transportation research part f - yonsei...

12
Non-driving-related tasks, workload, and takeover performance in highly automated driving contexts Sol Hee Yoon, Yong Gu Ji Dept. of Information and Industrial Engineering, Yonsei University, Seoul, Republic of Korea article info Article history: Received 26 March 2018 Received in revised form 27 November 2018 Accepted 27 November 2018 Keywords: Driver workload Eye-movement behavior Takeover performance Visual performance Physical performance abstract This study investigates the influence of non-driving-related task (NDRT) on takeover per- formance in a highly automated driving (HAD) context and the effect of workload on dri- ver’s takeover performance. A driving simulator was used to evaluate how well a driver resumes control of a vehicle after being in a HAD situation during which they performed a NDRT. For both the visual performance and takeover capability, there was a significant difference based on the task carried out; however, the reaction times when reaching for the steering wheel did not differ among the tasks. The result on workload demonstrate that NDRT type has significant effect while a positive correlation between the performance dimension and takeover was found. In addition, takeover performance for interaction with the entertainment console exhibits a significantly positive correlation, whereas watching a video or interacting with a smartphone exhibits mostly a significantly negative correlation with workload dimensions. These results provide implication on the effect of tasks desired and enabled to be performed by drivers in HAD and its influence on the transition of control. Ó 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction The automotive industry has been gradually making major changes to the classic concept of the car through innovative technologies including navigation, assistance, and communication systems (Brauer, 2015). Indeed, vehicles are rapidly becoming more intelligent and automated owing to the development and introduction of technologies that assist drivers, such as active cruise control (ACC), lane-keeping assistance (LKA), and intelligent braking systems (IBSs) (Choi & Ji, 2015; Flemish et al., 2008; Vollrath, Schleicher, & Gelau, 2011). These systems can reduce the number of accidents due to distrac- tions, poor performance, and human error (Yang & Coughlin, 2014). The main goal of these systems is to increase the comfort level of the driver by reducing the demand imposed by the driving tasks (Koo et al., 2015). Moreover, recent studies on autonomous vehicles have described the benefits of automated systems, presenting drivers with new in-vehicle experiences, such as the opportunity to engage in more dynamic social interaction, vehicle leisure, or the use of various technologies while relaxing or engaging in work activities (Kim, Yoon, Kim, & Ji, 2015; Ive, Sirkin, Miller, Li, & Ju, 2015; Yang & Coughlin, 2014). The US Department of Transportation’s National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) has proposed five levels of automation for automated vehicles where level 0 refers to no automation, and level 4 to full self-driving automation. How- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.11.015 1369-8478/Ó 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Corresponding author at: Dept. of Information and Industrial Engineering, Yonsei University, 262 Seongsanno, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120-749, Republic of Korea. E-mail address: [email protected] (Y.G. Ji). Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Transportation Research Part F journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/trf

Upload: others

Post on 04-Apr-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Transportation Research Part F

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate / t r f

Non-driving-related tasks, workload, and takeover performancein highly automated driving contexts

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.11.0151369-8478/� 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

⇑ Corresponding author at: Dept. of Information and Industrial Engineering, Yonsei University, 262 Seongsanno, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120-749,of Korea.

E-mail address: [email protected] (Y.G. Ji).

Sol Hee Yoon, Yong Gu Ji ⇑Dept. of Information and Industrial Engineering, Yonsei University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history:Received 26 March 2018Received in revised form 27 November 2018Accepted 27 November 2018

Keywords:Driver workloadEye-movement behaviorTakeover performanceVisual performancePhysical performance

This study investigates the influence of non-driving-related task (NDRT) on takeover per-formance in a highly automated driving (HAD) context and the effect of workload on dri-ver’s takeover performance. A driving simulator was used to evaluate how well a driverresumes control of a vehicle after being in a HAD situation during which they performeda NDRT. For both the visual performance and takeover capability, there was a significantdifference based on the task carried out; however, the reaction times when reaching forthe steering wheel did not differ among the tasks. The result on workload demonstrate thatNDRT type has significant effect while a positive correlation between the performancedimension and takeover was found. In addition, takeover performance for interaction withthe entertainment console exhibits a significantly positive correlation, whereas watching avideo or interacting with a smartphone exhibits mostly a significantly negative correlationwith workload dimensions. These results provide implication on the effect of tasks desiredand enabled to be performed by drivers in HAD and its influence on the transition ofcontrol.

� 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

The automotive industry has been gradually making major changes to the classic concept of the car through innovativetechnologies including navigation, assistance, and communication systems (Brauer, 2015). Indeed, vehicles are rapidlybecoming more intelligent and automated owing to the development and introduction of technologies that assist drivers,such as active cruise control (ACC), lane-keeping assistance (LKA), and intelligent braking systems (IBSs) (Choi & Ji, 2015;Flemish et al., 2008; Vollrath, Schleicher, & Gelau, 2011). These systems can reduce the number of accidents due to distrac-tions, poor performance, and human error (Yang & Coughlin, 2014). The main goal of these systems is to increase the comfortlevel of the driver by reducing the demand imposed by the driving tasks (Koo et al., 2015). Moreover, recent studies onautonomous vehicles have described the benefits of automated systems, presenting drivers with new in-vehicle experiences,such as the opportunity to engage in more dynamic social interaction, vehicle leisure, or the use of various technologieswhile relaxing or engaging in work activities (Kim, Yoon, Kim, & Ji, 2015; Ive, Sirkin, Miller, Li, & Ju, 2015; Yang &Coughlin, 2014).

The US Department of Transportation’s National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) has proposed five levelsof automation for automated vehicles where level 0 refers to no automation, and level 4 to full self-driving automation. How-

Republic

Page 2: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 621

ever, from levels 1 through 3, drivers need to be involved in certain aspects of the manipulation and control of the vehicle,depending on the systems that have been automated. Given these changes in the automotive industry, researchers and cardevelopers are faced with the challenge of developing safe vehicles by balancing the role of automated systems and the per-mitted changes in driver behaviors.

Concerns remain regarding the negative influence of high levels of automation, such as a decrease in context and situationawareness (Jamson, Merat, Carsten, & Lai, 2013; Strand, Nilsson, Karlsson, & Nilsson, 2014; Vollrath et al., 2011), whichdecreases driving performance. This decrease in driving performance is because drivers of highly automated vehicles arenot required to be continuously aware of their driving environment, and can thus undertake other non-driving-related tasks(NDRT) (Jamson et al., 2013). Such a shift in engagement can critically affect visual attention by increasing the amount oftime the driver glances out of the loop (Llaneras, Salinger, & Green, 2013), as well as physical behaviors, such as whenthe driver’s hand is removed from the steering wheel. Semi- and highly automated cars still require human involvementin driving tasks, and thus the attention of the driver is still required (Koo et al., 2015). That is, the involvement of driversis necessary on certain occasions, such as when resuming manual control in an emergency by switching from a NDRT tomanual operation of the vehicle. Given that research in this field is at a relatively early stage, the issues and phenomena thatarise when humans interact with highly automated systems require careful attention.

1.1. Non-driving-related task and vehicle automation

The cognitive, physical, and visual skills of drivers have traditionally played an important role in driving performance(Ball, Owsley, Sloane, Roenker, & Bruni, 1993; Matthews, Sparkes, & Bygrave, 1996; Young & Regan, 2007). Previous studieson secondary task demonstrated that drivers are not allowed to perform these tasks while driving owing to a decrease indriving performance (Blanco, Biever, Gallagher, & Dingus, 2006; Horberry, Anderson, Regan, Triggs, & Brown, 2006) and sit-uation awareness (Baumann, Rösler, & Krems, 2007; Ma & Kaber, 2005). Although some researchers have argued that newassistive technologies allow drivers to physically interact less frequently with vehicles, this does not imply that drivers cancompletely ignore driving tasks (Richards & Stedmon, 2016).

An assistive system can change drivers’ behaviors, such as enabling them to engage in NDRT (Carsten, Lai, Barnard,Jamson, & Merat, 2012; Jamson et al., 2013; Rudin-Brown & Parker, 2004). Rudin-Brown and Parker (2004) indicated thatdrivers using ACC are more likely to shift their attention from their driving task to a secondary task, which has an overallnegative effect on their driving performance. In addition, Jamson et al. (2013) demonstrated that drivers are more activelyinvolved in secondary tasks when using an assistive system, shifting their visual attention from the road, compared to whendriving manually. Carsten et al. (2012) investigated how driver engagement in secondary tasks changes depending on thelevel of automation, which is based on the number of assistive systems in the vehicle. Their results revealed that driversengage more in secondary tasks as the level of automation increases, thereby reducing drivers’ attention and causing themto take their eyes off the road.

According to the Society of Automotive Engineers (SAE), every system incorporated into a vehicle should allow the driverto complete a task within 15 s while the vehicle is parked (SAE J2364). To date, however, there have been no guidelines andonly a few studies on the types of tasks that drivers can undertake in a highly automated vehicle.

1.2. Driver’s transition of control in HAD

Vehicle automation does not necessarily imply a lack of human intervention in the manipulation of the vehicle (Casner,Hutchins, & Norman, 2016; Gold, Damböck, Bengler, & Lorenz, 2013; Gold, Damböck, Lorenz, & Benfler, 2013). Drivers arestill required to be able to take control of their vehicle in an emergency, such as when approaching the end of a road or whenfaced with an unexpected hazard (Gold & Bengler, 2014; Merat & Lee, 2012). In previous studies, the task of resuming controlof a vehicle from a highly automated situation was referred to as a ‘‘takeover” (Gold, Damböck, Lorenz, et al., 2013; Walch,Langer, Baumann, & Weber, 2015). The ability of drivers to resume control of their vehicle under a highly automated driving(HAD) situation mostly focuses on the environmental factors affecting such a takeover (Gold, Körber, Lechner, & Bengler,2016), the influence of visual attention on the resulting takeover (Merat, Jamson, Lai, Daly, & Carsten, 2014), and the timerequired for drivers to completely resume control of the vehicle (Giesler & Muller, 2013; Gold & Bengler, 2014; Gold,Damböck, Lorenz, et al., 2013). Studies have emphasized the amount of time required by driver to resume control of the vehi-cle and when an alert should be issued. Researchers have suggested that a request should be issued between 5.7 s and 8.8 sprior to takeover under a HAD situation, and the mean time required to re-engage control is between 2.1 s and 4.1 s (Giesler& Muller, 2013; Gold & Bengler, 2014; Gold, Damböck, Lorenz, et al., 2013).

Secondary tasks demanded under HAD situations have been identified as playing a critical role in the takeover time andperformance (Radlmayr, Gold, Lorenz, Farid, & Bengler, 2014). Radlmayr et al. (2014) stated that there is no significant dif-ference in the effect on a driver’s takeover for different NDRT. Gold, Damböck, Lorenz, et al. (2013) investigated drivers’ take-over by measuring the time required to complete a task using a driving simulator. Their study involved presenting theparticipants with critical driving situations in which they were required to carry out cognitively and manually demandingsecondary tasks under HAD conditions. The results of this experiment showed that there is no significant difference in thetwo conditions when taking over control of a vehicle. However, other studies have found that the driver’s state before takingover control of the vehicle greatly affects the quality of takeover performance (Naujoks, Mai, & Neukum, 2014).

Page 3: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

622 S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631

1.3. Importance of workload in HAD

One area of interest when discussing driving performance is the workload effect. Extensive research has been conductedregarding the effect of the workload during manual driving as well as when performing secondary tasks. For instance, duringmanual driving, there is an increase in the workload when drivers perform secondary tasks, leading to an increase in out-of-the-loop-type situations and degradation of driving performance (Horberry et al., 2006; Lansdown, Brook-Carter, & Kersloot,2004; Merat, Jamson, Lai, & Carsten, 2012; Niezgoda, Tarnowski, Kruszewski, & Kaminski, 2015; Yoon, Lim, & Ji, 2015).

With respect to HAD, studies focusing on the effect of the workload are at the initial stage. Young and Stanton (2002) haveindicated that a driver’s cognitive workload and automation are closely related. That is, in an HAD environment, the impactof a vehicle leads to changes in mental processing by drivers (Beattie, Baillie, Halvey, & McCall, 2014). Similarly, automatedvehicles have characteristics that allow drivers to engage in tasks other than driving, which might influence both their driv-ing performance and mindset (Banks, Stanton, & Halvey, 2014). In the early studies conducted by Stanton and Young (1998),the authors claimed that although vehicle automation reduces the workload, it also creates problems such as inefficiencywhen reclaiming control of the vehicle. Engagement in a secondary task while driving an automated vehicle plays a criticalrole in the driving performance, particularly when the driver is asked to take over control of the vehicle (Merat et al., 2012).However, very few studies have addressed the issue of the workload imposed by HAD, in which we might propose that theparadigm of ‘‘secondary tasks” changes to ‘‘NDRT”; that is, not only is driver overload a concern but also driver underload(Beattie et al., 2014; Jamson et al., 2013).

1.4. Research objective

This study focused on natural activities that might be performed by the drivers in a level 3 vehicle, which feature limitedself-driving automation. A driving simulator setup was used to investigate the drivers’ abilities to resume control in responseto takeover request alert after carrying out different types of NDRT in a HAD. The task workload was also of interest uponresuming control of the vehicle. We explored the relationship between the takeover performance and workload for eachNDRT. From the results obtained, we attempted to provide insight into the influence of NDRTs and the workload incurredunder HAD situations when a driver is required to take over control of the vehicle, and to identify issues that both industryand academia must consider in future research on autonomous vehicles.

2. Method

A laboratory experiment was conducted using a driving simulator to determine the influence of NDRT when drivers arerequired to resume control of the vehicle in a HAD situation. The simulator assisted drivers with both lateral and longitudinaldriving tasks, such that participants could engage in NDRT. This experiment was done in accordance with a protocolapproved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB), with written informed consent from all subjects.

2.1. Participants

Twenty-seven participants (17 males and 10 females) with a valid driver’s license were recruited for the driving simulatorexperiment through an advertisement placed on an Internet blog. Monetary compensation was offered for their participa-tion. They were aged between 24 and 38 (M = 29.1, SD = 3.78). The participants had between 1 and 15 years of driving expe-rience (M = 5.8, SD = 4.57). All the participants had normal vision and did not wear glasses during the eye-trackingexperiment.

2.2. Experimental design

The study consisted of a within-subject design of drivers’ takeover performance measurements (gaze on time, fixationtime, hands-on time, and takeover time) and subjective assessment of workload by NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX) forthree different NDRT (interacting with entertainment console, smartphone interaction, and video watching) in a HADcontext.

2.2.1. Takeover performance measure and self-reported workloadSince previous studies on takeover performance, gaze reaction time, first fixation on the road, the amount of time in

which the drivers’ hands were on the steering wheel, and the overall takeover time were selected as dependent variablesto collect data on takeover performance (Gold et al., 2016; Lorenz, Kerschbaum, & Schumann, 2014).

Table 1 lists the variables for the measurement of the performance based on the visual, physical, and overall performanceof the drivers when resuming manual control of a highly automated vehicle. Therefore, as a measure of visual performance,we determined the time required for drivers to fix their eyes on the road after an alert was issued, and the time required fortheir gaze to switch from a NDRT to the view ahead (through the windshield). To determine the level of physical perfor-mance, we selected the time required for a driver to grasp the steering wheel. We also focused on determining whether

Page 4: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

Table 1Summary of objective measurement variables used for the experiment.

Category Measurementvariables

Description

Visual performance Gaze-on time Time taken by driver to complete the first gaze reaction through the windshield after the issuance ofa takeover request

Fixation time Time taken by driver to complete the first fixation on the road (target in this case) after the issuanceof a takeover request

Physical performance Hands on steeringwheel time

Time taken by the driver to grasp the steering wheel after the issuance of a takeover request

Takeover performance Takeover time Total time taken by the driver to take over manual control of the vehicle after the issuance of atakeover requestIn this case, we considered that the driver had resumed control when their hands were completelyon the steering wheel and there was an interaction with the vehicle, such as a 5� rotation of thewheel or an interaction with the brake pedal

S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 623

visual performance and physical performance varied between tasks for the takeover performance of drivers, which has beenof significant importance since the study by Llaneras et al. (2013). Lastly, the overall performance was measured based onthe takeover time. The takeover time measures how quickly a driver can resume manual control of a vehicle upon the issu-ance of an alert. We measured the resumption of manual control as the moment when the participants interacted with thelongitudinal controller or turned the steering wheel by more than 5�, which was possible because the emergency alert totake over control required participants to change to the middle lane and slow down.

The NASA-TLX was used to assess the self-reported workload imposed on participants. This index is a retrospective mea-sure that allows participants to apply a rating after a set of tasks is completed (Bustamante & Spain, 2008). The index used sixdimensions to provide a rating of the overall workload, including the mental demand, physical demand, temporal demand,performance, effort, and degree of frustration. The rating of the task load index provided a range between 0 and 100 points.

2.2.2. Non-driving-related task (NDRT)NDRT selected for this study were based on previous studies (Kim et al., 2015; Llaneras et al., 2013; Merat et al., 2014) on

natural tasks desired to be performed or possible to be carried out in automated driving. Selection of tasks were donedepending on the degree of engagement required, specially taking into consideration the need for visual and physicaldemands. Using this approach, (1) interacting with an entertainment console, (2) watching a video, and (3) interacting witha smartphone were selected as NDRT for this experiment (Table 1). The interaction task with the entertainment consoleinvolves continuously searching for a radio station. The participants were asked to search for a radio station, and once itwas found, the participants were asked to search for the next radio station. The participants were asked to search for as manyradio stations as possible, such that the participants continuously engaged in the activity. Although the performance of theusers was measured, the results were not taken into consideration for the experimental analysis. For the video watching task,participants were required to watch a video of individuals (male and female) passing a ball. They were asked to count howmany times the male actors made a pass and inform the experimenter the total number of passes after the takeover requestwas issued. The participants were told that counting the number of passes was important, such that the participants willneed to pay attention to the video. Finally, for the smartphone interaction task, participants were asked to play the smart-phone game ‘‘1–50,” which involves pressing numbers from 1 to 50 as quickly as possible. This game was selected for theexperiment because it is easy to understand, and requires visual attention as well as physical interaction. We explainedto the participants that their mission was to obtain the best score in the game to keep them focused on the task.

2.3. Driving simulator

The driving simulator featured a real car seat that the participants could adjust as desired, a Logitech� MOMO� RacingForce Feedback Wheel and Pedal, and a 55-inch Samsung Smart television set. For the driving simulator, we used the CityCar software. To provide the participants with auto pilot mode, an experimenter in a separate room with an extra Logitech�

MOMO� Racing Force Feedback Wheel and Pedal simulated the auto driving by Wizard of Oz method. The experimenterswitched the input device for each transition of console. In addition, a SensoMotoric Instruments (SMI) eye-tracker was usedto collect data on the participants’ eye movements. Two video cameras were used to record the actions of the participants.For the experimental tasks, we used an LG G2 smartphone and an Apple iPad Air 2.

2.4. Procedure

Prior to taking part in the experiment, participants were asked to complete a brief demographic questionnaire on basicinformation and questions related to the driving experience. Next, we explained the processes of the driving simulator, theoverall objective of the experiment, and HAD. Next, the participants were given a practice session of approximately 5 min (or

Page 5: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

624 S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631

more if desired) to drive with the simulator along a road similar in characteristics to the one used for the experiment tofamiliarize themselves with the process of driving the simulator. They were also able to experience takeover request andpractice the transition from HAD to manual driving. During the practice session, participants did not engage in any NDRT,thus no task was given for the HAD (see Fig. 1).

The main experiment consisted of three sessions given in counterbalanced order, in which participants engaged in one ofthe NDRT for each session. In each session, the participants were asked to take over control of the vehicle at three pointsalong the overall track (after approximately 3.5, 8, and 14 km) while performing the same NDRT. At the end of the session,a self-assessment of the workload based on the NASA-TLX questionnaire was collected. The overall track was 15 km long, andthe takeover request was in the form of a beep sound warning. The takeover request was issued in a straight road segment, ina non-emergency situation, when the number of lanes changed (from 3 lanes to 4, and from 4 lanes to 3). Although the take-over was issued on a straight road segment, after re-engaging control of the vehicle, the participants were required to changelanes to maintain the second lane of the road. Manual driving was at a speed of 60 km/h. Before the beginning of each ses-sion, we explained the NDRT that participants would be involved in. Then, eye-tracking calibration was performed for eachindividual session. Between sessions, participants were allowed to take a break between 5 and 10 min. The overall experi-ment took approximately 90–120 min.

2.5. Data collection

An SMI goggle-type eye-tracker was used to collect data on the participants’ eye movements and gaze during the exper-iment. This wearable goggle-type eye-tracker records binary input data from both eyes at a sampling rate of 60 Hz. From theeye-tracking, we collected data on the time required for a participant’s gaze to move from a NDRT to a driving task (in-loop),and the time required for the driver’s eyes to be fixed on the road using SMI BeGaze 3.4 software. Two video recordersrecorded the takeover performance of the participants for each session. The video recording was done to obtain informationon the overall behavior when taking over control of the vehicle as well as quantitative data on the time required for the par-ticipants to grasp the steering wheel. For the quantitative data, each video was analyzed using the Adobe Premiere Pro CS6video editing software at a sampling rate of 60 fps and experimenters recorded each video by observation. To ensure relia-bility of the data obtained from video analysis, data collection was done by three different experimenters and cross-checked.

Fig. 1. Description of the experimental procedure.

Page 6: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 625

A total of 243 takeover trial data was gathered for the take-over performance, 81 for each NDRT. For the statistical anal-ysis, we excluded data obtained from the first trial of the first session (9 takeover data for each task) of the takeover perfor-mance experiment as the first trial of the takeover task from the first session was used as part of the practice session and wasnot taken into consideration for the statistical analysis. Since all participants participated in all NDRT, a total of eight take-over trial data was collected for each participant. Giving a total of 216 take-over data, with 72 for each NDRT. Subjective dataof participants’ workload was collected at the end of each session, thus a total of 27 responses for each NDRT were obtainedfor the statistical analysis.

2.6. Statistical analysis

We conducted three sets of analysis. First, we analyzed the effects of a NDRT on the takeover performance. For the anal-ysis of the takeover performance, a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used for the dependent variables: gaze-ontime, fixation time, hands-on time, and takeover time (a = 0.05). We then examined the effects of NDRT on drivers’ workloadwhen they resumed control of the vehicle as measured by the NASA-TLX for all sub-scales and overall workload score by theone-way ANOVA. Before carrying out the analysis, the Shapiro–Wilk test of normality was carried out to check normality.The significant difference was further examined by post-hoc pairwise comparison using the Tukey’s HSD test. Finally, wecompared the relationship between each workload dimension and the overall workload on the takeover performance.

3. Results

3.1. NDRT and re-assuming control of the vehicle

The effect of NDRT when re-assuming control of the vehicle was examined. The ANOVA results showed that there weresignificant differences between tasks in terms of the gaze-on time (F (2, 213) = 6.566, p < 0.01), time to first fixation (F(2, 213) = 6.400, p < 0.01), and overall takeover time (F (2, 213) = 3.914, p < 0.05). However, there was no significant effectfor the hands on the steering wheel time (F (2, 213) = 1.002, p = 0.369) (Table 2).

Post-hoc analysis revealed that the shifting of the participants’ gaze to the windshield was significantly faster for theinteraction with entertainment console (task 1) relative to the viewing tasks, which required the participants to use theirsmartphone (task 2) or watch a video (task 3) (Table 2). The same result was apparent for the time taken for the participantsto fix their eyes on the road. Finally, for the overall takeover time, the fastest response was obtained for the task involvingwatching a video, followed by the task involving interacting with a radio, and lastly, the task in which the driver interactedwith a smartphone.

3.2. Workload for NDRT

The Shapiro–Wilk test of normality gave the following statistical results for each of the tasks: W(27) = 0.960 (p = 0.362),W(27) = 0.938 (p = 0.109), andW(27) = 0.974 (p = 0.705). From the results of the normality analysis, we can conclude that thenull hypothesis was not rejected; therefore, the normality was not rejected. The results obtained from the ANOVA of theworkload for the different tasks showed that each task had a significant effect (F (2, 78) = 3.978, p < 0.05) (Table 3). Apost-hoc analysis showed that task 1, i.e., interaction with the entertainment console, scored significantly lower than task2, interaction with smartphone, and task 3, video watching.

A detailed analysis of the NASA-TLX dimension was carried out to determine which dimensions of the workload wereinvolved. The results showed that the task applied had a significant effect on some of the workload dimensions (Table 3).For instance, mental demand (F(2, 78) = 4.238, p < 0.05), temporal demand (F(2, 78) = 6.643, p < 0.01), and effort (F(2, 78)= 3.819, p < 0.05), all exhibited a significant difference depending on the task, while physical demand (F(2, 78) = 0.404,p = 0.669), performance (F(2, 78) = 2.548, p = 0.085), and level of frustration (F(2, 78) = 1.444, p = 0.242) did not.

Table 2Results of objective measurements of taking over control during HAD while interacting with a different task. Time in seconds (s).

Task Mean gaze-on time(SD)

Mean fixation time(SD)

Mean hands-on steering wheel time(SD)

Mean takeover time(SD)

Entertainment consoleinteraction

0.52(0.33)

0.80(0.40)

1.40(0.29)

1.62(0.36)

Smartphone interaction 0.66(0.27)

0.96(0.25)

1.41(0.30)

1.60(0.36)

Video watching 0.70(0.36)

0.98(0.33)

1.48(0.48)

1.80(0.58)

P-value 0.002** 0.002** 0.369 0.021*

Sig. at *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01.

Page 7: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

Table 3ANOVA results of subjective measurement through NASA-TLX questionnaire and its dimensions with respect to non-driving-related tasks conducted beforeresuming control of the vehicle.

Task Overallworkload

NASA-TLX dimensions

Mentaldemand

Physicaldemand

Temporaldemand

Performance Effort Frustration

Entertainment consoleinteraction

40.9(14.1)

44.8(20.4)

39.5(17.9)

37.7(18.9)

39.3(22.8)

43.6(20.4)

40.4(55.6)

Smartphone interaction 49.0(12.1)

59.8(16.9)

35.4(22.6)

41.3(19.0)

50.8(19.5)

56.1(20.0)

50.4(21.7)

Video watching 51.6(17.0)

57.1(22.8)

40.2(22.1)

57.5(25.2)

49.0(17.7)

57.7(21.2)

48.0(23.6)

P-value 0.023 0.018 0.669 0.002 0.085 0.026 0.242

Notation for post-hoc analysis = “a” & “b”Sig. at *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01

Fig. 2. Results of workload assessment. Task 1 = interacting with entertainment console (n = 27), Task 2 = smartphone interaction (n = 27), and Task3 = video watching task (n = 27).

626 S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631

A post-hoc analysis revealed that task 1, interaction with the entertainment console, had a significantly low score interms of mental demand and effort relative to tasks 2 and 3. Meanwhile, for the temporal demand, tasks 1 and 2 weregrouped as significantly low relative to interacting with the smartphone (Fig. 2).

3.3. Correlation between subjective workload and takeover performance

A correlation analysis was carried out between the results obtained from the NASA-TLX questionnaire and the resultsobtained from the objectively recorded measurements. The results showed that the overall workload was negatively corre-lated with the takeover performance (r = �0.203, p < 0.01) (Table 4). This indicates that an increase in the workloaddecreases the takeover time. Moreover, a significant positive correlation was identified between the mental demand andthe results obtained from the experiment addressing the amount of gaze-on time (r = 0.148, p < 0.05) and fixation time(r = 0.158, p < 0.05), while there was a negative correlation for the takeover performance (r = �0.198, p < 0.01). Surprisingly,for the physical demand dimension, a negative correlation was associated with the gaze-on time (r = �0.208, p < 0.01), fix-ation time (r = �0.174, p < 0.01), hands on the steering wheel time (r = �0.255, p < 0.001), and takeover time (r = �0.242,p < 0.001). The statistical output indicates that the correlation coefficient for the hands-on time was the strongest, followedby the takeover time. For the temporal demand dimension, there was a significant negative correlation for the hands-on time(r = �0.160, p < 0.05) and takeover time (r = �0.148, p < 0.05). For the performance dimension, the gaze-on time (r = 0.245,p < 0.001), fixation time (r = 0.255, p < 0.001), hands-on time (r = 0.167, p < 0.05), and takeover time (r = 0.090, p < 0.05) wereall found to have positive correlations. Finally, the frustration dimension was found to have a negative correlation withrespect to the takeover time (r = �0.162, p < 0.05). As indicated earlier, all the dimensions of the NASA-TLX were not corre-

Page 8: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

Table 4Results from correlation analysis between subjective workload of NASA-TLX and objective measurements during takeover experiment.

Takeover performance Overall workload NASA-TLX dimensions

Mental demand Physical demand Temporal demand Performance Effort Frustration

Gaze-on time 0.059 0.148a �0.208b 0.023 0.245c 0.039 �0.009Fixation time 0.070 0.158a �0.174b 0.003 0.255c 0.084 �0.036Hands on steering wheel time �0.131 �0.077 �0.255c �0.160a 0.167a �0.107 �0.118Takeover time �0.203b �0.198b �0.242c �0.148a 0.090b �0.191 �0.162a

Sig. at a: p < 0.05, b: p < 0.01, c: p < 0.001.

S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 627

lated with the experimental performance, and depending on the dimension of the workload, the correlation coefficient wasfound to have different indices.

A Pearson’s correlation analysis of the NASA-TLX dimensions and takeover performance was carried out with respect toeach task. The results of the analysis showed that there were significant differences in the correlation for each task. Forinstance, for task 1, that is, interaction with the entertainment console, the overall workload was significantly correlatedwith a positive slope for the gaze-on time (r = 0.274, p < 0.05) and fixation time (r = 0.313, p < 0.01). The conclusion is thatthe mental demand, performance, and effort dimensions were significantly correlated with the gaze-on time and fixationtime (Table 5). For task 2, interacting with a smartphone when a takeover request is issued, there was a significantly negativecorrelation between the amount of time the drivers’ hands were on the steering wheel with respect to the mental demand,physical demand, temporal demand, and overall workload (p < 0.01). At the same time, the takeover time, mental demand(r = �0.347, p < 0.01), and temporal demand (r = � 0.267, p < 0.05) were found to have a significantly negative correlation.Finally, for video watching task, the workload was found to have a negative correlation with the hands-on time(r = �0.231, p < 0.05) and takeover time (r = �0.360, p < 0.001). A detailed analysis of the workload dimension demonstratedthat there was a significantly positive correlation for the performance dimension with respect to the gaze-on time (p < 0.5)and fixation time (p < 0.5) for interaction with the entertainment console; and gaze-on time (p < 0.01), fixation time(p < 0.01) and hands-on time (p < 0.01) for video watching task. For smartphone interaction task, there was no significantcorrelation between performance measure and takeover behavioral measures. Meanwhile, for interaction with the entertain-ment console, the correlation with NASA-TLX dimensions were found to have a positive slope, while for smartphone inter-action and video watching task, negative slope between takeover behavioral measures and NASA-TLX were presented(Table 5). For instance, mental demand had a significantly negative correlation with respect to the takeover. The physicaldemand dimension was found to have the strongest negative correlation with the gaze-on time, fixation time, hands-ontime, and takeover time (p < 0.001). For temporal demand, the takeover time was the only item that had a significant cor-relation (r = �0.248, p < 0.05). Finally, the effort was negatively correlated with the fixation time (r = �0.243, p < 0.05),hands-on time (r = �0.244, p < 0.05), and takeover time (r = �0.358, p < 0.001). In addition, for the level of frustration dimen-sion, there was a significantly negative correlation for the fixation time (r = �0.303, p < 0.01), hands-on time (r = �0.244,p < 0.05), and takeover time (r = �0.376, p < 0.001).

Table 5Correlation analysis between NASA-TLX dimensions and objective measurements regarding takeover performance for each task.

Interaction with the entertainmentconsole

Smartphone interaction Video watching task

Gazeon

Fix. Handson

Takeover Gazeon

Fix. Handson

Takeover Gaze on Fix. Handson

Takeover

Mentaldemand

r 0.362b 0.351b 0.204 �0.047 �0.006 �0.137 �0.326b �0.347b �0.014 �0.071 �0.124 �0.244b

p 0.002 0.002 0.079 0.688 0.960 0.253 0.006 0.004 0.902 0.557 0.287 0.034Physical

demandr 0.074 0.108 0.134 0.018 �0.123 �0.140 �0.309b �0.183 �0.445c �0.514c �0.447c �0.469c

p 0.531 0.362 0.252 0.879 0.304 0.242 0.009 0.113 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000Temporal

demandr 0.039 0.123 �0.064 �0.104 �0.119 �0.142 �0.266a �0.267a �0.103 �0.198 �0.216 �0.248a

p 0.740 0.298 0.584 0.371 0.318 0.236 0.026 0.027 0.380 0.099 0.063 0.031Performance r 0.234a 0.232a 0.184 0.120 0.043 �0.026 �0.006 0.067 0.359b 0.383c 0.325b 0.129

p 0.045 0.048 0.115 0.303 0.717 0.829 0.958 0.587 0.002 0.001 0.004 0.265Effort r 0.275a 0.293a 0.167 �0.059 �0.105 �0.111 �0.181 �0.137 �0.169 �0.243a �0.244a �0.358c

p 0.018 0.012 0.152 0.611 0.382 0.354 0.135 0.261 0.147 0.041 0.035 0.001Frustration r 0.104 0.150 0.118 0.052 �0.099 �0.175 �0.135 �0.024 �0.183 �0.303b �0.244a �0.376c

p 0.337 0.204 0.315 0.657 0.408 0.141 0.265 0.847 0.115 0.010 0.035 0.001Overall

workloadr 0.274a 0.313b 0.186 0.001 �0.117 �0.205 �0.336b �0.235 �0.141 �0.231 �0.231a �0.360c

p 0.018 0.007 0.110 0.992 0.328 0.084 0.004 0.052 0.227 0.052 0.046 0.001

Sig. at a: p < 0.05, b: p < 0.01, c: p < 0.001.

Page 9: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

628 S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631

4. Discussion

Different types of NDRT influence a driver’s behavior when resuming control of a vehicle in a HAD situation. Based on theresults of previous studies (Endsley & Kiris, 1995; Giesler & Muller, 2013; Lorenz et al., 2014), it was possible to identify thatvisual attention, when out of the loop, is one of the most important factors related to the task of resuming control of an HAD.In the experiment, eye-tracking data, such as the time required for the participants to shift their gaze to the windshield or thetime required to fix their eyes on the road, were significantly affected by the characteristics of the NDRT being performed.Therefore, this had a significant effect on the total time taken for the participants to takeover control of the vehicle bydemanding more time for activities requiring more visual attention. Louw, Madigan, Carsten, and Merat (2016) that driverout of the loop due to automation influenced on the gaze fixation when reengagement to the driving task was asked, whichvaries within one second. As presented in the previous research by Louw et al. (2016), results from the present researchshowed that depending visual attentional resource demanded from the NDRT, the time taken for drivers to shift and fixon the road varies also for natural task in HAD. From the results of post-hoc analyses, it became clear why interacting withan entertainment console is significantly different from watching a video or interacting with a smartphone. Based on previ-ous studies focusing on secondary tasks performed during manual driving, visual attention was considered one of the mostimportant factors of driving performance (Horberry et al., 2006; Lee, Lee, & Boyle, 2007). The results from this study haveconfirmed that visual attention still plays an important role in HAD. In the experiment, tasks 2 and 3 required high levelsof visual attention, and even complete visual attention, which also influenced the time required for the participants to shifttheir visual attention back into the loop. In a real driving context, this might differ in that interacting with the entertainmentconsole is a task that is discrete and does not require full and continuous visual attention, allowing drivers to shift theirvisual attention from the loop to out of the loop. On the other hand, watching a video or using a smartphone required con-tinuous and total visual attention of the participant on the task, which was reflected in the results of the gaze-on time.

For the time that the participants’ hands were on the steering wheel, there was no significant difference between thethree tasks, even though there were tasks that did not require the drivers to perform any physical activity, such as watchinga video. Nevertheless, the fact that there was no significant difference could be explained by the instruction to the partici-pants to keep their hands off the steering wheel owing to the automated lateral control of the vehicle. On the other hand, theoverall takeover time was less when the driver was smartphone interaction (task 2), followed by interacting with the enter-tainment console (task 1), and finally, video watching (task 3). During manual driving, the interaction effect between thevisual demand and physical demand of a secondary task demonstrated an influence on driver distraction (Young, Regan,& Hammer, 2007). During HAD, it can be deduced that visual demand does not only influence the process of taking over con-trol of a vehicle, but in some respects, an interaction with the physical demands of the task will also influence takeover per-formance. That is, as proposed by previous researchers, NDRT demand a degree of physical, as well as mental, and visualinvolvement (Beattie et al., 2014; Carsten et al., 2012; Casner et al., 2016; Gold & Bengler, 2014; Gold, Damböck, Lorenz,et al., 2013; Hergeth, Lorenz, Vilimek, & Krems, 2016; Merat et al., 2012, 2014; Naujoks et al., 2014). However, in thisresearch, we focus on natural task with a combination of physical and visual resource demand as well as cognitive demand,and we were interested in analyzing the effect of each task on the transition of control in HAD.

A comparison of the gaze-on time, initial fixation time, hands on the steering wheel time, and takeover time of a taskallows an understanding of certain characteristics of a driver. However, the workload results revealed that the perceiveddegree of physical demand had no significant effect on resuming control of the vehicle. As indicated by the results of thehands on the steering wheel time, the three tasks did not show any significant difference, which could explain why therewas also no significant difference for the subjective assessment. Moreover, since the study by Matthews, Legg, andCharlton (2003), physical demand based on a self-rating of the NASA-TLX has shown certain limitations when assessingthe physical dimension compared to other dimensions. That is, the participants are less aware of the physical demand expe-rienced during driving simulator experiments when asked to conduct NDRT that are common in a driving context, and arethus potentially automated. Furthermore, during the experiment, the participants were asked to re-engage control of thevehicle, which is a new task for drivers, and requires high mental and cognitive engagement, while the act of moving theirhands towards the steering wheel is more automatic. The effort expended by the participants when switching their attentionfrom one task to another, regardless of what they were doing, is also illustrated by results obtained from the correlation anal-ysis between the NASA-TLX and experimental data, where physical demand showed a significantly negative correlation withthe eye-tracking data, as well as the hands on the steering wheel time and the overall takeover time.

Some of the results from the analysis exhibited a significantly negative correlation between the takeover performancemeasure and workload dimensions. For instance, the takeover time exhibited a significantly negative correlation withrespect to the overall workload score of the NASA-TLX. Also, a significant negative correlation was observed between themental demand and the takeover time for smartphone interaction and video watching task. However, an attempt was madeto understand this result by applying the workload underload theory (Hancock, Williams, & Manning, 1995). Young andStanton (2002) stated that a driver underload may have as much of a negative effect on the performance as an overload, indi-cating that a driver’s workload should be maintained at an optimal level. Previous researches on attention and vigilance alsostated that the very low level of workload leads to decrease in performance and error rate (Wickens & McCarley, 2007). Thus,presenting an invested U-shaped relationship between performance and workload, which states that certain level of work-load in necessary to maintain high performance and constant attention (Young & Stanton, 2002). Based on previous

Page 10: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 629

researches, for the underload case with NDRT, we obtained results indicating that an underload might have a negative rela-tionship with takeover performance of the drivers. During HAD, drivers are under less pressure to pay attention to the driv-ing task, making it difficult to re-engage after a sudden request compared to during a low level of automation. One possibleexplanation for this is that drivers tend to consider it a high workload when asked to manually drive compared to HAD, evenunder additional NDRT, as indicated by the results of a self-rating through a NASA-TLX questionnaire (Young & Stanton,2002). Moreover, another point is that, drivers’ transition of control requires to shift from a NDRT to the driving task, thatis, at the moment of takeover there might be a sudden increase of workload from low degree of workload to high degreeof workload. Meanwhile, the results obtained from the questionnaire helped to establish that the performance dimensionis the only dimension that has a significantly positive correlation with the time required for the driver to resume controlof the vehicle. The addition of a new task for HAD, which consists of taking over control of the vehicle, increased the per-ceived workload, while there was no significant difference between the three tasks. This results were also seen in the cor-relation analysis between the NASA-TLX and the objective measurement, were the slope for interaction with theentertainment console is slightly different than for smartphone interaction task and video watching task. There was a sig-nificantly positive correlation between the subjective and objective measurement for interaction with the entertainmentconsole. The results of the correlation for interaction with the entertainment console could be shown as positive correlationdue to the characteristic of the NDRT as it can be classified as a habitual action conducted by drivers during manual driving,and they were accustomed to it. This was also easily observed in the behaviors of the drivers during the experiment. Most ofthe participants shifted their eyes from the entertainment console to the road as during manual driving. Moreover, it waspossible to see from their behavior that they interacted with the entertainment system using only one hand, thereby prepar-ing for any emergency situation that might occur, which was different from their behavior during the video-viewing andsmartphone interaction tasks.

5. Conclusion

This study investigated the effects of NDRT when re-assuming control of a vehicle in a HAD situation. Three representa-tive tasks that drivers frequently and willingly performed in a highly automated vehicle were selected. The results from theexperiment showed that the effects of different tasks were significantly relevant when the driver resumes control of thevehicle. Based on the results, it can be concluded that automated systems should not change the driver’s responsibilitieswhen driving; rather, such systems should change the role that the driver must play within the vehicle.

The results of this research provide insights into how driver behaviors are changing due to the introduction of automatedsystems, and how these changes affect conventional driving tasks. We believe that the main task of a driver will shift fromcontrolling to monitoring, and that it will be important for the driver to be able to quickly resume control of the vehicle. Thisstudy yielded basic results on the influence of NDRT, as well as the workload when a driver resumes control of a vehicle afterbeing in a HAD situation. The results indicate that a mental underload have a negative relationship with drivers’ behaviorwhen reassuming control of the vehicle. However, there were some limitations in this study. First, we assessed the partic-ipants’ workload using a subjective self-rating of the NASA-TLX at the end of each session. Although this is a widely usedmethodology, the self-assessment of each participant’s workload might have led to a bias in the results obtained as partic-ipants were asked to recall the perceived workload during the engagement with the NDRT. Objective physiological measure-ments of the workload, such as the driver’s heart rate, could reinforce the study, as could an investigation into therelationship among the different situational awareness aspects. Second, although this study compared the influences of dif-ferent NDRT, further research with a control group where only a takeover task is required is necessary to further understandthe behavioral aspect of drivers when the transition from a NDRT to a driving task occurs.

Our results have permitted the understanding of the issues related to a driver’s resumption of control, where the drivermakes a transition from a HAD scenario to manual driving; further research on the aspects and characteristics of NDRT thatcould have a significant influence on the takeover task is required. Furthermore, the question of whether mental workload isstill one of the most important factors affecting changes in driver behaviors and mental processing when there is a transitionfrom manual to autonomous driving should be investigated deeper. Hence, research that focuses on understanding thechanges in a driver’s mental processing and how such an understanding can be utilized to help maximize the driver’s per-formance will be important. The results of this research could offer insight into the human behavior in highly automatedvehicles, although challenges remain in our understanding of how different levels of demands, such as cognitive, physical,and visual, might influence or affect a driver when resuming control of a vehicle.

Acknowledgement

This work was supported in part by the Yonsei University Research Fund (Post Doc. Researcher Supporting Program) of2018 (project no.: 2018-12-0136).

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.11.015.

Page 11: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

630 S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631

References

Banks, V. A., Stanton, N. A., & Halvey, C. (2014). Sub-systems on the road to vehicle automation: Hands and feet free but not ‘mind’ free driving. SafetyScience, 62, 505–514.

Ball, K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., & Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. InvestigativeOphthalmology & Visual Science, 34(11), 3110–3123.

Baumann, M. R., Rösler, D., & Krems, J. F. (2007). Situation awareness and secondary task performance while driving. In International conference onengineering psychology and cognitive ergonomics (pp. 256–263). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

Beattie, D., Baillie, L., Halvey, M., & McCall, R. (2014.). What’s around the corner? Enhancing driver awareness in autonomous vehicle via in-vehicle spatialauditory displays. In NordiCHI’14 (pp. 189–198).

Blanco, M., Biever, W. J., Gallagher, J. P., & Dingus, T. A. (2006). The impact of secondary task cognitive processing demand on driving performance. AccidentAnalysis & Prevention, 38(5), 895–906.

Brauer, K. (2015). Top 10 Advanced Car Technologies by 2020. Forbes. Retrieved from http://www.forbes.com/sites/kbrauer/2015/01/19/top-10-advanced-car-technologies-by-2020/#6730b40ab938.

Bustamante, E. A., & Spain, R. D. (2008). Measurement invariance of the Nasa TLX. In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting(pp. 1522–1526). SAGE Publications.

Carsten, O., Lai, F., Barnard, Y., Jamson, A. H., & Merat, N. (2012). Control task substitution in semiautomated driving does it matter what aspects areautomated? Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 0018720812460246.

Casner, S. M., Hutchins, E. L., & Norman, D. (2016). The challenges of partially automated driving. Communications of the ACM, 59(5), 70–77.Choi, J. K., & Ji, Y. G. (2015). Investigating the importance of trust on adopting an autonomous vehicle. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction,

31(10), 692–702.Endsley, M. R., & Kiris, E. O. (1995). The out-of-the-loop performance problem and level of control in automation. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human

Factors and Ergonomics Society, 37(2), 381–394.Flemish, F., Kelsch, J., Löper, C., Schieben, A., Schindler, J., & Heesen, M. (2008). Cooperative control and active interfaces for vehicle assistance and

automation. FISITA world automotive Congress; Munich.Giesler, B., & Muller, T. (2013). Opportunities and challenges on the route to piloted driving. 4th International Munich chassis symposium, Munich.Gold, C., & Bengler, K. (2014). Taking over control from highly automated vehicles. Advances in Human Aspect of Transportation: Part II, 8, 64–69.Gold, C., Damböck, D., Bengler, K., & Lorenz, L. (2013). Partially automated driving as a fallback level of high automation. Tagung Fahrerassistenz, 5, Munich.Gold, C., Damböck, D., Lorenz, L., & Benfler, K. (2013). ‘‘Take over!” How long does it take to get the driver back into the loop? In Proceedings of the human

factors and ergonomics society 57th annual meeting (pp. 1938–1942).Gold, C., Körber, M., Lechner, D., & Bengler, K. (2016). Taking over control from highly automated vehicles in complex traffic situations the role of traffic

density. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 0018720816634226.Hancock, P. A., Williams, G., & Manning, C. M. (1995). Influence of task demand characteristics on workload and performance. The International Journal of

Aviation Psychology, 5(1), 63–86.Hergeth, S., Lorenz, L., Vilimek, R., & Krems, J. F. (2016). Keep your scanners peeled gaze behavior as a measure of automation trust during highly automated

driving. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 58(3), 509–519.Horberry, T., Anderson, J., Regan, M. A., Triggs, T. J., & Brown, J. (2006). Driver distraction: The effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment

complexity and age on driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38(1), 185–191.Ive, H. P., Sirkin, D., Miller, D., Li, J., & Ju, W. (2015). ‘‘Don’t make me turn this seat around!” Driver passenger activities and positions in autonomous cars. In

Proceedings of the AutomotiveUI ’15 Adjunct, Nottingham, United Kingdom (pp. 50–55).Jamson, A. H., Merat, N., Carsten, O. M., & Lai, F. C. (2013). Behavioural changes in drivers experiencing highly-automated vehicle control in varying traffic

conditions. Transportation Research Part C: Emerging Technologies, 30, 116–125.Kim, H. S., Yoon, S. H., Kim, M. J., & Ji, Y. G. (2015). Deriving future user experience in autonomous vehicle. In Proceedings of the AutomotiveUI ’15 Adjunct,

Nottingham, United Kingdom (pp. 112–117).Koo, J., Kwac, J., Ju, W., Steinert, M., Leifer, L., & Nass, C. (2015). Why did my car just do that? Explaining semi-autonomous driving actions to improve driver

understanding, trust, and performance. International Journal on Interactive Design and Manufacturing (IJIDeM), 9(4), 269–275.Lansdown, T. C., Brook-Carter, N., & Kersloot, T. (2004). Distraction from multiple in-vehicle secondary tasks: Vehicle performance and mental workload

implications. Ergonomics, 47(1), 91–104.Lee, Y. C., Lee, J. D., & Boyle, L. N. (2007). Visual attention in driving: The effects of cognitive load and visual disruption. Human Factors: The Journal of the

Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 49(4), 721–733.Llaneras, R. E., Salinger, J., & Green, C. A. (2013). Human factors issues associated with limited ability autonomous driving systems: Drivers’ allocation of

visual attention to the forward roadway. In Proceedings of the 7th international driving symposium on human factors in driver assessment, training andvehicle design (pp. 92–98).

Lorenz, L., Kerschbaum, P., & Schumann, J. (2014). Designing take over scenarios for automated driving: How does augmented reality support the driver toget back into the loop? In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting (pp. 1681–1685). SAGE Publications.

Louw, T., Madigan, R., Carsten, O., & Merat, N. (2016). Were they in the loop during automated driving? Links between visual attention and crash potential.Injury Prevention. injuryprev-2016.

Ma, R., & Kaber, D. B. (2005). Situation awareness and workload in driving while using adaptive cruise control and a cell phone. International Journal ofIndustrial Ergonomics, 35(10), 939–953.

Matthews, G., Sparkes, T. J., & Bygrave, H. M. (1996). Attentional overload, stress, and simulate driving performance. Human Performance, 9(1), 77–101.Matthews, R., Legg, S., & Charlton, S. (2003). The effect of cell phone type on drivers subjective workload during concurrent driving and conversing. Accident

Analysis & Prevention, 35(4), 451–457.Merat, N., Jamson, A. H., Lai, F. C., & Carsten, O. (2012). Highly automated driving, secondary task performance, and driver state. Human Factors: The Journal of

the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 54(5), 762–771.Merat, N., Jamson, A. H., Lai, F. C., Daly, M., & Carsten, O. M. (2014). Transition to manual: Driver behaviour when resuming control from a highly automated

vehicle. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 27, 274–282.Merat, N., & Lee, J. D. (2012). Preface to the special section on human factors and automation in vehicles: Designing highly automated vehicles with the

driver in mind. Special section: Human factors and automation in vehicle. Human Factors, 54(5), 681–686.Naujoks, F., Mai, C., & Neukum, A. (2014). The effect of urgency of take-over request during highly automated driving under distraction conditions. Advances

in Human Aspect of Transportation: Part I, 47, 431–438.Niezgoda, M., Tarnowski, A., Kruszewski, M., & Kaminski, T. (2015). Towards testing auditory–vocal interfaces and detecting distraction while driving: A

comparison of eye-movement measures in the assessment of cognitive workload. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 32,23–34.

Radlmayr, J., Gold, C., Lorenz, L., Farid, M., & Bengler, K. (2014). How traffic situations and non-driving related tasks affect the take-over quality in highlyautomated driving. In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting (pp. 2063–2067). Sage Publications.

Richards, D., & Stedmon, A. (2016). To delegate or not to delegate: A review of control frameworks for autonomous cars. Applied Ergonomics, 53, 383–388.Rudin-Brown, C. M., & Parker, H. A. (2004). Behavioural adaptation to adaptive cruise control (ACC): Implications for preventive strategies. Transportation

Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7(2), 59–76.Stanton, N. A., & Young, M. S. (1998). Vehicle automation and driving performance. Ergonomics, 41(7), 1014–1028.

Page 12: Transportation Research Part F - Yonsei Universityinteraction.yonsei.ac.kr/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/TRPF.pdf · 2018-12-13 · involved in secondary tasks when using an assistive

S.H. Yoon, Y.G. Ji / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 620–631 631

Strand, N., Nilsson, J., Karlsson, I. C. M., & Nilsson, L. (2014). Semi-automated versus highly automated driving in critical situations caused by automationfailures. Psychology and Behaviour, 27(Part B), 218–228.

Vollrath, M., Schleicher, S., & Gelau, C. (2011). The influence of cruise control and adaptive cruise control on driving behaviour – A driving simulator study.Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 1134–1139.

Walch, M., Langer, K., Baumann, M., & Weber, M. (2015). Autonomous driving: Investigating the feasibility of car-driver handover assistance. Proceeding ofthe AutomotiveUI ’15 Adjunct, 11–18, Nottingham, United Kingdom.

Wickens, C. D., & McCarley, J. S. (2007). Applied attention theory. CRC Press.Yang, J., & Coughlin, J. F. (2014). In-vehicle technology for self-driving cars: Advantages and challenges for aging drivers. International Journal of Automotive

Technology, 15(2), 333–340.Yoon, S. H., Lim, J. H., & Ji, Y. G. (2015). Perceived visual complexity and visual search performance of automotive instrument cluster: A quantitative

measurement study. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 13(12), 890–900.Young, K., & Regan, M. (2007). Driver distraction: A review of the literature. In I. J. Faulks, M. Regan, M. Stevenson, J. Brown, A. Porter, & J. D. Irwin (Eds.),

Distracted driving (pp. 379–405). Sydney, NSW: Australasian College of Road Safety.Young, K., Regan, M., & Hammer, M. (2007). Driver distraction: A review of the literature. Distracted Driving, 379–405.Young, M. S., & Stanton, N. A. (2002). Attention and automation: New perspectives on mental underload and performance. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics

Science, 3(2), 178–194.