toddlers with asd are better at visual search without...

1
Toddlers with ASD are better at visual search without trying harder: a pupillometric study Motivation We found that toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder are better at visual search than age-matched controls. The cause is mysterious: enhanced perception? faster search? more attentional resources? Greater ‘focus’? Goal Use pupillometry to determine the attentional ‘mode’ of toddlers during visual search, to test the idea that toddlers with ASD do not search better than Typical toddlers, just more often. Erik Blaser, Luke Eglington, & Zsuzsa Kaldy, University of Massachusetts Boston Pupillometry of toddlers Toddlers with ASD are better at visual search Apparatus Tobii T120 Pupillometry & Locus Coeruleus Conclusions This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 2R15EY017985 awarded to the authors apple target flies in, dissolves (~3 s) search display appears (4 s) Eye tracking { ‘reward’ animation (~3 s) 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 % Success at Finding the Target Set Size ASD, conjunction TYP, conjunction ASD, single feature TYP, single feature Each block consisted of 4 familiarization trials and 13 test trials and lasted for approximately 4 minutes. The test trials consisted of 4 single feature trials (set size 5 or 9) and 9 feature conjunction trials (set size 5, 9 or 13). Phasic mode: focused attention on task- relevant stimuli (associated with better performance, e.g. on search tasks). -->determines task-evoked pupil response Tonic mode: diffuse attention marked by broad sampling of stimuli in the environment. -->determines baseline pupil level Aston-Jones, et al., 2007 Pupil changes due to mental activity are due to activity of the LC Firing rate of an LC neuron (in monkey) recorded simultaneously with the pupillary responses during a signal detection task. (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005) We’ll use pupil response to determine when LC is in phasic, ‘focused’ attention mode. -->and look for differences between groups Missing data is replaced with other eye’s data or interpolation. Data are averaged across eyes. Data are smoothed. Baseline is set at 500 ms before onset. Significant differences are highlighted. Analysis is based on Sirois’ Matlab scripts. The ASD group has greater average dilation during search. 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter TYP conjunction ASD conjunction 0 2000 4000 6000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter 0 2000 4000 6000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter TYP conj09 (all trials) ASD conj09 (all trials) 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter TYP conj05 (all trials) ASD conj05 (all trials) The LC hypothesis is supported by recent findings demonstrating that the NE reuptake inhibitor venlafaxine suppresses LC neuronal activity... ...and is also an effective treatment for attention- impairment symptoms associated with autism. This explains the ASD advantage without invoking perceptual enhancement. Is the LC implicated in ASD? Pupillometry reveals that the ASD group is in a ‘focused attention’ mode more often. While the Typical group is in a diffuse, ‘exploratory’ attention mode more often. Previously, we showed that 2.5-year-olds with ASD were more successful than age-matched controls at conjunction search. Here we used pupillometry to gain insight into attentional ‘mode’ 200 –200 400 600 800 1,000 1,200 1,400 1,600 1,800 2,000 ms –0.02 0.02 0.04 0.06 0.08 0.10 0.12 0.14 0.16 0.18 mm Congruent Incongruent Non-color words Laeng et al. (2011) Task-evoked pupillary responses Measure of the “intensive aspect” or “load on attentional capacity” This attentional mode and the pupil response is controlled by the Locus Coeruleus. 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter TYP conj09 (successes) ASD conj09 (successes) 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 0.1 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 Time (ms) Mean pupil diameter (mm) Baselinecorrected pupil diameter TYP conj05 (successes) ASD conj05 (successes) Trials had a fixed length and our dependent variable was ‘% success’ But why are toddlers with ASD better at visual search? We can use pupillometry as a window into attentional ‘mode’. Stroop task The ASD group does not try harder, they just try more often According to our eye movement data, the ASD group doesn’t search faster, longer, or more dedicatedly than age-matched controls. such changes are typically 0.1-0.5mm Beatty & Lucero-Wagoner, 2000 Kaldy, Kraper, Carter, & Blaser, 2011 Kahneman, 1973; Laeng, et al., 2012 Wilhelm, et al., 1999 ...is the ASD group devoting more attention to the task? Or just devoting attention more often? This implies greater ‘phasic’ LC mode and greater focused attention. We see the same trend when we look at the different set size conditions, but... Jackson, I., & Sirois, S., 2009 We can tease these possibilities apart by just looking at successful trials as set size increases. If the ASD group always just devotes more attention than the Typical group, we should not see a set size effect, just ASD>Typical across set sizes. But if the ASD and Typical groups are capable of devoting the same amount of attention (but the ASD group just does so more often) then the two groups should asymptote together. The ASD and Typical groups are already indistinguishable by set size 9. Successes in high-set size conditions rarely happen by chance; only through focused attention. Looking at just successful trials as set size increases allows us to isolate ‘best-case‘ focused attention (LC phasic) mode. Will it look the same in ASD and Typical groups? Phasic, focused attention mode is the same in both groups; the ASD group is just in this mode more often. 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 TYP ASD successes ** n.s. n.s. Dilation at end of search interval as a function of set size 0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 TYP ASD Dilation at end of search interval as a function of set size The Locus Coeruleus modulates phasic, ‘focused’ vs. tonic, ‘exploratory’ attention. (Indeed, when ‘best case’ trials are isolated, pupil responses look identical between groups) Aston-Jones, et al., 2007 Hollander, E., et al., 2000 Beique, J., , et al.,2000 Autism is thought to resemble a persistent, highly focused attentive state, with LC neurons in a persistent ‘hyperphasic’ mode. Febrile episodes normalize LC activity and mitigate ASD symptoms. Mehler & Purpura, 2009 The Locus Coeruleus and ASD References Developmental and Brain Sciences, Psychology. [email protected] Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. (2005). Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403–50. Aston-Jones, G., Iba, M., Clayton, E., Rajkowski, J., & Cohen, J. (2007). In: G.A. Ordway, M.A. Schwartz, A. Frazer (Eds.), Brain norepinephrine: Neurobiology and therapeutics, Hillsdale, NJ: Cambridge University Press. Beatty, J. & Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). In: J. Caccioppo, L.G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), The Handbook of Psychophysiology, Hillsdale, NJ: Cambridge University Press. Beique, J., de Montigny, C., Blier, P. & Debonnel, G. (2000). Neuropharmacology, 39, 1800–12. Hollander, E., Kaplan A., Cartwright, C., & Reichman, D. (2000). Journal of Child Neurology. 15, 132-135. Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Developmental Science, 12, 670–9. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall. Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A.S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Developmental Science, 14, 980–988. Laeng, B., Ørbo, M., Holmlund, T., & Miozzo, M. (2011). Pupillary Stroop effects. Cognitive Processing, 12, 13–21. Laeng, B., Sirois, S., & Gredeback, G. (2012). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 18–27. Mehler, M. F., & Purpura, D. P. (2009). Brain Research Review. 59, 38892. Wilhelm, B., Wilhelm, H., & Lűdtke, H. (1999). In J. Kuhlmann & M. Bottcher (Eds.), Pupillography: Principles, methods and applications. Műnchen, Germany: Zuckschwerdt Verlag.

Upload: others

Post on 24-Apr-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Toddlers with ASD are better at visual search without trying harder: a pupillometric study

MotivationWe found that toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder are better at visual search than age-matched controls.

The cause is mysterious: enhanced perception? faster search? more attentional resources? Greater ‘focus’?

GoalUse pupillometry to determine the attentional ‘mode’ of toddlers during visual search,

to test the idea that toddlers with ASD do not search better than Typical toddlers, just more often.

Erik Blaser, Luke Eglington, & Zsuzsa Kaldy, University of Massachusetts Boston

Pupillometry of toddlersToddlers with ASD are better at visual search

Visual Search without Instructions:

An Eye-Tracking Study with Toddlers

Zsuzsa Kaldy, Ph.D., Erik Blaser, Ph.D., Catherine Kraper, Emily Higgins, & Alice S. Carter, Ph.D.

University of Massachusetts Boston

Discussion

o As expected, 1-2.5-year-olds showed classicfeature and conjunction search results.o We used an entirely non-verbal paradigmwith no training, that is easy to use withyoung non-verbal (e.g. clinical) populations.

How do we know that toddlers weresearching for the target?

Our data shows that toddlers were treating thetarget differently from the distractors. Fixationlength was, significantly higher for the target thanfor distractors. Furthermore, in the single featuretrials, the target was fixated significantly fasterthan average distractors.

What are the advantages of a no-instructionparadigm?

There is considerable variability in the verbalabilities of young toddlers. By using a no-instruction paradigm, we can eliminate some ofthe variance that is due to differences incomprehension skills. Another advantage of ourparadigm is that it can be easily used to studyvery young atypical populations that are partiallyor totally non-verbal (e.g. toddlers with AutismSpectrum Disorder, Down Syndrome).

ReferencesAdler, S.A., & Orprecio, J. (2006). The eyes have it: Visual pop-out ininfants and adults. Developmental Science, 9, 189-206.

Atkinson, J., & Braddick, O. (1992). Visual segmentation of oriented texturesby infants. Behavioural Brain Research, 49, 123-131.

Gerhardstein, P., & Rovee-Collier, C. (2002). The development of visualsearch in infants and very young children. Journal of Experimental ChildPsychology, 81, 194-215.

Rovee-Collier, C., Hankins, E., & Bhatt, R. (1992). Textons, visual pop-outeffects, and object recognition in infancy. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology, 121, 435-445.

Sireteanu, R., & Rieth, C. (1992). Texture segregation in infants and children.Behavioural Brain Research, 49, 133-139.

Treisman, A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory ofattention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136.

Results

Familiarization trials: Nosignificant differences in eithertime to first fixation (TFF) orfixation length (FL) among thethree items (all p’s > 0.158).

Search trials: We replicated thecharacteristic RT pattern found inadult studies: the number ofdistractors did not affect time-to-target in single feature trials, butin feature conjunction trials time-to-target linearly increased withthe number of distractors (Fig. 2).

In general, toddlers spent longerfixating on the target than theaverage distractor (average targetFL: 937 ms; average distractorFL: 627 ms. t(278) = 7.417, p <0.001). Overall, time-to-first-fixation was also lower for thetarget than for the averagedistractor (target TFF: 1443 ms;average distractor TFF: 1680 ms.t(277) = 3.319, p < 0.001) (Fig. 3and 4.)

Methods

Participants: 30 healthy toddlers, age: 14-31 months(mean: 26.5 ± 6.0 months).

Materials: We used a Tobii T60 eye tracking systemto display stimuli and to record eye movements.

Goal

To study visual attention in young toddlers and verbally-compromisedpopulations (e. g. toddlers with ASD, Down Syndrome) by applying a visualsearch paradigm that does not require verbal instructions.

% of trials single, 4 single, 8 conj., 4 conj., 8 conj., 12

only target, no distractors 1 3 7 1 0

only distractors, no target 4 9 25 44 65

missed both 2 3 16 11 8

Stimuli: One target (a red apple), among ‘color’ (blue apples), and ‘shape’distractors (red, elongated, rectangular apples) (Fig. 1).

Procedure: Participants sat approx. 40 cm from the screen. After calibration,four familiarization trials were run, where the three object types (one each)were shown simultaneously on the screen in different configurations.Participants saw one or two mixed blocks of single-feature trials (distractors:either color or shape; number: 4 or 8) and feature conjunction trials (equalnumber of color and shape distractors; number: 4, 8, or 12). Trial lengthwas 4 sec. At the end of each trial, the target rotated in place for 2seconds accompanied by a clapping sound (providing minimal feedback).Between trials, a target item zoomed in from the top part of the screen,again accompanied by sound effects, and then stood at a central fixationpoint for 2 sec.

Figure 1. ‘Heat map’ showing typical

fixation density for a feature

conjunction trial.

Figure 2. Effect of set size on time-to-

first-fixation in single feature and feature

conjunction search.

Figure 3. Average fixation lengthfor target and distractors in singlefeature search, conjunction search,and averaged across all searches.

Unsuccessful search trials: The percent oftrials where either the target and/or thedistractors did not get any looks are asfollows:

Figure 4. Average time-to-first-fixation for target and distractorsby single feature trials.

There is a classic distinctionbetween ‘feature search’ tasks,where a target item is distin-guished from distractors by aunique feature value, and ‘conjunc-tion search’ tasks, in which thetarget can be distinguished onlyby a conjunction of features. ‘Set-size’ increases have little or noeffect on search time in featuresearch (the target ‘pops-out’,Treisman & Gelade, 1980), butresults in linear increases forconjunction search.

Visual Search

Apparatus

Tobii T120

Pupillometry & Locus Coeruleus

Conclusions

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 2R15EY017985 awarded to the authors

apple target flies in, dissolves (~3 s)

search display appears (4 s)

Eye tracking {

‘reward’ animation (~3 s)0!

10!

20!

30!

40!

50!

60!

70!

80!

90!

100!

4! 5! 6! 7! 8! 9! 10! 11! 12! 13! 14!

% S

ucce

ss a

t Fin

ding

the

Targ

et!

Set Size!

ASD, conjunction!TYP, conjunction!ASD, single feature!TYP, single feature!

Each block consisted of 4 familiarization trials and 13 test trials and lasted for approximately 4 minutes. The test trials consisted of 4 single feature trials (set size 5 or 9) and 9 feature conjunction trials (set size 5, 9 or 13).

Phasic mode: focused attention on task-relevant stimuli (associated with better

performance, e.g. on search tasks). -->determines task-evoked pupil response

Tonic mode: diffuse attention marked by broad sampling of stimuli in the environment.

-->determines baseline pupil level

Aston-Jones, et al., 2007

Pupil changes due to mental activity are due to activity of the LC

AR245-NE28-16 ARI 19 May 2005 12:10

that the LC-NE system serves to adjudicatethese trade-offs and thereby contributes tothe optimization of behavioral performance.This proposal contrasts with traditional viewsof the LC-NE system.

Neuromodulatory Systems and theRegulation of Behavior: A HistoricalPerspectiveThe LC-NE system is one of several brain-stem neuromodulatory nuclei with widelydistributed, ascending projections to theneocortex (see Figure 1); others include thedopaminergic, serotonergic, and cholinergicsystems. These neurons play critical roles inregulating cortical function, and disturbancesin these systems are central to major psychi-atric disorders, such as schizophrenia, de-pression, and bipolar disorder. Traditionallyinvestigators have assumed that these neu-rotransmitters serve relatively simple andbasic functions. For example, many havethought that dopamine (DA) release signalsreward or motivation, and NE mediatesarousal (Berridge & Waterhouse 2003, Jouvet1969, Robinson & Berridge 1993, Wise &Rompre 1989). Such functions seemed toaccord well with the characteristic anatomy ofthese systems (widely distributed projectionsthroughout the forebrain), and it is easy tounderstand how disturbances in such basicand pervasive functions would have profounddisruptive effects on cognition, emotion,and behavior such as those associated withpsychiatric disorders. Furthermore, althoughNE, DA, serotonin, and acetylcholine aresometimes referred to as “classical neuro-transmitters” (presumably because of theirearly discovery and their effects in peripheralsystems), an equally and perhaps more im-portant function of these substances in cortexis neuromodulation. That is, rather than pro-ducing direct excitatory or inhibitory effectson postsynaptic neurons, they modulate sucheffects produced by other neurotransmitterssuch as glutamate and gamma amino butyricacid (GABA). Neuromodulatory actions,

Figure 1Illustration of projections of the LC system. Saggital view of a monkeybrain showing LC neurons located in the pons with efferent projectionsthroughout the central nervous system. Note that only few areas do notreceive LC innervation (e.g., hypothalamus and caudate-putamen).

especially when they are distributed overa wide area, seemed well suited to basic,pervasive functions such as the signaling ofreward and the mediation of arousal.

Whereas functions such as reward andarousal have intuitive appeal, they have oftenescaped precise characterization, at both theneural and the computational levels. Recently,however, this has begun to change. For exam-ple, considerable progress has been made indeveloping a formal theory of the role of DAin reinforcement learning. According to thistheory, DA does not signal reward per se butrather mediates a learning signal that allowsthe system to predict better when rewardsare likely to occur and thereby contribute tothe optimization of reward-seeking behaviors(Montague et al. 1996, 2004). This representsa significant refinement in understanding ofthe relationship of DA to reward and therole that this neuromodulatory system playsin the regulation of cognition and behavior.In this review, we propose a theory that offersa similar refinement to our understanding ofLC-NE function and its relationship toarousal, and how this in turn relates to theoptimization of reward-seeking behaviors.

Arousal reflects a fundamental propertyof behavior that has proven difficult to define

www.annualreviews.org • LC and Optimization 405

Ann

u. R

ev. N

euro

sci.

2005

.28:

403-

450.

Dow

nloa

ded

from

ww

w.a

nnua

lrevi

ews.o

rgby

Bro

wn

Uni

vers

ity o

n 04

/23/

12. F

or p

erso

nal u

se o

nly.

Firing rate of an LC neuron (in monkey) recorded simultaneously with the pupillary responses during a signal detection task.(Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005)

We’ll use pupil response to determine when LC is in phasic, ‘focused’ attention mode.

-->and look for differences between groups

Missing data is replaced with other eye’s data or interpolation.

Data are averaged across eyes.Data are smoothed.

Baseline is set at 500 ms before onset.Significant differences are highlighted.

Analysis is based on Sirois’ Matlab scripts.

The ASD group has greater average dilation during search.

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conjunctionASD conjunction

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conj13 (successes)ASD conj13 (successes)

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conj13 (all trials)ASD conj13 (all trials)

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conj09 (all trials)ASD conj09 (all trials)

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conj05 (all trials)ASD conj05 (all trials)

The LC hypothesis is supported by recent findings demonstrating that the

NE reuptake inhibitor venlafaxine suppresses LC neuronal activity...

...and is also an effective treatment for attention-impairment symptoms associated with autism.

This explains the ASD advantage without invoking perceptual enhancement.

Is the LC implicated in ASD?

Pupillometry reveals that the ASD group is in a ‘focused

attention’ mode more often.

While the Typical group is in a diffuse, ‘exploratory’ attention mode more often.

Previously, we showed that 2.5-year-olds with ASD were more successful than age-matched controls at conjunction search.

Here we used pupillometry to gain insight into attentional ‘mode’

Pupillometry 19

1969; Karatekin, Couperous, & Marcus, 2004; Nuthmann & van der Meer, 2005; Piquado, Isaacowitz, & Wingfield, 2010; Stanners, Coulter, Sweet, & Murphy, 1979; Van Gerven, Paas, Van Merriënboer, & Schmidt, 2004; Võ et al., 2008) and inter-ference or competition between stimuli and/or responses (e.g., Laeng, Ørbo, Holmlund, & Miozzo, 2011; Moresi et al., 2008; Siegle, Ichikawa, & Steinhauer, 2008; Van der Meer et al., 2003). Figure 1, for example, illustrates how the pupillary response is clearly sensitive to the congruency effect of the classic Stroop color-naming task.

However, recent evidence from pupillometry studies within psychology and neuroscience indicates that the pupillary response might offer a wider window on cognition than previ-ously thought. Specifically, it may provide an index to pro-cesses that occur below the threshold of consciousness.

Through the Eye to the BrainPupillary measurements

The size of the pupil of the eye is determined by the tone of two muscles, the dilator and the constrictor; thus, a pupillary dilation can be the result of a stimulation of the dilator or an inhibition of the constrictor. In dim light or darkness, the pupil can enlarge to an average size of about 7 mm with a standard deviation (from this average) of about 0.9 mm (MacLachlan & Howland, 2002); in standard light conditions, its average size is about 3 mm (Wyatt, 1995). Thus, changes in illumination can provoke pupillary dilations of more than double its typical size (about 120%). Changes that are cognitively driven are

more modest and are rarely greater than 0.5 mm (Beatty & Lucero-Wagoner, 2000). Thus, the pupillary response to sex-ual stimuli, as originally measured by Hess and Polt (1960), approximated the maximal dilation (a 20% change) that can be elicited by psychologically relevant stimuli that are invariant in luminance.

Pupillary responses occur spontaneously and they are dif-ficult to control voluntarily. Specifically, a pupillary dilation may be voluntarily provoked only in an indirect manner by mentally imaging an object or event that would normally evoke a pupillary dilation (e.g., sexual imagery; Whipple, Ogden, & Komisaruk, 1992). However, it is impossible to sup-press a pupillary dilation at will, whether it is provoked by external stimuli or mental events (Loewenfeld, 1993). Pupil-lary dilations evoked by psychologically relevant stimuli occur as the result of a neural inhibitory mechanism on the parasympathetic oculomotor complex or Edinger–Westphal nucleus by the noradrenergic system’s locus coeruleus (LC; Wilhelm, Wilhelm, & L!dtke, 1999).

Norepinephrine and the LCThe LC is a subcortical brain structure that constitutes the nor-adrenergic system’s hub to the whole brain (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005; Sara, 2009). The LC is found on each side of the rostral pons in the brainstem and gives rise to the sole source of the neuro-transmitter norepinephrine (NE) to the cortex, cerebellum, and hippocampus. The LC may be most known among clinical psychologists for its role in syndromes like clinical depression, panic disorder, and anxiety (e.g., Carter

200–200

400 600 800 1,000 1,200 1,400 1,600 1,800 2,000ms

–0.02

0.02

0.04

0.06

0.08

0.10

0.12

0.14

0.16

0.18

mm

CongruentIncongruentNon-color words

Fig. 1. Mean pupil diameters (in mm) for each distractor condition during the classic color-word Stroop interference task (from Laeng et al., 2011). Time 0 represents the onset of each stimulus, and pupil size was sampled every 20 ms. The colored vertical lines represent the point in time of each condition’s mean reaction time. Pupillary responses clearly lag behind each explicit response (a key press indicating the color of the word), but they showed the same pattern of results across conditions (i.e., larger responses for incongruent combinations of pixel colors and color words than for the congruent combinations).

at BROWN UNIVERSITY on March 20, 2012pps.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Laeng et al. (2011)

Task-evoked pupillary responses

Measure of the “intensive aspect” or “load on attentional capacity”

This attentional mode and the pupil response is controlled

by the Locus Coeruleus.

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conj09 (successes)ASD conj09 (successes)

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000−0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Time (ms)

Mea

n pu

pil d

iam

eter

(mm

)

Baseline−corrected pupil diameter

TYP conj05 (successes)ASD conj05 (successes)

Trials had a fixed length and our dependent

variable was ‘% success’

But why are toddlers with ASD better at visual search?We can use pupillometry as a window into attentional ‘mode’.

Stroop task

The ASD group does not try harder, they just try more often

According to our eye movement data, the ASD group doesn’t search faster, longer, or more dedicatedly

than age-matched controls.

such changes are typically 0.1-0.5mmBeatty & Lucero-Wagoner, 2000

Kaldy, Kraper, Carter, & Blaser, 2011

Kahneman, 1973; Laeng, et al., 2012

Wilhelm, et al., 1999

...is the ASD group devoting more attention to the task? Or just devoting attention more often?

This implies greater ‘phasic’ LC mode and greater focused attention.

We see the same trend when we look at the different set size conditions, but...

Jackson, I., & Sirois, S., 2009

We can tease these possibilities apart by just looking at successful trials as set size increases.

If the ASD group always just devotes more attention than the Typical group, we should not see a set size effect, just

ASD>Typical across set sizes.

But if the ASD and Typical groups are capable of devoting the same amount of attention (but the ASD group just does so more often) then the two groups should asymptote together.

The ASD and Typical groups are already indistinguishable by set size 9.

Successes in high-set size conditions rarely happen by chance; only through focused attent ion. Looking at just successful trials as set size increases allows us to isolate ‘best-case‘ focused attention (LC phasic) mode. Will it look the same in ASD and Typical groups?

Phasic, focused attention mode is the same in both groups; the ASD group is just in this mode more often.

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

3 5 7 9 11 13 15

TYP ASD

successes

**

n.s.n.s.

Dilation at end of search interval as a function of set size

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

3 5 7 9 11 13 15

TYP ASD

Dilation at end of search interval as a function of set size

The Locus Coeruleus modulates phasic, ‘focused’ vs. tonic, ‘exploratory’ attention.

(Indeed, when ‘best case’ trials are isolated, pupil responses look identical between groups)

Aston-Jones, et al., 2007

Hollander, E., et al., 2000Beique, J., , et al.,2000

Autism is thought to resemble a persistent, highly focused attentive state, with LC neurons in a persistent ‘hyperphasic’ mode.

Febrile episodes normalize LC activity and mitigate ASD symptoms.Mehler & Purpura, 2009

The Locus Coeruleus and ASD

References

Developmental and Brain Sciences, Psychology. [email protected]

Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. (2005). Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403–50.

Aston-Jones, G., Iba, M., Clayton, E., Rajkowski, J., & Cohen, J. (2007). In: G.A. Ordway, M.A. Schwartz, A. Frazer (Eds.), Brain norepinephrine: Neurobiology and therapeutics, Hillsdale, NJ: Cambridge University Press.

Beatty, J. & Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). In: J. Caccioppo, L.G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), The Handbook of Psychophysiology, Hillsdale, NJ: Cambridge University Press.

Beique, J., de Montigny, C., Blier, P. & Debonnel, G. (2000). Neuropharmacology, 39, 1800–12.

Hollander, E., Kaplan A., Cartwright, C., & Reichman, D. (2000). Journal of Child Neurology. 15, 132-135.

Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Developmental Science, 12, 670–9.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall.

Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A.S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Developmental Science, 14, 980–988.Laeng, B., Ørbo, M., Holmlund, T., & Miozzo, M. (2011). Pupillary Stroop effects. Cognitive Processing, 12, 13–21.

Laeng, B., Sirois, S., & Gredeback, G. (2012). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 18–27.

Mehler, M. F., & Purpura, D. P. (2009). Brain Research Review. 59, 388–92.

Wilhelm, B., Wilhelm, H., & Lűdtke, H. (1999). In J. Kuhlmann & M. Bottcher (Eds.), Pupillography: Principles, methods and applications. Műnchen, Germany: Zuckschwerdt Verlag.