the role of cognition in marital relationships

8
Journal  of Consulting and Clinical  Psychology 1989,  Vol.  57, No.  1,31-38 Copyright  1989  b y  t he  American Psycho logical  Association,  Inc. 0022-006X/89/S00.75 T h e  Role  o f  C ogniti ons  i n  M ari tal elati on ships:  Definitional, ethodological, and  C oncept ual  Issues Donald  H  B aucom University of  North Ca rolina  at  Chapel Hill Norman  E pstein University  of Maryland,  College Park Steven Sayers and  Tamara  Goldman Sher University of  North Ca rolina  a t  Chap el Hill Although  there  have  recently been numerous investiga tions exploring the role of  couples'  cognitions in  a n  attempt  to  understand marital  distress,  a t  prese nt there  i s  little co hes ion  an d  direction  in the study  of how  couples  think abou t their  relationship s.  The c urrent article  asserts  that  this  lack of direction results  from  a t  least  three  factors:  (a) a lack of delineation of the important cogn itive variables  to be considered in marital  functioning,  (b) conceptual nd m ethodological  difficulties  that arise  in  attempts  to  operationalize  cognitivevariables,  and (c) a  dearth  of  models  o f  marital function- in g  that incorpo rate cognition s in a  detailed  manne r. Th ese three  factors  are  discussed,  along  with  a review  of  e mpirical investigations supporting the imp ortance  of  cognitions in intimate relationships. T h e  role  o f  cognitions  in  intimate relationship s  h a s  gained  in - creasing  attention  i n  recent  years.  Most  work  h a s  focused  on the area  o f  c aus al attributions,  a n d  numerous in vestigations  have demonstrated  empirical  relations  between various types  o f  attri- butions  a n d  level  o f  marital maladjustment (see B aucom, 1987, a n d  Thompson  & Snyder, 1986, for  recent  reviews of  this  work). However,  the study of c ogn itions in intimate relationship s has little coherent direction  o f  movement,  either  from a  research per- spective or in  terms  of the  treatment  o  ma rital distress. This  lack  o f  focus  a nd  direction probably stems  from  at  least three  factors:  (a )  insufficient  delineation  o f  important  catego- ries  o f  m arital cognitions othe r than attributions  (with  t h e  work o n  relationship  beliefs  by Eidelson &  Epstein,  1982,  being  o n e excep tion), (b)  methodological  a n d  conceptua l problems  in  o p - erationalizing  cognitive variables  a n d  comparing  disparate measures used  i n  different  studies,  and (c) few  descriptions  o f exp licit mod els  to  direct  future  research  and to  clarify  the  roles that  cognitions  play  i n  intimate relationships (w ith  th e  models proposed  by  Bradbury  &  Fincham ,  1987,  and Do herty, 198la, 1 9 8  Ib ,  being  t h e  exceptions). Consequ ently, this article  focuses on  three related issues :  (a) a  pro pos ed classification  o f  cogni- tions  that are potentially imp ortant in intimate  relationships, along with  a  review  of the  em pirical status  o f  these variables  as they  are  related  to  intimate relationships;  (b )  important meth- odolog ical and conceptual issue s to be considere d in the opera- tionalization of  these variables;  and (c) a  discussion  of  what should be  included  i n  cognitive models  of  intimate relation- ships and relationship maladjustm ent. Cognitive  Variables  in Intimate R elationships Derived  largely  from  Beck's  (1976)  an d Ellis'  ( 196 2)  cogn itive theories  o f  maladaptive  behavior,  five  categories  o f  interrelated Corre spon denc e concerning this  article  should  b e  addressed  t o Dona ld H. Baucom , Psychology  D epartment,  Campus Box 3270, Davie Hall,  University of  North Carolina, Cha pel H ill, North Carolina  27514. cognitive  phen ome na can be d elineated that appear to  play  i m - portant roles  in the  development  an d  m aintenance  of  marital maladjustm ent. The first of  these involves  th e  percep tual pro- cess of  selec tive attention.  T h e  other  four  categories  involve  t h e outcomes of  cogn itive  proc ess es: attributions (about  w h y  events occur), expe ctan cies (predictions  o f  what events  will  occur  in t h e  future),  assum ptions (about  t h e  nature  of  t h e  world  a n d  cor- relations betw een even ts  an d  characteristics),  an d  standards (about  what  "should"  be).  Although empirical investigations a re  only be ginning  in  most  of  these areas,  much  more  is  already known  about  the  outcomes  of the  cognitive  processes  than  is known  about  th e  cognitive processes themselves.  F or  example, a s  will  b e  discussed,  a  fair  amount  is  known  about  th e  content of  coup les' attributions and th eir relation to marital disco rd. Yet,  little  i s  known  about  th e  processes  b y  which  these attribu- tions  are  ma de; that  is, are  couples  la y  scientists advancing hypotheses  a n d  evaluating data?  A re  they cognitive opportun- ists who  advance  self-serving  attributions,  or are  they  simply responding  to class ically conditioned assoc iations  between emotion  a n d  thought? Cognitions  vary  in how  appropriate they  ar e  and,  thus,  in h o w  much they might  contribute  tomarital  distress.  T h e  appro- priateness  o f a  cognition  ca n  involve  (a) its  validity  a s a  repre- sentation  o f  reality  or (b) its  reasonableness  a s a  standard  or explanation for  relationship  events wh en  there  are not  objective criteria  available for  determining reality. Individu als  do not commonly  que stion their  o w n  mom ent-to-moment thoughts about  events in  their lives  (Beck,  Rush,  Shaw,  & Emery,  1979) o r  their long-standing assumptions and  standards  ab out the na- ture  o f  th e  world  (Nisbett  &Ross,  1980 ) .  Conseq uently, a  major task  of  cognitively  oriented marital therapy  is to  help spous es become  more active observers  and to  help the m evaluate their o w n  cognitions  s o  that their em otional  a n d  behavioral  re - sponses  to one  another  will  be  minimally  affected  b y  distorted cognitions (Epstein, 1982, 1 986).  T he  following  are descrip- 3 1

Upload: iliuta-florea

Post on 06-Jul-2018

221 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 1/8

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

1989,  Vol. 57, No. 1,31-38

Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological

 Association,

 Inc.

0022-006X/89/S00.75

The Role of Cognitions in Marital Relationships: Definitional,

Methodological, and Conceptual Issues

Donald

 H

 Baucom

University of

 North Carolina

 at

 Chapel Hill

Norman

 Epstein

University of Maryland, College Park

Steven Sayers and

 Tamara

 Goldman Sher

University of

 North Carolina

 at

 Chapel Hill

Although

 there

 have

 recently been numerous investigations exploring the role of

 couples'

 cognitions

in

 an attempt to understand marital distress, at present there is little cohesion and direction in the

study of how couples think about their relationships. The current article asserts that this lack of

direction results from  at

 least

 three

 factors:

 (a) a lack of delineation of the important cognitive

variables to be considered in marital functioning, (b) conceptual and methodological difficulties that

arise

 in

 attempts

 to operationalize

 cognitive variables,

 and (c) a

 dearth

 of

 models

 of

 marital function-

ing that incorporate cognitions in a detailed manner. These three factors are discussed, along

 with

 a

review of empirical investigations supporting the importance of cognitions in intimate relationships.

The

 role

 of

 cognitions

 in

 intimate relationships

 has

 gained

 in-

creasing

 attention in recent

 years.

 Most

 work

 has focused on the

area

 of

 causal attributions,

 and

 numerous investigations have

demonstrated empirical relations between various types of attri-

butions

 and level of marital maladjustment (see Baucom, 1987,

and Thompson & Snyder, 1986, for recent reviews of this work).

However, the study of cognitions in intimate relationships has

little coherent direction

 of

 movement, either

 from a

 research per-

spective or in

 terms

 of the

 treatment

 of

 marital distress.

This lack of

 focus

 and direction probably stems from at least

three

 factors:

 (a) insufficient

  delineation

 of

 important

 catego-

ries

 of

 marital cognitions other than attributions (with

 the

 work

on relationship beliefs by Eidelson & Epstein, 1982, being one

exception), (b) methodological and conceptual problems in op-

erationalizing  cognitive variables

  and

  comparing  disparate

measures used in different  studies, and (c) few descriptions of

explicit models to direct future research and to clarify the roles

that

 cognitions play in intimate relationships (with the models

proposed by Bradbury & Fincham, 1987, and Doherty, 198la,

198

 Ib,

 being

 the

 exceptions). Consequently, this article focuses

on three related issues:

 (a) a

 proposed classification

 of

 cogni-

tions that are potentially important in intimate relationships,

along with

 a

 review

 of the

 empirical status

 of

 these variables

 as

they

 are related to intimate relationships; (b) important meth-

odological and conceptual issues to be considered in the opera-

tionalization of

 these variables;

 and (c) a

 discussion

  of

 what

should be included in cognitive models of intimate relation-

ships and relationship maladjustment.

Cognitive Variables in Intimate Relationships

Derived largely from Beck's

 (1976)

 and Ellis'

 (1962)

 cognitive

theories

 of

 maladaptive behavior, five categories of interrelated

Correspondence concerning this  article  should  be  addressed  to

Donald H. Baucom, Psychology Department, Campus Box 3270, Davie

Hall,

 University of

 North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina

 27514.

cognitive

 phenomena can be delineated that appear to play im-

portant roles

 in the

 development

 and

 maintenance

 of

 marital

maladjustment. The first of these involves the perceptual pro-

cess of

 selective attention.

 The

 other four categories involve

 the

outcomes of

 cognitive processes: attributions (about

 why

 events

occur), expectancies (predictions of what events will occur in

the future), assumptions (about the nature of the world and cor-

relations between events

 and

 characteristics),

  and

  standards

(about

 what "should" be). Although empirical investigations

are only beginning in most of these areas, much more is already

known

 about

 the

 outcomes

 of the

 cognitive

 processes

 than

 is

known about

 the

 cognitive processes themselves.

 For

 example,

as

 will

 be discussed, a

 fair

 amount is

 known

 about the content

of

 couples' attributions and their relation to marital discord.

Yet,

 little is known about the processes by

 which

 these attribu-

tions

 are

  made; that

  is, are

  couples

 lay

 scientists advancing

hypotheses

 and evaluating data? Are they cognitive opportun-

ists who

 advance self-serving attributions,

 or are

 they simply

responding  to classically conditioned associations

  between

emotion

 and thought?

Cognitions vary in how appropriate they are and, thus, in

how much they might contribute

 to marital distress. The

 appro-

priateness

 of a

 cognition

 can

 involve

 (a) its

 validity

 as a repre-

sentation

 of

 reality

 or (b) its

  reasonableness

  as a

 standard

 or

explanation for

 relationship events when there

 are not

 objective

criteria  available for determining reality. Individuals do not

commonly  question their  own moment-to-moment thoughts

about events in their lives (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979)

or

 their long-standing assumptions and

 standards

 about the na-

ture of the world (Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Consequently, a major

task

 of

 cognitively oriented marital therapy

 is to

 help spouses

become

 more active observers

 and to

 help them evaluate their

own  cognitions so  that their emotional  and  behavioral re-

sponses to one another

 will

 be minimally affected by distorted

cognitions (Epstein, 1982, 1986).

 The  following  are descrip-

31

Page 2: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 2/8

32

BAUCOM,  EPSTEIN,  SAVERS,

  AND SHER

lions

 of the five categories of cognitions that are

 likely

 to play

roles in marital maladjustment and are potentially important

foci for

 treatment.

Assumptions

 and

 Standards

Spouses develop long-standing cognitions about the nature

of the

 world, including both

 the way

 that they think

 the

 world

actually is and the way that they think that the world should be,

which we respectively label assumptions and standards. These

classes of cognitions are important because, together, they serve

as the templates by which an individual processes the ongoing

events in his or her

 marriage. These cognitive

  structures,"

 knowledge structures, or "schemata" (Nisbett & Ross, 1980;

Seller, 1984)

 are an

 individual's internalized representations

 re-

garding rules for categorizing objects and events, for solving

problems, for evaluating the appropriateness  of events, and for

taking actions to achieve certain goals. Seller (1984) noted that,

beginning

 in

 infancy,

 the

 individual's repeated experiences

 in

the world produce complex concepts about the characteristics

of classes

 of

 objects

 and how one

 relates

 to

 them. Once estab-

lished, cognitive structures are hypothesized to

 have

 vital sur-

vival value by allowing people to understand and interact

 adap-

tively with the complexities in their lives.

Assumptions. Of relevance to marital interaction, an individ-

ual

 develops assumptions about

 the set of

 characteristics

  of a

person who fills the role of husband or wife (or comparable roles

in a

 nonmarital

 relationship) as well as assumptions about how

the members of a couple relate to one another. The

 former as-

sumptions focusing on characteristics of persons are commonly

labeled

 personae, and the

 latter assumptions, which focus

 on

events

 or

 chains

 of

 events,

 are

 referred

 to as

 scripts (Nisbett

 &

Ross,  1980). (This is a restricted  use of the term

 script,

 which

at

 times has been used by other theorists to include standards

and expectancies as

 they

 are

 defined

 in this article.) A persona

about a person who fills a particular role includes not only a

set of characteristics but also a set of correlations among the

characteristics.

  For

 example,

  an

  individual

 may

 assume that

wives

 tend to be loving, emotionally sensitive, cooperative, and

responsible. Also, the individual may assume that the degree to

which

 a wife is cooperative is

 highly

 associated with how re-

sponsible a person she is.

An individual's personae can easily

 influence

 other cogni-

tions and are probably related to marital discord. For example,

the

 assumption that men

 are

 only interested

 in

 sex could

 re-

sult in a

 wife

 making biased attributions for her husband's be-

havior

 and

 inaccurate expectancies about

 his

 future behavior

and could also lead to marital discord.

The scripts relevant to marriage involve sequences of events

that

 an

 individual assumes typically occur between spouses.

 An

individual's script

 for an

 argument with

 his or her

 spouse might

include

 a

 sequence

 of

 events such

 as, We

 begin debating

 a

point. Then she tells me that I

 don't know

 what I'm talking

about. Then I

 defend myself,

 and then she

 leaves

 the room or

the house."

Personae (e.g.,

 the

 henpecked husband)

 and

 scripts (e.g.,

 the

exploits of Archie and Edith Bunker) can be widely shared in a

culture,

 or

 they

 can be idiosyncratic to an individual (e.g., a

husband's

 fairly

 unique

 script about

 how he and his wife behave

in order to respect each

 other's

 privacy). Accurate assumptions

allow individuals' past experiences to guide their current under-

standing and

 interaction

 with

 their spouses,

 but

 inaccurate

 as-

sumptions

  may produce dysfunctional responses to marital

problems. Unfortunately, very little empirical research has in-

vestigated the importance of distorted assumptions in marital

discord,

 so

 their importance

 in

 marital distress

 has not yet

 been

demonstrated.

  However,

  Epstein and  Eidelson  (1981)  found

that the more distressed spouses assumed that their partners

could

 not

 change

 a

 relationship and

 that

 overt disagreement

 was

destructive to a relationship, the more they preferred individual

therapy

 to marital therapy and the lower were their estimates

that therapy would

 benefit

 their

 own

 marital problems.

Standards.  In contrast to the assumptions that a person

makes about the way relationships are, standards involve the

characteristics that

 the

 individual believes

 a

 partner

 or

 relation-

ship should

 have.

 As

 emphasized

 by

 rational-emotive therapists

(Dryden,

 1985; Ellis,

 1962), an

 individual may

 hold an

 extreme

or irrational standard about intimate relationships that no real-

life

 relationship could match. For example, one spouse might

cling to the standard that "\bu should be able to read my mind,

and I should not have to tell you what I want or

 need."

 In addi-

tion,

 he

 or she

 might also apply

 an

 extreme negative evaluation

when that standard is not met (e.g., It is awful if you do not

know what I want intuitively; I

 can't

 stand it ).

Standards per se are not dysfunctional; ethical and moral

standards

 are

 commonly quite functional guides

 for

 human

 re-

lationships. They become problematic when they are extreme

or

 rigid or when they detract from other aspects of an individu-

al's  life (e.g., the person who strives to do the best I can in

everything I do may

 suffer  exhaustion

 and may

 alienate

  ne-

glected

 family

 members).

As

 in the area of assumptions, investigation into the role of

extreme

 standards

 in

 marital discord

 is

 only beginning.

 The

 few

existing studies

 have

 typically distinguished between extreme

standards for individuals and extreme standards for intimate

relationships.

 For

 example, Epstein

 and

 Eidelson (1981) found

that spouses' marital distress and low involvement in marital

therapy were more strongly correlated with

 a

 measure

 of

 unre-

alistic assumptions

 and

 standards about intimate relationships

(Eidelson & Epstein,  1982) than with

 Jones's (1968)

 measure

of irrational

 beliefs (extreme standards) about individual func-

tioning described by Ellis (1962). Similarly, Jordan and McCor-

mick (1987)

 found

 that unrealistic assumptions and standards

about relationships were more predictive of general marital dis-

tress than were extreme standards about sexual relationships.

These findings indicate the importance of assessing the content

of

 assumptions and standards

 focal

 to relationships in order to

understand marital adjustment.

Selective Attention, Attributions, and  Expectancies

Spouses' perceptions and

 inferences

 about each other's be-

havior

 can contribute to marital distress independent of any

extreme

 standards

 and

 evaluations because they

 can

 serve

 as a

distorted and dissatisfying version of reality. That is, a spouse

might not violate standards about how a partner should behave,

Page 3: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 3/8

SPECIAL  SERIES: COGNITIONS AND  MARRIAGE

 

but his or her behavior might be perceived or interpreted as be-

ing inconsistent with what brings  the other person pleasure,

thus leading to dissatisfaction.

Selective

 attention.  Consistent with the idea that perception

is an active process rather than a passive reception of informa-

tion (Kelly, 1955),

 we

 define

 perceptions as

 those aspects

 of the

information available

 in a

 situation that

 an

 individual notices

and

 fits into cognitive structures (e.g., personae, scripts) that

have meaning to him or her. Social cognition researchers (cf,

Nisbett & Ross, 1980) and clinical writers (e.g., Beck et

 al.,

1979;

 Weiss,

 1980)

 have described how perceptions are suscep-

tible to selective attention because of factors such as emotional

states, fatigue, and preexisting cognitive structures. Perceptual

biases can

 have powerful effects

 on marital interaction because

spouses are normally unaware that the information they per-

ceive

 is only a subset of the data available in any situation.

Two

 types of related behavioral investigations

 have

 implica-

tions for the

  issue

 of selective attention. First, the extent to

which the two members of a married couple share a common

perception

 of

 what

 has

 occurred

 in

 their relationship provides

information

 regarding the selective abstraction of information.

Several

 investigations

 have

 demonstrated that married partners

have relatively different perceptions of what behaviors have oc-

curred

 in the

 marriage during

 a given

 24-hr period;

 the

 kappas

calculated between husbands' and wives' reporting of behavior

have averaged approximately .50

 (Christensen

 & Nies, 1980;

Christensen,  Sullaway,  & King, 1983;  Jacobson  & Moore,

1981). Jacobson and Moore (1981) concluded that two partners

in a marriage agreed less than half of the time about whether or

not a

 certain event

 had

 occurred during

 the

 past day.

 In

 addi-

tion, all three of these investigations, as well

 as

 a study by Chris-

tensen

 and

 Wallace (1976), indicated that more satisfied cou-

ples evidenced a higher rate of agreement than more distressed

couples. Thus, although a certain degree of

 differential

 percep-

tion of marital events appears  to be a way of

 life

 for most if

not all couples, couples who are more distressed seem to attend

selectively

 in different ways to a greater extent.

Other investigators

 have

 compared a spouse's report of mari-

tal events with a trained rater's report of the couple's behavior.

The

 logic

 underlying such investigations is that a trained rater

will

 have

 less reason to ignore certain events and will be sensi-

tized to the full

 range

 of

 behaviors under investigation.

 For ex-

ample, Robinson and Price

 (1980)

 trained observers to rate the

behaviors

 of both distressed and nondistressed couples in their

own homes. The couples also rated their own behaviors. A com-

parison

 of the

 raters' observations with

 the

 couples' observa-

tions

 revealed results consistent with the findings of

 differential

perception.

 The

 level

 of

 overall agreement between raters

 and

spouses

 was

 low, with correlations

 of

 approximately .50. Also,

there was greater agreement between raters and nondistressed

spouses than between raters and distressed spouses. In fact,

 dis-

tressed couples underestimated the frequency of pleasurable

events

 by 50%.

Attributions. As noted earlier, a rapidly growing body of liter-

ature

 has examined the attributions or causal explanations that

spouses provide

 for

 events in their relationships. The most com-

mon marital attribution dimensions investigated have been the

global-specific,

 stable-unstable, and internal-external dimen-

sions

 borrowed from

 the

 reformulated learned helplessness

 the-

ory (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). This is a logical

link because

 of the

 many cognitive

 and

 behavioral similarities

between depression and marital maladjustment (Epstein,

1985).

 In spite of several methodological issues to be discussed,

there  have been some common findings across studies (e.g.,

Baucom,

 Bell,

 &

 Dune, 1982;

 Fincham,

 Beach,

 &

 Baucom,

1987;

 Fincham & O'Leary, 1983; Holtzworth-Munroe

 &

 Ja-

cobson, 1985; Jacobson, McDonald, Follette,

 &

 Berley, 1985;

Kyle &

 Falbo,

 1985). Distressed spouses tend to rate causes of

negative partner behaviors

 as

 more global

 and

 stable than

 do

nondistressed individuals, whereas nondistressed spouses rate

causes

 of positive behavior of the partner as more global and

stable. In addition, distressed spouses have a tendency to blame

their partners for negative marital events. These

 attributional

tendencies serve to accentuate the positive in nondistressed re-

lationships and the negative in distressed relationships.

Recent studies have investigated other attributional dimen-

sions that focus on the characteristics and motives of the part-

ner, such as positive versus negative intent, blameworthiness,

selfish motivation, and lack of love (Epstein, Pretzer, & Fleming,

1987;

 Fincham, Beach, & Nelson, 1987; Fincham & Bradbury,

1988;

 Pretzer, Epstein,

 &

 Fleming, 1985). These studies have

consistently

  found

  strong  associations between such attribu-

tions and

 indices

 of

 marital maladjustment, again indicating

that distressed spouses explain their partners' behavior in ways

that

 focus on

 negative aspects

 of the

 partner.

As

 with almost all of the studies on cognitions and intimate

relationships, the investigations of attributions and marriage

are correlational, and conclusions about cause and

 effect

  rela-

tionships cannot

 be

 drawn (see Fincham

 &

 Bradbury,

 1987, for

an exception). Also, because there are no criteria for assessing

the validity of the attributions, it is unclear whether distressed

spouses are offering distorted attributions or whether their part-

ners

 actually

 do

 behave with negative motivations.

Expectancies.  Social learning theorists such as Rotter (1954)

and

 Bandura

 (1977)

 have described

 how

 people learn

 to

 antici-

pate probable consequences of their behavior and alter their

 be-

havior accordingly.

 For

 example,

 an

 individual

 may

 develop

 an

expectancy that behaving assertively will elicit intense criticism

from

  his or her

 partner and, consequently,

 the

 individual will

choose a submissive response. Expectancies can be situation-

or

 relationship-specific, or they can be more generalized (e.g.,

applied to a range of relationships, not only marriage, or ap-

plied across a

 wide

 range of situations within a marriage).

Bandura (1977) distinguished between an  outcome

  expec-

tancy

 (a prediction that a particular action will produce

 partic-

ular consequences

 in a

 certain situation)

 and an efficacy

  expec-

tancy (an

 estimate

 of the

 probability that

 one

 will

 be

 able

 to

carry

 out the

 particular action needed

 to

 produce those conse-

quences).

 Individuals' expectancies about interactions between

spouses tend to take an "if-then" form and can involve predic-

tions about reactions of the partner to one's own

 behavior,

 reac-

tions

 of the self to the

 partner's behavior,

 and

 outcomes

 of a

joint event (e.g.,

 "If we argue in

 front

 of the

 children, they will

be harmed psychologically").

As with some of the other cognitions described previously,

there is scant research on the role of expectancies in intimate

Page 4: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 4/8

 

BAUCOM,  EPSTEIN,  SAVERS,

 AND

SHER

relationships. Crossing two dimensions of expectancies—gen-

eralized versus specific

 by

 outcome expectancy

 versus

  efficacy

expectancy—results

 in at least

 four

 different types of expectan-

cies that can be explored. Pretzer et

 al.

 (1985) conducted one of

the

 only

 studies in this area, and it focused solely on generalized

efficacy

  expectancies. Their results supported  Doherty's

(1981a,

 1981b)

 hypotheses that spouses'

 low

 efficacy

 expecta-

tions regarding their ability to

 solve

 their marital problems are

associated with marital distress and depression and with attri-

butions of causality for relationship problems to their partner's

behavior, stable personality, malicious intent,

 and

 lack

 of

 love.

Operationalizing

 Terms

To evaluate a model of marital

  functioning

 that primarily

considers cognitive factors, these cognitive variables must

 be op-

erationalized.

 Although there has been considerable attention

given to the role of causal attributions in marital  functioning,

few attempts have been made

 to

 operationalize

 the

 other cogni-

tive variables previously discussed. Yet, even when  attributions

have been considered alone, a number of unresolved conceptual

and methodological issues

 have

 surfaced. Several of these issues

will

 be

 considered because they

 will

 probably resurface when

attempts are made to assess the other categories of cognitions.

One

 general issue that has arisen in assessing attributions is

the extent to which the assessment strategies mirror the attribu-

tional processes that occur in the

 day-to-day

 lives of couples.

This rather broad concern incorporates several

 specific

 issues.

First, the extent to which the individual is asked explicitly to

make attributions is

 important.

 That is, individuals do not

make attributions about every event;

 to do so

 would result

 in

an

 extreme amount

 of

 cognitive processing that

 would

 severely

curtail the

 person's

 ability to proceed through

 even

 uncompli-

cated events. Consequently, although most self-report invento-

ries

 of attributions concerning marital interaction explicitly

 ask

the respondent to explain why an event or behavior occurred,

it is

 unclear whether

 the

 person would have provided

 an

 attribu-

tion for the event in the natural environment (e.g., Pyszcynski

&Greenberg, 1981). Holtzworth-Munroeand Jacobson (1985)

both directly

 and

 indirectly

 probed

 for

 attributions regarding

marital events. They found that, although

 the two

 methodolo-

gies

 resulted in attributions that were statistically

 significantly

correlated,

  the

  correlations  were

  all

 modest

  in

  magnitude.

Thus, the manner in which attributions are elicited probably

influences

 what is

 obtained,

 and at present the relative merits

of the various strategies are unclear. Similarly, the extent to

which the assessment strategy directs the respondent's thoughts

is pertinent when cognitions other than attributions are consid-

ered.

The representativeness of the cognitive assessment strategy

for understanding

 the

 couple's marital

 functioning is influenced

by a second factor  whose relationship is

 focused

 on in the as-

sessment. Some inventories

 ask

 respondents only about their

cognitions regarding their own marriages (Baucom & Sayers,

1987; Pretzer et

 al.,  1985),

 yet other self-report measures ask

respondents about their cognitions regarding marriages in gen-

eral

 as well as their own marriages

 (Eidelson

 & Epstein, 1982).

Asking

 about both types of relationships is appropriate, but

items

  have

  typically been combined into a single score, al-

though the two foci  may yield very different information. For

example, clinical experience suggests that

 many

 distressed cou-

ples

 hold different  standards for their own marriage than for

marriages in general. To combine these two pieces of informa-

tion into a single score could result in misleading or confusing

results.

The extent to which the assessment strategy mirrors cogni-

tions in

 day-to-day living

 is

 also

 influenced by the specific

 stim-

uli,

 behaviors,

 or

 events with which

 the

 individual

 is

 presented.

Attributional assessment strategies can be divided according to

whether

 they

 ask

 couples

 to

 consider hypothetical

 or

 real events

in their

  relationships

  (e.g., Madden

 &

 Janoff-Bulman, 1981;

Orvis,

 Kelley, &

 Butler, 1976). Hypothetical events have

 the as-

set that all couples will be responding to the same set of stimuli,

which

 allows

 for easier comparison across couples; the liability

is that the events may not be representative of the couples' own

behavior.

 Using actual events from

  the

 couples'  lives ensures

that the events

 have

 relevance to them; however, comparison

across couples becomes more complex. Findings to date

 have

been similar

 when

 attributions

 for

 both

 actual

 and

 hypothetical

marital events

 have

 been considered; yet, only one study has

compared

 the

 equivalence

 of

 hypothetical versus actual events,

and then only for wives (Fincham & Beach, in press). Their

findings

 revealed that similar attributions were provided

 for the

two types

 of

 stimuli. Whether

 to use

 real

 or

 hypothetical events

must

 also be addressed when the other classes of

 cognitions

 are

operationalized.

As stated previously, one major reason for using hypothetical

marital events is that all spouses are thus providing cognitions

about the same set of stimuli. To maximize the representative-

ness

 of the stimuli and yet provide a set of questions or state-

ments to which all couples can

 respond,

 some investigators have

taken a different  approach. They

 have

 constructed statements

that ask about the relationship in a very general way (e.g., Burns

& Volpicelli,

 1987; Pretzer el al., 1985). Thus, in assessing ex-

pectancies, a

 respondent might

 be

 asked

 the

 extent

 to

 which

 he

or she agrees with the following statement, "No matter what I

do, my partner

  will

  not change." Such global expectancies

might be an

 important part

 of

 marital adjustment. However,

marital discord also seems to involve cognitions about very spe-

cific situations and aspects of the relationship, and the spouses'

thoughts

 about these specific components

 of the

 marriage also

need to be assessed.

In part,

 the

 relevance

 of

 obtaining cognitions about global

aspects or specific content areas of the relationship depends on

whether

 or not individuals have global cognitive

 styles

 that in-

volve thinking about many aspects

 of the

 relationship

 in the

same way. For example, if a spouse makes similar attributions

for marital events regardless

 of the

 specific content under con-

sideration (i.e., has a strong attributional

 style),

 then the content

provided

 is of

 less importance. Some marital investigators have

apparently assumed that spouses have an attributional style re-

garding

 their marriage (e.g., Doherty, 1982). However, Baucom,

Sayers, and Duhe (in press)

 found that,

 whereas some spouses

do provide consistent attributions across marital situations,

other

 spouses offer attributions that appear

 to be

 situation-spe-

cific.

Page 5: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 5/8

SPECIAL SERIES: COGNITIONS AND  MARRIAGE

35

Persons with individual

 psychopathology often

 have

 distorted

and

 extreme cognitions that should alert marital investigators

from  a

 methodological perspective.

 For

 example, ample evi-

dence exists that depressed individuals have negativistic attribu-

tions,

 expectancies,

 and so forth.

 Therefore, when investigating

the

 cognitions

 of

 maritally

 distressed

 couples, researchers must

also evaluate whether

 the

 individuals

 are

 depressed. Then, fac-

torial designs

 can be

 used

 or the level of

 depression

 can be

 co-

varied

 to insure

 that

 extreme or

 distorted

 cognitions are a

 func-

tion of the

 marital distress

 per se

 rather than

 a function of the

individual's

 depression. Whereas depression

 is one

 variable that

should

 be

 considered

 in

 evaluating

 spouses'

 cognitions, marital

researchers must also take into account any other disorder or

phenomenon that

 is likely to

 confound

 the relationship

 between

marital functioning and

 cognitions.

A

  final

 important issue

 in operationalizing

 cognitive vari-

ables relevant

 to

 marriage

 is

 whether

 to focus on the

 actual con-

tent

 of

 cognitions,

 on

 broader dimensions presumed

 to be im-

portant within certain categories

 of

 cognitions,

 or on

 some

 inte-

gration

 of

 the

 two.

 For

 example,

 in

 assessing attributions

 within

marriage, almost no investigators

 have focused

 on the actual

content

 of the

 attributions (see Pretzer

 et al.,

 1985,

 for an excep-

tion). Respondents

 are

 typically asked

 to offer

  attributions

 for

why

 some marital

 event occurred. Next, the

 respondent

 or

 out-

side raters evaluate

 the

 attribution

 on

 several dimensions

 such

as locus of control, globality, and stability.

 Almost invariably,

these dimensions become the

  foci

 of the investigation. Thus,

the

 content (e.g., whether

 the respondent

 believes that problems

result because the partner is stupid or lazy, etc.) is lost. Other

investigators have

 focused on content but have not been as care-

ful

  in

 delineating

 the

 categories

 of

 cognitions under consider-

ation. For example, Burns and

 Volpicelli's

 scale (1987)

 assesses

a broad range of content areas that cognitive theorists believe

are relevant to marital

 distress

 (e.g., partner is unwilling or un-

able to change). Whereas some of these areas are assessed with

questions focusing

 on

 assumptions

 and

 expectancies, other con-

tent areas are assessed by asking about standards and assump-

tions.

 At times, it is unclear why a

 given

 content area is assessed

in

 terms of certain

 categories

 of cognitions.

In  future

 attempts to investigate couples' cognitions and

marital

 functioning, a

 consideration

 of

 both content

 and

 type

of

 cognition

 could

 be a

 fruitful strategy.

 That is,

 investigators

might select

 what

 appear

 to be the

 important varieties

 of

 con-

tent

 in

 couples' cognitions

 and

 might then systematically

 ask

questions about the couples' perceptions, attributions, assump-

tions,

 expectancies, and standards in each particular content

area. Such systematic exploration

 would allow

 for a determina-

tion of how these different types of cognitive variables, such as

attributions and expectancies, interrelate in a

 given

  content

area. Certain patterns

 across

 cognitive variables might

 well be

related to marital

 distress.

 For example, a spouse might (a)

 have

a clear set of assumptions about a content

 area,

 make attribu-

tions about past behavior consistent

 with

 these assumptions,

and

 similarly provide expectancies about

  future

  behavior

 in

light

 of these assumptions and attributions but (b)

 have

 stan-

dards as to how the relationship

 should

 be that are in conflict

with

 these assumptions, attributions, and expectancies. As a re-

sult,

 this

 conflict

 between

 (a) how the

 relationship

 is

 perceived

to be

 versus

 (b) how the

 relationship should

 be in this

 content

area

 might be a source of distress to the couple.

Thus, attempts

 to

 operationalize cognitions

 and to

 deal with

the methodological and conceptual complexities inherent in as-

sessing

 couples' cognitions relevant

 to

 their marriages

 are only

beginning. However, even as these issues become resolved, theo-

reticians and investigators must continue to

 focus

 on an even

broader

 issue:

 how

 these cognitive variables

 are to be

 used

 in

building a

 model

 of

 marital

  functioning.

Building

 Models of

 Marital

 Functioning

Whereas new

 research might

 be

 pursued based

 on the defini-

tional

 and

 methodological issues already discussed, clear and

concise theoretical models of marital adjustment are needed to

embrace cognitive constructs

 and to

 provide direction

  for

 this

research.

 If

 models

 of

 marital adjustment

 are to be

 constructed

that include cognitions,

 the

 relations among

 the

 various catego-

ries

 of

 cognitions must

 be

 made explicit.

 In

 addition, relations

among

  cognitions

 and

 other variables such

 as

 behavior

 and

affect

 must be integrated into a model of marital

  functioning,

and

 theoreticians must be explicit about the causal relations

that are hypothesized to exist

 among

 these constructs.

A potential source

 of confusion

 regarding causal models con-

cerns the

 general

 statement by investigators that cognitions,

affect, and

 behavior interact

 in influencing the

 couple. Although

the

 meaning

 of

 this statement seems

 to be

 self-evident,

 a  diffi-

culty

 arises concerning

 the

 potentially different  connotations

for  the

  term  interact. Buss (1977) described these

  different

meanings in his discussion of the trait-situation controversy in

predicting

 and

 understanding behavior.

 In the first

 meaning,

two

 variables such

 as

 environment

 (E) and

 person

 (P) are

 used

to predict and explain a third variable, such as behavior  (B);

these relations

 can be

 characterized

 by the function, B = f(E,

P).

  This

 has

 been labeled

  the

 analysis

 of

 variance

 (ANOVA)

model because, conceptually,

 E

 and

 P

 variables are used as fac-

tors or as independent variables

 whose

 interaction is used to

predict B, the

 dependent variable. Quite appropriately,

 ANOVA

statistical techniques

 have

 been used

 to

 test this model.

 (In

 con-

sidering

 marital

 functioning,

 other variables, including cogni-

tions, can be

 substituted

 for E, P, and B.)

 This

 is

 generally

 re-

garded

 as a mechanistic

 model

 in that it embodies unidirec-

tional

 causality from environment

 and

 person

 to

 behavior.

 The

model

 allows

 for investigating whether two or more variables

are additively or interactively related to a dependent variable.

Interaction

 in

 this model means that

 the

 independent variables

need

 to be

 considered simultaneously

 in

 order

 to

 understand

their relation to the dependent variable.

Jacobson et al.

 (1985)

 utilized

 this

 meaning

 when

 examining

the effects of negative behavior and

 level

 of marital distress on

the

 types

 of

 attributions made

 by couples. The

 investigators

asked spouses

 to

 participate

 in a

 conflict resolution task

 with

their partners.

 Before the

 task,

 the

 experimenter

 privately in-

structed one spouse to

 behave

 either positively or

 negatively.

Both

 distressed

 and

 nondistressed couples

 were

 included

 in

 this

procedure, yielding a 2 X 2 (Behavior X Couple Type) design.

The results

 indicated

 an

 interaction

 effect

 such that distressed

spouses

 were

 more internal in their attributions concerning the

Page 6: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 6/8

36

BAUCOM, EPSTEIN,

  SAVERS, AND SHER

partner's behavior when that partner was instructed to behave

negatively and were more external when the partner was to be-

have positively. Conversely, nondistressed spouses were more

internal in their attributions for positive behavior and external

in their attributions for negative behavior. The investigators ap-

propriately noted that this type of design does not examine

whether there are reciprocal causal relations among the vari-

ables.

In

 the second meaning of interaction, there is a reciprocal

relationship between person

 (F)

  and environment (E), which is

construed as bidirectional or cyclical between these two sets of

variables (E  •*  » f).  Each variable is considered to be simulta-

neously both

 a

 dependent

 and

 independent variable,

 and

 this

is referred to as an organismic model.

 Thus,

 in

 considering

 a

married

 couple, a spouse's cognitions may

 influence

 that per-

son's emotions toward the partner, and those emotions might

then

 influence future

 cognitions which that individual experi-

ences. This example

 focuses on

 reciprocal causality within

 a

single  individual;  the  model becomes much more complex

when

 attempting

 to

 explain reciprocal causality between

 two

persons

 in a

 relationship.

Thus, in the first instance, the concept of interaction focuses

on the

 manner

 in which two

 variables combine

 in a

 unique

 way

in relation

 to a

 third variable,

 and the

 causality

 is

 often

 viewed

as unidirectional.

  However,

  in the second case, causality is

clearly

 bidirectional,

 and

 interaction  refers

 to the way in

 which

two (or more) variables mutually influence  and change each

other.

 The difficulty

 arises

 when

 researchers espouse

 a

 model

that defines interaction from

  one

 perspective

 but

 then empiri-

cally investigate the concept using a methodology appropriate

for

 the

 other meaning

 of

 interaction.

Consequently,

 in building models of marital distress that give

a

 significant

 role to couples' cognitions, the theoretician must

be attuned to several issues. Cognitions must not be described

in

 a

 general way; instead,

 the

 various types

 of

 cognitions must

be differentiated, and the relations between these cognitive vari-

ables and marital distress must be made clear (e.g., Fincham &

Bradbury, 1988). The relations among various cognitions and

among

 cognitions and other important constructs such as emo-

tions and behavior should also be specified. In doing so, the the-

oretician must go beyond the self-evident yet potentially confus-

ing general statement that these variables interact. Instead, the

two meanings of interaction,

 from  ANOVA

 and  reciprocal  in-

fluence

 perspectives,

 must be considered. At present, our

 efforts

in these directions

 have

 just begun. Investigators

 have

 estab-

lished with some consistency that attributions are related to

level of

 marital discord,

 and the few

 studies that exist confirm

a relation between standards or

 beliefs

 and

 level

 of marital ad-

justment. If these findings are to

 have

 maximal impact on our

understanding

 of relationships, it is important that they be inte-

grated into more complete models of marital functioning incor-

porating the guidelines outlined here.

Intervening

 in

 Marital Discord

Given

 that several of the previous categories  of cognitions

have rarely been discussed by marital investigators, that opera-

tionalization of the constructs is  only  beginning, and that

models

 of

 marital functioning that incorporate cognitions

 are

at an early stage of development, it is not surprising that at-

tempts to intervene with distressed couples' cognitions  have

rarely been evaluated. Three investigations have implemented

cognitive restructuring alone without other interventions

 (Em-

melkamp, 1985; Epstein, Pretzer, & Fleming, 1982; Huber &

Milstein,  1985),  and two other investigations  have explored

whether behavioral marital therapy (BMT) could be strength-

ened by

 adding

 a

 cognitive component (Baucom, 1985; Bau-

com &

 Lester, 1986).

 The

 results

 of

 these investigations

 have

indicated that cognitive restructuring with distressed couples

can produce meaningful cognitive

 changes,

 particularly in rela-

tionship standards, assumptions, and expectancies. Selective at-

tention has rarely been investigated, and the potential for alter-

ing

 attributions is unclear. Cognitive restructuring has also

 al-

tered  couples'  attitudes toward therapy  itself.  Huber and

Milstein  (1985) found that brief cognitive therapy increased

couples' expectancies that marital therapy would

 benefit

 them

and

 increased their desire

 to

 improve their relationships.

 In ad-

dition, the

 treatments

 were effective in

 increasing marital

 ad-

justment.

 However,

 as is consistent with other outcome studies,

cognitive  restructuring in isolation or in combination with

BMT

 appears

 to be no

 more effective than

 BMT

 alone

 when

couples are randomly assigned to treatment conditions (see

Baucom &

 Hoffman,  1986,

 for a

 review

 of

 marital outcome

 re-

search).

Thus, the findings indicate that cognitive therapy can alter

distressed couples' cognitions and increase their levels of mari-

tal adjustment. Yet, to be maximally effective, these treatment

interventions must draw on basic research. At the least, this

means that the constructs must be operationalized and the

many methodological issues resolved. In addition, treatment

interventions involving cognitions can benefit from well-devel-

oped

 models

 of

 marital

 functioning

 that incorporate cognitions.

Such

 models

 can

 provide guidelines regarding which couples

will benefit most from cognitive restructuring

 and how to im-

plement these interventions. Once these findings and theories

are available, there is an increased likelihood that cognitive in-

terventions will maximally

 benefit

 distressed couples.

References

Abramson, L. Y.,

 Seligman,

 M. E P., & Teasdale, J. D

 (1978).

 Learned

helplessness in

 humans:

 Critique and

 reformulation.

  Journal

 of Ab-

normal Psychology, 87, 49-74.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory

  Englevraod

 Cliffs,

  NJ:

 Pren-

tice-Hall.

Baucom,

 D H

 (1985, November).

 Enhancing behavioral marital ther-

apy

 with cognitive restructuring

 and

 emotional

 expressiveness

 train-

ing.

 Paper

 presented at the

 19th Annual

 Convention of the Associa-

tion forthe

 Advancement of Behavior Therapy,

 Houston, TX.

Baucom, D H

 (1987). Attributions

 in distressed relations:

 How

 can we

explain them? In S. Duck

  & D.

  Perlman (Eds.),

 Heterosexual

 re-

lations, marriage and

 divorce

 (pp.  177-206). London:

 Sage.

Baucom, D H,

 Bell,

 W. G., &

 Dune,

 A. (1982,

 August).

 The

 measure-

ment of

 couples'

 attributions

 or

 positive

 and

 negative

 dyadic interac-

tions. Paper presented at the 16th Annual

 Convention

 of the

 Associa-

tion forthe Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Los Angeles.

Baucom,

 D H, & Hoffman, J. A.

 (1986).

 The

 effectiveness

 of marital

Page 7: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 7/8

SPECIAL SERIES: COGNITIONS  AND  MARRIAGE  

therapy: Current

 status

 and  application to the

 clinical

 setting. In

N. S. Jacobson & A. S. Gurman (Eds.),

 Clinical handbook

 of marital

therapy (pp.  597-620). New

 York:

 Guilford Press.

Baucom, D. H.,  Lester; G. W. (1986). The

 usefulness

 of cognitive

restructuring as

 an

 adjunct

 to

 behavioral marital therapy.

 Behavior

Therapy,  17, 385-403.

Baucom, D. H, & Sayers, S. L. (1987, November). Attributional style

and attributional

 patterns among

 married

 couples.

  Paper presented

at the 21st Annual Convention  of the Association  for the Advance-

ment of Behavior Therapy, Boston.

Baucom,

 D.

 H, Sayers,

 S.

 L.,

 &

 Dune,

 A. D. (in

 press). Attributional

style

 and

  attributional patterns among married

 couples. Journal of

Personality and

 Social

 Psychology.

Beck,

 A. T. (1976). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical

aspects. New York: Hoeber.

Beck, A. T,

  Rush,

 A. J., Shaw, B. E, &

 Emery,

 G.

 (1979).

 Cognitive

therapy of depression. New York:

 Guilford

 Press.

Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1987). Affect and cognition in close

relationships:

 Towards

 an integrative

 model.

 Cognition and

 Emotion,

1, 59-87.

Bums,

 D.

 D,

 &

 Volpicelli,

 J. R.

 (1987).

 Cognitive

 and

 affective

  compo-

nents

  of

 marital

 satisfaction:  II.

  Development

  and

  validation

 of an

interpersonal attitude scale. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Buss, A. R.

 (1977).

 The trait-situation

 controversy

 and the

 concept

 of

interaction. Personality and

 Social

 Psychology

 Bulletin,

 3,

 196-201.

Christensen, A.,  Nies, D. C. (1980). The Spouse Observation Check-

list: Empirical analysis

 and

  critique. American

 Journal of Family

Therapy, 8, 69-79.

Christensen,

 A., Sullaway,

 M., &

 King,

 C. E. (1983). Systematic error in

behavioral

 reports

 of dyadic interaction: Egocentric

 bias and content

effects. Behavioral Assessment, 5,129-140.

Christensen,

 A., &

 Wallace,

 L. (1976).

 Perceptual accuracy

 as a

 variable

in

 marital adjustment.

 Journal

 of Sex and Marital

  Therapy,

  2, 130-

136.

Doherty, W. J. (I981a).

 Cognitive

 processes in intimate

 conflict:

 I. Ex-

tending attribution theory. American

 Journal

 of

 Family

  Therapy,

  9,

5-13.

Doherty,

  W. J.

  (1981b). Cognitive

 processes in

  intimate

 conflict:  II.

Efficacy and

 learned helplessness. American

 Journal of Family Ther-

apy,

 9, 35-44.

Doherty, W. J. (1982). Attributional style and negative problem solving

in marriage. Family Relations, 31,201-205.

Dryden,

 W. (1985). Marital therapy: The

 rational-emotive approach.

 In

W.

 Dryden (Ed.),

 Marital therapy  in

 Britain (Vol.

 1, pp.

  195-221).

London: Harper & Row.

Eidelson,

 R. J., &

 Epstein,

 N. (1982).

 Cognition

 and

 relationship malad-

justment: Development of a measure of

 dysfunctional

  relationship

beliefs. Journal of Consulting

 and

 Clinical Psychology,

 50, 715-720.

Ellis,

 A. (1962).

 Reason

 and

 emotion

 in psychotherapy.  New Ybrk: Lyle

Stuart.

Emmelkamp, P.

 (1985,

 August).

 Communication skills

 training

 and

cognitive therapy. Paper presented at the 15th Annual Convention of

the

 European Association

  for

 Behavior Therapy, Munich, West Ger-

many.

Epstein,

 N. (1982).

 Cognitive therapy

 with couples.

 American

 Journal

of Family  Therapy,  10, 5-16.

Epstein, N. (1985).

 Depression

 and marital dysfunction: Cognitive and

behavioral linkages. International

 Journal

 of Mental Health, 13, 86-

104.

Epstein, N.

 (1986).

 Cognitive marital therapy: Multi-level assessment

and

 intervention.

 Journal of Rational-Emotive Therapy,

  4,

 68-81.

Epstein,

 N., & Eidelson, R. J. {1981). Unrealistic

 beliefs of

 clinical

 cou-

ples:

 Their relationship

 to expectations,

 goals

 and

 satisfaction.

 A

 men-

can Journal of Family  Therapy,  9, 13-22.

Epstein, N., Pretzer,

  .,

 & Fleming, B. (1982, November). Cognitive

therapy and

 communication

 training: Comparisons

 of effects

  with

 dis-

tressed

 couples.

 Paper presented

  at the

  16th Annual Convention

 of

the

 Association for

 the

 Advancement

 of

 Behavior Therapy,

 Los Ange-

les.

Epstein, N., Pretzer,

 J.,

 & Fleming, B. (1987). The role of cognitive ap-

praisal in self-reports of marital communication.  Behavior  Therapy,

18, 51-69.

Fincham,

 F. D, &

 Beach,

 S. (in

 press). Attribution

  processes in dis-

tressed and nondistressed couples: 5. Real versus hypothetical events.

Cognitive

 Therapy and

 Research.

Fincham, F.

 D,

 Beach,

 S.,

 & Baucom, D H (1987). Attribution

 pro-

cessing in distressed and nondistressed couples: 4. Self-partner

 attri-

bution

 differences. Journal

 of Personality

 and

 Social

 Psychology,  52,

739-748.

Fincham, F. D, Beach, S., & Nelson, G. (1987). Attribution

 processes

in

 distressed

 and nondistressed couples: 3.

 Causal

 and

 responsibility

attributions

  for

 spouse behavior.  Cognitive

 Therapy

  and

 Research,

11, 71-86.

Fincham, F.

 D,

 &

 Bradbury,

 T. N.

 (1987).

 The

 impact

 of

 attributions

 in

marriage:

 A

 longitudinal analysis.

 Journal

 of Personality

 and

 Social

Psychology,

 53, 510-517.

Fincham,

 F.

 D,

 &

 Bradbury,

 T.

 (1988).

 The

 impact

 of

 attributions

 in

marriage: Empirical and conceptual foundations. British

 Journal

 of

Clinical

 Psychology,

 27, 77-90.

Fincham, F. D,

  .

 O'Leary, K. D. (1983). Causal inferences for spouse

behavior in maritally distressed and nondistressed couples.  Journal

of

 Social

 and Clinical Psychology, 1,42-57.

Holtzworth-Munroe,

 A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1985). Causal

 attributions

of

 married couples: When

 do

 they

 search for

 causes? What

 do they

conclude when they

 do?

 Journal

 ofPersonality

  and Social

 Psychology,

48,

  1398-1412.

Huber, C.

 H,

 & Milstein, B.

 (1985).

 Cognitive restructuring and a col-

laborative

 set in

 couples'

 work. American

 Journal of Family  Therapy,

13,  17-27.

Jacobson,

 N.

 S.,

 McDonald, D.

 W., Follette,

 W.

 C.,

 &

 Berley,

 R. A.

(1985). Attributional processes in distressed and nondistressed mar-

ried couples. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9,

 35-50.

Jacobson,

 N.

 S.,

 &

 Moore,

 D. (1981).

 Spouses

 as

 observers

 of the

 events

in

 their relationship.

 Journal of

 Consulting and Clinical

  Psychology,

49,

 269-277.

Jones, R. G. (1968). A

 factored  measure

 ofEllis' Irrational  BeliefSys-

tem,

  with

 personality

  and

  maladjustment correlates.  Unpublished

doctoral dissertation,

 Texas Technological College. (University Mi-

crofilms

 No.

 69-6443)

Jordan,

 T. J., &

 McCormick,

 N. B. (1987,

 April). The

 role of

 sex  beliefs

in intimate relationships. Paper

 presented

 at the annual meeting of

the American Association of Sex Educators, Counselors, and Thera-

pists,

 New

 York.

felly, G. A.

 (1955). The

 psychology

 of

 personal

 constructs.

 New York:

Norton.

Kyle,

 S. Q, &

 Falbo,

 T. (1985). Relationships between marital

 stress

and

 attributional preferences

 for own and

 spouse behavior.

 Journal of

Social

 and

 Clinical Psychology,

 3,

 339-351.

Madden, M. E., &

 Janoff-Bulman,

 R. (1981).

 Blame, control

 and

 mari-

tal

 satisfaction: Wives' attributions

 for conflict in

 marriage. Journal

of Marriage

 and the Family, 44,

 663-674.

Nisbett,

 R.,

 & Ross, L. (1980).

 Human

 inference:  Strategies

 and short-

comings

 of social udgment.

 Englewood Clifis, NJ:

 Prentice-Hall.

Orvis,

 B. R., Kelley, H. H, & Butler, D.

 (1976). Attributional

 conflict

in young

 couples.

 In J. H Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. Kidd (Eds.),

 New

Page 8: The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

8/17/2019 The Role of Cognition in Marital Relationships

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-role-of-cognition-in-marital-relationships 8/8

 8

BAUCOM,  EPSTEIN,  SAVERS,  AND  SHER

directions in attribution research (Vol. 1, pp.

 353-386).

 New \brk:

Erlbaum.

Pretzer, J. L.,

 Epstein,

 N., & Fleming, B.

 (1985).

 The Marital

 Attitude

Survey:

 A  measure of dysfunctional  attributions and

  expectancies.

Unpublished manuscript.

Pyszcynski,

 T. A., & Greenberg, J. (1981). Role of

 disconfirmed

 expec-

tancies

 in the

 instigation of

 attributions

processing.

 Journal of

 Per-

sonality and Social

 Psychology,

 40,

 31-38.

Robinson,

 E. A., &

 Price,

 M. G.

 (1980).

 Pleasurable behavior in marital

interaction: An observational

 study.

 Journal of Consulting and Clini-

cal Psychology, 48,

 117-118.

Rotter, J. B.

 (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology.

 Englewood

Cliffs,

 NJ:

  Prentice-Hall.

Seiler, T. B.

 (1984). Development

 of

 cognitive theory, personality,

 and

therapy. In N.

 Hoffmann (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive

 therapy: The-

oretical methods and practical applications

  (pp.

 11-49). New \fark:

Plenum

 Press.

Thompson, J. S., &

 Snyder,

 D K.

 (1986).

 Attribution theory in intimate

relationships: A methodological review.

 American

 Journal of  Family

Therapy,  14,

 123-138.

Weiss,

  R. L. (1980).  Strategic behavioral marital therapy:

 Toward

 a

model

 for

 assessment

  and

  intervention.

 In J. P.

 Vincent (Ed.), Ad-

vances in

 amily

  intervention, assessment, andtheory(VcA.  1, pp. 229-

271). Greenwich,

 CT:

 J

 AI

 Press.

Received March 7,1988

Revision received March

 21,1988

Accepted March

 21,1988

 

Mineka Appointed Editor

 of

 Journal

 of Abnormal Psychology,  1990-1995

The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Association an-

nounces the appointment of Susan Mineka, Northwestern University, as editor of the

 Journal

of Abnormal

 Psychology

 for a

 6-year term beginning

 in

  1990.

 As of

 January

 1,

 1989, manu-

scripts should

 be directed to

Susan

 Mineka

Northwestern University

Department

 of

 Psychology

102

 Swift

 Hall

Evanston, Illinois

 6Q208