the self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis

25
University of Connecticut OpenCommons@UConn CHIP Documents Center for Health, Intervention, and Prevention (CHIP) 1-1-1997 e Self-Reference Effect in Memory: A Meta- Analysis Cynthia S. Symons Houghton College Blair T. Johnson University of Connecticut, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: hps://opencommons.uconn.edu/chip_docs Part of the Cognitive Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Symons, Cynthia S. and Johnson, Blair T., "e Self-Reference Effect in Memory: A Meta-Analysis" (1997). CHIP Documents. 9. hps://opencommons.uconn.edu/chip_docs/9

Upload: others

Post on 18-Oct-2021

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

University of ConnecticutOpenCommons@UConn

CHIP Documents Center for Health, Intervention, and Prevention(CHIP)

1-1-1997

The Self-Reference Effect in Memory: A Meta-AnalysisCynthia S. SymonsHoughton College

Blair T. JohnsonUniversity of Connecticut, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://opencommons.uconn.edu/chip_docs

Part of the Cognitive Psychology Commons

Recommended CitationSymons, Cynthia S. and Johnson, Blair T., "The Self-Reference Effect in Memory: A Meta-Analysis" (1997). CHIP Documents. 9.https://opencommons.uconn.edu/chip_docs/9

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1997, Vol. 121, No. 3, 371-394 0033-2909/97/$3.00

The Self-Reference Effect in Memory: A Meta-Analysis

Cynthia S. Symons Houghton College

Blair T. Johnson Syracuse University

In this review, the authors examine the basis for the mnemonic superiority that results from relating material to the self. A meta-analysis confirms the expected self-reference effect (SRE) in memory, with self-referent encoding strategies yielding superior memory relative to both semantic and other- referent encoding strategies. Consistent with theory and research that suggest self-reference (SR) produces both organized and elaborate processing, the SRE was smaller (a) when SR is compared with other-reference (OR) rather than semantic encoding and (b) when the comparison tasks promote both organization and elaboration. Thus, the SRE appears to result primarily because the self is a well-developed and often-used construct that promotes elaboration and organization of encoded information. The authors discuss the implications of these and other findings for theories of the SRE and for future research.

Throughout the history of psychology, researchers have used the self as a central part of their explanations of various phenom- ena (see Banaji & Prentice, 1994; G. T. Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984; and James, 1890). A large body of research suggests that the self-structure is unique, relative to other concepts (e.g., those about other people; see Kihlstrom et al., 1988; Markus, 1977; and Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977), in its motivational and affective implications as well as in its structure and content. Social psychologists have long posited an important affective role for the self-concept (e.g., C. W. Sherif, Sherif, & Nebergall, 1965; M. Sherif & Cantril, 1947). More recently, appraisal theories of emotion have emphasized the phenomenological im- portance of the self in the interpretation of events and the re- suiting effect on emotions (Fiske & Taylor, 1991 ). From a moti- vational standpoint, examples of the self 's pervasive influence abound. For example, the tendency to attribute another person's behavior to dispositional factors but one's own behavior to situational factors presumably occurs because the self dominates one's phenomenal perspective (Ross & Nisbett, 1991; Storms, 1973). Similarly, both self-serving biases and defense mecha- nisms have been attributed to self-protective or self-enhancing

Cynthia S. Symons, Department of Psychology, Houghton College; Blair T. Johnson, Department of Psychology, Syracuse University.

This article is based on Cynthia S. Symons's doctoral dissertation completed at Syracuse University under the direction of Blair T. Johnson. The preparation of this article was facilitated by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH01377-01.

We would like to thank Hung-Yu Lin, Beth Pavlik, and Kathy Paca- nowski for their assistance in gathering studies; Katherine Brundza and Diana Nichols for their help with some analyses; and Francis Bellezza, Kenneth Blick, John Mueller, and Glen Reeder for their kindness in sending unpublished materials. We would also like to express our ap- preciation to Harold Gelfand, Kenneth D. Levin, and Constantine Sedi- kides for valuable comments on earlier versions.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Cyn- thia S. Symons, Department of Psychology, Houghton College, Houghton, New York 14744. Electronic mail may be sent via Interact to csymons @ houghton.edu.

371

motives (cf. Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Maddi, 1989). The motiva- tional influence of the self in persuasion is evident when people resist persuasive appeals because of self-presentational concerns (Johnson & Eagly, 1989). Indeed, self-attention theory (Carver & Scheier, 1981 ) emphasizes that conformity of behav- ior to salient behavioral standards requires a focus on the self.

Given the breadth of interest in self-related phenomena and theories thereof (e.g., Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1987; Markus & Wurf, 1987), it is not surprising that researchers have more recently examined whether self-related processes invoke differ- ent memory stores (Klein & Loftus, 1993) and the extent to which the self-structure can be distinguished from structures about others (e.g., Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991). In addition, research on self-schematicity (Markus, 1977; Mar- kus & Wurf, 1987) demonstrates that the content of self- schematic domains can have a wide variety of motivational, affective, and mnemonic consequences.

The focus of this article is on these purportedly unique mne- monic aspects of the self. Several researchers have argued that the self-structure in memory is unique relative to other concepts by virtue of its superior elaborative and organizational proper- ties as well as its frequent use in information processing (e.g., Kihlstrom et al., 1988; Maki & Carlson, 1993; Markus, 1977; Rogers et al., 1977; Singer & Kolligan, 1987). If the self indeed has superior elaborative and organizational properties, then in- formation actively related to the self should be better remem- bered than information that is processed in other ways (e.g., the relating of information to someone else or the processing of words for meaning). Researchers who initially obtained this pattern labeled the phenomenon the self-reference effect (SRE; Rogers et al., 1977). Although many subsequent studies found superior recall following self-reference (SR; e.g., Bellezza, 1984; Kuiper & Rogers, 1979; Maki & McCaul, 1985), other research suggest that the SRE was not so robust. Specifically, other kinds of non-SR processing appeared to promote memory as well as or better than SR (e.g., Bellezza & Hoyt, 1992; Keenan & Baillet, 1980; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986; Lord, 1980). These conflicting study findings led Higgins and Bargh (1987) to conclude that "self-reference is neither necessary nor suffi-

372 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

cient for memory of input to be facilitated in comparison to a semantic orientation task" (p. 392). Moreover, after 2 decades, researchers seem divided between those who are willing to attri- bute the SRE to special mnemonic properties of the self (e.g., Maki & McCaul, 1985; Rogers et al., 1977) and those who are not (e.g., Brown, Keenan, & Potts, 1986; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986).

These inconsistencies were the impetus for our meta-analytic investigation. The first important question we address is whether SR tends to facilitate memory more effectively than other encod- ing tasks. That is, how consistently robust is the SRE? By ad- dressing this question, we provide an empirical test of conclu- sions by narrative reviewers (e.g., Higgins & Bargh, 1987) that variation in the results of SRE studies imply that it is not SR per se that facilitates memory. Our second and more important question concerns the conditions under which the SRE is most likely to occur. That is, assuming that SRE reviewers were cor- rect to conclude that studies' findings were inconsistent, we feel that it is most important to detail methodological features of. SRE studies that can be used to account for these inconsisten- cies. Obviously, such explanations represent the most theoreti- cally interesting aspect of this investigation.

In this article, we adopt the perspective that the SRE results primarily because the self is a well-developed and often-used construct in memory that promotes both elaboration and organi- zation of encoded information--a perspective first advanced by Klein and Loftus (1988). The respective roles of elaboration and organization in the SRE have been discussed by several researchers (e.g., A. G. Greenwald & Banaji, 1989; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986; Klein & Loftus, 1988). In general, these re- searchers emphasized the role of ordinary memory processes, which leads to the conclusion that there is nothing special or unique about SR that renders it more effective as a mnemonic device than other encoding tasks. According to Klein and Loftus, although SR may be ordinary in the sense that it can be ex- plained by these properties, it is distinguished from many other comparison tasks (e.g., synonym judgments) in the sense that SR promotes both elaboration and organization simultaneously, resulting in a mnemonic advantage. However, only Klein and Loftus's study provides empirical support for the joint elabora- tion-organization model. Their findings imply that, across all the studies in the literature, SR tends to result in superior mem- ory compared with tasks that promote either organization or elaboration separately (e.g., a synonym judgment task) but not combined.

We further submit that, to the extent that SR is spontaneous or habitual (e.g., Markus, 1977), the major benefit of SR lies not in its ability to invoke organizational or elaborative pro- cessing per se but rather its likelihood to spontaneously create matching between encoding and retrieval conditions (see Wells, Hoffman, & Enzle, 1984). This effect distinguishes the SR task from other tasks and may be the primary reason why it promotes memory more than other kinds of processing in the typical incidental learning situation.

This study provides a meta-analytic integration of the SRE literature that allows an examination of the consistency and generality of the SRE and the conditions under which it is likely to occur. Furthermore, it provides a meta-analytic test of the joint roles of elaboration and organization. Specifically, we present

evidence that the SRE occurs as a result of two features of the experimental task in a typical SRE study: (a) the nature of the comparison task (person-reference vs. semantic processing) and (b) the likelihood that the semantic or other-reference (OR) comparison task promotes both organization and elaboration. First, we show that comparisons that involve person reference (e.g., SR vs. OR) have smaller SREs relative to SR- versus semantic-encoding comparisons. Even more interesting, we show that comparisons of SR versus OR appear to be sensitive to certain task parameters (e.g., expectation of a test) that do not affect comparisons of SR with semantic processing. In addi- tion, we show that the mnemonic advantage following SR tends to diminish when SR is compared with encoding tasks that invoke memory structures, which resemble the self in terms of the amount of development and use (e.g., OR tasks in which the target is very well known). Second, we show that, when the task compared with SR is judged to promote both elaboration and organization of stimulus words, the SRE is smaller than when the comparison task promotes only organization (rela- tional processing) or only elaboration (item-specific processing; Klein & Loftus, 1988).

Historical Overview of SRE Research

In a seminal pair of studies, Rogers et al. (1977) extended the depth of processing (DOP) paradigm (Craik & Tulving, 1975) to the realm of the self. The basic strategy of the DOP paradigm is to compare the responses produced by encoding tasks that are presumed to differ in depth, or extensiveness, of processing (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1979). Essentially, DOP the- ory assumes that recall is a function of trace elaboration at the time of encoding. Differences in responses to different tasks, therefore, reflect underlying differences in the processes used to encode stimulus materials, such as word lists. DOP research- ers had already demonstrated that semantic-encoding tasks ( "Does the word mean the same as xxx?" ) resulted in superior recall compared with phonemic ("Does the word rhyme with xxx?") or structural encoding ("Does the word have capital letters?") tasks (e.g., Craik & Tulving, 1975). Rogers et al. used these standard DOP encoding tasks and added a new one, an SR task (i.e., "Does the word describe you?"). Showing that memory was even better for the SR condition than the semantic condition, Rogers et al. concluded that the self acts as a "superordinate schema" (p. 686) to facilitate encoding and retrieval of the information.

Researchers in subsequent studies generally confirmed that SR produces superior memory relative to semantic encoding. These researchers obtained an SRE with different (a) SR-encod- ing tasks (e.g., self-descriptiveness, autobiographical retrieval, imagery; Bower & Gilligan, 1979; Brown et al., 1986); (b) to- be-remembered materials (e.g., traits, nouns, prose; Bellezza, 1984; Klein & Loftus, 1988; Maki & McCaul, 1985; Reeder, McCormick, & Esselman, 1987); and (c) populations (e.g., children and adults, Pullyblank, Bisanz, Scott, & Champion, 1985; participants with and without depression, Derry & Kuiper, 1981). The SRE appeared to be a robust phenomenon and quickly surfaced in introductory social and cognitive psychology textbooks.

However, along with those who found SREs, researchers also

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 373

obtained findings in which SR failed to facilitate memory better than other types of processing. Researchers soon observed that comparisons of SR tasks with semantic tasks were confounded: That SR denotes a social entity, whereas the semantic task does not, suggests that enhanced memory may be a mere artifact of this task feature (Bower & Gilligan, 1979). In an effort to solve this problem, researchers compared memory following SR to that following OR (e.g., "Does this word describe your mother?"). Studies usually showed that the SRE was reduced, if not eliminated entirely, when the target referenced in the comparison task was a highly familiar other (e.g., Bower & Gilligan, 1979; Kuiper, 1982; Kuiper & Rogers, 1979)• Further studies suggest other boundary conditions on the SRE. For ex- ample, the SRE is shown to be reduced or reversed when imag- ery tasks are used (Lord, 1980), the semantic comparison task is a desirability rating (Ferguson, Rule, & Carlson, 1983), or the semantic comparison task promotes organization (but the SR task does not; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986)• Despite these null findings and reversals, our narrative inspection of the literature suggests that the SRE appears more often than not.

Self as a Cognitive Construct That Promotes Elaboration

The most popular explanation of the SRE is that SR promotes elaborative processing of to-be-remembered information (Kee- nan, 1993; Rogers et al•, 1977)• Based on DOP theory, depth is equated with the extent or amount of processing that a stimulus receives, whereas elaboration involves item-specific processing (see Eysenck & Eysenck, 1979)• When a participant processes a word using elaboration, he or she attends to the specific mean- ing of the word and the semantic associations between the word and extra list material in semantic memory (Anderson & Reder, 1979; Einstein & Hunt, 1980; Klein & Loftus, 1988). According to Klein and Loftus, the effect of this kind of processing is to provide multiple routes for retrieval and create an environment in which "inference-based reconstruction . . . [is supported] • . . in the event of retrieval failure" (p. 6). Exemplifying this elaboration perspective, Anderson and Reder (1979) theorized that

it is not depth of processing per se that is important, but one's prior practice at making elaborations about various types of information and practice at interpreting the previously stored elaborations. The "better" processing is that which generates more elaborations of the input that can be interpreted at retrieval . . . . The instructions that can produce rich elaboration and the materials that can be richly elaborated must be defined with respect to the processor. The most critical determinant of retention is the number of elaborations. (p. 390)

Anderson and Reder further argued that certain kinds of elabora- tions may be easier for some people because they are practiced habitually. This ease of processing that develops as a conse- quence of repeated elaborations suggests a connection to the memorial advantage of SR processing• Many researchers have concluded that processing information in a self-relevant way may be a "normal" processing mode (e.g., Catrambone, Beike, & Niedenthal, 1996; Catrambone & Markus, 1987; Fong & Markus, 1982; Wells et al., 1984)• They argued that

the self is exceptionally well learned and often used (Kihlstrom, 1993; Maki & Carlson, 1993) and that, indeed, people generally possess more expertise about themselves than about any other structure in memory (Markus, 1977). Thus, the evidence sug- gests that SR constitutes a processing task that receives a great deal of practice. An important consequence of the facility with which one elaborates on information using SR is that such processing can become exceptionally efficient.

Meta-analytic predictions for SR-semantic and SR- OR com- parisons. Given the foregoing logic, we expected the results of our investigation to show that SR should produce superior memory when compared with tasks that promote less elabora- tion. Proponents of the elaboration hypothesis argued that, under most circumstances, SR results in greater elaboration of the stimulus word than that achieved by a semantic comparison task (e.g., Rogers et al., 1977). Thus, based on the elaboration hypothesis, the SRE should be smaller (or disappear altogether) when studies use semantic-encoding tasks that engender greater elaboration and larger when the semantic-encoding tasks engen- der less elaboration than the SR.

On the one hand, it is difficult to think of tasks that would promote more elaboration than SR when the stimulus words are trait adjectives because trait dimensions are the most common attributes along which people judge themselves (Maki & McCaul, 1985; see Markus & Kitayama, 1991 ). Thus, we might predict that when traits are used as stimulus items, SR should always result in memory superior to that of a semantic task. On the other hand, such mnemonic superiority should increase to the extent that a person is practiced at making such elaborations• Certainly self-relevant judgments about traits are often prac- ticed, however, certain semantic judgments are also often used. In particular, people commonly identify certain trait adjectives as more socially desirable than other adjectives, a process that is part of socialization (Ferguson et al., 1983). We thus expect our meta-analysis to show that the act of judging a word for its desirability produces memory equivalent to that of SR (Ferguson et al., 1983; cf. McCaul & Maki, 1984). Similarly, SR should theoretically facilitate processing of traits better than nouns be- cause it is more common for, people to judge themselves along trait dimensions than to judge, for example, which careers they have considered in the past (Maki & McCaul, 1985; cf. Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986)• Thus, on the basis of the elaboration hypothe- sis and findings that are consistent with it, we expect our meta- analysis to show that the SRE is larger for studies in the literature that used traits rather than nouns•

Similar arguments hold with regard to SR versus OR tasks: To reference highly intimate others (e.g., one's mother) should result in more elaborations than to reference a less intimate other, presumably because elaboration of information relevant to intimate targets is a highly practiced task undertaken many times before. This high degree of elaboration theoretically pro- motes superior memory because it increases the likelihood of additional retrieval routes at the point of recall. Thus, to process information about a highly intimate other ought to promote superior memory relative to that produced by reference to some- one less intimate• Elaboration of stimulus words during refer- ence to a highly intimate other (e.g., one's mother) is a fre- quently occurring task. It is conceivable that information about intimate others may be nearly as well known and well elaborated

374 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

as self information (Aron et al., 1991 ). Thus, to the degree that the target other is intimate, the SRE should less likely be ob- tained (e.g., Bower & Gilligan, 1979; Brown et al., 1986; Kuiper, 1982). Yet, because studies in this subliterature have not always used highly intimate target others (e.g., the experi- menter for the laboratory session), we still expect our meta- analysis to reveal a mnemonic advantage for SR conditions over OR conditions. When the rated target is highly intimate, how- ever, OR should result in memory of stimulus words nearly equivalent to that of SR.

We should note that many researchers did not distinguish between familiarity and intimacy in their operationalizations of OR tasks. For example, in the literature, it is common to see, for example, Johnny Carson and one's mother described as highly "familiar" stimulus others. However, although both their mothers and Johnny Carson may be rated by research partici- pants as very familiar target others, participants are, of course, much more likely to have an intimate knowledge of their moth- ers. Although this may seem like a minor and admittedly obvious point, we present it because it has contributed to the perception of inconsistency in findings in the SRE literature. Consistent with the elaboration hypothesis, researchers have examined the degree to which representations of intimate others may overlap that of the self (e.g., Aron et al., 1991 ). One's memory structure about one's mother should obviously be much more elaborate, differentiated, and well known than a memory structure about Johnny Carson. Thus, representations about one's mother are theoretically more likely to promote recall equivalent to that evoked by SR. Later, we show that the distinction between familiarity and intimacy, as they are used in the literature, is important: Only intimacy predicts variation in effect sizes in the SRE literature.

Encoding specificity. An important principle of memory is that elaboration at encoding cannot solely account for retrieval. A large body of literature asserts the importance of retrieval conditions as well as encoding conditions. Based on the encod- ing specificity principle (Fisher & Craik, '1977; Tulving, 1979; Tulving & Thompson, 1973), the best retention is obtained when retrieval conditions reinstate conditions that were present at en- coding. Wells et al. (1984) examined the effects of encoding- specific conditions on the SRE and showed evidence that partici- pants spontaneously reinstated SR conditions at retrieval. Nota- bly, even when encoding and retrieval conditions were matched (i.e., OR encoding was followed by an OR cue at retrieval), recognition memory was higher for the SR condition. This find- ing is consistent with DOP research (Fisher & Craik, 1977) that shows differences in recall across processing conditions despite matched encoding and retrieval conditions. These find- ings suggest that, even with matched conditions, encoding condi- tions have an effect. In the same way, SR encoding may promote better recall than comparison processing conditions because it promotes SR retrieval (Wells et al., 1984) and more elaboration of stimulus words.

In summary, the elaboration hypothesis predicts that, across the literature, SR should be superior to both semantic and OR processing. Moreover, if we extend the logic of the elaboration hypothesis, (a) in general, the SRE should be larger for studies that compared SR tasks with tasks that promote less elaboration; (b) within the class of studies that used SR and OR tasks, the

SRE should be larger for low-intimacy targets than for high- intimacy targets; and (c) within the SR-semantic class of com- parisons, SREs should be larger when studies used traits rather than nouns.

Self as a Construct That Promotes Organization

Ironically, a huge literature on the mnemonic effects of orga- nization predated the DOP perspective that elaboration is memo- ry 's driving force (see Bousfield & Bousfield, 1966; and Mand- ler, 1967), but it has only more recently been brought to bear on the SRE (Klein & Klhlstrom, 1986). Organization is the process of grouping items together. It (a) is essentially relational processing in which words are grouped based on some set of semantic criteria (Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986); (b) results in attention to similarities between list words (Hunt & McDaniel, 1993) as well as associations between the words and their cate- gory label (Battig & Bellezza, 1979; Klein & Klhlstrom, 1986); and (c) may take different forms (e.g., subjective organization or organizational strategies that are unique to the particular stimulus list or encoding situation; see Battig & Bellezza, 1979). According to Klein and Klhlstrom, organization facilitates recall in two ways. First, it encourages encoding of relationships be- tween list words that share the same category, resulting in the development of multiple retrieval paths. Second, the associations formed between the words and their category label allow the category label to act as a retrieval cue, thus facilitating recall.

Klein and his colleagues rallied evidence to show that organi- zational processing also plays a role in the SRE (Klhlstrom et al., 1988; Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986; Klein & Loftus, 1988; Klein, Loftus, & Schell, 1994). For example, Klein and Kihl- strom showed that organizational properties presumably inher- ent in a typical SR task could account for its mnemonic superior- ity relative to a semantic comparison task. They found that, when an SR task is compared with a semantic-encoding condi- tion designed to promote organization, the SRE disappeared. They concluded that organization was confounded with SR in the typical SRE paradigm.

In-line with this reasoning, if an organizational principle un- derlies the mnemonic superiority of SR encoding, to the degree that a comparison task promotes organization (e.g., a categori- zation task), then the difference in subsequent recall following the two tasks should decrease. However, organization can take several forms (Battig & Bellezza, 1979). For example, it can occur because participants become aware of a category due to its size (Hunt & Seta, 1984), can be subjective, or could be deliberately induced by the experimenter (e.g., through a cate- gory-sorting task). Although it has been demonstrated that a self-descriptiveness judgment task can elicit organizational pro- cessing (e.g., Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986), no researcher has specifically examined the degree to which specific kinds of organizing strategies or styles of organizational processing natu- rally arise out of an SR task or affect the SRE. On the basis of Klein and his colleagues' findings (e.g., Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986; Klein & Loftus, 1988), however, we can generally predict that, to the degree that SR processing elicits organization, recall should be greater than for a task that does not. Moreover, theoret- ically if both comparison tasks promote organization, to the

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 375

degree that SR results in more organization than the comparison task, an SRE should be observed.

SR as Organized and Elaborative

Under certain circumstances, either organization or elabora- tion may produce good recall or prove superior to the other. Although some researchers have discussed the idea that either organizational (relational) or elaborative (item-specific) pro- cessing may explain the mnemonic superiority of SR, only Klein and Loftus (1988) have performed an empirical test of the hypothesis that both elaboration and organization are invoked during SR. By doing so, they raised certain issues that had not been addressed directly in the SRE literature. The basic question that has driven investigations of SR is whether there is something inherent in the SR task that makes it likely to produce more recall than a semantic or OR task. The important focus that Klein and Loftus emphasized was the joint effects of elaboration and organization and the mnemonic advantages that occur when both processes are simultaneously invoked by a particular task.

In a review in which they discussed the joint role of item- specific and relational processing across a wide range of cogni- tive phenomena, Hunt and McDaniel (1993) summarized the basic logic that appears in different domains of the memory literature, including Klein and Loftus's (1988) experiment. In their article, Hunt and McDaniel discussed the idea that, when words are already related, similarities between words are natu- rally encoded during comprehension and attention to differences between the words (item-specific processing) facilitates mem- ory more than attention to similarities. However, when stimulus words are unrelated, differences between words are naturally encoded during comprehension and attention to similarities be- tween the words (relational processing) facilitates memory more than attention to differences (Einstein & Hunt, 1980; Hunt & Einstein, 1981 ). On the basis of this logic, Klein and Loftus (1988) hypothesized that, if organization underlies SR, then an SR task should have effects similar to that of an organi- zational task. Thus, if the stimulus words are already related, then attention during comprehension is naturally drawn to rela- tionships between words and both organizational processing and SR should be redundant. In this situation, elaborative processing should facilitate more memory than organization or SR. Klein and Loftus further hypothesized that, if elaboration underlies SR, then an SR task should have effects similar to an elaborative task. Thus, when words are unrelated, attention during compre- hension is naturally drawn to differences between words. In this situation, both elaboration and SR should be redundant and organization should serve to enhance recall more than either SR or elaboration. Klein and Loftus's results show a pattern that supports a joint elaboration-organization explanation. Specifi- cally, when list words were related, SR resulted in effects similar to elaboration: Both resulted in memory superior to organiza- tional processing. However, when the list words were not related, SR also resulted in effects similar to an organizational task. In this case, both SR and organizational tasks enhanced recall more than the elaborative task. Klein and Loftus, therefore, advocated what they called a "dual-processing approach" and concluded that both organizational and elaborative processing are invoked in an SR task.

Given these ideas about the joint roles of relational and item- specific processing and consistent with Klein and Loftus's (1988) experiment, several predictions that distinguish SR from other kinds of processing arise. (a) SR tasks, which presumably promote both relational processing and item-specific processing, ought to promote better memory than any other task that pro- motes only one of these processes. (b) When list words are already related, an SR task should promote more memory than a task that promotes organization (because, when the list is related, organizational processing is redundant [Hunt & Ein- stein, 1981], and elaborative processing, theoretically inherent in an SR task, promotes optimal recall [ Klein & Loftus, 1988 ]). (c) Conversely, when list words are already unrelated, an SR task should promote more memory than a task that promotes elaboration (because, when the list is unrelated, elaborative pro- cessing is redundant [Hunt & Einstein, 1981], and organiza- tional processing, theoretically inherent in an SR task, promotes optimal recall [Klein & Loftus, 1988]).

Meta-Analysis

Design

Boundaries for the sample of studies. We attempted to maintain in our sample only those studies that used the standard DOP incidental learning paradigm, in which an encoding (ori- enting) question is posed and a stimulus word is presented for the participant's judgment. By eliminating atypical studies from the sample, we were able to confine our recta-analysis to studies or portions of studies that were relatively consistent on all meth- odological dimensions except those that are important for meta- analytic moderator testing (Johnson, 1989). Thus, we excluded studies that used unusual methodologies relative to the rest of our sample. For example, some studies involved the reading of passages of prose, which participants were instructed to read and relate to themselves as much as possible (e.g., Reeder et al., 1987). Other studies used atypical stimuli that were not similar to those used in most of our sample (e.g., faces; Mueller, Bailis, & Goldstein, 1979) or participants were not asked to recall stimuli in a manner that conformed to the vast majority of SRE studies.

Theoretical plan. Our general plan for the meta-analysis was first to test our expectation that the SRE is significant yet inconsistent across all studies in the literature. We then planned to test our expectation that the SRE would be larger in studies where researchers compared SR with semantic encoding rather than SR with OR. Because most of our other hypotheses are specific to one or the other of these two general classes of studies, we then planned to examine our hypotheses separately within these two classes. Indeed, some moderators that we ex- amined could only be examined within one of the classes (e.g., type of semantic-encoding task).

On the basis of Klein and Loftus's (1988) conceptualization of the joint effects of relational and item-specific processing, we expect our meta-analysis to show that the SRE is smaller when the comparison task is judged to elicit both relational and item-specific processing rather than either one or the other of the two processes. Thus, for example, the SRE should decrease as intimacy of a rated target increases because the rating of a

3 7 6 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

highly intimate other conceivably involves relational processing in which a highly organized domain in memory is referenced (see Sedikides, Olsen, & Reis, 1993) and item-specific pro- cessing that involves increasing degrees of elaboration as one ' s knowledge of the OR target increases.

Exploratory analyses. On an exploratory basis, we exam- ined several other possible moderators of the SRE, which should prove of special interest to SRE researchers who have speculated about most of these dimensions. Indeed, in coding studies for their characteristics, we included any dimension of which we were aware that other SRE researchers had found to be plausible moderators of SRE magnitude. Because many of these modera- tors have been the subject of theoretical debate and the relevant studies have yielded quite inconsistent findings, it was difficult to make meta-analytic predict ions about the moderators ' roles in the SRE. Nonetheless, the exploratory moderators included (a ) specific aspects of the experimental situation (e.g., whether a distractor task was used and what the length of the stimulus presentat ion was ), ( b ) dependent variable (free recall vs. recog- n i t ion) , ( c ) design type (wi th in vs. between subjects) , and (d ) memory load (i.e., amount of to-be- remembered mater ial) . We expect results for these dimensions to provide important clues regarding the basis of the SRE. Thus, for example, i f the use of a distractor task has no impact on the SRE for s e m a n t i c - S R comparisons but increases the SRE for S R - O R comparisons, then it may be that OR tasks benefit more from rehearsal strate- gies, as some reseachers have argued (Kuiper & Rogers, 1979).

Method

Criteria for study inclusion. Our meta-analysis focuses on two com- parisons performed in the majority of SRE studies: (a) SR versus seman- tic encoding and (b) SR versus OR encoding. Studies had to meet two criteria:

1. Researchers had to manipulate encoding instructions by presenting participants with a task in which either (a) SR and semantic encoding or (b) SR and OR were compared. We generally included studies that presented traditional DOP paradigm encoding tasks; however, stud!es in which researchers did not use traditional tasks were included if we judged them to have manipulated the type of encoding in a relevant way, even though the task was not conventional (e.g., Mueller, Heesacker, & Ross, 1984). Both within- and between-subjects designs were included.

2. Acceptable dependent measures included free recall, recognition, or cued recall. Either adjusted or raw score means were acceptable; however, when both adjusted and raw score means were available, ad- justed means were used. The mere use of an SR trait descriptiveness task in a reaction-time paradigm was not sufficient for inclusion (e.g., Markus, 1977); the task had to specifically test the effects of type of encoding on some measure of recall or recognition.

Study retrieval. A thorough search of the SRE literature was con- ducted to find as many studies as possible. We obtained studies through literature searches using PsycL1T (1974-1994) and by searching refer- ence lists of relevant articles. In addition, we searched for unpublished dissertations and conference papers and made specific attempts to contact researchers of unpublished studies to avoid publication bias. Only studies that were available as of June 1994 were included in the sample.

Coding. We coded studies on several dimensions that were relevant for theoretical or exploratory purposes and on many more dimensions that were descriptive only. The theoretically relevant dimensions in- cluded (a) type of comparison (SR-semantic vs. SR-OR), (b) type of SR task (self-descriptive, autobiographical, imagery, associating self with nouns [e.g., body parts or boats], other), (c) type of semantic

task (synonym decision, generate a definition, fits sentence, fits category, other), (d) relatedness of stimulus words (high, low, unable to rate), (e) type of stimuli presented (trait adjectives, nouns, other), ( f ) famil- iarity of the other-referent target (high, low, unable to rate), (g) intimacy of the other-referent target (high, low, unable to rate), and (h) type of processing promoted by the comparison task (relational, item-specific processing, or both). The exploratory dimensions included (a) type of dependent measure (free recall, cued recall, recognition), (b) design of study (within or between subjects), (c) nature of timing interval for retrieval measure (fixed, unlimited; recorded time if fixed), (d) type of OR task (descriptive, biographical, imagery, associated target other with nouns, other), (e) timing of stimulus presentation (fixed, variable based on response latency, variable based on experimenter judgment, other, unknown), ( f ) length of stimulus presentation, (g) duration of interval between task and retrieval, (h) presence of distractor task, (i) expecta- tion of a memory task (i.e., use of an incidental vs. an intentional learning paradigm), and (j) memory load (coded as number of words in the stimulus list). The merely descriptive characteristics included (a) year of publication, (b) source of publication (journal, other publication, dissertation or master's thesis, unpublished), (c) source of participant population (undergraduate or other ), (d) percentage of male participants in the sample, (e) method to test participants (individual or group), (f) geographic area in which the study was conducted, (g) mode of stimulus presentation (index cards, monitor, projector, read by experimenter, booklet, tachistoscope, other), and (i) expectation of a retrieval test by participants (i.e., incidental or intentional learning paradigm).

Computation of effect sizes. Effect sizes for the studies were re- corded, along with their significance and direction. In so far as possible, separate effect sizes (gs) were computed for each relevant manipulation (i.e., SR vs. semantic encoding and SR vs. OR encoding) with the following formula:

Msel f - - Msemantic (other) g = SDpoo~ed

where Ms,if is the mean recall for the SR-encoding condition, M~,,~tlc ~oth~r) is the mean recall for the semantic- (or OR-) encoding condition, and SDpoot~d is the pooled standard deviation (see Johnson, 1989). We converted the gs to ds by correcting them for the bias that occurs, especially with small sample sizes (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Some studies yielded multiple effect sizes because (a) more than one dependent measure was used (e.g., two different recall measures) or (b) several different levels of a variable were manipulated (e.g., several different target others; Keenan & Baillet, 1980). The effect sizes were analyzed using standard meta-analytic techniques (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Johnson, 1989).

Results

Characteristics of studies. We began our analyses by sum- marizing the characteristics of studies reviewed in the meta- analysis. Table 1 shows the summary of study characteristics aggregated across all SRE studies and then across the two sub- sets of studies that represent the separate classes of (a ) SR versus semantic manipulat ions and (b ) SR versus OR manipula- tions. Table 2 presents the calculated effect size (d ) for each study in our meta-analysis, along with its important study attributes.

As Table 1 shows, studies in our meta-analysis ( a ) were published relatively recently; ( b ) were primarily published re- ports; ( c ) tested 39 participants on average; (d ) tested partici- pants primarily f rom college populations; ( e ) usually tested adults; ( f ) were primarily conducted in North America; (g ) tended to test participants individually ( 7 5 % ) , with 24% using

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 377

group methodologies; (h) tended to use within-subjects (75%) versus between-subjects designs (25%); (i) had an average memory load of about 54 words, with studies in the SR versus OR class tending to have longer stimulus lists than the SR versus semantic class; (j) used trait words as stimuli more than nouns; (k) primarily used stimulus lists that contained unrelated words; (1) used a small number of encoding tasks on average, with SR versus OR studies tending to use more tasks than did SR versus semantic studies; (m) tended to present stimuli at a fixed rate or measure participants' reaction times; (n) used a variety of modalities to present stimulus materials, with the majority using computers; (o) tended to use free recall as the dependent mea- sure, with a small percentage using recognition and very few using cued recall; (p) tended to allow participants a fixed time period to respond during the retrieval task; (q) were slightly less likely to use distractor tasks (47 % ) than not (53 % ) between presentation and retrieval; and (r) tended to use an incidental learning paradigm (87%) in which participants did not expect a recall test rather than an intentional learning situation in Which testing was expected.

Overall SRE. After summarizing study char~icteristics, we analyzed the entire set of SRE studies to test the hypothesis that SR results in greater memory than OR or semantic encoding. These analyses appear at the bottom of Table 1 and show that SR encoding does promote better recall on average than other types of encoding, as evidenced by a mean weighted effect size that differed significantly from the 0.00 value that indicates exactly no effect, d = 0.50, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.45-0.54. Also as expected, the assumption of homogeneity of effect sizes was rejected, Q(128) = 451.40, p < .0001. Consistent with the conclusion that study results were inconsis- tent, homogeneity could not be achieved until we discarded 34 (26%) outlying effect sizes. The resultant mean effect size was still significant, d = 0.45, 95% CI = 0.39-0.50. Thus, the hypothesis of an overall SRE across the literature was supported, although its magnitude Varied considerably.

Cross-Literature Models for SRE Magnitude

Following the overall analysis, we fitted models using coded study characteristics to explain variation in effect sizes. With our first model, we examined whether the SRE varied as a function of studies' manipulation class (SR-semantic vs. SR- OR). As predicted, the SRE did vary as a function of manipula- tion class, with a significantly smaller SRE for the SR-OR versus the SR-semantic class (see Table 3). However, also as expected, each of these mean SREs was highly significant. Ef- fect sizes within each class were also found to be heterogeneous; it was necessary to remove 11 studies (18%) from the SR- semantic class and 14 studies (20%) from the SR-OR class to achieve homogeneity. After removal of these studies, however, the resulting mean effect sizes were still significant (see Table 1). Thus, results show that, although study effect sizes are inconsistent, the SRE does tend to occur when one compares SR encoding with semantic encoding and OR, as predicted.

To test our hypothesis that the SRE would be smaller when the comparison task promotes both relational and item-specific processing, we performed a model test by collapsing across manipulation class. As the second model in Table 3 shows and

as we predicted, when the comparison tasks used in studies in the literature promoted both relational and item-specific pro- cessing, the SRE was significantly smaller than when the com- parison task promoted either relational or item-specific pro- cessing. Within the class of studies that used tasks that were judged to promote both relational and item-specific processing, the effect sizes were homogeneous; moreover, this class differed significantly from the separate classes for relational and item- specific processing. However, the relational and item-specific classes did not differ significantly from each other. In all three classes, significant SREs were observed.

Two continuous predictors--(a) time between encoding and memory tasks and (b) length of stimulus presentation--were significant predictors of the magnitude of SR effect sizes across the literature. Specifically, the SRE tended to increase as the time between the encoding and memory tasks increased and to decrease as the length of stimulus presentation grew longer. Note that the latter finding is based on the minority of studies that used fixed stimulus presentation times (i.e., k = 51 ). As Table 4 shows, both of these pattems generalized across the two ma- nipulation classes. We next attempt to explain inconsistencies within the SR-semantic and SR-OR manipulation classes, respectively.

Moderators of SR-Semantic Effect Size Magnitude

Theoretical moderators. For the SR-semantic manipulation class, model tests for relatedness of stimuli reveal that studies using highly related stimulus items obtained a smaller mean SRE than those in the low-relatedness class (see Table 5 ). Indeed, the mean SRE in the high-relatedness class was not significant, and effect sizes in this class were consistent in contrast to the low- relatedness set in which a significant mean SRE occurred.

A model for type of stimulus materials used shows that, con- sistent with the elaboration hypothesis, the mean SRE for studies that used traits was significantly greater than that produced in studies that used nouns or other types of stimulus materials (see Table 5; as Table 6 shows and as discussed below, this pattern also appears for the SR-OR subliterature). However, the mean SRE for both the traits and the nouns classes was significant. Effect sizes for both classes of stimulus materials were inconsistent.

The next model was assessed to examine whether the seman- tic-encoding task used was related to SRE magnitude. Although there were eight classes of tasks in all, a few clear differences did emerge (see Table 5 for specific differences). Consistent with the organization hypothesis, the mean SRE for the fits- category class was marginally smaller than the mean SRE for the synonym judgment class (p = .057); however, it was not smaller than other classes of semantic tasks. Also, studies that used desirability ratings tended not to observe a significant mean SRE, in contrast to studies in the other classes. Studies that used tasks in which participants had to generate definitions and those that used synonym judgment tasks obtained significantly larger mean SREs than those that used desirability ratings. However, desirability judgments did not differ significantly from any of the other tasks, and the synonym judgment class differed sig- nificantly only from tasks involving desirability judgments.

Only three studies in the entire sample tested children as

378 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

Table 1 Summary of Study Characteristics for SRE Studies and Within Manipulation Class

All SR-semantic S R - O R Variables and class (k = 129) (k = 60) (k = 69)

Publication characteristics

Median publication year of studies 1985 1985 Publication form of studies

Journal articles or book chapters 126 58 Unpublished reports (including theses, etc.) 3 2

1985

68 1

Participant and study method characteristics

M number of participants 38.75 47.17 31.43 Participant population

College undergraduate 106 54 52 Other 23 6 17

Age of participants Adult 126 57 69 Child 3 3 0

Geographic area of study United States 85 36 49 Canada 38 22 16 Europe 6 2 4

Method used to test participants Individual 97 46 51 Group 31 13 18 Other a 1 1 0

Experimental design Within subjects 97 32 65 Between subjects 32 28 4

M memory load (in words) 53.64 46.25 60.09 Stimulus type used in included studies

Trait words 90 47 43 Nouns 37 11 26 Other 2 2 0

Relatedness of stimulus items High 13 9 4 Low 116 51 65

M number of encoding tasks presented 3.53 2.53 4.39 Timing of stimulus presentation

Fixed time (e.g., every 7 s) 51 23 28 Variable, based on participant response 54 18 36 Variable, determined by experimenter 16 12 4 Other 7 6 1 Unable to rate 1 1 0

Mode of stimulus presentation Index cards 11 11 0 Monitor 67 23 44 Overhead projector 23 8 15 Read by experimenter 18 10 8 Tachistoscope 4 4 0 Booklet (read by participant) 5 4 1 Other 1 0 1

Type of dependent variable used in studies Free recall 105 54 51 Cued recall 2 2 0 Recognition 22 4 18

Time allowed for recall-recognition test Fixed 93 49 44 Unlimited 36 11 25

Distractor task Present 60 32 28 Absent 69 28 41

Participants' expectation of memory test Expect 17 2 15 Do not expect 112 58 54

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 379

Table 1 (continued)

All SR - semantic SR - OR Variables and class (k = 129) (k = 60) (k = 69)

Effect size summary

M weighted effect size (d÷) 0.50 0.65 0.35 95% CI 0.45-0.54 0.58-0.71 0.29-0.42

Homogeneity of effect sizes (Q)b 451.40"** 178.30"** 230.40*** n outliers removed to achieve homogeneity 34 (26%) 11 (18%) 14 (20%) M weighted d÷ excluding outliers 0.45 0.59 0.26

95% CI 0.39-0.50 0.52-0.66 0.19-0.33 M unweighted d 0.53 0.72 0.35

95% CI 0.42-0.62 0.58-0.86 0.22-0.45

Note. For the purposes of these analyses, each effect size represents a separate study. CI = confidence interval; OR = other reference; SR = self-reference; SRE = self-reference effect. a Davis (1979) manipulated this variable, b Significant value indicates rejection of hypothesis of homogeneity. .*** p < .001.

participants, and all three of these studies also used SR versus semantic tasks. However, model tests reveal that studies with adults as participants obtained significantly larger mean SREs than studies with children as participants; in both classes, the mean SRE was significant. In addition, whereas effect sizes in the children class were homogeneous, effect sizes in the adult class were quite heterogeneous.

Exploratory analyses. Exploratory analyses reveal, first, that type of SR task related significantly to the magnitude of the SRE. 1 Specifically, self-descriptiveness, autobiographical, and other tasks do not differ in promoting recall. However, the mean effect size for the association with nouns class differs signifi- cantly from the other tasks (see Table 5).

As Table 5 shows next, a significant model test examining the SRE as a function of dependent variable used shows that the mean SRE for studies using recognition was not significant but that the mean SREs for studies using free and cued recall were significant. Study findings were quite inconsistent within the free recall class but consistent in the other two classes.

Two other model tests were significant. First, the model for mode of stimulus presentation shows that the use of projectors resulted in the largest SREs for this class, but mean effect sizes were not significantly different from studies that used index cards or booklets to present stimuli. Second, the model for par- ticipant population reveals that studies that tested undergradu- ates as participants obtained significantly larger SREs than stud- ies that tested participants who were not undergraduates,

In contrast to the usual expectation that within-subjects de- signs are more powerful statistically than between-subjects de- signs, type of experimental design did not affect the magnitude of the SRE. F ina l l y - - a s Table 4 de ta i l s - -as the memory load induced in the studies increased, so did the magnitude of the semant ic -SR effect sizes (a pattern that did not appear in the S R - O R class).

Moderators of S R - O R Effect Size Magnitude

Theoretical moderators. As Table 6 shows, within the S R - OR class and, consistent with expectations, the mean SRE was larger (and significant) in studies using traits rather than nouns.

Moreover, the mean SRE for studies using nouns was not sig- nificant. (This pattern contrasts with the SR-semant ic manipu- lation class in which results reveal significant mean SREs in both the traits and nouns classes.) However, both classes' effect sizes were quite heterogeneous.

Also as expected, model tests reveal that familiarity of the rated target other did not predict the magnitude of the SRE, whereas intimacy did. Specifically, as Table 6 shows, studies in which participants rated highly intimate targets obtained a significantly smaller mean SRE than studies in which target others were low in intimacy. 2 However, although the SRE was smaller when highly intimate others were rated, SR still tended to result in better memory.

Model tests also show that studies using self-descriptiveness tasks produced a significantly larger mean SRE than did studies using imagery tasks, as hypothesized and compared with other tasks (see Table 6). In addition, use of SR-imagery tasks within the SR- versus OR-manipulation class did not result in a signifi- cant mean SRE, as evidenced by the CI for that class. Within all three classes of SR tasks, study effects were inconsistent.

Exploratory moderators. Exploratory model tests for OR task used reveal that studies in the descriptiveness class had a significantly larger mean SRE than did the imagery, nonspecific- other, and other classes. Moreover, the mean SRE for studies using imagery tasks was not significant, as its CI shows. Model tests of dependent variable used reveals a significantly larger SRE when recognition versus free recall was used. This pattern is exactly opposite from that obtained for studies in the SR versus semantic manipulation class, which obtained a larger mean SRE when recall was used rather than recognition (see Table 5).

(text continues on page 384)

l In this case, type of SR task was considered to be exploratory because we felt that we could not make a priori predictions about the patterns in the literature. This is in contrast to the same model test for the SR-OR manipulation class; the literature predicts that imagery task studies should obtain smaller SREs than other kinds of tasks.

2 For both the familiarity and intimacy classes, studies in the unable to rate class tended to use nonspecific target others. It was difficult to classify these targets as either high or low in familiarity or intimacy.

380 S Y M O N S A N D J O H N S O N

@

o ~ ~

• H o

i= o

8 ~

8

@

<<~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ± ~ < < ~

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . O . 8 ~ m . * # # = . . . . . . . . . . . . ~

0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ~ O 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 O 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 d d 4 4 d d d d d d d d d ~ d d 5 o d d o d d ~ 5 ~ o ~ " " ~ ~ ~ d o ~

~ : 1 , _ : 1 1 1 4 4 ~ m , ~ 4 I ~ . . : ~ ~ . ~ . ~ 4 ~ 1 I I 1 : = I I I I 1 ~ : = ~ 1 4 1 I I

1 4 I I I . , . :=:=:=:=:=:= I := := := := := := := = := I I I I := I I I I I :==::=:=:= I : = 1 I I

0 e ~ # # ~ ~ .~ .,-, ~ ~ ~ ~ ,-,

0 ~ 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ~ 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ~ ~ 0 ~ O 0 0 0 0 m O ~

I I I I I I I I | I I I I I I I I I I I

I I I I

~ ~ o

~ ~ ~ ¢ J ~ o o ~ o oo ~ o o o o ~ o oo . ~= o . . . . . . . . . ~ o , o

M N E M O N I C ADVANTAGES OF T H E SELF 3 8 1

~ ~ ~o ~ ~ ~ o ~ ~ ~ o ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ o ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ e ~ e~ e~ e~ ~ . ~

' 6 ' 6 "6 " 6 ' 6 "6 "6 "6 "6"6 "6 '6 "6 "6 "6 '6 "6 " 6 " 6 " 6 ' 6 "6 "6 "6 '6 "6 "6 '6

o . . o o. o. Q o o o

~ ~ ~ o o o ~ o o ~ ~ ~ o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o

I I I I t

I I I I I

o o o o n ~ ~l ~ ~ ~ ~i ~l ~ o o o ~ . ~ ~ ~ ~. ~ o

o o o o o o o o ~ o o o o o o o o ~ m m ~ ~ ~ m ~ ~ ~

o

C3C~ C~

= = ~ = ==~=~=~=o ~ .... ~ ~ ~o . . . . "~°'~'aL'~ ,~,~=.~ .~.~o~.=_~=~= c~ -~-~ ~ ~ ~

~ ~ ~ ~ ' ~ "~=

~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ -

382 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

?5

Y E-

[,,.

,-1

os~ U,.,

o = o ~

b e ~

0 ,,,,~ ¢1

I

m

d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d d ~ d ~ ~ ~ N ~ d N d

z z z ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ z z z z z

I I I t I I I I I ~ = I ~ I ~ I ~ I I ~ = = ~ ~ ~

I I I t I I I I I = ~ = I ~ I ~ t = = I I = = = = = = =

• -: ~ , - :~ ,-4 ,.-: ~ o o ~ ~ ,-; ,-; ,-: o ~ ~ c~ ~ ,-: ~ ,-: c ~ ~ ~

I 1 I I I I I I I I I I '~

~ ~ .,..,. .,- .,. .= ' ~ " o, ~ ,'I, , , ' t~ ~ ~ ~ ~ . = =

..% - ~ c ~ . ~ °= ~ r., r., o., o ~L 8 . 8 , 8 . ~ 'd

o~ • . ~ ~,~ ~ ~ ~ s ~ o : ~ , . . ;,

M N E M O N I C A D V A N T A G E S O F T I l E S E L F 383

~ .... u°o ~ ° ~

u~= = = = = =.=. .~.= .=~.=.=. ~.=.= .= .= . ._

0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 O 0

o 0 o o 0 o o 0 o O O m O 0 o o 0 o o 0 o 0 o o ~

- - . = = = = . . = . . . . . = . - = = - = =

= . = - ~ = = = = = = I I = ~ l ~ l I I I I I I I I I I I I =

.~,~zz,~zzz

ooooooooo

d ~ o o ~ o o o ~ o o o ~ o ~ o o ~ . . . . . . . . I

I [ l l l l l l I

I I I i i

o [ . .

o ,

~u

~o~ " ~ I I ~

~ . . ~

. ~ z ~

.o u o , ~,

o .b" o~ ;..-~

o II II "~

.~ . ~ ' ~

~ ' ~ .

II il = [ - '

o ~ ) ~ ,

, . o tl

384 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

Table 3 Cross-Literature Models for SRE Magnitude

Variable and class

95% CI Between- Mean weighted for d÷ Within-class

class effect effect size homogeneity (QB) k (d+) Lower Upper (Qwi) a

Manipulation class SR vs. semantic SR vs. OR

Type of processing induced by comparison task

Relational Item specific Both relational and

item specific

42.70***

21.29"**

60 0.65 0.58 0.71 178.30"** 69 0.35 0.29 0.42 230.40***

37 0.51~ 0.43 0.59 115.68"** 67 0.56a 0.50 0.62 282.92***

25 0.2% 0.19 0.39 31.50

Note. Mean effect sizes not sharing the same subscript significantly differed (p < .05, a priori). CI = confidence interval; OR = other reference; SR = self-reference; SRE = self-reference effect. a Significance indicates rejection of the hypothesis of homogeneity. *** p < .001.

Several models emerge that implicate task restrictions used in SRE studies that mediate the SRE. It is interesting that these restrictions were not found to affect the magnitude of SREs in the SR-seman t i c manipulation class. First, results show that the magnitude of the SRE was related to the use of distractor tasks. Specifically, within both the distractor-present and the distractor-absent classes, significant mean SREs resulted. How- ever, the mean SRE for the distractor-present class was signifi- cantly larger than the mean SRE for the distractor-absent class. Further analyses reveal that distractor tasks had no influence on the magnitude of the SRE for studies that used high-intimacy targets (but did for low-intimacy targets). Second, results show that for studies in which participants did not expect a memory test, there was a significantly larger mean SRE than for studies in which participants did expect a test. Moreover, the mean SRE for studies in which participants expected a test was not significant. Further analyses reveal that an expectation of a test had no effect on the magnitude of the SRE for studies that used high-intimacy targets (but it did for low-intimacy targets).

Finally, two other exploratory models were significant for this manipulation class. The model test for mode of stimulus

presentation shows that when researchers used a projector to present stimulus materials, they obtained a significantly smaller mean SRE than those who used monitors or presented stimuli orally. Moreover, studies in the projector class obtained a non- significant mean SRE. In each case, study findings were heterogeneous.

The model test for participant population reveals that studies that tested undergraduates as participants obtained a smaller mean SRE than studies that tested other populations. (Note that this pattern is opposite of that found for SR-semant ic studies.) However, both mean SREs were significant; in both cases, study findings are inconsistent.

D i s cus s ion

The SRE has been of interest to researchers because of the assumption that it could tell them something about the self in memory and its relationship to other kinds of encoding pro- cesses. Most of the researchers cited in the literature have at- tempted to investigate this relationship through manipulation of various task parameters or with various populations. Judging by

Table 4 Continuous Models for Study Effect Sizes Across the Literature and Within Manipulation Class

All SR - semantic SR-OR (k = 129) (k = 60) (k = 69)

Predictor b /3 b /3 b /3

Memory load -.0004 -.03 .0041" .25 -.0009 -.07 Time between encoding and

memory task .0160"** .16 .0135" .16 .0240** .21 Length of stimulus presentation -.0667*** - .64 -.1060"** - .60 -.0545*** - .66

Note. Models are least-square regressions with weights equivalent to the reciprocal of the variance of each effect size. k was smaller for some models. OR = other reference; SR = self-reference. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p <.001.

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF

Table 5 Effect Sizes as a Function of Task for SR-Semantic Manipulation

Variable and class

Between- Mean weighted class effect effect size

(QB) k (d,)

95% CI for d÷

Lower Upper

Within-class homogeneity

(Qwi) ~

Relatedness of stimuli 17.33"** High 9 0.20 -0.01 0.42 11.55 Low 51 0.69 0.62 0.75 149.41"**

Stimulus type 12.79"* Traits 47 0.67, 0.60 0.73 119.72"** Nouns 11 0.40b 0.21 0.59 42.79*** Other 2 1.68c 0.86 2.51 3.00

Type of semantic task 44.54*** Synonym judgment 16 0.72~d 0.63 0.82 39.16"* Generate definition 9 1.04cd 0.84 1.24 37.74*** Fits sentence 4 0.32ab 0.01 0.63 0.97 Fits category 7 0.45ab¢ 0.19 0.72 22.31"* Meaningful 12 0.62,~ 0.49 0.75 15.15 Familiar 3 0.57~ 0.28 0.86 3.96 Desirable 5 0.16, -0.06 0.37 11.86" Other 4 0.81,~ 0.58 1.19 2.61

Age of participants 13.41"** Adult 57 0.69 0.62 0.76 164.30"** Child 3 0.34 0.17 0.52 0.58

Type of SR task 9.09* Self-descriptiveness 42 0.66a 0.59 0.72 13.81"** Autobiographical 11 0.74~ 0.54 0.94 52.28*** Association with nouns 4 0.14b -0.22 0.49 1.00 Other 3 0.61, 0.28 0.94 2.12

Dependent variable 16.79"** Free recall 54 0.69, 0.62 0.75 151.78"** Cued recall 2 0.60ab 0.02 1.18 1.30 Recognition 4 0.21b 0.00 0.43 8.42

Mode of stimulus presentation 34.92*** Index cards 11 0.73~ 0.56 0.90 43.81"** Monitor 23 0.57~ 0.47 0.67 76.10"** Projector 8 1.05, 0.86 1.24 12.92 Read by experimenter 10 0.55t~ 0.41 0.68 7.63 Tachistoscope 4 0.43b 0.25 0.61 2.19 Booklet 4 0.98~c 0.74 1.22 0.74

Participant population 11.16"* Undergraduates 54 0.69 0.62 0.76 159.79"** Other 6 0.39 0.23 0.55 7.34

Experimental design 3.08 Within subjects 32 0.68 0.61 0.76 60.66** Between subjects 28 0.57 0.46 0.67 114.56"**

Note. Mean effect sizes sharing the same subscript interval; SR = Self-reference. a Significance indicates rejection of the hypothesis of *p < .05. **p <.01. ***p < .001.

do not differ

homogeneity.

(p > .05, post hoc). CI = confidence

385

the relatively small number of new SRE studies in the last few years, we could easily conclude that the field has reached an impasse in terms of explaining why the SRE occurs. There is, of course, more recent work (e.g., Klein & Loftus, 1993; Klein, Loftus, & Burton, 1989) that focuses specifically on the poten- tial causal cognitive mechanisms that underlie the SRE. This review represents an important empirical contribution to the SRE literature because, in addition to answering questions about the robustness of the SRE, it (a) clarifies the roles of most of the important moderators of the effect and suggests some more that ought to be investigated, (b) confirms most of the theoreti- cal expectations that the SRE results because of elaborative and organized processing, and (c) suggests some important avenues

that should be investigated that have not yet been explored in primary-level research.

We approach our discussion from the theoretical perspective that many of our findings may be explained by two aspects of processing tasks that may play important roles in determining the magnitude of the SRE: (a) the degree to which the task promotes organization and elaboration and (b) the nature of the referenced memory domain and the particular type of informa- tion being processed and how they may interact with these processes. We begin by reviewing our general findings of an overall SRE and the results of the models for the comparison class and nature of processing induced by the task. Then, we discuss pragmatic concerns for the design of SRE studies, re-

386 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

Table 6 Effect Sizes as a Function of Task for SR-OR Manipulation

Variable and class

Between- Mean weighted 95% CI for d÷ Homogeneity class effect effect size within each class

(QB) k (d÷) Lower Upper (Qwi) a

Stimulus type 57.81 *** Trait 43 0.53 0.45 0.60 75.95** Noun 26 0.03 -0.08 0.13 96.64***

Familiarity of rated other 0.69 High 61 0.36a 0.30 0.43 225.89*** LOW 4 0.24~ -0.04 0.53 1.38 Unable to rate 4 0.34a 0.16 0.51 2.44

Intimacy of rated other 7.57* High 17 0.20a 0.07 0.33 18.26 Low 48 0.41~ 0.34 0.49 202.12"** Unable to rate 4 0.34ac 0.16 0.51 2.44

Type of SR task b 54.46*** Self-descriptiveness 39 0.53a 0.45 0.61 72.78** Imagery 13 --0.01b --0.16 0.13 27.96*** Other 15 0.12b -0.02 0.26 75.00***

Type of other task b 57.17"** Descriptiveness 35 0.58a 0.49 0.67 64.23** Imagery 13 -0.01b -0.16 0.13 27.96** Nonspecific other 14 0.19b 0.07 0.32 22.87 Other 5 0.25b 0.02 0.47 57.95***

Dependent variable 10.54"* Free recall 51 0.28 0.21 0.36 149.35"** Recognition 18 0.50 0.39 0.61 70.51"**

Distractor task 15.69"* Present 28 0.48 0.39 0.56 75.66*** Absent 41 0.23 0.14 0.32 139.05"**

Expectation of test 19.72"** Expect 15 0.02 -0.14 0.18 46.90*** Do not expect 54 0.41 0.35 0.48 163.77"**

Mode of stimulus presentation c 26.96***

Monitor 44 0.42~ 0.34 0.50 129.58"** Projector 15 -0.02b -0.18 0.14 37.82*** Read (by experimenter) 8 0.64a 0.44 0.84 18.08"

Participant population 10.07"* Undergraduates 52 0.29 0.22 0.36 156.89"** Other 17 0.51 0.40 0.62 63.44***

Experimental design 1.25 Within subjects 65 0.35 0.29 0.41 226.45*** Between subjects 4 0.61 0.16 1.06 2.70

Note. Mean effect sizes sharing the same subscript do not differ (p > .05, post hoc). CI = confidence interval; OR = other reference; SR = self-reference.

Significance indicates rejection of the hypothesis of homogeneity, b One study, Bower and Gilligan (1979, Experiment 2), included two SR versus OR comparisons that were omitted from this model test because they involved the only autobiographical-biographical tasks in the SR-OR manipulation class, c Two studies (Brown et al., 1986, Experiment 3; Wells et al., 1984) were omitted from this model test because they did not fit any coding class. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

gardless of manipulat ion class. Finally, we draw conclusions based on significant model tests that are unique to each particu- lar manipulat ion class.

SRE in Memory Is Robust

One impor tant finding of our study is that the SRE does occur with highly significant regularity. Al though study findings are inconsistent, SR was superior to semantic and OR encoding in facilitating memory in the studies reviewed. It may seem to some readers that this is a foregone conclusion of any review

of the SRE, but there is some disagreement among researchers in this literature as to whether SR tends to promote memory better overall than other kinds of processes. For example, as we noted earlier, Higgins and Bargh ' s (1987) important narrative review concludes that "self-reference is neither necessary nor sufficient for memory of input to be faci l i ta ted" (p. 392) . Our meta-analytic review suggests that, in fact, S R - - a l t h o u g h not necessary to promote good r e c a l l - - i s sufficient, by virtue of its ability to promote both item-specific and relational processing as well as its ability to promote compatible encoding and retrieval conditions.

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 387

Effects of Manipulation Class and the Nature of Processing

Manipulation class. The first moderator that affected the magnitude of the SRE across the literature was that a larger mean SRE was obtained in SR-semantic studies than in SR- OR studies. First, note that the SRE occurs on the average within both manipulation classes, although study findings were inconsistent. This finding begins to suggest possible differences between person-reference versus semantic processing as tasks, a topic that we discuss below.

Within the class of studies that used SR-OR manipulations, given the debates that have occurred in the literature, the finding that SR tends to promote better memory than OR may be surpris- ing (cf. Bower & Gilligan, 1979; Brown et al., 1986; Lord, 1980, 1987; Maki & McCaul, 1985). Those debates arose be- cause of inconsistent study findings; for example, reference to familiar others apparently causes the SRE to "disappear," lead- ing researchers to argue that SR creates no memory advantage over reference to a familiar other. The results of our study show that, although the SRE was weaker in SR-OR studies, it was nevertheless present and statistically significant, as predicted.

The finding that the SRE was larger for SR-semantic manipu- lations is also intriguing if one begins to consider the nature of the underlying memory processes and structures. If, for example, it is assumed that both self and semantic representations (or processing, for that matter) are qualitatively similar, then one must also be able to explain why SR tends to facilitate memory better than semantic encoding overall. None of the models that we analyzed could completely explain the variation in the SR- semantic cases. Although we must be careful about conclusions with regard to the structures that underlie SR and semantic processing that are based on correlational evidence, one plausi- ble conclusion is that SR is more effective in producing good recall than is a semantic task for a number of reasons. Specifi- cally, the typical SR task (a) uses traits as stimulus items more than 80% of the time, (b) taps trait domains likely to have been elaborated on many times using SR, (c) is likely to promote an SR mode in retrieval because of this practiced elaboration of traits, and (d) taps trait domains that are, because of the two points just mentioned (b and c), likely to be highly organized along self-related (or at least person-referent) dimensions. Con- sistent with this reasoning, SR promotes better recall than se- mantic processing across the literature; but, perhaps more im- portant, when we examined only SRE studies that used SR and semantic tasks that promote both relational and item-specific processing, the SRE was much smaller than it was for the set of all SR-semantic cases, k = 7, d = 0.29, 95% CI = 0.09- 0.49. However, even in this subclass of studies, there was sig- nificant heterogeneity, Qwi(6) = 22.41, p < .01; thus, although our codings of relational and item-specific processing predict some of the variation in SR-semantic studies, it does not ac- count for all of the variation (cf. Klein & Loftus, 1988). As we discuss later, some models we examined within the SR- semantic class help to shed light on important moderators of the relationship, however, because in almost every case study findings were inconsistent, primary-level research is still needed to isolate the causal mechanisms responsible for the difference between SR and semantic encoding.

Model test for nature of processing induced. The second important model test shows that the degree to which compared encoding tasks are equivalent in promoting item-specific and relational processing affects the magnitude of the SRE. As stated earlier, for each study in the literature, the comparison tasks were judged to promote either relational processing, item-spe- cific processing, or both (e.g., "Does the word mean the same as xxx?" probably promotes elaborative processing predominantly, whereas "Does the word describe your mother?" likely pro- motes both kinds of processes). Results show significantly smaller SREs on average for tasks that involved both kinds of processing. This would be expected if SR and the comparison task both promote the same kinds of processing. In addition, the effect sizes for this class were consistent, thus no further model testing is needed to explain that variation. Nonetheless, it is important to note that, even when the comparison task involved both kinds of processes, there was still a significant SRE for that class of studies. Presumably, SR not only invokes both processes but possibly more relational and item-specific processing. Of course, other alternatives are possible: For exam- ple, some additional process or condition may be invoked that results in superior memory, such as encoding specificity, or, because the majority of studies in this class used trait adjectives, it may be that SR poses an advantage because of the sheer frequency with which the average person relates traits to himself or herself (Wells et al., 1984).

Also interesting, and not wholly unexpected, is the finding that there was no difference in the magnitude of the SRE for studies in which the comparison task involved either relational or item- specific processing. In both cases, SR was superior in producing memory because theoretically SR invokes both. In both classes, however, there was significant variation in effect sizes. 'However, based on our data, the process of controlling for organization or elaboration does not entirely eliminate the SRE.

Roles of Organization and Elaboration

Understanding the role of organization can help us to explain the difference between semantic processing and SR if we take some of the following points into account. We have identified studies in which the comparison task has the potential to invoke relational (vs. item-specific) processing; however, just because a task promotes relational processing does not mean that mem- ory will necessarily be facilitated. For example, the provision of cues to help recall a category may facilitate memory for the category but not necessarily items in the category (Tulving & Pearlstone, 1966). In addition to the possibility that organiza- tion will not facilitate memory in all circumstances, we note too that, except for a few instances in the SRE literature, organi- zational instructions to the participant are rarely used in the design of studies (for notable exceptions, see Klein & Kihl- strom, 1986; and Klein & Loftus, 1988). Nonetheless, Klein and his colleagues have produced evidence that some SR tasks naturally promote such organization and that such organization does facilitate memory. Other evidence suggests that people will use categorization in the absence of category labels (e.g., Lewis, 1971), and that category size is an important task parameter. For example, Hunt and Seta (1984) showed that large categories were more likely to promote relational processing. However,

388 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

they also showed that, because relational processing is naturally promoted already, an encoding task that promotes item-specific processing is more beneficial for recall. Thus, relational pro- cessing is more likely to help recall of small categories than large ones.

These points may help to explain why SR promotes recall better than semantic encoding. On the one hand, SR has the potential (a) to provide self-relevant category labels, which may be helpful to facilitate subsequent recall because it may facilitate the reinstatement of encoding conditions at retrieval; (b) to help with the efficient processing of trait adjectives, despite the size of such a category, because of frequent processing of that do- main; as well as (c) because it arguably promotes item-specific as well as relational processing, thus complementing the rela- tional processing that is already present to create optimal recall conditions. On the other hand, without specific attention drawn to categories, the typical semantic-processing task is at a dis- advantage on several counts because (a) for most people, pro- cessing trait adjectives (a large category) semantically is not a frequent or well-practiced task; consequently, (b) category la- bels may not be as readily available; moreover, (c) although the nature of most semantic tasks in the literature is to promote item-specific versus relational processing, to the extent that SR promotes both, it is the more versatile of the two tasks.

One notable exception to this conclusion concerns the desir- ability task. We believe that the pattern we found in our meta- analysis lends support to the idea that, although pleasantness- desirability ratings have been traditionally used in the cognitive literature as a task thought to promote elaboration (e.g., Hunt & Einstein, 1981), in the typical SRE paradigm task, it is likely to promote a certain degree of relational processing as well (Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986). It fits the criteria for relational processing in that (a) there is potential for recognition of a category label and (b) such processing draws attention to the fact that, in the stimulus list words, there are words that are related in the sense that they are either desirable or not. This assumption, of course, represents an empirical question that could be addressed in further investigations.

Along this same line of reasoning, person reference may gen- erally promote more memory than a semantic task when list words are adjectives. It is logical to assume that some of the same mechanisms that govern an SR task may operate in any person-reference task. A person-reference task probably pro- vides a potential for recognition of an obvious category label, a task that is frequently practiced, and the potential for the development of an organized domain in memory around that person because the task is frequently practiced. As an application of this logic, the difference between SR and other person refer- ence is, of course, one of degrees. In other words, information about certain specific people (your mother, best friend, or worst enemy) is more frequently processed than information about other people (Johnny Carson or the experimenter at your study). People who are more often part of the information-processing environment are likely to be more accessible. Certainly, a partic- ipant who has engaged in an encoding task involving questions about himself or herself and about another person still has that information accessible in memory when asked to retrieve it. However, the more well known the person referenced is, the more organized and elaborated the information about the person

in memory is and the more accessible the person category is. One important consequence of this idea is the potential for reinstatement of conditions at retrieval that are compatible with those that were present when the information was encoded. Thus, SR should pose an advantage in processing over other tasks, as our findings suggest.

Pragmatic Concerns for the Design of SRE Studies

Certain task variations used in SRE studies appear to affect the size of the SRE. These moderating variables suggest practi- cal concerns that should be confronted by researchers when they design studies to investigate the SRE. It is interesting that, in some cases, moderators that have a significant effect on the magnitude of the SRE have apparently been ignored as factors in SRE study designs. The first of these moderators is memory load. Our results show that, as memory load increases, the mag- nitude of the SRE also increases but that this pattern exists only for SR-semantic comparisons. This model implies that, when there is a great deal to remember, SR is a relatively efficient processing strategy when compared with semantic encoding. One practical implication of this finding is that memory load may be an important consideration for researchers who wish to maximize (or minimize) the advantage of SR in their studies.

From a theoretical standpoint, this finding is a bit puzzling. Earlier, we discussed the effects of category size (Hunt & Seta, 1984): Large categories automatically promote relational pro- cessing, thus an item-specific processing task should facilitate memory more than a relational-processing task. Because most semantic tasks involve item-specific processing, why would the SRE increase as the memory load increases? To answer the question, we looked at the types of stimulus items that were processed. When traits are used as stimulus items, the SRE does increase as memory load increases, k = 47,/3 = .26, p < .01, demonstrating an advantage for person reference when pro- cessing trait items, a finding that is in-line theoretically with the patterns we predicted earlier. However, when the stimulus items are nouns, the pattern reverses itself, k = 11, /3 = -.47, p < .01. In this case, as memory load increases, the SRE gets smaller; this is a case where semantic processing is the normal processing mode for these kind of stimuli, thus posing an advan- tage because nouns are often elaborated and organized throtigh semantic processing.

It is interesting that the choice of experimental design had no impact on the magnitude of the SRE. Our data show that the choice of a within- or between-subjects design made no differ- ence for studies across the literature (or in either manipulation class). This is a curious finding because one would expect that controlling individual differences in processing would make a marked difference in the kinds of processes investigated in our meta-analysis. Thus, the SRE should have been larger in both manipulation classes when researchers used within- versus be- tween-subjects designs. We speculate that interference may oc- cur between encoding conditions in a within-subjects design. For example, Aron et al. (1991) have suggested that intimate others may actually overlap the self. Consequently, there is a confounding that arises that may render SR less advantageous than it is when a less intimate other is referenced. Similarly, participants may sometimes spontaneously consider whether a

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 389

word in a semantic condition describes themselves. This kind of interference should occur less frequently in a between-subjects design, thus the SRE could be larger. The question is an empiri- cal one, however, because virtually all studies in the literature that involved intimate other targets used within-subjects designs.

Finally, two moderators--participant population and mode of presentation--were found to exert opposite effects for the two manipulation classes. For the SR-OR class, larger SREs were found when undergraduate students were not the participants; in the SR-semantic class, when undergraduates were the partici- pants, significantly larger SREs were obtained than for other populations. This is a difficult finding to explain, and one that has received little attention because it has not been considered important theoretically (except to the degree that, e.g., the popula- tion possessed some characteristic that affects SR, such as depres- sion; Kuiper & Rogers, 1979). An examination of the 17 nonun- dergraduate studies shows that the samples were from a variety of other populations (e.g., participants with depression, healthy adults recruited from local communities, etc.). We present the results of this model as a design consideration that may be of importance to some researchers who choose to test other popula- tions, whether out of convenience or necessity.

The second moderator, mode of presentation, is similarly in- triguing in that it results in opposite patterns for the two manipu- lation classes. Specifically, in the SR-OR class, SREs were not obtained on average when a projector was used (monitors or orally presented stimuli resulted in significantly larger SREs). In contrast, projectors resulted in the largest SREs for the SR- semantic class. Different modes of presentation have largely been taken for granted in SRE study designs; presumably, the choice has been made based on convenience and history. The differences that we found may point to subtle differences in processing that may occur as a consequence of the choice of presentation mode. In particular, it should be noted that, in many cases in which projectors are used, the choice is often made, because the experimenter wishes to test more than one partici- pant at a time. It may be that, when an SR-OR manipulation is used, the presence of other people may provide cues for participant s (even though fellow participants are not the refer- enced target) that may help to facilitate subsequent retrieval. Although admittedly speculative, it is an interesting question that merits consideration in future primary research.

Variation Within the Two Manipulation Classes

At this point, we turn to a number of task parameters in SRE studies that explain variation within the two manipulation classes (SR-OR and SR-semantic). Several of these model tests suggest pragmatic issues in the design of SRE studies that researchers may have overlooked that directly affect the magnitude of the SRE; others have theoretical significance and may suggest new avenues of exploration. We begin with the set of moderators, unique to a particular manipulation class, that suggest either important task parameters that significantly affect the magnitude of the SRE or important theoretical variables.

Models for the SR-OR class. Three model tests that pertain only to the SR-OR class were significant: (a) intimacy of the target other, (b) type of SR task, and (c) type of OR task. The results of the first model test show that, although studies that

used highly intimate target others did obtain a significant mean SRE, this effect was significantly smaller than that obtained for studies that used low-intimacy target others. It should be noted that familiarity of the target other had no effect on the magnitude of observed SREs for studies in this class? We specifically included ratings of both familiarity and intimacy in the meta- analysis because it was obvious that researchers did not discrim- inate between these variables. For example, Johnny Carson may be referred to as a highly familiar target in some studies, but he is not likely to be a highly intimate target to the average participant. Thus, it does not come as a complete surprise that familiarity does not result in a significant model test when both one's mother and Carson are classified as highly familiar targets. Our hypothesis that intimacy is a significant predictor of the magnitude of the SRE was supported. Results show a weak, but still significant, SRE for studies in the high-intimacy class, and study findings are homogeneous. Even though the referenced target is highly intimate, there is still a slight advantage of SR. Moreover, there is a significant difference between the class of studies that used highly intimate targets and low-intimacy tar- gets, with significantly larger SREs in the low-intimacy class. Thus, reference to highly intimate others promotes memory al- most as well as SR.

Thus, consistent with our hypotheses and contentions in the literature, results suggest that referencing a very well-known other, information about whom is presumably well organized and elaborated on in memory, has a facilitating effect on recall. Because the self is, presumably, even better known and informa- tion about the self has been even more frequently elaborated on and organized, SR facilitates memory even better. As Aron et

al . (1991) have suggested, to the degree that a relationship is intimate, it is also likely to overlap with one 's self-representa- tions. Memory representations of one's mother are very likely to be fraught with SR associations, for example. It is possible that autobiographical retrieval may be more sensitive to this possibility than are trait descriptiveness tasks. However, only two studies in the literature used an episodic retrieval task in the SR-OR class .(Bower & Gilligan, 1979), and both used high-intimacy targets, thus no comparison to test this possibility was possible. Future research should address this possibility.

Type of task was shown to be a significant moderator of SRE size in the SR-OR class. Results show that studies that used imagery tasks did not observe SREs on average, confirming Lord's (1980) hypothesis that the SRE will not occur with imagery instructions. Moreover, whereas studies that used imag- ery tended not to obtain SREs, all other types of tasks (SR or OR) in the SR-OR class did tend to produce SREs. Model tests for both type of SR task and type of OR task reveal that studies that used descriptiveness tasks found significantly larger SREs, on average, than any other type of task. Lord offered an explana- tion for this finding: He theorized that imagery involves the same kind of visual processing a person uses when he or she

3 We performed continuous model tests as well for both familiarity and intimacy. The continuous models were based on ratings made by undergraduates who had judged the intimacy and familiarity of each referenced target used in the SRE literature. The findings for the continu- ous models parallel the categorical models we presented based on our codings. We thus omitted them to avoid redundancy.

390 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

actually views an object. When imagining others, he argued, one assumes one's usual visual orientation. In contrast, SR imagery forces participants to imagine themselves, causing them to as- sume an unusual visual orientation. Because imagining oneself is not a customary visual perspective, it does not help to facilitate memory. Imagining others, however, is customary; thus imagin- ing others facilitates memory as well as SR imagery. Our results provide no evidence to contradict Lord' s explanation. We would note, however, that Lord's formulation suggests that the imagin- ing of others should promote recall superior to that of SR. This hypothesis was not supported by our data: Results of the meta- analysis do not show that the imagining of others facilitates memory better than does SR.

Models for the SR-semantic class. Three model tests unique to the SR-semantic class were significant: (a) age, (b) type of SR task, and (c) type of semantic task. Results from the model test for age show that the SRE was significantly larger for studies that tested adults than for those that tested children. This finding is consistent with the hypotheses of the researchers who investigated age differences (e.g., Halpin, Puff, Mason, & Marston, 1984; Pullyblank et al., 1985). It has been argued that a likely explanation for this difference is that it is related to the rate of development of the self-concept. Specifically, children may exhibit a smaller advantage of SR relative to semantic encoding because their self-concepts are in an earlier develop- mental stage than adults. Consequently, the self-concept either has not developed sufficiently to facilitate processing to the degree that the adult self-concept can or has not been sufficiently elaborated to provide a ready network of potential retrieval cues to facilitate recall.

We offer a different explanation: An examination of the tasks used in studies that compare SR in children with SR in adults suggests that, although task adaptations were made for younger participants, the tasks were still very similar to those presented to adults. Thus, our alternative explanation is that researchers may not have observed SREs because task demands were inap- propriate to participants' developmental stage, not because chil- dren lack sufficiently developed self-concepts to produce SREs. Some data are available to support this contention. Barnas and Symons (1995) compared preschool children (4 -5 years old) with older children (in kindergarten and first grade). The encod- ing task was modified such that pictorial stimuli were presented with each corresponding stimulus word at encoding. Children were then given a standard free-recall memory test. Results show that not only do very young children demonstrate an SRE but also there was no interaction between encoding task and age. Thus, we suggest that conclusions about the relationship be- tween self-concept development and the SRE may be premature. More research is needed to address this issue.

The model for type of SR task shows that there was a signifi- cant difference between studies that used either self-descriptive- ness tasks or autobiographical retrieval tasks and those that used tasks involving association of the self with nouns (e.g., professions: "Did you ever wish to be a 9; doctor"). Studies that used tasks involving noun associations consistently did not observe SREs- -a finding that may well be due to the use of nouns as stimuli, an issue that we discuss at some length below. More interesting is the finding that there is no difference between self-descriptiveness tasks and autobiographical re-

trieval tasks in terms of the magnitude of the SRE they produce. Klein and his colleagues (Klein & Loftus, 1993; Klein et al., 1989) have made convincing arguments that to engage in a self- descriptiveness judgment versus an autobiographical retrieval task may actually involve the tapping of separate memory repre- sentations. They showed, for example, that a person who en- gages in a self-descriptiveness task before engaging in an auto- biographical retrieval task does not shorten response latencies on the second task, implying that information obtained during the self-descriptiveness judgment does not facilitate the autobio- graphical retrieval task. They showed the same pattern when autobiographical retrieval precedes the self-descriptiveness tasks. Klein et al. concluded that the two tasks tap different sources of information. Although their findings are compelling, we note that, on the basis of our meta-analysis, the differences that they observed do not seem to extend to dependent variables involving retrieval. That is, although self-descriptiveness judg- ments and autobiographical retrieval may involve accessing dif- ferent areas of memory and some researchers have argued that the two tasks should generate different levels of recall (Bellezza, 1993), the magnitude of the SRE is equivalent in the two task classes. Thus, the two tasks appear to generate equivalent levels of recall. An extension of Klein and Loftus's model to other dependent variables may help to explain the bases of the SRE.

Although the model for type of semantic task used compares several types of encoding tasks, some patterns emerge that may suggest important theoretical issues. The first pattern is that studies that used desirability ratings did not observe SREs on average, consistent with Ferguson et al.'s (1983) hypothesis. These researchers argued that the evaluative component inherent in SR tasks was a confounding variable and that, if SR is com- pared with an evaluative-judgment semantic task (i.e., judg- ments of the stimulus word's desirability), the SRE would disappear.

Our results do confirm this pattern. However, an obvious alternative explanation, given the focus of our article, is that the desirability task promotes both relational and item-specific processing, as we discussed earlier. If future research supports this conclusion, then it will explain why studies thai used desir- ability ratings obtained significantly smaller SREs than studies that used either synonym judgments or generate-definition tasks, which primarily promote elaborative processing (cf. Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986; Klein & Loftus, 1988). One other purely spec- ulative explanation may be offered for researchers not finding a signficant mean SRE in studies in which desirability ratings were used. It may be that, when participants judge the desirabil- ity of a trait adjective, there is a degree of SR involved, thus the same sort of interference that may result when one is processing information about an intimate other target. That is, an answer to the question, "Is this a desirable word?," may implicitly involve a judgment regarding whether "this is a desirable word for me" from the participant's perspective. Thus, ironically, it is possible that desirability and SR are confounded but not in the way Ferguson et al. (1983) hypothesized. Future researchers should specifically address the nature of the personal relevance of the to-be-processed words for the participant and how SR and desirability judgments facilitate memory. To date, very few researchers have done this, with the exception of researchers who investigated the effects of self-schematicity on the SRE

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 391

(e.g., Kuiper & Derry, 1982; Symons, 1990). As researchers continue to examine specific processing strategies that may pro- duce the SRE, these issues should be illuminated.

Theoretical Differences Between Person-Reference and Semantic Processing

The results of several model tests are interesting in that they allow us to speculate about the differences between person- reference and semantic processing. Our conclusions are based on a complex of model tests that were significant (or not sig- nificant) for one manipulation class versus the other. Specifi- cally, some model tests were significant for the SR-OR class but not for the SR-semantic class; some, such as stimulus type, show different patterns for the two classes. The results of these model tests allow us to theorize about differences between the SR-OR and SR-semantic manipulation classes and, in general perhaps, between person-reference and semantic encoding.

The first important theoretical model that we tested that re- flects differences between person-reference per se (SR-OR comparisons) and SR-semantic comparisons is the model test for stimulus type. Results show that, when nouns were used as stimuli, there was no SRE for the SR-OR class. However, when traits were used as stimuli, the studies obtained a significant mean SRE that was significantly larger than the mean SRE found for nouns. In contrast, in the SR-semantic class, a sig- nificant mean SRE appeared when either traits or nouns were used as stimuli, but the SRE was significantly larger with traits than nouns.

Many researchers have speculated about the ways in which traits are represented in memory (e.g., Breckler, Pratkanis, & McCann, 1991 ). It is interesting that nouns seem to be equally well remembered in both SR and OR (i.e., person-referent) conditions across studies in the SR-OR manipulation class. This finding may have implications for the assertion that nouns are sometimes part of the self-representation (e.g., Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986). If we make the assumption that nouns are not part of the self-representation and the SRE is indeed a "self- based" phenomenon, then SR encoding of nouns should not be expected to pose an advantage for later memory (Maki & McCaul, 1985). However, if we hold to the assumption that nouns are not an integral part of the self-concept (and that, therefore, SR should not facilitate memory for nouns), we must also be able to explain why it is that we find an SRE for SR- semantic manipulations that used nouns as stimuli. The most likely and parsimonious explanation for the finding of no SRE when nouns are used in SR-OR studies is that, of the 26 studies that used nouns, 13 also used imagery tasks, which tend not to find SREs on average, as we discussed earlier. The rest of the studies used nonstandard tasks, either judgments about a non- specific other (for the OR task; k = 8) or tasks that were judged to be unusual and could not be included with other classes. Again, this is an important line of questioning by which pro- cessing assumptions about SR versus other kinds of processes could be tested by careful attention to the kinds of stimuli that are presented to participants.

The model test for dependent variable is consistent with our earlier discussion of the effects of organization and elaboration. Results show opposite patterns for recall and recognition in the

two manipulation classes. In both manipulation classes, there is a significant difference between studies that used recognition versus those that used free recall. In the SR-semantic class, however, SREs were significantly larger for studies that used recall rather than those that used recognition; in fact, studies that used recognition tended not to observe SREs at all. In contrast, SR-OR studies that used recognition observed sig- nificantly larger SREs on average than those that used free recall. It seems then that the presence of retrieval cues following an SR-semantic manipulation provides an advantage following se- mantic encoding. A recognition task may provide cues that sup- ply an advantage equivalent to that inherent in an OR or SR task, which may have the benefit of a category label to facilitate retrieval. In contrast, the presence of retrieval cues following OR seems to disrupt retrieval, resulting in larger SREs than those obtained with free-recall tasks. It may be that experi- menter-imposed retrieval cues may actually interfere with sub- jective categorical structures imposed by the participant when encoding words about another person. Although this explanation is purely speculative, this pattern may be related to our findings for the next set of model tests, which indicate that OR may be sensitive to interference with short-term memory stores. As a final precaution, we note that we did not compare semantic encoding with OR directly; therefore, care should be observed with regard to comparisons of the two types of processing.

OR Tasks: Sensitive to Disruptions in Short-Term Memory?

The final set of models that we examined are specific to the SR-OR manipulation class and suggest that OR may be particularly sensitive to task restrictions that interfere with short- term memory stores. First, the model for expectation of test shows that there was no SRE when participants expected to have their recall tested following the encoding task but that studies using incidental learning paradigms observed SREs on average. Second, the use of distractor tasks resulted in a larger mean SRE than when distractor tasks were not used in SR-OR studies. Because (a) the expectation of a memory test should increase the use of rehearsal strategies and (b) distractor tasks disrupt any rehearsal strategies that participants may decide to undertake, both of these model tests suggest that rehearsal is disrupted and results in a disadvantage for retrieval following OR that does not affect SR-semantic comparisons. It is interest- ing that both of these models are consistent with Kuiper and Rogers's (1979) theory that OR may require some sort of re- hearsal strategy to be mnemonically effective.

Two interesting continuous models suggest that OR may be sensitive to task restrictions that affect time or short-term mem- ory immediately following encoding tasks: SREs increased (a) as time between encoding and retrieval increased and (b) as length of stimulus presentation decreased. These findings sug- gest that, the longer the time between encoding and retrieval, the more likely it is that SR will promote more memory than OR. (These patterns also hold true for semantic encoding, al- though the magnitude of the effect is somewhat smaller.) For OR manipulations, this finding is further support for the idea that OR may benefit from a rehearsal strategy: The longer parti- cipants are required to retain the information in short-term mem-

392 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

ory, the less likely that OR will prove as effective a mnemonic strategy relative to SR.

The finding that SR has an advantage when length of stimulus presentation is briefer for both the S R - O R and the SR-semant ic classes is important because it suggests the possibility that SR may promote good retention more quickly than either semantic or OR processing; both semantic and OR processing seem to require more processing time to be mnemonically effective. One interpretation of these findings is that the SRE is " t ime sensi- tive": SR may be an especially effective strategy, compared with OR, if participants have to wait a long time between encod- ing and retrieval or if they have to encode stimuli very rapidly. Thus, our findings suggest that SR may be more spontaneous or automatic in promoting memory than OR.

However, before special mnemonic properties are attributed to SR, we thought it wise to investigate the possibility that these effects exist also when participants refer a word to memory structures that may be very similar to the self in memory (Aron et al., 1991). When these models are tested using only highly intimate others, we found that the short-term memory interfer- ence created by either a distractor task or failure to expect a test, as well as the findings with regard to length of stimulus presentation, all fall away when highly intimate others are refer- enced. However, these model tests with high-intimacy others involved a small number of studies. These possibilities are par- ticularly intriguing and deserve to be investigated in primary- level research.

These findings suggest patterns that have been virtually ig- nored in the SRE literature until our meta-analysis. Based on our assumption that SR is constant across both manipulation classes, the patterns suggest an important difference between OR and semantic encoding. Because, in the SR-semant ic class, the magnitude of the SRE is unaffected by distractor tasks or expectation of a test, we assume that semantic encoding is resis- tant to the effects of these task parameters on short-term memory.

C o n c l u s i o n s

Researchers repeatedly have asked the question, "Is the self unique?"4 Although our study cannot directly address questions about the self-structure in memory, it indicates that certain as- pects of engaging in an SR task may indeed pose special mne- monic advantages. First, although SR is posited to promote both relational and item-specific processing, even when SR is paired with a task judged to also promote these processes, an SRE emerges. Of course, we have not made assumptions about the degree to which these tasks promote both kinds of processing, but our results suggest that SR is more effective in promoting memory. Results with regard to stimulus items and length of stimulus presentation suggest that SR may be particularly spon- taneous and efficient when stimulus items are commonly judged

th rough person reference (e.g., traits; Markus, 1977). Moreover, although SR is superior to OR in promoting memory and even when intimate other targets were judged, we observed that in many ways the referencing of an intimate other appears to have similar effects to SR. For example, even the short-term memory disruptions that we observed disappear for judgments of highly intimate others.

The everyday implications of these findings extend to many different areas. Certainly the power of SR has been underesti- mated in some areas and, possibly, overestimated in others. One implication of our study is that SR works best to facilitate memory when certain kinds of stimuli are used--s t imul i that are commonly organized and elaborated on through SR. When these stimuli are used, however, SR appears to be a very efficient, possibly spontaneous processing mode. Of course, future re- searchers must address these questions with more direct compar- isons of SR, OR, and semantic encoding, with particular atten- tion to differences in the kinds of stimulus material used and their importance to the individual participant. More important, we hope that researchers will address several new potential causal processes suggested by our meta-analysis that may ex- plain why the SRE occurs. Our evidence suggests that SR is a uniquely efficient process; but it is probably unique only in the sense that, because it is a highly practiced task, it results in spontaneous, efficient processing of certain kinds of information that people deal with each day--mater ia l that is often used, well organized, and exceptionally well elaborated.

4 We recognize that there is still debate in cognitive literatures as to the question of the self's uniqueness (Breckler, Pratkanis, & McCann, 1991; Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1987; A. G. Greenwald & Banaji, 1989; Higgins & Bargh, 1987). As Breckler et al. pointed out, it is premature to conclude that the self is a unique structure. For one thing, theoretical models of the self are not sufficiently specified to assert that the self is a unique structure in memory (G. T. Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984). Moreover, the criteria for uniqueness are unclear, although such criteria have been pursued by researchers in other areas of memory (see Oster- gaard, 1992).

References

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Anderson, J. R., & Reder, L. M. (1979). An elaborative processing ex- planation of depth of processing. In L. S. Cermak & E I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing in human memory (pp. 385-403 ). Hills- dale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Aron, A., Aron, E., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other and self, Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 60, 241-253.

Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D.A. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 297-332.

*Bargh, J. A., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic pro- cessing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 925-939.

Barnas, M. V., & Symons, C. S. (1995, April). Self-reference encoding as a memory aid in young children. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, MA.

*Baron, R. S., & Moore, D. L. (1987). The impact of exercise-induced arousal on the self-other effect. Social Cognition, 5, 166-177.

Battig, W. E, & Bellezza, E S. (1979). Organization and levels of pro- cessing. In C. R. Puff (Ed.), Memory organization and structure (pp. 321-346). New York: Academic Press.

Bellezza, E S. (1984). The self as a mnemonic device: The role of internal cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 506- 516.

Bellezza, F. S. (1993). Does "perplexing" describe the self-reference

MNEMONIC ADVANTAGES OF THE SELF 393

effect? Yes! In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 51-59). New York: Erlbaum.

Bellezza, E S., & Hoyt, S. K. (1992). The self-reference effect and mental cuing. Social Cognition, 10, 51-78.

Bousfield, A. K., & Bousfield, W. A. (1966). Measurement of clustering and of sequential constancies in repeated free recall. Psychological Reports, 19, 935-942.

*Bowel; G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information related to one's self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 420-432.

*Bradley, B., & Mathews, A. (1983). Negative self-schemata in clinical depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22 (3), 173-181.

*Bradley, B.P., & Mathews, A. (1988). Memory bias in recovered clinical depressives. Information processing and the emotional disor- ders [Special issue]. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 235-245.

*Breck, B. E., & Smith, S. H. (1983). selective recall of self-descriptive traits by socially anxious and nonanxious females. Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 71-76.

Breckler, S. J., Pratkanis, A. R., & McCann, C. D. ( 1991 ). The represen- tation of self in multidimensional cognitive space. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 97-112.

*Brown, P., Keenan, J. M., & Potts, G. R. (1986). The self-reference effect with imagery encoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 51, 897-906.

*Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Morris, K. J. ( 1985 ). Semantic, evalua- tive, and self-referent processing: Memory, cognitive effort, and soma- tovisceral activity. Psychophysiology, 22, 371-384.

Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom, J. E (1987). Personality and social intelli- gence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. E (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer- Verlag.

Catrambone, R., Beike, D., & Niedenthal, P. (1996). Is the self-concept a habitual referent in judgments of similarity? Psychological Science, 7, 158-163.

Catrambone, R., & Markus, H. (1987). The role of self-schemas in going beyond the information given. Social Cognition, 5, 349-368.

*Clifford, P. I., & Hemsley, D. R. (1987). The influence of depression on the processing of personal attributes. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 98-103.

Craik, E I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: General 11, 268-294.

*Davis, H. (1979). Self-reference and the encoding of personal informa- tion in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3, 97-110.

*Derry, P., & Kuiper, N. (1981). Schematic processing and self-refer- ence in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 286-297.

Einstein, G. O., & Hunt, R. R. (1980). Levels of processsing and organi- zation: Additive effects of individual item and relational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 588-598.

Eysenck, M. W., & Eysenck, M. C. (1979). Processing depth, elaboration of encoding, memory stores, and extended capacity. Journal of Experi- mental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 472-484.

*Ferguson, T. J,, Rule, G. R., & Carlson, D. ( 1983 ). Memory for person- ally relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 44, 251-261.

Fisher, R.P., & Craik, E I. M. (1977). Interaction between encoding and retrieval operations in cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 701-711.

Fiske, S.T., & Taylor, S.E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Fong, G. T., & Markus, H. (1982). Self-schemas and judgments about others. Social Cognition, 1, 192-205.

*Ganellen, R. J., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Why does self-reference pro-

mote incidental encoding? Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- ogy, 21, 284-300.

Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1989). The self as a memory sys- tem: Powerful, but ordinary. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 57, 41-54.

Greenwald, G. T., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 129- 178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

*Halpin, J. A., Puff, R., Mason, H. E, & Marston, S. P. (1984). Self- reference encoding and incidental recall by children. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 87-89.

Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.

Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social per- ception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369-425.

*Hull, J. G., Van Treuren, R. R., Ashford, S.J., Propson, P., & Andrus, B.W. (1988). Self-consciousness and the processing of self-relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 452- 465.

Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item-specific in- formation in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behav- ior, 20, 497-514.

Hunt, R. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization and distinctiveness. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 421-445.

Hunt, R. R., & Seta, C. E. (1984). Category effects in recall: The roles of relational and individual item recall. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 3, 454-464.

James. W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. Johnson, B.T. (1989). DSTAT: Software for the meta-analytic review

of research literatures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Johnson, B.T., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). The effects of involvement on

persuasion: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 290-314. *Katz, A. N. (1987). Self-reference in the encoding of creative-relevant

traits. Journal of Personality, 55, 97-120. Keenan, J. M. (1993). An exemplar model can explain Klein and Loftus'

results. In 1". K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cogni- tion (VoL 5, pp. 69-78). New York: Erlbaum.

*Keenan, J. M., & Baillet, S.D. (1980). Memory for personally and socially significant events. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 8, pp. 651-669). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

*Keenan, J. M., Golding, J. M., & Brown, E (1992). Factors controlling the advantage of self-reference over other-reference. Social Cognition, 10, 79-94.

*Kendzierski, D. (1980). Self-schemata and scripts: The recall of self- referent and scriptal information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 23-29.

Kihlstrom, J. E (1993). What does the self look like? In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (VoL 5, pp. 79-90) . New York: Eflbanm.

Klhlstrom, J, E, Cantor, N., Albright, J. S., Chew, B. R., Klein, S. B., & Niedenthal, P.M. (1988). Information processing and the study of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 145-180). New York: Academic Press.

*Klein, S. B., & Kihlstrom, J. E (1986). Elaboration, organization, and the self-reference effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ogy: General 115, 26-38.

*Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: The contribution of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 5-11.

Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1993). The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self. In T.K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 1-49). New York: Erlbaum.

*Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Burton, H. (1989). Two self-reference ef- fects: The importance of distinguishing between self-descriptiveness

394 SYMONS AND JOHNSON

judgments and autobiographical retrieval in self-referent encoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 853-865.

Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Schell, T. (1994). Repeated testing: A tech- nique for assessing the roles of elaborative and organizational pro- cessing in the representation of social knowledge. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 5, 830-839.

Kuiper, N.A. (1982). Processing personal information about well- known others and the self: The use of efficient cognitive schemata. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 14, 1 - 12.

*Kuipet; N. A., & Derry, E (1982). Depressed and nondepressed content in self-reference in mild depressives. Journal of Personality, 50, 62-74.

*Kuiper, N. A., & MacDonald, M. R. (1982). Self and other perception in mild depressives. Social Cognition, 1, 223-239.

*Kuiper, N. A., 0linger, L. J., MacDonald, M. R., & Shaw, B. E (1985). Self-schema processing of depressed and nondepressed content: The effects of vulnerability to depression. Social Cognition, 3, 77-93.

*Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). Encoding of personal informa- tion: Self-other differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 37, 499-514.

Lewis, M.Q. (1971). Categorized lists and cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 129-131.

*Lord, C. G. (1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: Two modes of processing social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 257-269.

*Lord, C. G. (1987). Imagining self and others: Reply to Brown, Kee- nan, and Potts (1986). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 445-450.

Maddi, S. R. (1989). Personality theories: A comparative analysis (5th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Maki, R.H., & Carlson, A. K. (1993). Knowledge of the self: Is it special? In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cogni- tion (Vol. 5, pp. 101-110). New York: Erlbaum.

*Maki, R. H., & McCaul, K. D. (1985). The effects of self-reference versus other reference on the recall of traits and nouns. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 169-172.

Mandler, G. (1967). Organization and memory. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 1, pp. 327-373). New York: Academic Press.

Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.

Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

Markus, H., & VV~rf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299- 337.

*McCaul, K. D., & Maki, R. H. (1984). Self-reference versus desirabil- ity ratings and memory for traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 953-955.

*Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Weinman, J. (1987). Memory bias in clini- cal anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 94-98.

Mueller, J. H., Bailis, K. L., & Goldstein, A. G. (1979). Depth of pro- cessing and anxiety in facial recognition. British Journal of Psychol- ogy, 70, 511-515.

Mueller, J., Heesacker, M., & Ross, M. (1984). Body-image conscious- ness and self-reference effects in face recognition. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 277-279.

*Mueller, J. H., Thompson, W. B., & Grove, T. R. (1991, May). Self- referent processing and hemisphericity. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.

*Mueller, J. H., Wonderlich, S., & Dugan, K. (1986). Self-referent pro- cessing of age-specific material. Psychology and Aging, 1, 293-299.

*Myers, J. E, Lynch, P.B., & Bakal, D.A. (1989). Dysthymic and

hypomanic self-referent effects associated with depressive illness and recovery. Cognitive Theory and Research, 13, 195-209.

Ostergaard, A. L. (1992). A method for judging measures of stochastic dependence: Further comments on the current controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 413-420.

PsycLIT [Database]. (1974-1994, June). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association [Producer]. Available from: SilverPlatter [Distributor].

*Pullyblank, J., Bisanz, J., Scott, C., & Champion, M. A. (1985). Devel- opmental invariance in the effects of functional self-knowledge on memory. Child Development, 56, 1447-1454.

Reeder, G.D., McCormick, C.B., & Esselman, E.D. (1987). Self- referent processing and recall of prose. Journal of Educational Psy- chology, 79, 243-248.

*Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. E (1987). Hypnotic effects on hyper- mnesia. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 35(3), 155-170.

*Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N.A., & Kirker, W.S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677-688.

Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). Theperson and the situation: Per- spectives of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

*Schmeck, R.R., & Meier, S.T. (1984). Self-reference as a learning strategy and learning style. Human Learning, 3, 9-17.

Sedikides, C., Olsen, N., & Reis, H. T. (1993). Relationships as natural categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 71-82.

Sherif, C. W., Sherif, M., & Nebergall, R. E. (1965). Attitude and atti- tude change: The social judgment-involvement approach. Philadel- phia: Saunders.

Sherif, M., & Cantril, H. (1947). The psychology of ego-involvements: Social attitudes and identifications. New York: Wiley.

Singer, J. L., & Kolligan, J., Jr. (1987). Personality: Developments in the study of private experience. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 533-574.

Storms, M. D. (1973). Videotape and the attribution process: Reversing actors' and observers' points of view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 165-175.

*Sutton, L.J., Teasdale, J. D., & Broadbent, D.E., (1988). Negative self-schema: The effects of induced depressed mood. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 188-190.

*Symons, C. S. (1990). The role of self-schematicity in the self-refer- ence effect. Unpublished master's thesis, Syracuse University, Depart- ment of Psychology, Syracuse, NY.

Tulving, E. (1979). Relation between encoding specificity and levels of processing. In L. S. Cermak & E I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of pro- cessing in human memory (pp. 405-428). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tulving, E., & Pearlston, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381-391.

Tulving, E., & Thompson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and re- trieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352- 373.

*Warren, M. W., Hughes, A. T, & Tobias, S. B. (1985). Autobiographi- cal elaboration and memory for adjectives. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60( 1 ), 55-58.

*Wells, G. L., Hoffman, C., & Enzle, M. E. (1984). Self- versus other- referent processing at encoding and retrieval. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 574-584.

Received December 6, 1995 Revision received June 10, 1996

Accepted June 17, 1996 •