the effects of group membership on judgments of …

57
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF MORAL VIOLATIONS BY STEPHANIE ELIZABETH MCKEE A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of WAKE FOREST UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF ARTS Psychology December, 2015 Winston-Salem, North Carolina Approved By: E. J. Masicampo, Ph.D., Advisor Lara K. Kammrath, Ph.D., Chair Eranda Jayawickreme, Ph.D. Adam Kadlac, Ph.D.

Upload: others

Post on 28-Jan-2022

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON

JUDGMENTS OF MORAL VIOLATIONS

BY

STEPHANIE ELIZABETH MCKEE

A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of

WAKE FOREST UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree of

MASTER OF ARTS

Psychology

December, 2015

Winston-Salem, North Carolina

Approved By:

E. J. Masicampo, Ph.D., Advisor

Lara K. Kammrath, Ph.D., Chair

Eranda Jayawickreme, Ph.D.

Adam Kadlac, Ph.D.

Page 2: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

ii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

First, I would like to take this opportunity to acknowledge and thank my advisor,

Dr. E. J. Masicampo, who has provided valuable knowledge and insight into both

research and beer. Without your support and guidance over the last two years, I do not

know where I would be today. Your passion for research is infectious and I value the

time I spent learning under you.

Next I would like to thank my committee members for taking the time to read and

consider my work. Specifically I would like to thank Dr. Adam Kadlac for agreeing to

join my committee and provide an outside prospective on my research. Next, I thank Dr.

Eranda Jayawickreme for coming through at the last minute to make my defense possible.

Additional thanks goes to Dr. Cathy Seta who helped inform and shape my thesis

research. This project would not have been the same without your guidance. Lastly, I

want to thank Dr. Lara Kammrath for constantly being a source of support for not only

research, but teaching as well. I cannot begin to explain how much I have learned from

you in the past two years. Thank you for all you have done.

Further, I would like to thank Dr. Heather Smith for all of her guidance over the

last 7 years. She allowed a naïve undergrad to join her lab, and hasn’t stopped supporting

and mentoring me since. She instilled in me a passion for research, and my only hope is

to one day make her proud.

Lastly I want to thank my family, and the friends that have become family. I

thank Teresa Hill, Janice Jennings, and Debbie Best for always having my back and

providing a safe place to land. I thank my Mother for listening to me both rant about my

thesis and excitedly explain new research ideas all the while having no idea what I was

Page 3: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

iii

talking about. Finally, I can never express enough thanks to my amazing friends who

were there for every high and low I experienced during the last two years. Your constant

support will never be forgotten, nor will the endless Nerf wars and countless nights spent

together in the basement. The Heathers are the best!!

Page 4: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES AND APPENDICES ...........................................................................v

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... vi

INTRODUCTION ..............................................................................................................1

THE PRESENT RESEARCH ..........................................................................................16

METHOD .........................................................................................................................21

RESULTS ..........................................................................................................................27

DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................................33

REFERENCES ..................................................................................................................41

APPENDIX .......................................................................................................................45

CURRICULUM VITAE ...................................................................................................47

Page 5: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

v

LIST OF TABLES AND APPENDICES

TABLE 1: Means and Standard Deviations for Variables of Interest ................28

TABLE 2: Correlations Among Primary Study Variables .................................31

APPENDIX A: Morality Scenarios ............................................................................45

Page 6: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

vi

ABSTRACT

Increasing attention has been given to the field of moral psychology, especially in regards

to differences in moral judgment and behavior. While some scholars believe that moral

judgment is driven by reactions to harmful behavior (e.g., Gray, Young, & Waytz, 2012;

Turiel, 1983), others still posit that moral judgment is in fact based on five moral

domains, of which harm is one (Haidt & Joseph, 2004). Additionally, within moral

foundation theory, these five domains can further be broken into two distinct categories –

those that are binding (related to groups - in-group/loyalty, authority/respect, and

purity/sanctity), and those that are individuating (related to individuals – harm/care and

fairness/reciprocity). Given the different nature of the two categories (group vs.

individual), the primary purpose of the current study was to examine how group

membership could lead to differing judgments within these different domains. More

specifically, I hypothesized that people would judge in-group members more harshly than

out-group members for committing purity violations, at least when those people strongly

endorse purity themselves and have a high collective self-esteem. Although the

hypothesized four-way interaction was not found, I did find a significant two-way

interaction such that purity endorsement is a significant predictor of negative purity

judgment, but not of care.

Page 7: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

1

INTRODUCTION

Humans are intensely critical beings, constantly processing and assessing new

information, sorting data, and deciding what is useful. However, is all judgment the same?

While we judge ourselves, friends and strangers for choices made about everyday things

such as wearing a seatbelt, or the color of the clothes we wear, we also make judgments

about the (im)moral actions and behavior of these people as well. Upon initial

assessment, it would seem that we tend to be more forgiving of those closer rather than

more distant to us (Hewstone, Rubin, & Willis, 2002). However, is this true of all types

of transgressions? More specifically, are certain moral transgressions (those that are

impure/disgusting) viewed more harshly than other transgressions (those that are

careless/harmful) within certain group contexts? If so, does the group affiliation of the

transgressor result in harsher judgment and punishment for one type but not the other?

The purpose of this paper is to provide and briefly review current psychological

literature exploring the link between moral judgment and intergroup relations. More

specifically, this paper seeks to explore how different moral domains (e.g., purity vs. care)

may be differentially affected by one’s group membership. Lastly, I will review a study

aimed at closing a gap in the morality and intergroup relations literature. This study

examines the effect that group membership has on the severity of both judgment and

punishment for different (im)moral actions. More specifically, I seek to examine whether

actions that are commonly viewed as being harmful (hurting another person in some

manner) are judged as being more or less severe than similar actions that are commonly

viewed as disgusting (violating a sense of decency or purity), when these acts are

committed by either an in-group or out-group member.

Page 8: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

2

I hypothesize that participants will judge in-group members more harshly than

out-group members for purity violations, when participants highly endorse purity as a

moral domain and have high self-esteem that is related to their in-group. For example,

people who are sensitive to purity and disgust and have high group self-esteem will find

the act of drinking urine much more upsetting if someone with a shared group identity

does it (e.g., a fan of the same sports team), as opposed to someone without a shared

group identity (e.g., the fan of a rival sports team). However, keeping additional

unearned money should not elicit different feelings regardless of who commits the act.

MORALITY

Morality is a nebulous concept that is not easily defined. According to the

Merriam Webster dictionary (2014), morality is defined as: a) a doctrine or system of

moral conduct, or b) as conforming to ideals of right human conduct. Yet, the concept of

morality is heavily dependent on the group applying it. There is no universal definition

since there are differences between cultures as to what morality actually entails (Haidt &

Joseph, 2004). For example, do all cultures equally hold that violence is bad? Moreover,

how do different groups rank offensive behavior? Is casual sex more abhorrent than

murder? To further complicate this discussion, the definition of morality also varies

greatly among researchers. There is no consensus about how to quantify moral research,

and the questions asked and the methods used are far from universal.

In one commonly accepted definition, the ‘moral domain’ is described as

“prescriptive judgment of justice, rights, and welfare pertaining to how people ought to

relate to each other” (Turiel, 1983, p. 3). However, this definition is incomplete in that it

ignores types of morality relating to community (those around us) and divinity (moral

Page 9: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

3

rules drawn from religion; Haidt & Joseph, 2004). Mapping onto Turiel’s original

definition of morality, other researchers define morality as “inter-individual treatment of

others with respect to fairness, others’ welfare, equal treatment, and justice” (Killen &

Rizzo, 2014, p. 340). Conversely, other researches have defined morality in terms of

systems, such that they are interlocking sets of values, virtues, norms, practices, identities,

institutions, technologies, and evolved psychological mechanisms that work together to

suppress or regulate self-interest and make cooperative societies possible (Haidt, 2008).

One such moral system is the social intuitionist model (Haidt, 2001).

The social intuitionist model focuses on novel facets of moral judgment;

specifically the role that intuitions and emotions play in both changing and making moral

judgments (Haidt, 2001, 2007). This model proposed that a moral situation would result

in an automatic response dictating rightness or wrongness of a moral act. This immediate

and automatic response is considered an intuition, and Haidt believes that intuitions will

affect the judgments that are made about the situation. The most unique aspect of the

social intuitionist model is that it highlights the impact of emotions on moral judgment,

which the rationalist models completely ignore. This model lays the groundwork for one

of the most utilized yet criticized theory of morality – moral foundations theory (Haidt,

2001; Haidt & Joseph, 2004), which I will utilize in the current study.

Moral Foundations Theory

The moral foundations hypothesis proposed that morality is based on five

different foundations (harm/care, fairness/reciprocity, in-group/loyalty, authority/respect,

and purity/sanctity) that contain the areas in which most, if not all, moral judgments are

made (Haidt & Joseph, 2004). Additionally, within these foundations, research has found

Page 10: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

4

that different groups endorse different foundations at varying rates (Graham, Haidt, &

Nosek, 2009). For example, Graham and colleagues (2009) found that liberals and

conservatives show different levels of endorsement for different moral domains.

Additionally, they attribute this finding to the fact that liberal and conservative ideologies

inherently endorse different moral ideals (e.g., purity vs. care).

This difference in domain endorsement can further be broken down into two

distinct moral foundation categories – those that are binding, and those that are

individuating. Liberals tend to endorse what are considered to be the “individuating”

domains of harm/care and fairness/reciprocity. These foundations are individuating in

that they are focused on protecting the rights of the individual, as opposed to the rights of

groups. Conservatives on the other hand, while still supporting the individuating

foundations, also support what are known as the binding foundations. These binding

foundations consist of in-group/loyalty, authority/respect, and purity/sanctity. What sets

these foundations apart from the individuating foundations is that they are used to

promote group cohesiveness as opposed to individual rights.

Although there is a distinction between binding and individuating foundations,

one thing that is true of both is that morality tends to bind and blind (Haidt, 2008). This

statement is meant to reflect the fact that morality enmeshes and supports group norms,

insulating them against outside pressures regardless of the individuating or binding nature

of the domain. However, it is this cohesion that can lead to individuals being overly

invested in group identity and beliefs and thus less open and understanding of differing

viewpoints.

Page 11: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

5

Aside from differences in domain endorsement as a result of political ideology,

religiosity also plays a role in the endorsement of binding moral domains. Although

researchers have suggested that religion plays an important role in all foundations as it

works to integrate values and virtues to make social life possible (Haidt, 2008), the fact

that religion itself can universally be seen as a binding agent leads researchers to find that

religious people tend to endorse the binding foundations more so than the individuating

ones (Graham & Haidt, 2010). As with political ideology, religious people tend to think

in terms of the group as opposed to the individual (Graham & Haidt, 2010). This group

mindset is evident in the fact that religion is often used to inform social viewpoints and

behavior, therefore acting as a primary moral motivator and binding factor to those who

are religious. However, it is not just religious groups who have a clear sense of what is

right and wrong, Haidt (2008) argues that moral judgments of what is “right” and “wrong”

help people define who they are and where they belong in a social sense.

MORAL JUDGMENT AND PUNISHMENT

Research has found that the way in which people determine what is right and

wrong is related to the social closeness and group membership of the individuals

involved. Specifically, results showed that people reacted more negatively to in-group

suspects than out-group suspects when the person’s guilt was certain in regards to harm

acts. However, when guilt was uncertain, out-group members were rated more

negatively (van Prooijen, 2006). These findings highlight that when in-group and out-

group members are equally likely to be guilty for committing an immoral act, we are still

more concerned about the guilt probability of in-group members as opposed to out-group

members because in-group offenders reflect negatively on the group (Abrams, Marques,

Page 12: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

6

Brown, & Henson, 2010) and more importantly can negatively impact ones social

identity.

Moreover, moral judgments are often viewed as informed by both a desire to

conform to group norms and to consider ones responsibility to the needs of others

(Aquino & Freeman, 2009). Put another way, people must tow the group line in order to

protect those involved. As a result, people often make decisions based on what their

group thinks is correct, and not what may be the best in regards to the needs of others.

Along with the idea of moral judgment is the idea of punishment. According to

some scholars, in order to punish others, one must first disapprove of their actions (Joyce,

2014). Additionally, motivations to punish others should be sensitive to both the social

identity of the persons involved in the interaction, and whether or not the behavior takes

place within a group context or not (Lieberman & Linke, 2007). More specifically, these

researchers found that participants had a stronger desire to punish transgressions against

themselves or their group, as opposed to transgressions toward an out-group.

To summarize, previous research in regards to moral judgment and punishment

found that social identity and group context influenced the severity of and desire to judge

negative actions. The present research aims to determine the effect that group

membership and social identity have on the severity of judgment and punishment in

regards to purity and care violations.

INTERGROUP RELATIONS

Being a part of groups has been a building block of society, as it has been

advantageous to not only make friends, but to maintain group memberships. Of

particular interest today is the way groups behave both amongst themselves and with

Page 13: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

7

other groups. When looking specifically at how groups interact, based on Allport’s (1954)

pioneering work, we define intergroup relationships as pertaining to the attitudes, biases,

judgments and dispositions that a member of one group (in-group) holds about a member

of another group (out-group).

Social Identity Theory

Building from Allport’s pioneering work on intergroup relations, Tajfel and

Turner developed a new theory to examine how people navigate their social world called

Social Identity Theory (SIT). Tajfel and Turner (1982, 1986) state that as part of SIT (for

a review on intergroup relations and SIT see Brown, 2000), people separate their social

world into different groups and categories, some of which they will belong to and others

in which they will not. An important facet to groups is that of “group identification.” In

order to achieve identification within a group, one must not only be aware of their

membership, this awareness needs to be related to some benefit a person gets from being

a member of the group (Tajfel, 1982). This corresponds to social identity, which is

defined as “that part of the individuals’ self-concept which derives from their knowledge

of their membership of a social group (or groups) together with the value and emotional

significant of that membership” (Tajfel, 1981, p. 255). It is important to note however,

that the social group only acts to provide positive social identity for its members through

the use of intergroup comparison on salient dimensions (Commins & Lockwood, 1979).

Put differently, intergroup evaluation is necessary in order to gain positive social identity

from ones in-group membership. Therefore, the separation of people into groups with

varying degrees of membership creates in-groups and out-groups, and further, a person’s

social identity is dictated by the relationship between these groups.

Page 14: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

8

In summation, we gain positive psychological benefits from being members of an

in-group (positive self-esteem, a sense of belonging, etc.), and it is the in-group/out-group

distinction that provides us with these benefits. Additionally, people with high in-group

identity should be more sensitive to threats to their in-group benefits, while people with

low identity would care less about how member’s actions looked to other (Luhtanen &

Crocker, 1992). Therefore, in the current study, I am interested in whether these positive

benefits and more specifically if group based self-esteem, influences the severity of

judgment within a group context.

In-group Love and Out-group Hate

As it is evident in morality research that the in-group/out-group dynamic is

important, it is necessary to discuss the theory behind how these groups relate to each

other. Research has shown that people tend to value the members of their in-group more

highly, and thus treat members of their own group better than members of other groups.

By protecting and supporting in-group membership, individuals receive mutual benefits

and support (see Hewstone et al., 2002 for a complete review on intergroup bias).

Additionally, both Social Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) and other theories

(Social Dominance Orientation; Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth & Malle, 1994) predict that

individuals will favor in-group members over out-group members.

While there has been clear evidence supporting an in-group bias, there has been

more variety in the findings of out-group derogation and intergroup contention. There is

a consensus that individuals within the in-group rely on and support each other, but what

happens when different groups encounter each other? Research has found that intergroup

bias is more rooted in a desire to promote the welfare of the in-group, than it is to put

Page 15: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

9

down the out-group (Brewer, 1999). Once a person views themselves as a member of a

particular group, the individual is then biased in a positive way toward that group. This

finding supports the assertion that the in-group bias we see is the result of self-serving

motives (Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Additional research has also found that in-group bias

serves not only to reinforce positive self-regard, but also to insure personal survival

(Stevens & Fiske, 1995).

As an individual’s behavior reflects on the in-group, and in-group norms dictate

an individual’s behavior, it is necessary to examine the role that in-group bias has on

judgment. Researchers found that in-group love is a stronger motivator for behavior than

out-group hate (Halvey, Weisel, & Bornsetein, 2012). This finding is evident in many

everyday situations (e.g., we are more likely to help someone that is a member of our in-

group over someone who is a relative stranger). In addition, if we help people in our in-

group, it is more likely that when we need help, other members of our in-group will come

to our aid more so than relative strangers. This helping behavior relates back to the idea

that we “get” something from in-group members that we do not get from strangers, and

therefore it is necessary to keep these relationships in harmony.

While numerous studies have shown that the phenomenon of in-group love is not

driven by out-group derogation, other research has found that out-group hate is a

necessary component for defining group belonging (Hewstone et al., 2002; Tajfel, 1981),

and also plays a role in punishment (Halvey, Bornstein, & Sagiv, 2008). The dynamic of

strong in-group love and out-group disdain is particularly noticeable in research by

Schiller and colleagues (2014), who found that individuals punish out-group perpetrators

more harshly than in-group perpetrators (when the person making the judgment shares a

Page 16: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

10

group with the victim). Additionally, they found that this phenomenon was driven by

both the presence of in-group favoritism and strong out-group discrimination. This

finding supports other research on the traditional in-group bias in that people show a

preference for our in-group (e.g., Bernhard, Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2006). Furthermore,

other research has shown that when group identity is important for social identification;

one needs both an in-group and an out-group to drive this identification (Parker & Janoff-

Bulman, 2013). This research is all in line with traditional social identity theory research

that states that group comparison is necessary in order to obtain positive social

identification from groups (Commins & Lockwood, 1979).

Although the research reviewed above lends support to the traditional in-group

love and out-group hate dynamic, this is not the whole picture. Having a strong positive

social identification is a benefit of belonging to a group, however when someone does

something that can make the whole group look bad, this person tends to be ostracized.

More specifically, these traditional in-group and out-group roles are more likely to

change when someone in our in-group does something that violates in-group norms; this

leads to the black sheep effect.

THE BLACK SHEEP EFFECT

Although there is a substantial amount of research aimed at discussing the

commonality of in-group bias and favoritism, in-group bias is most commonly the result

of either a desire to increase the standing of the in-group, or decrease the standing of the

out-group (Abrams et al., 2013). Additionally, research has also found that judgments

within the in-group also allow individuals to maintain the positive identity they would

receive from intergroup differentiation (Marques, Abrams, & Serodio, 2001). Therefore,

Page 17: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

11

people would make stronger judgments towards in-group members as opposed to out-

group members for the simple fact that in-group deviants pose a larger risk to the in-

group dynamic and value, than do out-group deviants (Marques, Paez, & Abrams, 1998;

see also Aquino & Freeman, 2009). Taken together, this phenomenon is commonly

referred to as the black sheep effect.

The black sheep effect is a special and sophisticated form of in-group favoritism

that states that there are more differences in how we evaluate in-group members than in

how we evaluate out-group members (BSE; Marques, Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988). In

other words, there is more variability in thought that goes into judging members of our

in-group for questionable acts than for out-group members. This variability in thought

occurs because people rate individual members of both the in-group and out-group to

determine the best way to bolster their own group’s positive self-image (Abrams et al.,

2013). This intragroup and intergroup comparison is similar to the comparisons in social

identity theory where they are used to maintain positive social identity. Therefore, it is

not always the case that people support in-group members over out-group members.

In certain situations, if supporting an in-group member would make the in-group

look worse than the out-group (and by extension, the in-group individual making the

judgment), then the out-group would be more likely to be supported favorably over the

in-group. A recent study in support of this found that people are more critical of deviant

in-group members than of any other type of group member when judging behavior

(Abrams et al., 2013). This finding supports the idea that the preservation of in-group

status is an important aspect relating to intergroup relations, as this status is where we

draw a positive social identity (Tajfel & Tuner, 1982). More specifically, being critical

Page 18: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

12

of deviant in-group members re-establishes the importance of the in-groups shared

proscriptive norms, and strengthens the cohesion and power of the remaining loyal

members (Marques, Abrams, & Serodio, 2001).

Within the desire for harsher treatment for deviant in-group members, we find

that there is a constant desire and need to advance and reaffirm one’s individual identity

in such a way that it meshes with the social identity displayed by their chosen in-group

(i.e., being a moral upstanding citizen, being politically active, environmentally

conscious, etc.). This desire to reaffirm a strong positive social identity within the in-

group leads to situations in which in-group members are excluded who deviate from

prescribed group norms. Therefore, in certain instances the desire to maintain a positive

and self-serving social identity leads to a necessity to distance oneself and by extension

the in-group from deviant members (Wohl & Reeder, 2004). Thus, one potential

conclusion from this research is that the worst place to be in terms of intragroup relations

is the in-group member who has violated the group’s norms – in a desire to protect in-

group identity and status, other in-group members will harshly judge and potentially

replace the violator for threatening the group (Abrams et al., 2013).

While subjective group dynamics aids in our understanding of why we punish

deviant in-group members, other research has examined the conditions in which our

traditional view of in-group bias (in-group love) overrides our desire to punish these

deviant in-group members. Research has shown that when third party judges were

allowed to maintain the possibility that the wrongdoer was not completely responsible for

their action, in-group perpetrators were given the “benefit of the doubt” which allowed

them to be judged less harshly than out-group members. However if this question of guilt

Page 19: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

13

was not raised, harsh in-group judgment followed, at least in regards to harm violations

(van Prooijen, 2009). Essentially a question of culpability allowed for the natural in-

group preference to override the potential harm caused by the perpetrators’ actions. In

the present study, all participants were judging acts where there is no doubt about the

people’s wrongdoing, therefore it should follow that harsh in-group judgment should

occur, even when looking at purity violations.

Given that groups are an integral part of social identity, it is important to examine

norms that directly influence our identification with groups. Research conducted by

Leach and colleagues (2007) found that people care more about belonging to groups that

are seen as moral than to groups that are valued for their competence or sociability.

Therefore, when looking at intergroup relations, specifically in-group love and the black

sheep effect, it is necessary to consider the role that morality, and more specifically the

role that moral transgressions play in the judgment of deviant behavior of in-group and

out-group members.

Morality and Intergroup Relations

When looking at moral research, one would be remiss to ignore the study of

intergroup relations. For example, the interactions between different moralizing groups

such as religious and political parties sheds light on how differences in moral values can

lead to in-group cohesion. In fact, Kitcher (2014) holds that the purpose of morality is in

a broad sense to support social cohesion (for an in-depth review of morality in the context

of intergroup relationships (see Killen, Margie, & Sinno, 2006). Additionally, other

researchers believe that morality in particular is a special case in which in-group love and

the desire to strengthen those within group moral ties will lead to situations in which out-

Page 20: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

14

group hate will occur (Parker & Janoff-Bulman, 2013). For example, these researchers

(in line with social identity theory) found that groups that were formed as a result of a

morally based consensus (e.g., ‘pro-choice’ vs. ‘pro-life’) needed a strong opposing out-

group, in order to establish a strong in-group identity. Therefore, social identification

within the in-group loses all meaning without the presence of the out-group for morally

based groups. This out-group comparison is particularly important for the relationship

between in-groups and out-groups in relation to moral judgments. As Parker and Janoff-

Bulman (2013) point out, the “us” versus “them” mentality is particularly important when

examining moral judgments because the degradation of the out-group is necessary to

make the in-group stronger.

Aside from just intergroup bias, moral emotions (e.g., anger and disgust) also play

an important role in moral judgment. Research has shown that there are stronger

negative emotions associated with out-groups when looking at morality-based situations

when compared to non-morality based situations (Parker & Janoff-Bulman, 2013);

however, within morality based groups, positive evaluations for both in-group and out-

group members were necessary.

Ellemers and van den Bos’ (2012) work on morality and intergroup relations

found that the desire to bolster the standing of the in-group is what drives how people

tend to communicate with and behave towards members of groups that have different

moral standards through judgment and punishment. Put another way, the desire to

maintain the positive benefits of intergroup comparisons leads to situations in which the

in-group uses severe judgment and punishment of the moral out-group in order to

maintain their positive social identity.

Page 21: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

15

In a similar vein, researchers found that people report less desire to interact with

targets depicted as lacking moral qualities as opposed to those who were depicted as

being highly moral. If these individuals were put into the same group, those who hold

highly moral beliefs are more likely to want to interact with those who hold the same

beliefs, as this shared prescriptive norm bolsters the shared positive social identification

(Commins & Lockwood, 1979). Further, individuals would be more critical of those who

do not share the same moral value, regardless of whether the individuals are a member of

the in-group or out-group, as this bolsters the highly moral in-group identification by

degrading the non-moral out-group.

Overall, the research on intergroup relations and morality has found that an

intergroup comparison is necessary when dealing with moral values. However overall,

moral individuals only want to interact with other moral individuals and they separate

themselves from those who do not share the same values. This finding is important for

the present study, as I predict that people who care about purity will be more bothered by

in-group members that violate this moral value, as opposed to out-group members.

Page 22: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

16

THE PRESENT RESEARCH

The main purpose of this research is to examine the role that intergroup relations

has on the severity of judgment of moral violations. While each of these bodies of

research has been linked in various ways, the proposed link hypothesized by the current

study is novel. Specifically, this study seeks to examine the effect of group membership

on judgments of moral purity and care violations, as well as the role that individual

differences in moral values and group self-esteem has in moderating this relationship.

Although previous research has identified links between group membership and severity

of judgment, no work has directly examined the differences in these judgments (e.g., act

vs. perpetrator) nor have they examined the effect that moral value endorsement or group

self-esteem will have in changing these judgments. In order to examine this effect, this

study focused on moral purity and care acts that were perpetrated by either a salient in-

group or out-group member.

A group distinction was chosen because past research on intergroup relations has

found that people tend to be more forgiving of in-group members for transgression (SIT;

Tajfel & Turner, 1986). However, additional research on the black sheep effect

specifically, has shown that in certain situations we tend to punish in-group members

more harshly for violating important group norms (Aquino & Freeman, 2009). Given

these findings around group norms, the moral domains of care and harm were chosen for

our target judgments.

Prior research within moral foundations theory (see Haidt, 2013 for a review) has

found group differences in moral judgments for the care and purity domains, as the care

domain promotes individuation, while the purity domain promotes group cohesion

Page 23: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

17

(Graham & Haidt, 2010). More specifically, the care domain is about protecting the

individual from harmful acts, while the purity domain is about protecting the group from

harm. Given that the black sheep effect is found when in-group members are trying to

reaffirm the power and cohesion of their group, and subsequently the positive group

identity derived from it (Marques, Paez, & Abrams, 1998), it should follow that

judgments about purity violations should be we worse when perpetrated by an in-group

member as opposed to an out-group member, as they pose the biggest risk to positive

group identity for those who highly endorse purity. As the care domain is about

protecting the individual, violations of this domain should be equally bothersome across

in-groups and out-groups.

Additionally, I will examine the impact that moral value endorsement will have

on the relationship between group membership and moral judgments. Previous research

by McKee and Masicampo (2014) found that close others were rated more harshly than

strangers for purity violations, however, this effect was found only for participants who

strongly endorsed moral purity. Although the black sheep effect inherently examines the

role that domain endorsement has on moral judgment as it examines whether norms apply

to both the in-group and out-group (universal endorsement) versus just in-group

endorsement, the present research looks to extend the findings of the black sheep effect

into the moral domain. Specifically, there might be something special about moral purity

such that even if there is universal domain endorsement (i.e., everyone believes you

should behave in a certain moral way), it is still more bothersome to have an in-group

member violate this norm as compared to an out-group member, or when compared to

another shared norm (e.g., regarding harmful acts).

Page 24: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

18

While no other research has specifically examined the role that domain

endorsement has on moral judgment, let alone the black sheep effect, the present research

aims to extend the findings of the black sheep effect into the moral domain, while also

showing that even when both in-groups and out-groups think a violation is wrong, if you

highly endorse the domain, an in-group deviant will be worse than an out-group deviant.

Given the important role that in-group member behavior plays in our acquisition of

positive in-group benefits, it is easy to see why someone whose identity is closely tied to

an in-group identity would take particular offense to another in-group member who

threatens these highly valued norms.

Lastly, we will examine the additional impact of group based self-esteem on the

relationship between group membership and moral judgment. Collective self-esteem

(CSE) measures how important membership in a group in general is to self-concept and is

often marked by feelings of emotion such as pride and shame toward an in-group (Kaiser

& Major, 2004). Additionally, people high in CSE tend to show in-group favoritism

(Crocker & Luhtanen, 1990), as research has shown that people with high CSE tend to

evaluate in-group members as more competent and fairer than participants with low CSE

(De Cremer, van Vugt, & Sharp, 1999). With these findings in mind, I used the

Collective Self Esteem scale (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992), to see if high CSE would lead

to harsh in-group judgment within a moral context, instead of traditional out-group

derogation. While traditional social identity theory work cites various ways individuals

try to restore a threatened social identity including leaving the group and reevaluating

group attributes, people who are high in collective self-esteem may be more likely to

Page 25: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

19

engage in strategies to restore the threatened identity (Crocker & Luhtanen, 1990), which

may include ostracizing a deviant in-group member to restore group identity.

In addition to the CSE measure which measures a general valuation of groups, I

will also utilize the single item social identification measure (SISI; Postmes, Haslam, &

Jans, 2013) and a subscale from the Overlap of Self, Ingroup and Ougroup measure

(OSIO; Schubert & Otten, 2002) to measure people’s identification with a specific group

(i.e. the group being measured in the study). The SISI has been found to be a reliable

measure of in-group identity and the positive relationship between self and the in-group.

Additionally, the Inclusion of Self in the In-group scale from the Overlap of Self, Ingroup

and Ougroup measure provides a pictoral representation of how close one feels to a

chosen in-group. More importantly however, this scale has also been found to be reliable,

and to provide conceptually different results from pure verbal measures as it allows the

mapping of social and self-categorizations on a spatial dimension (Aron, Aron, &

Smollan, 1992).

Given these previous findings, we will look at violations that pertain to both care

and purity to determine when in-group members will be judged and punished more

severely than out-group members. In order to measure moral value endorsement to

determine the effect endorsement has on the severity of ratings, we will use the Moral

Foundations Questionnaire-20 (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009). Additionally, previous

research by Linke (2007) found no difference in ratings of moral wrongness across group

domains pertaining to the act, (although there were differences in deservedness of

punishment). Therefore, we will also measure ratings of moral wrongness and

deservedness of punishment in order to tease apart the intricacies of moral judgment.

Page 26: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

20

The purpose of this research is to examine the role that intergroup relations has on

the severity of judgment of moral violations, when taking into account ones moral

endorsement and self-esteem related to their in-group. Given this, I hypothesize that

participants will judge in-group members more harshly than out-group members for

purity violations, when participants highly endorse purity as a moral domain and have

high group based self-esteem.

Page 27: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

21

METHOD

Participants

Our initial sample consisted of 332 adults. However, 83 participants failed to

correctly answer one of three manipulation checks, or had incomplete data, thus resulting

in a final sample of 249. This sample ranged in age from 19-74 years (M = 33.75, SD =

10.97). Among participants, we had 118 males, 103 females and 28 people decline to

answer. The majority of participants were White (72.3%) followed by Asian (8.8%),

Black or African American (3.2%), more than one race (3.2%). Additionally, we were

missing race for 12.4% of participants.

Procedure

Participants were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), a

website that allows “workers” to be compensated for completing Human Intelligence

Tasks (HITs) that have been posted by researchers. As noted by Buhrmester and

colleagues (2011), MTurk samples have been shown to provide reliable data and are

typically more representative of the larger population as compared to samples that draw

from undergraduate participant pools. After selecting the HIT, participants were

redirected to complete the survey. Upon reading the informed consent and agreeing to

participate, participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions

where they responded to a series of questionnaires, and rated 35 different scenarios.

Upon completing the experiment, participants were debriefed via an online debriefing

summary and given a code to enter into Mturk to receive payment.

Page 28: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

22

Measures

Moral Foundations Questionnaire - MRQ. The MFQ-20 is a shortened version

of the MFQ-30 (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009), with two subscales designed to measure

the moral relevance/endorsement (MRQ) and judgment (MJQ) of the five moral domains

(harm, fairness, ingroup, authority, and purity). As we were only interested in

endorsement scores, participants only completed the MRQ. The MRQ specifically

measures how relevant certain moral considerations are when deciding something is

either right or wrong. This scale consists of 10 items (two from each moral foundation)

and one attention check item. All items start with the stem: “When you decide whether

something is right or wrong, to what extent are the following considerations relevant to

your thinking?” with items such as “Whether or not someone suffered emotionally” and

“Whether or not someone acted unfairly.” The attention check for the MRQ is “Whether

or not someone was good at math.” All items are scored on a 6-point Likert-type scale

anchored by “0 – not at all relevant” and “5 – extremely relevant.” Endorsement scores

are created for each domain by averaging the two items in that domain together. However,

in accordance with Graham, Haidt, and Nosek (2009), participants who respond to the

attention check with a 2 (slightly relevant) or higher, were removed from analyses.

Collective Self-Esteem Scale. In order to measure how important belonging to

groups in general was to our participants, we used the importance to identity subscale

from the Collective Self-Esteem Scale (CSES; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992). This

subscale measures how important membership in a group is to self-concept of the

individual and is comprised of four items (two positive and two negative). The positive

items are comprised of: “The social groups I belong to are an important reflection of who

Page 29: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

23

I am,” and “In general, belonging to social groups is an important part of my self-image,”

while the negative items that are reverse scored are “Overall, my group memberships

have very little to do with how I feel about myself,” and “The social groups I belong to

are unimportant to my sense of what kind of person I am.” These items are rated on a “1

– strongly disagree” to “7 – strongly agree” scale. A construct score is created by

summing all items, and dividing the answer by 4, with higher scores indicating higher

levels of self-esteem related to group identity.

Group selection. Participants were presented with instructions to name both an

in-group and out-group. In regards to the in-group, participants are first given examples

of types of social groups (e.g., sports teams, religious groups, political groups, members

of a profession, etc.). They are then asked to think of a group they belong to that meets

the following criteria: “The fact that you belong to this group should be an important part

of your identity,” “This group should not be your family,” and “This group should be

large enough that there are members of the group that you do not personally know.”

After providing the name, they were then asked the approximate size of the group.

As in the in-group selection, participants were asked to think of a group that they

do not belong to, that also meets these criteria: “The fact that you do NOT belong to this

group should be an important part of your identity” and “This group should be large

enough that there are members of the group that you do not personally know.” Finally,

they were again asked to provide the approximate size of the group.

Social identity. In order to measure how close participants felt to their chosen in-

group, participants were presented with a single item social identification (SISI) measure

(Postmes, Haslam, & Jans, 2013). This item simply states, “I identify the group (or

Page 30: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

24

category),” and is rated on a 7-point likert scale from “1 – fully disagree” to “7 – fully

agree.” Higher scores on this item relate to higher group identification.

Overlap of in-group and self. In order to measure the relationship between

participants and their chosen in-group, participants were presented with the Inclusion of

Self in the Ingroup Scale, from the Overlap of Self, Ingroup, and Outgroup Scale (OSIO;

Schubert & Otten, 2002). This scale uses a series of seven pictures and asks respondents

to choose the picture that best describes their relationship to their chosen in-group, with

choices ranging from the circles being completely separate (coded as 1), to the circles

being completely overlapped (coded as 7). Higher values correspond to more feelings of

inclusion in their chosen in-group.

Scenarios. In order to determine differences in reactions to moral scenarios,

participants were randomly presented with 35 different scenarios, and asked to think of a

member of their ingroup [outgroup] they do not know committing each of the acts. Out

of the 35 acts, there are seven each of negative purity and negative care violations that

were modeled after previous examples (e.g., Haidt, 2001, 2003) and were used in

previous research by McKee and Masicampo (2014). Examples of negative purity items

included imagining members, “collected and ate roadkill” and “had sex with a blowup

animal,” while negative care items included: “threw an acorn at a dog for no reason,” and

“made fun of a person’s clothes behind their back.”

Additionally, there are seven each of positive purity and positive care scenarios.

These scenarios have the same stem as the above items and include, “abstained from sex

until married” and “washed their hands after going to a petting zoo” for the purity items,

and “donated a bike to a family in need” and “gave change to a panhandler” for the care

Page 31: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

25

items. Lastly, there are seven neutral items that do not pertain to any sort of moral

domain. These items were used a control and include, “goes to see a movie every Friday,”

and “gets 7 hours of sleep every night.” A complete list of all scenarios can be found in

Appendix A.

Judgment and punishment. In order to determine differences in judgments

between the person and the act, each participant responded to four questions following

each of the scenarios. Previous research has found that differences in the desire to punish

do not always correspond to differences in judging the severity of acts themselves (Linke,

2007), so these judgments provided a way to tease the two apart. To gauge the morality

of the act, participants responded to “How immoral/moral do you find this act” on a

“-100 – most immoral act possible” to “0 – neither immoral nor moral” to “100 – most

moral act possible” scale.” In order to determine the morality rating of the person,

participants responded to, “how immoral/moral do you find this person for committing

this act” on a “-100 – extremely immoral” to “ 0 – neither immoral nor moral” to “100 –

extremely moral” scale.

In regards to punishment, participants first rated how punishable they found the

act to be by responding to: “How punishable/praiseworthy do you find this act to be” on a

“-100 – most punishable act possible” to “0 – neither punishable nor praiseworthy” to

“100 – most praiseworthy act possible” scale. Participants then rated how much they

wanted to punish the individual by answering, “How punishable/praiseworthy is the

person for committing this act,” rated as “-100 – extremely punishable” to “0 – neither

punishable nor praiseworthy” to “100 – extremely praiseworthy.”

Page 32: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

26

In order to create mean scores for these items, all of the responses to the scenarios

in a domain (e.g., all positive purity scenarios) were averaged across each judgment type.

This resulted in 4 separate outcome variables for the five different sets of scenarios.

Demographics and debriefing. Finally, participants responded to basic

demographic questions. Participants’ political orientation was measured on a 7-point

scale from “1 – extremely liberal” to “7 – extremely conservative” with higher scores

relating to more conservative ideology with one additional “not sure” option.

Additionally, participants’ religiosity was measured using the single item, “How religious

do you consider yourself” with responses coded as “1 – extremely religious, 2 – religious,

3 – somewhat religious, 4 – not at all religious, 5 – agnostic, 6 – atheist, and 7 –

unsure/decline to answer.” Education was measured in a similar manner on a 7 point

response scale (1 - 7) with higher scores indicating more education. Gender was a

dichotomous variable with male coded a “1” and female coded as “2.” Additionally we

measured participants race as, “1 – American Indian/Alaska Native, 2 – Asian, 3 – Native

Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander, 4 – Black or African American, 5 – White, 6 – More

than one race, and 7 – unknown or not reported.” There was an additional question about

ethnicity coded as, “1 – Hispanic or Latino” or “2 – Not Hispanic or Latino.” Lastly,

participants were presented with open-ended questions asking them to report their age,

comments about the study, and what they thought the study was about.

Page 33: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

27

RESULTS

Domain, Group Membership, Group Identification, and Purity Endorsement

In order to test my hypothesis that people will judge in-group members more

harshly than out-group members for committing purity violations (but not care), when

those people strongly endorse purity and have high in-group self-esteem, I entered group

membership (in-group vs. out-group), moral domain (purity vs. care), purity endorsement,

and collective self-esteem into a mixed model ANOVA with moral domain as the within

groups factor predicting the severity of judgment of the person (i.e., how moral vs.

immoral) committing the act. See Table 1 for basic descriptive statistics and Table 2 for

the correlations of the primary study variables.

Counter to my hypothesis, the predicted four-way interaction between moral

domain, group membership, purity endorsement, and collective self-esteem was not

significant, F(1, 227) = 1.414, p = .236. Additionally, none of the three-way interactions

were significant, Fs < 2.665, ps > .104. There was however, a significant two-way

interaction between moral domain and purity endorsement, F(1, 227) = 8.653, p = .004.

More specifically, in regards to judgments of people who committed care violations,

purity endorsement marginally predicted the severity of care judgments, β = -.112, p

= .085, however significantly predicted the severity of purity judgments, β = -.414, p

< .001. Put another way, as purity endorsement increased, so too did the severity of

judgment for perpetrators of both negative purity and care acts, with negative purity acts

being more affected. No other two-way interactions were significant, Fs < 3.285, ps

> .071. Lastly, there was a significant main effect of moral domain such that negative

Page 34: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

28

purity acts (M = -27.97, SD = 25.76) were rated significantly harsher than care acts (M =

-22.41, SD = 16.41).

Table 1

Means and Standard Deviations for Variables of Interest

PurityEnd CSE SISI OSIO Politic Age

Mean 2.433 3.798 6.2250 5.112 3.367 33.24

Std Deviation 1.424 1.404 0.7544 1.270 1.666 10.74

Exploratory Analyses

Act judgment. Although there were no hypotheses regarding differences in the

judgment of the act versus the judgment of the person, I conducted a mixed model

ANOVA to examine possible differences in these outcome measures. The results of the

four-way ANOVA examining the severity of act judgment yielded the same results as the

hypothesized four-way interaction examining person judgment. There was a significant

interaction between moral domain and purity endorsement, F(1, 227) = 5.787, p = .017,

such that purity endorsement significantly predicts the severity of act judgment for both

care violations, β = -.151, p = .020, and purity β = -.414, p < .001. There was also a

significant main effect of moral domain, F(1, 227) = 7.683, p = .006, such that purity acts

(M = -27.16, SD = 25.05) are judged more harshly than care acts (M = -22.70, SD =

16.04). As before, there were no other significant results, Fs < 2.890, ps > .090.

Page 35: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

29

Person punishment. In order to examine possible differences relating to the

desire to punish people for committing moral acts, I conducted another mixed model

ANOVA with severity of person punishment as the outcome variable. This analysis also

revealed the same pattern of results. There was a significant interaction between moral

domain and purity endorsement, F(1, 227) = 13.241, p < .001, such that purity

endorsement significantly predicts the severity of person punishment for purity violations,

β = -.401, p < .001, but not care violations, β = -.078, p = .228. There was also a

significant interaction between moral domain and CSE, F(1, 227) = 4.549,

p = .034, such that such that collective self-esteem significantly predicts the severity of

person punishment for purity violations, β = -.190, p = .003, but not care violations, β = -

.006, p = .931. Additionally, there was again a significant main effect of moral domain,

F(1, 227) = 15.731, p < .001, such that purity acts (M = -24.19, SD = 24.48) are punished

more harshly than care acts (M = -21.39, SD = 17.71). There were no other significant

results, Fs < 2.782, ps > .097.

Act punishment. Lastly, in order to examine the desire to punish the immoral act

as opposed to the person, I conducted a mixed model ANOVA with the severity of act

punishment as the outcome variable. This analysis again followed the same pattern of

results with a significant interaction between moral domain and purity endorsement,

F(1, 227) = 9.931, p = .002, such that purity endorsement significantly predicts the

severity of person punishment for purity violations, β = -.413, p < .001, but not care

violations, β = -.110, p = .090. There was again a significant main effect of moral

domain, F(1, 227) = 11.442, p = .001, such that purity violations (M = -24.63, SD = 24.48)

Page 36: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

30

are punished more harshly than care violations (M = -21.51, SD = 16.30). As before,

there were no other significant results, Fs < 2.723, ps > .100.

Single item social identification. Although I had a specific hypothesis regarding

the influence of collective self-esteem on my proposed interaction, as I had additional

measures of in-group identity, I wanted to see if they had a different effect regarding the

proposed interaction. To test this, I entered group membership (in-group vs. out-group),

moral domain (purity vs. care), purity endorsement, and the SISI into a mixed model

ANOVA with moral domain as the within groups factor predicting the severity of

judgment of the person committing the act. This analysis revealed no significant main

effects or interactions, Fs < 1.392, ps > .239.

Inclusion of self in the in-group. As with the SISI, I had no specific hypothesis

regarding the OSIO measure, however to test the possible effects of this measure I mixed

model ANOVA measuring severity of person judgment. As with the previous analysis,

this analysis also yielded no significant main effects nor any interactions, Fs < 2.328, ps

> .128.

In-group bias and positive behavior. Although I did not find my hypothesized

interaction, I was interested in examining whether an in-group bias existed in regards to

positive moral actions. In order to test this I entered group membership (in-group vs. out-

group), moral domain (positive purity vs. positive care), purity endorsement, and

collective self-esteem into a mixed model ANOVA with moral domain as the within

groups factor predicting the positive judgment of the person (i.e., how moral vs. immoral)

committing the act. Overall, there was a main effect of domain, F(1, 227) = 25.700,

Page 37: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

Table 2

Correlations among Primary Study Variables

HarmEnd FairnessEnd LoyaltyEnd AuthorityEnd PurityEnd CSE SISI OSIO Politic

HarmEnd — 0.631* 0.250*** 0.117 0.083 0.039 0.255*** 0.142* -0.216**

FairnessEnd — — 0.150* 0.016 -0.020 0.017 0.197** 0.115 -0.243***

LoyaltyEnd — — — 0.731*** 0.612*** 0.282*** 0.134* 0.147* 0.276***

AuthorityEnd — — — — 0.683*** 0.284*** 0.080 0.181** 0.309***

PurityEnd — — — — — 0.284*** 0.128* 0.138* 0.319***

CSE — — — — — — 0.188** 0.232*** 0.114

SISI — — — — — — — 0.486*** 0.073

OSIO — — — — — — — — 0.040

Politic — — — — — — — — —

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

31

Page 38: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

32

p < .001, such that ratings for positive care acts (M = 45.74, SD = 23.33) were

significantly higher than positive purity ratings (M = 27.59, SD = 27.02). This means that

overall; people rated others doing positive care acts as being more moral than for doing

positive purity acts. Aside from this main effect, there were no other significant results,

Fs < 1.054, ps > .306.

Political ideology and moral domain. Lastly, I was interested in seeing whether

the data replicated traditional moral foundations theory work regarding differences in

political ideology - that the more conservative people are, the more harshly they will

judge purity violations as opposed to care. To test this, I entered overall moral judgment

(negative care vs. negative purity) and political ideology as a continuous variable into a

mixed model ANOVA with moral domain as the within subjects factor. This analysis

yielded a significant domain by political ideology interaction, F(1, 220) = 30.103, p

< .001. More specifically, political ideology was a significant predictor of overall

negative purity ratings, β = -.245, p < .001, but not of overall care ratings, β = -.023,

p = .728. So as one becomes more conservative, their rating of negative purity acts

become stronger, but has no effect for negative care acts.

Page 39: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

33

DISCUSSION

Past research on the relationship between moral domain, social closeness, and

moral domain endorsement found that close friends are judged more harshly than

strangers for purity violations, when participants highly endorse purity as a moral domain

(McKee & Masicampo, 2014). The purpose of the current research was to test whether

the effect extended into intergroup dynamics, and additionally to see if levels of

collective self-esteem influenced the relationship between moral domain, group

membership and domain endorsement regarding severity of moral judgment.

The results of the current study did not support the hypothesis. There was not a

significant interaction between moral domain, group membership, purity endorsement,

and collective self-esteem, which means even if participants highly endorse purity as a

moral domain and have high collective self-esteem, they do not judge in-group members

more harshly than out-group members for purity violations. So put another way,

participants who have higher or lower endorsement of purity along with any level of

collective self-esteem do not judge in-group and out-group members differently for purity

or care violations.

Although the predicted four-way interaction was not found, the current study does

validate the research around domain endorsement and domain violation, as there was a

significant interaction between these two variables. In other words, when people cared

about moral purity, they judged purity violations more harshly than care violations.

This finding replicates previous research around the moral domain (e.g., Graham et al.,

2011) that when you endorse a moral domain, violations of those domains are more

bothersome than if you did not endorse the domain.

Page 40: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

34

Additionally, there was a main effect of domain such that on average, purity

violations were rated more harshly than care violations. This finding supports Turiel

(1983) who believed that moral violations not related to harm would be treated

differently from those that were related to harm, although does not say how they would

be treated differently. In the case of this study, it may be that participants rated purity

violations more strongly than care violations, because the violations in question invoked

the feeling of disgust. More specifically, researchers have found that feelings of

cleanliness resulted in less harsh judgments of moral transgressions (Schnall, Benton, &

Harvey, 2008), while feelings of disgust led people to avoid the individuals, situations,

and actions that elicited this feeling (e.g., Horberg, Oveis, Keltner, & Cohen, 2009; Inbar,

Pizarro, & Bloom, 2012). However, this finding could also be an artifact of the scenarios

chosen in each domain. Although these scenarios had been used in previous research

(McKee & Masicampo, 2014), and were matched as closely as possible in regards to

overall mean score, it is possible that with different scenarios, I would actually find the

opposite effect, such that harm violations would be rated more severely than purity

violations. The effect of harm as being more bothersome is in line with previous research

regarding the nature of morality, such that all judgments of morality relate to moral harm

(e.g., Gray et al., 2012; Turiel, 1983). Future research should account for both

possibilities and use scenarios that have been validated in order to be able to draw

appropriate conclusions regarding any main effect findings.

As there was a significant two-way interaction in the same direction as the

hypothesized four-way interaction, it leads to the question of why collective self-esteem

and group membership did not interact with these variables. In regards to group

Page 41: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

35

membership, although I asked participants to self-select an in-group to which they

belonged and was important to them, and followed up with multiple scales of in-group

identification and self-esteem, it appears that the in-groups that participants chose did not

provide a positive social identity. Although the inclusion of self in the in-group scale and

the single item social identification measure had positive means, reflecting closer feelings

to the in-group, the social identity subscale I used from the CSE scale actually had a

below average mean, indicating that participants actually felt their self-esteem was not

tied to being a member of a group. Therefore, it is possible that although participants

chose groups they felt they belonged to, on average they did not seem to feel a benefit

from their chosen group. This could explain why there were no group membership

differences within the study in either direction; from a social identity theory perspective,

if you have a strong in-group identity, you tend to make strong negative judgments of the

out-group. Additionally, from the perspective of the black sheep literature, if you care

about your in-group, threats from within are also severely judged (Marques et al., 1998),

however neither of these events occurred. Future research should either remind

participants why they selected the group they did (e.g., it is important to them and they

want to belong to it), or to allow participants an opportunity to reaffirm the core values of

the group they chose. By allowing participants an opportunity to reaffirm their group’s

ideals, they would be more attuned to threats against this ideal and by extension their

social identity (Harvey & Oswold, 2000).

In regards to collective self-esteem, I hypothesized that high collective self-

esteem would lead to harsh in-group judgment within a moral context, instead of

traditional out-group derogation, as collective self-esteem is intrinsically tied with

Page 42: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

36

maintaining a positive social identity (Crocker & Luhtanen, 1990). It is for this reason

that in-group deviants should be perceived as more of a threat to group norms which

should lead to harsher in-group judgment. This however, was not the case. Not only did

I not find the hypothesized effect of collective self-esteem in the four-way interaction,

higher scores on collective self-esteem did not result in harsher judgment of out-group

members, which is what would be expected given that group serving biases are

particularly strong when looking at individuals who are highly identified with their group

(Castano, Yzerbert, Bourguignon, & Seron, 2002). This lack of finding however can be

explained by the fact that overall, participants in the study rated their in-group based

collective self-esteem as less than neutral. Therefore on average, the sample

predominantly had low collective self-esteem, which in the literature has shown to have

no effect on out-group judgment (Castano et al., 2002). Additionally, as only one

subscale from the entire CSE scale was used, it may be necessary to utilize the complete

measure to gain a more accurate assessment of people’s group based self-esteem. Future

research using a more diverse sampling of CSE scores may be able to address the

hypothesized impact collective self-esteem may have on moral domain judgment.

Exploratory Variables

Judgment. Previous research around moral judgment has found that in relation

to the judgment of moral domain acts, there tends to be no difference in ratings of moral

wrongness (Linke, 2007). That being said, while this study found a significant difference

in the judgment severity of purity and care acts such that purity was worse than care

(which is counter to previous research), this same pattern of results was found when

looking at the judgment of the perpetrator as well. Although this research presents an

Page 43: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

37

apparent inconsistency with past work, it is most likely the result of context which has

been found to strongly influence the relative judgment of people and acts across various

domains (Bartels, Bauman, Cushman, Pizarro, & McGraw, 2015).

Punishment. In regards to severity of punishment of both the act and the person,

this study found no overall difference between person punishment and act punishment.

The differences that were found related to an overall finding that purity violations were

punished more harshly than care violations, and that purity endorsement predicted the

severity of punishment for purity violations. The consistency in these findings could be

the result of various influences around punishment. Much of the research relating to the

desire to punish moral wrong doing has found that the desire to punish is rooted in

disapproval in the action (Joyce, 2014), certain guilt of the perpetrator (van Prooijen,

2006), and intentionality of the action (Cushman, 2008). All of the moral scenarios in

this study were such that guilt was certain and the act was intentional. The only criterion

of the three mentioned that was not met was the disapproval of the action, as I wanted

purity endorsement, group membership, and self-esteem to influence the disapproval of

the actions and therefore the severity of punishment. Given that purity endorsement was

a predictor of harsh punishment for purity (but not care), and that purity endorsement is

indicative of agreement with the principles of the domain, it appears that disapproval of

the act is necessary in order to elicit the desire to punish. Future research in this area

should be conducted to determine whether all domain endorsements lead to the desire to

punish violation in the given domain, or whether this is something related to either just

purity violations or the binding domain in general.

Page 44: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

38

In-group identity measures. Although I had a specific hypothesis regarding the

in-group identity subscale of the CSE, I had no such hypotheses regards either the single

item social identification measure (SISI) or the overlap of in-group and self measure

(OSIO). Previous research found that positive in-group social identity results in a desire

to maintain the positive standing of the group (Marques et al., 2001) and that this desire

can either manifest itself in harsh in-group judgment (Wohl & Reeder, 2004) or harsh

out-group derogation (Hewstone et al., 2002). My hypothesis regarding the CSE was

higher levels of collective self-esteem should lead participants to judge in-group

members more harshly for negative acts, as their negative action reflects poorly on the in-

group. Since both the SISI and OSIO measure in-group identification and feelings of

belonging with the in-group respectively, higher scores on these items should also lead to

differences in judgment severity outcomes. This however, was not the case as there were

no significant effects of either variable found. This finding was surprising as the overall

means for each item indicated an overall positive association with the in-group.

Therefore, high in-group identity may not differentially lead to severe judgment for one

group as opposed to the other, and it may be other factors that drive the severity of

judgment.

Positive in-group bias. While the purpose of the current study was to examine

the effect of group membership, moral domain, purity endorsement and collective self-

esteem in regards to negative violations, I wanted to also examine whether there was an

in-group bias for positive acts. While social identity theory and the black sheep effect

often pertain to elevating the in-group status following a threat, the presence of positive

actions should elicit no such threat, and therefore in-group members should be judged

Page 45: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

39

more favorably than out-group members for the sheer fact that they share a group identity.

This however, was not the case, as I found no difference in positive ratings for in-group

and out-group members. Rather, the only significant finding was that positive care acts

were rated more positively than positive purity acts. This could be because in-group love

is a strong motivator for behavior (Halvey et al., 2012) and overall the care scenarios

involved actions that helped others, while the purity scenarios only involved the

individual. Future research should take this difference into account and aim to create

scenarios that are either all about the individual or all about others.

Political Ideology. In regards to political ideology, I was interested in whether

this study replicated the finding that different political ideologies endorse different moral

domains (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009). In line with previous research, the results

supported political differences within the moral foundations. More specifically, I found

that political ideology predicted judgment of purity violations, but not care violations -

the more conservative participants were, the more harshly they judged purity violations.

This replication adds support to the overall political differences in the moral foundations

theory, as conservatives tend to endorse the binding foundations (e.g., purity) and both

liberals and conservatives endorse individuating foundations (e.g., care), it is no surprise

that we see a difference in the two domains, such that purity is worse.

Conclusion

The current study failed to find the proposed four-way interaction between moral

domain, group membership, purity endorsement, and collective self-esteem. It did,

however, provide support for the link between purity endorsement and domain judgment,

while also replicating the political ideological difference findings in moral foundations

Page 46: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

40

theory. Future work in this area is necessary to determine whether in-group identity and

group membership plays a role with purity endorsement and moral domain in eliciting a

black sheep effect, or whether social identity theory will predominate judgment in the

moral domain.

Page 47: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

41

REFERENCES

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the

structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

63(4), 596-612. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

Abrams, D., Marques, J. M., Bown, N., & Henson, M. (2000). Pro-norm and anti-norm

deviance within and between groups. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology,78, 906-912. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.906

Abrams, D., Palmer, S. B., Rutland, A., Cameron, L., & Van de Vyver, J. (2013).

Evaluations of and reasoning about normative and deviant ingroup and outgroup

members: Development of the black sheep effect. Developmental Psychology, 50(1),

258-270. doi:10.1037/a0032461

Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley

Publishing.

Aquino, K., & Freeman, D. (2009). Moral identity in business situations: A social-

cognitive framework for understanding moral functioning. In D. Narvaez & D. K.

Lapsley (Eds.), Personality, identity, and character: Explorations in moral

psychology (pp. 375-396). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Bartels, D. M., Bauman, C. W., Cushman, F. A., Pizzaro, D. A., & McGraw, A. P. (2015).

Moral judgment and decision making. In G. Keren & G. Wu (Eds.), Wiley Blackwell

handbook of judgment and decision making. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature,

442, 912-915.

Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate?

Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429-444.

Brown, R. J., (2000). Social identity theory: Past achievements, current problems, and

future challenges. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 745-778.

Buhrmester, M. D., Kwant, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A

source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? Perspectives on Psychological Science,

6, 3-5.

Castano, E., Yzerbyt, V., Bourguignon, D., & Seron, A. (2002). Who may enter? The

impact of in-group identification and in-group/out-group categorization. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 315-322.

Page 48: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

42

Commins, B., & Lockwood, J. (1979). The effects of status differences, favored

treatment, and equity on intergroup comparisons. European Journal of Social

Psychology, 9, 281-289.

Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. (1990). Collective self-esteem and ingroup bias. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 60-67.

Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and

intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2008), 353-380.

De Cremer, D., van Vugt, M., & Sharp, J. (1999). Effect of collective self-esteem on

ingroup evaluations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139(4), 530-532.

Ellemers, N., & van den Bos, K. (2012). Morality in groups: On the social-regulatory

functions of right and wrong. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(12),

878-889.

Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different

sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5),

1029-1046.

Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to

moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814-834.

Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316, 998-1002.

Haidt, J. (2008). Morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 65-72.

Haidt, J. (2013). Moral psychology for the twenty-first century. Journal of Moral

Education, 42(3), 281-297.

Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral

intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98-116.

Halvey, N., Bornstein, G., & Sagiv, L. (2008). “In-group love” and “out-group hate” as

motives for individual participation and intergroup conflict. Psychological Science,

19(4), 405-411.

Halvey, N., Weisel, O., & Bornstein, G. (2012). “In-group love” and “out-group hate” in

repeated interaction between groups. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25,

188-195.

Harvey, R. D., & Oswold, D. L. (2000). Collective guilt and shame as motivation for

White support of Black programs. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 72, 1268-

1283.

Page 49: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

43

Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of

Psychology, 53, 575-604.

Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the

moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 963-

976. doi:10.1037/a0017423

Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2012). Disgusting smells cause decreased liking

of gay men. Emotion, 12, 23-27. doi: 10.1037/a0023984

Joyce, R. (2014). The origins of moral judgment. Behaviour, 151(2-3), 261-278.

Kaiser, C. R., & Major, B. (2004). Judgments of deserving and the emotional

consequences of stigmatication. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach (Eds.), The social

life of emotions (pp. 270-291). New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:

10.1017/CB09780511819568.015

Killen, M., Margie, N., & Sinno, S. (2006). Morality in the context of intergroup

relationships. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development

(pp. 155-183). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Kitcher, P. (2014). Is a naturalized ethics possible? Behaviours, 151, 245-260.

Leach, C., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of morality

vs. competence and sociability in the evaluation of in-groups. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 93(2), 234-249.

Lieberman, D., & Linke, L. (2007). The effect of social category on third party

punishment. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(1), 289-305.

Linke, L. H. (2012). Social closeness and decision making: Moral, attributive and

emotional reactions to third party transgressions. Current Psychology: A Journal for

Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 31(3), 291-312.

doi:10.1007/s12144-012-9146-1

Marques, J. M., Abrams, D., & Serodio, R. G. (2001). Being better by being right:

Subjective group dynamics and derogation of in-group deviants when generic norms

are undermined. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(3), 436-447.

Marques, J. M., Paez, D., & Abrams, D. (1998). Social identity and intergroup

differentiation as subjective social control. In J. F. Morals, D. Paez, J. C. Deschamps,

& S. Worchel (Eds.), Current perspectives on social identity and social

categorization (pp. 124-141). New York: Sage.

Page 50: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

44

Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J. (1988). The ‘Black Sheep Effect’:

Extremity of judgments towards ingroup members as a function of group

identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(1), 1-16.

doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420180102

McKee, S., & Masicampo, E. J. (2014). The effects of perpetrator closeness on judgments

of moral violations. Unpublished data.

Parker, M. T., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (2013). Lessons from morality-based social identity:

The power of outgroup “hate,” not just ingroup love. Social Justice Research, 26,

81-96. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0175-6

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance

orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741-763.

Schiller, B., Baumgartner, T., & Knoch, D. (2014). Intergroup bias in third-party

punishment stems from both ingroup favoritism and outgroup discrimination.

Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(3), 169-175.

doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.12.006

Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness

reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19, 1219-1222.

Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of

Psychology, 33, 1-39.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of inter-group behavior. In J.

Jost (Ed.), Political psychology: Key readings (pp. 276-293). New York: Psychology

Press.

Van Prooijen, J. W. (2006). Retributive reactions to suspected offenders: The importance

of social categorizations and guilt probability. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 32(6), 715-726.

Van Prooijen, J. W. (2009). Offenders’ social categorization: Ingroup bias or black sheep

effect? In M. E. Oswald, S. Bieneck, & J. Hupfeld-Heinemann (Eds.), Social

psychology of punishment of crime (pp. 211-229). New York: Wiley.

Wohl, M. A., & Reeder, G. D. (2004). When bad deeds are forgiven: Judgments of

morality and forgiveness for intergroup aggression. In J. P. Morgan (Ed.), Focus on

aggression research (pp. 59-74). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

Page 51: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

45

Appendix A

Morality Scenarios

Neutral Acts

1. Person purchased a hammock for the yard

2. Separated whites and colors when doing laundry

3. Gets 7 hours of sleep every night

4. Person listened to the radio in the car

5. Goes to see a movie every Friday

6. Person purchased new phone

7. Goes for a drive every Sunday afternoon

Positive Purity Acts

1. Person went to a religious service for the holidays

2. Refused an offer to try cocaine by a friend at a concert

3. Person never uses profanity

4. Abstained from sex until married

5. Person has never looked at pornography

6. Washed their hands after going to a petting zoo

7. Fasted for a week for religious reasons

Positive Care Acts

1. Volunteered at a local homeless shelter for a couple hours

2. Leaves food outside for a stray cat everyday

3. Helped build a house with Habitat for Humanity

4. Volunteered to help when a co-worker needed a ride home

5. Donated a bike to a family in need

6. Gave change to a panhandler

7. Babysat a neighbor’s child during their neighbors’ special event

Negative Purity Acts

1. Had casual sex with someone they have known less than an hour

2. Accepted money from someone in exchange for sleeping with them

3. Thawed a frozen turkey, had sex with it, and then washed it, cooked it, and ate it

for dinner

4. Collected and ate roadkill

5. Was babysitting for a co-worker and drank some leftover breast milk that the

baby didn’t want

Page 52: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

46

6. Drank their own pee to see how it tasted

7. Had sex with a blowup animal

Negative Care Acts

1. Made fun of a person’s clothes behind their back

2. Called someone a mean name behind their back

3. Didn’t help a friend move when that friend sent out a group e-mail soliciting help

4. Got mad at a child for spilling a drink on them

5. Threw an acorn at a dog for no reason

6. When given five extra dollars in change at the store, kept the five dollars instead

of giving it back

7. Hit someone’s mailbox accidentally while texting and driving

Page 53: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

47

Curriculum Vitae

Stephanie E. McKee [email protected]

ACADEMIC HISTORY

2013-2015 M.A. Psychology

Wake Forest University, Winston Salem NC

2009 B.A. Psychology, Summa Cum Laude with Distinction

Sonoma State University, Rohnert Park CA

PUBLICATIONS

Smith, H. J., & McKee, S. (2015). Reality and perception: Power and paranoia within the

political sphere [Review of the book Power, Politics, and Paranoia]. Social

Justice Research, doi: 10.1007/s11211-015-0238-6

McKee, S., Smith, H. J., Koch, A., Balzarini, R., Georges, M., & Callahan, M. P. (2013).

Looking up and seeing green: Women’s everyday experiences with physical

appearance comparisons. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(3), 351-365.

TALKS

McKee, S. (2015, April). The effects of perpetrator closeness on judgments of moral

violations. Carolina Research in Social Psychology, Duke University, Durham,

NC.

McKee, S. (2015, March). The effects of group membership on judgments of moral

transgressions. Seminar in Self-Regulation, Wake Forest University, Winston-

Salem, NC.

McKee, S. (2014, October). The effects of social closeness on moral transgressions.

Seminar in Self-Regulation, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC.

McKee, S. (2014, May). The effects of perpetrator closeness on judgments of moral

purity violations. Psychology Department Colloquium Series - First-Year

Presentations, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC.

PRESENTATIONS

McKee, S., & Masicampo, E. J. (2015, February). The effects of perpetrator closeness on

judgments of moral purity violations. Poster presented at the Society for

Personality and Social Psychology’s Annual Conference, Long Beach, CA.

Page 54: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

48

McKee, S., & Masicampo, E. J. (2014, October). The effects of perpetrator closeness on

moral purity violation judgments. Poster presented at the Society of Southeastern

Social Psychologists Annual Conference, Athens, GA.

McKee, S., Nethels, J., & Masicampo, E. J. (2014, June). Political differences in

perceptions of minority progress. Poster presented at the Biennial Conference of

the International Society for Justice Research, New York University, NY.

McKee, S., Nethels, J., & Masicampo, E. J. (2014, May). Temporal framings and

perceptions of minority progress. Poster presented at the American Psychological

Society’s Annual Conference, San Francisco, CA.

Woychik, M., Geroges, M., McKee, S., Pesseran, E., Callahan, M., & Smith, H. J. (2014,

April). When forgiveness fails: Observers’ judgments of an ingroup victim’s

reaction to an insult. Poster presented at the Western Psychological Association

Annual Convention, Portland, Oregon.

McKee, S., Georges, M., Smith, H. J. (2014, February). Age differences moderate the

impact of physician behavior on observers’ attitudes. Poster presented at the

annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Austin,

Texas.

Georges, M., Ryan, D., Snyder, A., Solari, M., Pasion-Caiani, T., Balzarini, R., McKee,

S., Goode, C., & Smith, H. J. (2012, April). Observers’ reactions to infidelity:

Victims who forgive damage ingroup’s reputation. Poster session presented at the

annual meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Burlingame,

California.

Koch, A., Balzarini, R. N., Georges, M., & McKee, S. (2011, May). Looking up and

feeling blue: Antecedents and consequences of women’s physical appearance

comparisons. Paper presentation at the annual Society and Culture Undergraduate

Research Forum, Rohnert Park, California.

Weil, J., Grenier, G., Balzarini, R. N., Villagomez, T., Bellagio, P., McKee, S., & Smith,

H. J. (2011, April). Feeling good but doing little: Value affirmation after

witnessed injustice. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Western

Psychological Association, Los Angeles, California.

Koch, A., Balzarini, R. N., Georges, M., McKee, S., & Smith, H. J. (2011, April). Social

comparison orientation, physical appearance comparisons and the importance of

physical appearance. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the

Western Psychological Association, Los Angeles, California.

Page 55: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

49

Wilson, J., McKee, S., Box, P., & Smith, H. J. (2009, April). Diverse close friends,

everyday social comparisons and physical appearance importance. Poster

presentation at the annual meeting of the Western Psychological Association,

Portland, Oregon.

MANUSCRIPTS IN PREPARATION

McKee, S., & Smith, H. J. (in preparation). When age matters: An experimental test of

perceived doctor-participant age gaps and willingness to comply.

McKee, S., Masicampo, E. J., & Nethels, J. (in preparation). Political differences in

perceptions of minority progress.

RESEARCH EXPERIENCE

2015-Present Research Intern/Lab Manager

University of Iowa, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: C. Daryl Cameron, Ph.D.

2013-2015 Graduate Assistant

Wake Forest University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: E. J. Masicampo, Ph.D.

2010-2013 Lab Manager

Sonoma State University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Heather Smith, Ph.D.

2008-2010 Research Assistant

Sonoma State University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Heather Smith, Ph.D.

TEACHING EXPERIENCE

Spring 2015 Head Graduate Lab Instructor, Research Methods and Statistics

Wake Forest University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Lara Kammrath, Ph.D.

Fall 2014 Head Graduate Lab Instructor, Research Methods and Statistics

Wake Forest University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Eric Stone, Ph.D.

Fall 2013-Sp 2014 Graduate Lab Instructor, Research Methods and Statistics II

Wake Forest University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Lara Kammrath, Ph.D.

Page 56: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

50

2009 Teaching Assistant, Abnormal Psychology

Sonoma State University, Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Maria Hess, Ph.D.

GRANTS

2015 Summer Research Support Grant, Wake Forest University

2014 Alumni Travel Award, Wake Forest University

2014 Summer Research Support Grant, Wake Forest University

2014 Travel Grant, Association for Psychological Science

2013-2015 Full Tuition Award, Wake Forest University

2008 Undergraduate Research Travel Grant, Sonoma State

HONORS & AWARDS

2014 College Teaching Preparation Program, Wake Forest University

2009 Distinction Award, Psychology Department, Sonoma State

2009 Summa Cum Laude, Sonoma State University

2005-2008 Dean’s List, Sonoma State University

2005 Honors at Entrance, Sonoma State University

SERVICE

2013-2015 Representative, Graduate Student Association, Wake Forest

2007-Present Member, Psi Chi National Honor Society, Sonoma State

2008-2009 Vice-President & Treasurer, Psi Chi, Sonoma State University

SKILLS & PROFECIENCY

SPSS

Qualtrics

Integrative Complexity Qualitative Scoring

Aplia Interactive Teaching Software

Microsoft Office including Word, Excel and PowerPoint

DirectRT

MediaLab

PROFESSIONAL AFFILIATIONS

2014-Present Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues

2014-Present Southeastern Society of Social Psychology

2014-Present International Society of Justice Researchers

2013-Present American Psychological Science

2012-Present American Psychological Association

Page 57: THE EFFECTS OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON JUDGMENTS OF …

51

2012-Present American Psychological Association of Graduate Students

2012-Present Society for the Psychology of Women

2012-Present Society for Personality and Social Psychology

2009-Present Western Psychological Association

REFERENCES

Heather Smith, Ph.D., Professor, School of Social Sciences, Department of Psychology,

Sonoma State University, 1801 East Cotati Avenue, Rohnert Park, CA, 94928

Voice: (707) 664-2587; e-mail: [email protected]

E.J. Masicampo, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, School of Social Sciences, Department of

Psychology, Wake Forest University, 415 Greene Hall, P.O. Box 7778 Reynolda

Station, Winston Salem, NC, 27109. Voice: (336) 758-6192; email:

[email protected]

Lara Kammrath, Ph.D., Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, 415 Greene

Hall, P.O. Box 7778 Reynolda Station, Winston Salem, NC, 27109. Voice: (630)

608-3891; email: [email protected]