, any of a variety of mechanismsmight explain the association. Previous research suggests somepossibilities that should inform future efforts, although not all ofthese possibilities can be addressed with meta-analytic methods atpresent.

    Potentially Common Influences on Religiousness andDepressive Symptoms

    Genetic influences. Religiousness might be associated withdepressive symptoms because of similar genetic influences. Sev-eral studies suggest that 40%50% of the variance in religiousnessmay be attributable to additive genetic factors (DOnofrio, Eaves,Murrelle, Maes, & Spilka, 1999; Waller, Kojetin, Bouchard,Lykken, & Tellegen, 1990). To the extent that certain genes thatconfer resistance to depressive symptoms also contribute to thedevelopment of religious sentiments, one might expect a negativecorrelation between religious involvement and depressive symp-toms. However, the multivariate behaviorgenetic studies thatwould shed light on this issue have not been conducted to date.

    Developmental influences. Developmental factors that conferresistance to depressive symptoms may also foster the develop-ment of religious interests in children and adolescents. For exam-ple, warm and caring childparent bonds may be both a protectivefactor against depression (Ingram & Ritter, 2000) and a positivefactor in development of religious interests (at least within reli-gious families, J. Wilson & Sherkat, 1994). Therefore, parentalwarmth, closeness, and caring may explain some of the associationof religiousness and resistance to depressive symptoms. Con-versely, negative life events during childhood that might conferrisk for depressive symptoms might also disincline people frominterests in or concerns with religious matters. Some research hasindicated that poor relationships with parents may predispose an

    individual to depression and contribute to a lack of interest inreligious matters (Hunsberger, 1980). It may also be that someindividuals seek out religion to compensate for poor relationshipswith parents (Granqvist, 1998; Kirkpatrick & Shaver, 1990).

    Depressive Influences on ReligiousnessIn addition to the possibility that the associations between

    religiousness and depression are influenced by common develop-mental factors, there are good reasons to believe that depressivesymptoms might influence religiousness. People who are experi-encing high levels of depressive symptoms may find a lack ofpleasure in former religious involvements, which may over timeerode their public and even private engagements with their reli-gious faith. Moreover, to the extent that a persons depressivesymptoms include a lack of energy or a physical disability, previ-ously religious people may find themselves unable to engage inreligious pursuits, by extension making them seem less religiouson many metrics for assessing religiousness. On the other hand,depressive symptoms apparently prompt some people to seekcomfort in their religion (e.g., by attending religious services orreading holy scripture), which might, de facto, increase theirapparent religiousness (Ferraro & Kelley-Moore, 2000).

    Religious Influences on Depressive SymptomsAnother possibility, that factors associated with religiousness

    influence symptoms of depression, has received the most attentionin the recent empirical literature. Many researchers have suggestedthat religiousness may reduce vulnerability to depressive symp-toms by way of a variety of substantive psychosocial mechanisms.Although an exhaustive list of these potential mechanisms is notfeasible here, we illustrate with four possible mechanisms. (For amore extensive review, see Koenig et al., 2001.)

    Lower substance use. National surveys point to high rates ofcomorbidity between depressive symptoms and both drug abuseand drug dependence (Grant, 1995), which suggests that drugabuse and dependence may be risk factors for the development ofdepressive symptoms. Evidence also suggests that religious in-volvement is related to lower rates of substance use (e.g., tobacco,alcohol, and drugs) among both adolescents (DOnofrio, Murrelle,et al., 1999) and adults (Kendler, Gardner, & Prescott, 1997). Theeffects of religion in deterring drug use may be largely throughendorsing moral proscriptions and discouraging interaction withdrug-using peers (DOnofrio, Murrelle, et al., 1999). Therefore, bydeterring drug use among adolescents and adults, religious in-volvement might indirectly influence peoples susceptibility todepressive symptoms.

    Social support. Religious involvement may afford people op-portunities for social support, which has been found to protectagainst depressive symptoms (Koenig et al., 2001; George, Larson,Koenig, & McCullough, 2000). People who are involved in reli-gion have substantially more informal social contacts and are moreactive in civic engagements than people who are not (Putnam,2000). Insofar as religious involvement puts people in touch withsuch sources of social support, this participation may be a mech-anism that accounts for some of the inverse association of reli-giousness and depressive symptoms.

    615RELIGIOUSNESS AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS

  • Appraisal of life events. Religiously involved people mayhave resources for appraising negative life events that reduce theperceived stressfulness of those events (Pargament, 1997). To theextent that religious people believe that their lives are controlledby a higher power or that negative life events happen for a reasonor that life events are opportunities for spiritual growth, they mayexperience life events as less threatening and less stressful (Georgeet al., 2000). As a result, these positive appraisals may help toprotect some religious individuals from depressive symptoms byhelping them to perceive negative events as less stressful.

    Coping with stress. Pargament (1997) also described how avariety of religious cognitions and behaviors that help religiouslyinvolved people to cope with stress may deter negative mentalhealth outcomes. Relatedly, Koenig et al. (1992) reported thatmedically ill men who reported using religion to cope with theirphysical health problems also reported less severe depressivesymptoms, even with a variety of other demographic, physical, andpsychosocial predictors of depression controlled. Such findingssuggest that religiousness may protect people against depressivesymptoms by helping them to avert the negative psychologicalsequelae that are frequently associated with stressful life events.

    Main Effect or Stress-Buffering Effect?Insofar as religious involvement is inversely related to depres-

    sive symptoms, it is conceivable that this association appliesequally to persons irrespective of the amount of life stress that theyexperience. This hypothesis has been called the main effect hy-pothesis, signifying that the association of religiousness and de-pressive symptoms is significant and negative when averagedacross all levels of life stress. On the other hand, it is conceivablethat, like many psychosocial factors that are related to depressivesymptoms, the negative association of religious involvement anddepression is even stronger for people who have recently under-gone high amounts of life stress. This view has been called thebuffering hypothesis (S. Cohen & Wills, 1985).

    The main effect hypothesis and buffering hypothesis are notmutually exclusive, of course: It is possible for religious involve-ment to be related to significantly lower degrees of depressivesymptoms for all persons and for the association to become evenstronger for people who are undergoing particularly high levels oflife stress. Indeed, some investigators have found evidence forboth hypotheses (e.g., Kendler, Gardner, & Prescott, 1999). Re-latedly, it is conceivable that the negative association betweenreligious involvement and depressive symptoms becomes particu-larly strong among people who are undergoing moderate or severedepressive difficulties. Depressive symptoms themselves can be asubstantial source of life stress that may require secondary copingefforts for the individual to avoid being caught in a downwardspiral. Theory development in the study of religion and mentalhealth would benefit greatly from knowing whether the religiousinvolvementdepressive symptoms relationship is consistent withthe main effect hypothesis, the buffering hypothesis, or both(Schnittker, 2001).

    Other Potential Moderators of the AssociationGiven reports of differences in the rates of religiousness and

    depression across sociodemographic groups (e.g., American Psy-

    chiatric Association, 2000; Levin, Taylor, & Chatters, 1994;Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1999; Taylor, Chatters, Jayakody, &Levin, 1996), it is conceivable that the religiousnessdepressionassociation is not uniform across ethnicity, age, and gender. If so,the apparent disparities among some of the results that investiga-tors have obtained may be caused by differences in the nature ofthe samples studied. For example, the religiousnessdepressionrelationship may be stronger for women than for men, for olderadults than for younger adults, or for African Americans than forEuropean Americans (Blaine & Crocker, 1995; Musick, Koenig,Hays, & Cohen, 1998).

    Relatedly, it is likely that the methods used to measure reli-giousness and/or depression within these studies have a reliableimpact on the strength of the associations (Schnittker, 2001). In arecent narrative review, McCullough and Larson (1999) noted thatthe negative association of religiousness and depressive symptomsappeared to be particularly strong when religiousness was mea-sured in terms of public religious involvement or intrinsic religiousmotivation (Allport & Ross, 1967) but less so when religiousnesswas measured in terms of private religiousness, such as thestrength with which people hold particular religious beliefs. More-over, McCullough and Larson (1999) also noted that manifesta-tions of religiousness that are extrinsically motivated, perhaps as ameans to nonreligious ends (Allport & Ross, 1967), may actuallybe associated with a higher risk for depressive symptoms. Thepostulate that extrinsic religiousness is a maladaptive form ofreligiousness, predictive of negative outcomes such as mentalillness as opposed to mental health, has been supported by others(e.g., Donahue, 1985; Richards & Bergin, 1997). For example,Burris (1994) found that extrinsic religiousness is positively pre-dictive of depression unless intrinsic religiousness is either veryhigh or very low.

    These findings are conceptually similar to claims concerningnegative forms of religious coping, such as blaming God for lifesproblems or avoiding problems through religious activities, thatmay also predict mental illness as opposed to mental health (e.g.,Pargament, 1997). Nevertheless, the purported association be-tween religiousness and depression as well as the effects of dif-ferent types of religiousness on that association observed in nar-rative reviews have yet to be confirmed using empirical methodsfor synthesizing scientific literatures.

    We investigated issues raised above that were possible to ad-dress using extant data from the literature on religiousness anddepressive symptoms. In this meta-analysis, our major goals wereto (a) estimate the average magnitude of the association betweenmeasures of religiousness and depressive symptoms; (b) assesswhether the association of religiousness and depressive symptomsis consistent with a main effect model, a stress-buffering model, orboth; and (c) identify characteristics of study samples and methodsthat might explain variation in the religiousnessdepression asso-ciation. In addition, we assessed the vulnerability of our conclu-sions to publication bias (Rosenthal, 1979; Sutton, Song, Gilbody,& Abrams, 2000).

    Method

    Literature SearchTo identify published and unpublished studies examining the association

    of religious involvement with symptoms of depression, we used three

    616 SMITH, MCCULLOUGH, AND POLL

  • techniques. First, we conducted searches with several electronic databases:Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL),Dissertation Abstracts International, Education Resources InformationCenter database, HealthSTAR, MEDLINE, Mental Health Abstracts, Pro-gramme Applique a` la Selection et la Compilation Automatique de Lit-terature, PsycINFO, Religion Index, Social Sciences Abstracts, SocialSciSearch, Sociological Abstracts via SocioFile, Social Work Abstracts,and TGG Health & Wellness database. To capture the broadest possiblesample of relevant articles, we used multiple search terms: all wordsbeginning with the root depress, as well as terms such as affective disorder,mood, and affect. These search terms were crossed with a series of searchwords related to religion and religious spirituality (all words beginningrelig, spirit, church, mosque, synagogue, temple, worship, and pray). Toreduce inadvertent omissions, databases yielding the most citations (CI-NAHL, Dissertation Abstracts International, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, andSocial Science Abstracts) were searched one to three additional times bydifferent members of the research team. Second, we manually examinedthe reference sections of past reviews and of studies meeting the inclusioncriteria for articles not identified in the database searches. Finally, we sentsolicitation letters to authors who had published three or more articles onthe topic to obtain several additional unpublished studies. Unpublishedstudies and dissertations obtained by contacting the authors or universitylibraries eventually accounted for 24% of included reports.

    Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria for Relevant StudiesStudies included in the present investigation met the following criteria:

    First, they were written in English and were identified by searches con-ducted through February 2000. Second, they provided an estimate of thebivariate association of religiousness and depressive symptoms at the levelof an individual person (studies reporting group-level data on incidencerates were not included). Thus, each study involved at least one measure ofan individuals depressive symptoms and at least one measure of that sameindividuals religiousness.

    Religiousness was defined broadly as any attitude, belief, motivation,pursuit, or behavior involving spiritual or religious content or processes.The terms religiousness and spirituality can both refer to an individualssearch for that which is sacred, with the former implying group or socialpractices and doctrines and the latter tending to refer to personal experi-ences and beliefs. This meta-analysis uses the word religiousness becausethe majority of research studies reviewed used similar terminology andbecause religion encompasses pursuits and behaviors that are not neces-sarily spiritual (e.g., seeking social support from a congregation) (Hill etal., 2000). Because existing measures of religiousness and spiritualitygreatly overlap in content (Tsang & McCullough, 2003), studies that usedeither term were included in the analyses. Studies that reported onlyreligious affiliation or spiritual group membership were excluded becausemembership status alone cannot be reliably inferred to represent an indi-viduals beliefs, motivations, and behaviors (e.g., Richards & Bergin,1997).

    We defined symptoms of depression more narrowly, only includingstudies with measures that explicitly used the term depression and thatassessed a cluster of symptoms consonant with and specific to depressivedisorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Consistent with theview that depressive symptoms and depressive disorder reside on a singlecontinuum (see also Hankin & Abramson, 2001), studies were includedthat involved measures of depressive symptoms as well as those thatdiagnosed participants into depressed and not depressed categories on thebasis of psychiatric diagnoses or the use of cut-off scores. Studies that usedonly measures of global mental health or affect or measures specific to lifesatisfaction, loneliness, or suicidal ideation were excluded.

    Data CodingCoders received extensive training to increase the reliability of their

    efforts. To minimize the likelihood of rater bias, Method sections of studies

    were coded separately from the results (Owin, 1994; Sacks, Berrier, Reit-man, Ancova-Berk, & Chalmers, 1987). To decrease the likelihood ofhuman error in coding data, teams of two raters reviewed each article.Team members helped one another to verify the accuracy of coding anddata entry. Subsequently, each article was coded by a different team of tworaters (57%) or by Timothy B. Smith (43%). The coders were advancedpsychology students who coded the Method sections of the studies sepa-rately from the Results sections.

    Coders extracted several objectively verifiable characteristics of thestudies: (a) the field of inquiry (psychology, medicine, etc.) and outlet(journal article, dissertation, etc.); (b) the number of participants and theircomposition by gender, ethnicity, age, and religious affiliation if reported;(c) the location of data collection if reported (nursing home, universityclass, etc.); (d) the design type (cross-sectional, longitudinal, experimental,quasi-experimental, and archival, with archival studies using data collectedfrom extant records [archives] and not from recruited participants); and (e)the name of the measure or measures of religious involvement. Becausemost of the information listed above was extracted verbatim from thereports, interrater agreement was quite high for categorical variables (Co-hens .85.97 across variables, mean .90) and for continuousvariables (intraclass rs .65.99, M .89; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). Aftercomputation of interrater reliability coefficients, interrater discrepancieswere resolved by discussion and consensus.

    Coders also made inferences, on the basis of information in the reports,regarding three variables: participants average levels of stress, partici-pants average levels of depression, and the aspect of religiousness mea-sured in the study. The inferred average level of depression of the partic-ipants was coded as minimal, mild, or moderate. For each effect sizeinvolving a standard measure of depression, we determined the severity ofdepressive symptoms by comparing the mean depression score for thesample with published norms. When mean scores were not reported, codersexamined the descriptions of the study samples. If no pertinent informationwas provided, the sample was coded as having minimal levels of depres-sion. When descriptions of the sample yielded information suggesting ahigher rate of depressive symptoms than in normal populations, the samplewas coded as having a mild level of depression. Finally, if the descriptionsindicated that participants had been diagnosed with major depression, thestudy was coded as having a moderate level of depression.

    Similarly, the research participants average level of stress associatedwith life events was coded on the basis of descriptions of sample charac-teristics. If the authors of the studies reported no unusual circumstances orsample characteristics, the study was coded as having minimal life eventstress. Wherever any descriptions yielded pertinent information about thesample, these characteristics were compared with the ratings of perceivedlife event severity provided by Hobson et al. (1998), based on the originalwork by Holmes and Rahe (1967). The continuous ratings provided byHobson and colleagues were trichotomized in such a way that peopleundergoing life events that were of similar severity to being the victim ofa crime or going through a recent divorce were rated as undergoing severelife stress. People undergoing events similar to having major disagreementswith an employer or being involved in an auto accident were characteristicof the bottom two thirds of Hobson et al.s scale. People undergoing suchstressors were rated as experiencing mild to moderate life stress.

    If the dimension of religiousness measured in a study was not explicitlystated, it was inferred by the coders. Each measure was assigned to one ofthe following seven categories, which were similar to those used by Hilland Hood (1999) in their comprehensive review: (a) religious behaviors(measures of the frequency of saying prayers, participating in worshipservices, reading sacred texts, and/or performing religious service), (b)religious attitudes and beliefs (measures of the perceived importance ofreligion, belief in deity, religious values), (c) religious orientation (mea-sures of the motivation for religious behaviors, intrinsic and extrinsic; e.g.,the Religious Orientation Scale; Allport & Ross, 1967), (d) religiouscoping (measures of the use of positive and negative strategies for dealing

    617RELIGIOUSNESS AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS

  • with difficulties, such as seeking support from God vs. blaming God for theproblem; e.g., the Religious Coping Activities Scale; Pargament et al.,1990), (e) religious well-being (measures of life satisfaction in relation toreligious beliefs, such as personal feelings of a connection with God; e.g.,the Religious Well-Being subscale of the Spiritual Well-Being Scale;C. W. Ellison, 1983), (f) God concept (measures that assessed perceptionsof the extent to which one holds a positive or negative image of God), and(g) multidimensional aspects of religiousness (measures that included morethan one of the above content areas).

    In coding the three inferred variables, the two coding teams demon-strated adequate reliability (Cohens .85, .87, and .86 for inferreddepression, life stress, and type of religiousness, respectively ). As withother inferences and classifications, discrepancies were resolved by dis-cussion and consensus.

    Computation of Effect Size EstimatesThe majority (76%) of identified studies reported the association of

    religious involvement and depressive symptoms with the correlation coef-ficient. The data from other studies were transformed to correlation coef-ficients using the Meta-Analysis Calculator software (Lyons, 1996). Whenno statistic was provided but an analysis was reported as significant, wedetermined the correlation coefficient corresponding to the reported alphalevel (assuming two-tailed .05, unless reported otherwise). When ananalysis was reported as nonsignificant but no additional information wasavailable, we set the correlation coefficient to .00. These proceduresyielded conservative effect size estimates. For the purposes of aggregation,correlation coefficients reported in individual studies were transformedinto Fishers stabilizing z scores (Shadish & Haddock, 1994) to avoid thesmall amount of bias in Pearsons r estimates. The resulting average acrossall studies was then converted back to the metric of a correlation. Thedirection of all effect sizes was coded uniformly, such that positive valuesindicated that religiousness was associated with greater symptoms ofdepression and negative values indicated that religiousness was associatedwith fewer symptoms of depression.

    Several studies reported data on either multiple measures of religiousinvolvement or multiple measures of depression. For example, some stud-ies assessed attendance at religious services and frequency of personalprayer as well as aspects of religious belief or commitment. Use of each ofthese effect sizes in the omnibus analysis would have violated the assump-tion of independent samples (Cooper, 1998; Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Wetherefore used the shifting units of analysis approach to analysis, whichminimizes the threat of nonindependence in the data while allowing moredetailed follow-up analyses to be conducted (Cooper, 1998). Each singleeffect size estimate within each study was coded as if it were an indepen-dent event. However, in determining the overall association of religiousinvolvement and depression, the effect sizes within each study were aver-aged so that each study contributed only one data point to the omnibusanalysis (Mullen, 1989). This average effect size was used in subsequentanalyses except where more detailed information was required. In suchcases, the specific effect size representing the information needed was usedin the analysis rather than the average of all effect sizes extracted from thestudy.

    Analyses

    To aggregate effect sizes and to estimate the reliability of these aggre-gates, we calculated random effects models with Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (Borenstein, 2000). A random effects approach yieldsresults that generalize beyond the sample of studies actually reviewed(Hedges & Vevea, 1998). The assumptions made in this meta-analysisclearly warrant this method: The belief that certain variables serve asmoderators of the observed association between symptoms of depressionand religious involvement implies that the studies reviewed would estimate

    different population effect sizes. Random effects models take suchbetween-studies variation into account, whereas fixed effects models donot (Mosteller & Colditz, 1996).

    Following the computation of the overall association of symptoms ofdepression to religious involvement, random effects weighted regressionmodels and mixed effects analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conductedusing SPSS macros developed by Lipsey and Wilson (2001) to examine theinfluence of potential moderating variables. Such analyses are useful indetermining circumstances under which the association between symptomsof depression and religious involvement varies in strength, such that a moreaccurate depiction of the association is provided. To correct for the numberof analyses conducted in this study, we set the level of statistical signifi-cance at .0035.

    Results

    Descriptive Characteristics

    Effect sizes based on nonoverlapping samples of participantswere extracted from 147 studies. Table 1 provides an overview ofdescriptive information about these studies. Table 2 provides adescription and the effect size estimate for each individual study.Across all studies, data were reported from a total of 98,975participants. Participant gender was reported in 137 studies (93%),

    (text continues on page 623)

    Table 1Characteristics of Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis

    CharacteristicNo. of studies

    (k)

    Year of reportBefore 1990 2719901994 5419952000 66

    Field of inquiryPsychology 56Medicine 42Nursing 15Sociology 15Religious studies 13Other (e.g., education) 6

    Population sampledAdults from the general population 44College students 30Adults receiving medical care 23Adults with psychological concerns

    (e.g., outpatients, bereaved) 19Adults from religious settings 15Other (e.g., caregivers, adolescents) 16

    Sampling procedure usedConvenience 87Representative 28Unable to determine from report 32

    Sample size 50 1350100 36101250 392511,000 37 1,000 22

    DesignCross-sectional 125Longitudinal 15Other (e.g., archival, experimental) 7

    618 SMITH, MCCULLOUGH, AND POLL

  • Tabl

    e2

    Des

    crip

    tions

    oft

    he14

    7In

    divi

    dual

    Stud

    ies

    Incl

    uded

    inth

    eM

    eta-

    Anal

    ysis

    Stud

    yN

    Mea

    nag

    eSa

    mpl

    ety

    peSt

    ress

    full

    ifeev

    ents

    Dep

    ress

    ion

    leve

    lTy

    peofr

    elig

    ious

    mea

    sure

    Effe

    ctsiz

    e(r)

    95%

    CI

    Aus

    tin&

    Lenn

    ings

    (1993

    )57

    33G

    rievi

    ng/b

    erea

    ved

    adul

    tsM

    oder

    ate

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .03

    .29

    ,.24

    Bai

    rd(19

    90)

    164

    16H

    igh

    scho

    olst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    ildM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .17

    .32

    ,.02

    Bek

    arog

    luet

    al.(

    1991

    )29

    272

    Nur

    sing

    hom

    ere

    siden

    ts&

    oth

    erold

    erad

    ults

    Mild

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .31

    .41

    ,.21

    Bel

    avic

    h(19

    95)

    222

    19U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usco

    ping

    .09

    .22

    ,.04

    Ber

    gin

    etal

    .(19

    87)

    32(Y

    oung

    adul

    ts)R

    elig

    ious

    univ

    ersit

    yst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .02

    .34

    ,.36

    Bic

    kele

    tal

    .(19

    98)

    245

    53Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usco

    ping

    .00

    .13

    ,.13

    Bie

    nenf

    eld

    etal

    .(19

    97)

    89(S

    enior

    adul

    ts)R

    elig

    ious

    nurs

    ing

    hom

    ere

    siden

    tsM

    ildM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .40

    .58

    ,.22

    Big

    gare

    tal

    .(19

    99)

    205

    35H

    IV-p

    ositi

    vead

    ults

    &nonin

    fect

    edad

    ults

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.00

    .14

    ,.14

    Bish

    opet

    al.(

    1987

    )17

    3(A

    dults

    )Ps

    ychi

    atric

    inpa

    tient

    s&

    contr

    olad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    oder

    ate

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .16

    .31

    ,.02

    Bla

    ine

    &Cr

    ocke

    r(19

    95)

    146

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .06

    .22

    ,.11

    Blu

    mel

    (1993

    )12

    ,007

    (Adu

    lts)

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .06

    .08

    ,.04

    Boh

    rnste

    dtet

    al.(

    1968

    )3,

    666

    (You

    ngad

    ults)

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .18

    .22

    ,.15

    Boz

    arth

    (1997

    )43

    (Adu

    lts)

    HIV

    -pos

    itive

    adul

    tsSe

    vere

    Mod

    erat

    eR

    elig

    ious

    wel

    l-bei

    ng

    .10

    .40

    ,.20

    Bra

    am,B

    eekm

    an,D

    eeg,

    etal

    .(19

    97)

    177

    (Sen

    iorad

    ults)

    Seni

    orad

    ults

    Mild

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .30

    .43

    ,.16

    Bra

    am,B

    eekm

    an,v

    anTi

    lbur

    g,et

    al.(

    1997

    )2,

    817

    70A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .09

    .13

    ,.05

    Bro

    wn

    &G

    ary

    (1985

    )95

    (Adu

    lts)

    Une

    mpl

    oyed

    adul

    tsM

    ildM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .03

    .23

    ,.18

    Bro

    wn

    etal

    .(19

    90)

    438

    42A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .17

    .26

    ,.08

    Bro

    wn

    etal

    .(19

    92)

    927

    43A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .23

    .29

    ,.17

    Bro

    wn

    &G

    ary

    (1994

    )53

    743

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .15

    .23

    ,.06

    Buc

    hana

    n(19

    93)

    160

    74Ps

    ychi

    atric

    inpa

    tient

    s,co

    ntr

    olad

    ults,

    &m

    enta

    lhea

    lthoutp

    atie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    oder

    ate

    Rel

    igio

    usw

    ell-b

    eing

    .54

    .65

    ,.43

    Bur

    ris(19

    94)

    100

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .11

    .30

    ,.09

    Carro

    ll(19

    92)

    450

    56Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    God

    conce

    pt

    .06

    .14

    ,.03

    Chan

    cey

    (1997

    )34

    940

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.00

    .11

    ,.11

    Chan

    get

    al.(

    1998

    )12

    762

    Care

    give

    rsofa

    dult

    patie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng

    .09

    .26

    ,.08

    Clin

    e(19

    92)

    8073

    Nur

    sing

    hom

    ere

    siden

    tsM

    ildM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .21

    .41

    ,.02

    Coat

    es(19

    96)

    100

    38A

    buse

    vic

    tims

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .02

    .22

    ,.18

    Cole

    man

    (1996

    )11

    638

    HIV

    -pos

    itive

    adul

    tsSe

    vere

    Mod

    erat

    eR

    elig

    ious

    wel

    l-bei

    ng

    .21

    .38

    ,.03

    Com

    mer

    ford

    (1996

    )81

    81N

    ursin

    gho

    me

    resid

    ents

    Mild

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .05

    .27

    ,.17

    Corz

    o(19

    81)

    123

    36Ps

    ychi

    atric

    inpa

    tient

    s&

    contr

    olad

    ults

    Mild

    Mod

    erat

    eG

    odco

    nce

    pt.00

    .18

    ,.18

    Crow

    e(19

    90)

    389

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .07

    .03

    ,.17

    Dom

    anic

    o&

    Craw

    ford

    (2000

    )96

    37H

    IV-p

    ositi

    vead

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    wel

    l-bei

    ng

    .45

    .61

    ,.29

    Dox

    eyet

    al.(

    1997

    )4,

    920

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .10

    .13

    ,.07

    Duc

    harm

    e(19

    88)

    5739

    Psyc

    hiat

    ricin

    patie

    nts

    &co

    ntr

    olad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    oder

    ate

    God

    conce

    pt

    .29

    .53

    ,.05

    C.G

    .Elli

    son

    (1991

    )2,

    956

    43A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .02

    .02

    ,.05

    Faul

    kner

    (1994

    )2,

    834

    59A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .02

    .01

    ,.06

    Fehr

    ing

    etal

    .(19

    87)

    9519

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    wel

    l-bei

    ng

    .04

    .24

    ,.16

    619RELIGIOUSNESS AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS

    (tabl

    eco

    ntin

    ues)

  • Stud

    yN

    Mea

    nag

    eSa

    mpl

    ety

    peSt

    ress

    full

    ifeev

    ents

    Dep

    ress

    ion

    leve

    lTy

    peofr

    elig

    ious

    mea

    sure

    Effe

    ctsiz

    e(r)

    95%

    CI

    Fehr

    ing

    etal

    .(19

    87)

    7520

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .25

    .46

    ,.04

    Fehr

    ing

    etal

    .(19

    97)

    100

    73Te

    rmin

    ally

    illad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .44

    .59

    ,.28

    Fern

    ando

    (1978

    )87

    (Adu

    lts)

    Med

    ical

    inpa

    tient

    sSe

    vere

    Mod

    erat

    eR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .25

    .44

    ,.05

    Fior

    i(19

    91)

    722

    16H

    igh

    scho

    olst

    uden

    tsM

    ildM

    ildG

    odco

    nce

    pt

    .31

    .38

    ,.24

    Fitc

    hett

    etal

    .(19

    99)

    9565

    Med

    ical

    inpa

    tient

    sSe

    vere

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .01

    .21

    ,.19

    Gen

    ia&

    Shaw

    (1991

    )30

    929

    Chur

    chm

    embe

    rsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .02

    .09

    ,.13

    Gra

    y(19

    87)

    5016

    Grie

    ving

    /ber

    eave

    dad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .30

    .55

    ,.04

    Grif

    fin(19

    88)

    171

    (Ado

    lesce

    nts)

    Hig

    hsc

    hool

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es.00

    .15

    ,.15

    Gro

    sse-

    Hot

    forth

    etal

    .(19

    96)

    9769

    Med

    ical

    inpa

    tient

    sM

    oder

    ate

    Mild

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .25

    .43

    ,.06

    Gup

    ta(19

    83)

    313

    17H

    igh

    scho

    olst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .24

    .13

    ,.34

    Hal

    lstro

    m&

    Pers

    son

    (1984

    )45

    347

    Men

    talh

    ealth

    outp

    atie

    nts

    &co

    ntr

    olad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    oder

    ate

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .07

    .16

    ,.02

    Her

    tsga

    ard

    &Li

    ght

    (1984

    )76

    044

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .13

    .20

    ,.06

    Het

    tler&

    Cohe

    n(19

    98)

    124

    48Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .01

    .18

    ,.17

    Hin

    tikka

    etal

    .(19

    98)

    1,17

    941

    Men

    talh

    ealth

    outp

    atie

    nts

    Mild

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .11

    .17

    ,.06

    Hoh

    man

    net

    al.(

    1990

    )65

    3(A

    dults

    )A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usw

    ell-b

    eing

    .11

    .03

    ,.19

    Hon

    g&

    Gia

    nnak

    opou

    los

    (1994

    )1,

    722

    25A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .06

    .10

    ,.01

    Hon

    g&

    May

    o(19

    88)

    208

    (You

    ngad

    ults)

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .04

    .09

    ,.18

    Hus

    aini

    etal

    .(19

    99)

    995

    72A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .15

    .21

    ,.08

    Hyl

    and

    (1996

    )60

    81N

    ursin

    gho

    me

    resid

    ents

    Mild

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .19

    .42

    ,.07

    Idle

    r(19

    87)

    2,75

    674

    Old

    erad

    ults

    Min

    imal

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .13

    .17

    ,.09

    Idle

    r&K

    asl(

    1992

    )1,

    447

    (Olde

    radu

    lts)

    Old

    erad

    ults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .04

    .09

    ,.01

    Jens

    enet

    al.(

    1993

    )3,

    835

    21U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .06

    .09

    ,.03

    Ken

    dler

    etal

    .(19

    97)

    1,86

    030

    Twin

    sM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .04

    .08

    ,.01

    Ken

    nedy

    etal

    .(19

    96)

    1,85

    575

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .07

    .12

    ,.03

    Kiv

    ela

    etal

    .(19

    96)

    679

    74A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .35

    .42

    ,.29

    Kla

    as(19

    98)

    7782

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .11

    .33

    ,.11

    Koe

    nig

    (1995

    )96

    57Pr

    isone

    rsSe

    vere

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .17

    .37

    ,.02

    Koe

    nig

    etal

    .(19

    88)

    9374

    Med

    ical

    outp

    atie

    nts

    Min

    imal

    Mild

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .20

    .40

    ,.01

    Koe

    nig

    etal

    .(19

    92)

    841

    70M

    edic

    alin

    patie

    nts

    Seve

    reM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng

    .16

    .23

    ,.09

    Koe

    nig

    etal

    .(19

    92)

    202

    (Sen

    iorad

    ults)

    Med

    ical

    inpa

    tient

    sSe

    vere

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usco

    ping

    .24

    .37

    ,.11

    Koe

    nig

    etal

    .(19

    97)

    4,00

    073

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .07

    .10

    ,.04

    Koe

    nig,

    Geo

    rge,

    &Pe

    ters

    on(19

    98)

    8767

    Med

    ical

    inpa

    tient

    sM

    oder

    ate

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .11

    .32

    ,.10

    Koe

    nig,

    Parg

    amen

    t,&

    Nie

    lsen

    (1998

    )57

    768

    Med

    ical

    inpa

    tient

    sSe

    vere

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.06

    .02

    ,.14

    Koh

    bod

    (1996

    )78

    41A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usw

    ell-b

    eing

    .35

    .55

    ,.16

    Kot

    eske

    yet

    al.(

    1991

    )10

    919

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .29

    .46

    ,.12

    Levi

    net

    al.(

    1996

    )61

    755

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .02

    .10

    ,.05

    Lind

    gren

    &Co

    urse

    y(19

    95)

    3041

    Men

    talh

    ealth

    outp

    atie

    nts

    Seve

    reM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .42

    .72

    ,.12

    Littr

    ell&

    Bec

    k(19

    99)

    9138

    Hom

    eles

    sad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng.00

    .21

    ,.21

    Mae

    set

    al.(

    1998

    )2,

    844

    48A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .06

    .10

    ,.02

    Mal

    tby

    &D

    ay(20

    00)

    360

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .11

    .01

    ,.21

    Mal

    tby

    etal

    .(19

    99)

    474

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .00

    .09

    ,.09

    Mar

    tin(19

    94)

    100

    76M

    edic

    aloutp

    atie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .14

    .33

    ,.05

    Mat

    on(19

    89)

    8146

    Grie

    ving

    /ber

    eave

    dad

    ults

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng

    .23

    .44

    ,.02

    620 SMITH, MCCULLOUGH, AND POLLTa

    ble

    2(co

    ntin

    ued)

  • Stud

    yN

    Mea

    nag

    eSa

    mpl

    ety

    peSt

    ress

    full

    ifeev

    ents

    Dep

    ress

    ion

    leve

    lTy

    peofr

    elig

    ious

    mea

    sure

    Effe

    ctsiz

    e(r)

    95%

    CI

    May

    oet

    al.(

    1969

    )16

    5(Y

    oung

    adul

    ts)U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .19

    .33

    ,.04

    McD

    ouga

    ll(19

    98)

    169

    68A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usco

    ping

    .08

    .23

    ,.07

    McI

    ntos

    h&

    Dan

    igel

    is(19

    95)

    1,64

    4(S

    enior

    adul

    ts)A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .30

    .34

    ,.26

    Mic

    kley

    etal

    .(19

    98)

    9257

    Care

    give

    rsofa

    dult

    patie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .16

    .35

    ,.05

    Mill

    eret

    al.(

    1997

    )60

    55A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .33

    .56

    ,.11

    Miro

    la(19

    90)

    700

    (Adu

    lts)

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .13

    .20

    ,.06

    Mol

    enka

    mp

    (1993

    )39

    445

    Chur

    chm

    embe

    rsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .12

    .22

    ,.02

    Mor

    se&

    Wiso

    cki(

    1987

    )14

    872

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .23

    .38

    ,.08

    Mur

    phy

    etal

    .(20

    00)

    271

    (Adu

    lts)

    Psyc

    hiat

    ricin

    patie

    nts

    &m

    enta

    lhea

    lthoutp

    atie

    nts

    Seve

    reM

    oder

    ate

    Rel

    igio

    usw

    ell-b

    eing

    .32

    .42

    ,.21

    Mus

    ick

    etal

    .(20

    00)

    1,89

    773

    Chur

    chm

    embe

    rsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s.01

    .04

    ,.05

    Mus

    ick,

    Koe

    nig,

    etal

    .(19

    98)

    235

    73A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .03

    .16

    ,.10

    Mus

    ick

    &St

    rulo

    witz

    (2000

    )8,

    866

    42A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .04

    .06

    ,.02

    Mus

    ick,

    Will

    iam

    s,&

    Jack

    son

    (1998

    )58

    443

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .04

    .12

    ,.04

    Nee

    lman

    &Le

    wis

    (1994

    )51

    38Ps

    ychi

    atric

    inpa

    tient

    s,co

    ntr

    olad

    ults,

    &m

    enta

    lhea

    lthoutp

    atie

    nts

    Seve

    reM

    oder

    ate

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .16

    .12

    ,.43

    Nef

    f&H

    oppe

    (1993

    )1,

    784

    38A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .13

    .18

    ,.08

    Nel

    son

    (1989

    a)68

    72A

    dults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .16

    .39

    ,.08

    Nel

    son

    (1989

    b)26

    81N

    ursin

    gho

    me

    resid

    ents

    Mild

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usbe

    havi

    ors

    .39

    .72

    ,.06

    OCo

    nnor

    &V

    alle

    rand

    (1990

    )14

    682

    Nur

    sing

    hom

    ere

    siden

    tsM

    ildM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .08

    .09

    ,.24

    OLa

    oire

    (1997

    )88

    (Adu

    lts)

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .36

    .54

    ,.18

    Ole

    r(19

    97)

    273

    42Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .04

    .08

    ,.16

    Olsz

    ewsk

    i(19

    94)

    9514

    Chur

    chm

    embe

    rsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng.01

    .20

    ,.21

    Palin

    kas

    etal

    .(19

    90)

    1,61

    575

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .07

    .12

    ,.02

    C.Pa

    rket

    al.(

    1990

    )St

    udy

    183

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .01

    .22

    ,.21

    C.Pa

    rket

    al.(

    1990

    )St

    udy

    283

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .05

    .26

    ,.17

    H.S

    .Par

    ket

    al.(

    1998

    )95

    35Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .11

    .30

    ,.10

    Pato

    ck-P

    eckh

    amet

    al.

    (1998

    )26

    320

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .03

    .09

    ,.15

    Plan

    te&

    Boc

    cacc

    ini

    (1997

    )10

    219

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .16

    .35

    ,.03

    Pres

    sman

    etal

    .(19

    90)

    30(S

    enior

    adul

    ts)M

    edic

    alin

    patie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .30

    .63

    ,.03

    Quinn

    etal

    .(19

    96)

    8172

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .21

    .42

    ,.00

    Rat

    anas

    iripo

    ng(19

    96)

    244

    41Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es.12

    .00

    ,.24

    Ren

    froe

    (1990

    )25

    31M

    enta

    lhea

    lthoutp

    atie

    nts

    Mild

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usw

    ell-b

    eing

    .21

    .59

    ,.17

    Ric

    hard

    s(19

    91)

    268

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .07

    .05

    ,.19

    Rob

    inso

    n&

    Kay

    e(19

    94)

    1765

    Care

    give

    rsofa

    dult

    patie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ildM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .21

    .68

    ,.26

    Rob

    inso

    n&

    Kay

    e(19

    94)

    2362

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.05

    .37

    ,.47

    Ros

    ik(19

    89)

    159

    64G

    rievi

    ng/b

    erea

    ved

    adul

    tsM

    oder

    ate

    Mild

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .16

    .01

    ,.31

    Ros

    s(19

    90)

    401

    (Adu

    lts)

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .12

    .22

    ,.02

    621RELIGIOUSNESS AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMSTa

    ble

    2(co

    ntin

    ued)

    (tabl

    eco

    ntin

    ues)

  • Stud

    yN

    Mea

    nag

    eSa

    mpl

    ety

    peSt

    ress

    full

    ifeev

    ents

    Dep

    ress

    ion

    leve

    lTy

    peofr

    elig

    ious

    mea

    sure

    Effe

    ctsiz

    e(r)

    95%

    CI

    Row

    ley

    (1998

    )23

    038

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.00

    .13

    ,.13

    Rya

    net

    al.(

    1993

    )15

    123

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    orie

    ntat

    ion

    .12

    .17

    ,.05

    Rya

    net

    al.(

    1993

    )42

    35Ch

    urch

    mem

    bers

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .06

    .36

    ,.25

    Scho

    enba

    chet

    al.(

    1982

    )13

    6(A

    doles

    cents

    )H

    igh

    scho

    olst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .18

    .01

    ,.34

    Sher

    kat&

    Ree

    d(19

    92)

    156

    (Adu

    lts)

    Grie

    ving

    /ber

    eave

    dad

    ults

    Seve

    reM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .06

    .21

    ,.10

    Shul

    eret

    al.(

    1994

    )50

    30H

    omel

    ess

    adul

    tsSe

    vere

    Mod

    erat

    eR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng

    .29

    .54

    ,.03

    Sim

    on(19

    92)

    5084

    Nur

    sing

    hom

    ere

    siden

    tsM

    ildM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .14

    .41

    ,.14

    Snyd

    er(19

    96)

    8542

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.15

    .06

    ,.36

    Sore

    nson

    etal

    .(19

    95)

    261

    (Ado

    lesce

    nts)

    Teen

    age

    moth

    ers

    Seve

    reM

    ildM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l.09

    .04

    ,.21

    Stav

    ros

    (1998

    )88

    (Adu

    lts)

    Chur

    chm

    embe

    rsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .29

    .48

    ,.09

    Stee

    r&Sc

    hut(

    1979

    )20

    0(A

    dults

    )Su

    bsta

    nce

    abus

    ers

    Seve

    reM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .09

    .23

    ,.05

    Stra

    wbr

    idge

    etal

    .(19

    98)

    2,53

    765

    Adu

    ltsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .04

    .08

    ,.00

    Stra

    yhor

    net

    al.(

    1990

    )27

    2(A

    dults

    )A

    dults

    Mild

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .12

    .24

    ,.01

    Teel

    (1993

    )11

    347

    Grie

    ving

    /ber

    eave

    dad

    ults

    &ca

    regi

    vers

    ofa

    dult

    patie

    nts

    Mild

    Mild

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .00

    .19

    ,.19

    Thea

    rleet

    al.(

    1995

    )63

    1(A

    dults

    )G

    rievi

    ng/b

    erea

    ved

    adul

    ts&

    oth

    erad

    ults

    Mild

    Mod

    erat

    eR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s

    .09

    .16

    ,.01

    Tren

    tet

    al.(

    1983

    )50

    21U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es

    .42

    .65

    ,.19

    Vai

    llant

    etal

    .(19

    98)

    223

    50N

    orm

    alad

    ults

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usat

    titud

    es.11

    .02

    ,.24

    Van

    deCr

    eek

    etal

    .(19

    95)

    150

    43Ca

    regi

    vers

    ofa

    dult

    patie

    nts

    Seve

    reM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    copi

    ng

    .23

    .38

    ,.08

    Virg

    inia

    (1998

    )14

    2(A

    dults

    )R

    elig

    ious

    lead

    ers

    (clerg

    y)M

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    beha

    vior

    s.06

    .11

    ,.22

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    89a)

    221

    19U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .07

    .20

    ,.06

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    89b)

    1,39

    720

    Uni

    vers

    ityst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alM

    ultid

    imen

    siona

    l

    .08

    .14

    ,.03

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    90)

    203

    21U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usco

    ping

    .27

    .40

    ,.14

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    92)

    307

    21U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .08

    .19

    ,.04

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    93)

    331

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .02

    .09

    ,.12

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    94)

    237

    21U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .05

    .18

    ,.08

    Wat

    son

    etal

    .(19

    94)

    351

    20U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .16

    .27

    ,.06

    H.P

    .Wils

    on(19

    94)

    118

    40A

    buse

    vic

    tims

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    oder

    ate

    Rel

    igio

    usw

    ell-b

    eing

    .20

    .37

    ,.03

    Woo

    dset

    al.(

    1999

    )10

    635

    HIV

    -pos

    itive

    adul

    tsSe

    vere

    Mild

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .24

    .42

    ,.05

    Wrig

    htet

    al.(

    1993

    )45

    1(A

    doles

    cents

    )H

    igh

    scho

    olst

    uden

    tsM

    inim

    alM

    inim

    alR

    elig

    ious

    wel

    l-bei

    ng

    .15

    .24

    ,.06

    Wya

    ttet

    al.(

    1999

    )69

    972

    Term

    inal

    lyill

    adul

    tsSe

    vere

    Min

    imal

    Rel

    igio

    usorie

    ntat

    ion

    .15

    .22

    ,.08

    Zhan

    g&

    Jin(19

    96)

    772

    21U

    nive

    rsity

    stud

    ents

    Min

    imal

    Min

    imal

    Mul

    tidim

    ensio

    nal

    .01

    .08

    ,.06

    Zunz

    uneg

    uiet

    al.(

    1999

    )19

    466

    Care

    give

    rsofa

    dult

    patie

    nts

    Mod

    erat

    eM

    ildR

    elig

    ious

    attit

    udes

    .06

    .09

    ,.20

    Note

    .CI

    confid

    ence

    inte

    rval

    .

    622 SMITH, MCCULLOUGH, AND POLLTa

    ble

    2(co

    ntin

    ued)

  • with 61% of the total participants being female. Participant eth-nicity was reported in 95 studies (65%), with a breakdown of 53%European Americans, 24% African Americans, 12% NorthernEuropeans, 5% Hispanic/Latino Americans, 1% Asian Americans,1% other Americans (e.g., Native Americans), and 4% participantsfrom other nations (e.g., Australia, China, India). Religious affil-iation was reported in 45 studies (31%), with 49% Protestants,30% Catholics, 17% unspecified or unaffiliated, 3% Jewish, andless than 1% others (e.g., Muslim).

    Descriptions of participant characteristics resulted in 90 (61%)studies being coded as having participants with minimal life eventstress, 32 (22%) having participants with mild to moderate stress,and 25 (17%) having participants with severe stress. Mean scoreson measures of depressive symptoms as well as descriptions ofparticipant characteristics resulted in 104 (71%) studies beingcoded as having participants with minimal symptoms of depres-sion, 25 (17%) studies having participants with mild symptoms,and 18 (12%) studies having participants with moderate symp-toms. Descriptions of instrumentation indicated that 35% of thestudies used multidimensional measures of religiousness, 20%used measures of religious behaviors, 12% used measures ofreligious attitudes and beliefs, 15% used measures of religiousorientation, 8% used measures of religious coping, 7% used mea-sures of religious well-being, and 3% used measures of Godconcept.

    Omnibus Analysis

    As indicated previously, each study contributed only one datapoint in the omnibus analysis. Across all 147 studies, the randomeffects weighted average effect size was .096 (SE .009, p .000001, 95% confidence interval .11, .08). Of 140 nonzeroeffect sizes, 113 (81%) were negative and 27 (19%) were positive,ranging from .54 to .24. The variability of effect sizes was quitehigh, and the index of heterogeneity was statistically significant,Q(146) 814.00, p .001, suggesting that systematic effect sizevariability was unaccounted for. We therefore conducted addi-tional analyses to determine the extent to which the variability inthe magnitude of the effect size was moderated by other variables.

    A visual display of the effect sizes (x axis) by the number ofparticipants per study (logarithmic y axis) is presented in Figure 1.The scatterplot demonstrates a funnel-shaped pattern around themean effect size, with the exception that extreme positive andextreme negative effect size estimates may have been underrepre-sented in studies with small sample sizes. Because studies with lowsample sizes tend to remain unpublished, examination of potentialpublication bias was necessary (Rosenthal, 1979).

    Assessment of Publication BiasPublication bias, sometimes called the file drawer effect

    (Rosenthal, 1979), is a tendency for studies on a given topic withlarge (and/or statistically significant) effect size estimates to havea higher likelihood of being published than do studies that yieldsmaller (and/or statistically nonsignificant) effect size estimates.Because published studieswhich tend to possess strongest effectsize estimatesare easier for meta-analysts to obtain than areunpublished studies, the effect size estimates resulting from ameta-analysis can be biased against the null hypothesis.

    To examine the extent to which our results might be biased bythe file drawer effect, we first compared the mean weighted effectsize from unpublished reports to the mean weighted effect sizefrom published reports. As seen in Table 3, studies that werepublished in journals (weighted mean r .096) yielded slightlylarger effect sizes than did studies that were unpublished (viz.,conference presentations, theses, and dissertations; weighted meanr .087). A mixed effects ANOVA conducted between pub-lished and unpublished studies revealed no statistically significantdifference (QB 0.15, p .70). Thus, although we found thatunpublished studies may yield slightly smaller estimates of thereligiousnessdepressive symptoms association than do publishedones, the amount of bias appears to be trivial (i.e., less than 1correlation point) and is statistically nonsignificant. Calculation ofa fail-safe N (Begg, 1994) showed that 4,128 studies averaging aneffect size of .00 would be required to render the present omnibuseffect size estimate statistically nonsignificant.

    Because of limitations of the fail-safe N and similar approaches,nonparametric methods based on the funnel plot distribution (Fig-ure 1) have recently been developed to test and adjust for possiblepublication bias (Sutton et al., 2000). Duval and Tweedie (2000a,2000b) described the trim and fill method of estimating thenumber of missing studies due to publication bias and recalculat-ing the weighted mean effect size accordingly. In an iterativeprocess, outlying studies that have no corresponding values on theopposite side of the funnel plot distribution are temporarily re-moved (trimmed), and the mean effect size is recalculated,repeating the procedure until the distribution is symmetrical withrespect to the mean. In our analyses, we followed the recommen-dations of Duval and Tweedie (2000b) in using L0 to estimate thenumber of missing studies using formulae provided by Jennionsand Moller (2002). The final step in the procedure is to replace thetrimmed studies along with filled estimated values of the missingstudies on the other side of the funnel plot distribution. The valuesfor the filled studies are exactly opposite those trimmed. Theresulting data set inclusive of filled missing studies is then used to

    Figure 1. Plot of effect sizes (Pearsons r) as a function of sample size(log).

    623RELIGIOUSNESS AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS

  • calculate a new omnibus effect size and its confidence intervals,with statistically nonsignificant values indicating potential publi-cation bias. In the present study, the recalculated random effectsweighted mean effect size was .088 ( p .00001, 95% confi-dence interval .10, .07). Based on these analyses, publica-tion bias seems an unlikely threat to the results presented here.

    Moderation by Sociodemographic VariablesTo examine whether the association of religiousness and depres-

    sive symptoms varied as a function of various sociodemographicvariables, we conducted a series of categorical (and where appro-priate, continuous) moderator analyses, using the data from all 147studies (see Table 3). The amount of between-studies variance thatis accounted for by an individual moderator variable is indexed bythe QB statistic. Statistically significant QB values for an individualmoderator variable indicate that the mean effect sizes differ acrossvarious levels of the moderator variable. The amount of remaining

    variance within studies is indexed by the QW statistic, for whichsignificant values indicate that the remaining variance is greaterthan zero.

    To determine whether differences in the gender composition ofthe samples account for significant between-studies variance, wefirst correlated the percentage of females from the 137 studies thatreported participant gender with the corresponding average effectsize. This random effects weighted correlation was .001 ( p .98).We then analyzed effect sizes calculated with nonoverlappingfemale only or male only samples. As can be seen in Table 3, thisbreakdown by gender did not account for substantial between-studies variation (QB 0.01, p .98). To hold other variations inmethod constant, we then compared effect sizes from 18 studiesthat reported data separately for both men and women. However,the differences observed between the estimates using female par-ticipants (weighted mean r .134) and those using male partic-ipants (weighted mean r .138) were not statistically significant

    Table 3Weighted Mean Correlations (r) Across Levels of Several Moderator Variables, 95% Confidence Intervals, and Amount of Between-Groups Variance Accounted For

    Variable QB p N k r 95% CI QW p

    Data source 0.15 .70Published 66,299 111 .096 .12, .08 156.0 .003Unpublished 32,676 36 .087 .12, .05 58.8 .007

    Gender 0.01 .98Female 19,848 37 .123 .16, .08 45.9 .13Male 10,127 28 .122 .17, .08 40.3 .05

    Ethnicity 0.51 .78African American 5,632 16 .108 .18, .04 13.8 .54European American 7,017 17 .073 .15, .01 15.6 .48European (Northern) 7,051 10 .105 .19, .02 10.0 .35

    Age group 18.90 .009Adolescence (1318) 6,029 10 .067 .13, .01 29.3 .001College age (1924) 15,949 29 .065 .10, .03 27.4 .50Early adulthood (2535) 5,662 14 .092 .15, .03 9.7 .72Middle age (3645) 35,751 37 .098 .13, .06 53.7 .03Late middle age (4655) 4,299 9 .050 .12, .02 7.9 .44Late adulthood (5665) 6,621 11 .023 .09, .04 6.6 .76Retirement age (6675) 24,124 30 .155 .19, .12 54.6 .003Elderly adults (76) 540 7 .093 .20, .01 4.6 .60

    Inferred life event stress 15.90 .0004Minimal 88,139 90 .071 .09, .05 116.1 .03Mild to moderate 5,785 32 .141 .18, .10 50.7 .01Severe 5,051 25 .152 .20, .11 38.5 .03

    Inferred depression 10.00 .007Minimal 87,357 104 .078 .10, .06 134.3 .02Mild 5,748 25 .135 .18, .09 43.3 .01Moderate 5,870 18 .151 .21, .10 31.1 .02

    Measure of religiousness 170.50 .0001Religious behaviors 43,572 30 .124 .16, .09 34.2 .23Religious attitudes and beliefs 16,725 18 .053 .10, .01 35.4 .006Religious orientation

    Intrinsic 4,445 22 .175 .22, .13 53.8 .0001Extrinsic 6,361 23 .155 .11, .20 30.2 .11

    Religious copingPositive 2,274 11 .167 .24, .10 7.8 .65Negative 1,999 8 .136 .06, .21 8.4 .30

    Religious well-being 2,106 11 .197 .27, .12 24.5 .006God concept 1,352 4 .199 .31, .09 4.4 .22Multidimensional 28,345 51 .095 .13, .06 37.4 .91

    Note. QB variance between groups; k number of studies; CI confidence interval; QW variance within groups.

    624 SMITH, MCCULLOUGH, AND POLL

  • (QB 0.01, p .93). Therefore, the religiousnessdepressivesymptoms association does not appear to be moderated by gender.

    To determine whether differences in the ethnic composition ofthe sample accounts for significant between-studies variance, wecorrelated the percentage of European Americans in the 95 studiesthat reported ethnicity with the corresponding average effect size.This random effects weighted correlation was .05 ( p .60). Wethen analyzed effect sizes calculated exclusively with samples of aspecific ethnic group. These categorical analyses were only pos-sible for contrasting the mean effect sizes for samples of AfricanAmericans, European Americans, and Northern Europeans be-cause fewer than three articles reported data specific to any otherethnic group. As may be seen in Table 3, this ethnic breakdown didnot account for significant between-studies variation in effect sizes(QB 0.51, p .78). To hold other variations in method constant,we then compared effect sizes from six studies that reported dataseparately for African American and European American partici-pants (no other ethnic groups were represented by separate datawithin studies). The differences observed between the estimatesusing African American participants (weighted mean r .132)and those using European American participants (weighted meanr .114) were not statistically significant (QB 0.02, p .88).Thus the relationship between religiousness and depressive symp-toms does not appear to be moderated by ethnicity.

    To investigate the potential effect of participant age upon therelationship between religiousness and depressive symptoms, weconducted a random effects weighted correlation with the meanage of the sample and the effect size of the study, with the resultingcoefficient being .19 ( p .007). To further explore this relation-ship, we broke down participants average age into eight catego-ries. As can be seen in Table 3, the magnitude of the association ofreligiousness and depressive symptoms was highest at retirementage. However, the mixed effects ANOVA conducted between theage groups failed to reach the p .0035 level of statisticalsignificance (QB 18.90, p .009). Fewer than three studiesreported data broken down across equivalent age groups, makingit impossible to conduct more detailed analyses within studies.Nevertheless, the association of religiousness and depressivesymptoms does not appear to be moderated by age.

    Main Effect Versus Stress-Buffering EffectsIf religiousness buffers people against the effects that stressful

    life events can have on depression, then the association of reli-giousness and depression should be stronger at high levels of stressthan at low levels of stress (S. Cohen & Wills, 1985). If religious-ness exerts a main effect on depression, then the association ofreligiousness and depressive symptoms should be significant at alllevels of life stress. Of course, if both the buffering model and themain effect model are accurate, then both sets of conditions mayhold. We evaluated both hypotheses by inspecting the differencesin mean effect sizes obtained from samples with differing degreesof life events stress and with differing degrees of depressivesymptoms (which themselves are a source of significant lifestress).

    The inferred amount of life event stress attributed to eachsample accounted for substantial between-studies variation(QB 15.90, p .0004). The religiousnessdepressive symptomsassociation for people with minimal life stress (weighted mean r

    .071) was smaller in magnitude than was the association forpeople with mild to moderate (weighted mean r .141) or severelife stress (weighted mean r .152). The random effectsweighted correlation between rated life stress and mean effect sizewas .26, which was statistically significant ( p .0001). Thus,higher rated life stress was associated with stronger negativecorrelation of religiousness and depressive symptoms. These re-sults suggest that the negative association of religiousness anddepressive symptoms is at its strongest when people are undergo-ing stressful life events, which is consistent with the bufferinghypothesis. However, at all levels of inferred life stress the asso-ciation of religiousness and depressive symptoms was significantand negative, which is also consistent with the main effect hypoth-esis. Thus, our results provide support for both models of thereligiousnessdepressive symptoms association.

    The degree of depressive symptoms attributed to each samplewas also investigated. The association among studies in which themean level of depression was minimal (weighted mean r .078)was smaller in magnitude than for samples that were judged ashaving a mild (weighted mean r .135) or moderate (weightedmean r .151) degree of depressive symptoms, but these dif-ferences were not statistically significant (QB 10.00, p .007)by the stringent criterion that we adopted for statistical signifi-cance ( p .0035). Thus, the rated level of depression did notmoderate the association between religiousness and symptoms ofdepression.

    Insufficient numbers of studies reported data for nonoverlappingsamples with differing levels of stress and/or with differing levelsof depression. We were therefore unable to perform additionalanalyses to confirm these findings within studies. However, as anadditional step complementary to the above analyses, we providehere a narrative summary of the findings from the studies thatexplicitly assessed the influence of level of depression and distresson the religiousnessdepression association.

    Only one study was identified that specifically assessed thereligiousnessdepression association across different levels of de-pression. In a survey of older adults, Klaas (1998) found that thecorrelation between religiousness and symptoms of depression was.29 among participants who were depressed compared with .07among participants who were not depressed.

    Eight studies were identified that assessed the religiousnessdepression association as a function of life stress. In a survey ofbereaved parents, Maton (1989) reported that the correlation be-tween religiousness and depression was .33 among participantswho had experienced the recent death of a child (a group identifiedas experiencing high distress) compared with .16 among parentswho had lost a child more than 2 years previously (a groupidentified as experiencing lower levels of distress). Similarly,Robinson and Kaye (1994) found a correlation of .21 betweenreligiousness and depressive symptoms in a sample of caregiversof dementia patients compared with a correlation of .05 among asample of partners of healthy adults. Bickel et al. (1998) dividedparticipants into high- and low-stress groups based on scores froma measure of perceived stress and found that correlations amonghigh-stress participants averaged .27 whereas correlations amonglow-stress participants averaged zero. In two separate studies, C.Park, Cohen, and Herb (1990) found interactions between reli-giousness and life stress in the prediction of depression, withreligious coping buffering Catholics depression at high levels of

    625RELIGIOUSNESS AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS

  • controllable life stress and with intrinsic religious orientation buff-ering Protestants depression at high levels of uncontrollable lifestress. Kendler et al. (1997) also found that religious devotionbuffered the relationship between recent stressful life events anddepression. Similarly, Hettler and Cohen (1998) reported a signif-icant interaction effect between intrinsic religiousness and thenumber of negative life events in predicting symptoms of depres-sion 8 months later, with the positive relationship between nega-tive life events and depression being weaker at higher levels ofintrinsic religiousness compared with moderate and low levels ofintrinsic religiousness. However, although Strawbridge, Shema,Cohen, Roberts, and Kaplan (1998) reported that both organiza-tional and nonorganizational religiousness buffered nonfamilystress (medical problems, financial problems, etc.), a finding rep-licated by those results reported above, they also found that in thepresence of family distress organizational religiousness (but notnonorganizational religiousness) was positively correlated withdepressionthe opposite direction of their other results and theresults of the studies cited previously. Similarly, although Belavich(1995) reported that religious coping activities predicted adjust-ment to daily hassles and stressful life events when controlling fornonreligious coping, there were no interaction effects between thenumber of daily hassles and religious coping when predictingdepression. Finally, when dividing participants into three catego-ries of church attendance (weekly, less often, and never), Thearle,Vance, Najman, Embelton, and Foster (1995) found little differ-ence in the magnitude of the religiousnessdepression associationamong bereaved parents (r .10) compared with a control grou