the effects of hypoxia and hypercapnla on … · paper or electronic formats. the author retains...

150
THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON HAMSTER ACTIVITY IRHYTHMS by Tim M. Jarsky A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Science, Graduate Department of Zoology, in the University of Toronto O Copyright by Tim Jarsky 1997

Upload: hoangkien

Post on 27-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA

ON

HAMSTER ACTIVITY IRHYTHMS

by

Tim M. Jarsky

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Science, Graduate Department of Zoology, in the University of Toronto

O Copyright by Tim Jarsky 1997

Page 2: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Bibliographie Services services bibliographiques

395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington Ottawa ON KI A ON4 Ottawa ON K I A ON4 Canada Canada

Your hie Votre reierence

Our file Notre réiérence

The author has granted a non- exclusive licence allowing the National Libraq of Canada to reproduce, loan, distribute or sel1 copies of this thesis in microfom, paper or electronic formats.

The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts fkom it may be printed or otherwise reproduced without the author's pemission.

L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive permettant a la Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou vendre des copies de cette thèse sous la forme de microfiche/filrn, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique.

L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés ou autrement reproduits sans son autorisation.

Page 3: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) were exposed to 3 h of constant hypoxia (O,

concentration = 5%), episodic hypoxia (O, concentration = 17-5%), or constant

hypercapnia (CO, concentration = 15%) at CT6, CT 1 1 ancilor CT 15 to deterrnine if a phase

shift in activity rhythrns is induced by a respiratory stimulus. Hamsters were kept in a

14: 10 LD cycle before the pulse and at the onset of a respiratory stimulus they were

released into constant darkness (DD). A separate experiment rneasuring metabolism

(oxygen consumption; V0,) and inspiratory ventilation (V,) using open flow respirometry

and whole-body plethysmography, respectively, demonstrated that al1 chernical stimuli

were powerful respiratory stimuli. Constant hypoxia caused 0.2 If: 0.06 h, 0.3 + 0.08 h,

0.4 f O. 1 h phase delays in hamster activity rhythms at CT6, CT11 and CT15,

respectively, when the first day post pulse was used to calculate phase shifts. However,

when the constant hypoxia experiment was repeated at CT6 and a post-pulse regression line

was used to caiculate the phase shifi no phase change was induced. Constant hypoxia given

on 3 consecutive days at CT6 caused a 0.68 f 0.18 h phase delay using both calculation

methods. Episodic hypoxia caused a 0.16 f 0.06 h phase delay at CT15. Hypercapnia iiid

not alter activity rhythms. Apparent phase delays obtained using the first day post pulse for

phase shift calculation were strongly correlated with reductions in activity levels on the day

of the pulse. These results suggest that the phase delays in the circadian rhythm of activity

induced by the different hnds of hypoxia are mediated by acute decreases in activity levels.

It is concluded that respiratory stimuli have little or no direct influence on circadian rhythms

in activity in hamsters. Episodic hypoxia did not induce long tenn facilitation (LTF) or

phase advances as hypothesized. Absence of LTF may represent an adaptation to a

burrowing lifestyle.

Page 4: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

I thank, Sara Shettleworth, Rob Hampton, Rick We~itwood and C. Cammeron, for

having given me the assistance 1 required to lay the foundation for acceptance into the

M.Sc. program.

I thank Dr. Nicholas Mrosovsky for his technical support and useful comrnents on

the manuscript, Matthew Godfrey for his computer support, Kasia Bobrzynska, Peggy

Salmon and Jon Stone for their invaluable help-

Finally, 1 thank Richard Stephenson for his patient assistance and careful

supervision.

III

Page 5: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

'l'able of Contents

Abstract

Acknowledgements

Table of Contents

List of Figures

1 Introduction Review of Circadian Rhythm

Photic Influences on Circadian Clocks 3 Non-Photic Influences in Circadian Clocks 7 Mechanism of Non-photic Phase Shifts 9

Review of Respiration Respiratory Neuroanatomy 1 1 Sensory Input 14 Responses to Hypoxia 14

A. Ventilation 14 B. Long-Terrn Facilitation 19 C. Arousal State 20 D. Hypometabolism and Thermogenesis 2 1

Responses to Hypercapnia 22

Circadian Rhythms in Respiration A. Sleep 23 B. Metabolism 24 C. Therrnogenesis 24 D. Exercise 24 E. Daily Rhythms in Respiration by Masking 25

Can a Respiratory Stimuli Phase-Shift the SCN

Rationale for the Feedback Hypothesis A. Direct Feedback 27 B. Indirect Feedback 28

Experimental Animal Selection

Experimental Test Selection

Respiratory Stimuli Selection Respiratory Stimuli Response Characterization 30

2 Materials and Methods Effect of Respiratory Stimuli on Hamster Activity Rhythms

Animals and Housing 3 1 Data Recording 34 Data Analysis 34

Part 1. Aschoff Type II Experimental Design 34

II

III

IV

vi1

Page 6: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

L Y

Part 3 Activity Levels 35 ' ' Part 4 Statistical Analysis of Actogram Data 35

Non-Photic Stimuli Novelty-Induced Wheel-Running 35 Cage Change 36 Respiratory Stimuli 36 Visual Observations of Behavior 37 Hypoxia 37 Additive Effect of Hypoxia on Phase Shifts 38 Episodic Hypoxia 38 Hypercapnia 4 1

Respiratory Responses to Hypoxic and Hypercapnic Pulses Apparatus Design 41 Calculation of Ventilatory Parameters 44 Calculation of Metabolic Rate 45 Experimental Protocol 46 Statistical Analysis of Respiratory Data 49

3 Results Non-Photic Stimuli

Respiratory Control Experiment

Hypoxia 1 ,a Respiratory Responses 60 1 ,b Wheel Running Rhythms 64 1 ,c.Activity 65

Consecutive Days of Hypoxia 2,a Wheel Running Rhythms 65 2,b Activity 65

Episodic Hypoxia 3,a Respiratory Responses 65 3,b Wheel Running Rhythm 80 3,c Activity 88

Hypercapnia 4,a Respiratory Responses 88 4,b Wheel Running Rhythms 94 4,c Acitivity 94

Activity Change vs. Phase Change

4 Discussion Standard Non Photic and Respiratory Stimuli Constant Hypoxia Consecutive Days of Hypoxia Hypoxia as an Inducer of Phase Shifts Episodic Hypoxia Hypercapnia

Page 7: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -- ------------ - - - ------

Respiratory Stimuli and Activity Future Experiments Critique of Method Conclusion

5 References

6 Appendix 1

Page 8: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Figure Page

Phase Response Curve

Main groups of respiratory neurons

Metabolic and ventiiatory response to sustained hypoxia --

Sealed animal chamber

Episodic hypoxia circuit diagram

Six channel open-circuit plethysmograph

Sample plethysmograph recording

Novelty -induced w heel-ninning actogram

Cage change phase shift

Lights out phase shift

Air respiratory measurements

Constant hypoxia respiratory measurements

Constant hypoxia phase shifi

Constant hypoxia actogram, 1 st day post pulse

Constant hypoxia regression vs. 1 st day post pulse analysis

Constant hypoxia actogram, regression

Constant hypoxia activity change

Consecutive days hypoxia phase shift

Consecutive days hypoxia actogram

Page 9: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Episodic hypoxia phase shift

Episodic hypoxia actograrn

Episodic hypoxia activity change

Hypercapnia respiratory measurements

Hypercapnia phase shift

Hypercapnia Ac tograrn

Hypercapnia activity change

Activity change vs. Phase change

Body temperature vs. Nasal temperature

Page 10: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

This study is one in a series designed to determine if and how the circadian systern

interacts with respiration. The purpose of this study was to determine whether a respiratory

stimulus can phase shift the circadian pacemaker, the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), in

hamsters. It is known that respiration is controlled by a system with multiple levels involving

simple stimulus response reactions (feedback control) and more complex central command of

rhythm generation and sensory information integration (feedforward control). However, the

possibility that respiratory control could also involve moduIation by the circadian timing system

has rarely been considered.

The rationale for this study is based on work in rats, humans and ducks which exhibit a

time-of-day dependent sensitivity to a respiratory stimulus (Peever and Stephenson 1997,

Raschke and Moller 1989, Woodin and Stephenson 1998). This finding suggests that the

respiratory system rnay experience modulation by the circadian timing system. If respiration is

under circadian control it raises the possibility that a respiratory stimulus could affect the clock

as it has been shown that manipulation of many dock controlled variables affect clock function

(Boulos and Rusak 1982, Mrosovsky 1988, Reebs and Mrosovsky 1989, Mrosovsky and

Salmon 1989). Thus, the following experimental objectives were developed:

1. To determine if continuous hypoxia (5%), episodic hypoxia (5- l7%), and

hypercapnia (15%) can phase shift the activity rhythms in the golden hamster.

2. To characterize the respiratory responses to hypoxia (5%), episodic hypoxia

(5-17%), and hypercapnia (15%) in the hamster.

The focus of this introduction is to review the relevant literature on rnamrnalian circadian

rhythms and respiratory control. The evidence which indicates that respiration is Iikely to be

subject to circadian control, the rationale for hypothesizing that respiratory outputs might

feedback on the circadian pacemaker, and the practical implications of such feedback are also

Page 11: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

experimental animal and respiratory stimuli. Where possible, anatomical, physiological, and

behavioral data are integrated in an attempt to illuminate possible mechanisms of respiratory

feedback on the SCN.

REVIEW OF CIRCALIIAN RHYTHMS

Circadian rhythms are physiological processes andor behavioral systems that oscillate

under constant environmental conditions with a penod close to, but rarely exactly, 24 hours.

The timing of such behaviors or physiological processes is controlled by an internal clock

(endogenous pacemaker). The endogenous pacemaker has two principal properties: it is

normally synchronized (entrained) to the light-dark cycle but, in the absence of temporal cues, it

'free runs' with a period approximating 24 hours (Moore 1995). Endogenous control of

physiological and behavioral systems confers the ability to anticipate environmental changes

aliowing an organism to avoid dangers and exploit the advantages of events such as the day-

night cycle and the seasons of the year (Foster 1993). Therefore, a major action of the

environment is to entrain the internal system to a period of precisely 24 hours, so that it and the

expressed rhythms that it controls will be in an adaptive temporal relationship with the day-

night cycle (Menaker et ai. 1978).

Three basic elements are necessary in order for an organism to predict dawn and dusk.

The organisrn must have an internal timer, producing a stable self-sustained oscillation. The

internal time signa1 must be entrained by an environmental time signal (Zeitgeber). Finally, the

temporal information provided by the clock must eventually be transduced into a signal that can

drive the appropriate rhythms in physiology and behavior in a precise manner.

In fact, precision is one of the most striking features of biological clocks. The accuracy is

evident in rodents which have an inclination for exercise on mnning wheeIs. The time at which

rodents begin and continue exercise (activity onset) can have a standard error which can

sometimes be about a minute, from day to day, under constant conditions. In other words, the

Page 12: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

r i i r ViUbL VI luUV \I IIIGIIUIIg~~ 17UJ). ~ V W G V G I , auivlLy unser can De airerea DY

photic and non-photic events resulting in activity onset delays (phase delays) and activity onset

advances (phase advances). Since light is the dominant synchronizing agent of biological

clocks, most early experimental work has ernployed manipulation of light cycles to study phase

changes in activity (Menaker et al. 1978).

Photic Influences on Circadian Clocks

The prirnary strategy used to understand the influence of Iight on the dock has been the

administration of light pulses to animals, usually nocturnal animals, kept in constant darkness

while monitoring wheel mnning activity. The predicted time of activity onset on the day of the

pulse is subtracted from the time of activity onset on the day following the pulse to give the

immediate phase change. The size of the phase change depends upon the time of day at which

the light pulse is presented. This is illustrated by a phase response curve (PRC; Fig. 1.1). Photic

stimulation produces no change of phase if the pulse is given in the hamster's subjective day.

However, light pulses evoke phase delays when presented in early evening, and phase advances

when given late in the subjective night (Summer and McCormack 1984). The magnitude of

light-induced phase shifts also depends on intensity, wavelength, and duration of light exposure

(Takahashi et al. 1984, Nelson and Takahashi 199 1). The effect of both phase advances and

phase delays in response to light is to keep activity confined to the dark portion of the light-dark

cycle in nocturnal animals.

The anatomical route by which Iight modifies the circadian pacemaker in mammals starts

at the eye. Al1 vertebrates, including mammals, appear to have specialized non-visual (Le. not

involved in spatio-temporal imaging) photoreceptors. In mammals these photoreceptors are

located in the retina (Foster 1993). This specialized population of photic receptors mediates

photic entrainment of the SCN (Moore 1995). The SCN is the site of the master oscillator or

clock and its function is the generation of circadian rhythms (Rusak and Zucker 1979, Miller

1993). The SCN is localized in a region of the hypothalamus, just dorsal to the optic chiasma,

Page 13: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

K I U U ~ ~ 1.1 r i iax icq~urist: curve IOT novelty maucea wneei t-unnlng (non-photic) and Iight

pulses (photic). Modified from Mrosovsky et al. 1992 and Takahashi et al. 1984, Shaded regions

indicate times at which respiratory stimuli were given in the present study. By convention in

nocturnal animals CT12 is the tirne at which activity onset occurs.

Page 14: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Circadian Time (h)

Page 15: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

V 4 - - - - -. -- -. - -- --- -- --- - - ---- T m - - m --a '-"" ",""

neurons and associated glia (Miller 1993). The SCN and the retinal cells are connected via the

retinohypothalamic tract (RHT) (Moore and Lenn 1972, Eichler and Moore 1974). The RHT

has several components, however, the largest projection is to the SCN (Johnson et al. 1989). In

addition to the RHT, an indirect projection to the SCN originating in retinorecipient cells of the

intergeniculate leaflet (IGL) of the lateral geniculate nucleus - the geniculohypothalamic tract

(GHT) - is also involved in entraining circadian rhythms to the Iight-dark cycle (Pickard et al.

1987).

The cellular and rnolecular mechanisms by which light causes phase shifts of the SCN

pacemaker are not known. However, recently, clear photic regulation of immediate-earIy gene

expression in the SCN has been identified and has been correlated with the effects of

illumination of the retina. It has been demonstrated that light pulses that induce phase shifts also

induce Fos protein expression in the SCN and IGL regions (Komhauser et al. 1990, Rea 1989,

Rusak et al. 1990). Photic induction of Fos in the SCN is gated by the circadian clock. C-fos is a

proto-oncogene, a member of a farnily of transcription factors (Komhauser et al. 1993) and it

appears to function in cellular signal transduction by coupling transient stimuli to gene

regulation in the nucleus (Muller et al. 1984, Greenberg and Ziff 1984). The discovery of c-fos

within the SCN enables Fos expression to be used as a marker for light cells within the SCN.

In addition to pulses of light, pulses of darkness can phase shift the activity rhythms of

hamsters kept in constant light. Hamsters maintained under constant illumination and exposed

to 2 or 6 hour pulses of darkness undergo phase changes, both transient and long lasting (Boulos

and Rusak 1982). These results were interpreted by the authors to indicate that the PRC for dark

pulses is the mirror image of the PRC for light pulses. However, Reebs et al. (1989)

demonstrated that running activity or a correlated variable mediates the phase-advancing effects

of dark pulses on hamster circadian rhythms. It was demonstrated that dark pulses induce

activity and when activity is blocked during a dark pulse, phase shifts no longer occur or are

minimized (Reebs et al. 1989, Van Reeth and Turek 1989). Therefore dark pulses may affect the

Page 16: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -

clock (Reebs et al. 1989). Therefore dark pulses may be considered as non-photic rather than

photic stimuli, as far as the dock is ccincerned.

Non-photic Influences on circadian clocks.

In addition to environmental stimuli such as light, behavioral experiences (non-photic

stimuli) are capable of altering the phase of circadian rhythrns. The possibility that clock-

controlled variables might have feedback effects has been considered by a number of

investigators, as it seems likely that something controlled by the clock could also affect the

clock. Sexual arousal (Honrado and Mrosovsky 1989), hoarding opportunities (Rusak et al.

1988), novel running wheels (Reebs and Mrosovsky 1989a), triazolam, a benzodiazepine (Turek

1988), dark pulses, social interactions between males and cage changes (Mrosovsky 1988) are

examples of non-photic phase-shifting stimuli. A goal of most early research on non-photic

stimuli was to demonstrate the existence of feedback and assess its strength (Mrosovsky 1996).

The most effective of the above mentioned non-photic stimuli is novelty-induced wheel

running. In the golden hamster three hours of wheel running in the subjective day produces up

to 3-hour phase advances, while smaller phase delays (30 min.) occur if pulses are given in the

subjective night (Fig. 1.1) (Mrosovsky et al. 1992). The shape of the novelty-induced wheel

running PRC is similar, but not necessarily identical, to those of other non-photic stimuli for

which PRC have been plotted.

Activity is a common feature of rnany non-photic stimuli studied. Triazolam, for

example, initiates prolonged bouts of activity. However, when activity is blocked following

injection, phase shifts are greatly reduced (Mrosovsky et al. 1989). For most of these procedures

it has been demonstrated that phase shifts or entrainment usually do not occur if running is

absent or prevented (Mistleberger et al. 1996). Therefore, it is likely that the phase-resetting

Page 17: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Via..Y. .. ..VUIUl r l l J J I V I w E j I ~ ~ ~ b u I L b 1 a L c z UI IIIUULCU ~ L L I V I L Y . nt)wt;Vt;r, IL 1s no1 Known

whether it is activity per se, or some associated variable that is critical in producing the phase

shift.

Many non-photic stimuli have sorne or al1 of the following components in comrnon:

1. Activity must last a Iong tirne (Gannon and Rea 1995). The maximum phase advance for

novelty-induced wheel running often requires as much as three hours of running. This contrasts

with the light PRC where large phase changes can be obtained with relatively short light pulses

of only a few minutes (Mrosovsky 1996).

2. Novelty. The largest phase changes are obtained when the stimulus is used for the first time.

However, novelty may just be a way of evoking arousal or activity (Mrosovsky 1996).

3. Motivation. Mistlberger (1991) suggested that 'the motivational significance of a scheduled

behavioral arousal' and the 'motivational context within which physical activity occurs' may be

important.

4. Reward. A common interpretation is that non-photic stimuli are rewarding. However, the

necessity of reward for non-photic shifting is by no means solidly established (Mrosovsky

1996).

None of the experiments done to date clearly demonstrate the role(s) of any of these

variables in phase-resetting . However, there are circumstances where induced activity has not

caused phase shifts (Sayeski et aI. 1990, Edgar 1991 Janik and Mrosovsky 1993, Mrosovsky

and Biello 1994, De Vries and Meijer, unpublished, cited in Mrosovsky 1995, Mistlberger

1996). These experiments, and particularly the experiment where hamsters which did not run in

a novel wheel ("sluggards") but would run in the cold and did not phase-reset (Janik and

Mrosovsky 1993, Mrosovsky and Biello 1994), have Ied to the hypothesis that it is not activity

but the motivational context which determines phase-resetting. The question of relative

contributions of activity time, novelty, motivation and reward remains unresolved.

Page 18: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

The central neural pathways which mediate non-photic phase shifts are not known.

However, stimuli that induce locomotor activity produce changes in the phase of free-running

activity rhythms, with a PRC that is similar to that produced by stimulation of the

geniculohypothalarnic tract (GHT) (Moore 1994). In addition, phase shifts to non-photic stimuli

are eliminated by lesions of the intergeniculate Ieaflet (IGL) (Johnson et al 1989, Janik and

Mrosovsky 1994, Wickland and Turek 1994). The GHT may be a pathway through which

activity or a correlated variable provides input to the SCN to modulate pacemaker function

(Wickland and Turek 1994).

The IGL receives serotonergic input from the dorsal raphe nucleus (Meyer-Bernstein and

Morin 1996). The dorsal raphe is rich in benzodiazepine receptors (Michels et al. 1990),

therefore, the projection from the dorsal raphe to the IGL may have a rote in mediating non-

photic phase shifts (Meyer-Bernstein and Morin 1996). Neuropeptide Y (NPY) is a major

neurotransmitter of the GHT (Albers and Ferris 1984). The SCN also receives serotonergic

inputs from raphe nuclei (Morin 1994). The serotonergic system apparently provides inhibitory

modulation of the circadian systems response to light (Duncan et al. 1988, Meijer et al. 1988,

Morin and Blanchard 1991) but the roIe of serotonin in non-photic phase shifting is not as clear.

Pharmacological manipulations of serotonergic neurotransmission, using agonists, affects

the free-running pacemaker both in vitro (Prosser et al. 1990, Lovenberg et al. 1993) and in

vivo (Edgar et al. 1993). Specifically, quipazine, a non-specific serotonin agonist, applied to rat

SCN slices produced a PRC that was more similar to that evoked by non-photic stimuli than that

produced by light pulses (Prosser et al. 1990). Also, serotonergic antagonists impair arousal-

induced phase shifts of the circadian system in the Syrian hamster (Sumova et al. 1996).

However, in hamsters quipazine did not phase shift activity rhythms but 8-OH-DPAT, a 5HT 1~

and 5HT7 receptor agonist, did (Bobrzynska et al. 1996a).

The firing rate of serotonergic cells in the dorsal raphe is directly correlated with the

degree of arousal of the animal (Jacobs and Azmitia 1992). Serotonergic terminals are found

Page 19: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - - U

projection to the SCN does not appear to be involved in non-photic phase shifting as phase

shifts persist after 95% serotonin depletion in the hamster SCN (Bobrzynska et al. 1996b).

Despite the fact that serotonin depletion in the SCN does not influence arousal induced phase

shifts, it still remains possible that endogenous serotonergic pathways are mediators of non-

photic entrainment of the circadian system. One possibility is the serotonergic projection from

the dorsal raphe to the IGL (Meyer-Bernstein and Morin 1996). Serotonergic mediation does not

exclude the possibility that other afferent systems also contribute to entrainment by arousal

(Sumova et al. 1996).

Neuropeptide Y transmission from the IGL to the SCN appears to be necessary for at

least one type of non-photic phase shifting (novelty-induced wheel running) in one species

(hamsters) (Biello et al. 1994). Injections of NPY into the SCN produce phase shifts (Albers and

Ferris 1984, Huhman and Albers 1994, Biello and Mrosovsky 1993). These NPY induced phase

shifts are still obtained when hamsters are prevented from being very active (Biello et al. 1994).

NPY antiserum administration to the SCN blocks the phase-advancing effect of novelty-induced

wheel mnning (Biello et al. 1994). Thus, phase-shifts induced by wheel mnning depend on the

NPY projection from the IGL to SCN in hamsters. It remains to be deterrnined how important

NPY and serotonin are in other species or in dock-resetting by other manipulations.

Light pulses that induce phase shifts also induce Fos protein expression in SCN and IGL

regions. However, the role of c-fos in non-photic phase shifting is not clear. Non-photic

manipulations induce Fos expression in the SCN (Amir and Stewart 1996). However, there is no

difference in Fos levels in the SCN across time of day. This indicates that the sensitivity of SCN

cells to non-photic manipulations is not circadian in nature. In contrast to the SCN, the IGL Fos

expression in response to non-photic treatment is phase-dependent. These results suggest that

cells in SCN and IGL respond to several types of non-photic manipulations (Arnir and Stewart

1 996).

Page 20: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - . -. - - - -

A third manipulation exists which has a PRC different from the photic and non-photic

PRC. If hamsters are prevented from being active during their active period it causes phase

delays as large as 45 min.. However, restraint at other times of the day does not induce phase

changes (Van Reeth et al. 199 1). This result suggest that reductions in activity can cause phase

delays.

REVEW OF RESPIRATION

Respira tory Neuroanatomy .

The brain mechanisms controlling breathing in intact mammals are subject to blood and

brain gas and pH values and behavioral state (e.g. emotive, cognitive, or movement related). In

its simplest forrn, neural respiratory control is an oscillatory process that starts with the

altemating activation of inspiratory and expiratory neurons in the brain stem (Eldridge and

Millhorn 1986). The anatomical location of the respiratory controI centers have been fairly well

elucidated (Fig. 1.2). The purpose of this section is to farniliarize the reader with the primary

respiratory brain centers.

The phrenic motor neurons driving the diaphragm, and the intercostal rnotorneurons

driving the intercostal muscles are located in the spinal cord and receive their rhythm mainly

from the medullary respiratory neurons. The isolated medulla oblongata is able to generate a

respiratory rhythm in the absence of peripheral feedback (Feldman and Smith 1995). In the

medulla, respiratory neurons are found in three primary locations, the dorsal respiratory group

(DRG), the ventral respiratory group (VRG), and the motor nuclei of some cranial nerves. The

mechanism by which the basic rhythm is generated is still under intense investigation. In vitro

studies have identified putative respiratory pacemaker cells in the pre-Botzinger complex

(Smith et al. 199 1) but these have not been found in intact adult preparations. Neuronal network

modeis of central rhythm generation are often proposed (eg. Cohen 1979, Duffin 1991) but no

single mode1 has yet received convincing empirical validation.

Page 21: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

location of the main groups of respiratory neurons. NTS nucleus tractus solitarius, DRG dorsal

respiratory group, VRG ventral respiratory group. Modified from Mateika and Duffin (1995).

Page 22: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

POSTERIOR

Page 23: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

numerous inputs from both central and peripheral neural sources (Feldman and Smith 1995).

These sources are concerned with the homeostatic functions of respiration, especially in relation

to regulation of the partial pressures of 0, and CO,, body temperature, and the acid-base balance

of the body fluids. Other important inputs include mechanoreceptors which serve to modulate

the breath to breath activity of respiratory muscles, and the influence of arousal state which can

alter numerous properties of the control system.

Sensory Input.

The partial pressures of oxygen (Pao2) and carbon dioxide (Paco2) in arterial blood are

sensed by the peripheral chemoreceptors located in the carotid and aortic bodies (Fidone and

Gonzales 1986). The former are positioned along the two common carotid arteries and the latter

are located along the aortic arch. They relay information to the NTS in the rnedulla via the

glossopharyngeal andor vagus nerves. The peripheral chemoreceptors are primarily asphyxia

sensors as the sum of the response to hypoxia and hypercapnia administered separately is less

than the response to the same levels of hypoxia and hypercapnia given together (asphyxia)

(Duffin 1990).

When al1 peripheral chemoreceptors are denervated an increase in ventilation in response

to inhaled CO, is still observed (Bruce and Chemiack 1987). This indicates that there are

receptors in the brain which are excited by changes in CO,, called central chemoreceptors. The

central chemoreceptors are thought to be located along the ventrolateral surface of the medulla

in a region called the ventrolateral medullary shell (VMS) (Loeschcke et al. 1970, Mitchell et al.

1963, Schlaefke 198 1) . The VMS is exposed to cerebrospinal fluid (Bruce and Cherniack

1987). The central chemoreceptors probably monitor changes in [H+] in the interstitial fluid of

the brain rather than the direct measurement of CO, levels (Chemiack and Longobardo, 1995).

The changes in the partial pressure of CO, at the central chemoreceptors lag behind those of

arterial blood (Duffin 1990).

Page 24: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

A. Ventilation

Hypoxia is any reduction in the oxygen partial pressure or content of blood. Upon

exposure to hypoxia, for example by inhalation of air low in O,, lung ventilation increases - abruptly then it declines. The latter part of this biphasic response is referred to as the hypoxic

ventilatory decIine (HVD) (Fig. 1.3). The response is dependent on the severity and duratiori of

the hypoxia and is species and body size dependent (Mortola and Gautier 1995). Differences in

HVD among species are evident in animals which live either permanently (fossorial) or

intermittentiy (semifossorial) in burrows. In burrows the gas composition can be substantially

asphyxic (Kuhnen et al. 1987). Fossorial animals such as hamsters tend to have a diminished

response to hypoxia and a blunting of the ventilatory response to hypercapnia. These

characteristics are believed to be genetic traits for which the expression does not depend on

environmental conditions. This conclusion is based on the fact that adult hamsters which have

never been exposed to burrows have the same respiratory response as those raised in burrows

(Mortola 199 1 ).

The acute response to hypoxia is dramatically modified depending on the severity and

duration of hypoxic exposure. For example, more prolonged exposure to hypoxia (hours to

several days) produces a secondary increase in breathing, a phenornenon that has been temed

ventilatory acclimatization to hypoxia (Fig. 1.3) (Bisgard and Neubauer 1995). Metabolic rate,

temperature and the hypocapnia that accompanies hyperventilation, are only a few of the many

variables known to change during hypoxia and influence respiratory drive (Mortola and Gautier

1995).

The mechanisms underlying this complex respiratory response to hypoxia are not fully

understood. However, it is widely accepted that peripheral chemoreceptors are almost

exclusively responsible for the observed initial increase of ventilation, whereas the subsequent

ventilatory attenuation is of central origin and not the result of an alteration in hypoxic

peripheral chemoreceptor discharge (Millhorn et al. 1984, Neubauer et al. 1985,

Page 25: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

responses to sustained moderate or severe hypoxia. The figure is based on long terrn,

intermediate, and short term experiments on different species at ambient temperatures below

thermoneutrality. YE expired minute ventilation, VQ oxygen uptake (rnetabolisrn). Dotted red

line indicates where the metabolic response is variable.

Page 26: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

1 Biphasic Response 1

O min hrs

Time

days

Page 27: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

chemoreceptors with regard to ventilatory stimulation during hypoxia (Bisgard and Neubauer

1995).

When the peripheral chemoreceptors are removed, hypoxia tends to decrease ventilation

(Cherniack et al. 1970, Orr et al. 1975). This occurs because hypoxic blood stimulates an

elevation of cerebral blood Row which in tum washes CO, out of the central chemoreceptor

tissues. This withdraws the CO, ventilatory stimulus from the central chemoreceptors, thus

reducing ventilatory drive. Estimates of the central chemoreceptor sensitivity to CO, can be, on - average, halved because of the cerebral blood flow effect (Duffin 1990).

Several nonspecific neuronal responses to hypoxia are known that could be responsible

for hypoxic ventilatory depression. They can be classified into presynaptic and postsynaptic

events. Presynaptic reductions in neurotransmitter release, reuptake, or synthesis reduce the net

excitatory input required to reach the threshold for neuronal activation. Postsynaptic events

result in an initial transient intracellular aIkalinization that is rapidly followed by an intracellular

acidosis, a small rise in intracellular [ca2+], an increase in potassium conductance, membrane

hyperpolarization, and a reduction in ce11 excitability (Bisgard and Neubauer 1995). In addition,

reductions in O, availability prornote metabolic production of adenosine and lactic acid, which

act as neurornodulators to reduce excitability of CNS neurons (Neubauer et al. 1990, Yan et aI.

1995).

Not al1 areas of the brain are equally susceptible to the depressant action of hypoxia

(Chapman et al. 1979, Tenney and St. John 1980). At certain levels of hypoxia, areas of the

brain that stimulate ventilation may be more depressed than areas that decrease ventilation. Thus

the net ventilatory response to exposures to hypoxia is deterrnined by the time constants of the

stimulating effects of hypoxia on the peripheral chernoreceptors and the depressant effects on

inhibitory and excitatory regions of the brain (Chemiack and Longobardo, 1995). Therefore, it

is hypothesized that the biphasic hypoxic response is the result of those effectors which have

short time constants and powerful stimulatory effects acting early to cause the increase in

Page 28: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

-

effects, resulting in a subsequent reduction in ventilation (Mortola and Gautier 1995). The

central depression affects regions in or above the upper pons rostral to the midbrainfpontine

junction in the rabbit (Martin-Body and Johnston 1988), and may reside in the red nucleus

(Waites et al. 1996).

B . Long-temz Facilitation

An additional affect of hypoxia occurs when hypoxic bouts are given in short succession

(episodic hypoxia). Episodic hypoxia may produce prolonged augmentation of breathing whic h

is maintained for an hour or more following the termination of the stimulus (Millhom et al.

1980a, Cao et al. 1992, Powell and Aaron 1993, Bach et al. 1992, Turner and Mitchell 1997). A

long-lasting increase on ventilatory drive (long-term facilitation) was evoked in awake dogs by

short exposure to repetitive isocapnic hypoxia (Cao et al. 1992). In the anesthetized and

ventilated cat, repeated carotid sinus nerve stimulation induced a progressive increase of

baseline nonnoxic phrenic nerve activity (Milhorn et al. 1980a,b). The hypoxic bouts were of

short duration and therefore the ventilatory response was characteristic of the increase in

ventilation which occurs before the onset of hypoxic ventilatory decline during more prolonged

stimuli. Since the increase in ventilation is due to peripheral chemoreceptor output it is possible

that the sustained high level of ventilation is due at least in part to altered peripheral

chernoreceptor output following stimulation.

The mechanism by which repetitive hypoxic exposure induces an increase in ventilatory

drive is unknown. Tt is possible that either carotid body input or central nervous system hypoxia

may mediate the long-lasting respiratory stimulation after cessation of hypoxic stimulation

(Cao et al. 1992). However, there is evidence that central serotonergic neurons in the raphe

obscurus nucleus of the brain stem could mediate the long-lasting stimulation of respiratory

drive. When a serotonin receptor antagonist was rnicroinjected into the raphe nuclei of the cat

long-terrn facilitation was not evoked (Millhorn et al. 1980b, Bach and Mitchell 1996). Also,

Page 29: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

.VUyLUI.Y I n I a I I V I ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ I L L L ~ U I I ~ U JILUIL L G A ~ I ~ ~ ~ I u u u ~ a u u ~ ~ VL LIIC c A C ~ L ~ S C VCllLllilLULY

response in goats (Henderson et al. 1996). There are many other potential mechanisrns. It is

possible that brief but repetitive exposures to hypoxia rnay raise metabolic rate as a result of

sympathetic nervous system activation (Cao et al. 1992). This in turn would influence normoxic

ventilation. Unfortunately steady state O2 consumption or CO, - production recordings have not

yet been obtained in studies of long-term facilitation.

C . Arousal State

Studies in sleeping humans and other marnrnals have shown that increases in both

hypoxia and hypercapnia can cause arousal from sleep (Bowes et al. 198 1, Gothe et al. 198 1,

Neubauer et al. 198 1). Unless changes in ventilation are great, the ventilatory movements do not

produce awakening by themselves (Neubauer et al. 198 1). In addition, peripheral chemoreceptor

stimulation was shown to evoke a cardiorespiratory response similar to that evoked by

electrical stimulation of the defense arousal areas of the brain, leading to the suggestion that

carotid chemoreceptor stimulation may activate the brainstem defence arousal systern (Marshall

1987). It has been demonstrated in cats and rats anaesthetized with Althesin (alphaxalone-

alphadalone) that carotid chemoreceptor stimulation is one of the many different types of

stimuli that serve as excitatory inputs to the brainstem defense areas (Hilton and Marshall 1982,

Marshall 1984). Stimulation of the defense reaction by carotid body chemoreceptor stimulation

was previously overlooked because the anesthetics used in respiration expenments blocked the

response.

The defense reaction is characterized by hyperventilation, tachycardia, renal and

mesenteric vasoconstriction, skeletal muscle vasodilatation, pupillary dilation, retraction of the

nictitating membrane, and piloerection (Marshall 1987). This set of responses is termed the

visceral alerting response (VAR). The VAR is superimposed on the primary hypoxic response

of bradycardia and vasoconstriction. In the conscious animal the response of carotid body

chemoreceptor stimulation is dependent on the strength of the stimulus and activity already

Page 30: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

accompanying fear and rage reaction can best be understood as adaptations which prepare an

organism to cope with an emergency and specifically to perform the extreme muscular exertion

of flight or attack. Stimulation of the defense arousal areas is an appropriate reaction to hypoxia

as escape from the hypoxic environment is another form of respiratory compensation.

D. Hypometabolism and Thermogenesis

A reduction in 902 occurs with the HVD during hypoxia. The hypoxic hypometabolism

has been observed in cats (Gautier et al. 1989), rats (Gautier et al. 1992), hamsters (Mortola

1991), and many other rodents (Frappell et al. 1992). The reduction in Yo2 during hypoxia is

inversely dependent on body size (Frappell et al. 1992). However, the reduction in 90, during

moderate hypoxia is not a universal occurrence. The +,/Yq ratio (metabolism-specific

exspiratory minute ventilation) is relatively similar among species, and in hypoxia it increases

approximately by the same magnitude (Mortola et al. 1989, Frappell et al. 1992). During

hypoxia the ambient temperature (Tamb) can cause changes in YE and 9% , however, the

VE/?o2 ratio remains constant. For exarnple, hypoxia given at thermoneutrality (25°C) in rats

almost doubles vE and has a minimal hypometabolic effect (Gautier et al. 1992, Saiki et al.

1994). In the cold (5°C) metabolism decreases by approximately 30% and ventilation increases

by only 15% (Fig. 1.3). In both cases the same hypoxic stimulus caused the same

hyperventilation i.e. sarne \j6\102 ratio, but with different combinations of vE and Yq . Cold

hypoxic exposure also reduces the threshold for nonshivering therrnogenesis (Kuhnen et al.

1987) indicating a lowering in the set point for body temperature (Tb). From this information it

has been hypothesized that the hypometabolic effect is due to a reduction in therrnogenesis.

A reduction in thermogenesis can not be the only explanation for the observed hypoxic

hypometabolism as in some species, and particularly in newborns, hypometabolism can

manifest even at themioneutrality. A possible explanation is that O, exerts its effect on

metabolic rate via direct effects on cellular function. A drop in O, supply can interfere with a

Page 31: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- 1 - -

phosphorylation (Mortola and Gautier 1995). Despite the fact that changes in Po, and Tarnb

inf uence many variables, including physiochemical relations determining the acid-base status

of blood and brain tissue, the respiratory output seems to perfectly track the metabolic response.

RESPONSES TO HYPERCAPNIA

Hypercapnia is an increase in the CO, partial pressure of blood. Hypercapnia stimulates

lung ventilation (Chapin 1954, Mortola 199 1, Saki and Mortola 1996). In the human, the

central chernoreceptors mediate a linear increase in ventilation as arterial CO, increases above a

threshold of about 40 mmHg (Duffin 1990). The magnitude and relative contribution of the

carotid body to the CO2 response increases with hypoxia (Cherniack and Longobardo, 1995). In

hamsters, it has been demonstrated that there is a biphasic response of ventilation to

hypercapnia which peaks at an inhaled CO, concentration of approximately 21 % (Chapin 1954).

Hypercapnia has three primary effects which detennine the final respiratory output.

Hypercapnia hyperpolarizes respiratory neurons and thereby decreases their excitability (Wyke

1963). It causes metabolic depression due to acidernia (Stupfel 1974). Finally, hypercapnia

stimulates catecholamine release which induces therrnogenesis (Tenney 1956, Mortola and

Gautier 1995).

CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS AND RESPIRATION

Hypoxia and hypercapnia, through the respiratory system, have a wide range of effects on

other physiological systems. These physiological systerns, such as the sIeep-wake cycle,

metabolism, thermoregulation, also affect respiration. Al1 of these effectors of respiration have

in common a strong interaction with the circadian system. In addition, exercise (activity) is a

behavioral state that has profound effects on respiration and is also under control of the

circadian system. The purpose of this section is to give a brief review of the effect that the sleep

wake cycle, metabolism, thermoregulation, and exercise have on respiration. Al1 these systems

Page 32: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

separation into sections is artificial, purely for the purpose of clarity of presentation.

A. Sleep

In the waking state, automatic and behavioral influences interact to establish the level and

pattern of breathing (Cherniack and Longobardo, 1995). However, during sleep different States

and stages associated with characteristic electroencephalograph (EEG), behavioral, and

physiological changes can affect the controI of breathing. Sleep begins with the slow-wave or

quiet stage. This stage causes significant changes in respiration. Minute ventilation decreases

because of reductions in tidal volume and breathing frequency (Cherniack and Longobardo,

1995). Breathing in slow-wave sleep is thought to be governed almost exclusively by automatic

mechanisms. Absence of environmental stimulation seems to account for the depression of

ventilation observed in slow-wave sieep (Orern 1995, Philipson and Bowes 1986).

In humans, REM sleep replaces slow-wave sleep at intervals of 60-90 min. The REM

sleep has been divided into tonic and phasic periods (Heller 1987). The primary change in

ventilation in REM sleep is an increased variability from breath to breath. The source of this

variability is the specific changes in the brain and brainstem that occur in relationship to the

phasic events of this sleep state (Pack 1995).

In general, sleep is usually associated with decreased ventilation, increased resting levels

of Pacoz, reduced Pao, (Birchfield et al. 1958, Gothe et al. 1981) and causes a reduced

metabolic rate (Brebbia and Altshuler 1965). Studies in humans and diurnal animals show that

metabolic rate falls gradually during sleep, reaches a minimum late in the night, then rises

before awakening (Fraser et al. 1989). The observed fa11 in organismal metabolic rate during

sleep may be caused by a general reduction in tissue rnetabolic rate (Shapiro et al. 1984), or a

state dependent change in a specific mechanism such as muscle activity (Kleitman 1963), or

thermoregulatory set-point (Fraser et al. 1989). An alternative view is that the changes during

sleep are a consequence of circadian variations in metabolic activity (White et al. 1985) or body

Page 33: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

mainly due to the change in arousal state (Fraser et al. 1989).

B. Metabolism

Circadian rhythrns of oxygen consumption (GO,) have been described in white-footed

mice and pocket rnice (Chew et al. 1965, Kayser and Heusner 1967). The daily increase in V O ~

often precedes the daily onset of activity and the onset of food intake (Aschoff and Pohl 1970).

Ventilation levels change accordingly to meet the changing metabolic demands. The rhythm in

v o and the rhythm in body temperature Vary with time of day in a similar fashion. It was

shown in birds as well as in man that when body temperature increased by 0.5 OC VO* increased

by 20-40% during the resting stage of the activity rest cycle (Aschoff and Pohl 1970). However,

during the active phase V O ~ increased by 10-25% in response to a similar change in body

temperature. These results are compatible with the idea that the rhythm of VO, and the rhythm

of body temperature each are controlled by a separate circadian oscillator (Aschoff and Pohl

1970).

C . Themogenesis

Body temperature varies throughout the day according to its own rhythm. The respiratory

system is a major heat loss site, so changes in ventilatory pattern can alter heat loss from the

lung. The daily changes in body temperature are a strong influence on the final ventilatory level.

The underlying mechanisms responsible for the temperature cycle are unknown. It has been

shown that the rhythm in body temperature is not solely due to changes in metabolic rate

(Aschoff 1970, Heller and Glotzbach 1977). In the pigeon, central nervous system mechanisms

controlling arousal States and circadian rhythmicity have separate and additive influences on

temperature regulation (Heller et al. 1983).

Page 34: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Exercise causes an increase in ventilation that is proportional to metabolic rate. YE

increases not only to satisfy the higher metabolic demands but also to increase the respiratory

evaporative heat loss (Mortola and Gautier 1995). Ventilation is increased during exercise by

signals arising from thermal receptors, from proprioceptors in the lirnbs, and from the CNS. The

intensity of such signals is not necessarily proportional to metabolism. Paco2 is held virtually

constant during exercise despite large increases in CO2 production. This could be explained as

an effect on breathing of a signal that is directly coupled to metabolic rate. Because such a

metabolic signal would not be reduced by increasing ventilation, breathing during exercise

might be under substantial feed-forward rather than feedback control (Mateika and Duffin 1995,

Cherniack and Longobardo 1995).

Daily rhythms in respiration by masking

The above mechanisms al1 directly influence respiration. Consequently, the daily rhythms

in these physiological systems could give rise to a daily rhythm in respiration (i.e. masking).

The existence of a underlying rhythm in respiration could only be demonstrated by experiments

which control for these confounding variables.

CAN RESPIRATORY STIMULI PHASE-SHIFT THE SCN?

The following evidence indicates that respiration is a potential candidate for circadian control:

1. Biological clocks coordinate daily changes in thermoregulation, metabolism, sleep-wake

cycle, and activity. These clock-controlled physiological systems and activity have strong

interactions with each other and with respiration.

2. Circadian rhythms have been demonstrated in different cardiovascular parameters ( h e m rate,

blood pressure) (Gautherie 1973, Halberg 1969, Lucente et al. 1987, Mainardi et al. 1987).

Since respiration and cardiovascular systems serve the same primary function and since neural

Page 35: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

circadian control of respiration.

3. It has been deterrnined that materna1 pinealectomy alters the daily pattern of fetaI breathing in

sheep (McMillen et al. 1990). Melatonin is rhythmically released by the pineal gland during the

dark hours and crosses the placenta into fetal circulation providing the fetus with information

about time of day.

4. There is preliminary evidence suggesting that ventilatory chemoreflexes exhibit day-night

changes in rats (Peever and Stephenson 1997), humans (Raschke and Moller 1989), and ducks

(Woodin and Stephenson 1998).

The above discussion considers whether respiration is affected by the circadian timing system. It

is important to emphasize that the above findings are suggestive, but not conclusive, and

experiments have not yet been cornpleted that control for the confounding effects of correlated

rhythms (sleep state, metabolism, activity etc.).

Another unanswered question concerns whether respiration can affect the clock. Phase

changes have been obtained by manipulation of many clock controlled variables (e.g. Aschoff

1965, Eastman 1982, Turek and Gwinner 1982, Rusak et al. 1989, Mrovsovsky 1988).

Furthermore, since humans, rats and ducks exhibit a time of day dependent change in apparent

chemoreceptor sensitivity which suggests the respiratory system may be modulated by the

circadian system, it is possible that respiration could be involved in a feedforward-feedback

loop with the circadian system. The possibility that respiration could affect the clock is not just

of theoretical interest, it also has practical implications, since it raises the possibility that an

experiment on one day may affect the results of the same test at the same time on the next day.

This is not an unreasonable hypothesis as a common characteristic of respiratory experiments is

daily repetition of the protocol and day to day variabiIity in the data.

The experiment reported in this thesis tests the feedback portion of the loop. It was

hypothesized that respiratory stimuli can reset the SCN, as indicated by a phase change in an

overt circadian rhythm.

Page 36: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

RATIONALE FOR THE FEEDBACK HYPOTHESIS

A. Direct Feedback

A study on humans demonstrated that exposure to altitude and hypoxia simultaneously

caused a transient phase shift in several physiological parameters (e.g. peak epiratory air flow,

grip strength, oral terriperature) (Ashkenazi et al. 1982). However, the effects of the altitude

associated reduction in barometric pressure and hypoxia were not separated and therefore the

effect of each stimulus is not distinguishable. Nonetheless, at least one neural pathway exists

between the respiratory system and the SCN making feedback a possibility. The NTS is the

recipient of carotid body chemoreceptor output. The NTS has an efferent to the dorsal raphe

nucleus which primarily releases epinephrine (Hokfelt et al. 1974, Aghajanian and Wang 1977).

The dorsal raphe then have a serotonergic projection to the IGL (Vertes 1991, ~ a t e r h o u s e et al.

1993, Meyer-Bernstein and Morin 1996). It may be through this neural connection that phase

shifts were obtained when humans were exposed to altitude and hypoxia.

An alternative means by which a respiratory stimulus could affect the SCN is that

hypoxia could cause a general reduction in neuronal excitability thereby altering clock function.

It is also feasible for hypercapnia to have a sirnilar effect. Metabolic effects are to be expected

in non-fossorial animals exposed to CO, at concentrations higher than IO%, since an acute fa11

of pH impairs the utilization of glucose and fat by an inhibition of the activity of pH-dependent

enzymes.

In contrat, episodic hypoxia strongly stimulates the defense arousal areas, and rnay

cause general arousal. A common characteristic of non-photic stimuli is that they induce

arousal. In fact, entrainment has been obtained by rattling the bars of bird cages (Reebs et al.

1989), or even dropping cages (Enright 1975), both are situations which would stimulate the

defense reaction. Therefore, it is plausible that episodic hypoxia rnight produce a PRC sirnilar to

that of other non-photic stimuli. Since hypercapnia also stimulates carotid body chemoreceptors,

it may also stimulate the defense reaction.

Page 37: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Y . - . I C I * , - U t . C L i U U U b I b

The sleep-wake cycle, thermoregulation, and metabolism are al1 influenced by respiratory

stimuli (MortoIa and Gautier 1995). Both hypoxia and hypercapnia can disrupt sleep patterns

(Neubauer et al. 1981, Marshall 1987). As well, hypoxia can cause a reduction in body

temperature and metabolic rate (Kuhnen et al. 1987, Mortola 1991). Hypoxia and hypercapnia

may also affect the level of activity due to the stress placed on the cardiovascular system (Aaron

et al. 1992). This effect may be particularly apparent if the stimulus falls within the daily

activity period. A change in activity may in turn reset the SCN (Reebs et al. 1989). If the

behavioral entrainment system is non-specific, as has been suggested (Mrosovsky et al. 1989), it

would be expected that a variety of behavior altering stimuli like hypoxia and hypercapnia

would be capable of producing phase shifts. It is evident that the respiratory stimuli have a wide

range of effects on physiologicaI systems and behaviors and the respiratory stimuli can

potentially affect the SCN via these systems and not directly.

Obviously, there are many alternative mechanisms by which the respiratory system

could affect the SCN and if feedback occurs further experiments will be required to detennine

the mechanism. The primary purpose of the present study is to test for the existence of such

feedback in the respiratory and circadian system of the hamster.

EXPERIMENTAL ANIMAL SELECTION

The golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus) was chosen for this experirnent because they

have high cycle to cycle circadian timing accuracy so that small phase shifts can be identified.

Hamsters do, however, present a disadvantage due to their respiratory adaptations to a fossorial

lifestyle. The relative insensitivity of their respiratory system to hypoxia and hypercapnia

requires that the respiratory stimuli be rnuch more severe than would be used for non-fossorial

rodents.

Page 38: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

There are two prirnary methods by which PRCs are obtained. The Aschoff Type 1 (AI)

(Aschoff 1965), the most commonly used of the two methods, has the animals free running in

constant conditions (DD or LL) and a pulse (eg. light, wheel running, respiratory stimulus) is

then given at the desired circadian time and the phase change is measured. However, a practical

disadvantage of this technique is that it is difficult to give pulses at the same CT to many

anirnak as activity onsets are spread out over the day because individual animals have different

free running periods.

The Aschoff Type II test (AU) (Aschoff 1965) was the test used in this study because it

enables pulses to be given to many animals at the same CT. Animals are at similar CTs because

prior to administration of the pulse they are entrained to a LD cycle (Moore-Ede 1982). At the

onset of the pulse anirnals enter constant conditions. Allowance must be made for the effects on

the circadian system caused by the release from LD to constant conditions (DD or LL). The

transition from LD to DD commonly causes phase advances of about 30 min. (Mrosovsky and

Salmon 1990, Mrosovsky 199 1, Janik and Mrosovsky 1993). Thus, a disadvantage of this

technique is that two variables are manipulated simultaneously. This problem is overcome by

comparing the resultant phase change with that from control studies in which the animal was

released from the same LD cycle into constant conditions but with no pulse given. Phase

changes are interpreted relative to the control. For example, if a pulse attenuates the phase

advance obsemed in controls, it is inferred that it caused a phase delay.

RESPIRATORY STIMULI SELECTION

In choosing the respiratory stimuli three parameters need to be taken into account: type,

level, and duration. Three hours was chosen as the standard duration of al1 the respiratory

stimuli because non-photic stimuli tend to be long-lasting (Mrosovsky 1996). Also, in hamsters

the largest phase changes for novelty-induced wheel running are obtained after about three

hours of continuous running (Reebs et al. 1989). Continuous hypoxia, episodic hypoxia, and

Page 39: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

discussed earlier.

Pulse levels of O2 and CO, were based on hamster burrow concentrations. For the

changes in 0, and CO2 levels to be considered respiratory stimuli it was assurned that they must - be greater than what a hamster would normally encounter in the burrow. Hamster burrow

concentrations of CO, can be as high as 10.8% (Kuhnen et al. 1987). Ventilation in the hamster

has been shown to increase steadily from 0% to 21 % CO, (Chapin 1954, Walker et al. 1985,

Mortola 1991). Based on this information and the fact that the CO, analyzer had a upper

working lirnit of 15.9%, 15% was the chosen level of hypercapnia. Burrow O, concentration has

been measured at 10% (Kuhnen et al. 1987). Therefore, 5% 0, was deemed to be a sufficient

stimulus. Although 0, and CO, pulse levels appear significantly more extreme than burrow

levels, this is not the case, because in the burrow the conditions are asphyxie. Asphyxia is a

much stronger respiratory stimulus than 0, and CO, administered separately.

Respiratory Stimuli Response Characterization

The respiratory responses to each of the stimuli were recorded. This was done to confirm

that the pulse was in fact stimulating the respiratory system. Furtherrnore, knowledge of the

respiratory response might help in the interpretation of the activity rhythms data.

Page 40: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

V I I U ~ - ~ ~ ri

Materials and Methods

Effect of respiratory stimuli on hamster activity rhythms

Anirnals and Housing

Eighteen male virus-free golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) aged 42 days were

obtained frorn Harlan Sprague-DawIey (Indianapolis, IN). They were housed singly in plastic

cages (45 X 25 X 20 cm) with 17.5 cm diameter running wheels. The running surface of the

wheels was covered with 4 mm plastic mesh. Temperature during expenments was

20 + 2°C. Purina 5001 pellets and water were available ad libitum. Routine maintenance, when

necessary, was perforrned quietly. Bi-weekly cage changes entailed providing the animals with a

clean cage containing wood chips (N.E. Products Corp. Hardwood laboratory bedding), food and

water.

Nine hamster cages were housed in each of two sealed animal charnbers (Fig. 2.1). The

chambers were impermeable to light. Two 2.44 m cold white fluorescent bulbs provided

approximately 140 lux within the chamber during lights-on. Light levels were measured with a

Gossen, Lunasix 3 light meter. Light function within the chamber was monitored by a photocell

which, when activated by light, caused a pen deflection on an Esterline-Angus event recorder.

The animal chambers were ventilated continuously at a rate sufficient to maintain CO,

concentration less than 0.5%. The air or test gas was distributed evenly within the chamber via a

multiport ventilation duct and four mixing fans. Gas exhaust ports were designed to rninimize

pressure fluctuations when gas flow rates were adjusted and to prevent light from entering the

chamber. However, the high gas flow rates required to rapidly raise and lower oxygen or carbon

dioxide concentrations were observed to cause srnaIl( 12 mrnH,O) increases in pressure.

Page 41: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

activity rhythms.

Page 42: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts
Page 43: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- ----- -----. --.-

Data recording instruments were kept outside of the experimental room to avoid disturbing

the animals when collecting data. Wheel revolutions were detected by micro switches hard wired to

an Esterline-Angus event recorder (Mode1 620X).

Data Analysis

Part 1. Aschoff Type II (AII) Experirnental Design.

The ALI experimental design was used for al1 non-photic manipulations because it enabled

pulse administration to occur at the same circadian time for al1 animals. Hamsters were kept in LD

14: 10 for 3 days or until they were entrained (Aschoff 1965). The hamsters were then

simultaneously transferred into darkness and given a pulse on day 4. They remained in darkness

from day 5 until day 7. Phase shifts were calculated by subtracting activity onset on the day before

the pulse (day 3) from activity onset of the day following the pulse (day 5). Activity onset was

defined as: 1. A continuous 5 min. bout of activity followed by at least one additional 5 min. bout

within the subsequent 30 min. (Lees et al. 1983). 2. Onset had to follow at least 6 hr. without

activity which met onset criteria (Mrosovsky 1988).

Part 2. Modzfied AII Experimental Design

The respiratory stimuli for which apparent phase shifts were observed were repeated at

CT6 using a modified MI procedure (Bobrzynska et al. 1996a) to check for the occurence of

transients. A regression line was calculated for 6 consecutive activity onsets in DD, starting on the

third day following the pulse. The predicted activity onset on the day following treatment was

estimated by backward extrapolation of the post-pulse regression line. It was assumed that the

onset of activity on the day of the expenmental treatment was the sarne as it was on the day before

the pulse. The phase shift was defined as the difference between the onset on the last pre-pulse day

and the predicted onset on the day following the pulse (Bobrzynska et al. 1996a). The modified

AI1 result was then compared with the unmodified AII phase shift calculation.

Page 44: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Total daily wheel-running time was quantified to deter-rnine the effect of the respiratory

stimulus on activity. One bin of activity was defined as a 10 min section of continuous pen

deflections. The effect of a pulse or sharn pulse was caIculated by subtracting the activity on the

day of the pulse from that on the day before the pulse. Changes in activity were expressed either as

a percent or in number of 10 min. bins. The activity change of the tests was then subtracted from

the activity change of the control to quanti@ the effect of the respiratory stimuli on activity. Activity

change for consecutive days of hypoxia was cdculated by subtracting the three days imrnediately

before the pulse frorn the three days during the pulse.

The total difference in activity between the test and the control (test activity change - control

activity change) were also compared to the phase shift to determine if a significant correlation

existed.

Part 4. Statistical Analysis of Actograrn Data

The effects of the various pulses were assessed by comparing test data to the appropriate

controls. Significance was tested using a two-tailed, paired student t-test. Correlations were tested

using the Pearson Product - Moment Correlation coefficient (r). Differences are considered

significant at the 95% confidence level (P c 0.05).

Non-photic Stimuli

Novelty-Induced Wheel Running

Purpose: To confirm that the experimentai anirnals would respond in the expected way to

an established standard test.

Methods: A procedure similar to that applied by Reebs and Mrosovsky (1989) was used.

Hamsters (21 weeks old) were placed in running-wheels similar to the ones in their home cages,

but encased in Plexiglas, for three hours at CT6. Lights were then turned-off. Wheel revolutions

were counted by eIectronic counters. An infrared night scope was used to remove the hamsters

Page 45: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

shifts were measured with the unmodified AII procedure described above.

Cage Changing

Purpose: The AII procedure has been shown to be a valid method to study circadian

rhythms (Mrosovsky 1996). In addition, it has been shown to be in agreement with AI (Aschoff

1965) for several stimuli which produce relatively large phase shifts (i.e. novelty-induced wheel

running, triazolarn, NPY). However, 1 remained unsure as to whether the AI1 method had the

resolution to yield significant small phase shifts with only 9 hamsters because the size of the phase

shift associated with the transfer from a LD cycle to constant darkness is quite variable. It ranges

from almost nothing (Reebs and Mrosovsky 1989, Biello and Mrosovsky 1993), to about half an

hour (Mrosovsky and Salmon 1990). Cage changes have been show to cause small phase shifts

(Le. 25 min.) (Mrosovsky et al. 1989). A cage change experiment was carried out to determine if a

sample size of 9 is sufficient to identify significant srnall phase shifts.

Methods: The cage change was performed at CT6. Eight hamsters (16 weeks old) were

transferred to cages previously occupied by other hamsters, and one hamster received a fresh cage.

Following the cage change the lights were switched off. The control experiment consisted of lights

out at CT6 on a later date in the same group of hamsters at age 18 weeks.

Respiratory Stimuli

Three hour long pulses of sustained hypoxia (inhaled O, concentration = 5% ), episodic

hypoxia (inhaled O, concentration dtemated between 17% and 5%), or sustained hypercapnia

(inhaled CO, concentration = 15% ) were given at CT6, and CTI 1 and/or CT15 (Fig 1.1). A

respiratory pulse was obtained by blowing the appropriate gas into the chamber until the desired

concentration levels were obtained. Immediately prior to the respiratory stimuli, in addition to the

transfer to constant darkness, normal chamber air ventilation was shut-off. Following the pulse

ventilation was turned back on. During al1 respiratory stimuli 0, and CO, were monitored by

Page 46: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

were hard wired to a cornputer (MacClassic) for recording.

Controi experiments consisted of substituting compressed air for the gas (i.e. nitrogen,

oxygen and CO,) used in a particular test pulse. Flow rates into the charnber were matched to those

used in each test pulse. As in the tests, chamber air ventilation and lights were shut-off at the onset

of the pulse. Following the sham pulse, chamber ventilation was reinstated. Control phase-shifts

were calculated using the AI1 procedure described above. Control experiments were done for each

respiratory manipulation.

Visual Observation of Hamster Behaviour During Sustained Hypoxia and Hypercapnia

- mirpose: Visual observations were performed in order to ensure that the respiratory stimuli

did not induce behavioural changes that would indicate that the animals were in discornfort.

Methods: Three hamsters were placed in a sealed Plexiglas charnber and exposed to

hypoxia and hypercapnia at CT6 for a minimum of 30 min., or until what appeared to be a steady

state. Observations were rnanually recorded.

Hypoxia

Purpose: To determine if a 3 h. exposure to 5% 0, can cause a phase shift in the circadian

wheel running rhythm.

Methods: Hypoxia was obtained by blowing compressed nitrogen into the sealed animal

chamber until oxygen levels fell to 5%. Hypoxia was then sustained with a low flow of nitrogen.

Following 3 hours of 5% O2 the nitrogen was shut off. Nitrogen flow at the onset of the pulse was

monitored by an uncalibrated custom built, high capacity, flow indicator. For the remainder of the

pulse, nitrogen flow (approx. 2.5 Urnin) was monitored by a calibrated flow meter (Model NO 14-

96, Cole-Parmer Instrument Co.). Hamsters were 23-27 weeks old during these tests.

Page 47: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Additive EfSect of Hypoxia on Phase Shifrs.

Purpose: To determine whether exposure to 3 hr. of 5% O2 on three concecutive days at

CT6 would cause a phase change in hamster activity rhythms. This experiment was intended to

determine whether a hypoxic stimulus given on consecutive days at the sarne time would have a

cumulative effect. A further experiment was conducted to determine if the effect of constant

hypoxia on activity was maintained in a 14: 10 LD cycle.

Methods: Hypoxic bouts were given on three consecutive days for three hours at CT6.

Hamsters (aged 20 weeks) entered DD at the onset of the first hypoxic pulse or remained in 14: 10

LD throughou t the experiment.

Episodic Hypoxia

Purpose: To determine whether a series of short exPosures to hypoxia can cause phase

shifts in hamster wheel running rhythms.

Methods: Episodic hypoxia was induced by alternately ventilating the chamber with

nitrogen and oxygen. Oxygen levels oscillated between 5% for 3 min. and 17% for 3 min. for a

total duration of three hours. The timing of O2 and N, fiow was automated (Fig 2.2). In each case

gas flow was controlled by a solenoid valve (models 01-10 and V52LB2026, M&M International

and Honeywell Inc.). The electronic circuit controlling each solenoid valve consisted of a timer-

controlled AC supply in combination with a feedback system that served as a limit switch to ensure

that oxygen concentration did not oscillate past user-defined lirnits (Fig. 2.2 A). Each feedback

system consisted of a oxygen-dependent voltage from a oxygen analyzer which sarnpled from the

animal charnber.

Episodic hypoxia was initiated by activating the timer which turned on the set-point

comparator which controlled the solenoid valve in the nitrogen supply line. This comparator was

activated by oxygen levels above 5%. If the input to the comparator was above 5% it sent power to

Page 48: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

episodic hypoxia. Red line represents the currently active feedback loop. Black line represents the

currently inactive feedback loop. As drawn, the diagram represents the state of the system at the

beginning of a hypoxic pulse (Le. initiation of N, flow).

B. A graph of the 0, oscillations during episodic hypoxia. Solid red lines indicate

a device is active. Solid black lines indicate a device is inactive. The beginning of a hypoxic pulse

is represented by the points marked with an asterisk (*).

Page 49: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts
Page 50: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts
Page 51: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - - - - within the charnber fell to 5% the set-point comparator would cause the solenoid valve to close

stopping N2 flow and preventing oxygen from dropping any further. The nitrogen set-point

comparator was then deactivated by a timer, thus preventing the N, - solenoid from opening again in

the same cycle. At the same time the oxygen set-point comparator was activated by its timer. The

set-point comparator which controlled O, flow was set so that oxygen levels below 17% would

open the solenoid valve controlling oxygen flow to the chamber. This would result in oxygen

levels rising in the chamber until they reached 17%. These feedback loops were alternately

activated by the timers in a 3 minutes on, 3 minutes off sequence, resulting in a 6 min. duty cycle.

For each three minute section gas levels were changing for approximately two minutes and heId

constant for the remaining minute. Hamsters were 30-34 weeks old dunng these tests.

Hypercapnia

hrpose: To determine whether hypercapnia can phase change hamster activity rhythms.

Methods: CO, was maintained at 15% for 3 hours by blowing compressed CO, into the

chamber at approximately 0.2 L/rnin. Hamsters were 35-37 weeks old dunng these tests.

Respiratory Responses to Hypoxic and Hypercapnic Pulses

These experiments were designed to study the metabolic and ventilatory responses to hypoxic and

hypercapnic stimuli at CT6.

Apparatus Design

A 6-channel open-circuit whole-body plethysmograph (Bartlett and Tenney 1970)

was used to measure lung ventilation and metabolic rate (Fig 2.3). Whole-body plethysmography

operates on the principte that during inspiration air is warmed and hurnidified and, in a fixed

volume chamber, this causes an increase in pressure (Chapin 1954). Pressure changes were

monitored between a reference and an animal chamber using a differentiai pressure transducer

Page 52: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - r - - - - - ---------- -y-.m -..--a+ r l V C . . J U.*.V b A U Y h A '

B. Schematic diagram of a single channel of the six channel open-circuit

plethysmograph. Thick solid lines indicate primar-y animal chamber flow. Thin solid lines represent

the air flow for gas analysis and pressure transducer ports. Dotted arrows indicate the direction of

gas flow.

Page 53: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Outlet Valves

Pressure Transducer Hook-UPS

Inlet Valves

Compressed Gas

Animal Chamber

Carbon Oxygen Dioxide Anaiyzer Analyzer

PWP I _ p - q , z q Hygrometer

4+---"---

Refereiice Exhaust Chamber Outiet

Solenoid Valve

Humidification Bottle

y Desiccating A - $ Coiumn - A

-

Flow Meter

Page 54: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

(mode1 DP45-14; Validyne Engineering Corp.) The differential pressure transducer was hard wired

to a digital data acquisition system (MacLabI8, Analog Digital Instruments Ltd.) for recording of

the respiratory wave form. Seven 850 ml sealed Plexiglas cylinders served as six animal charnbers

and one reference charnber. The chambers were submerged in a water bath maintained at a constant

temperature of 22 -23" C.

Air-flow through the chambers was generated by cylinders of premixed compressed gas.

The gases passed through a regulator and a series of valves which maintained flow in each

charnber at 1.5 litres min.-'. Gas was dried p i o r to flow measurements and humidified before

entering the animal charnbers. During ventilation measurements flow into and out of the chamber

were sequentially shut-off. At al1 other times gas exited each chamber via a cornrnon oudet pipe

which tenninated in a jar which housed a digital hygrometer (Model 57550-21, Canadawide

Scientific Co.). Between the charnber and the comrnon outlet pipe was a one-way valve which

allowed outlet gas samples to be taken from each charnber. Outlet gas concentrations were

measured using oxygen and carbon dioxide analyzers (Model S-3All and Model 3D-3A, Ametek

Corp.). Irnrnediately pnor to entering the analyzers the outlet gas passed through a desiccating

column of Drierite (WH. Hammond Drierite Co.).

Calculation of Ventila tory Parameters

Inspired ventilation (YI) = f *VI where, f is the number of breaths in a minute (f=

~OKTOT), and V, is the tidal volume (ml, BTPS). VT was calculated using the equations

developed by Jacky (1980):

VI = [(P m /P cal V cal GAI 1 U4T/TT,,) (l-G,IG,)]

P = respiratory pressure defiection rn

P = the pressure defiection of a known volume gas cal

V = volume of the gas injected into the chamber a 1

TI = average time for one inspiration TT,, = average time for one complete respiratory cycle

Page 55: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

u 1 3 a U I I I I G I I ~ I U I I I ~ ~ ~ p ~ u p u i ~ i u l i ~ i i ~ y L U I I ~ L ~ L I I L I C I ~ I L I I I ~ LIK c ~ p i l l 1 s l u 1 1 UL LIX warmea ana A

humidified inhaled gas to the total volume of inhaled gas (Jacky 1980):

Tb = body temperature ("K) T = animal charnber temperature ("K)

PB = barometric pressure in the chamber (rnmHg)

PA, = water vapour pressure of the gas in the alveoii (mmHg)

Pc,, = water vapour pressure of the gas in the chamber (mmHg)

G, is a dimensionless proportionality constant relating the contraction of the exhaled gas as

it cools to T, to the volume of exhaled gas (Jacky 1980):

PN, = water vapour pressure of saturated gas at the nares (rnmHg)

TN = nasal temperature ('K).

Tb and TN used to calculate VT were estimated from separate experiments (see Appendix 1)

Calculation of Metabolic Rate

Metabolic rate or oxygen consumption (+O,, ml min.-l STPD) was measured by open

flow respirometry (Withers 1977):

V, = dry gas flow out of the animal chamber (ml min.-1 STPD)

Fro2 = fractional concentration of oxygen entering the charnber

FE& = fractional concentration of oxygen leaving the chamber

Page 56: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

RE is the respiratory exchange ratio where:

RE = ( F E C O ~ - F I C O ~ ) (F102 - F E O ~ )

Experimental Protocol

The oxygen and carbon dioxide analyzers were calibrated against gas sarnples of known O*

and CO, concentrations before each experirnent. The differential pressure transducer was calibrated

in the prepulse period while the animals were in the charnbers by injecting 1 mi of air several times

and recording the pressure signal (Fig 2.4).

The hamsters were weighed and then placed into the animal charnbers about 35 min. before

CT6. Several ventilation and metabolic rate measurements were taken in the pre-pulse period. To

simulate the transfer to DD which occurred in the circadian experiments, the animal chambers were

covered with an opaque lid. To reduce the transduction of light into the chambers via apparatus

tubing room light dunng the experiment was provided by a dim red light. The circadian

photoreceptors have a very low quantum sensitivity to wavelengths greater than 6ûû nm

(Talcahashi et al. 2984).

Six hamsters (aged 36-38 weeks) were exposed to air, constant hypoxia, episodic hypoxia,

and hypercapnia in four separate experiments. Each pulse started at CT6. After four minutes the

system reached equilibriurn and three consecutive measurernents of metabolic rate and one

ventilation measurernent were taken. Then, at each subsequent 20 min. interval a ventilation and a

metabolic rate measurernent were taken. Air was reintroduced to the system following the last

measurement at 3h. Measurements were taken at 4,20,40, and 60 min. of the recovery period.

The protocol was rnodified during the episodic pulse. The first pulse measurernent was taken at 20

min instead of 4 min. The recovery period was also extended from one to two hours to see if long-

term facilitation occurred. Since the equilibration time of the plethysmograph was 4 min. the

changes in O7 levels

Page 57: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -

circuit plethysmography. CO, and 0, represent the percentage of carbon dioxide or oxygen in the

animal chamber. Vent'n represents the ventilatory trace measured in volts. Dotted vertical line

indicates where a 1 ml calibration injection was given.

Page 58: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

21.2-

20.8 -

20.4- 2 %

20-

19.6-

. . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 . . 1

m . . . * . .

- * - 1 - -

. . * .

. . . - . ,

. . . . . . . 0.8- -

0.4- . . a . . . . .

. . . . . . . 0.2- . . m .

. . . . 200- . - . -

. . - .

. . .

. . .

. . . . . . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 :57:19 1 :57:29

Time (hr:min:s) El ' :57:3

Page 59: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - - measurements could not be taken until equilbration was reached resulting in the extension of a duty

cycle until measurements were cornpleted. After the experiments the hamsters were removed from

the animal charnber and returned to their home cages.

Statistical Analysis of Respira tory Data

Al1 statistics were computed using Data Desk software (Data Description, Inc.).

Signifïcance was tested using a repeated measures ANOVA. If a significant difference was

obtained a post-hoc Least Significant Difference Test was used for multiple cornparisons.

Differences are considered significant at the 95% confidence level (P c 0.05).

Page 60: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Chapter 3

Results

Non-photic Stimuli

Three hours of novelty-induced wheel-running at CT6 produced a mean phase advance of

3.2 + 0.12 h in hamsters which ran more than 4000 revolutions (n = 8) (Fig. 3.1). A single

hamster ran 1627 revolutions and did not phase shift. The mean number of wheel revolutions

was 6776 $: 956 (n = 9). The maximum number of revolutions obtained over the three hour pulse

was 10 186.

The mean cage change phase advance at CT6 (0.88 + 0.13 h) was significantly larger

than the control response (0.47 11: 0.06 h) (Fig 3.2).

Turning the lights-off produced a significant phase advance (0.52 t 0.04 h) in circadian

rhythms when administered at CT6 (Fig. 3.3). The phase advances obtained in both animal

chambers (labelled Box 1 and Box 2 in Fig. 3.3) were not significantly different from each other.

Respiratory Control Experiment

A three hour pulse of air at CT6 caused no significant change in the respiratory

parameters measured (Fig. 3.4). Furthermore, the prepulse measurements obtained in the

constant hypoxia (5%), episodic hypoxia (5- 17%), hypercapnia (1 5%) experiments, were not

significantly different from the air pulse values. Consequently, the prepulse measurernents from

each experiment were used to measure the effect of each respiratory stimulus, not the air pulse.

This was done to minimize the effect of possible differences between days.

Page 61: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

=SUI Li ALLUSI al11 iiiusuauiig p ; d ~ SIIILL alta noveiry-inaucea wneel-running. HO~iZOntal

arrows indicate the period of novel wheel mnning. DD started immediately after al1 the animals

were placed in their novel wheels. Vertical arrow indicates the time at which lights turned-off

during LD 14: 10. Time 24:OO to 08:00 is not shown, no wheel mnning took place during this

period. The hamster in this example ran 6086 wheel revolutions during the 3 h penod in the

novel wheel.

Page 62: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . S . . . . - > - , - j , , , , , , , , ,- , . - . - . , . . , , , . . . . . . . . - . . . . 1 1 L . . 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

Time (h)

Page 63: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -

a control experiment. (*) significant difference from control experiment.

Page 64: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Cage Change

Page 65: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts
Page 66: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Lights out CT6

Box 1 Box 2

Page 67: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

frequency. B. VT, tidal volume. C. VJïI, tidal volume divided by the mean inspiratory time

(rnean inspiratory airflow). D. YI , inspiratory minute ventiiation. E. +O,, rate of oxygen

consumption. F. YI NO,, rnetabolism specific inspiratory minute ventilation. Where error bars

are not visible they are within the point.

Page 68: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

f 200

(cycles min- 1) 150

(ml S-') 15

10

Air

1 I 1 I I I I I I I 1 1 I I

-20 O 20 40 60 80 100120 140160180 200 220 240

Time (min)

Page 69: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Air

94 (ml min-' 4

I

600

500

400

y, 300

(ml min - 1 200

100-

O 12

Time (min)

l I I I I I I 1 1 I -

- D. -

-

-

1 I I -

1°- E. a = 1 A -

Page 70: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

1. Constant Hypoxia

Visual observations indicated that hamsters were initially aroused from sleep by 5% 0,.

After 3 - 4 min. the hamsters curled-up in a corner of their cage and appeared to fa11 asleep.

Sleep was occasionally interrupeted by periods of feeding and grooming. Hamsters started to

shiver immediately folIowing the retum to air. Shivering was visible for several min..

1 ,a. Respira tory responses

Three hours of constant hypoxia at CT6 stimulated the hamster respiratory system (Fig.

3.5). f increased to approximately double the prepulse values (5 1.8 f 5.6 breaths min-') within 4

min. after the onset of hypoxia and remained significantly elevated above resting for a further

120 min. of hypoxia. Four min. after the return to air, f was elevated to 235 i 32 breaths min-',

more than four times the prepulse value and approximately twice the hypoxic values. f retumed

to prepulse levels 40 min. after the return to air (Fig 3.5 A).

The response of VT to hypoxia was biphasic (Fig 3.5 B). VT was significantly elevated

above resting levels ( 1.82 I 0.1 1 ml) 20 min. after the onset of hypoxia (2.49 f 0.22) and

subsequently fell to prepulse values. V, remained depressed for the remainder of the hypoxic

pulse. Four min. after the retum to air VT was significantly elevated above prepulse levels. VT

had retumed to prepulse values after 20 min. in air.

The response of V F I was biphasic, peaking at 20 min. (13.0 i 0.9 ml s-') (Fig 3.5 C).

Four min. after the return to air V/î, was elevated to 24.6 f 4.1 ml i l . Twenty min. after the

return to air V(TI returned to prepulse values (4.5 f 0.2 ml s"). The 9, response to hypoxia was biphasic (Fig 3.5 D). QI retumed to prepulse values (89

t 7 ml min-l), following the peak at 20 min. (244.3 I 23.3), at 60, 80, 120, 160, and 180 min. of

the hypoxic pulse (144.3 + 20.3, 147.0 k 34.7, 135.3 + 15.4, 121.7 + 18.1, 141.6 + 14.1,

respectively). Y, was elevated to 569 k 88 ml min-1, over six times prepulse values, 4 min. after

the ieturn to air. YI returned to prepulse values 40 min. after the retum to air.

Page 71: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -

180 min. (*) significant change from prepulse values. (7) significant decrease within the hypoxic

pulse from the peak value at 20 min. (Y) measurement is significantly different from al1 other

values.

Page 72: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Hypoxia

30C

25C

200

f (cycles min-1) 150

V T ~ I ' ~ (ml d')

-20 O 20 40 60 80 10C 120 140 160 180 200 220 240

Time (min)

Page 73: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Hypoxia

(mi min-')

vo, (ml min-')

Time (min)

Page 74: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

. - - - min-l) until60 min. (1.89 + 0.17 ml min-') after the onset of hypoxia. $0, was depressed below

prepulse 90, for the remainder of the pulse. Four min. after the retum to air metabolic rate was

elevated to 10.9 t 0.4 ml min-1. 90, returned to resting levels 40 min. after the retum to air (Fig

3.5 E).

The response of VI NO, was variable (Fig. 3.5 F). QI Ho2 appeared to be elevated to

three to four times resting values at the onset of hypoxia, however, it only became statisticaliy

significantly elevated above prepulse values (25.6 + 2.1) at 100, 140, and 180 min (1 25.8 t 26.3,

106.4 I 27.8, 100.2 f 14.9, respectively) after the onset of hypoxia. fil /gro2 retumed to prepulse

values four min. after the return to air.

I,b. Wheel running rhythm

According to the AI1 phase shift calculation method hypoxia caused a reduced phase

advance at CT6, CT11, and CT15 relative to the control (Fig. 3.6 and 3.7). The difference in

phase advance between the test and the control was 0.2 f 0.06 h, 0.3 k 0.08 h, 0.4 k 0.1 h at CT6,

CT11, CT 15 respectively.

Since constant hypoxia caused a significant phase delay relative to the control, the test

was repeated at CT6 to determine if the phase shift obtained was permanent. The effect of the

pulse on the phase of activity was calculated using the AII and modified AI1 experimental design

(Fig. 3.8 and 3.9 ). Use of the first day post-pulse to calculate the phase shift resulted in a phase

change (0.3 f O. 1) significantly reduced from the control pulse (0.73 k 0.14 h) but not

statistically different from the earlier test at CT6 (0.5 1 + 0.1 3). The post pulse regression line

predicted a first-day post-pulse that was statistically different from the actual first-day post-

pulse. When the predicted first day post pulse was used to calculate the phase shift (0.83 + 0.17)

it was not statistically different from the sham pulse (0.91 + 0.06 h ).

Page 75: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Hypoxia caused a significant reduction in activity at CT6, CTl 1, and CT15 relative to the

control (-1 6.5 + 2.47, - 17.3 + 3.5 1, -16.0 + 3.1 1 # of 10 min. bins, respectively) (Fig 3.10). The

reduction in activity was similar at a11 CTs. The sham pulses did not cause significant changes in

activity. The control pulses caused small increases which were not statistically different from

zero (Fig. 3.10)

2. Consecutive Days of Hypoxic Pulses

2,a. Wheel running rhythms

The results of 3 consecutive days in which 3 h long pulses of constant hypoxia were

administered are illustrated in Fig 3.1 1. Consecutive hypoxia caused a reduced phase advance

relative to that of the sham pulse (net phase delay = 0.68 + 0.18 h) (AI1 experimental design)

(Fig 3.12). However, when the hamsters remained in LD during the pulse regime activity onset

returned to prepulse values by the first day following the final pulse. On the days in which pulses

occurred activity onsets were variable but consistently delayed.

2, b. Activiiy

Consecutive days of hypoxia significantly reduced activity levels by 24 + 7 10 min. bins

relative to the control over al1 three days.

3. Episodic Hypoxia

3,a. Respiratory responses

Three hours of episodic hypoxia at CT6 induced a significant respiratory response (Fig.

3.13). Episodic hypoxia did not induce Iong-term facihtation of ventilation following terrnination

of the stimulus. Respiratory frequency did not significantly increase above prepulse values (70 k

1 1.4 breaths min") during episodes of 5% 4. However, dunng episodes of 17% O2 f (122.81

Page 76: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

hatching indicates a control experiment. (*) significant difference from control.

Page 77: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

1.50

1.25

1 .O0

h s & 0.75 E m

B f o.,, E

0.25

0.00

Constant Hypoxia (5% 0 2 )

n = 9 n = 9 n = 9 n = 9 n = 9 n = 8

CT6 CTll CT15

Page 78: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

CT15. Test and sham puIse time is demarcated by the horizontal arrows. Vertical arrows indicate

when the lights were turned off in the 14: 10 LD cycle before the pulse. Anirnals were transferred

to DD at the onset of each pulse.

Page 79: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

H ypoxia CTl f

Hypoxia Cr15

Time ih)

Page 80: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

rnethods. Cross hatching indicates control experiment. (*) significant difference from the control.

Page 81: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Constant Hypoxia (5% Oz)

1st day post pulse vs. regression analysis

n = 9 n = 9

1 st day post pulse

CT6

regres sion

CT6

Page 82: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

L 1 6 U l W r r . ~ L ~ L U ~ ~ U I I I I I I U J C L U L ~ ~ I ~ ~ I ~ U J C , J L U L L J u u L a l I l b u C L L L ~ I J /V wS U U I I ~ I ~ I ~ ~ L I ~ L I U I I a~ L L W.

Horizontal arrows demarcate when the pulse was given. Vertical arrows indicate when the lights

went out during LD 14: 10. Hamsters entered DD at the onset of hypoxia.

Page 83: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Time (h)

Page 84: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- , " - - - - - - - - - - - - -- - - --, -.---. -' -" "-'-'a... a a.. U.VU."U

control experiment. (*) significant difference from the control.

Page 85: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Constant Hypoxia (5% 02)

Activity Change

n = 9 n = 9 n = 9 n = 9 n = 8 n = 9

CT6 CTl 1 CT15

Page 86: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

CT6 in DD at the onset of the first pulse, and LD for the duration of the experiment. Cross

hatching indicates control experiment. (*) significant difference from the control.

Page 87: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Consecutive Days of Hypoxia

1 st day post pulse

DD

regression

Page 88: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- u . - - - - - - - - Y - - - - - y - ----- ---- -- - - . m u - - - - . , - --J " U A

constant hypoxia at CT6. Vertical arrows indicate when the lights went out during LD 14: 10.

Hamsters entered DD at the onset of the first pulse.

Page 89: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

12 15 18 21

Time (h)

Page 90: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

prepulse levels four min. after the return to air (Fig 3.13 A).

VT did not change significantly from prepulse values (1.7 1 i: .2 1 ml) during hypoxic

episodes or after 4 min. into the post pulse recovery period. During episodes of 17% O2 VT was

reduced (0.8 1 f 0.06 ml) to about 47% of prepulse values (Fig 3.13 B).

V J ï , was elevated relative to prepulse levels (4.9 k 0.24 ml sel) during al1 hypoxic

episodes. The response of V f ï I during hypoxic episodes was biphasic, V p l peaked at 20 min.

(12.1 + 0.9 ml s-1) and subsequently declined to its minimum hypoxic value at 180 min. (8.57 + 0.456 ml s-'). V(T1 remained depressed during hypoxic episodes relative to the 20 min. peak I

value for the remainder of the stimulus. Vfï, retumed to prepulse values dut-ing each 17% O2

episode and throughout the recovery period (Fig 3.13 C).

YI values did not change significantly from prepulse values (96.4 f 6.4 ml mi r1 ) during

al1 17% O2 episodes. 9, responded biphasically during the hypoxic episodes. V, peaked at

twenty min. (229.7 f 12.4 ml min-l), and declined until the last measurement at 180 min. (161 f

29.7 ml min-') (Fig 3.13 D).

$0, in air remained at prepulse levels (3.5 f 0.3 ml m i r 1 ) during al1 the 17% O2

episodes. However, after 100 min. Yo, was significantly depressed below prepulse values during

5% O2 episodes (mean 90, = 1.7 + 0.2 ml min-1) (Fig 3.13 E).

YI NO, was elevated to approximately 2.2 times prepuise levels during 5% O2 episodes,

and YI NO, retumed to prepulse levels during the 17% O2 episodes and the recovery period (Fig

3.13 F).

3,b. Wheel running rhythms

Episodic hypoxia did not produce a significant phase change relative to the control

experiments at CT6 and CT11. At CT15 episodic hypoxia caused a phase advance (0.14 + 0.06

h) that was significantly reduced relative to the control experiment (0.3 1 + 0.07 h) (Fig 3.14 and

3.15).

Page 91: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - Y .*- A..Vu Y..ULUIJ , u,,yVLI~u u p ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ LI J YUAICI. L~IDUUIL LIJ YWAIU w a3 I I I I L I ~ L L U QL LIIIIC u

and terminated at 180 min. (*) significant change relative to prepuke values. (Y) measurement

that is significantly different from al1 other measurements.

Page 92: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

J

(cycles min-1) 150

O

30

25

V T ~ 20 (ml S- l )

15

Episodic Hypoxia

I I l I i 1 1 1

Time (min)

Page 93: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Time (min)

600

500

400

(ml min-') 300

200

100

O l 2

10

00, 8

(ml min-') 6

4

I t l I l l 1 l

- -

- D.

-

- -

- !lJ -

- O * O * 6 * -

O O mm@ a a * O . l . a -

I I I I I l I I

- E. d

-

- -

- -

-j j O A- 0

I 1 6 $ - 2

O 250

200

150

100

50

O

O L

- * O O

* O 6 -

I I 1 1 I I I I -

F. -

- -

- -

* *

- * O * O

0 * - O O

- - m.0 a a a . . . * * a . .

I I 1 1 I I I I

O 40 80 120 160 200 240 280

Page 94: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

experiments. (*) significant difference from control.

Page 95: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Episodic Hypoxia

n = 9

CTl 1

Page 96: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Horizontal arrows demarcate when the pulse was given. Vertical arrows indicate when the lights

went out during LD 14: 10. Hamsters entered DD at the onset of episodic hypoxia.

Page 97: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

. * . .

. , I . . . , . . . - .< . .... -. - l ' I I I I I ~ r n , . . .

Episodic H-moxia

CTl 1

Control

Episodic Hypoxia

CT15

15 18

Time (h)

Page 98: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Episodic hypoxia caused a significant reduction in activity at CT6, CT 1 1, and CT 15 (-

7.22 + 1.75, - 1 1.3 I 2.6, - 15.3 + 4.0 # of I O min. bins, respectively) relative to the respective

controls (Fig 3.16).

4. Hypercapnia

Behavioral responses of the hamsters to 15% CO, were similar to those during 5% 0,. - -

However, no shivering was observed to occur following the return to air.

4,a. Respiratory responses

Three hours of hypercapnia (1 5% CO2) at CT6 induced a significant respiratory response

(Fig. 3.17). Respiratory frequency was only significantly elevated above prepulse values (53.5 k

3.9 breaths min-1) at 10 min. and 160 min (99.4 + 27.3 breaths m i r 1 , 84.3 f 1 1 breaths rnin-1

respectively) after the onset of hypercapnia (Fig. 3.17 A).

VT was elevated significantly relative to prepulse values (2.68 k 0.26 ml) by 20 min (4.3

i 0.16 ml) (Fig. 3.17 B). VT remained at approximately double prepulse values for the duration

of the pulse. VT declined to prepulse values by the fourth min. of the recovery period.

V P , rose significantly in cornparison to prepulse values (5.5 f 0.46 ml s- l ) by the tenth

min. of hypercapnia (10.97 f 0.5 1 ml s-1) (Fig. 3.17 C). VJîI remained elevated for the duration

of the pulse. VJïl retumed to prepulse values within 4 min. after termination of the pulse.

YI was significantly elevated (295.2 f 16.9 ml min-l) relative to prepulse values (133.4

+ 12 ml mi r1 ) for the entire pulse (Fig. 3.17 D). Al1 recovery measurements were statistically

equal to the prepulse values.

?oz was significantly lower than prepulse values (4.06 & 1.25 ml min-l) at 7, 10, 160,

and 180 min. (2.24 f 0.14 ml min-1, 2.31 k 0.19 ml m i r 1 , 2.03 10.32 ml min-1, 1.96 i 0.27 ml

min- l , respectively) of the hypercapnic pulse. vol returned to prepulse values by the first air

measurernent following the pulse (4 min.) (Fig. 3.17 E).

Page 99: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

control experiment. (*) significant difference from the control.

Page 100: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Episodic Hypoxia

Activity Change

Page 101: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

" . -=- - - - - - - , ---=-.--- -- y-* -ri-y...... La, y V . " U y I . A U .I Ucl I 1 I I L I U l . C U U L L I I L I L , V UllU

concluded at 180 min. (*) significant change from prepulse values.

Page 102: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

J

(cycles min- 1) 150

(mi S.')

Tirne (min)

Page 103: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

YI (mi min-')

(mi min-')

-20 O 20 40 60 80 100120140160180200220240

Tirne (min)

Page 104: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

measurement ( 133.5 f 19.6, t = 4min). YI No2 remained elevated for the duration of the

hypercapnic pulse. V, NO, retumed to prepulse values by the first measurement taken in the - recovery period (t = 184 min.) (Fig. 3.17 F).

4,b. Wheel running rhythms

Three hours of constant hypercapnia at CT6 and CT15 did not cause a significant phase

change relative to the controls (Fig. 3.18 and 3.19).

4,c. Activity

- Activity was significantly reduced relative to the control experiments at CT6 and CT15 (-

8.5 + 1.8, -1 1.8 I 2.6 number of 10 min. bins) (Fig 3.20). Activity was not affected by the sham

pulses.

5. Activity vs. Phase Change.

Changes in activity were not significantly correlated (r = 0.01 1) with phase changes

(difference between test and control experiments) when individual data points were used (Fig

3.21 A). However, when two outliers are removed the correlation coefficient r is raised to 0.262

(P < 0.05). When the mean phase changes were plotted against mean activity changes between

the pulse and control, reductions in activity were significantly correlated with phase delays (r =

0.92; P < 0.001) (Fig 3.21 B). The differences between the two tests indicates that individual

variability is the reason no significant correlation was obtained when al1 individual data points

were used. The calculated ? value was 84% indicating that 16% of the variance in the group data

cannot be explained by changes in activity.

Page 105: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

control experiments.

Page 106: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Hypercapnia

Page 107: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - - - Horizontal arrows demarcate when the pulse was given. Vertical arrows indicate when the lights

went out during LD 14: 10. Hamsters entered DD at the onset of hypercapnia.

Page 108: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Hy percapnia CT15

Control

15 18

Time (h)

Page 109: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -- -- - - -- - - . - - - m m -- -- . --J ------ O- - ---.-- -" " J l"'-r"'-. -'.,W... '^--"'*..b "'-"-'-' """W.

experiment. (*) significant difference from the control.

Page 110: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Hypercapnia

Activity Change

Page 111: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -

B. Activity vs. phase change group data for al1 respiatory stimuli. O,CT6 / 02CT11

/ 0,CT 15, group data for constant hypoxia. EO,CT6 / E02CT 1 1 / EO,CTl5, group data for

episodic hypoxia. C02CT6 I C0,CT 15, group data for hypercapnia. 302CT6, group data for

consecutive days of hypoxia.

Page 112: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Activity Change (number of 1 O min. bins)

Page 113: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Discussion

This study has shown that hypoxia can cause small phase delays in hamster activity

rhythms. It is unclear whether phase shifts were transient or long lasting. The phase delays may

have been mediated through reductions in activity levels. The phase delay caused by hypoxic

pulses presented on three consecutive days did not occur when the hamsters remained in a

regular LD cycle.

Standard Non Photic and Respiratory Stimuli

The main purpose of this study was to determine whether chemical stimuli that are

known to affect lung ventilation will also influence the phase of the circadian pacemaker, as

measured by the timing of the onset of wheel-mnning activity. Preliminary experiments were

conducted to confirm (a) that the chosen gas concentrations elicited a respiratory response in the

hamster, and (b) that the hamster wheel running rhythms would respond to standard non-photic

stimuli in the predicted way under the present experimental conditions.

The whole body-plethysmography experiments confirmed that 3 h pulses of constant

hypoxia (O, concentration = 5%), episodic hypoxia (O, concentration = 17 - 5%) and constant

hypercapnia (CO, concentration = 15%) significantly affected lung ventilation and metabolic

rate.

Novelty-induced wheel running and cage changes were chosen as standard non-photic

stimuli that are known to cause large and small phase advances, respectively, when presented at

CT6 (Mrosovsky 1988, Reebs et al. 1989). The mean phase shift in response to 3 h of novelty

induced wheel running was 3.2 h, which is sirnilar to results previously obtained in hamsters

(Reebs et al. 1989, Janik and Mrosovsky 1993). Furtherrnore, a cage change at CT6 produced a

statistically significant mean phase advance of 25 min., again similar to the phase advances of

about half an hour observed by Mrosovsky (1988). Novelty-induced wheel-mnning and the cage

Page 114: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

produce data similar to those obtained in other studies.

Constant Hypoxia

The YI response elicited by hypoxia was biphasic, similar to those previously reported in

the literature (see Mortola and Gautier 1995 for review). Changes in 9, over the 3 h pulse were

almost entirely caused by changes in VT. Respiratory frequency was elevated by hypoxia until

the last two measurements (t = 140, 160 min.) taken in 5% O,, whereas VT responded in a

biphasic rnanner with a statistically significant decline frorn the peak value by the first

measurernent (t = 40 min.) after the peak. The increase in V, at the onset of hypoxia is attributed

to peripheral chemoreceptor stimulation (Nishimura et al. 1987), whereas the attenuation of Y, is

caused by washout of CO, from the central chemoreceptors (Neubauer et al. 1985) and a reduced

central diive for respiration (Millhorn et al. 1984, Neubauer et al. 1985, Martin-Body and

Johnston 1987). In rabbits the central depressant effects originate in regions rostral to the

midbrainlpontine junction (Martin-B ody and Johnston l988), and possibl y in the red nucleus

(Waites et al. 1996).

The hypoxic hypometabolism observed in the present experiment has previously been

observed in cats (Gautier et al. 1 989), rats (Gautier and Bonora l992), hamsters (Mortola 199 l),

and many other rodents (Frappell et al. 1992). The reduction in metabolic rate is attributed to a

inhibition of therrnogenesis and direct effects on cellular function (Mortola and Gautier 1995).

The temperature data and the shivering following the hypoxic pulse indicate that thermogenesis

was inhibited in the present experiment.

Hypoxia typically causes a sustained increase in the Yi/Yo2 ratio at the onset of hypoxia

(Mortola et al. 1989, Frappell et al. 1992). The O,No, ratio remains relatively constant despite

the fact that VO, decrease over the hypoxic period. In the present experiment 3 h of constant

hypoxia (5%) induced an increase in the v,No, ratio at the onset. However, the V,NO, ratio was

variable throughout the pulse. Changes in the +,No, ratio were mainly caused by changes in V,.

Page 115: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

in metabolic rate following the return to air. The increase in VO, coincided with the behavioral

observation of shivering. The shivering likely occurred to compensate for the reduction in Tb

which took place during hypoxia (Mortola and Dotta 1992, Saiki et al. 1994). The increase in

Vo2 following the return to air was likely the result of a combination of shivering and non-

shivering therrnogenesis (brown fat metabolism) (Girardier and Stock 1983).

Constant hypoxia administered at CT6, CT 1 1 and CT 15 caused a significant 12- 18 min.

reduction in the instantaneous phase advance relative to the control when the first day post-pulse

was used to cakulate the instantaneous phase shift. It is assumed that the reduced phase advances

obtained in the Aschoff type II test (hamsters entered DD at the onset of the pulse) would have

been phase delays in an Aschoff type 1 test (where hamsters are in DD for the duration of the

experiment) because the phase advance caused by the transition from LD to DD does not occur

in the Type 1 test (Moore-Ede et al. 1982). However, when the first day post-pulse activity onset

was determined by extrapolation of the post-pulse regression line, the phase shift obtained was

not statistically different from the control at CT6. This result would indicate that the reduced

phase advance obtained on the first-day post-pulse at CT6 was a temporary response (Le.

transient). Based on the present data, 1 tentatively conclude that a single 3 h pulse of constant

hypoxia (5%) did not cause a significant long-lasting phase shift in the activity rhythms of

hamsters despite the fact that it was a powerful respiratory stimulus. Further experiments, using

the AI procedure, are needed to clarify this contradictory result.

Consecutive Days of Hypoxia

Calculation of the phase change caused by 3 h of constant hypoxia adrninistered on three

consecutive days using the post-pulse regression line and the first-day post-pulse resulted in a

phase advance that was 41 f: 10 min. less than the control advance. Consequently, consecutive

days of hypoxia caused a long lasting phase delay in the activity rhythms of the hamster. This

Page 116: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

------ --Pb---- --a-' - v a . . &.Y n . J y V I . . V YUaYV I..UJ I I .UVVU l l U T U bUUJbU CI YIILWb Ubl(ly L l J a L ullly

became discernible with the cumulative effect of multiple pulses.

Humans, rats and ducks exhibit a time of day dependent change in apparent

chemoreceptor sensitivity which suggests that the respiratory system may be modulated by the

circadian system (Raschke and Moller 1989, Peever and Stephenson 1997, Woodin and

Stephenson 1998). Furtherrnore, constant hypoxia can phase delay hamster activity rhythms. If

respiration is under circadian control and manipulation of respiration does phase shift circadian

rhythms in respiration, as the aforementioned results suggest, then it is possible that a respiratory

stimulus on one day can alter the response to the same stimulus on the next day at the sarne time.

This type of feedforward-feedback interaction would be of interest to investigators who use

respiratory stimuli to determine the mechanisms involved in respiratory control. Such

investigators would benefit from a tool to control for respiratory stimulus induced phase shifts.

Maintaining hamsters in a 14: 10 LD cycle during consecutive days of hypoxia eliminated

the phase change obtained when animals free-ran in DD following the first pulse. This result

would suggest that, with hypoxia in hamsters at least, a maintained LD cycle can prevent a

respiratory stimulus from phase shifting activity rhythms. Light is the dominant entrainer of

circadian rhythms. However, it has previously been shown that a non-photic stimulus (novelty

induced wheel running) can reduce the phase advancing effects of a weak light pulse (10 lux)

(Ralph and Mrosovsky 1992). In addition, preliminary experiments have demonstrated that

stronger light pulses (100 lux) eliminated the blunting effect that the non-photic stimulus had on

the light pulse (Mrosovsky 1993). It appears that possible effects that non-photic stimuli may

have on light pulses may be light intensity dependent. Since light b e l s were approximately 140

lux in the present experiment it is not surprising that the small phase shifts induced by hypoxia

were eliminated by light. To further ensure that the experimental subject is at the same circadian

tirne, investigators should allow several days between experiments as this would give the

subjects time to reentrain to the prevailing LD cycle.

Page 117: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- -, - ----- --- -. - -. -J - "-"" J'"

It has previously been reported that an acute exposure to low barornetric pressure can

cause phase changes in human circadian rhythms (Ashkenazi et al. 1982). A 30 min. exposure to

a sirnulated altitude of 7620m in combination with a 2-3 min. pulse of hypoxia "early in the day"

resulted in transient phase delays of up to three hours in certain physiological and cognitive

performance parameters (oral temperature, grip strength, peak expiratory air fiow and cognitive

ability tests) in human subjects. At 7620 rneters the decrease in barometric pressure from 1 atm.

to 0.35 atm. causes the partial pressure of oxygen to fa11 to approximately 57 mmHg. This is

equivalent to breathing a gas mixture containing approximately 7.5% 0, at sea level. The

experimental subjects remained in LD following the pulse and al1 parameters reentrained by

three days post pulse. Ashkenazi et al. (1982) concluded that altitude andor hypoxia would have

caused a long lasting phase change had the experimental subject been isolated from al1 time cues

following the pulse. However, there are other ways to interpret this result. Altitude and/or

hypoxia may have altered the phase angle of entrainment to the LD cycle implying a change in

the period of the SCN output. Altitude andor hypoxia may have had a direct effect on the

measurec! variables and not on the clock which controls them (i.e. masking).

Due to the various possible interpretations of the Ashkenazi et al. study (1982) it is

difficult to make comparisons with the present study. Difficulties in comparing the two studies

are further compounded by differences in duration and intensity of hypoxia, species differences,

and the combination of hypoxia and altitude. It is possible that the change in pressure was at least

partially responsible for the phase shift in hurnan subjects as it has been demonstrated in pocket

mice that circadian rhythms in Tb are entrained by large cyclic pressure changes (Hayden and

Lindberg 1969). The fact that phase delays were obtained in both experiments lends weight to

the hypothesis that hypoxia caused the phase shift in the Ashkenazi et al. study (1982), although

the results of the present experiment do not support the role of a single pulse of hypoxia as a

powerful inducer of phase shifts.

Page 118: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Episodic Hypoxia

The respiratory response to episodic hypoxia observed in hamsters differed in two

respects frorn those observed previously in rats, cats, goats and dogs (Millhom et al. 1980ab,

Bach et al. 1992, Cao et al. 1992, Powell and Aaron 1993, Turner and Mitchell 1997). Despite

the fact that each hypoxic episode was followed by an air episode, the hypoxic episodes appeared

to have had a cumulative effect, resulting in a biphasic response similar to that seen during

constant hypoxia. Mechanisms mediating the ventilatory decline may be the sarne mechanisms

that are believed to be involved in the V, attenuation to constant hypoxia (HVD). These include

washout of CO2 from central chemoreceptors due to increased cerebral blood flow stirnulated by

hyqoxic blood (Duffin 1990), or pre- and post-synaptic events causing altered neuronal threshold

for excitability within the respiratory control network (Bisgard and Neubauer 1995). The

neurotranmitter GABA may be a key mediator of the HVD because GABA antagonists can

reverse respiratory depression (Melton et al. 1990). Finally, reductions in oxygen availability

promote the rnetabolic production of adenosine and lactic acid which act as neuromodulators to

reduce excitability of CNS neurons (Neubauer et al. 1990, Yan et al. 1995). Changes in some or

al1 of these parameters may cause the progressive hypoxic ventilatory decline seen during

episodic hypoxia.

Long-term facilitation (LTF) is a long-lasting increase of ventilation after cessation of a

series of brief hypoxic challenges (Cao et al. 1992). LTF was not induced by episodic hypoxia in

hamsters as no increase in Y, over the prepulse values was observed during the two-hour

recovery period. Previous experiments which have evoked LTF in awake dogs and goats have

used a 80 and 93% arterial blood saturation (Cao et al. 1992, Turner and Mitchell 1997). Duty

cycles were in the range of 2-5 min. of hypoxia followed by 2-5 min. normoxia lasting for 40 to

90 min. (Bach et al. 1992, Turner and Mitchell 1997). Most experiments which induce LTF have

maintained arterial isocapnia during episodic hypoxia (Milihorn et al. 1980ab, Bach et al. 1993,

Turner and Mitchell 1997) but not al1 (Powell and Aaron 1993). It appears that the regimen used

Page 119: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- differences in the episodic hypoxia protocol were responsible for the lack of LTF. However,

further experiments, with various gas concentrations (8% O,, 8% 0, and 3% CO,, 5% 0, and

2% CO,) have failed to elicit LTF in the hamster (Stephenson, unpublished data).

One speculative explanation for the absence of LTF in hamsters is that they may have

evolved defense arousal system which is not activated by hypoxia. During hypoxia, stimulation

of the defense arousal system results in the release of catecholamines in the cat and rat (Hilton

and Marshall 1982, Marshall 1984). Recruitment of the defense arousal system as part of the

general hypoxic response is interpreted as a beneficial adaptation which allows an animal to

prepare itself to escape from a hypoxic environment (Marshall 1987). One of the mechanisrns

postulated to be responsible for LTF is catecholamine release which results in an increase in

metabolic rate (Cao et al. 1992). Repeated bouts of hypoxia, it is hypothesized, may result in a

build-up of catecholamines leading to a sustained elevated level of metabolism and Y, following

the pulse. Since hamsters frequently encounter asphyxic conditions in the burrow (Kuhnen et al.

1987), they may have adapted a respiratory response that does not involve recruitment of the

defense arousal system, and the subsequent release of catecholamines, as there is no need for

escape from the burrow.

It was hypothesized that LTF may cause phase shifts in hamster activity rhythms because

the release of serotonin in the central nervous system is necessary for the inducement of LTF in

cats (Millhorn et al. 1980b) and serotonin may be involved in non-photic phase shifting as there

is a serotonergic projection from the dorsal raphe to intergeniculate leaflet (IGL) (Meyer-

Bernstein and Morin 1996). Phase shifts to non-photic stimuli are eliminated by lesions of the

IGL (Johnson et al. 1989, Janik and Mrosovsky 1994, Wickland and Turek 1994). Since, LTF

was not induced by episodic hypoxia this hypothesis remains untested.

Episodic hypoxia did not cause a phase shift in hamster running activity at CT6 and

CTI 1. However, a statistically significant 8 + 3.3 min. reduced phase advance relative to the

control was obtained at CT15. Since the respiratory response to episodic hypoxia was similar to

Page 120: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

L I I U L V I C i V l l J L Q l i L llypUA14, I L 1 3 p U J 3 1 U I G LIIUL L l l C 3QlllC l l l G L l l a 1 1 1 3 1 l l W U 3 l C 3 ~ U I I 3 I U I C L U I L l l C U C l d Y C U

activity onset on the first day post pulse.

Episodic hypoxia occurs cornmonly in patients with sleep apnea where frequent apneic

episodes result in recurrent bouts of oxyhemoglobin desaturation in sleep. The findings of

episodic hypoxia tests would suggest that, in hamsters at Ieast, circadian rhythrns are not

disrupted by cyclic reductions in the oxygen content of blood. The clinical relevance of this

finding is limited because the atypical respiratory response of the hamster suggests that the phase

response could also be atypical.

Hypercapn ia

The ventilatory response to hypercapnia was consistent with responses previously

reported in the literature (Chapin 1954, Walker et al. 1985, Maskrey 1990). The effect of

hypercapnia on metabolism in the present study is interesting because the effects of CO, on

metabolism have not been studied exhaustively. The results presently available on adult

mammals appear to be contradictory, since hypercapnia has been said to increase, have no effect,

or decrease metabolic rate (Stupfel 1974, Jennings and Laupacis 1982, Kaminski et al. 1985,

Gautier et al. 1993). However, cornparison of data between published studies, and the present

unpublished data, suggest that the metabolic response to hypercapnia may be concentration

dependent. For example, in adult and newbom rats 2-5% CO2 had no effect on metabolism (Saiki

and Mortola 1996), whereas 19.5-32.5% CO2 caused a significant decrease in metabolic rate

(Stupfel 1974). In the present study, 15% CO, reduced metabolism to about two-thirds of resting

levels. In animals exposed to CO, concentrations higher than 1095, metabolic effects are to be

expected, since an acute fa11 of pH impairs the utilization of glucose and fat due to an inhibition

of the activity of pH-dependent enzymes (Stupfel 1974, Kuhnen et al. 1987). The fa11 in pH is

dependent on the buffering capacity of the blood which tends to be higher in fossorial anirnals

(Boggs et al. 1984). At lower levels of CO, sympathetic reactions to the chemoreceptor

stimulation may prevail (Saiki and Mortola 1996).

Page 121: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

-

significant phase change in hamster activity rhythms. However, the hypercapnic pulse at CT6

was a powerful respiratory stimulus.

Mechanism of Phase Shifts

Although it was hypothesized that the respiratory stimuli would be arousing, the

respiratory data and behavioural observations indicate that the stimuli were in fact depressing.

Consequently, the possibility that an arousing respiratory stimulus can cause a phase shift in

hamster activity rhythms remains untested. Nonetheless hypoxia did induce phase delays. There

are several mechanisms which could explain the effect that hypoxia had on circadian wheel

running rhythms:

1. Phase delays might occur if hypoxia slowed or temporarily stopped the pacemaker.

Hypoxia can cause reductions in neuronal excitability (Bisgard and Neubauer 1995). However,

whether or not hypoxia-induced reductions in neuronal excitability have any effect on the SCN is

unknown. The SCN may in fact be able to compensate for changes in neuronal excitability as it

does compensate for changes in temperature (Rawson 1960, Richter 1975, Gibbs 1983). The

circadian system is considered to be temperature-compensated because temperature changes do

not affect circadian rhythms to the degree that they affect biochemical reactions (Moore-Ede et

al. 1982).

2. A variety of stimuli that induce changes in locomotor activity also induce phase-

dependent shifts in the circadian rhythm of locomotor activity in hamsters (Boulos and Rusak

1982, Mrosovsky 1988, Reebs and Mrosovsky 1989, Mrosovsky and Salmon IWO). Therefore,

the role that changes in activity, caused by constant hypoxia and episodic hypoxia, may have had

on the circadian rhythm of locomotor activity should be considered.

Reductions in activity were significantly correlated with instantaneous phase delays

calculated using the first-day post-pulse (Fig. 3.21). Reduction in activity might have the same

effect as immobilization experiments where preventing a hamster from running during the highly

Page 122: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Reeth et al. 1991). Hypoxia may be an indirect way of immobilizing hamsters. However, if

reductions in activity induce phase delays why did hypercapnia, which caused significant

reductions in activity, fail to induce phase delays?

Hypercapnia rnay not have caused phase delays because there may be a minimum

threshold for the reduction in activity. Activity thresholds have been dernonstrated in novelty

induced wheel running experiments which typically require that a hamster run more than 4000

revolutions, in a 17 cm diameter wheel, before a phase shift occurs (Janik and Mrosovsky 1993).

Perhaps the reduction in activity caused by hypercapnia was not large enough to cause a phase

change in hamster activity rhythms.

The correlation between activity reduction and phase delays is complicated by the fact

that the first day post pulse and regression analyses gave conflicting results for constant hypoxia

at CT6 (i.e. phase delay vs. no phase delay, respectively). It is possible that the regression

analysis did not yield a significant delay because the sample size was too small, although, there

is no trend in the data which would indicate this. It is also possible that the reduction in activity

caused by hypoxia is close to the threshold for activity reduction induced phase delays. Further

experiments are required to determine the role of activity reductions in hypoxia induced phase

delays.

3. Constant hypoxia caused a reduction in Tb and metabolic rate, and subsequent

shivering in the recovery period. It has been demonstrated in rats and hamsters that periods of

hypothermia induce phase delays at several circadian times (Rawson 1960, Richter 1974, Gibbs

1983). Therefore, changes in body temperature may be responsible for the phase delays caused

by hypoxia. The phase shifts caused by the reductions in Tb have been interpreted as the result of

Q,, effects combined with unknown clock mechanisms which partially compensate for the

temperature changes. Q,, is a measure of the temperature-sensitivity of rates of reaction, and the

rate of most biochemical processes changes two- or threefold with each 10°C change in

temperature (that is, QI, = 2-3) (Moore-Ede et al. 1982). Based on the conclusions of the

Page 123: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

induced hypothermia may be responsible for the phase delays.

However, the Tb manipulation experiments (Rawson 1960, Richter 1974, Gibbs 1983) did

not control for confounding behavioral correlates. Examination of the representative actograms

in the Rawson (1960) study reveats that hypothermie pulses visibly reduced activity Ievels even

when they occurred outside of the period when hamsters were normally active. For exarnple,

mice experienced a reduction in Tb to an average of 3 1.2 "C (min. of approx. 28°C) over 3 h,

which caused a 35 min. phase delay and what appears to be a 40 - 60% reduction in activity

(Rawson 1960). It may be that the phase delays obtained for reductions in Tb in mice were

partially the result of reduced activity levels as immobilization of hamsters during their active

period also causes phase delays (Van Reeth et al. 1991). Tt appears that the mechanisms

postulated to be mediating hypothennia-induced phase changes need to be re-exarnined to

account for changes in activity levels.

The fact that activity changes may play a role in the phase delays caused in the

temperature manipulation studies does not mean that changes in Tb do not have an effect on

biological time-keeping. However, if a small reduction in temperature over a short period of time

causes a large reduction in activity, as in the present experiment, it seems likely that the changes

in activity would have a stronger effect on the circadian activity rhythms of hamsters than

reductions in Tb would. It would be interesting to know whether changes in activity correlated

with the phase changes in the temperature experiments, as they did in the present study.

4. The evidence for reductions in activity mediating the changes in activity rhythms by

hypoxia is correlative and therefore not conclusive. Thus, it is still possible that the respiratory

stimulus is directly responsible for the phase delays. However, it is unlikely that changes in a

specific respiratory parameter is directly responsible for the phase delays. Al1 the respiratory

stimuli used in the present experiment resulted in changes in respiration. Al1 the respiratory

stimuli caused increases inf, VT, Y, , $,NO,, and VT/Ti and a reduction in ?O,, yet phase shifts

were only induced by hypoxic stimuli. No correlation was detected between any of the

Page 124: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

similar respiratory effects does not imply that they were mediated by the same respiratory

mechanism and therefore it is still possible that the hypoxic stimuli were directly responsibIe for

the observed phase delays.

Respiratory Stimuli and Activiiy

The mechanism by which hypoxia and episodic hypoxia induced large reductions in

activity levels is unknown. Reductions in activity caused by pulses which occurred during the

active period can conceivably be explained by experiments which demonstrated that CO, ,,.

(maximum rate of oxygen consumption) is reduced at high altitude (Tucker et al. 1984,

Cymerman et al. 1989). It is possible that severe hypoxia could reduce + O , , to a level where

wheel running is no longer aerobically sustainable.

The mechanisms mediating the reduction in activity caused by hypoxic pulses that

occurred outside of the active period (i.e. CT6) are more difficult to understand because hamsters

would often run immediately following a pulse that occurred during the active period or even

between hypoxic episodes. Perhaps running following the pulse which occurred dunng the active

period (CT 1 1 and CT15) is for a different reason than the mnning following a pulse which

occurred outside the active period (CT6). Shivering following a hypoxic pulse and temperature

data indicate that Tb was reduced during hypoxic pulses. Perhaps the hamsters were mnning to

rewarrn following the pulse. It has previously been demonstrated that reduced ambient

temperatures cause hamsters to run (Mrosovsky and Biello 1994, Mistlberger et al. 1996).

The attenuation in activity following a pulse at CT6 could be caused by long lasting

changes in blood lactate flux. Exposure of rnarnrnals to hypoxia results in immediate

hyperventilation which in turn, leads to respiratory alkalosis because CO, is blown off. The

alkalosis is compensated principally by reduced net acid excretion via the kidneys (Gonzalez and

Clancy 1986). If the kidneys were to maintain a low level of acid excretion following the pulse it

is possible that the lactate levels nomally produced during activity bouts could not be removed

Page 125: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

reduced kidney lactate excretion would contribute to muscle fatigue and may reduce the overall

activity levels. However, a reduction in the rate of acid excretion has only been demonstrated

after several weeks at high altitude (Gonzalez and Clancy 1986, Brooks et al. 1991).

Furthermore, the tirne constant of changes in kidney excretion rates in hypoxia and on the retum

to normoxia are unmeasured. Other possible mechanisrns include a reduction in energy

substrates such as glycogen or long lasting changes in neurochemicals.

Future Experiments

The fact that three powerful respiratory stimuli did not directly influence the activity pacemaker

in hamsters raises several questions:

1. Do hamsters exhibit a tirne-of-day sensitivity to a respiratory stimulus?

Daily changes in respiratory chemosensitivity have already been demonstrated in

humans, rats and ducks (Raschke and Moller 1989, Peever and Stephenson 1997, Woodin and

Stephenson 1998). It would also be expected that hamsters would have a daily rhythm in

chemosensitivity .

2, a. Can LTF be induced in fossorial animals? 2, b. Do respiratory stimuli cause phase shifts in

non-fossorial animals?

One of the major reasons for maintaining that hypoxia and hypercapnia could cause

phase shifts in hamster activity rhythms is that these respiratory stimuli have been shown to be

arousing by stimulation of the defense reaction in several non-fossorial species (see Marshall

1987 for review). Arousal is a common feature in many non-photic stimuli (Mrosovsky 1996).

However, the behavioral and respiratory data obtained in the present study suggests that hypoxia

and hypercapnia were not arousing in the hamster. The possibility that adaptations to a

burrowing lifestyle have eliminated the defense reaction from the hypoxic and hypercapnic

respiratory response might explain the absence of both LTF and phase shifts in activity rhythms.

Page 126: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

mammals and (b) if respiratory stimuli cause phase shifts in non-fossorial mammals.

3. What is the role of exercise in hypoxia induced phase delays?

Reductions in activity levels are the most likely cause of the observed phase delays.

Therefore, it would be interesting to see if periods of immobilization similar to those induced by

hypoxia would cause similar phase delays. Matching phase delays would lend weight to the

hypothesis that hypoxia induced phase shifts are activity mediated. Developing a curve for

different levels of irnrnobilization versus the phase shift obtained would also determine if an

activity threshold exists for the phase shifts caused by reductions in activity.

Critique of the Method

Despite the fact that the apparatus was capable of producing data similar to that of other

studies using non-photic stimuli, the lack of a permanent phase shift by a single pulse of hypoxia

at CT6 suggests that the sample size may have been too small. Repetition of the experiment

would elucidate the effect of hypoxia on hamster activity rhythm.

Whole-body plethysmography demonstrated that constant hypoxia, episodic hypoxia and

hypercapnia evoked a respiratory response. However, Tb and TN used to calculate VT were

estimated from separate experiments. Since a 1 OC error in Tb would cause a 5% error in V, the

results of the statistical tests could have been affected.

In order to obtain an accurate measurernent of Tb implantable radio transmitters should

have been used. However, transrnitters were unavailable. The remaining option was the use of

thermocouples to obtain rectal temperature. Obtaining temperature measurements in this way

may cause hamsters discornfort due to the total restraint required and therefore placed limits on

the measurements that could be obtained. Hamsters have been immobilized for three hours in

other work (Van Reeth et al. 1990) and therefore this was taken to be the upper limit of restraint

time.

Page 127: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

O -

made:

1. The relationship between Tb and TN in air was assumed to be the same during hypoxia.

Over the small range of Tb observed in air there was no correlation between Tb and TN,

supporting the assumption that the difference between Tb and TN is constant.

2. Tb was assumed to fa11 at a constant rate throughout the period of hypoxia. This

assumption is supported by the fact that the reduction on T, during hypoxia is linear over 2

h period of observation. However, it is possible that the assumption caused a

underestimation of Tb during the final hour of constant hypoxia.

3. Tb was assumed to be unchanged during episodic hypoxia. Assuming that each four

minute hypoxic period within the episodic pulse caused the same reduction in Tb as the

initial four minutes of constant hypoxia, there would be a O. 1 "C reduction in Tb.

Consequently, any changes in Tb dunng episodic hypoxia would cause 0.5% change in V,

calcuIations. However, since episodic hypoxia evoked a hypometabolism it is likeiy that

temperature did fa11 further than O. 1 OC. Overestimating temperature would exaggerate the

difference in V, and VI between pre-pulse and pulse.

Despite the fact that these assumptions probably caused small errcsrs in V, it is unlikely that they

would have resulted in misinterpretation of the respiratory responses.

Conclusion

The results of this study would suggest that respiratory stimuli do not directly phase shift

the SCN. Since the small phase delays obtained correlate with reductions in activity it is quite

possible that alterations in behavioral states were responsible for the phase shifts obtained. This

study appears to be another example of the importance of activity and/or its correlate in phase

shifting of the SCN in hamsters.

Page 128: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

i ne possi~iiiry mat respiratory stimuli might rnodiIy the perlorrnance of the circadian

pacemaker has not previously been considered by most investigators. Although the results of the

present experiment indicate that respiratory stimuli do not cause large phase shifts, it is important

not to overgeneralize from these results. The possibility exists that adaptations to a fossorial

lifestyle may have prevented the respiratory stimuli from inducing phase shifts. Further

investigations are necessary to determine possible interactions between respiration and the

circadian system.

Page 129: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Aaron, E. A., Seow, K. C., Johnson, B. D. and Dempsey, J. A. 1992. Oxygen cost of

exercise and hyperpnea: implications for performance. Journal of Applied

Physiology 72: 18 18- 1825.

Aghajanian, G. K. and Wang, R. Y. 1977. Habenular and other rnidbrain raphe afferents

demonstrated by a modified retrograde tracing technique. Brain Research 122: 229-

242.

Albers, H. E. and Ferris, C. F. 1984. Neuropeptide Y: role in light-dark cycle entrainment

of hamster circadian rhythms. Neuroscience Letters 50: 163- 168.

Arnir, S. and Stewart, J. 1996. Resetting the circadian clock by a conditioned stimulus.

Nature 379: 542-545.

Aschoff, J. 1965. Response curves in circadian periodocity. In: Circadian Clocks. (Ed.)

North-Holland PubIishing Co: Amsterdam. pp. 96- 1 1 1.

Aschoff, J. 1970. Circadian rhythms of activity and of body temperature. In: Physiological

and behavioral temperature regulation. J. D. Hardy, A. P. Gagge and J. A. J.

Stolwijk (Ed.) Springfield IL. pp.

Aschoff, J. and Pohl, H. 1970. Rhythmic variations in energy metabolism. Federation

Proceedings 29: 154 1 - 1552.

Ashkenazi, 1. E., Ribak, J., Avgar, D. M. and Klepfish, A. 1982. Altitude and hypoxia as

phase shift inducers. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine 53: 342-346.

Bach, K. B., Ling, L., McCrimmon, D. R. and Mitchell, G. S. 1992. Repeated isocapnic,

hypoxic episodes cause long term facilitation of phrenic nerve activity in rats.

FASEB 7: 396.

Bach, K. B. and Mitchell, G. S. 1996. Hypoxia-induced long-term facilitation of

respiratory activity is serotonin dependent. Respiration Physiology 104: 25 1-260.

Page 130: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

induced phase shifts. Neuroscience 62: 273-279.

Biello, S. M. and Mrosovsky, N. 1993. Circadian phase-shifts induced by chlordiazepine

without increased locornotor activity. Brain Research 622: 58-62.

Birchfield, R. I., Sieker, H. 0. and Heyman, A. 1958. Alterations in blood gases during

natural sleep and narcolepsy. Neurology 8: 107- 1 12.

Bisgard, G. E. and Neubauer, J. A. 1995. Peripherai and central effects of hypoxia. In:

Regulation of Breathing. J. A. Dempsey and A. 1. Pack (Ed.) Marcel Dekker: New

York. pp. 617-668.

Bobrzynska, K. J., Godfrey, M. H. and Mrosovsky, N. 1996a. Serotonergic stimulation

and nonphotic phase-shifting in hamsters. Physiology and Behavior 59: 221-230.

Bobrzynska, K. J., Vrang, N. and Mrosovsky, N. 1996b. Persistence of nonphotic phase

shifts in hamsters after serotonin depletion in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Brain

Research 741: 205-214.

Boggs, D. F., Kilgore, D. L. and Birchard, G. F. 1984. Respiratory physiology of

burrowing marnrnals and birds. Comparative Biochernistry and Physiology 77A:

1-7.

Boulos, Z. and Rusak, B. 1982. Circadian phase response curves for dark pulses in the

Hamster. Journal of Comparative Physiology 146: 41 1-4 17.

Bowes, G. M., Townsend, E. R., Kozar, L. F., Brornley, S. M. and PhilIipson, E. A.

198 1. Effect of carotid body denervation on arousal response to hypoxia in sleeping

dogs. American Journal of Physiology 51: 40-45.

Brebia, D. R. and Altshuler, K. 2. 1965. Oxygen consumption rate and

electroencephalographic stage of sleep. Science 150: 162 1 - 1623.

Brooks, G. A., Butterfield, G. E., Wolfe, R. R., Groves, B. M., Mazzeo, R. S., Sutton,

J. R., Wolfel, E. E., et al. 1991. Decreased reliance on lactate during exercise after

acclirnatization to 4,300~11. Journal of Applied Physiology 71: 333-341.

Page 131: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

of Applied Physiology 62: 389-402.

Cao, K., Zwillich, C. W., Berthon-Jones, M. and Sullivan, C. E. 1992. Increased

normoxic ventilation induced by repetitive hypoxia in conscious dogs. Journal of

Applied Physiology 73: 2083-2088.

Chapin, J. L. 1954. Ventilatory response of the unrestrained and unanesthtized hamster to

C02. Arnerican Journal of Physiology 179: 146-148.

Chapman, R. W., Santiago, T. V. and Edelman, N. H. 1979. Effects of graded reduction

of brain blood flow on ventilation in unanesthetized goats. Arnerican Journal of

Physiology 47: 104- 1 1 1 .

Cherniack, N. S. and Longobardo, G. S. 1986. Abnormalities in respiratory rhythm. In:

Handbook of Physiology. N. S. Cherniack and J. G. Widdicombe (Ed.) American

Physiological Society: Bethesda, MD. pp. 729-749.

Chew, R. M., Lindberg, R. G. and Hayden, P. 1965. Journal of Marnmal46: 477.

Cohen, M. 1. 1979. Neurogenesis of respiratory rhythm in the marnmal. Physiology

Review 59: 1 105- 1 173.

Cymerman, A., Reeves, J. T., Sutton, J. R., Rock, P. B., Groves, B. M., Malconian, M.

K., Young, P. M., et al. 1989. Operation Everest II: maximal oxygen uptake at

extreme altitude. Journal of Applied Physiology 66: 2446-2453.

Duffin, J. 1990. The chernoreflex and the control of breathing. Canadian Journal of

Anesthesia 37: 933-942.

Duffin, J. A. 1991. A mode1 of respiratory rhythm generation. Neuroreport 2: 623-626.

Duncan, W. C., Tamarkin, L., Sokolove, P. G. and Wehr, T. A. 1988. Chronic

clorgyline treatment of the syrian hamster: an anaiysis of effects on the circadian

pacemaker. Journal of Biological Rhythm 3: 305-322.

Page 132: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

--- -- - - -- - , -. - s V-. AL'-. " . . *AC ..."UV. -1 U~VI.LUIIUVU~ LLILUIILUI U ~ J J I I ~ I V l l I L i I U L I V L 1 111

humans. In: Vertebrate Circadian Systems. J. Aschoff, S. Daan and G. Groos

(Ed.) Springer-Verlag: Berlin-Heidelberg. pp. 262-267.

Edgar, D. and Dement, W. C. 199 1. Regularly scheduled voluntary exercise syncronized

the rnouse circadian clock. American Journal of Physiology 261: R928-R933.

Edgar, D. M., Miller, J. D., Prosser, R. A., Dean, R. R. and Dement, W. C. 1993.

Serotonin and the marnrnalian circadian system: II. Phase shifting rat behavioral

rhythms with serotonergic agonists. Journal of Biological Rhythms 8: 17-32.

Eichler, V. B. and Moore, R. Y. 1974. The primary and accessory optic systems in the

golden hamster, Mesocricetus auratus. Acta Anatomica 89: 359-37 1.

- Eldridge, F. L. and Millhorn, D. E. 1986. Oscillation, gating, and memory in the

respiratory control system. In: Handbook of Physiology II. N. S. Cherniack and J.

G. Widdicombe (Ed.) American Physiological Society: Bethesda, MD. pp. 93-1 14.

Enright, J. T. 1975. The circadian tape recorder and its entrainment. In: Physiological

Adaptations to the Environment. F. J. Vernberg (Ed.) Intext Educational

Publishers: New York. pp. 465-476.

Feldman, J. L. and Smith, J. C. 1995. Neural control of respiratory pattern in rnarnmals:

an overview. In: Regulation of Breathing. J. A. Dempsey and A. 1. Pack (Ed.)

Marcel Dekker: New York. pp. 39-69.

Foster, R. G. 1993. Photoreceptors and circadian systems. Current Directions in

Psychological Science 2: 34-39.

Frappell, P., Lanthier, C., Baudinette, R. V. and Mortola, J. P. 1992. Metabolism and

ventilation in acute hypoxia: a comparative analysis in small marnrnalian species.

American Journal of Phy siology 262: R 1040-R 1046.

Fraser, G., Trinder, J., Colrain, 1. M. and Montgomery, 1. 1989. Effect of sleep and

circadian cycle on sleep period energy expenditure. Journal of Applied Physiology

66: 830-836.

Page 133: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - r - i i - . v a a , y. U A . - &-YU) AIA. 1 L I I J - A I IU1 V U A L V U 1 rllb&Jb J l l l L L J 1 1 1 I l u I I k a L b k b l l b a U l a l I

rhythms elicited by voluntary wheel running. Journal of Biological Rhythms 10:

196-2 1 O.

Gautherie, M. 1973. Circadian rhythm in vasornotor oscillations of skin temperature in

man. International Journal of Chronobiology '1: 103- 139.

Gautier, H., Bonora, M. and Lahiri, S. 1992. Control of metabolic and ventilatory

responses to cold in anesthetized cats. Respiration Physiology 87: 309-324.

Gautier, H., Bonora, M. and Remrners, J. E. 1989. Effects of hypoxia on metabolic rate

of consicous adult cats and cold exposure. Journal of Applied Physiology 67: 32-

38.

Gautier, H., Bonora, M. and Trinh, H. C. 1993. Ventilatory and metabolic responses to

cold and CO2 in intact and carotid body-denervated awake rats. Journal of Applied

Physiology 75: 2570-2579.

Gibbs, F. P. 1983. Temperature dependence of the hamster circadian pacemaker. American

Journal of Physiology 244: 607-610.

Girardier, L. and Stock, M. J. 1983. Marnmalian Thermogenesis. Chapman and HaIl:

London.

Gonzalez, N. C. and Clancy, R. L. 1986. Intracellular pH regulation during prolonged

hypoxia in rats. Respiration Physiology 65: 2 13-227.

Gothe, B., Altose, M. D., Goldman, M. D. and Chemiack, N. S. 1981. Effect of quiet

sleep on resting and C02-stimulated breathing in humans. Journal of Applied

Physiology 50: 57-64.

Greenberg, M. E. and Ziff, E. B. 1984. Stimulation of 3T3 cells induces transcription of

the c-fos proto-oncogene. Nature 311: 433-437.

Guy, J., Bosler, O., Dusticier, G., Pelletier, G. and Calas, A. 1987. Morphologicai

correlates of serotonin-neuropeptide Y interactions in the rat suprachiasmatic

Page 134: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Research 250: 657-662.

Hagberg, H., Lehmann, A., Sandberg, M., Nystrom, B., Jacobson, 1. and Hamberger,

A. 1985. Ischemia-induced shift of inhibitory and excitatory arnino acids form

intra- and extracelllar compartments. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and

Metabolism 5: 4 13-4 19.

Halberg, F. 1969. Human circadian circulatory rhythms during weightlessness. Space Life

Science 2: 18-32.

Hayden, P. and Lindberg, R. 1969. Circadian rhythm in marnmalian body temperature

entrained by cyclic pressure changes. Science 164: 1288- 1289.

Heller, H. C. and Glotzbach, S. F. 1977. Thermoregulation during sleep and hibernation.

International Review of Physiology 25: 147- 188.

Heller, W. C., Graf, R. and Rautenberg, W. 1983. Circadian and arousal state influences

on thermoregulation in the pigeon. American Journal of Physiology 245: R32 1-

R328.

Heller, H. G. 1987. Sleep and Hypometabolism. Canadian Joumal of Zoology 66: 61-69.

Henderson, D. R., Konkle, D. M., Bartholow, S. D. and Mitchell, G. S. 1996. Effects of

chronic serotonin reuptake inhibition on long term modulation of the exercise

ventilatory response in goats. Physiologist 39: 178.

Hilton, S. M. and Marshall, J. M. 1982. The pattern of cardiovascular response to carotid

chemoreceptor stimulation in the cat. Journal of Physiology 326: 495-5 13.

Holkfelt, T., Fuxe, K., Goldstein, M. and Johanson, 0. 1974. Immunohistochernical

evidence for the existence of adrenaline neurons in the cat brain. Brain Research

66: 235-251.

Honrado, G. 1. and Mrosovsky, N. 2989. Arousal by sexual stimuli accelerates the re-

entrainment of hamsters to phase advanced light-dark cycles. Behavioral Ecoiogy

and Sociobiology 25: 57-63.

Page 135: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

- - - ..----.--, --- -. ---------- -y--.I. a s - . . - i - - i ~ ~ ~ y ç . ~ ~ i. . ~ . A V I V I I . Y V L Y U A l l L W L41W J

suprachiasmatic region phase shifts circadian rhythms in constant darkness.

Peptides 15: 57-63.

Jacky, J. P. 1980. Bwometric rneasurement of tidal volume: effects of pattern and nasal

temperature. Journal of Applied Physiology 49: 3 19-325.

Jacobs, B. L. and Azmitia, E. C. 1992. Structure and function of the brain serotonin

system. Physiological Reviews 72: 165-228.

Janik, D. and Mrosovsky, N. 1993. Nonphotically induced phase shifts of circadian

rhythms in the golden hamster: Activity-response curves at different arnbient

temperatures. Physiology of Behavior 53: 43 1-436.

- Janik, D. and Mrosovsky, N. 1994. Intergeniculate leaflet lesions and behaviorally-

induced shifts of circadian rhythms. Brain Research 651: 174- 182.

Jennings, D. B. and Laupacis, A. 1982. The effect of body warming on the ventilatory

response to CO2 in the awake dog. Respiration Physiology 49: 355-369.

Johnson, R. F., Moore, R. Y. and Morin, L. P. 1989. Lateral geniculate lesions alter

activity rhythms in the hamster. Brain Research Bulletin 22: 41 1-422.

Kaminski, R. P., Forster, H. V., Bisgard, G. E., Pan, L. G., Dorsey, S. M. and Barber,

B. J. 1985. Effects of altered arnbient temperature on metabolic rate during CO2

inhalation. Journal of Applied Physiology 58: 1592- 1596.

Kayser, C. and Heusner, A. 1967. Journal of Physiology 59: 3.

Kleitman, N. 1963. Sleep and Wakefulness. University of Chicago Press: Chicago.

Kornhauser, J. M., Mayo, K. E. and Takahashi, J. S. 1993. 12. Immediate-early gene

expression in a marnmalian circadian pacemaker: the suprachiasmatic nucleus. In:

Molecular Genetics of Biological Rhythrns. (Ed.) pp. 27 1-307.

Kornhauser, J. M., Nelson, D. E., Mayo, K. E. and Takahashi, J. S. 1990. Photic and

circadian regulation of c-fos gene expression in the hamster suprachiasmatic

nucleus. Neuron 5: 127- 134.

Page 136: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

hypercapnia on body temperatures and cold induced thermogenesis in the golden

hamster. Journal of Thermal Biology 12: 103-107.

Lees, J. G., Hallonquist, J. D. and Mrosovsky, N. 1983. Differential effects of dark

pulses on the two components of split circadian activity rhythm in golden

hamsters. Journal of Comparative Physiology 153: 123- 132.

Loeschcke, H. H., DeLattre, J., Schlaefke, M. E. and Trouth, C. 0. 1970. Effects on

respiration and circulation of electrically stimulating the ventral surface of the

medulla oblangata. Respiratory Physiology 10: 184- 197.

Lovenberg, T. W., Baron, B. M., de Lecea, L., Miller, J. D., Prosser, R. A., Rea, M.

A., Foye, P. E., et al. 1993. A novel adenylyl cyclase-activating serotonin receptor

(5HT7) implicated in the regulation of mammalian circadian rhythms. Neuron 11:

449-458.

Lucente, M., Rebuzzi, A. G., Lanza, G. A. and Tamburi, S. 1987. Circadian rhythms in

pacemaker dependent patients. In: Social Diseases and Chronobiology. B. Tarquini

(Ed.) Societa Editrice Esculapio: Bologna, Itaiy. pp.

Mainardi, G., Maggioni, C., Halberg, F. and Hurley, P. 1987. Family history of high

blood pressure and diabetes and newborn's circadian bIood pressure amplitude. In:

Social Diseases and Chronobiology. B. Tarquini (Ed.) Societa Editrice Esculapio:

Bologna, Italy. pp.

Mainwood, G. W. 1987. The pH dependence of the contractile response of fatigued

skeletal muscle. Second international symposium on excitation-contraction coupling

in skeletal, cardiac and smooth muscle f38-658.

Marshall 1984. Cardiovascular responses to peripheral chemoreceptor stimulation in the

rat. Journal of Physiology 354: 64.

Marshall, J. M. 1987. Contribution to overalI cardiovascular controI made by the

chemoreceptor-induced alerting/defence response. In: Neurobiology of the

Page 137: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

VU. Y . V L V Y J~I IULVA J U J L I L w L L L i Ui 1 V . 1 UJ L V A \bu./ ATAUll\rll\rJLQl U LI1 V b I J l L J 1 1 Q 3 3 .

Manchester. pp. 222-240.

Martin-Body, R. L. and Johnston, B. M. 1988. Central origin of the hypoxic depression

of breathing in the newborn. Respiration Physiology 71: 25-32.

Maskrey, M. 1990. Body temperature effects on hypoxic and hypercapnic responses in

awake rats. American Journal of Physiology 259: 492-498.

Mateika, J. H. and Duffin, J. 1995. A review of the control of breathing during exercise.

European Journal of Applied Physiology 71: 1-27,

McGinty, D. J. 1982. Sleep mechanisms: biology and control of REM sleep. International

Review of Neurobiology 23: 39 1-437.

McMillen, 1. C., Nowak, R., Walker, D. W. and Young, 1. R. 1990. Maternal

pinealectomy alters the daily pattern of fetal breathing in sheep. Amencan Journal of

Physiology 258: R284-R287.

Meijer, J. H. and Groos, G. A. 1988. Responsiveness of suprachiasmatic and ventral

lateral geniculate neurons to serotonin and irniprarnine: A rnicroiontophoretic study

in normal and imipramine-treated rats. Brain Research Bulletin 20: 89-96.

Melton, J. E., Neubauer, J. A. and Edelman, N. H. 1990. GABA antagonism reverses

hypoxic respiratory depression in the cat. Journal of Applied Physiology 69: 1296-

1301.

Menaker, M. 1978. The physiology of circadian pacemakers. Annual Review of

Physiology 40: 50 1-526.

Meyer-Bernstein, E. L. and Morin, L. P. 1996. Differentiâl serotonergic innervation of the

suprachiasmatic nucleus and the intergeniculate leaflet and its role in circadian

rhythm modulation. The Journal of Neuroscience 16: 2097-21 1 1.

Michels, K. M., Morin, L. P. and Moore, R. Y. 1990. GABA~/benzodiazepine receptor

localization in the circadian timing system. Brain Research 531: 16-24.

Page 138: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Physiology 264: R82 1 -R832.

Millhorn, D. E. and Eldridge, F. L. 1986. Role of ventrolateral medulla in regulation of

respiratory function, at the ventral surface of the cat's medulla. Neuroscience 18:

43-49.

Millhorn, D. E., Eldridge, F. L., Kiley, J. P. and Waldrop, T. G. 1984. Prolonged

inhibition of respiration followed by acute hypoxia in glomectornized cats.

Respiratory Physiology 57: 33 1-340.

Millhorn, D. E., Eldridge, F. L. and Wddrop, T. G. 1980a. Prolonged stimulation of

respiration by a new central neural mechanism. Respiration Physiology 41: 87-

103.

Millhorn, D. E., Eldridge, F. L. and Waldrop, T. G. 1980b. Prolonged stimulation of

respiration by endogenous central serotonin. Respiration Physiology 42: 17 1 - 188.

Mistlberger, R. E. 1991. Scheduled daily exercise or feeding alters the phase of photic

entrainment in Syrian hamsters. Physiology and Behavior 50: 1257-1 260.

Mistlberger, R. E., Marchant, E. G. and Sinclair, S. V. 1996. Nonphotic phase-shifting

and the motivation to run: cold exposure reexamined. Journal of Biological

Rhythms 11: 208-215.

Mitchell, R. A., Loeschchke, H. H., Severinghaus, J. W. and Massion, W. H. 1963.

Respiration and cerebrospinal fluid pH in metabolic acidosis and alkalosis. Journal

of Applied Physiology 20: 905-9 1 1.

Moore, R. Y. 1995. Organization of the mammalian circadian system. In: Circadian clocks

and their adjustment. (Ed.) Ciba Foundation Symposium: pp. 88-106.

Moore, R. Y. and Card, J. P. 1994. The intergeniculate leaflet: an anatornically and

functionalIy distinct subdivision of the lateral geniculate compkx. Journal of

Comparative Neurology 344: 403-430.

Page 139: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

. - - -

Comparative Neurology 146: 1- 14.

Moore-Ede, Sulzman, F. M. and Fuller, C. A. 1982. The CIocks That Time Us. Harvard

University Press: Cambridge, Massachusetts, and London, England.

Morin, L. P. 1994. The circadian visual system. Brain Research Reviews 67: 102-127.

Morin, L. P. and Blanchard, J. H. 1991. Depletion of brain serotonin by 5,7-DHT

modifies hamster circadian rhythm response to light. Brain Research 566: 173-

185.

Mortola, J. P. 199 1. Hamsters versus rats: ventilatory responses in adults and newborns.

Respiration Physiology 85: 305-3 17.

Mortola, J. P. and Dotta, A. 1992. Effects of hypoxia and arnbient temperature on gaseous

metabolism of newborn rats. Amencan Journal of Physiology 267-272.

Mortola, J. P. and Gautier 1995. Interaction between metabolism and ventilation: effects of

respiratory gases and temperature. In: Regulation of Breathing. J. A. Dempsey and

A. 1. Pack (Ed.) Marcel Dekker: New York. pp.

Mortola, J. P., Rezzonico, R. and Lanthier, C. 1989. Ventilation and oxygen consumption

during acute hypoxia in newborn mammals: a comparative analysis. Respiration

Physiology 78: 3 1-43.

Mrosovsky, N. 1988. Phase response curves for social entrainment. Journal of

Comparative Physiology 1988: 35-46.

Mrosovsky, N. 1993. Photic phase shifting in hamsters: more than meets the eye. Light

Treatment and Biological Rhythms 5: 34-36.

Mrosovsky, N. 1996. Locomotor activity and non-photic influences on cirdadian clocks.

Biological Review 71: 343-372.

Mrosovsky, N. 1996. Methods of measuring phase shifts: why 1 continue to use an

Aschoff type II procedure despite the skepticism of referees. Chronobiology

International 13: 387-392.

Page 140: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

4 - r a vu- 7 ui-J r 7 . u r i u u r u s a v , W . A V A - A x/r. A ~ v i i y i i w r l ~ yiiu~u Q I d 1 1 L 1 1 1 6 1 1 1 L 1 1 b UlU U A I U C L L b bV1U.

Chronobiology International 11: 232-252.

Mrosovsky, N., Reebs, S. G., Honrado, G. 1. and Salmon, P. A. 1989. Behavioral

entrainment of circadian rhythms. Expenmentia 45: 696-702.

Mrosovsky, N. and Salmon, P. A. 1990. Triazolam and phase-shifting acceleration re-

evaluated. Chronobiology International 7: 35-41.

Mrosovsky, N., Salmon, P. A., Menaker, M. and Ralph, M. R. 1992. Nonphotic phase

shifting in hamster dock mutants. Journal of Biological Rhythms 7: 4 1-49.

Muller, R., Brvo, R., Burckhardt, J. and Curran, T. 1984. Induction of c-fos gene and

protein by growth factors precedes activaiton of c-mye. Nature 312: 7 16-720.

Nelson, D. E. and Takahashi, 1. S. 1991. Sensitivity and integration in a visual pathway

for circadian entrainment in the hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Journal of

Physiology 439: 1 15- 145.

Neubauer, J. A., Melton, J. E. and Edelman, N. M. 1990. Modulation of respiration

during brain hypoxia. Journal of Applied Physiology 68: 44 1-45 1.

Neubauer, J. A., Santiago, T. V. and Edelman, N. H. 198 1. Hypoxic arousal in intact and

carotid chemodenervated sleeping cats. Journal of Applied Physio1ogy:Respiration

Environment and Exercise Physiology 51: 12%- 1299.

Neubauer, J. A., Santiago, T. V., Posner, M. A. and Edelman, N. H. 1985. Ventral

medullary pH and ventilatory responses to hyperperfusion and hypoxia. Journal of

Applied Physiology 58: 1659- 1668.

Nishimura, M. A., Suzuki, A., Nishiura, Y., Yamamoto, H., Miyamoto, K., Kishi, F.

and Kawakarni, Y. 1987. Effects of brain blood flow on hypoxic ventilatory

response in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology 63: 1100-1 106.

Orem, J. 1995. The wakfulness stimulus for breathing. In: Regdation of Breathing. J. A.

Dempsey and A. 1. Pack (Ed.) Marcel Dekker: New York. pp. 1 13- 155.

Page 141: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

1975. Cerebrospinal fluid alkalosis during high-altitude sojum in unanesthetized

ponies. Respiraiton Physiology 25: 23-37.

Pack, A. 1. 1995. Changes in respiratory motor activity during rapid eye movement sleep.

In: Regulation of Breathing. J. A. Dempsey and A. 1. Pack (Ed.) Marcel Dekker:

New York. pp. 983-1010.

Peever, J. and Stephenson, R. 1997. Day-night differences in the respiratory response to

hypercapnia in awake adult rats. American Journal of Physiology 52: 233-245.

Phillipson, E. A. and Bowes, G. 1986. Control of breathing during sleep. In: Handbook

of Physiology. N. S. Cherniack and J. G. Widdicombe (Ed.) American

Physiological Society: Bethesda, MD. pp. 649-689.

Pickard, G. E., Ralph, M. R. and Menaker, M. 1987. The intergeniculate leaflet partially

mediates effects of light on circadian rhythms. Journal of BioIogical Rhythms 2:

35-56.

Pittendrigh, C. S. 1965. Biological clocks: the functions, ancient and modem, of circadian

oscillators. In: The tenth anniversary AFOSR scientific serninar. D. L. (Ed.)

University of New Mexico: Albuquerque. pp. 96- 1 1 1.

Powell, F. L. and Aaron, E. A. 1993. Long-Tenn Facilitation of ventilation after hypoxia

in awake rats. FASEB 7: 397.

Prosser, R. A., Miller, J. D. and Heller, H. C. 1990. A serotonin agonist phase-shifts the

circadian dock in the suprachiasmatic nuclei in vitro. Brain Research 534: 336-

339.

Ralph, M. R. and Mrosovsky, N. 1992. Behavioral inhibition of circadian responses in

light. Journal of Biological rhythms 7: 353-359.

Raschke, F. and Moller, K. H. 1989. Untersuchungen zur tagesrhythmik der

chemosensitivitat und deren beitrag zu nachtlichen atmungsregulationsstorungen.

Pneumologie 43: 568-57 1.

Page 142: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

Harbor Symposium of Quantitative Biology 25: 105- 1 13.

Rea, M. A. 1989. Light increases fos-related protein immunoreactivity in the rat

suprachiasmatic nuclei. Brain Research Bulletin 23: 577-58 1.

Reebs, S. G. and Mrosovsky, N. 1989a. Acoustical entrainment of circadian activity

rhythms in house sparrows: constant light is not necessary. Ethology 80: 172- 18 1.

Reebs, S. G., Lavery, R. J. and Mrosovsky, N. 1989b. Running activity mediates the

phase-advancing effects of dark pulses on hamster circadian rhythms. JournaI of

Comparative Physiology A 165: 8 1 1-8 18.

Reebs, S. G. and Mrosovsky, N. 1989. Effects of induced wheel running on the circadian

activity rhythms syrian hamsters: entrainment and phase response curve. Journal of

Biological Rhythms 4: 39-48.

Richter, C. P. 1975. Deep hypothemiia and its effect on the 24-hour clock of rats and

hamsters. Johns Hopkins Medical Journal 136: 1- 10.

Rusak, B., Meijer, J. H. and Harrington, M. E. 1989. Hamster circadian rhythms are

phase shifted by electricd stimulation of the geniculo-hypothalamic tract. Brain

Research 493: 165- 17 1.

Rusak, B., Mistlberger, R. E., Losier, B. and Jones, C. H. 1988. Daily hoarding

opportunity entrains the pacemaker for hamster activity rhythms. Journal of

Comparative Physiology 164: 165- 17 1.

Rusak, B., Robertson, H. A., Wisden, W. and Hunt, S. P. 1990. Light pulses that shift

rhythms induce gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Science 248:

1237- 1240.

Rusak, B. and Zucker, 1. 1979. Neural regulation of circadian rhythms. Physiological

Reviews 449-526.

Page 143: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

--..-A) U.) A I I U L U U V I \ U , A . UllU I T I W L L V I U ) Ji A A . / / 7 - I I A V C U U V I L ~ b U 4 U T S r l l C L l U L U L J L L I L b L U b L I V L l 1 1 1

conscious rats: effect of hypoxia and ambient temperature. Journal of Applied

Physiology 76: 1594- 1595.

Saiki, C. and Mortola, J. P. 1996. Effect of CO2 on the metabolic and ventilatory

responses to arnbient temperature in conscious adult and newborn rats. Journal of

Physiology 491: 261 -269.

Sayeski, P. P., Edgar, D. M., Miller, J. D. and Dement, W. D. 1990. The role of

aminergic agents in circadian time keeping. Soc. Res. Biol. Rhythms 2: 5 1.

Schlaefke, M. E. 198 1. Central chemosensitivity: a respiratory drive. Reviews of

Physiological and Biochemical Pharmacology 90: 172-244.

Shapiro, C. M., Goll, C. R., Cohen, G. R. and Oswald, 1. 1984. Heat production during

sleep. Journal of Applied Physiology 56: 67 1-677.

Smith, J. C., Ellenberger, M. R., Ballanyi, K., Richter, D. W. and Feldman, J. L. 1991.

Pre-Botzinger complex: a brainstem region that may generate respiratory rhythm in

mammals. Science 254: 726-729.

Stupfel, M. 1974. Carbon dioxide and temperature regulation of homeothermic animals. In:

Carbon Dioxide and Metabolic Regulation. G. Nahas and K. E. Schaefer (Ed.)

Springer-Verlag: New York. pp. 163- 186.

Summer, T. L. and McCormack, C. E. 1984. Phase-response and Aschoff illumination

curves for the locomoter activity rhythm of the rat. American Journal of Physiology

246: R299-R304.

Sumova, A., Maywood, E. S., Selvage, D., Ebling, F. J. P. and Hastings, M. H. 1996.

Serotonergic antagonists impair arousal-induced phase shifts of the circadian

system of the hamster. Brain Research 709: 88-96.

Takahashi, J. S., DeCoursey, P. J., Bauman, L. and Menaker, M. 1984. Spectral

sensitivity of a novel photoreceptive system mediating entrainment of marnmalian

circadian rhythms. Nature 308: 186-1 88.

Page 144: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

effect of carbonic acid on epinephrine response. Arnerican Journal of Physiology

187: 34 1 -346.

Tenney, S. M. and St. John, W. M. 1980.1s there localized cerebral cortical influence on

hypoxic ventilatory response? Respiration Physiology 41: 227-232.

Tucker, A., Stager, J. M. and Cordain, L. 1984. Arterial02 saturation and maximum 0 2

consumption in moderate-altitude runners exposed to sea level and 3,050 m. JAMA

252: 2867-287 1.

Turek, F. W. and Gwinner, E. 1982. Role of hormones in the circadian organization of

vertebrates. In: Vertebrate Circadian Systems. J. Aschoff, S. Daan and G. Groos

(Ed.) Springer-Verlag: Berlin-Heidelberg. pp. 173- 182.

Turek, F. W. and Losee-Olson, S. 1988. The circadian rhythm of LH release can be

shifted by injections of a benzodiazepine in female golden hamsters. Endocrinology

122: 756-758.

Turner, D. L. and Mitchell, G. S. 1997. Long-term facilitation of ventilation following

repeated hypoxic episodes in awake goats. Journal of Physiology 499: 543-550.

Van Reeth, O., Hinch, D., Tecco, J. M. and Turek, F. W. 1991. The effects of short

periods of immobilization on the hamster circadian dock. Brain Research 545:

208-2 14.

van Reeth, 0. and Turek, F. W. 1989. Stimulated activity mediates phase shifts in the

hamster circadian clock induced by dark pulses or benzodiazepines. Nature 339:

49-5 1.

Vertes, R. P. 199 1. A PHA-1 analysis of ascending projections of the dorsal raphe nucleus

in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 313: 643-668.

Waites, B. A., Ackland, G. L., Noble, R. and Hanson, M. A. 1996. Lesions to the red

nucleus abolish the biphasic respiratory response to isocapnic hypoxia in

decerebrate young rabbits. Journal of Physiology 495: 2 17-225.

Page 145: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

and rats to hypoxia and hypercapnia. Journal of Applied Physiology 59: 1955-

1960.

Waterhouse, B. D., Border, B., Wahl, L. and Mihailoff, G. A. 1993. Topographic

organization of rat locus coenileus and dorsal raphe nuc1ei:distribution of cells

projecting to visual system structures. Journal of Comparative Neurology 336:

345-36 1.

White, D. P., Weil, J. V. and Zwillich, C. W. 1985. Metabolic rate and breathing during

sleep. Journal of Applied Physiology 59: 384-39 1.

Wickland, C. and Turek, F. W. 1994. Lesions of the thalarnic intergeniculate leaflet block

activity-induced phase shifts in the circadian activity rhythm of the golden hamster.

Brain Research 660: 293-300.

Withers, P. C. 1977. Measurement of Vos, VC02, and evaporative water loss with a flow

through mask. Journal of Applied Physiology 42: 120- 123.

Woodin, M. and Stephenson, R. 1998. Circadian rhythms in diving behavior and the

ventiIatory response to asphyxia in Canvasback ducks. American Journal of

Physiology in press:

Wyke, B. 1963. NeurologicaI effects of changes in hydrogen ion concentration. In: Brain

Function and Metabolic Disorders. (Ed.) Buttenvorths: London. pp. 37-55.

Yan, S., Laferriere, A., Zhang, C. and Moss, 1. R. 1995. Microdialyzed adenosine in

nucleus tractus solitarii and ventilatory response to hypoxia in piglets. Journal of

Applied Physiology 79: 405-41 0.

Page 146: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

The correlation of nasal temperature with body temperature

Pressure changes associated with ventilation are dependent upon changes in temperature

of the inspired air. During inhalation the air is heated to Tb. Air is subsequently cooled rapidly to

TN during exhalation (Epstein and Epstein 1978, Jacky 1980). Consequently, VT calculations

rely on a measure of Tb and TN. In order for the respiratory experiment to demonstrate that the

respiratory stimuli are in fact stimulating the respiratory systern a normoxic-pulse cornparison

was al1 that was necessary. As long as Tb rernains constant, any enor associated with inaccurate

temperature data will be systematic with negligible effects on the comparisons.

Hypercapnia is known not to affect Tb at a Tc of about 20°C (Stupfel et al. 1974, Saiki

and Mortola 1996, Peever and Stephenson 1997). However, Tb has been shown to fa11 during

continuous hypoxia (Frappe11 et al. 1992, Mortola and Dotta 1992). To estimate for the reduction

in Tb, an experiment was conducted in which measured Tb was measured over a two hour

hypoxic pulse and a one hour recovery. It was assumed that Tb remained constant throughout the

episodic hypoxia pulse.

Methods:

Five hamsters were used. Al1 experiments were performed between CT6 and CT9. The

hamsters were restrained in a Plexiglas cylinder which was just small enough to prevent the

hamsters from turning around but allowed some head and leg movement. Tb was measured by

placing a thermocouple probe (Bat- 10, Physiotemp) approximately 4 cm into the rectum. TN was

obtained by placing a simiiar thermocouple in the expiratory airstream irnmediately outside the

nostril. The thermocouples were hard wired via a universal amplifier ( mode1 G-4123-01, Gould

Electronics) to a computer (MacLab/8, Analog Digital Instruments Ltd.) for recording. Once the

rectal therrnocouple was in place the animals were left for ten minutes. Tb was recorded over the

entire experimental period while TN was obtained for at least two uninterrupted fifteen second

intervals.

Page 147: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

restrainer were placed in the plethysmograph where they were exposed to 5% 0 2 for two hours

and then air for one hour. The third hour of Tb was obtained by extrapolation of a regression line

based on the first two hours of hypoxia. The animals were not exposed to three hours of hypoxia

in the restrainer in order to reduce their discornfort.

Results:

The mean Tb was 37.1+1- 0.2 "C (n = 5) in animals breathing air. The mean TN was 3 1.4

+/- 0.5 "C (n = 5). Their was no correlation between Tb and TN over the srnall range of Tb used

in the expenment (P < 0.05). The relationship between Tb and TN in hypoxia and hypercapnia

was assumed to be the same as it was in air.

The effect of episodic hypoxia on Tb is unknown.The equation of the regression line of

Tb during hypoxia is (Fig A 1.1):

Tb = -0.034(Time) + 37.7

Page 148: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

represent TN in air. Solid circles represent Tb during 5% constant hypoxia.

Page 149: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

-40 -20 O 2 0 40 6 0 8 0 100 120 140

Tirne (min.)

Page 150: THE EFFEcTS OF HYPOXIA AND HYPERCAPNLA ON … · paper or electronic formats. The author retains ownership of the copyright in this thesis. Neither the thesis nor substantid extracts

APPLlED 1 I M G E . lnc - - - 1653 East Main Street - - * - - Rochester. NY 14609 USA -- -- - - Phone: i l 61482-0300 -- -- - - Fax: i l 61288-5989

O 1993. Applied Image. Inc.. All Rights Resewed