toward an understanding of developmental …...while dcd and sdd-mf have a relatively formal status...

21
11 The Second G. Lawrence Rarick Commemorative Lecture 2001 Presented at the 13 th International Symposium for Adapted Physical Activity July 3–7, 2001, Vienna, Austria Toward an Understanding of Developmental Coordination Disorder by Sheila E. Henderson and Leslie Henderson

Upload: others

Post on 09-Jul-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 11

11

The SecondG. Lawrence Rarick

Commemorative Lecture 2001

Presented at the 13th International Symposium for Adapted Physical Activity

July 3–7, 2001, Vienna, Austria

Toward an Understandingof Developmental Coordination Disorder

bySheila E. Henderson and Leslie Henderson

Page 2: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson12

12

Sheila E. Henderson is with the School of Psychology at the Institute of Education,University of London, UK. Leslie Henderson is with the Division of Neuroscience andPsychological Medicine at Imperial College of Science, Technology and Medicine, Lon-don, UK. E-mail: <[email protected]>.

ADAPTED PHYSICAL ACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 2002, 19, 12-31© 2002 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

Toward an Understanding of DevelopmentalCoordination Disorder

Sheila E. HendersonUniversity of London

Leslie HendersonImperial College of Science Technology and Medicine, London

We consider three issues concerning unexpected difficulty in the acquisitionof motor skills: terminology, diagnosis, and intervention. Our preference forthe label Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) receives justification.Problems in diagnosis are discussed, especially in relation to the aetiology-dominated medical model. The high degree of overlap between DCD and otherchildhood disorders appears to militate against its acceptance as a distinctsyndrome. In this context, we emphasize the need to determine whether inco-ordination takes different forms when it occurs alone is combined with gen-eral developmental delay or with other specific disorders in children of nor-mal intelligence. Studies of intervention have mostly shown positive effectsbut do not, as yet, allow adjudication between different sorts of content. Wesuggest that the study of DCD and its remediation would benefit greatly fromthe employment of the simple but rich paradigms developed for the experi-mental analysis of fully formed adult movement skills.

Prefatory Note

The form and style of this paper were determined by its initial presentation as aninvited lecture, delivered by SEH to an audience with a wide range of interests, fewof whom spoke Scottish. In responding to the Editor’s request for a version to bepublished in APAQ, we have tried to preserve the informality of the spoken versionwhile adding a few references and some technical material where we judged itappropriate. The paper states a personal view rather than a detailed and schol-arly argument. In two respects, the written version is impoverished. The lecturewas accompanied by videos that illustrate the difficulties of children with a devel-opmental disorder of coordinated movement more vividly than any marks on a

Page 3: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 13

page can do. Thanks to our Viennese hosts, the paper was presented as a rococcosandwich, between two delectable pieces of Mozart, played by the Opus 4 stringquartet.

It has been recognized for a long time that severe and persistent difficultiesin spoken language, reading and writing, attention, and motor coordinationnot infrequently occur in children of normal general intelligence withoutany obvious neurological disorder, who come from an unremarkable familybackground, and who have apparently adequate schooling and other educa-tional experiences. (Rutter, 1998, p. ix)

In this paper, our concern is with unaccountable failure to acquire adequatemotor skills. Motor skills are not generally as dependent on formal tuition as, say,mathematics or reading. Many of the actions we perform in every day life havebeen learned informally and can be performed without close attention. Indeed,under normal circumstances, we only become aware of the intricacy of our move-ments when suddenly deprived of our skill, as in the attempt to fasten buttons withicy fingers or the coordination of actions of hands and feet when learning to drive.A similar reminder of the movement skills we take for granted is furnished by thechild who cannot acquire even the simplest motor skill without help. Being unableto fasten buttons, use a knife and fork, or ride a bike to school may seem trivial insome regards, but such failures can have far reaching consequences on the educa-tional progress of children, their social relationships, and self-esteem.

Figure 1 shows some examples of deficient graphic skills in children abovethe age of 9 with normal IQ, who speak fluently and clearly and read well above

Figure 1 — Examples of defective graphic skills drawn from three children with gen-eral movement problems, all over 8 years of age, with IQ above normal.

Page 4: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson14

their age levels. These children come from supportive home backgrounds and goto caring schools. Their parents and teachers are concerned about how these diffi-culties might affect the children’s progress in school, but pediatricians can offerneither an explanation of nor a solution to their problems. Of course, display of theproduct of the child’s activity is no substitute for watching on video the child’sstruggle to generate that product.

Although awareness of children with difficulties of this sort has greatly in-creased over the last quarter century, there is still much to be learned. While a fullycomprehensive review of the area might have started with the question of aetiol-ogy and concluded with an examination of the growing experimental literature onthe possible deficits underlying the condition, in the space available, we have electedto focus on three issues that continue to generate debate among theoreticians andpractitioners: terminology, diagnosis, and intervention. In their different ways, wehope that all three sections will contribute to an interest in and understanding ofthis condition.

Part I: Terminology

Debate about the appropriate label for a disorder might seem to be of purely “aca-demic” interest. However, the choice of terms has important implications, boththeoretically and practically. In research, for example, terminological ambiguitiescreate difficulties in the definition and comparison of samples. In the realm of thepractical, a condition defined by society in one way may confer special entitle-ment to benefits and services, while the same condition defined in another maynot. Labels, moreover, tend to be adhesive and may be difficult to shed, even whenthe child has changed for the better. Such issues have been addressed in an admi-rably succinct series of editorials in the journal Developmental Medicine and ChildNeurology (e.g., Bax, 1999; Davies, 1994; Gardner-Medwin, 1995; Hart, 1999;MacKeith, 1968).

It is not surprising to find, therefore, that recent reviews of the labeling ofthe disorder of concern to us here have concluded that terminology continues topresent a problem (Henderson & Barnett, 1998; Polatajko, 1998). To some extent,the variations in usage are systematic, differing characteristically from country tocountry, from profession to profession, and even according to the theoretical biasof the individual. Surveys by Missiuna and Polatajko (1995), Miyahara and Regis-ter (2000), and Peters, Barnett, and Henderson (2001) have shown that some pro-fessionals view the competing labels as interchangeable, while others choosedifferent labels for slightly different groups of children. In Sweden, these childrenare nearly always assigned the label Disorder of Attention and Motor Performance(DAMP; e.g., Gillberg, 1992; Gillberg & Gillberg, 1989; Gillberg, Gillberg, &Groth, 1989). Transported to Italy, they become Dyspraxic (Zoia, 2000), whereasin Holland most now have DCD (Geuze, Jongmans, Schoemaker, & Smits-Engleman, 2001). Many Australian practitioners, indifferent to fashion, still usethe term Minimal Neurological Dysfunction (Larkin, personal communication).In New Zealand, Developmental Dyspraxia is more common (Miyahara & Regis-ter, 2000). In Canada, 6 years after the ironically named “London (Ontario) Con-sensus Statement” (Polatajko, Fox & Missiuna, 1995), the term physicalawkwardness remains popular in some circles. In the USA, too, different profes-sions employ labels of their own, but for parents seeking private services, insurance

Page 5: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 15

companies recognize DCD (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor-ders IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), opening their coffers only whenthe DSM IV diagnostic criteria are fully met. The situation in the UK is unre-solved, with some therapists and teachers devoted to the term dyspraxic and othersbeginning to use the term DCD (Peters et al., 2001). However, our National HealthService (NHS, 1999) has recently decreed that all hospital recording systems willemploy the World Health Organization classification scheme, the result being thatsuch children shall henceforth bear the label, Specific Developmental Disorder ofMotor Function (SDD-MF; ICD 10, World Health Organization, 1992).

DCD and Its Rivals – A Component Analysis

At an important interdisciplinary meeting held in London, Ontario, now known asthe Consensus Forum, researchers and practitioners agreed on the recommenda-tion that the term DCD be universally adopted (Polatajko et al., 1995). In whatfollows, we come to the same conclusion via a detailed comparison of the compo-nents of the DCD label with those of its principal rivals, SDD-MF and (Develop-mental) Dyspraxia. While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status dueto their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen eitherby misguided analogy with certain neurological acquired disorders of action or foressentially political reasons because of its medical resonance. Neither the APA norWHO attempt to justify their choice of term. Instead, both organizations assert,somewhat gnomically, that (a) the classification scheme adopted and the terminol-ogy used is not based on theory but on clinical expertise, (b) terms derived fromcontroversial theories have been avoided, and (c) the current state of knowledge ofchildhood disorders rules out reference to aetiology or pathophysiology. If onlythis agreement on strategy had yielded an agreed label.

Why not Dyspraxia? Let us dispose of this possibility without further ado.The over-riding reasons for declining to adopt the term dyspraxia to encompassmovement disorders in children are (a) longstanding confusion about the meaningof the term apraxia in the literature on acquired disorders and (b) the questionablerelevance of this literature to the developmental disorder.

In adult neurology, it is not difficult to find patients who when asked, “showme how you wave goodbye” are unable to do so but then do so spontaneously andcompetently as they depart. In the past, the prevailing uncertainty about the natureof this disorder was evident from an unhealthy proliferation of labels, such asideational, ideomotor, and constructional apraxia, or dressing, gaze, and limbapraxia, etc. Perhaps in reaction to this, the literature on the apraxias has recentlytended to become much more empirical and locationalist, attempting to tie de-tailed neuropsychological description of impaired function to the patho-anatomy(e.g., Square, Roy, & Martin, 1997) . These objectives, however worthy, are un-likely to commend the program to those primarily concerned with developmentalmatters. Moreover, attempts to define apraxia continue to generate confusion, rang-ing from the overinclusive (e.g., Rothi & Heilman, 1997, p. 3), “a neurologicaldisorder of learned purposive movement skill that is not explained by deficits ofelemental [sic] motor or sensory systems,” to excessively narrow formulationsthat assert, for example, that apraxia is a disorder of gesture.

It does not appear that apraxic difficulties, whatever their nature, involve adisturbance in the coordination of movement (but see Poizner, Merians, Clark,Rothi & Heilman, 1997). Instead, it is widely agreed that the disorder is located at

Page 6: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson16

a higher level in the hierarchy of action control in which the planning or retrievalof action plans has become unduly context-dependent (the patient can only per-form the act of waving good-bye spontaneously in “natural” circumstances).

Those who continue to advocate adoption of the label developmental dys-praxia as an umbrella term, despite its evident inappropriateness for the vast ma-jority of developmental cases, may, as with parent groups in the UK, be motivatedby political rather than scientific considerations. Others may have been seducedby the medical resonance of the term praxis into believing that neurological stud-ies of the acquired disorder, however different, will somehow nurture understand-ing of the developmental condition (e.g., Ayres, Mailloux, & Wendler, 1987;Denckla, 1984; Dewey, 1995). Finally, a complaint that might seem pedantic isthat the distinction between the prefix dys- meaning a disturbance of and the pre-fix a- signifying absence of has somehow become entangled with the distinctionbetween developmental and acquired disorders.

Until recently, the labels used by the APA and WHO for the entire set ofdevelopmental disorders included the term specific. However, DSM IV (1994)departs from this usage, abandoning the whole notion of specificity. Readers ofAPAQ will be familiar with the broader issue, in which learning difficulties areheld to be specific only if the child’s achievements fall below what might havebeen expected on the basis of his or her general aptitude. The underlying assump-tion is that specific disorders differ in nature from more global, developmentalretardation. Over the last decade, this idea has been refuted in many domains,particularly language impairment and dyslexia (see Bishop, 1998; Dowdney &Scott, 1998; Rispens, Van Yperen, & Yule, 1998 for penetrating reviews). How-ever, it remains almost untested in the motor domain.

During his lifetime, Larry Rarick provided us with one of the most detaileddescriptions of the motor problems experienced by children then described asmentally retarded (e.g., Rarick, Dobbins, & Broadhead, 1976). Others among ushave attempted to do the same for children of normal IQ with movement prob-lems. Unfortunately, a quarter of a century after Rarick’s seminal work, no directcomparison between these two groups has yet been published, so it remains to bedetermined whether, and if so how, the nature of their motor deficiencies differs.

So, how are we to proceed? Pragmatism suggests that from a research stand-point, we should continue to employ a discrepancy criterion to select participants,if only to avoid dipping deeply into the murky etiological soup of profound retar-dation. However, we need to be clear that this is not a principled adoption of thespecificity notion nor an endorsement of IQ as a divine basis for the calculation ofdiscrepancies (see also Geuze et al., 2001). We are learning from other domains,such as reading, that discrepancy probably has little role to play in deciding whoshould receive intervention and what the nature of that intervention should be (see,for example, Stanovitch, 1998). As Bishop has observed of the language domain,“there seems little justification for continuing to place heavy reliance on IQ -language discrepancies in determining who should receive extra help at school”(1998, p. 146).

Both manuals (DSM-IV & ICD-10) include the term developmental. In ad-dition to reminding us of the longitudinal perspective, this term encompasses “. . .not only the roots of behaviour in prior maturation, in physical influences (bothinternal and external), and in the residues of earlier experiences, but also themodulations of that behaviour by the circumstances of the present” (Rutter,1980, p. 1).

Page 7: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 17

It is now well established that the notion that most children with movementdifficulties will spontaneously out grow them is ill-founded (e.g., Cantell, Smyth,& Ahonen, 1994; Geuze & Borger, 1993; Henderson, 1993; Losse et al., 1991).Without intervention, the educational, social, and psychiatric consequences can besubstantial (e.g. Hellgren, Gillberg, & Gillberg, 1994; Hellgren, Gillberg,Bagenholm, & Gillberg, 1994; Hellgren, Gillberg, Gillberg, & Enerskog, 1993;Skinner & Piek, 2001). However, we cannot predict with any degree of accuracyregarding which child will grow out of the problem or which will become seri-ously depressed. Only further research will help us disentangle the complex interac-tions between organic and environmental factors that might affect the course of DCD.

If one crucial aspect of the term developmental is reference to the interplayof biological with personal and social factors in the child’s progressive mastery ofits environment, another must surely be the contrast between developmental disor-ders, on one hand, with an implied failure of learning and acquired disorders, onthe other hand, where a once-learned skill has been traumatically lost. Among thechildren with whom we are concerned, the most basic of skills such as reaching,grasping, standing, and walking may be intact, but more complicated acts such ascutting with scissors, using a knife and fork, or catching a ball represent entirelynovel learning tasks that are often acquired with only the utmost difficulty.

A disorder of . . . what? We have already disposed of the idea of a disorder ofpraxis. Whereas DSM IV considers the answer to this question to be motor coordi-nation, ICD-10 speaks of a disorder of motor function. Function is elucidated bythe Oxford English Dictionary (OED) as “activity (or) action in general, whethermental or physical.” In the present context, therefore, the child who has a disorderof motor function might be deemed to be one who fails to perform motor actseffectively. A possible disadvantage of the term, however, is that it leaves wideopen the possibility that a child has poor motor function for reasons unrelated tohis/her ability to control the motor system. For example, some children with ADHDappear clumsy because they are inattentive rather than actually incapable of perform-ing the required actions. Physical weakness, consequent on a muscle-wasting disease,may also impair motor function without being a primary disorder of motor control.

In contrast to the term function, the term coordination seems to refer moredirectly to the heart of the problem while at the same time remaining fairly neutralas to detailed causality. Collins English Dictionary defines the verb, to coordinateas “to organize or integrate diverse elements in a harmonious operation” and thenoun, coordination as “balanced and effective interaction of movement.” The OEDentry, on the other hand, emphasizes the sequential nature of the movement ele-ments of which most actions are composed: “consisting of a number of actions orprocesses properly combined for one purpose.”

Within the scientific literature on motor control, definitions of coordinationvary considerably in their level of discourse. For Schmidt (1988), coordinationoccurs at a level involving individual joints and muscles. Thus, he defines coordi-nation as “behaviour of two or more joints and muscles in relation to each other toproduce skilled activity” (p. 265). Gallistel, in his monumental treatise The Orga-nization of Action (1980), takes a broader perspective (see especially Chapter 8),quoting Weiss (1941) on coordination at some length. Gallistel’s main preoccupa-tion in this work was to delineate the principal functional units that contribute tosustained purposive action. He discusses three types of unit (reflexes, oscillators,and servomechanisms) and considers how they may interact to produce a largervocabulary of actions. Then in a key passage, he argues, “These diverse modes of

Page 8: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson18

interaction do not, however, by themselves ensure the coherence of coordinatedaction.” He continues, “The coherence and purposefulness of sustained action de-pends upon a hierarchical structuring of the units. By means of this hierarchicalstructure, the highest levels of neural integration impose an overall direction onbehaviour” (p. 210). For Gallistel, coordination is a feature of skilled, fluent actionthat takes place at various levels in the hierarchy of action control. To producemovement of a limb, opposing muscles must be activated or inhibited. This paral-lel coordination must be modulated over time as in oscillator-driven movement,requiring coordination of temporal patterns. Such patterns must in turn be selectedor deselected by a higher level decision-making system, in the light of environ-mental data, and so on up the hierarchy. We can quite properly speak of coordina-tion at each hierarchical level. As we ascend the hierarchy, information from theenvironment becomes increasingly important so that not only do the componentsof the movement have to be coordinated, actions have to fit the coordinates of theenvironment. The grasping hand has to be shaped to achieve a fit with the arrivingball. At each new level, coordination will exhibit new properties of fluency, appro-priateness, and expertise.

In sum, four attractive features emerge from our examination of the termcoordination. First, the notion of intentionality is embraced. Second, our attentionis drawn to the spatial and temporal organization of actions. Third, we are re-minded that most actions involve the sequencing of movement elements. Fourth,we are also reminded that actions have to be appropriately calibrated to environ-mental coordinates. Given our implacable hostility to the association of the devel-opmental disorder with those acquired disorders referred to as apraxias, our approvalof the term coordination as a descriptor of the processing demands of movementcontrol at various hierarchical levels and the eschewal of the problematic notion ofspecificity, we have no alternative but to endorse the label “DCD.”

Part II: Diagnosis

In DSM IV, a disorder is conceptualized as a clinically significant behavioral orpsychological syndrome that is associated with impairment in one or more impor-tant areas of functioning. A syndrome may, in turn, be regarded as a set of co-occurring signs and symptoms indicative of a particular disease or disorder.

In medicine, diseases are diagnosed in terms of a pattern of abnormal signsand symptoms that are sometimes assigned different weights and not all of whichmay be present in a given case. In well-understood diseases, the aetiology is knownand this defines the disease and specifies any underlying pathophysiology. An ef-fective therapy stems from understanding the aetiology, while failure of this therapyconstitutes a prima facie reason for doubting the diagnosis.

Thus, in congenital hypothyroidism, for example, which is a genetic condi-tion resulting in a hormonal deficit, doctors have available to them a proactivescreening program, which leads to early diagnosis and in turn a rapidly institutedtreatment. If instigated early enough, the catastrophic effects on cognitive and onmotor development are averted (see Figure 2).

I mention the admirably clear medical disease model precisely because ittells us so little about the diagnosis of DCD. In this, my inspiration might havebeen that of Artemus Ward’s celebrated lecture, of which the author remarked thatits principal virtue was that “ . . . it contains so many things that don’t have any-thing to do with it” (Browne, 1865).

Page 9: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 19

Consider the diagnostic guidelines for DCD offered in DSM IV. Four crite-ria are applied. Two are inclusive (the criteria must be satisfied if the diagnosis isto be assigned), and two are exclusive (meeting the criteria entails rejection of thediagnosis). As shown below, Criterion A states that a child’s performance in dailyactivities that require motor coordination is substantially below that expected givenhis or her chronological age and IQ. “Substantially” is often operationalized asmeaning a score on a standardized test of motor performance lying more than 2standard deviations below the age norm. According to Geuze et al. (2001), theMovement Assessment Battery for Children (ABC; Henderson & Sugden, 1992) isnow the test most widely cited in the literature for this purpose. Criterion B ismuch more difficult to operationalize, requiring the assessor to judge whether thedeficit interferes to a significant extent with academic achievements or the activi-ties of daily living (e.g., Watkinson et al., 2001). Criterion C states that the deficitmust not be ascribable to a medical condition such as cerebral palsy and must notsatisfy the diagnostic requirements of the generally debilitating group of disordersknown as pervasive developmental disorders (which include autism). Finally, Cri-terion D states that if mental retardation is present, the motor difficulties must begreater than those to be expected on that basis alone (for extensive reviews ofthese criteria and their application, see Henderson & Barnett, 1998; Dewey &Wilson, 2001; Geuze et al., 2001).

The Four DSM IV Diagnostic Criteriafor Developmental Coordination Disorder

A. Performance in daily activities that require motor coordination is sub-stantially below that expected given the person’s chronological age and measuredintelligence. This may be manifested by marked delays in achieving motor mile-stones (e.g. walking, crawling, sitting), dropping things, “clumsiness,” poor per-formance in sports, or poor handwriting.

Figure 2 — What makes doctors happy: Congenital hypothyroidism as a prototype ofmedical diagnosis. (Reprinted with permission of Professor Hilary Cass.)

Page 10: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson20

B. The disturbance in Criterion A significantly interferes with academicachievement or activities of daily living.

C. The disturbance is not due to a general medical condition (e.g. cerebralpalsy, hemiplegia, or muscular dystrophy) and does not meet the criteria for aPervasive Developmental disorder.

D. If mental retardation is present the motor difficulties are in excess ofthose usually associated with it.

Deviations of DCD From the Medical Model

How does this kind of diagnostic protocol deviate from the classic medical model?In acquired disorders of movement such as Parkinson’s disease, the cardinal symp-toms of the disease include positive features such as resting tremor and “cogwheel”rigidity, which are present in patients but not in healthy individuals, as well asnegative features such as abnormal slowness of movement, where the patient lacksthe speed of movement available to the healthy individual. In contrast, the perfor-mance features specified in DSM for DCD are not pathological and can only besaid to be abnormal in a statistical sense.

Positive features are not, however, the preserve of acquired disorders. Theobsessional interests and stereotyped actions of autism and the hyperactivity ofchildren with ADHD seem to have discriminable positive aspects. If positive symp-toms could be said to characterize DCD, then they might comprise an awkward orinfluent style of performance, an aspect of performance we can all observe andagree on but which is difficult to assess with any objectivity. Another very interest-ing possibility is that in DCD, difficulties with motor control might exert demandson attentional resources and working memory that are not present in children whosedevelopment is normal. However, the lack of agreed positive features means thatdiagnosis has to be based on norm-referenced test items that yield a continuousmeasure of performance, such as time spent balancing on one leg, for which wecan derive statistically determined cut-off points.

If the signs and symptoms of DCD do not comprise a distinct set ofpathognomic features, we may still inquire whether they form a cohesive patternat a functional level. Like schizophrenia, DCD is of unknown aetiology and lacksa biological marker. However, the symptoms of schizophrenia do seem to formtwo distinct clusters. A set of positive symptoms (abnormal by their presence),such as hallucinations and delusions, consists of features in which reality testinghas broken down in a manner unknown to normal individuals. In contrast, thenegative signs (abnormal by their absence) of schizophrenia are characterized bythe lack of attributes like motivation or expressions of emotion, which are presentin normal individuals. Crude though it be, this distinction has been a fertile sourceof further hypotheses about the action of neuroleptic medication, the chronic ef-fects of institutionalization, and the possibility of subtypes.

Within the literature on DCD, there are a number of studies that have at-tempted to define subtypes of the disorder (e.g., Dewey & Kaplan, 1994; Hoare,1994; Jongmans, 1993; Macnab, Miller, & Polatajko, 2001). However, in our view,none of the subtypes proposed have been properly validated and there is, as yet, noreally persuasive evidence to suggest that the motor features form cohesive andcontrasting clusters. One problem is the lack of general theories in the motor domain

Page 11: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 21

that delineate important distinctions between tasks, in terms of their processingrequirements. In the construction of most standardized tests, some attempt hasbeen made to include a range of items that straddle an intuitive taxonomy of every-day actions; these different types of test item cannot be said to reflect theoretically-motivated distinctions of a similar status, say, to that drawn in reading research betweenprinted words whose pronunciation can be assembled by phonographic rule (d/o/g =“dawg”) and those requiring lexical retrieval (two = “tooh”).

Another problem is the lack of conclusive evidence that the features of DCD,as a whole, comprise a set that is clearly distinguishable from the features of otherdevelopmental disorders. The notion of separable syndromes implies that eachdisorder constitutes a discrete clinical entity. This issue is sometimes expressed inthe question whether coordination difficulties amount to a syndrome or are merelya symptom.

In this regard, concern with what sort of creature DCD is reflects widerdoubts about the whole idea of specific developmental disorders as distinct andindependent clinical entities. These doubts are nicely captured by two quotationsfrom the recent literature that have a remarkable consonance:

Those with highly specific deficits are the exception rather than the rule.(Hill, Bishop, and Nimmo-Smith, 1998, p. 656)

Co-morbidity is the rule, rather than the exception. (Kaplan, Wilson, Dewey,& Crawford, 1998, p. 484)

In the limited space available, just two studies will be mentioned that ad-dress these issues in relation to DCD, in rather different ways. The first surveys asubstantial portion of the territory of co-morbidity; the second concentrates on thenature and extent of motor difficulties in children with DCD and in a quite differ-ent disorder, Asperger’s syndrome.

In 1998, Kaplan and colleagues examined over 200 children in Canada whohad been referred to their clinic for either attention problems or learning difficul-ties. All were tested “blind” on measures of cognitive ability, literacy, attention,and motor performance, and strict criteria were adopted for each of three specificlearning difficulties. Only children with IQs over 75 were included. This was notan epidemiological study, and it is not entirely clear to what population it can begeneralized. Nevertheless, its results are very thought provoking.

As Figure 3 shows, 115 children met the criteria for at least one of the threedevelopmental disorders. Of these, only a minority (46%) were “pure” cases. Onein five actually met the criteria for all three diagnostic categories. Of special inter-est to us here is that the proportion of children exhibiting comorbidity was particu-larly high for children with motor coordination problems. Of a total of 81 childrenexhibiting coordination difficulties, over two thirds (68%) were diagnosed withmultiple disorders.

From these data, Kaplan et al. concluded that it was likely that a variety oflearning disabilities could occur when the early development of the brain was dis-rupted. However, the specific pattern of the deficits that resulted would depend onthe site and the extent of the damaged neural substrate.

Before abandoning the notion of syndromes entirely, however, it seems worthinquiring whether the motor difficulties experienced by these various groups ofchildren are really identical in nature and extent or whether the difficulties take a

Page 12: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson22

discernibly different form when combined with another disorder. Were this to bethe case, knowledge of the precise nature of these differences might enhance ourability to differentiate between different developmental disorders.

This very question is posed in a more narrowly circumscribed domain by aninvestigation in which one of us has recently been involved. Essentially, the workis directed toward a comparison between two developmental disorders that appearto share a component of motor impairment but are very different in other respects(Green, 1997; Green, Baird, Barnett, Huber, & Henderson, in press). One of thesegroups comprised children assigned a diagnosis of Asperger’s syndrome by veryexperienced pediatricians using ICD-10 criteria. The other group had been as-signed the diagnosis DCD, also by appropriately qualified and experienced pro-fessionals. While the clinical literature on Asperger’s syndrome abounds withdescriptions of movement difficulties, it is important to note that these played norole in the formal diagnosis.

We aimed to compare the severity of the movement deficit experienced bythese two groups and to explore the possibility that the nature of the deficit mightdiffer in the two groups. To this end, we examined the profiles of the children onthe Movement ABC and various aspects of their performance on a gesture test.Because Asperger’s syndrome is thought to involve defective socialization, wealso sought to detect any differential effect of social pressure on motor learningand performance in the two groups. So far, we have found no evidence that clum-siness was more severe or took a systematically different form in DCD where itwas not accompanied by the features of Asperger’s syndrome than when it was.

Part III: Intervention

It is often difficult for practitioners to appreciate why researchers spend so muchtime debating issues of the type we have just discussed. If we can describe a child’sdifficulties clearly, then we are ready to intervene. So, what do labels matter?However, resources for children with special needs are limited. Governments wantto know how many children of a particular type there are in the system, what is the

Figure 3 — Venn diagram of comorbidities. Shows number of participants meeting strictcriteria for dyslexia, developmental coordination disorder (DCD), and attention deficit hy-peractivity disorder (ADHD), in isolation and in combination, out of a total sample of 224children referred for learning/attentional difficulties.

Page 13: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 23

best (or cheapest) way of identifying them, and what is the most effective (orcheapest) way of helping them to realize their potential. Rigorous evaluations ofintervention are essential both for theoretical and practical reasons.

Over a quarter of a century ago, Rarick and Broadhead (1968) conducted alarge-scale assessment of intervention that had its origins in work by Rarick in the1950s. Their objective was to determine the effects of a school-based, physicalactivity program on the development of children whom they described as educa-tionally disabled. A total of 481 children between the ages of 6 and 13 were in-volved, subdivided into those categorized as educable mentally retarded (EMR),with IQs in the 40-70 range, and those described as having “minimal brain injury”(MBI) whom one presumes to have had IQs above 70. These two sets of childrenwere allocated to four treatment groups. Two received a remedial physical educa-tion (PE) program – one in a group setting, the other in a one-to-one setting. Athird group (acting as “Hawthorne” controls), engaged in an art program, and thefourth acted as a “passive” control group, continuing with regular classes. Theintervention of 35 min daily lasted for 20 weeks. Motor, cognitive, and social/emotional measures were monitored using standardized assessment by staff notinvolved in the intervention. We have attempted to recast these design featureswithin a general framework, which is shown in Figure 4.

Being in the special programs, whether PE or art, had a positive effect on thechildren in all the domains measured. Especially when the program was individu-alized, PE exerted a greater beneficial effect on motor performance than the artprogram. Moreover, all these effects were greater for the children with higher IQs.

Rarick and Broadhead’s (1968) design was sophisticated for its time and theoutcome is correspondingly interesting. The remainder of this paper employs theframework used to describe Rarick’s study as a means of examining some of themore important issues in the design of intervention studies for the 21st century,dealing first with the selection and description of participants.

Figure 4 — Schema for encoding Rarick’s intervention design. Two groups of subjects,termed educable mentally retarded (EMR) and minimal brain injury (MBI) were randomlyallocated in equal numbers to one of four treatment groups (PE = Physical Education).Motor, cognitive, and social/emotional measures were employed before and after training.

Page 14: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson24

The one aspect of Rarick’s study that clearly fails to meet contemporarystandards is the description of his participants. Should we regard minimal braininjury as an archaism, to be translated simply as DCD? Or might the MBI group bedivisible into ADHD, Asperger syndrome, and DCD? Since the London Consen-sus meeting, a number of researchers in the field have discussed the possibility ofagreeing upon a minimum data set requirement for all published studies of DCD,comparable to that prescribed by the UK Medical Research Council for studies ofschizophrenia. Ideally, this would include demographic data, data on the child’smotor difficulties, data on IQ, and data on comorbidity. Since some professionalsare not qualified to administer IQ tests and others are refused access to existingdata, we realize that the provision of IQ data might be problematic. Nevertheless,the presentation of such data is extremely desirable whenever possible. Recall thatRarick and his colleagues found that the PE programs they investigated were moreeffective for the children with higher IQs. Yet, few subsequent studies have sys-tematically manipulated IQ as a factor.

For the motor difficulties, Criterion A and (possibly B) of DSM IV providethe basis for a descriptive requirement, but it must be acknowledged that the stan-dardized tests currently available vary considerably in content. This means, ofcourse, that we cannot simply assume that identical scores on two different testsare directly comparable (Crawford, Wilson, & Dewey, 2001; Dewey & Wilson,2001; Geuze et al., 2001; Tan, Parker, & Larkin, 2001). In addition, there is alsothe problem of what cut-off point to use. Although for certain experimental stud-ies, a laxer criterion might be best, for intervention studies, our own view is thatthe 5th percentile is not only an appropriate cut point for Criterion A but is alsolikely to be politically acceptable to those charged with resource provision. Just asimportant as a global estimate of ability, however, is the presentation of a profile ofthe child’s performance across the motor domain. (This requirement would inci-dentally militate against the use of standardized tests that draw upon an exces-sively narrow range of skills.) Such profiles, together with a requirement thatpublished studies present full data sets on individual children, would give greatimpetus to the search for consistent subtypes.

With regard to data on comorbidity, the real problem is where to stop. Asnoted in our discussion of diagnosis, we do not yet know whether motor difficul-ties that occur in isolation take the same form as motor difficulties that occur inconjunction with reading difficulties, language impairment, or attentional disor-ders. Even if the motor difficulties do turn out to be the same, it is yet anothermatter whether the methods used to treat those that occur in isolation are equallyappropriate to the treatment of those same difficulties in a child with ADHD orAsperger syndrome. Given our belief that the incidence of both of these conjunc-tions is likely to be high, this issue is not inconsequential.

A rather different issue from the foregoing is what should be included asoutcome measures (see Figure 5). Our follow-up studies offer us a wealth of infor-mation on the long-term consequences of DCD that goes untreated. They reportproblems of self-esteem, peer relationships, loneliness, depression, and educationalattainment that is far below what would be predicted from the child’s IQ. Althoughthe idea that improving a child’s motor difficulties would inevitably have a positiveeffect on IQ was dispelled a long time ago, the notion that such programs mightserve to raise a child’s self-esteem and confidence is very much to the fore.

When discussing the data we need to provide on participants, we identifiedthe question of the compass of the measures taken, in motor and nonmotor do-

Page 15: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 25

mains. Space constraints now dictate that we concentrate on issues in the motordomain. The first of these issues concerns the relationship between the content ofthe measurement instrument and the content of the intervention program. The sec-ond concerns the assessment of generalization (see Figure 5; also see Mandich,Polatajko, Macnab, & Miller, 2001).

The idea that little would be gained by simply training a child to do better onthe test items themselves is readily grasped. However, it is difficult to see clearlybeyond that point. For the task–orientated provider of intervention, for example,the objective may well be to teach the child to catch a ball, so in the selection of atraining task, how close may we come to the ball skill items in, say, the Bruininks-Oseretsky (Bruininks, 1978) or the Movement ABC test (Henderson and Sugden,1992)? Can the problem be minimized by using a test such as Ulrich’s Gross Mo-tor Development Test (Ulrich, 1985), which is capable of revealing that the chil-dren had changed in the way they catch a ball as well as in the number of catchesthey could make? For the “process-oriented” therapist, such problems are less likelyto arise. Swinging a child in a hammock to improve vestibular functioning, and hencebalance, is clearly very different from practicing a series of balance activities directly.

Regardless of the content of the intervention, however, the demonstrationof generalization represents a major issue. In the past, it was thought that thebeneficial impact of a motor intervention would automatically spread to a child’sreading ability or, more precisely, to the child’s readiness to read. While the issuehas not been entirely resolved, this particular generalization premise is much lesslikely to receive unqualified assent. Moreover, the focus of debate has become

Figure 5 — Issues concerning measures. The type of measure is an important decision.Where a test is repeatedly administered, its susceptibility to practice effects should be known.Test content should not be too close to the content of training. Any generalization of train-ing effects, whether immediate or delayed, should be determined. The persistence of train-ing effects should be tested. N.B. Rarick’s design lacked follow-up measures. Individualsubject data should be included.

Page 16: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson26

more refined. Consider that Adapted PE programs frequently lack any fine motoractivities. Should we expect such a program to improve a child’s handwriting?Currently, available data on the specificity of skill would seem to suggest that nosuch effect would be obtained. Should we, therefore, confine our tests of generaliza-tion to conditions involving a distinct shift in the performance context, for example,evaluating handwriting training in the form of coloring, shape matching, copying,drawing, etc., by means of examination of the child’s essay writing in the classroom?

There is also the question of what epoch we should consider. Maybe it takestime for a child to adapt to a newly learned skill so that using it in a new contextonly emerges later. Once again, we return to the need to be more explicit about thepattern of impairment shown by individual children and the individual’s responseto intervention. We need not be despondent about showing that one child respondsto a particular approach and another does not—this is the way forward!

Sugden and Chambers (1998) concluded from a recent review of interven-tion studies that there was cause for optimism. “Intervention has been shown to beeffective,” they proclaimed (p. 146), speaking of “overwhelming evidence thatdemonstrates that most approaches work” (p. 145). What has not been shown,according to Sugden and Chambers, are “identifiable and consistently differenteffects of the different approaches” (p. 145). We do not entirely share Sugden andChamber’s optimism. As practitioners, it may be mildly reassuring to be told thatmost approaches work, at least to the extent that the intervention can be statisti-cally discriminated from no intervention at all. However, from a research point ofview, the absence of any persuasive evidence favoring one kind of interventionover another is profoundly depressing (a melancholy view apparently also takenby Mandich et al., 2001).

What kind of intervention works? Most reviews draw a broad distinctionbetween process-oriented approaches and task-oriented approaches to interven-tion. In a recent meta-analysis, Pless and Carlsson (2000) agreed with Sugden andChambers in finding that the evidence for efficacy is less persuasive in the case ofthe process view than in the task-oriented view. However, the utility of makinggeneral pronouncements about process-oriented interventions is worth question-ing since it obliges us to lump together hugely diverse approaches. For example,under this heading, we find interventions designed to remedy deficits ranging froma narrowly conceived kinesthetic one (proposed by Laszlo & Bairstow, 1983;Laszlow, Bairstow, & Bartrip, 1988; Laszlo & Sainsbury, 1993) to one involvingthe integration of information across all sense modalities (e.g., Ayres, 1972). More-over, no process-oriented approach of which we are aware presents anything ap-proaching a comprehensive theory, tying the supposedly defective processes to themany interacting variables that underlie even the most simple of everyday tasks.In Laszlo’s reports of the efficacy of her approach, for example, nothing is saidabout the precise nature of the deficit, nor about how exactly simply exercisingkinesthetic judgment “fixes” it. Most importantly, nothing is said about how fixingthe putative deficit leads to the acquisition of normal skilled action. Fortunately,all of this may not matter, since our group and others have failed to find any evi-dence in support of this approach (Sims, Henderson, Hulme, & Morton, 1998a;Sims, Henderson, Moron, & Hulme, 1998b; Polatajko et al., 1995).

Another consideration in relation to intervention is that in the wider universeof deficits, some are simply not fixable. A corollary of this would be that in suchcases, our interventions should not focus on the problem itself but on strategies forfinding a way around it.

Page 17: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 27

If our current approaches to understanding and helping children with DCDare inadequate, how should we proceed? Progress in our understanding of skilledaction has come through the development of rich and detailed paradigms for theexperimental analysis of particular components of action, such as walking or reach-ing and grasping. Consider, for a moment, the apparently simple sequence of ac-tions involved in walking up to a table, picking up a pen, and beginning to write.Entire symposia have been devoted to components of this action sequence. Theoculomotor subsystem uses saccadic search to locate the pencil. The locomotorsystem transports the agent to the vicinity of the pencil. Visually guided reachingextends the hand toward the object. This displaces the agent’s center of gravity sothat the postural control system must make postural corrections. Overlapping withextension of the hand, the prehension system adjusts grasp to the dimensions ofthe pencil. The necessary unimanual adjustments are then made to the pen grip toensure that the ideal, dynamic tripod grip can be adopted and writing can commence.

Very little of this rich and often elegant work has informed or illuminatedthe investigation of DCD—far less the content of interventions. At present, thegulf between experimental studies of normal performance and studies of develop-mental coordination difficulties seems wide and deep. Bridging it will take time.Moreover, we cannot be sure that experimentally derived insight will furnish usautomatically with improved means of intervention. However, we find it difficultto believe that the greater understanding mentioned in the title of this paper will beunaccompanied by any practical application.

Meanwhile, what is the therapist or the student of intervention to do whilethe chasm of which we spoke is slowly being filled with grant applications, PhDtheses, and other academic building materials? The content of presently availableinterventions needs to be considered more carefully. Above all, we require manymore comparisons of treatment A with treatment B and not simply of A versus acontrol condition (see Figure 6). Taking account of comorbidity, adding follow-up

Figure 6 — Decisions about treatment. Comparisons of a treatment with a control condi-tion must increasingly give way to contrasts between rival treatments. The type of schedulegoverning the delivery of training remains a live issue, as does the locus and whether train-ing is on an individual basis. These factors may contribute to the cost as well as the efficacyof training. Crossover designs are increasingly used in order that participants receive treat-ment at some stage in the investigation. These also allow effects due the provider of trainingto be isolated.

Page 18: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson28

measures to determine the persistence of training effects and their potential tobecome generalized and using cross-over designs as an orthogonal means of repli-cation and a way of controlling for therapist efficacy means much bigger and re-source demanding studies of intervention. These should keep us all in work formost of the new century.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis-orders (4th ed.) Washington, DC: Author.

Ayres, A.J. (1972). Sensory integration and learning disorders. Los Angeles: Western Psy-chological Services.

Ayres, A.J., Mailloux, Z.K., & Wendler, C.L.W. (1987). Developmental dyspraxia: Is it aunitary function? Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 7, 93-110.

Bax, M.C.O. (1999). Specific learning disorders/neurodevelopmental disorders. Develop-mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 147.

Bishop, D. (1998). Is specific language impairment a valid diagnostic category? Geneticand Psycholinguistic evidence. In J. Rispens, T.A. van Yperen, & W. Yule (Eds.),Perspectives on the classification of specific developmental disorders (pp. 139-154).Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Browne, C.F. (1865). Artemis Ward’s Lecture. Cited in The Oxford Dictionary of Quota-tions. (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bruininks, R.H. (1978). Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. Circle Pines, MN:American Guidance Service.

Cantell, M.H., Smyth, M.M., & Ahonen, T.P. (1994). Clumsiness in adolescence: Educa-tional, motor, and social outcomes of motor delay detected at five years. In S.E.Henderson (Ed.), Developmental coordination disorder: Special issue. Adapted Physi-cal Activity Quarterly, 11, 111-236.

Crawford, S., Wilson, B.N., & Dewey, D. (2001). Identifying developmental coordinationdisorder: Consistency between tests. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediat-rics, 20, 29-50.

Davies P.A. (1994). Speaking the same language. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-rology, 36, 189-190.

Denckla, M.B. (1984). Developmental dyspraxia: The clumsy child. In M.D. Levene & P.Satz (Eds.), Middle childhood: Developmental dysfunction (pp. 245-260). Baltimore,MD: University Park Press.

Dewey, D. (1995). What is developmental Dyspraxia? Brain and Cognition, 29, 254-274.Dewey, D., & Kaplan, B.J. (1994). Subtyping of developmental motor deficits. Develop-

mental Neuropsychology, 10, 265-284.Dewey, D., & Wilson, B.N. (2001). Developmental coordination disorder: What is it? Physical

and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 20, 5-27.Dowdney, L., & Scott, S. (Eds.). (l998). Forum on Dyslexia. Child Psychology and Psy-

chiatry Review, 3, 2-25.Gallistel, C.R. (1980). The organisation of action: A new synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.Gardner-Medwin, D. (1995). New medical classifications and languages. Developmental

Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 283-284.Geuze, R.H., & Borger, H. (1993). Children who are clumsy: Five years later. Adapted

Physical Activity Quarterly, 10, 10-21.

Page 19: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 29

Geuze, R.H., Jongmans, M.J., Schoemaker, M.M., & Smits-Engleman, B.C.M. (2001). Clini-cal and research diagnostic criteria for developmental coordination disorder: A re-view and discussion. In R.H. Geuze, M.J. Jongmans, M.M. Schoemaker, & B.C.M.Smits-Engleman (Eds.), Special Issue of Human Movement Science, 20, 7-47.

Gillberg, C. (1992). Deficits in attention, motor control and perception and other syndromesattributed to minimal brain dysfunction. In J. Aicardi (Ed.), Diseases of the nervoussystem in childhood (pp. 1321-1337). Oxford: Blackwell.

Gillberg, I.C., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Children with preschool minor neurodevelopmentaldisorders IV: Behaviour and school achievement at age 13. Developmental Medicineand Child Neurology, 31, 3-13.

Gillberg, I.C., Gillberg, C., & Groth, J. (1989). Children with preschool minorneurodevelopmental disorders V: Neuro-developmental profiles at age 13. Develop-mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 14-24.

Green, D. (1997). Clumsiness in Asperger’s syndrome. Unpublished master’s thesis, Uni-versity of Surrey, UK.

Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A.L., Huber, J., & Henderson, S.E. (in press). The severity andnature of motor impairment in Asperger’s syndrome: A comparison with specificdevelopmental disorder of motor function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.

Hart, H. (1999). Terminology to benefit children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-rology, 41, 651.

Hellgren, L., Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I.C., & Enerskog, I. (1993). Children with deficits inattention, motor control and perception (DAMP) almost grown up: General health atage 16 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 881-892.

Hellgren, L., Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, I.C. (1994). Children with deficits in attention, motorcontrol and perception (DAMP) almost grown up: The contribution of various back-ground factors to outcome at the age of 16 years. European Child and AdolescentPsychiatry, 3, 1-15.

Hellgren, L., Gillberg, I.C., Bagenholm, A., & Gillberg, C. (1994). Children with deficits inattention, motor control and perception (DAMP) almost grown up: Psychiatric andpersonality disorders at age 16 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,35, 1255-1271.

Henderson, S.E. (1993). Motor development and minor handicap. In A. Kalverboer & R.Geuze (Eds.), Motor development in early and later childhood: Longitudinal ap-proaches (pp. 286-306). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Henderson, S.E., & Barnett, A. (1998). The classification of specific motor coordinationdisorders: Some problems to be solved. Human Movement Science, 17, 449-470.

Henderson, S.E., & Sugden, D.A. (1992). The Movement Assessment Battery for Children.London: The Psychological Corporation.

Hill, E.L., Bishop, D.V.M., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Representational gestures in Devel-opmental Coordination Disorder and specific language impairment: Error types andthe reliability of ratings. Human Movement Science, 17, 655-679.

Hoare, D. (1994). Subtypes of developmental coordination disorder. Adapted Physical Ac-tivity Quarterly, 11, 158-169.

Jongmans, M.J. (1993). Perceptuo-motor competence in prematurely born children at schoolage: Neurological and psychological aspects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,University of London, UK.

Kaplan, B.J., Wilson, B.N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S.G. (1998). DCD may not be a dis-crete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471-490.

Laszlo, J.I., & Bairstow, P.J. (1983). Kinaesthesis: Its measurement, training and relation-ship to motor control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 411-421.

Page 20: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Henderson and Henderson30

Laszlo, J.I., Bairstow, P., & Bartrip. J. (1988). A new approach to treating perceptuo-motordysfunction: Previously called “clumsiness.” Support for Learning, 3, 35-40.

Laszlo, J.I., & Sainsbury, K.M. (1993). Perceptual-motor development and the preventionof clumsiness. Special issue: Motor control and sensory-motor integration. Psycho-logical Research/Psychologische Forschung, 55, 167-174.

Losse, A., Henderson, S.E., Elliman, D., Hall, D.B., Knight, E., & Jongmans, M. (1991).Clumsiness in children - do they grow out of it? A ten year follow-up study. Develop-mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 55-68.

MacKeith, R. (1968). Maximum clarity on neurodevelopmental disorders. DevelopmentalMedicine and Child Neurology, 10, 143-144.

Macnab, J.J., Miller, L.T., & Polatajko, H.J. (2001). The search for sub-types of DCD: Iscluster analysis the answer? Human Movement Science, 20, 49-72.

Mandich, A.D., Polatajko, H.J., Macnab, J., & Miller, T. (2001). Treatment of children withdevelopmental coordination disorder: What is the evidence? Physical and Occupa-tional Therapy in Pediatrics, 20, 51-68.

Missiuna, C., & Polatajko, H. (1995). Developmental dyspraxia by any other name: Aren’tthey all just clumsy children? American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 49, 619-627.

Miyahara, M., & Register, C. (2000). Perceptions of three terms to describe physical awk-wardness in children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 367-376.

National Health Service Executive. (1999). Clinical governance, quality in the new NHS.Bristol, UK: author.

Peters, J., Barnett, A., & Henderson, S.E. (2001). Clumsiness, dyspraxia and developmen-tal coordination disorder: How do health and educational professionals in the UKdefine the terms? Child, Care, Health and Development, 27, 399-413.

Pless, M., & Carlsson, M. (2000). Effect of motor skill intervention on developmental coor-dination disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 17, 381-402.

Poizner, H., Merians, A.S., Clark, M.A., Rothi, L.J., & Heilman, K.M. (1997). Kinematicapproaches to the study of apraxic disorders. In L.J.G. Rothi, & K.M. Heilman (Eds.),Apraxia: The neuropsychology of action (pp. 93-109). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Polatajko, H.J. (1998). Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) alias the clumsy child.In K. Whitmore, H. Hart, G.W. Willems (Eds.), A neurodevelopmental approach tospecific learning disorders: The clinical nature of the problem (pp. 119-133). Lon-don: MacKeith Press.

Polatajko, H., Fox, M., & Missiuna, C. (1995). An international concensus on children withdevelopmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy,62, 3-6.

Polatajko, H.J., Macnab, J.J., Anstett, B., Malloy-Miller, T., Murphy, K., & Noh, S. (1995).A clinical trial of the process-oriented treatment approach for children with develop-mental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37,310-319.

Rarick, G.L., & Broadhead, G.D. (1968). The effects of individualised versus group-ori-ented physical education programmes on selected parameters of the development ofeducable mentally retarded and minimally brain-injured children. Report to the UnitedStates Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Office of Education, and theJoseph P.Kennedy, Jr. Foundation. University of Wisconsin, Department of PhysicalEducation, Madison.

Rarick, G.L., Dobbins, D.A., & Broadhead, G.D. (1976). The motor domain and its corre-lates in educationally handicapped children. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Page 21: Toward an Understanding of Developmental …...While DCD and SDD-MF have a relatively formal status due to their APA and WHO provenance, dyspraxia seems to have been chosen either

Understanding DCD 31

Rispens, J. van Yperen, T.A. & Yule, W. (Eds.). (1998). Perspectives on the classification ofspecific developmental disorders. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Rothi, L.J.G., & Heilman, K.M. (1997). Apraxia: The neuropsychology of action. Hove,UK: Psychology Press.

Rutter, M. (Ed.). (1980). Scientific foundations of developmental psychiatry. London: WiilianHeineman Medical Books Ltd.

Rutter, M. (1998). Foreword. In J. Rispens, T.A. van Yperen, and W. Yule (Eds.), Perspec-tives on the classification of specific developmental disorders (pp. ix-xviii). Dordrecht:Kluwer.

Schmidt, R.A. (1988). Motor control and learning (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.Sims, K., Henderson, S.E., Hulme, C., & Morton, J. (1998a). The remediation of clumsi-

ness. I: An evaluation of Laszlo’s kinaesthetic approach. Developmental Medicineand Child Neurology, 38, 967-987.

Sims, K., Henderson, S.E., Morton, J., & Hulme, C. (1998b). The remediation of clumsi-ness. II: Is kinaesthesis the answer? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,38, 988-997.

Skinner, R.A., & Piek, J.P. (2001). Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination inchildren and adolescents. Human Movement Science, 20, 73-94.

Square, P.A., Roy, E.A., & Martin, R.E. (1997). Apraxia of speech: Another form of praxisdisruption. In L.J.G. Rothi, & K.M. Heilman, (Eds), Apraxia: The neuropsychologyof action (pp. 173-206). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Stanovitch, K.E. (1998). Refining the phonological core deficit model. Child Psychologyand Psychiatry Review, 3, 1-25.

Sugden, D.A., & Chambers, M. (1998). Intervention approaches and children with devel-opmental coordination disorder. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 2, 139-147.

Tan, S.K., Parker, H.E., & Larkin, D. (2001). Concurrent validity of motor tests used toidentify children with motor impairment. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18,168-182.

Ulrich, D. (1985). Test of Gross Motor Development. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Watkinson, E.J., Causgrove-Dunn, J., Cavaliere, N., Calzonetti, K., Wilhelm, L., & Dwyer,

S. (2001). Engagement in playground activities as a criterion for diagnosing devel-opmental coordination disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18, 18-35.

Weiss, P. (1941). Self-differentiation of the basic patterns of coordination. ComparativePsychology Monographs, 17 (No. 4).

World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behaviouraldisorders. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Zoia, S. (1999). Normal and impaired motor skills development. Unpublished PhD thesis,University of Trieste, Italy.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Professor Hilary Cass for permission to reproduce Figure 2, toBonny Kaplan for permission to reproduce the data in Figure 3, and to Linda Richards forclerical assistance. Anna Barnett provided helpful comments on a draft. We would also liketo acknowledge the help and encouragement of Greg Reid, Dale Ulrich, and Claudine Sherrill.