what process works for whom: individual differences and

131
University of Pennsylvania University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons ScholarlyCommons Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations 2018 What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms John Raymond Keefe University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations Part of the Clinical Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Keefe, John Raymond, "What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms" (2018). Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations. 3432. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/3432 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/3432 For more information, please contact [email protected].

Upload: others

Post on 18-Jun-2022

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

University of Pennsylvania University of Pennsylvania

ScholarlyCommons ScholarlyCommons

Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations

2018

What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The

Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms

John Raymond Keefe University of Pennsylvania, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations

Part of the Clinical Psychology Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Keefe, John Raymond, "What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms" (2018). Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations. 3432. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/3432

This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/3432 For more information, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The Impact Of What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And The Impact Of Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms Therapy Techniques And Treatment Mechanisms

Abstract Abstract While psychotherapy treatment manuals define the broad structure and targets of a therapy, therapists must decide how to implement treatments with a specific patient. Yet, patients are heterogeneous even within a disorder class, and there is little systematic research to guide a therapist to make principled adaptations. We examined the question of whether individual differences moderate the treatment effects of therapist interventions and both in-session and between-session processes of change, using data from a randomized controlled trial for panic disorder comparing panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy (PFPP) to cognitive-behavioral therapy. In Chapter 1, adherence to PFPP (n = 65) was observer rated in Sessions 2 and 10 to predict panic change after the rated session. Panic-specific interpretations predicted improvements, while non-panic-focused interventions did not. Concordant with dynamic theory, patients with more interpersonal problems benefitted especially from heightened focus on the interplay between interpersonal-emotional conflict and panic. In Chapter 2, we examined whether higher levels of observer-rated emotional expression—a marker of therapeutic engagement—across early PFPP sessions predicted subsequent panic improvements. We hypothesized that this relationship would be moderated by certain personality disorder traits related to emotionality: (1) borderline traits, which denote heightened, labile, dysregulated emotionality; and (2) obsessive-compulsive traits, related to muted, constrained emotionality. As predicted, borderline traits attenuated the otherwise positive relationship between emotional expression and symptom improvement, but obsessive-compulsive traits had inconsistent relationships between trial sites. Finally, in Chapter 3, we built on mediational analyses for both CBT and PFPP (n = 138), examining whether the presence of different psychological vulnerabilities moderated the symptomatic impact of improvement in two mediators of panic change: catastrophic, body-focused interpretation style, and panic-specific reflective functioning (PSRF). Patients beginning treatment with a more catastrophic style benefitted more from improvements in either mediator. Personality disorder traits blunted the impact of improvements in PSRF, but patients with no personality disorder evidenced high benefit from PSRF improvements. This set of findings suggests that personalization of psychotherapy can be empirically grounded. Clinical characteristics may inform how a therapist chooses to intervene, what in-session processes they focus on, and what types of psychological changes they aim to encourage.

Degree Type Degree Type Dissertation

Degree Name Degree Name Doctor of Philosophy (PhD)

Graduate Group Graduate Group Psychology

First Advisor First Advisor Robert J. DeRubeis

Second Advisor Second Advisor Dianne L. Chambless

Keywords Keywords mediation, personalized medicine, psychotherapy, psychotherapy process

Page 3: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

Subject Categories Subject Categories Clinical Psychology

This dissertation is available at ScholarlyCommons: https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/3432

Page 4: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

WHAT PROCESS WORKS FOR WHOM: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND THE

IMPACT OF THERAPY TECHNIQUES AND TREATMENT MECHANISMS

John Raymond Keefe

A DISSERTATION

in

Psychology

Presented to the Faculties of the University of Pennsylvania

in

Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the

Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

2019

Supervisor of Dissertation

_________________________________

Robert J. DeRubeis, PhD, Samuel H. Preston Term Professor in the Social Sciences

Graduate Group Chairperson

__________________________________

John Trueswell, PhD, Professor of Psychology

Dissertation Committee

Dianne L. Chambless, PhD, Professor of Psychology

Sara R. Jaffee, PhD, Professor of Psychology

Page 5: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

ii

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

I must begin my acknowledgments by recognizing the incomparable contribution

and influence of my dissertation advisor Robert DeRubeis on my career and my person.

Like a securely attached parent, Rob steadfastly encouraged me to go out on my own

intellectually and academically, but he was always there with Dutch M&Ms, espresso,

and counsel when I got lost along the way. I greatly admire Rob’s understated

inquisitiveness and flexibility, and his commitment to long-spanning, ambling intellectual

dialogue, which deepened my thinking and work. If I ever have students of my own, I

hope to emulate Rob.

I owe a special debt to Jacques Barber, who by recommendation of my

undergraduate abnormal psychology professor Michelle Reimer adopted a young post-

bac neuroscientist and retrained him as a clinical psychologist at the Center for

Psychotherapy Research. Jacques, along with his then-postdoctoral fellow Kevin

McCarthy, gave generous, warm, sagacious mentorship and great opportunities that made

it even possible for me to apply to graduate school.

My dissertation chair Dianne Chambless modeled for me impeccable grace as a

person and precision in thinking and research design, and I am a better person and

researcher from knowing her. I also must thank Barbara Milrod for bringing me into the

fold of her research team in New York, for downloading into me her gifted clinical

acumen, for her sharp, direct, but always on point critique of my writing and ideas, and

for her almost unbelievable faith in me. I also thank the great professors I have learned

from at Penn, especially Sara Jaffee and Geoff Goodwin.

Page 6: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

iii

The extended research families of Rob and Jacques have served as amazing

intellectual interlocutors and social raconteurs during my degree. I am particularly

grateful for my fellow graduate students in the DeRubeis lab—Lorenzo Lorenzo-Luaces,

Zachary Cohen, Colin Xu, and Thomas Kim. Ramaris German should be particularly

thanked as a surrogate parent on behalf of Rob when he was out of the country my first

semester at Penn. I have sincerely appreciated how Jacques has kept me connected to his

active lab group, and particularly for the strong personal and professional relationships I

formed with Kevin McCarthy, Sigal Zilcha-Mano, Ulrike Dinger, and Nili Solomonov.

I also must give unique thanks to my clinical supervisors at Penn, whose insights

into my patients and my clinical work shaped not only my clinical skills but also my

research: Sydney Pulver, Alan Goldstein, Rob DeRubeis (a twofer), and Melissa Hunt. I

also have to thank the support staff at Penn, who have organized me and tolerated my

bizarre, last-minute requests and queries with warmth, competence, and humor: Yuni

Thornton, Joan Massimo, Pat Spann, Claire Ingulli, and Paul Newlon.

I also wish to express gratitude to past teachers who have shaped me: Allen

Schneider, my undergraduate mentor in psychobiology; Sara Hiebert-Burch, who

unwittingly talked me out of quitting the Honors Program at Swarthmore; and Margie

Craig, my high-school journalism mentor.

To get through a doctorate a whole person, it is important to have a “life worth

living” (as Marsha Linehan emphasized), and for making that possible I am in profound

gratitude for the many people who fill my life with joy: my erstwhile friends back home

in Virginia (Justin Belcher, Jessica Johnson, Caroline Mayen, Kelsey Sowers, Michael

Terlecki); my friends in Philly (Hannah Blake, Jen Brown, Celia Caust-Ellenbogen, Lisa

Page 7: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

iv

Drago, Lori Felipe-Barkin, Lindsay Friedman, Elizabeth Jenkins, Elizabeth

Laudenslager, Christiane Moore, Aaron Stock, Jessica Swaysland, Joshua Warner,

Zachary Wiener); my Dungeons and Dragons group (!) (Mark Lewis, Lauren Lewis,

Jasmine Erdener, Danielle Hanley); my friends in the diaspora (Toby Altman, Aaron

Brecher, Suzanne van Bronswijk, Chelsea Davis, Jessa Deutsch, Jess Engebretson, Alex

Ho, Lotte Lemmens, John Maltempo, Charles Rabideau, James Robinson, Melvin Pelaez,

Smith Koy Smith, Lauren Stokes, Kate Swisher, Laura Wang, Julia Wittes); and my

friends here in the program (especially Kelly Allred, Corey Cusimano, Eliora Porter, and

Rachel Schwartz).

I also must thank my loving family for everything they have done: my mother

Joan Pease, who instilled in me great determination and whose optimism and pride

sustain me, and my father Richard Keefe, who taught me to be gentle, curious, and odd;

my Italian family, my dear cousins Chiara and Stephanie Ferraro and my aunt Eva Anne

Ferraro who taught me kindness; my endlessly supportive maternal grandmother

Madeline Pease; and my paternal grandmother Eloise Keefe, who did not live to see me

enter graduate school but who is single-handedly the person most responsible for making

me the psychologist I am today.

Finally, I would also like to thank the many, many cats who provided me

unconditional positive regard during my doctoral process: Lenny & Belli at Fitzwater;

and the full rescue litter my parents took care of—Momma, Willie, Rosie, Thelma,

Louise, Right-Eye, Scooter, Blue, Bear, Princess, Abe, Mr. Two, Lily, and Bo.

Page 8: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

v

ABSTRACT

WHAT PROCESS WORKS FOR WHOM: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND THE

IMPACT OF THERAPY TECHNIQUES AND TREATMENT MECHANISMS

John Raymond Keefe

Robert J. DeRubeis

While psychotherapy treatment manuals define the broad structure and targets of a

therapy, therapists must decide how to implement treatments with a specific patient. Yet,

patients are heterogeneous even within a disorder class, and there is little systematic

research to guide a therapist to make principled adaptations. We examined the question of

whether individual differences moderate the treatment effects of therapist interventions

and both in-session and between-session processes of change, using data from a

randomized controlled trial for panic disorder comparing panic-focused psychodynamic

psychotherapy (PFPP) to cognitive-behavioral therapy. In Chapter 1, adherence to PFPP

(n = 65) was observer rated in Sessions 2 and 10 to predict panic change after the rated

session. Panic-specific interpretations predicted improvements, while non-panic-focused

interventions did not. Concordant with dynamic theory, patients with more interpersonal

problems benefitted especially from heightened focus on the interplay between

interpersonal-emotional conflict and panic. In Chapter 2, we examined whether higher

levels of observer-rated emotional expression—a marker of therapeutic engagement—

across early PFPP sessions predicted subsequent panic improvements. We hypothesized

that this relationship would be moderated by certain personality disorder traits related to

emotionality: (1) borderline traits, which denote heightened, labile, dysregulated

emotionality; and (2) obsessive-compulsive traits, related to muted, constrained

Page 9: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

vi

emotionality. As predicted, borderline traits attenuated the otherwise positive relationship

between emotional expression and symptom improvement, but obsessive-compulsive

traits had inconsistent relationships between trial sites. Finally, in Chapter 3, we built on

mediational analyses for both CBT and PFPP (n = 138), examining whether the presence

of different psychological vulnerabilities moderated the symptomatic impact of

improvement in two mediators of panic change: catastrophic, body-focused interpretation

style, and panic-specific reflective functioning (PSRF). Patients beginning treatment with

a more catastrophic style benefitted more from improvements in either mediator.

Personality disorder traits blunted the impact of improvements in PSRF, but patients with

no personality disorder evidenced high benefit from PSRF improvements. This set of

findings suggests that personalization of psychotherapy can be empirically grounded.

Clinical characteristics may inform how a therapist chooses to intervene, what in-session

processes they focus on, and what types of psychological changes they aim to encourage.

Page 10: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

vii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGMENT ........................................................................................ IV

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................... VIIII

LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................................ X

LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS ................................................................................. XI

SPECIFIC PSYCHODYNAMIC TECHNIQUES IN PANIC TREATMENT ............ 1

PATIENT PERSONALITY AND EMOTIONAL PROCESS ................................ 34

WHAT MECHANISM WORKS FOR WHOM IN PANIC ..................................... 79

APPENDICES .................................................................................................. 109

Page 11: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

viii

LIST OF TABLES

Table Title Page

1.1 Descriptive data for baseline characteristics and for symptom change,

in Study 1.

32

1.2 Descriptive statistics for technique process measurements (average 15

minutes per session), in Study 1.

33

1.3 Correlations between technique process measurements and baseline

characteristics, in Study 1.

33

2.1 Descriptive data for baseline characteristics and for symptom change,

in Study 2.

76

2.2 Example transcripts from sessions rated high/low in emotional

expression, in Study 2.

77

3.1 Descriptive data for baseline characteristics and for symptom change,

in Study 3.

107

S1 Examples of panic-focused clarification and interpretation, for Study

1 in Appendix C.

117

S2 Correlations between emotional expression ratings within the same 5-

minute segment, for Study 2 in Appendix D.

118

S3 Average levels of emotional expression per 5-minute segment across

sampled sessions, for Study 2 in Appendix D.

118

S4 Sources of variance in emotional expression per 5-minute segment,

for Study 2 in Appendix D.

119

Page 12: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

ix

LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS

Figure Title Page

1.1 Estimated effect sizes for the relationship between psychodynamic

technique use at a given session and subsequent improvement in

panic symptoms, in Study 1.

34

1.2 Estimated change in panic symptoms between Weeks 5 to 9, as a

function of degree of interpersonal problems and their interaction

with use of panic-focused interpretations at session 10, in Study 1.

35

2.1 Estimated effect sizes for the relationship between the average level

of emotional expression and subsequent improvements in panic

symptoms, in Study 2.

79

2.2 Estimated change in panic symptoms as a function of overall

emotional expression in interaction with borderline personality

disorder criteria, in Study 2.

80

3.1 Predicted degree of later symptom change attributable to early

improvements in PSRF, as a function of baseline severity of

catastrophic interpretive style, in Study 3.

109

3.2 Predicted degree of later symptom change attributable to early

improvements in PSRF as a function of baseline personality disorder

pathology, in Study 3.

110

Page 13: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

1

Specific Psychodynamic Techniques in Panic Treatment

Panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy (PFPP; Busch, Milrod, Singer, &

Aronson, 2012) is a 24-session, twice weekly brief psychodynamic psychotherapy (PDT)

formulated for the treatment of panic disorder (PD) with and without agoraphobia. In

several randomized controlled trials (RCTs), PFPP has been shown to be equivalent in

efficacy to various forms of cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy (CBT; Beutel et al.,

2013; Keefe, McCarthy, Dinger, Zilcha-Mano, & Barber, 2014; Milrod et al., 2016;

Milrod et al., 2007). The exception comes from one site of a two-site study at which

PFPP was inferior to CBT (Milrod et al., 2016). Further trials are underway (Sandell et

al., 2015).

The PFPP treatment model is based on the assumption that the acute emergence

of panic and the developmental vulnerability toward PD has underlying psychological

meanings related to emotional-interpersonal conflicts and attachment dysregulation

(Busch, Milrod, & Singer, 1999; Busch et al., 2012). For example, a patient who

experiences ambivalence regarding a life-long romantic commitment to his partner may

develop a PD shortly after announcing their engagement. The patient’s panic attacks may

be triggered by anxious moments of unacknowledged intolerable ambivalence toward his

partner. He may either be unaware of or perhaps frightened to recognize this ambivalence

and its potentially frightening consequences, such as breaking up with or losing the

support of his partner, which could result in a vicious cycle of unrecognized conflicted

feelings and anxious arousal leading to panic. Consistent with PFPP’s etiological

hypothesis, patients with PD report higher rates of alexithymia (i.e., difficulty verbalizing

one’s emotions and motives), experiential avoidance, and lack of emotional acceptance,

Page 14: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

2

compared to psychiatrically healthy controls and persons with simple phobias (Galderisi

et al., 2008; Izci et al., 2014; Parker, Taylor, Bagby, & Acklin, 1993; Tull & Roemer,

2007).

One important intervention in PFPP, clarification, is the attempt by the therapist

to help the patient become aware of the avoided and unconscious intrapsychic conflicts

that give rise to panic (Busch et al., 2012). This process entails gathering information

concerning the potential meanings of symptoms and actively helping the patient

recognize, verbalize, and reflect on those meanings; the context of symptoms also helps

to identify meanings. Successful clarifications along these lines lay the groundwork for

accurate interpretations of conflicts. For example, clarification could help the

aforementioned patient become aware that he habitually assigns frightful somatic sources

(e.g., heart disease) to anxiety that emerges when he became engaged and works on

wedding details, triggering intensely mixed feelings about his fiancée. This would enable

the therapist to help the patient focus on specific causes for these feelings.

Another important PFPP intervention is interpretation—attempts to help the

patient identify the specific dynamics and conflicts that underlie PD (Busch et al., 2012;

Summers & Barber, 2010). Types of interpretations include: (a) defense (i.e., of a

particular manner in which the patient avoids experiencing a particular distressing feeling

or issue); (b) dynamic/conflict (i.e., of how a patient’s experiences are the result of a

conflict between unacceptable wishes and the defenses against these wishes); (c) genetic

(i.e., of how early, formative attachment relationships may have made the patient’s

approach to interpersonal situations fraught with specific, identifiable vulnerabilities);

and (d) transference (i.e., how the patient’s recurring underlying formative attachment

Page 15: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

3

patterns and conflicts emerge in the relationship with the therapist) (Busch et al., 2012).

PFPP therapists should attempt to link, whenever possible, the specifics of the patient’s

panic symptoms to his/her specific underlying dynamics (Busch et al., 1999; Busch et al.,

2012). A PFPP therapist who treats the engaged PD patient described above might

interpret that he appears to be afraid of experiencing and expressing his anger with his

partner. The therapist might cite the observation that the patient frequently talks about

how much he loves his partner immediately after expressing his frustrations, thereby

magically “undoing” his anger. The therapist could also point out that the unresolved and

frightening feelings of anger and frustration seem to emerge as sensations of physical

discomfort that trigger panic. By doing so, the therapist attempts to help the patient own

and work through these conflicts, diminishing their power as panic triggers and helping

the patient attribute any lingering anxiety to personal psychological meanings rather than,

for example, somatic problems.

Techniques Contributing to Efficacy in PDT for Anxiety

Previous studies have demonstrated that use of specific psychodynamic

techniques is predictive of outcomes in depression and personality disorder treatment

(Barber, Crits-Christoph, & Luborsky, 1996; Barber, Muran, McCarthy, & Keefe, 2013;

Hoglend, Dahl, Hersoug, Lorentzen, & Perry, 2011; Levy et al., 2006; McCarthy, Keefe,

& Barber, 2016). However, there is a lack of empirical evidence supporting the efficacy

of specific psychodynamic therapy (PDT) techniques for anxiety disorders, with only one

small sampled study (n = 20) showing that patients whose therapists employed more

interpretations across two sessions (3 and 9 out of an average of 20 sessions) of short-

term PDT tended to have superior symptomatic outcomes at termination (Pitman, Slavin-

Page 16: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

4

Mulford, & Hilsenroth, 2014). However, because the authors were unable to establish

temporal precedence (i.e., that technique use preceded symptom change), a plausible

account of the finding is that, for instance, patients with better prognoses pulled for more

interpretations from their therapists in this setting. Moreover, disorders were not

diagnosed with a reliable measure. Further research is clearly required to investigate the

efficacy of specific PDT techniques for anxiety.

When considering the active ingredients that characterize effective PFPP, it

should be noted PFPP is distinguished from generic short-term PDT through its explicit

emphasis on panic and its associated dynamics (Busch et al., 2012). Clinical trials in

which PDTs have been operationalized without a specific focus on the primary

symptoms, or in which such a focus was proscribed or discouraged, have generated some

of the most disappointing findings regarding the efficacy of PDTs (Durham et al., 1994;

Garner et al., 1993; Gilboa-Schechtman et al., 2010; Poulsen et al., 2014). The rationale

behind PFPP is that by focusing on experiences proximal to panic and by linking

interpretations coherently to panic vulnerability, patients gain insight as to the specific

underpinnings of their panic attacks (Rudden, Milrod, Target, Ackerman, & Graf, 2006).

In PDT and other therapies, gains in insight have been found in different investigations to

both predict further functional improvements post-treatment as well as protection against

relapse across follow-up (Barber et al., 2013; Gibbons et al., 2009; Johansson et al., 2010;

Kallestad et al., 2010). However, the specific hypothesis that psychodynamic focus on

symptoms promotes greater symptom relief (Summers & Barber, 2010) has never been

addressed directly in empirical investigations.

Page 17: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

5

In the context of a randomized controlled trial of PD with and without

agoraphobia evaluating manualized PFPP (Milrod et al., 2016), we measured the use of

psychodynamic psychotherapy interventions in two sessions of psychotherapy taken from

the early and middle phases of a 24-session treatment protocol (Sessions 2 and 10). Based

on PFPP’s conceptual model (Busch et al., 2012), we hypothesized that therapists’ more

frequent use of psychodynamic interpretations made with connections to symptoms of

and/or vulnerabilities to panic, agoraphobia, or anxiety would predict greater subsequent

symptom improvement in PFPP. In the PFPP manualization, therapists are expected to,

on average, focus on clarification of panic meanings in the beginning of treatment, but to

become progressively more interpretive as these meanings become clearer and the patient

becomes socialized to psychodynamic treatment (Busch et al., 2012). We hypothesized

that interpretations in the session taken from the middle phase of treatment (Session 10)

would be more predictive of improvement than interpretations in the earlier session

(Session 2), as by the middle phase of treatment the therapist has sufficient information

about the patient to make meaningful and accurate interpretations (Andrusyna, Luborsky,

Pham, & Tang, 2006; Crits-Christoph, Cooper, & Luborsky, 1988). We anticipated that

interpretations made without reference to panic would not predict subsequent symptom

improvement, when accounting for panic-focused interpretations.

In addition, we hypothesized that panic-focused clarifications made early on in

therapy (Session 2) would contribute to prediction of psychotherapy outcomes, as

clarification in this phase can help the therapist gather information and can encourage

initial exploration of dynamics underlying the patient’s panic symptoms.

Page 18: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

6

Moderators of technique-outcome relationships. Like many manualized

psychotherapies, PFPP allows for therapist flexibility in focus and application of

therapeutic techniques (Busch et al., 2012), but relatively little empirical data exist to

help guide moment-to-moment judgments of how to respond to an individual patient. As

specific therapeutic techniques can be more or less conducive of change among particular

patients (Keefe, Webb, & DeRubeis, 2016; Sasso, Strunk, Braun, DeRubeis, & Brotman,

2015), we furthermore hypothesized that specific patients would be likely to evince

apparent benefit from particular psychodynamic interventions.

Specifically, we hypothesized that panic-focused interpretations would be more

important to outcomes when patients entered the trial with more interpersonal problems

as measured by the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Horowitz, Alden, Wiggins,

& Pincus, 2000). Interpersonal conflicts and transitions are frequently stressors

surrounding the onset of PD (Klass et al., 2009; Scocco, Barbieri, & Frank, 2007), and

couples in which one patient has PD often exhibit relational distress and avoidant

conversational and cognitive styles (Chambless, 2010). As unresolved and/or

unconscious interpersonal conflicts such as intolerable dependency and anger can be

viewed as a trigger for panic in PFPP, we hypothesized that individuals with more

interpersonal problems might be more likely to have such conflicts or to have relatively

more pervasive conflicts, and thus would benefit relatively more from panic-focused

interpretations addressing those conflicts. In addition, the dynamics associated with

panic, including difficulties with separation, dependency and anger could contribute to

both panic and interpersonal problems. In other words, our hypothesis was based on the

notion that patients with more interpersonal problems may be more likely to have the

Page 19: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

7

dynamic-interpersonal contributions to panic that panic-focused interpretations may

specifically help reveal and resolve.

Method

Participants

Patients. The present study is a secondary analysis of patients randomized to the

PFPP condition (N = 80) of a two-site randomized controlled trial comparing PFPP,

CBT, and applied relaxation training among patients with primary DSM-IV panic

disorder with or without agoraphobia. Treatment took place twice a week for 12 weeks.

Patients were recruited at New York Presbyterian/Weill-Cornell (hereafter, “Site A”) and

the University of Pennsylvania (hereafter, “Site B”). Participants received study treatment

gratis. Participants gave informed written consent. Both sites’ institutional review boards

approved the protocol, and the study is registered with ClinicalTrials.gov (identifier:

NCT00353470).

Patients were included in the trial if they had the spontaneous occurrence of one

or more panic attacks for the month before trial entry, and qualified for DSM-IV panic

disorder diagnosis determined as per the ADIS-IV (DiNardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1995).

Cross-site agreement on ADIS ratings for panic severity (with a “4” indicating the

diagnostic threshold) was excellent (ICC = 1.00). See Milrod et al., 2016 for further

details.

Non-study psychotherapy was prohibited. Medications were permitted if stable

for at least two months at presentation, and were recorded, held constant, and monitored

during the trial. Exclusion criteria were active substance dependence (less than 6 months’

remission), a history of psychosis or bipolar disorder, acute suicidality, and organic

Page 20: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

8

mental syndrome. Additional details on trial design, independent evaluator training, and

therapy adherence can be consulted in the primary outcome paper (Milrod et al., 2016).

Of the 80 randomized patients, 65 (n = 30 Site A, n = 35 Site B) provided the

necessary data to be included in the present study (see section on missing data).

Descriptive data on patients’ demographics may be found in Table 1.

Therapists. Sixteen doctoral-level therapists (11 M.D., 5 Ph.D.) administered

PFPP across the two sites. Therapists had an average of 15 years of post-graduate

experience (SD = 8.2), and an average of 5 years’ experience in some form of time-

limited psychodynamic therapy (SD = 6.3). The average total caseload was four (median

= 3.5, range = 1 to 11) for therapists whose patients were included in the present

analyses. All therapists were experienced therapists who were specifically trained in

PFPP over the span of a 2-day, 10-hour course. Therapists participated in monthly group

supervision and received regular individual supervision from senior clinicians. For the

primary outcome paper, basic adherence to PFPP was established (Milrod et al., 2016).

Outcome Index

Panic Disorder Severity Scale (PDSS; Shear et al., 1997). The PDSS is a

widely used diagnosis-based, composite, global rating of panic disorder severity, with

acceptable psychometric properties. The PDSS was administered by trained, master’s-

level independent evaluators who were uninformed as to treatment condition. Interrater

reliability on the PDSS was excellent (ICC[2,1] = 0.95). The PDSS was administered five

times during treatment: at baseline (Week 0), Week 1, Week 5, Week 9, and termination

(Week 12).

Moderator Measures

Page 21: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

9

Inventory of Interpersonal Problems—Circumplex (IIP; Horowitz et al.,

2000)). The IIP-Circumplex is a 64-item self-report measure of maladaptive interpersonal

problems that is a shorter version of the full 127-item IIP. The sum score of the IIP

reflects an individual’s degree of interpersonal distress. The IIP exhibits adequate internal

reliability and 10-week test-retest reliability (Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 1990), and

exhibited excellent internal reliability in this sample (alpha = 0.95).

Process Measure

Panic-Focused Psychodynamic Psychotherapy Rating Scale (PFPP-RS;

Keefe, Phillips, Busch, & Milrod, 2016). The PFPP-RS is an observer-rated scale

developed to assess the degree to which therapists used general psychodynamic

interpretive techniques, as well as more specific panic-focused psychodynamic

techniques. It was developed by the author JRK in conjunction with PFPP developers

Fred Busch and Barbara Milrod. The use of each technique was rated on a 5-point Likert

scale ranging from 0 (technique not present in section) to 2 (at least one clear example of

the technique in section) to 4 (technique applied fully and comprehensively in section).

Scores reflected the degree to which a technique was prototypic to the rated segment (i.e.,

adherence), not whether the raters believed the therapist applied the technique in a

particularly apt way (i.e., competence). For items measuring the use of interpretation, a

score of 2 would indicate that the therapist made at least one clear, identifiable

interpretation. A score of 4 could represent either multiple individual interpretations

made within a rating segment, or a single, continually developed interpretation over an

extended period of time. Sessions were divided into 15-min segments, with each segment

Page 22: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

10

rated for each technique. A single rating per session was calculated by averaging ratings

made across the three 15-min segments.

Scores of the PFPP-RS items were summed to produce the following subscales:

(a) Psychodynamic Interpretations – the degree to which the therapist used any of the

four types of interpretations (defense, genetic, dynamic, and transference); and (b) Panic-

Focus: the degree to which the therapist focused on panic, agoraphobia, and anxiety

symptoms and made connections between these symptoms and panic dynamics, including

underlying conflicts. For Panic-Focus items, to score a 2 or higher on any given item, a

therapist had to make a clear, unambiguous reference to panic, agoraphobia, or anxiety in

relation to the intervention. This subscale includes two items assessing use of

clarification with regard to panic symptoms and their personal meaning (PF-

Clarification), and three items assessing interpretations that address the emergence of and

vulnerability to experiences of panic, agoraphobia, and anxiety (PF-Interpretation).

Examples of therapist interventions qualifying as clarifications and interpretations can be

found in Supplemental Table 1. To separate panic-focused interpretations from non-

panic-focused interpretations, the PF-Interpretation subscore was subtracted from the

total Psychodynamic Interpretations score, as PF-Interpretations reflect a subset of

Psychodynamic Interpretations. This score will be referred to as Non-Panic-Focused

Interpretations.

Video-recorded sessions were rated using the PFPP-RS by six advanced

undergraduate psychology majors at the University of Pennsylvania who each received

approximately 20 hours of training by the developers of the scale. Two authors who were

PFPP developers (FB & BR), and a graduate student author familiar with the model (NS)

Page 23: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

11

provided additional consultation as to the validity and reliability of ratings during

training. Training included a review of the rater manual, the PFPP therapy manual (Busch

et al., 2012), and rating of several training tapes of PFPP. Training was continued until

raters consistently rated single items within a point (+/-) of their graduate student trainer.

Additionally, raters met approximately every other week with the study leader JRK to

rate a tape collectively and discuss rating challenges and questions.

All available Sessions 2 and 10 were rated for each PFPP patient. Sessions 3 and

9 were rated in cases wherein Session 2 or 10 (respectively) was not available. Sessions

were randomly assigned to raters, who were uninformed of the outcome data. Two raters

rated each tape, and ratings were averaged across raters.

Random effects ICCs were calculated using variance estimates from an REML

mixed model in the R package “lme4” (Bates et al., 2016; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979).

Interrater reliability per 15-minute segment was good for all Psychodynamic

Interpretations (ICC[2,2] = 0.80) and adequate for Panic-Focused Clarification (ICC[2,2]

= 0.71), Panic-Focused Interpretations (ICC[2,2] = 0.70), and the difference score

reflecting Non-Panic-Focused Interpretations (ICC[2,2] = 0.68).

Statistical Analyses

All analyses were conducted using the R statistical programming language (R

Core Team, 2017) and run using robust regressions as implemented in the R package

“Robustbase” (Maechler et al., 2016). Given the effective sample size (n = 65), robust

regression was selected over standard regression for its superior properties of robustness

against multivariate outliers and deviation from homoscedasticity (Huber & Ronchetti,

2009). A robust regression (a) retains full information on all observations in an initial

Page 24: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

12

estimate of parameters; (b) iteratively determines weights for each observation based on a

particular estimator function from this initial estimate, such that points much farther from

model predictions in the previous iteration are given lower weight; and (c) recalculates

final parameter estimates based on the final weighting when the values of the coefficients

converge within a specified tolerance (Koller & Stahel, 2011). Semi-partial correlation

effect sizes (sr) were estimated for parameters of interest from linear regressions.

Missing data. In the primary trial, a not missing at random (NMAR) pattern of

treatment dropout was detected, such that patients with worse PDSS symptom trajectories

were more likely to terminate from treatment prematurely (Milrod et al., 2016). When

outcomes for treatment noncompleters are imputed in the NMAR context, imputation and

other missing data methods can lead to biased estimates and confidence intervals

(Graham, 2009). As such, only individuals who provided data up to the Week 9

assessment point were included in our analyses (n = 65; 81.3% of the intention-to-treat

sample).

Several trial completers were missing videotapes of one of the two sessions due to

technical issues or therapist/research assistant error (n = 27), but not treatment dropout.

Process ratings for completers missing a video-recording of a session can be presumed to

be missing at random in relation to panic symptom outcomes (Rubin & Little, 2002). This

degree of missingness is not considered prohibitive in the missing data literature (White,

Royston, & Wood, 2011), and process ratings have been successfully imputed in the past

(e.g., Forand et al., 2018; Lorenzo-Luaces et al., 2017). Accordingly, random forest

imputation (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, 2011) was used to impute missing data for

completers, using all baseline data, in addition to all PDSS scores and termination and

Page 25: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

13

pre-to-post treatment change scores on the Sheehan Disability Scale, Inventory of

Interpersonal Problems, and Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression. Furthermore, all

rated technique process ratings from observed sessions (the non-missing session and

Session 5, which was rated but not used in this manuscript) were included in the

imputation model, such that all patients had observed session process during their therapy

contributing to the imputation of their missing session ratings. For this data set, a

normalized root mean square error of prediction was estimated at 0.28, indicating that

imputation accuracy was adequate (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, 2011). Analyses for patients

with complete data for a given session were also run and compared to imputed data. No

changes in patterns of statistical significance were detected.

Analytic strategy. Two sets of analyses were performed. Within a robust linear

regression framework (Koller & Stahel, 2016), technique use at Session 2 (end of Week

1) was used to predict PDSS symptom change between Weeks 1 and 5 of treatment,

while technique use at Session 10 (end of Week 5) was used to predict symptom change

between Weeks 5 and 9 of treatment. For two reasons, we examined change in the

symptom measurement interval following the sampled session rather than the entire

remainder of the therapy: (a) This permitted establishment of closer temporal precedence

between techniques and outcomes than is often performed in “long reach” studies that

sample from an early session to predict change throughout the entire treatment (e.g.,

Keefe et al., 2016) ; and (b) as improvement in the trial was linear and technique use

following the sampled session may also influence symptom change, using too large a

prediction interval may obfuscate the signal of how technique use occurring in the

sampled session per se relates to subsequent symptom change.

Page 26: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

14

Three process terms were included in each regression unless otherwise specified:

PF-Interpretation, PF-Clarification, and Non-PF-Interpretation. In each analysis, baseline

PDSS symptom score and PDSS change prior to the measured session were included as

covariates. Prior panic symptom levels were included as a covariate to allay the

possibility that patients who were low severity or getting better could have “pulled” for

more or fewer techniques, generating an epiphenomenal relationship. Due to a site by

treatment interaction reported in the primary outcome paper across the three tested

treatments (Milrod et al., 2016), we also examined whether any process measures

interacted with site to predict outcomes, and we planned to report any such interactions if

they were found at least at trend level (p <.10). However, no such interactions with site

were detected, suggesting that process relationships were not detectably different across

sites.

Furthermore, IIP scores were examined as a moderator of the relationship of

Session 10 panic-focused interpretations to subsequent change, tested by specifying an

interaction between the two variables. The Johnson-Neyman technique was applied to

probe the regions of significance of the interaction (Johnson & Fay, 1950).

Given that we conducted seven statistical tests, we adjusted p-values using the

Benjamini-Hochberg correction to control for the false discovery rate at an alpha of 0.05

(Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995), employing the core R function “p.adjust.” These are

reported as adjusted p-values.

In addition, we conducted two secondary, post-hoc statistical checks on the

robustness of our obtained findings. In the first, we employed a mixed model to estimate

therapist-level variance simultaneously with our model estimates, which did not result in

Page 27: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

15

substantively different conclusions. In the second, we analyzed whether the technique

variables related to pre-to-post treatment functional and interpersonal outcomes. Both

analyses are reported in Appendix B.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

The average patient in this study had a baseline PDSS score of 13.9 (range 7 to

20), considered to be in the moderately-ill severity range for patients with comorbid

agoraphobia (Furukawa et al., 2009). More than three-quarters of patients (n = 53;

81.5%) qualified for a co-morbid DSM-IV diagnosis of agoraphobia. The average patient

reported interpersonal problems in the high-normal range of severity (approximately

+0.64 SD over the normative mean). Other baseline demographic and clinical

information can be found in Table 1.

Technique scores at Sessions 2 and 10, as well as indices of their consistency over

time, are presented in Table 2. Reliable within-patient stability between sessions was

observed only for panic-focused interpretations. Within a given case, mean levels of

panic-focused and non-panic focused interpretations increased from Session 2 to Session

10.

Prior to data analysis, the correlations between every process measurement and

baseline PDSS severity and our proposed moderator variable (IIP scores) were examined.

As displayed in Table 3, there were no significant correlations between baseline panic

and interpersonal problem severity and any of the technique variables at either time point.

Early Panic Symptom Change (Weeks 1 to 5)

Page 28: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

16

There was no significant relationship between panic-focused clarifications at

Session 2 (end of Week 1) and symptom change between Weeks 1 to 5 (B = 1.16 [95%

CI: -0.20, 2.52], SE = 0.68, t[59] = 1.71, p = 0.092, adjusted p = 0.184, sr = 0.21).

Neither panic-focused interpretations (B = -1.34 [95% CI: -3.32, 0.64], SE = 0.99, t[59] =

-1.35, p = 0.181, adjusted p = 0.290, sr = -0.15) nor non-panic focused interpretations (B

= -0.60 [95% CI: -2.88, 1.68], SE = 1.14, t = -0.53, p = 0.599, adjusted p = 0.599, sr = -

0.09) yielded significant predictions of symptom change in the subsequent measurement

interval.

Later Panic Symptom Change (Weeks 5 to 9)

Higher levels of panic-focused interpretations at mid-treatment (Session 10, end

of Week 5) predicted greater panic symptom improvement between Weeks 5 and 9 (B =

1.79 [95% CI: 0.61, 2.97], SE = 0.59, t = 3.04, p = 0.004, adjusted p = 0.016, sr = 0.37),

subsequent to the measured session. By contrast, non-panic focused interpretations were

unrelated to subsequent outcomes (B = -0.47 [95 CI: -1.54, 0.60], SE = 0.54, t[59] = -

0.88, p = 0.382, adjusted p = 0.437, sr = -0.09). Panic-focused clarification at this session

was also unrelated to outcomes (B = -0.56 [95% CI: -1.37, 0.25], SE = 0.41, t[59] = -1.38,

p = 0.175, adjusted p = 0.351, sr = -0.20). Figure 1 summarizes all technique-outcome

relationships for early and later panic symptom change.

Interpersonal Problems as a Moderator of Technique-Outcome Relationships

The higher the score on the IIP at intake, the stronger the relation of panic-

focused interpretations at Session 10 was to subsequent symptom change (B = 3.65 [95%

CI: 1.24, 6.05], SE = 1.20, t[57] = 3.04, p = 0.004, adjusted p = 0.016, sr = 0.29). The

Johnson-Neyman technique identified an IIP score of 1.0 as the cutoff for exhibiting a

Page 29: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

17

significant, positive relationship between panic-focused interpretations and subsequent

improvement (n of individuals with IIP > 1.0 = 38, 58.5%; see Figure 2). For patients

with scores greater than or equal to 1.0, indicating higher levels of interpersonal problems

at baseline, there was a significant relation between panic-focused interpretations and

subsequent symptom improvement (sr = 0.41, p = 0.001), whereas for patients with less

interpersonal distress the association was not significant (sr = 0.11, p = 0.255), in large

part because patients with lower interpersonal distress did well symptomatically

regardless of interpretation level (see Figure 2).

Discussion

We investigated the relation between use of specific PFPP techniques and

symptomatic outcomes in the treatment of panic disorder. Our first important finding

suggests that at mid-therapy (Session 10), panic patients receiving a high level of panic-

focused interpretations exhibited greater subsequent symptom improvement. However,

non-panic-focused interpretations did not predict subsequent symptom improvement

during either the earlier or later periods of treatment. Moreover, panic-focused

interpretations at Session 10 also predicted to pre-to-post improvements in interpersonal

functioning (see Appendix B). These findings lend support to the importance of taking a

symptom-focused approach in short-term psychodynamic therapies for anxiety (Busch et

al., 2012; Tasca, Hilsenroth, & Thompson-Brenner, 2014). Past process findings

demonstrating a positive relationship between interpretations and symptom change

(Pitman et al., 2014) could reflect that many interpretations in short-term psychotherapy

in fact are symptom-focused, even when that is not the specific intent of the study.

Page 30: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

18

On the other hand, interpretations in an early session, whether or not they were

related to panic symptoms, did not predict subsequent panic symptom improvement over

the following four weeks. Possibly, early interpretations are not as accurate as those made

after the therapist has learned more about the patient. In previous psychodynamic process

studies, observer-rated accuracy of interpretations derived from themes coded from early

session transcripts or pre-treatment history interviews predicted greater symptom change

and likelihood of having a “sudden gain” (Andrusyna et al., 2006; Crits-Christoph et al.,

1988). Alternatively, interpretation at a very early stage may sometimes be experienced

by the patient as overwhelming (McCarthy et al., 2016). At an early stage of therapy, it is

also potentially less likely that patients have been fully socialized to the both the structure

and tasks and goals of psychodynamic therapy (Luborsky, 1984), which may make it

harder for them to build on therapists’ interpretations with further personal exploration

and development of insight.

Taken together, it is possible that early interpretations are both less accurate to the

patient’s dynamics, and less likely to be perceived by the patient as well-timed, which

may be considered matters of intervention competence. Future process studies on

interpretation accuracy and the role of supportive techniques and alliance in PFPP would

help distinguish between these and other hypotheses explaining our findings. However,

our findings do not support the conclusion that the other types of dynamic techniques

assessed are necessarily without use or are counterproductive, but rather that panic-

focused interpretations are the only statistically reliable signal of positive process for the

average patient in this sample. It is also plausible that more complex process relationships

exist (e.g., interactions between early and mid-technique use; levels of clarification and

Page 31: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

19

interpretation), but we did not explore these possibilities due to our need to limit the

number of tests performed in this small sample.

As we hypothesized, patients with more interpersonal problems at baseline

exhibited a stronger relationship between mid-therapy panic-focused interpretations and

subsequent change. This finding is consistent with PFFP’s theoretical model, which

proposes that unconscious conflict in the context of relationships may contribute to

experiences of panic, such that patients with more interpersonal distress may need a more

intense focus on the emotions and conflicts underlying this distress (Busch et al., 2012).

Panic dynamics are typically interwoven with the interpersonal problems that these

patients struggle with. For example, many panic patients are prone to being in

relationships where they struggle to assert their own needs (Zilcha-Mano et al., 2015).

This linkage may allow for more readily identifiable dynamics and conflicts, and more

opportunities to identify them in relation to interpersonal difficulties, compared to

patients with relatively fewer interpersonal problems. Ergo, it may be the case that

therapists were more accurate in their interpretations for patients with stronger

interpersonal issues.

However, patients with lower levels of interpersonal problems had good symptom

improvement in this interval (i.e., between Weeks 5 to 9) regardless of panic-focused

interpretations, whereas increasing interpersonal distress was more predictive of poor

symptom improvement in less panic-focused interpretive therapies (see Figure 2). We

would argue that this pattern of results is concordant with the perspective that patients

with more interpersonal problems particularly need a more panic-focused, interpretive

therapy. Past psychodynamic process-outcome research has rarely sought to identify

Page 32: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

20

beneficial matches between techniques and patient characteristics, with one exception

being the body of literature suggesting an important role for transference interpretations

in treating personality disorder (Hoglend et al., 2011; Keefe & DeRubeis, 2018). Overall,

our finding is consistent with a perspective wherein patients with more complicated or

treatment-resistant presentations may require more active or skillful approaches, and may

reveal more about process-outcome relationships than those DeRubeis and colleagues

(2014) have called “easier” (or more straightforward) patients (DeRubeis, Gelfand,

German, Fournier, & Forand, 2014; Keefe et al., 2016).

Limitations and future directions

Fifteen cases (18.8%) were unable to be used for the present analyses due to

dropout, which was nonrandom and related to poorer symptom trajectories in the parent

trial (Milrod et al., 2016). The remaining patients included in our study represent a

subsample of individuals who improved relatively more symptomatically. It is possible

that the observed technique-outcome relationships would not be obtained among the

dropout patients; alternatively, relatively less efficacious therapy process may have led to

worse symptom trajectories, promoting dropout. Examining the relationship between

technique use and treatment dropout would be an interesting way to disambiguate these

possibilities, although this effort would be poorly powered in our sample due to the low

base rate of dropout and lack of early therapy tapes due to dropout. In addition, several

sessions were not available to be rated due to protocol error. However, we employed a

standard, validated method for imputing the missing ratings (Stekhoven & Bühlmann,

2011), and results obtained with data only from cases with complete data mirrored those

obtained when the imputations were included.

Page 33: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

21

The patterns obtained using ratings from early and middle sections of therapy

were observed using only one session from each phase. A better approach would be to

sample multiple sessions from each phase (Dennhag, Gibbons, Barber, Gallop, & Crits-

Christoph, 2012). Unfortunately, at one of the two treatment sites, only recordings of

Sessions 2, 5, and 10 were available on a systematic basis. Future studies that include

ratings of multiple recordings within the same interval would allow for the investigation

of more complex patterns of the relation between process and symptom change, such as

variability in technique use across sessions (Owen & Hilsenroth, 2014). In addition, the

average therapy was not intensely interpretive (e.g., at session 10, less than one panic-

focused interpretation per fifteen minutes), indicating that we could not meaningfully

examine hypotheses that that intensities of interpretations in between the extremes is

more effective than very low or very high intensities (see McCarthy et al., 2016).

Finally, our findings suggest but cannot confirm the presence of a causal

relationship between the intensity of panic interpretations and symptom change. A

stronger test of the causal hypothesis would require experimental manipulation of the

causal variable, in a manner like Hoglend et al.’s (2008) randomized comparison of

psychodynamic therapy with versus without transference interpretations.

Conclusions

Psychodynamic therapists implementing PFPP should focus on interpretation of

the possible conflicts underlying panic as they enter the middle phase of therapy.

Particularly tying the patient’s dynamics to experiences of panic, anxiety, and

agoraphobia—rather than making general interpretations concerning relational or

personal patterns—may be especially important for effective short-term treatment of

Page 34: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

22

panic. For patients presenting with higher levels of interpersonal distress, an emphasis on

panic-focused interpretations may be especially important in promoting remission from

panic disorder.

References

Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1990). Construction of circumplex scales

for the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. J Pers Assess, 55(3-4), 521-536. doi:

10.1080/00223891.1990.9674088

Andrusyna, T. P., Luborsky, L., Pham, T., & Tang, T. Z. (2006). The mechanisms of

sudden gains in Supportive-Expressive therapy for depression. Psychotherapy

Research, 16(5), 526-535. doi: 10.1080/10503300600591379

Barber, J. P., Crits-Christoph, P., & Luborsky, L. (1996). Effects of therapist adherence

and competence on patient outcome in brief dynamic therapy. J Consult Clin

Psychol, 64(3), 619-622.

Barber, J. P., Muran, J. C., McCarthy, K. S., & Keefe, J. R. (2013). Research on dynamic

therapies. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield's Handbook of

Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (6th ed., pp. 443-494). New York, NY:

Wiley.

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., Singmann, H., . . .

Green, P. (2016). lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4

(Version 1.1-12). Retrieved from https://cran.r-

project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html

Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical

and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical

Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289-300.

Page 35: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

23

Beutel, M. E., Scheurich, V., Knebel, A., Michal, M., Wiltink, J., Graf-Morgenstern, M.,

. . . Subic-Wrana, C. (2013). Implementing panic-focused psychodynamic

psychotherapy into clinical practice. Canadian journal of psychiatry, 58(6), 326-

334.

Busch, F. N., Milrod, B. L., & Singer, M. B. (1999). Theory and technique in

psychodynamic treatment of panic disorder. The Journal of psychotherapy

practice and research, 8(3), 234-242.

Busch, F. N., Milrod, B. L., Singer, M. B., & Aronson, A. C. (2012). Manual of panic

focused psychodynamic psychotherapy - extended range (Vol. 36). New York,

NY: Routledge.

Chambless, D. L. (2010). Interpersonal aspects of panic disorder and agoraphobia. In J.

G. Beck (Ed.), Interpersonal processes in the anxiety disorders: Implications for

understanding psychopathology and treatment (pp. 209-233). Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

Crits-Christoph, P., Cooper, A., & Luborsky, L. (1988). The accuracy of therapists'

interpretations and the outcome of dynamic psychotherapy. J Consult Clin

Psychol, 56(4), 490-495.

Dennhag, I., Gibbons, M. B., Barber, J. P., Gallop, R., & Crits-Christoph, P. (2012). How

many treatment sessions and patients are needed to create a stable score of

adherence and competence in the treatment of cocaine dependence?

Psychotherapy Reseaerch, 22(4), 475-488. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2012.674790

DeRubeis, R. J., Gelfand, L. A., German, R. E., Fournier, J. C., & Forand, N. R. (2014).

Understanding processes of change: how some patients reveal more than others-

Page 36: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

24

and some groups of therapists less-about what matters in psychotherapy.

Psychotherapy Research, 24(3), 419-428. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2013.838654

DiNardo, P. A., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Anxiety Disorders Interview

Schedule for DSM-IV: Current Version (ADIS-IV). New York, NY: Graywinds

Publications.

Dunlap, W. P., Cortina, J. M., Vaslow, J. B., & Burke, M. J. (1996). Meta-analysis of

experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychol

Methods, 1(2), 170-177. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.170

Durham, R. C., Murphy, T., Allan, T., Richard, K., Treliving, L. R., & Fenton, G. W.

(1994). Cognitive therapy, analytic psychotherapy and anxiety management

training for generalised anxiety disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry,

165(3), 315-323. doi: 10.1192/bjp.165.3.315

Forand, N. R., Barnett, J. G., Strunk, D. R., Hindiyeh, M. U., Feinberg, J. E., & Keefe, J.

R. (2018). Efficacy of Guided iCBT for Depression and Mediation of Change by

Cognitive Skill Acquisition. Behavior Therapy, 49(2), 295-307. doi:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.04.004

Furukawa, T. A., Katherine Shear, M., Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Woods, S. W.,

Money, R., . . . Leucht, S. (2009). Evidence-based guidelines for interpretation of

the Panic Disorder Severity Scale. Depress Anxiety, 26(10), 922-929. doi:

10.1002/da.20532

Galderisi, S., Mancuso, F., Mucci, A., Garramone, S., Zamboli, R., & Maj, M. (2008).

Alexithymia and cognitive dysfunctions in patients with panic disorder.

Psychother Psychosom, 77(3), 182-188. doi: 10.1159/000119738

Page 37: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

25

Garner, D. M., Rockert, W., Davis, R., Garner, M. V., Olmsted, M. P., & Eagle, M.

(1993). Comparison of cognitive-behavioral and supportive-expressive therapy

for bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150(1), 37-46. doi:

10.1176/ajp.150.1.37

Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Barber, J. P., Wiltsey Stirman, S., Gallop, R.,

Goldstein, L. A., . . . Ring-Kurtz, S. (2009). Unique and common mechanisms of

change across cognitive and dynamic psychotherapies. J Consult Clin Psychol,

77(5), 801-813. doi: 10.1037/a0016596

Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Foa, E. B., Shafran, N., Aderka, I. M., Powers, M. B.,

Rachamim, L., . . . Apter, A. (2010). Prolonged Exposure Versus Dynamic

Therapy for Adolescent PTSD: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of

the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 1034-1042.

doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.07.014

Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu Rev

Psychol, 60, 549-576. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530

Hoglend, P., Bogwald, K. P., Amlo, S., Marble, A., Ulberg, R., Sjaastad, M. C., . . .

Johansson, P. (2008). Transference interpretations in dynamic psychotherapy: do

they really yield sustained effects? American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 763-

771. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07061028

Hoglend, P., Dahl, H. S., Hersoug, A. G., Lorentzen, S., & Perry, J. C. (2011). Long-term

effects of transference interpretation in dynamic psychotherapy of personality

disorders. European psychiatry, 26(7), 419-424. doi:

10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.05.006

Page 38: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

26

Horowitz, L. M., Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (2000). IIP: Inventory of

Interpersonal Problems manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Huber, P. J., & Ronchetti, E. M. (2009). Robust statistics. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Izci, F., Gultekin, B. K., Saglam, S., Koc, M. I., Zincir, S. B., & Atmaca, M. (2014).

Temperament, character traits, and alexithymia in patients with panic disorder.

Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat, 10, 879-885. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s62647

Johansson, P., Hoglend, P., Ulberg, R., Amlo, S., Marble, A., Bogwald, K. P., . . .

Heyerdahl, O. (2010). The mediating role of insight for long-term improvements

in psychodynamic therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol, 78(3), 438-448. doi:

10.1037/a0019245

Johnson, P. O., & Fay, L. C. (1950). The Johnson-Neyman technique, its theory and

application. Psychometrika, 15(4), 349-367. doi: 10.1007/bf02288864

Kallestad, H., Valen, J., McCullough, L., Svartberg, M., Høglend, P., & Stiles, T. C.

(2010). The relationship between insight gained during therapy and long-term

outcome in short-term dynamic psychotherapy and cognitive therapy for cluster C

personality disorders. Psychotherapy Research, 20(5), 526-534. doi:

10.1080/10503307.2010.492807

Keefe, J. R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2018). Changing character: A narrative review of

personality change in psychotherapies for personality disorder. Psychotherapy

Research, 1-18. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1425930

Keefe, J. R., McCarthy, K. S., Dinger, U., Zilcha-Mano, S., & Barber, J. P. (2014). A

meta-analytic review of psychodynamic therapies for anxiety disorders. Clinical

Page 39: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

27

Psychology Review, 34(4), 309-323. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.004

Keefe, J. R., Phillips, A. C., Busch, F. N., & Milrod, B. L. (2016). Panic-focused

psychodynamic psychotherapy rating scale - manual. Unpublished manuscript,

University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.

Keefe, J. R., Webb, C. A., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2016). In cognitive therapy for depression,

early focus on maladaptive beliefs may be especially efficacious for patients with

personality disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol, 84(4), 353-364. doi:

10.1037/ccp0000071

Klass, E. T., Milrod, B. L., Leon, A. C., Kay, S. J., Schwalberg, M., Li, C., & Markowitz,

J. C. (2009). Does Interpersonal Loss Preceding Panic Disorder Onset Moderate

Response to Psychotherapy? An Exploratory Study. The Journal of clinical

psychiatry, 70(3), 406-411.

Koller, M., & Stahel, W. A. (2011). Sharpening Wald-type inference in robust regression

for small samples. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 55(8), 2504-2515.

doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.csda.2011.02.014

Koller, M., & Stahel, W. A. (2016). Nonsingular subsampling for regression S estimators

with categorical predictors. Computational Statistics, 1-16. doi: 10.1007/s00180-

016-0679-x

Levy, K. N., Meehan, K. B., Kelly, K. M., Reynoso, J. S., Weber, M., Clarkin, J. F., &

Kernberg, O. F. (2006). Change in attachment patterns and reflective function in a

randomized control trial of transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline

Page 40: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

28

personality disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol, 74(6), 1027-1040. doi:

10.1037/0022-006x.74.6.1027

Lorenzo-Luaces, L., Driessen, E., DeRubeis, R. J., Van, H. L., Keefe, J. R., Hendriksen,

M., & Dekker, J. (2017). Moderation of the Alliance-Outcome Association by

Prior Depressive Episodes: Differential Effects in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy

and Short-Term Psychodynamic Supportive Psychotherapy. Behavior Therapy,

48(5), 581-595. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.011

Luborsky, L. (1984). Principles of psychoanalytic therapy: A manual for supportive-

expressive treatment. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Maechler, M., Rousseeuw, P., Croux, C., Todorov, V., Ruckstuhl, A., Salibian-Barrera,

M., . . . diPalma, M. A. (2016). robustbase: Basic Robust Statistics (Version 0.92-

7). Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/robustbase/index.html

McCarthy, K. S., Keefe, J. R., & Barber, J. P. (2016). Goldilocks on the couch: Moderate

levels of psychodynamic and process-experiential technique predict outcome in

psychodynamic therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 26(3), 307-317. doi:

10.1080/10503307.2014.973921

Milrod, B., Chambless, D. L., Gallop, R., Busch, F. N., Schwalberg, M., McCarthy, K.

S., . . . Barber, J. P. (2016). Psychotherapies for Panic Disorder: A Tale of Two

Sites. Journal of clinical psychiatry, 77(7), 927-935. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09507

Milrod, B., Leon, A. C., Busch, F., Rudden, M., Schwalberg, M., & Clarkin, J. (2007). A

randomized controlled clinical trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for panic

disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 265-272.

Page 41: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

29

Owen, J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2014). Treatment adherence: The importance of therapist

flexibility in relation to therapy outcomes. Journal of Counseling Psychology,

61(2), 280-288. doi: 10.1037/a0035753

Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Acklin, M. W. (1993). Alexithymia in panic

disorder and simple phobia: a comparative study. American Journal of Psychiatry,

150(7), 1105-1107. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.7.1105

Pitman, S., Slavin-Mulford, J., & Hilsenroth, M. (2014). Psychodynamic techniques

related to outcome for anxiety disorder patients at different points in treatment.

Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 202(5), 391-396. doi:

10.1097/nmd.0000000000000137

Poulsen, S., Lunn, S., Daniel, S. I., Folke, S., Mathiesen, B. B., Katznelson, H., &

Fairburn, C. G. (2014). A randomized controlled trial of psychoanalytic

psychotherapy or cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 171(1), 109-116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12121511

R Core Team. (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing

(Version 3.4.3). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

Rubin, D. B., & Little, R. J. A. (2002). Statistical analyses with missing data (2nd ed.).

New York, NY: Wiley.

Rudden, M., Milrod, B., Target, M., Ackerman, S., & Graf, E. (2006). Reflective

functioning in panic disorder patients: a pilot study. J Am Psychoanal Assoc,

54(4), 1339-1343.

Page 42: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

30

Sandell, R., Svensson, M., Nilsson, T., Johansson, H., Viborg, G., & Perrin, S. (2015).

The POSE study - panic control treatment versus panic-focused psychodynamic

psychotherapy under randomized and self-selection conditions: study protocol for

a randomized controlled trial. Trials, 16(1), 130. doi: 10.1186/s13063-015-0656-7

Sasso, K. E., Strunk, D. R., Braun, J. D., DeRubeis, R. J., & Brotman, M. A. (2015).

Identifying moderators of the adherence-outcome relation in cognitive therapy for

depression. J Consult Clin Psychol, 83(5), 976-984. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000045

Scocco, P., Barbieri, I., & Frank, E. (2007). Interpersonal Problem Areas and Onset of

Panic Disorder. Psychopathology, 40(1), 8-13.

Shear, M. K., Brown, T. A., Barlow, D. H., Money, R., Sholomskas, D. E., Woods, S.

W., . . . Papp, L. A. (1997). Multicenter collaborative panic disorder severity

scale. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(11), 1571-1575. doi:

10.1176/ajp.154.11.1571

Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater

reliability. Psychol Bull, 86(2), 420-428.

Stekhoven, D. J., & Bühlmann, P. (2011). MissForest—non-parametric missing value

imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics, 28(1), 112-118. doi:

10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597

Summers, R. F., & Barber, J. P. (2010). Psychodynamic therapy: A guide to evidence-

based practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Tasca, G. A., Hilsenroth, M., & Thompson-Brenner, H. (2014). Psychoanalytic

Psychotherapy or Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Bulimia Nervosa. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 171(5), 583-584. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121616

Page 43: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

31

Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2007). Emotion regulation difficulties associated with the

experience of uncued panic attacks: evidence of experiential avoidance, emotional

nonacceptance, and decreased emotional clarity. Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 378-

391. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.006

White, I. R., Royston, P., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Multiple imputation using chained

equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med, 30(4), 377-399. doi:

10.1002/sim.4067

Zilcha-Mano, S., McCarthy, K. S., Dinger, U., Chambless, D. L., Milrod, B. L., Kunik,

L., & Barber, J. P. (2015). Are there subtypes of panic disorder? An interpersonal

perspective. J Consult Clin Psychol, 83(5), 938-950. doi: 10.1037/a0039373

Page 44: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

32

Table 1.1

Descriptive Data for Baseline Characteristics and for Symptom Change

Baseline Measure Mean (SD) or # (%)

Baseline PDSS 13.9 (3.2)

PDSS Change Week 1 to 5 -1.5 (3.6)

PDSS Change Week 5 to 9 -1.2 (3.4)

SDS 16.1 (6.3)

HAM-D 10.6 (4.8)

IIP 1.2 (0.5)

Agoraphobia Diagnosis 53 (81.5%)

Age 39.5 (14.0)

Gender (Female) 44 (67.7%)

Ethnicity (Hispanic) 8 (12.3%)

Race (Black, Other Non-Caucasian) 11 (16.9%), 4 (6.2%)

Concurrent Psychopharmacology 16 (24.6%)

Age of Panic Onset (years) 27.5 (11.4)

SCID-II PersD Diagnosis 32 (49.2%)

Cluster A PersD Traits 1.4 (1.9)

Cluster B PersD Traits 2.7 (3.2)

Cluster C PersD Traits 4.0 (3.1)

Total PersD Traits 8.0 (6.3)

Note. HAM-D = Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; IIP = Inventory of Interpersonal

Problems; PDSS = Panic Disorder Severity Scale; SCID-II = Structured Clinical

Interview for the Diagnosis of Axis-II Disorders; SDS = Sheehan Disability Scale

Page 45: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

33

Table 1.2

Descriptive Statistics for Technique Process Measurements (Average per 15-minute

Segment)

Process Measurement Session 2

M (SD)

Session 10

M (SD)

Stability

Coefficient

Change in

Technique Use

Panic-Focused

Interpretation

0.9 (0.6) 1.2 (0.7) r =

0.50***

d = 0.55***

Panic-Focused

Clarification

2.3 (1.0) 2.3 (1.1) r = 0.09 d = 0.11

Non-Panic-Focused

Interpretations

0.2 (0.4) 0.5 (0.6) r = 0.03 d = 0.53**

Note. * = p <.05; ** = p <.01; *** = p <.001; Cohen’s d for paired t test calculated using

formula tc (Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, & Burke, 1996)

Table 1.3

Correlations between Technique Process Measurements and Baseline Characteristics

Baseline

Characteristic

Panic Focus—

Interpretation

(S2/S10)

Panic Focus—

Clarification

(S2/S10)

Non-Panic Focused

Interpretations

(S2/S10)

PDSS r = -0.06 / -0.00 r = -0.13 / 0.08 r = -0.12 / 0.07

IIP r = 0.19 / 0.06 r = -0.07 / -0.02 r = -0.03 / 0.24

Note. All ps > .05. IIP = Inventory of Interpersonal Problems, PDSS = Panic Disorder

Severity Scale

Page 46: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

34

34

Figure 1.1. Estimated effect sizes for the relationship between psychotherapy technique

use at a given session and subsequent improvement in panic symptoms as measured by

the PDSS. Positive semipartial correlations indicate that higher levels of the intervention

are associated with more subsequent symptom improvement. Bars are 95% confidence

intervals.

Page 47: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

35

Figure 1.2. Estimated change in panic symptoms as measured by the PDSS between

Weeks 5 to 9, as a function of degree of interpersonal problems as measured by the IIP

and their interaction with use of panic-focused interpretations at session 10. Positive

values represent predicted symptom worsening, while negative values represent predicted

symptom improvement. All regression variables not displayed in the figure were set to

the sample means.

Page 48: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

36

Patient Personality and Emotional Process

Panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy (PFPP) (Busch, Milrod, Singer, &

Aronson, 2012) is a 24-session, 12-week evidence-based treatment for panic disorder

(PD; Beutel et al., 2013; Keefe, McCarthy, Dinger, Zilcha-Mano, & Barber, 2014; Milrod

et al., 2016; Milrod et al., 2007) that focuses on understanding and working through

unrealized or disavowed meanings surrounding the onset of acute attacks and associated

anxiety in patients with panic disorder. One common meaning might be real or imagined

loss of attachment figures, as suggested by epidemiological data showing high

comorbidity or past history of separation anxiety among panic patients (Kossowsky et al.,

2013; Milrod et al., 2014), and the fact that emotional stressors, such as relationship

conflict and interpersonal loss, frequently precede the development of panic disorder

(Klass et al., 2009; Scocco, Barbieri, & Frank, 2007). Recognizing the sometimes-

conflicted emotions and fantasies connected with experiences of panic and anxiety (such

as unacknowledged rage at attachment figures) is hypothesized to improve panic-specific

reflective functioning (PSRF; Rudden, Milrod, Target, Ackerman, & Graf, 2006). PSRF

is an interview-based measured intended to tap into the degree to which patients can

identify and discuss potential psychological meanings surrounding and triggers to

experiences of panic and anxiety (Rudden et al., 2006). Early improvement in PSRF has

been shown to predict subsequent improvement in panic symptoms in both PFPP and

cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) (Barber et al., under review).

The theory that informs CBTs for panic disorder focuses on patients’ tendencies

to catastrophically misinterpret bodily sensations (Clark et al., 1997) and thereby to fear

Page 49: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

37

those sensations (Boswell et al., 2013). However, specific problems in emotional

recognition and acceptance have also been observed in studies of patients with panic

disorder, suggesting that these factors may also contribute to panic experiences. Patients

with PD report higher rates of alexithymia, experiential avoidance, and lack of emotional

acceptance, compared to psychiatrically healthy controls and persons with simple phobias

(Galderisi et al., 2008; Izci et al., 2014; Parker, Taylor, Bagby, & Acklin, 1993; Tull &

Roemer, 2007). Relative to non-psychiatric controls, panic patients have also been

observed to use relatively more emotional avoidance strategies in response to viewing

negatively or positively-valenced film clips (Tull & Roemer, 2007). Experimentally

instructing use of such strategies in healthy controls (relative to allowing emotional

experience) promotes subjective distress, heightened physiological arousal in the

moment, and also physiological reactivity in a subsequent stressful interpersonal task

(Tull, Jakupcak, & Roemer, 2010).

Avoidance of emotions in day-to-day life may promote development of panic

attacks if emotional contents are not addressed—for example, a person may get strongly

physiologically aroused due to unacceptable emotions, but be unable to dissipate that

arousal due to lack of emotional awareness and a consequent inability to acknowledge

what is upsetting him/her. A patient may also attribute emotional arousal to frightening

somatic causes that can psychologically stand in for conflicted feelings. In support of this

conceptualization, a recent process study of CBT for panic disorder found that reductions

in patient reports of emotional suppression preceded improvements in catastrophic, body-

focused cognitions and panic symptoms (Strauss, Kivity, & Huppert, 2018).

Emotions and Outcome in Psychodynamic Therapies

Page 50: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

38

Psychodynamic frameworks conceptualize attempts to avoid affects and particular

affectively charged contents as contributing to the emergence of psychiatric symptoms

and psychosocial dysfunction (Subic-Wrana et al., 2016). Within these frameworks,

defense mechanisms work to maintain lack of awareness (Perry & Bond, 2012), which

occurs to protect the person from experiencing psychic danger, yet precludes the

individual’s ability to process and address the relevant conflicts or wishes. Panic patients

have been observed to have heightened use of so-called neurotic and immature defenses

relative to healthy controls (Busch, Shear, Cooper, Shapiro, & Leon, 1995; Calati, Oasi,

De Ronchi, & Serretti, 2010; Kipper et al., 2004), and to exhibit a unique defensive

profile compared to depression patients (Busch et al., 1995).

Therapeutic focus on difficult-to-express or disavowed affect has been commonly

considered to be a feature distinguishing psychodynamic therapies (PDT) from cognitive-

behavioral approaches (Blagys & Hilsenroth, 2000; McCarthy & Barber, 2009). In PDT,

such focus can be achieved via supportive interventions encouraging expression of affect,

clarification/confrontation highlighting important areas of affective exploration, or

interpretations of affectively-laden meanings. In a meta-analytic examination,

psychodynamic therapies in which therapists were coded by observers as being especially

affect-focused were more successful in symptomatic outcomes, with a medium effect size

(Diener, Hilsenroth, & Weinberger, 2007), although temporal precedence was not

established in most studies and some studies in fact involved patient expression. In two

recent studies assessing psychodynamic therapist technique specifically in anxiety

disorder therapies (Pitman, Slavin-Mulford, & Hilsenroth, 2014) and in a transdiagnostic

sample examining anxiety symptom improvement (Pitman, Hilsenroth, Weinberger,

Page 51: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

39

Conway, & Owen, 2017), higher focus by the therapist on unexpressed/avoided affects

was related to more pre-to-post treatment symptom improvement.

In PDTs, links (often temporally sequenced) have been found between the degree

to which a patient engages in emotional experiencing or processing and positive

outcomes (Abbass, Town, Ogrodniczuk, Joffres, & Lilliengren, 2017; Fisher, Atzil-

Slonim, Bar-Kalifa, Rafaeli, & Peri, 2016; Friederich et al., 2017; Johansson, Town, &

Abbass, 2014; Kramer, Pascual-Leone, Despland, & de Roten, 2015; Town, Abbass, &

Bernier, 2013; Town, Salvadori, Falkenström, Bradley, & Hardy, 2017), convergent with

findings in humanistic-experiential therapies (Pascual-Leone & Yeryomenko, 2016). A

recent meta-analysis suggested that, across psychotherapies and psychiatric disorders,

increased expression of emotion by the patient although many of the included studies had

unclear temporal precedence between the affective measurement and outcome (Peluso &

Freund, 2018). In their transtheoretical conception of the importance of emotional

expression and experiencing in psychotherapy, Lane, Ryan, Nadel, & Greenburg (2015)

propose that emotional activation of episodic and semantic memory content facilitates the

reconsolidation of those memories into new, potentially more adaptive forms. From a

psychodynamic perspective, heightened affective experiences in therapy may indicate

that a patient is tolerating more affectively charged material, allowing for working

through of conflicts and ultimately improvements in reflective functioning. Heightened

patient emotional experiencing has been examined as a positive predictor of improvement

in a study of intensive short-term psychodynamic therapy for patients with generalized

anxiety disorder (Lilliengren, Johansson, Town, Kisely, & Abbass, 2017), but we are not

aware of any other such study in anxiety.

Page 52: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

40

Study Hypotheses: Main Effects and Moderators

PFPP’s clinical theory predicts that patient affective engagement is important to

exploration of meanings and conflicts surrounding panic and anxiety experiences (Busch

et al., 2012). To examine the relationships between early in-session emotional

engagement and subsequent changes in symptoms and PSRF, we developed a measure of

in-session emotional expression. Emotional expression can be thought of as a broad

process marker of engaged emotions, incorporating basic aspects of both emotional

experience (i.e., does the patient exhibit non-verbal signs of emotional activation?;

McCullough et al., 2003) and processing (i.e., does the patient speak in an identifiable

manner about specific, current emotional experiences; Klein, Mathieu, Gendlin, &

Kiesler, 1969; Pascual-Leone & Greenburg, 2005)? Levels of emotional expression were

assessed in early sessions (2, 5, and 10) of a 24-session PFPP protocol from a two-site

randomized controlled trial comparing PFPP to CBT and applied relaxation training for

panic (Milrod et al., 2016). We hypothesized that patients with higher levels of emotional

expression in early PFPP sessions would experience greater symptom and PSRF

improvement subsequent to the process-measured sessions. Moreover, we examined both

overall patient emotional expression and expression of specific emotional states, to help

determine whether patient engagement with particular affects is especially important to

treatment process (e.g., grief; anger) or potentially deleterious to treatment (e.g., anxiety

in-session, as might be predicted by psychodynamic conflict models).

Moderators. Although many psychotherapy processes are thought of as

universally positive (e.g., the therapeutic alliance), it may be that some processes are

more important in some psychotherapies, relative to others. Also, the same process,

Page 53: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

41

depending on traits that can capture an aspect of specific functioning relating to affect,

may yield strong positive effects in some patients, little effect in others, and negative

effects in still others (cf. Lorenzo-Luaces et al., 2017). We hypothesized that patient

personality disorder traits related to constrained (obsessive-compulsive personality

disorder; OCPD) versus heightened (borderline personality disorder; BPD) affect would

moderate the relationship between emotional expression and symptom improvement.

A psychodynamic conception of OCPD might focus on OCPD patients’ tendency

to intellectualize emotional experiences (Caligor, Kernberg, & Clarkin, 2007; Summers

& Barber, 2010). The typically theorized repertoire of defense for OCPD patients

emphasizes focus on cognition and circumstance over affects (e.g., intellectualization;

isolation of affect; rationalization); a strong need for control, order, and perfection

defends against the dangers of experiencing and acting on affect and wishes that feel

destabilizing. Relative to psychiatrically healthy individuals, OCPD patients report being

less accepting of their emotions, less clear about what their emotions mean, and more

distressed by feeling emotional (Steenkamp, Suvak, Dickstein, Shea, & Litz, 2015). In

treatment with OCPD patients, focusing on affect may help to counteract defenses that

represent avoidance of emotional conflicts (Barber & Muenz, 1996). We thus

hypothesized that patients with more OCPD personality traits as indicated by the SCID-II

(First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997) would show a stronger relationship

between early levels of emotional expression and later symptomatic improvements, as for

these patients emotional expression may particularly indicate a more flexible use of

defense in-session, relative to their typical profile.

Page 54: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

42

In contrast, we hypothesized that patients with increasingly more BPD traits

would show no or even negative relationships between early emotional expression and

later improvements. While BPD is a relatively uncommon comorbidity to PD (Friborg,

Martinussen, Kaiser, Overgard, & Rosenvinge, 2013), many patients exhibit elevated

BPD pathology. In general and clinical populations, qualifying for even one DSM-

defined BPD criterion is uniquely prognostic of significant interpersonal dysfunction and

psychosocial disability (Ellison, Rosenstein, Chelminski, Dalrymple, & Zimmerman,

2016; Zimmerman, Chelminski, Young, Dalrymple, & Martinez, 2012), indicating that

even non-diagnostic BPD may be clinically relevant. Affective dysregulation is a

common if not defining feature of BPD. BPD patients show stronger reactivity to

interpersonal events (Santangelo, Bohus, & Ebner-Priemer, 2012), as well as more labile

mood around a more negative baseline (Ebner-Priemer et al., 2015), relative to healthy

controls and MDD patients. Accordingly, the therapist’s efforts to help patients regulate,

understand, and usefully work with difficult-to-comprehend affects has been identified as

a common feature across many empirically supported psychotherapies for BPD

(Bateman, Gunderson, & Mulder, 2015). For patients with more BPD traits, relatively

lower levels of emotional expression in PFPP may reflect successful work by the

therapist and patient to contain affect in order to better work with the meanings and

circumstances surrounding panic and anxiety (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009). Relatively lower

emotional expression may also indicate defensive flexibility among BPD patients, who

typically engage in defenses focusing on prominent, moment-to-moment affects (e.g.,

splitting; acting out; Kramer, de Roten, Perry, & Despland, 2013; Perry, Presniak, &

Page 55: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

43

Olson, 2013), such that emotional expression per se may sometimes involve typical

defensive processes and not primarily a flexible use of defense or processing of conflict.

Method

Participants

Patients. The present study is a secondary analysis of patients randomized to the

PFPP condition (N = 80) of a two-site randomized controlled trial comparing PFPP,

CBT, and applied relaxation training among patients with primary DSM-IV panic

disorder with or without agoraphobia. Patients were recruited at <site A> and <site B>.

Participants received study treatment gratis. Participants gave informed written consent.

Both sites’ institutional review boards approved the protocol, and the study is registered

with ClinicalTrials.gov (identifier: NCT00353470).

Patients were included in the trial if they had one or more weekly spontaneous

panic attacks for the month before trial entry, and qualified for DSM-IV diagnosis of

primary panic disorder with or without agoraphobia determined as per the ADIS-IV

(DiNardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1995). Cross-site agreement on ADIS ratings for panic

severity (with a “4” indicating the diagnostic threshold) was excellent (ICC = 1.00; per

the norms of Portney & Watkins, 2000). See Milrod et al., 2016 for further details.

Non-study psychotherapy was prohibited. Medications were permitted if stable

for at least two months at presentation, and were recorded, held constant, and monitored

during the trial. Exclusion criteria were: active substance dependence (less than 6

months’ remission), a history of psychosis or bipolar disorder, acute suicidality, and

organic mental syndrome. Additional details on trial design, independent evaluator

Page 56: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

44

training, and therapy adherence can be consulted in the primary outcome paper (Milrod et

al., 2016).

Assessment of personality disorder (PersD). The Structured Clinical Interview

for Axis-II disorders (First et al., 1997) was used to assess the presence of PersD criteria

and diagnoses as defined by DSM-IV. Trained, independent masters’ level diagnosticians

uninformed to treatment condition administered the interviews. Cross-site interrater

reliability was excellent for number of OCPD criteria scored as present (ICC[2,1] =

1.00), and moderate for BPD criteria (ICC[2,1] = 0.78).

Therapists. All therapists were experienced therapists (Ph.D. or M.D.) who were

specifically trained in PFPP over the span of a 2-day, 12-hour course. Therapists had an

average of 15 years of post-graduate experience (SD = 8.2), and an average of 5 years’

experience in some form of time-limited psychodynamic therapy (SD = 6.3). Therapists

participated in monthly group supervision and received regular individual supervision

from senior clinicians. For the primary outcome paper, adherence to PFPP was

established, and additional information on the number and training of therapists can be

found there (Milrod et al., 2016).

Outcome Indices

Panic Disorder Severity Scale (PDSS; Shear et al., 1997). The PDSS is a

diagnosis-based, composite, global observer rating of panic disorder severity, with

acceptable psychometric properties. The PDSS was administered by trained, master’s-

level diagnosticians, uninformed as to treatment condition, based on an interview guide.

Interrater reliability on the PDSS was excellent (ICC[2,1] = 0.95). The PDSS was

Page 57: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

45

administered five times during treatment: at baseline (Week 0), Week 1, Week 5, Week

9, and termination (Week 12).

Panic-Specific Reflective Functioning (PSRF; Rudden et al., 2006). PSRF is

an interview-based measure that assesses the degree to which patients can recognize the

psychological contributions to their panic symptoms. Respondents are queried as to their

understanding of panic, how it has changed over time, and whether they notice any

concordance between panic and emotional states. Each response is rated for

psychological mindedness and complexity, and an overall score is based on item scores.

An example of a more impaired PSRF answer might be: “It’s the heat, the heat brings

them on” in response to “Why do you think you have panic attacks?”; a less impaired

response to that question might be: “I notice that I get them when I am feeling a lack of

control in my personal relationships. I fear that others will leave me, or that I may want to

leave them.” PSRF narratives were reliably scored by trained raters (ICC[2,1] = 0.80).

Process Measures

Emotional Expression Rating Scale (EERS; Huque & Keefe, 20). The EERS is

an observer-rated scale developed to assess the degree to which patients are engaged in

emotional discourse. It was developed for this investigation by two study authors, with

reference to prior attempts to rate emotions in-session (e.g., Klein et al., 1969;

McCullough et al., 2003). The measure was designed to be relatively atheoretical and

broadly descriptive rather than reflecting a particular perspective on what constitutes (for

example) adaptive emotional experience or deep emotional processing. Contrasted with

other scales, it was designed for ease of use by individuals with less theoretical or clinical

Page 58: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

46

training. To incorporate information on both verbal and non-verbal indicators of

emotional expression, only sessions with usable video recordings could be sampled.

Ratings were made every five minutes to allow for a more precise and potentially valid

assessment of patient emotion expression relative to whole-session ratings, and to collect

temporal information about development of emotional responses throughout a session.

Raters made both an omnibus rating for overall emotional expression, and ratings for 4

broad emotion categories.

The primary rating concerns a patient’s overall emotional expression, which is

rated on a 0 to 5 scale. Higher scores are dependent on the peak of emotional intensity

during the rated segment, but also on the duration of non-neutral emotional expressivity.

A score of 0 indicates no notable emotional expression, or a relatively neutral or flat

affective state. A score of 2 indicates a low-to-medium but clear presence of emotional

expression lasting more than a few seconds, up to a minute, with use of affectively

charged language or verbal statements of feeling, and non-verbal indicators such as

choking up, tearing up, emphatic gestures, muscular tension, smiling, or physical

agitation/restlessness. Scores of 4 and 5 indicate a high, unconstrained peak affective

arousal with sustained emotional expression across the majority of the rated segment.

Ratings were made for emotional expression within-session, and not when a patient

reported how they were feeling in the past, with no clear indication that they are currently

feeling that particular emotion (e.g., reporting with a neutral tone that they were sad when

a parent died). In Table 2, examples of transcripts from higher- versus lower-emotional

expression therapy sessions from the present study are given.

Page 59: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

47

Expressions of four specific broad categories of emotions were coded, three of

which are referenced in the following analyses1:

Grief/Sadness: Grief/Sadness referred to the expression of grief, sadness,

sorrow, or regret. Raters were trained to distinguish between sadness

surrounding (for example) an interpersonal figure, and indistinct

depressive distress or self-attack, which was rated as Anxiety/Distress.

Nonverbal indicators included tearing up, sad vocal tone, choking up,

quavering voice, frowning, and crying. Raters were trained to pay

attention to clear verbal expressions of sadness, regret, or loss, or

particular expressions such as feelings of closeness and tenderness while

talking about a close attachment figure who died or whom they fear may

no longer be available to them.

Anger/Assertion: Anger/Assertion made a distinction between anger

directed toward an external or interpersonal figure, and anger directed

toward the self (e.g., self-attack; self-punishment), which was rated as

Anxiety/Distress. Anger/Assertion was rated if patients indicated clear

anger, criticized someone, asserted their needs or desires to someone, or

voiced wishes for recompense for a past misdeed. Nonverbal markers

included patients’ showing angry facial expressions, angry vocal tone,

becoming more tense, clenching their fists, gesturing aggressively, or

gritting their teeth.

1 Positive Affect was rarely rated as present and had low variance, and consequently we did not analyze

these scores.

Page 60: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

48

Anxiety/Distress: Anxiety/Distress was intended to be rated when a

patient appeared activated with clearly negative feeling and arousal, such

as negative global feelings about the self, anxious tension, or experiencing

undifferentiated aversive affect. Non-verbal Anxiety/Distress indicators

included visible discomfort, fidgeting and drawing inwards,

hyperventilating or full panic attack, and uncontrollable negative affect.

Verbal indicators included explicit references to feeling anxious or

distressed or statements of self-attack and hopelessness.

Correlations between emotion ratings on a per-5 minute basis indicated

independence of the specific emotions (see Appendix D).

A total of 15 undergraduate psychology majors were trained on this measure by

study authors JRK and ZH. Raters were trained by rating sessions not used in this

investigation until they reliably rated within one point of the trainers. Weekly-to-

biweekly anti-drift sessions were held to maintain reliability and discuss rating

challenges. Raters were uninformed to patient outcomes and other study patient data.

Videotapes were coded in a random order, and the average score between three raters was

used for all analyses.

Interrater reliability (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) as calculated in a linear mixed model

(Bates et al., 2017) was good to moderate per-5 minutes for all emotions (ICC [2,3]

Overall = 0.74, Grief/Sadness = 0.71, Anger/Assertion = 0.76, Anxiety/Distress = 0.66).

Statistical Analyses

Page 61: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

49

All analyses were conducted in the R statistical computing language (R Core

Team, 2017). All primary analyses were run using robust regressions as implemented in

the R package “Robustbase” (Maechler et al., 2016). Given the effective sample size (n =

44), robust regression was selected over standard regression for its superior properties of

robustness against multivariate outliers and deviation from homoscedasticity (Huber &

Ronchetti, 2009). Semi-partial correlation effect sizes (sr) were estimated for parameters

of interest.

Missing data. In the parent clinical trial, a not missing at random (NMAR)

pattern of treatment dropout was detected, such that patients with worse PDSS symptom

trajectories were more likely to terminate from treatment prematurely (Milrod et al.,

2016). When outcomes for treatment noncompleters are imputed in the NMAR context,

imputation and other missing data methods can lead to biased estimates and confidence

intervals (Graham, 2009). As such, only individuals who provided data up to Week 9 (the

4th assessment) were eligible for inclusion (n = 65; 81.3% of the intent-to-treat sample).

Moreover, due to the need for video to rate nonverbal indicators of emotional expression,

only individuals with videotaped sessions (rather than audio backups) were used. The

final sample size having videotaped sessions was n = 44.

Some trial completers were missing one out of three videotaped session due to

technical issues or therapist/research assistant error (n = 13, 29.5% of the reduced

sample). In contrast to noncompleters, process ratings for completers missing a video-

recording of a session can be presumed to be missing at random in relation to panic

symptom outcomes (Rubin & Little, 2002), and psychotherapy process ratings have been

imputed in past investigations (Keefe, Solomonov, et al., 2018; Lorenzo-Luaces et al.,

Page 62: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

50

2017). Random forest imputation (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, 2011) was used to impute

missing data for completers. For this dataset, a normalized root mean square error of

prediction was estimated at 0.12, indicating that imputation accuracy was estimated to be

more than adequate (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, 2011).

Analytic strategy. Using robust linear regressions, the average level of overall

emotional expression between Sessions 2, 5, and 10 (i.e., three sessions between baseline

and Week 5) was used to predict subsequent change in PDSS-measured panic

symptomatology between Week 5 and treatment termination (Week 12). We also

analyzed PSRF as a secondary outcome. Because site differences were detected in the

parent trial (Milrod et al., 2016), we explored whether site moderated focal effects for

this and all subsequently described analyses at a significance of at least p <.10. Control

covariates in each regression included the baseline value for either outcome, and the

degree of change in the variable of interest that occurred between baseline and Week 5

(i.e., during the measurement of emotional expression). In addition, two planned analyses

of moderators of the relationship between average emotional expression and PDSS

symptom improvement were conducted: baseline SCID-II OCPD criteria, and SCID-II

BPD criteria.

We also performed two secondary, exploratory analyses. First, for both PDSS and

PSRF change, we examined whether any of three specific types of emotional expression

(i.e., Grief/Sadness, Anger/Assertion, Anxiety/Distress) were predictive of subsequent

change. Second, in the context of recent debates about the degree to which stable therapy

process across sessions merely reflects trait-like features of patients (Falkenström, Finkel,

Sandell, Rubel, & Holmqvist, 2017), we examined whether having emotional expression

Page 63: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

51

at Session 10 greater than one’s average across Sessions 2, 5, and 10 predicted

subsequent symptom change (i.e., a term reflecting Session 10 minus the average of all

sessions). This analysis also simultaneously controlled for average emotional expression

within the same model.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

The average patient in this study had a baseline PDSS score of 13.8 (range 9 to

20), considered to be in the moderately-ill severity range for patients with comorbid

agoraphobia (Furukawa et al., 2009). Other patient demographics and clinical

information can be found in Table 1.

Descriptive statistics on mean levels of emotional expression across each session

can be found in Appendix D. We also examined the consistency of emotional expression

across measured sessions, using a linear mixed model framework (Bates et al., 2017;

Appendix D), which revealed that the majority of variance in emotional expression was

not trait-like across sessions.

PDSS Symptom Change

Patients who had higher levels of emotional expression across the three sampled

sessions (2, 5, and 10) experienced more panic symptom improvement subsequent to

Session 10 (B = -3.10 [95% CI: -5.27 to -0.93], SE = 1.07, t[40] = -2.88, p = 0.006, sr =

0.31), controlling for baseline panic symptoms and prior change in panic symptoms.

In a secondary analysis, having emotional expression at Session 10 that was

higher than one’s average emotional expression across Sessions 2, 5, and 10 was also

predictive of having more symptom improvement subsequent to Session 10 (B = -3.58

Page 64: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

52

[95% CI: -5.78 to -1.37], SE = 1.09, t[39] = -3.28, p = 0.002, sr = 0.38). This was true

even though the model also controlled for a patient’s average level of emotional

expression, which remained itself a statistically significant predictor of subsequent panic

symptom improvements (B = -2.90 [95% CI: -4.70 to -1.09], SE = 0.89, t[39] = -3.24, p =

0.002, sr = 0.29).

We further explored whether any specific type of emotional expression was

particularly responsible for this relationship, employing a model simultaneously

including as predictors average expression levels of grief/sadness, anger/assertion, and

anxiety/distress. Patients who expressed more grief/sadness across the sampled sessions

had superior subsequent panic outcomes (B = -5.25 [95% CI: -9.38 to -1.14], SE = 2.18,

t[38] = -2.41, p = 0.021, sr = 0.28), while neither anger/assertion (B = 2.43 [95% CI: -

1.61 to 6.46], SE = 1.93, t[38] = 1.25, p = 0.217, sr = -0.16) nor anxiety/distress (B = 0.01

[95% CI: -5.37 to 5.40], SE = 2.68, t[38] = 0.01, p = 0.996, sr = 0.00) were significant

predictors of symptom change. Figure 1 displays these relationships for overall emotional

expression and specific emotions.

Personality Moderators of Symptom Change

Next, we examined our moderation hypotheses as to whether BPD and OCPD

personality traits would predict a smaller or greater relationship (respectively) between

emotional expression and symptom improvements. Consistent with our hypothesis, the

number of baseline SCID-II BPD criteria a patient met significantly interacted with

overall emotional expression, such that meeting more BPD criteria attenuated the

relationship between overall emotional expression and symptom improvement (B = 2.36

[95% CI: 1.09 to 3.63], SE = 0.63, t[38] = 3.76, p <.001, sr = 0.29). Unpacking this

Page 65: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

53

continuous interaction using the Johnson-Neyman technique, we found that patients

meeting 1 or no BPD criteria evidenced a significant, positive relationship between

emotional expression and symptom improvement (sr = 0.33, p = 0.003; see Figure 2). By

contrast, there was a nonsignificant, negative relationship between emotional expression

and improvement for patients meeting 2 or more BPD criteria (sr = -0.20, p = 0.062).

Thus, patients qualifying for a relatively low number of DSM-defined BPD criteria (2 or

more) exhibited no beneficial (or potentially even a negative) relationship to degree of

emotional expression compared to those with minimal (1) or no BPD pathology, who had

a significant, positive relationship.

However, as concerned SCID-II OCPD criteria, there was a significant interaction

between OCPD criteria, emotional expression, and site (p = 0.036). At Site A, the

hypothesized interaction was obtained, whereby patients meeting more OCPD criteria at

baseline had an increasingly positive relationship between early emotional expression and

subsequent symptom improvement (B = -4.48 [95% CI: -7.72 to -1.24], SE = 1.59, t[34] =

-2.81, p = 0.008, sr = 0.34). At Site B, there was no significant interaction (B = 0.48

[95% CI: -2.11 to 3.07], SE = 1.27, t[34] = 0.38, p = 0.707, sr = -0.04). Thus, OCPD

criteria cannot be considered a clear moderator.

Panic-Specific Reflective Functioning Change

Mirroring the model analyzing symptomatic outcomes, patients with a higher

level of emotional expression across the three sessions had greater gains in PSRF

subsequent to the measured sessions (B = 0.93 [95% CI: 0.03 to 1.83], SE = 0.45, t[40] =

2.10, p = 0.042, sr = 0.23), controlling for their baseline PSRF and early PSRF changes.

Neither the number of OCPD (B = 0.42 [95% CI: -0.27 to 1.11], t[38] = 1.24, p = 0.223,

Page 66: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

54

sr = 0.21) nor BPD criteria (B = -0.42 [95% CI: -1.12 to 0.27], t[38] = -1.23, p = 0.226, sr

= 0.20) a patient met significantly moderated this relationship.

We examined whether any specific type of emotional expression drove this

relationship. In this case, there were no specific significant relations as concerned

expression of grief/sadness (B = 1.43 [95% CI: -1.62 to 4.48], SE = 1.51, t[38] = 0.95, p =

0.350, sr = 0.15), anger/assertion (B = 1.20 [95% CI: -0.54 to 2.95], SE = 0.86, t[38] =

1.40, p = 0.170, sr = 0.17), or anxiety/distress (B = -0.15 [95% CI: -2.02 to 1.71], SE =

0.92, t[38] = -0.17, p = 0.869, sr = -0.02).

Discussion

In panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy, patients who engage in more

emotional expression over the course of the first five weeks of therapy have superior

symptomatic outcomes across the remainder of the treatment. For interpretive context,

just under half of the average symptomatic change occurs after the first five weeks of

treatment (see Table 1). Our results are convergent with findings from psychodynamic

psychotherapy process studies investigating other operationalizations of emotional

processing or experiencing in-session (Abbass et al., 2017; Fisher et al., 2016; Friederich

et al., 2017; Johansson et al., 2014; Kramer et al., 2015; Town et al., 2013; Town,

Salvadori et al., 2017). These findings expand the literature on emotions in

psychodynamic therapy to the context of short-term manualized panic disorder-focused

treatment, and provide further evidence that emotional expression precedes rather than

contemporaneously occurs with improvements. This study thus provides relatively

stronger evidence that emotional expression is a therapy process that may help give rise

to good outcomes, rather than being merely a product of symptomatic alleviation.

Page 67: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

55

Notably, early emotional expression also predicted subsequent gains in PSRF,

which were found in the broader trial to predict subsequent panic improvements (Barber

et al., under review). Emotional expression may be reflective of mentalization in-session

of emotional meanings and conflicts contributing to instances of panic and anxiety. As

the patient works to tolerate and understand affectively-laden meanings and conflicts,

they are expected to be able to engage with them rather than feel them as undifferentiated

anxiety or frightening somatic fantasy (Busch et al., 2012). Emotionally working through

conflict may also serve to detoxify certain affects, meanings, or wishes for patients,

which may be typically experienced as distressing, dangerous, uncontrollable, or guilt-

provoking to the point of having a panic attack.

Clinically, our results suggest that emotionally flat or withdrawn discussions of

the contributors and contexts of anxiety are unlikely to help a patient substantively

experience and work with these meanings in psychodynamic therapy. Concordantly, past

work has found that psychodynamic interventions focused on expression of difficult-to-

express affects appear to be correlated with improvements in anxiety (Pitman et al., 2014;

Pitman et al., 2017), and that anxiety patients rate as especially helpful attempts to

explore unexpressed/avoided feelings in-session (Glock, Hilsenroth, & Curtis, 2018). The

presence versus absence of emotional expression may help a therapist distinguish

between a patient’s compliant or pseudo-insightful (intellectualized) acquiescence to the

therapist’s attempts to explore the emotional underpinnings of their anxiety, versus

productive, affective engagement in the therapeutic process.

In our exploratory analyses concerning which specific emotions contribute to the

observed relationships, only expression of grief/sadness significantly predicted

Page 68: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

56

subsequent symptom outcomes. PFPP’s clinical theory emphasizes the degree to which

actual or prospective attachment losses can precipitate panic disorder itself, often because

patients have difficulty acknowledging the emotional importance of the loss (Busch et al.,

2012; Klass et al., 2009; Milrod, Leon, & Shear, 2004). Expression of grief/sadness may

sometimes reflect patients engaging with and becoming more tolerant of the true

emotional impact of the actual or feared loss.

Anger may not have emerged as a significant predictor despite its role in

psychodynamic models of panic (Busch et al., 1999; Busch et al., 2012), as anger may

sometimes have been directed toward figures regarding whom the patient already feels

comfortable experiencing anger. In PFPP, anger is conceptualized as being repressed

particularly when the patient is worried that his/her anger will provoke

separation/retaliation and hence loss of an ambivalently-held attachment figure (Busch et

al., 1999; Busch et al., 2012; Rudden et al., 2003). As such, access to and expression of

grief/sadness about loss that might be imagined to emerge as a result of angry feelings

might indicate a deeper processing of the underlying conflict. Expression of disavowed or

unconscious anger, for instance regarding attachment figures, may be found to have

stronger relationships to outcomes compared to general anger. Interestingly, in contrast

with our findings, in an investigation of intensive short-term dynamic psychotherapy for

treatment-resistant major depressive disorder, experiencing of anger but not grief was

related to better depression outcomes (Town, Falkenstrom, Abbass, & Stride, 2017;

Town, Salvadori et al., 2017). It may be that difficulties tolerating particular, core

emotions are more common to specific symptoms.

Page 69: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

57

Finally, anxiety/distress in-session was nonpredictive of outcome. In PFPP,

presence of anxiety in-session is not considered negative per se, as for example the

patient may feel anxious because the therapeutic work is addressing important conflicted

material, and that anxiety can be dealt with in real-time (Busch et al., 2012). From the

perspective of PFPP, a therapy that transpires without affects sometimes considered

“negative” or “inhibitory” (McCullough et al., 2003) may reflect that particular important

dynamics are not being brought into the treatment by the patient or pursued by the

therapist. Our metric of Overall emotional expression incorporated all deviations from a

neutral emotional state—which could include so-called negative/inhibitory affects—in

part to reflect our interest in emotional engagement in treatment writ large relative to

therapies in which comparatively minimal affect was mobilized in-session. Other

investigations with different goals may choose to use a modified version of the EERS

explicitly excluding Anxiety/Distress codes from consideration in the Overall score, or

other metrics that attempt to explicitly distinguish negative/inhibitory affects, such as the

Achievement of Therapeutic Objectives Scale (McCullough et al., 2003).

We also examined whether personality disorder traits theoretically related to

constrained (OCPD) or heightened (BPD) emotional expression moderated the

relationship between emotional expression and symptom change. OCPD traits only

moderated the predictive value of emotional expression at one of the two treatment sites,

such that perhaps unmeasured differences in process or patient population further

affected this relationship—for instance, patients at Site B in the trial were much more

likely to be on psychotropic medications and had a significantly higher number of

medication classes taken (Milrod et al., 2016). On the other hand, the presence of

Page 70: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

58

elevated BPD traits diminished the relationship between emotional expression and

symptom improvements. With patients meeting relatively more BPD criteria (>1), the

PFPP therapist may wish to focus on helping a patient slow down and contain their strong

affects to better work with the meanings giving rise to powerful emotional experiences.

Of note, meeting relatively more subthreshold BPD criteria per se was not a negative

prognostic indicator for PFPP patients in this trial and in fact BPD symptoms improved

more in PFPP than in CBT in this study (Keefe, Milrod, Gallop, Barber, & Chambless,

2018), suggesting that these patients can benefit from PFPP, given therapeutic processes

that are tailored to their needs. However, few patients in this study met SCID II criteria

for a full comorbid BPD diagnosis, potentially because acute suicidality was an exclusion

criterion, limiting our assessments to PD patients with low levels of borderline pathology.

Limitations and future directions

Several patients had one session (of three) unavailable to be rated due to

videotape missingness (n = 13, 29.5% of the reduced sample). However, we employed a

standard, validated method for imputing missing ratings (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, 2011),

and a complete data analysis showed similar results. In addition, our effective sample size

was small (n = 44).

In our study, average levels of emotional expression on the level of the session

were relatively low (around a 1 on the 5-point scale), with the range of rated values

consisting of emotionally flat sessions (mean 0) to heightened but not (consistently)

highly activated sessions (mean 2). Our obtained relationships for emotional expression

generally do not include representation of consistently high-to-extremely emotional

sessions, and it is possible that such consistently high expression would not relate to

Page 71: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

59

positive outcomes. There was mixed evidence whether maximum emotional expression

rated in a session (rather than average across segments) was predictive of outcomes (see

Footnote 2), with a trend level relationship for grief/sadness (cf. Kramer et al., 2015).

Emotional expression may also have a different relationship to outcome later in

treatment.

Our process measure to assess emotional expression, based on the apparent

intensity and duration of expression, consisted of a simpler operationalization of

emotional experiencing/processing than other measures used heretofore by

psychodynamic and process-experiential researchers. For example, there is compelling

evidence that in emotion-focused experiential psychotherapy (Greenberg, 2015), a

prototypical sequence of emotional processing moves from feelings of undifferentiated

global distress toward feeling self-assertive anger or adaptive, relieving experiences of

grief over past hurts and losses (Pascual-Leone, 2017). It could also be that emotional

expression at particular moments in therapy—such as discussing specific relationship

episodes—may be a stronger marker of good process.

Emotional expression as measured by the EERS also did not attempt to

distinguish between more versus less adaptive expressions. We were instead primarily

interested in how manifest emotional expression was a marker of good clinical process in

this treatment. Defining adaptive versus maladaptive/inhibitory affect in-session is a

potentially important but complex task, and different psychodynamic approaches use

varying lenses for understanding affect as a treatment mechanism (for a broad survey of

heterogeneity in defining adaptiveness of affect in different therapy schools, see the

different measures used in the Peluso & Freund et al., 2018 meta-analysis). What is

Page 72: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

60

“adaptive” affect may also strongly differ from patient to patient: for many patients

entering PFPP, expressing anger to a romantic partner might be a novel experience contra

their typical defenses against aggression, whereas for a patient with a more borderline

personality organization this same action could more frequently reflect splitting as a

defense. To our knowledge, our study is one of the first to examine how patient context

(e.g., BPD traits) influences how emotional expression relates to treatment success; such

personalized examinations may more specifically elucidate the vagaries of emotional

process in-session, even when using scales that aim to identify only adaptive or deep

emotional expression.

However, one advantage of our method is that, by definition, for more nuanced

emotional experiencing or processing to take place, a basic level of emotional expression

must nearly always also be present. Future work on this dataset might use our assay of

emotional expression across segments of sessions to orient and focus more detailed

research.

This study does not address the question of how a therapist may best affectively

engage patients in-session. Process work in psychodynamic therapy indicates that

focused confrontation interventions (Town, Hardy, McCullough, & Stride, 2012) or

interventions attempting to orient patients to their affects (Ulvenes et al., 2014) tend to be

associated with greater emotional experiencing on the part of the patient. Preliminary

work in this sample suggests that panic-focused interpretations—found to relate to

subsequent outcomes in PFPP (Keefe, Solomonov et al., 2018)—in one segment of

treatment may predict higher emotional expression in the next segment of treatment when

Page 73: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

61

patients were discussing an attachment relationship (Keefe, Huque et al., 2018). This

research is ongoing.

We also did not examine the extent to which patients engaged in emotional

expressions in the CBT and applied relaxation therapies in the trial. In CBT for

depression, some studies report that emotional processing in-session may predict better

post-treatment outcomes (Aafjes-van Doorn & Barber, 2017), but the role of emotional

processing and experiencing is less commonly explored in this family of treatments. In

CBT for panic, we might expect emotional expression to be unrelated or positively

related to treatment outcomes (e.g., restructuring “hot cognitions”; intensely engaging in

interoceptive exposure). For applied relaxation, emotional expression may have a

negative relationship to outcomes, given the goal of in-session progressive relaxation.

Future work could apply the EERS or other experiencing scales to less affect-focused

psychotherapies for panic.

Finally, in psychotherapy research, there has been increasing attention on

distinguishing a patient’s tendency to have a given process score across all sessions (i.e.,

the “between-patients” component) from within-patient changes in process scores,

accounting for their average levels of that process (Falkenström et al., 2017). Our

secondary analysis partially addresses this critique, suggesting that having higher than

one’s average level of emotional expression (Sessions 2, 5, 10) at Session 10 predicted

superior subsequent symptom improvement, even when simultaneously modeling the

predictive value of one’s average (i.e., “between-patients”) emotional expression. This

provides limited evidence that, even among patients who have a relatively higher level of

emotional expression across their early therapy, emotional expression over one’s typical

Page 74: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

62

level at a later session is positively prognostic of symptom improvement. Moreover, the

majority of variance in emotional expression was not trait-like and consistent across

patient sessions, indicating that it is less likely (though not impossible) that the “between-

patients” component of emotional expression is primarily driving the observed relations.

On the other hand, consistently high levels of a particular process, such as emotional

expression, may also reflect specific work within a unique therapeutic dyad, such that the

same patient working with a different or more/less skillful therapist would not show such

a pattern.

Conclusion

Emotional expression in short-term psychodynamic treatment of panic disorder

early in the course of therapy predicts greater symptomatic improvements later in

treatment, possibly through encouraging insight into the emotional meanings and

conflicts surrounding episodes of panic and anxiety. These results require replication.

References

Aafjes-van Doorn, K., & Barber, J. P. (2017). Systematic Review of In-Session Affect

Experience in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Depression. Cognitive Therapy

and Research, 41(6), 807-828. doi: 10.1007/s10608-017-9865-6

Abbass, A., Town, J., Ogrodniczuk, J., Joffres, M., & Lilliengren, P. (2017). Intensive

Short-Term Dynamic Psychotherapy Trial Therapy: Effectiveness and Role of

“Unlocking the Unconscious”. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,

205(6), 453-457. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000000684

Barber, J. P., & Muenz, L. R. (1996). The role of avoidance and obsessiveness in

matching patients to cognitive and interpersonal psychotherapy: empirical

Page 75: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

63

findings from the treatment for depression collaborative research program. J

Consult Clin Psychol, 64(5), 951-958.

Bateman, A. W., Gunderson, J., & Mulder, R. (2015). Treatment of personality disorder.

Lancet, 385(9969), 735-743. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(14)61394-5

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., Singmann, H., . . .

Green, P. (2017). lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4

(Version 1.1-13): Comprehensive R Archive Network. Retrieved from

https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html

Beutel, M. E., Scheurich, V., Knebel, A., Michal, M., Wiltink, J., Graf-Morgenstern, M.,

. . . Subic-Wrana, C. (2013). Implementing panic-focused psychodynamic

psychotherapy into clinical practice. Canadian journal of psychiatry, 58(6), 326-

334.

Blagys, M. D., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2000). Distinctive Features of Short-Term

Psychodynamic-Interpersonal Psychotherapy: A Review of the Comparative

Psychotherapy Process Literature. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,

7(2), 167-188. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.7.2.167

Boswell, J. F., Farchione, T. J., Sauer-Zavala, S., Murray, H. W., Fortune, M. R., &

Barlow, D. H. (2013). Anxiety Sensitivity and Interoceptive Exposure: A

Transdiagnostic Construct and Change Strategy. Behavior Therapy, 44(3), 417-

431. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.03.006

Busch, F. N., Milrod, B. L., Rudden, M., Shapiro, T., Singer, M., Aronson, A., & Roiphe,

J. (1999). Oedipal Dynamics in Panic Disorder. J Am Psychoanal Assoc, 47(3),

773-790. doi: 10.1177/00030651990470030401

Page 76: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

64

Busch, F. N., Milrod, B. L., Singer, M. B., & Aronson, A. C. (2012). Manual of panic

focused psychodynamic psychotherapy - extended range (Vol. 36). New York,

NY: Routledge.

Busch, F. N., Shear, M. K., Cooper, A. M., Shapiro, T., & Leon, A. C. (1995). An

empirical study of defense mechanisms in panic disorder. Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease, 183(5), 299-303. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199505000-00004

Calati, R., Oasi, O., De Ronchi, D., & Serretti, A. (2010). The use of the defence style

questionnaire in major depressive and panic disorders: A comprehensive meta-

analysis. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 83(1),

1-13. doi: 10.1348/147608309X464206

Caligor, E., Kernberg, O. F., & Clarkin, J. F. (2007). Handbook of dynamic

psychotherapy for higher level personality pathology (1st ed.). Washington, DC:

American Psychiatric Publishing.

Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Öst, L.-G., Breitholtz, E., Koehler, K. A., Westling, B.

E., . . . Gelder, M. (1997). Misinterpretation of body sensations in panic disorder.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(2), 203-213. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.65.2.203

Diener, M. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Weinberger, J. (2007). Therapist Affect Focus and

Patient Outcomes in Psychodynamic Psychotherapy: A Meta-Analysis. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 936-941. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.936

Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Houben, M., Santangelo, P., Kleindienst, N., Tuerlinckx, F.,

Oravecz, Z., . . . Kuppens, P. (2015). Unraveling affective dysregulation in

Page 77: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

65

borderline personality disorder: a theoretical model and empirical evidence. J

Abnorm Psychol, 124(1), 186-198. doi: 10.1037/abn0000021

Ellison, W. D., Rosenstein, L., Chelminski, I., Dalrymple, K., & Zimmerman, M. (2016).

The Clinical Significance of Single Features of Borderline Personality Disorder:

Anger, Affective Instability, Impulsivity, and Chronic Emptiness in Psychiatric

Outpatients. J Pers Disord, 30(2), 261-270. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2015_29_193

Falkenström, F., Finkel, S., Sandell, R., Rubel, J. A., & Holmqvist, R. (2017). Dynamic

models of individual change in psychotherapy process research. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85(6), 537-549. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000203

First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997).

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders, (SCID-

II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Fisher, H., Atzil-Slonim, D., Bar-Kalifa, E., Rafaeli, E., & Peri, T. (2016). Emotional

experience and alliance contribute to therapeutic change in psychodynamic

therapy. Psychotherapy, 53(1), 105-116. doi: 10.1037/pst0000041

Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2009). A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the

understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Dev

Psychopathol, 21(4), 1355-1381. doi: 10.1017/s0954579409990198

Friborg, O., Martinussen, M., Kaiser, S., Overgard, K. T., & Rosenvinge, J. H. (2013).

Comorbidity of personality disorders in anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of 30

years of research. J Affect Disord, 145(2), 143-155. doi:

10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.004

Page 78: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

66

Friederich, H. C., Brockmeyer, T., Wild, B., Resmark, G., de Zwaan, M., Dinkel, A., . . .

Herzog, W. (2017). Emotional Expression Predicts Treatment Outcome in Focal

Psychodynamic and Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Anorexia Nervosa:

Findings from the ANTOP Study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 86(2),

108-110.

Furukawa, T. A., Katherine Shear, M., Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Woods, S. W.,

Money, R., . . . Leucht, S. (2009). Evidence-based guidelines for interpretation of

the Panic Disorder Severity Scale. Depress Anxiety, 26(10), 922-929. doi:

10.1002/da.20532

Galderisi, S., Mancuso, F., Mucci, A., Garramone, S., Zamboli, R., & Maj, M. (2008).

Alexithymia and cognitive dysfunctions in patients with panic disorder.

Psychother Psychosom, 77(3), 182-188. doi: 10.1159/000119738

Glock, G., Hilsenroth, M., & Curtis, R. (2018). Therapeutic interventions patients with

anxiety problems find most helpful: A case for integration? Journal of

Psychotherapy Integration, 28(2), 233-241. doi: 10.1037/int0000075

Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu Rev

Psychol, 60, 549-576. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530

Greenberg, L. (2015). Emotion-focus therapy: Coaching clients to work through their

feelings (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Huber, P. J., & Ronchetti, E. M. (2009). Robust statistics. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Huque, Z. M., & Keefe, J. R. (2017). A manual for rating basic emotional expression in

psychotherapy. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.

Page 79: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

67

Izci, F., Gultekin, B. K., Saglam, S., Koc, M. I., Zincir, S. B., & Atmaca, M. (2014).

Temperament, character traits, and alexithymia in patients with panic disorder.

Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat, 10, 879-885. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s62647

Johansson, R., Town, J. M., & Abbass, A. (2014). Davanloo’s Intensive Short-Term

Dynamic Psychotherapy in a tertiary psychotherapy service: overall effectiveness

and association between unlocking the unconscious and outcome. PeerJ, 2, e548.

doi: 10.7717/peerj.548

Keefe, J. R., Huque, Z. M., DeRubeis, R. J., Chambless, D., Barber, J. P., & Milrod, B. L.

(2018). A cross-lagged examination of panic-focused psychodynamic

interventions and emotional expression in-session. Paper presented at the Society

for Psychotherapy Research, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Keefe, J. R., McCarthy, K. S., Dinger, U., Zilcha-Mano, S., & Barber, J. P. (2014). A

meta-analytic review of psychodynamic therapies for anxiety disorders. Clinical

Psychology Review, 34(4), 309-323. doi:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.004

Keefe, J. R., Milrod, B. L., Gallop, R., Barber, J. P., & Chambless, D. L. (2018). What is

the effect on comorbid personality disorder of brief panic-focused psychotherapy

in patients with panic disorder? Depression and Anxiety, 35(3), 239-247. doi:

doi:10.1002/da.22708

Keefe, J. R., Solomonov, N., Derubeis, R. J., Phillips, A. C., Busch, F. N., Barber, J. P., .

. . Milrod, B. L. (2018). Focus is key: Panic-focused interpretations are associated

with symptomatic improvement in panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy.

Psychotherapy Research, 1-12. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1464682

Page 80: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

68

Kipper, L., Blaya, C., Teruchkin, B., Heldt, E., Isolan, L., Mezzomo, K., . . . Manfro, G.

G. (2004). Brazilian Patients with Panic Disorder: The Use of Defense

Mechanisms and Their Association with Severity. J Nerv Ment Dis, 192(1), 58-

64. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000106001.84359.0f

Klass, E. T., Milrod, B. L., Leon, A. C., Kay, S. J., Schwalberg, M., Li, C., & Markowitz,

J. C. (2009). Does Interpersonal Loss Preceding Panic Disorder Onset Moderate

Response to Psychotherapy? An Exploratory Study. The Journal of clinical

psychiatry, 70(3), 406-411.

Klein, M. H., Mathieu, P., Gendlin, E. T., & Kiesler, D. J. (1969). The experiencing

scale: A research and training manual (Vol. 1). Madison, WI: Wisconsin

Psychiatric Institute.

Kossowsky, J., Pfaltz, M. C., Schneider, S., Taeymans, J., Locher, C., & Gaab, J. (2013).

The separation anxiety hypothesis of panic disorder revisited: a meta-analysis. Am

J Psychiatry, 170(7), 768-781. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070893

Kramer, U., de Roten, Y., Perry, J. C., & Despland, J.-N. (2013). Beyond splitting:

Observer-rated defense mechanisms in borderline personality disorder.

Psychoanalytic Psychology, 30(1), 3-15. doi: 10.1037/a0029463

Kramer, U., Pascual-Leone, A., Despland, J.-N., & de Roten, Y. (2015). One minute of

grief: Emotional processing in short-term dynamic psychotherapy for adjustment

disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83(1), 187-198. doi:

10.1037/a0037979

Page 81: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

69

Lane, R. D., Ryan, L., Nadel, L., & Greenberg, L. (2015). Memory reconsolidation,

emotional arousal, and the process of change in psychotherapy: New insights

from brain science. Behav Brain Sci, 38, e1. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x14000041

Lilliengren, P., Johansson, R., Town, J. M., Kisely, S., & Abbass, A. (2017). Intensive

Short-Term Dynamic Psychotherapy for generalized anxiety disorder: A pilot

effectiveness and process-outcome study. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy,

24(6), 1313-1321. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2101

Lorenzo-Luaces, L., Driessen, E., DeRubeis, R. J., Van, H. L., Keefe, J. R., Hendriksen,

M., & Dekker, J. (2017). Moderation of the Alliance-Outcome Association by

Prior Depressive Episodes: Differential Effects in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy

and Short-Term Psychodynamic Supportive Psychotherapy. Behavior Therapy,

48(5), 581-595. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.011

Maechler, M., Rousseeuw, P., Croux, C., Todorov, V., Ruckstuhl, A., Salibian-Barrera,

M., . . . diPalma, M. A. (2016). robustbase: Basic Robust Statistics (Version 0.92-

7). Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/robustbase/index.html

McCarthy, K. S., & Barber, J. P. (2009). The Multitheoretical List of Therapeutic

Interventions (MULTI): Initial report. Psychotherapy Research, 19(1), 96-113.

doi: 10.1080/10503300802524343

McCullough, L., Larsen, A. E., Schanche, E., Andrews, S., Kuhn, N., Hurley, C., &

Wolf, J. (2003). Achivement of Therapeutic Objectives Scale: ATOS scale.

Harvard Medical School.

Page 82: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

70

Milrod, B., Chambless, D. L., Gallop, R., Busch, F. N., Schwalberg, M., McCarthy, K.

S., . . . Barber, J. P. (2016). Psychotherapies for Panic Disorder: A Tale of Two

Sites. Journal of clinical psychiatry, 77(7), 927-935. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09507

Milrod, B., Leon, A. C., Busch, F., Rudden, M., Schwalberg, M., & Clarkin, J. (2007). A

randomized controlled clinical trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for panic

disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 265-272.

Milrod, B., Leon, A. C., & Shear, M. K. (2004). Can interpersonal loss precipitate panic

disorder? Am J Psychiatry, 161(4), 758-759. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.758

Milrod, B., Markowitz, J. C., Gerber, A. J., Cyranowski, J., Altemus, M., Shapiro, T., . . .

Glatt, C. (2014). Childhood separation anxiety and the pathogenesis and treatment

of adult anxiety. Am J Psychiatry, 171(1), 34-43. doi:

10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13060781

Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Acklin, M. W. (1993). Alexithymia in panic

disorder and simple phobia: a comparative study. American Journal of Psychiatry,

150(7), 1105-1107. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.7.1105

Pascual-Leone, A. (2017). How clients “change emotion with emotion”: A programme of

research on emotional processing. Psychotherapy Research, 1-18. doi:

10.1080/10503307.2017.1349350

Pascual-Leone, A., & Greenburg, L. S. (2005). Classification of Affective-Meaning

States (CAMS). In A. Pascual-Leone (Ed.), Emotional processing in the

therapeutic hour: Why "The only way out is through" (pp. 289-366). Toronto,

Ontario, Canada: York University.

Page 83: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

71

Pascual-Leone, A., & Yeryomenko, N. (2016). The client “experiencing” scale as a

predictor of treatment outcomes: A meta-analysis on psychotherapy process.

Psychotherapy Research, 1-13. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2016.1152409

Peluso, P. R., & Freund, R. R. (2018). Therapist and client emotional expression and

psychotherapy outcomes: A meta-analysis. Psychotherapy, 55(4), 461-472. doi:

10.1037/pst0000165

Perry, J. C., & Bond, M. (2012). Change in defense mechanisms during long-term

dynamic psychotherapy and five-year outcome. Am J Psychiatry, 169(9), 916-

925. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091403

Perry, J. C., Presniak, M. D., & Olson, T. R. (2013). Defense Mechanisms in Schizotypal,

Borderline, Antisocial, and Narcissistic Personality Disorders. Psychiatry:

Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 76(1), 32-52. doi:

10.1521/psyc.2013.76.1.32

Pitman, S., Slavin-Mulford, J., & Hilsenroth, M. (2014). Psychodynamic techniques

related to outcome for anxiety disorder patients at different points in treatment. J

Nerv Ment Dis, 202(5), 391-396. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000000137

Pitman, S. R., Hilsenroth, M. J., Weinberger, J., Conway, F., & Owen, J. (2017).

Psychotherapy Technique Related to Changes in Anxiety Symptoms With a

Transdiagnostic Sample. J Nerv Ment Dis, 205(6), 427-435. doi:

10.1097/nmd.0000000000000689

Portney, L. G., & Watkins, M. P. (2000). Foundations of clinical research: Applications

to practice. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

Page 84: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

72

R Core Team. (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing

(Version 3.4.3). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/

Rubin, D. B., & Little, R. J. A. (2002). Statistical analyses with missing data (2nd ed.).

New York, NY: Wiley.

Rudden, M., Busch, F. N., Milrod, B., Singer, M., Aronson, A., Roiphe, J., & Shapiro, T.

(2003). Panic disorder and depression: A psychodynamic exploration of

comorbidity. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 84(4), 997-1015. doi:

10.1516/VCKY-5EWM-7PT4-LRWJ

Rudden, M., Milrod, B., Target, M., Ackerman, S., & Graf, E. (2006). Reflective

functioning in panic disorder patients: a pilot study. J Am Psychoanal Assoc,

54(4), 1339-1343.

Santangelo, P., Bohus, M., & Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2012). Ecological Momentary

Assessment in Borderline Personality Disorder: A Review of Recent Findings and

Methodological Challenges. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28(4), 555-576.

doi: 10.1521/pedi_2012_26_067

Scocco, P., Barbieri, I., & Frank, E. (2007). Interpersonal Problem Areas and Onset of

Panic Disorder. Psychopathology, 40(1), 8-13.

Shear, M. K., Brown, T. A., Barlow, D. H., Money, R., Sholomskas, D. E., Woods, S.

W., . . . Papp, L. A. (1997). Multicenter collaborative panic disorder severity

scale. Am J Psychiatry, 154(11), 1571-1575. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1571

Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater

reliability. Psychol Bull, 86(2), 420-428.

Page 85: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

73

Steenkamp, M. M., Suvak, M. K., Dickstein, B. D., Shea, M. T., & Litz, B. T. (2015).

Emotional Functioning in Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder:

Comparison to Borderline Personality Disorder and Healthy Controls. J Pers

Disord, 29(6), 794-808. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2014_28_174

Stekhoven, D. J., & Bühlmann, P. (2011). MissForest—non-parametric missing value

imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics, 28(1), 112-118. doi:

10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597

Strauss, A. Y., Kivity, Y., & Huppert, J. D. (2018). Emotion Regulation Strategies in

Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Panic Disorder. Behavior Therapy. doi:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2018.10.005

Subic-Wrana, C., Greenberg, L. S., Lane, R. D., Michal, M., Wiltink, J., & Beutel, M. E.

(2016). Affective Change in Psychodynamic Psychotherapy: Theoretical Models

and Clinical Approaches to Changing Emotions. Z Psychosom Med Psychother,

62(3), 207-223. doi: 10.13109/zptm.2016.62.3.207

Summers, R. F., & Barber, J. P. (2010). Psychodynamic therapy: A guide to evidence-

based practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Town, J., Falkenstrom, F., Abbass, A., & Stride, C. (2017). Predicting change in

depression with Intensive Short-Term Dynamic Psychotherapy for Treatment

Resistant Depression. Paper presented at the Society for Psychotherapy Research,

Toronto, Ontario.

Town, J. M., Abbass, A., & Bernier, D. (2013). Effectiveness and cost effectiveness of

Davanloo's intensive short-term dynamic psychotherapy: does unlocking the

unconscious make a difference? Am J Psychother, 67(1), 89-108.

Page 86: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

74

Town, J. M., Hardy, G. E., McCullough, L., & Stride, C. (2012). Patient affect

experiencing following therapist interventions in short-term dynamic

psychotherapy. Psychother Res, 22(2), 208-219. doi:

10.1080/10503307.2011.637243

Town, J. M., Salvadori, A., Falkenström, F., Bradley, S., & Hardy, G. (2017). Is affect

experiencing therapeutic in major depressive disorder? Examining associations

between affect experiencing and changes to the alliance and outcome in intensive

short-term dynamic psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 54(2), 148-158. doi:

10.1037/pst0000108

Tull, M. T., Jakupcak, M., & Roemer, L. (2010). Emotion Suppression: A Preliminary

Experimental Investigation of Its Immediate Effects and Role in Subsequent

Reactivity to Novel Stimuli. Cogn Behav Ther, 39(2), 114-125. doi:

10.1080/16506070903280491

Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2007). Emotion regulation difficulties associated with the

experience of uncued panic attacks: evidence of experiential avoidance, emotional

nonacceptance, and decreased emotional clarity. Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 378-

391. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.006

Ulvenes, P. G., Berggraf, L., Wampold, B. E., Hoffart, A., Stiles, T., & McCullough, L.

(2014). Orienting patient to affect, sense of self, and the activation of affect over

the course of psychotherapy with cluster C patients. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 61(3), 315-324. doi: 10.1037/cou0000028

Zimmerman, M., Chelminski, I., Young, D., Dalrymple, K., & Martinez, J. (2012). Does

the presence of one feature of borderline personality disorder have clinical

Page 87: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

75

significance? Implications for dimensional ratings of personality disorders. The

Journal of clinical psychiatry, 73(1), 8-12. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06784

Page 88: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

76

Table 2.1

Descriptive data for baseline characteristics and for symptom change

Baseline Measure Mean (SD) or # (%)

Baseline PDSS 13.8 (2.9)

Baseline to Week 5 PDSS Change -3.9 (4.0)

Week 5 to Termination PDSS Change -2.9 (2.9)

Baseline PSRF 3.3 (1.1)

Baseline to Week 5 PSRF Change 0.9 (1.4)

Week 5 to Termination PSRF Change 0.0 (1.2)

SDS 16.9 (6.1)

HAM-D 10.4 (3.8)

Agoraphobia Diagnosis 35 (79.5%)

Age 37.8 (14.0)

Gender (Female) 31 (70.5%)

Concurrent Psychopharmacology 12 (27.3%)

Age of Panic Onset (years) 26.6 (11.2)

SCID-II OcPD Criteria 2.1 (1.7)

SCID-II BPD Criteria 1.0 (1.7)

Note. HAM-D = Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; IIP = Inventory of Interpersonal

Problems; PDSS = Panic Disorder Severity Scale; SCID-II = Structured Clinical

Interview for the Diagnosis of Axis-II Disorders; SDS = Sheehan Disability Scale

Page 89: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

77

Table 2.2

Example transcripts from sessions rated high/low in emotional expression

High Emotional Expression Transcript

(Session Average 1.75) Low Emotional Expression Transcript

(Session Average 0.28)

T: Is your boyfriend being on this

nocturnal schedule and maybe not

finishing his program a worry for you? I

feel that you’re frustrated and anxious in

an immediate sense of what’s going on

right now, but there’s some concern about

getting married, proceeding with that.

P: No, I mean, the pros outweigh the cons

< quiet, restrained tone of voice>.

T: I think maybe part of what might go on

inside is that thinking about these concerns

is at times a scary, anxiety-provoking

thought… because you want to proceed

with marrying him, and the pros outweigh

the cons, but he’s not taking care of

himself.

P: Yes… yes, I guess so. <tearing up>

Maybe that’s why… I don’t know, I mean,

he’s really smart, I just hope that he’ll

soon figure what he wants to focus on… I

know that in a few years if he didn’t

he’d… <begins to fully cry, cries for a few

moments>…<therapist offers tissue>…

Thank you…

T: You’re worried about that, and what

might happen to you both if that were to

happen.

P: <teary, weepy> I don’t know. He’s such

a Type-A perfectionist, I don’t know how

he keeps spiraling. I’m so worried about

him all the time, it really gets to me. I’m

really sad... <coded elevation in

Grief/Sadness>

P: It’s awful. I hate saying goodbyes. I

hate driving my son to the station to say

goodbye. I hate driving my sister to say

goodbye. <neutral tone and facial

expression>

T: How does it make you feel when you

do that?

P: I guess, and this is just pure

speculation, but I guess…

T: What is it that you feel? / P: Um. / T:

Sorry I cut you off. / P: No, that’s fine. It

makes me feel anxious, it makes me feel

sad, um, it’s just very hard, I don’t like it

at all <laughter; smiling incongruously to

statement>.

T: Just like the way you feel here.

P: When I have tears, they could literally

be for you and what you’re going

through, they could be for us, or it could

be, I don’t know. Because, I mean, there

would have to be a thought associated

with the emotion. A thought that, I’m

assuming, triggered the emotion.

<continued neutral tone/expression> And

I don’t know what that thought is. I’d like

to know what that thought is. But, um, the

leaving I don’t like. I don’t like goodbyes.

Never liked goodbyes. Well, I don’t know

if that’s true, but I don’t like goodbyes.

<coded small elevation in

Anxiety/Distress from increase in

physical agitation and pace of speech, but

overall still relatively neutral/flat>

Page 90: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

78

Note. Both transcripts are adapted from sessions from the trial, particularly Session 10,

for a patient with one of the top 3 ratings for emotional expression at this session versus a

patient with one of the bottom 3 ratings for emotional expression. Transcripts have been

edited for clarity and to eliminate potentially identifying information.

Page 91: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

79

Figure 2.1. Estimated effect sizes for the relationship between the average level of

emotion expression across sessions 2, 5, and 10, and subsequent improvement in panic

symptoms as measured by the PDSS. Positive semipartial correlations indicate that higher

levels of expression are associated with more subsequent symptom improvement. Bars

are 95% confidence intervals.

Page 92: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

80

Figure 2.2. Estimated change in panic symptoms as measured by the PDSS between

Weeks 5 to Termination (Week 12), as a function of the average overall emotional

expression between sessions 2, 5, and 10, and their interaction with the number of SCID-

II borderline personality disorder criteria a patient met at baseline. Increasingly negative

values represent greater predicted symptom improvement. All regression variables not

displayed in the figure were set to the sample means.

Page 93: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

81

What Mechanism Works for Whom in Panic

Heterogeneity between patients within a DSM-described disorder is widely

recognized clinically and, increasingly, empirically. Personalized medicine approaches to

psychiatric treatment seek to capitalize on this heterogeneity either by matching patients

to the treatment most likely to target their primary and comorbid DSM disorders, or, for

psychotherapies, by modifying a given treatment to best address the needs of a given

patient (Cohen & DeRubeis, in press).

While some dominant models of psychiatry operate explicitly or implicitly from a

latent trait conception—that different symptoms emerge from an underlying, common

disease—an alternative conception is that different causal biopsychosocial factors

interacting with one another (e.g., in a network) may promote and maintain different

patterns of presenting symptomatology (Hofmann, Curtiss, & McNally, 2016; Kendler,

Zachar, & Craver, 2011). Treatment selection models attempting to precisely allocate

patients on the basis of their personal characteristics to the treatment most likely to

benefit them may capitalize on the fact that different treatments activate different

mechanisms of change, which might be more or less likely to help a particular patient

(DeRubeis, Cohen, et al., 2014; Wallace, Frank, & Kraemer, 2013). If linked factors may

cause symptoms to correlate and “hang together” in a disorder-like manner (Kendler et

al., 2011), there is no particular reason why two individuals must share the same pathway

to manifesting an overt, diagnosable symptom. For example, a longitudinal network

analysis of symptoms and experiences among depressed and anxious patients examined

how a patient’s reports of these factors at one time point predicted higher or lower levels

Page 94: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

82

of the other measured variables at a subsequent timepoint (e.g., from feelings of anger at

T0 to depressed mood at T1) (Fisher, Reeves, Lawyer, Medaglia, & Rubel, 2017). While

some cross-patient commonalities could be extracted across networks of relationships

between the measured psychological variables across time, patients nevertheless

exhibited substantive heterogeneity in their networks. For patient A, sleep deprivation

could lead most to lowered mood, while for patient B, experiences of anger were most

related to later lowered mood. This suggests that patients with the same set of symptoms

could differ in the paths that led to their development.

This view has strong implications for mediation analyses that aim identify a

potential mechanism by which a treatment leads to the amelioration of symptoms of a

disorder. A mechanism can be improvement in a process that led to symptom

development (e.g., defense mechanisms; Perry & Bond, 2012) or a gain in a capacity that

counteracts a psychopathological process (e.g., cognitive therapy skill use; Strunk,

DeRubeis, Chiu, & Alvarez, 2007). If such changes are found to promote symptom relief

in some patients and not others, and insofar as this can be predicted on the basis of

measurable, individual differences, the term moderated mediation is applicable (Preacher,

Rucker, & Hayes, 2007).2 Differential responsiveness between patients to change in

2 Note that the term “moderated mediation” has been used to characterize instances in which the

symptomatic impact of a psychological change is dependent on the type of treatment a patient received

(e.g. (Zilcha-Mano et al., 2016)). The theoretical implications of this type of finding are arguably unclear.

If a particular psychological change X is helpful in treatment A but not treatment B, this would necessitate

that at least one of the following is also true: (a) there is another change encouraged by treatment A but not

B that, when combined with change X, allows for X to have a salutary effect; (b) there is another change

encouraged in treatment B but not A that attenuates the impact of change X; or (c) the treatment a patient

receives affects the meaning of scores (in one treatment or both) on the measure that assesses change X,

such that shifts in the scores that index X reflect different constructs in treatments A and B.

Page 95: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

83

psychological mechanisms may indicate more or less precise targeting of their particular

deficits or dysfunctions that maintain their symptoms.

Mechanisms of change in therapies for panic disorder

We examined the notion of moderated mediation in the context of a 2:2:1

randomized, two-site controlled trial (Milrod et al., 2016) of cognitive-behavioral therapy

(CBT; Craske, Barlow, & Meadows, 2000) and panic-focused psychodynamic

psychotherapy (PFPP; Busch, Milrod, Singer, & Aronson, 2012) for the treatment of

panic disorder with and without agoraphobia.3 We predicted that individual patient

differences would influence the symptomatic impact of change in two putative mediators

of panic improvement, catastrophic cognitions concerning bodily sensations (Clark,

1986), and panic-specific reflective functioning (PSRF) concerning the interpersonal-

emotional contexts and triggers of panic and anxiety (Rudden, Milrod, Target, Ackerman,

& Graf, 2006).

Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) and psychodynamic therapy (PDT) for the

treatment of panic are each associated with their own theory of the nature of panic attacks

and of the mechanisms that treatment will marshal to ameliorate panic disorder. There is

increasing evidence that changes in panic-related cognitions can precede and predict

improvements in panic symptoms (Lorenzo-Luaces, Keefe, & DeRubeis, 2016). In CBT

models of panic disorder, catastrophic cognitions concerning the ramifications of specific

3 The ART condition was not included in the present investigation due to the smaller group of patients

randomized to ART, in addition to significantly higher dropout that was related to being more

symptomatically and psychosocially impaired at baseline (only in ART), and having worse symptomatic

trajectories during treatment. These facts imply that the few treatment-completing ART patients were

particularly unrepresentative compared to CBT and PFPP patients.

Page 96: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

84

frightening body sensations, found to be elevated among patients with panic disorder

compared to other anxiety disorders (Clark et al., 1997), are considered causal to the

emergence of panic attacks (Clark, 1986). Panic patients are conceptualized as frequently

interpreting normal body sensations or day-to-day anxiety as indicating somatic problems

and distress, which initiates a vicious cycle of increases in arousal and a belief that a

catastrophe is imminent, leading to a panic attack. Cognitive restructuring of these

cognitions is thought to alleviate panic as it disrupts the cycle of interpretation and

arousal. (Teachman, Marker, & Clerkin, 2010), reported that improvements on the Brief

Body Sensations Interpretation Questionnaire (BBSIQ), a measure of catastrophic

interpretation, predicted subsequent improvements in core panic and agoraphobic

symptoms in CBT. Barber and colleagues (under review) replicated this finding, and

found that this pattern held also in panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy (PFPP;

Busch, Milrod, Singer, & Aronson, 2012).

By contrast, psychodynamic theories of panic emphasize how the inability to

recognize, experience, and tolerate affects and fantasies that lead to anxiety promotes

panic. From a psychodynamic perspective, these causes often derive from unconscious

conflict (e.g., repressed anger against a loved one you are fearful of losing if anger is

acknowledged). Panic patients report high alexithymia and experiential avoidance

(Galderisi et al., 2008; Izci et al., 2014; Parker, Taylor, Bagby, & Acklin, 1993; Tull &

Roemer, 2007) and, despite the fact that in the DSM, a panic attack is defined as

appearing to “come out of the blue,” emotional-interpersonal anxiety triggers can often

be identified (Busch, Shear, Cooper, Shapiro, & Leon, 1995; Chambless, 2010; Klass et

al., 2009; Scocco, Barbieri, & Frank, 2007). Moreover, panic patients exhibit

Page 97: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

85

emotionally-avoidant strategies in response to both positively- and negatively-valenced

stimuli (Tull & Roemer, 2007), and experimentally inducing use of these strategies

promotes distress and physiological arousal (Tull, Jakupcak, & Roemer, 2010). This lack

of awareness of triggers, whether facilitated by ignorance or avoidance, can produce the

upsetting sense that arousal and consequent panic comes "out of the blue” (Busch et al.,

1995). Panic-specific reflective functioning (PSRF), which refers to the tendency of an

individual to be aware of and to understand emotional meanings surrounding panic, is

typically impaired in patients with PD (Rudden, Milrod, Target, Ackerman, & Graf,

2006). Early improvements in PSRF were found to predict subsequent symptom change

in both PFPP and CBT, controlling for early symptom change (Barber et al., under

review). These results are consistent with research findings on gains in insight in the

context of psychodynamic psychotherapy in treating other disorders, which typically

demonstrate that patients who have improved insight over the course of psychotherapy

show further or more stable improvements across post-treatment follow-up (Gibbons et

al., 2009; Johansson et al., 2010; Kallestad et al., 2010; Ulberg, Amlo, Dahl, & Høglend,

2017).

Moderation Hypotheses

For both the BBSIQ and PSRF, we hypothesized that improvements in the BBSIQ

and PSRF were expected to be more predictive of subsequent symptom improvement

when a patient’s baseline values for that measure were in the more impaired range. We

based this on the conjecture that, for patients with these clinical impairments, these core

problems likely have a greater role in sustaining the symptom disorder. In other words,

we proposed that the presence of impaired values on these measures would indicate the

Page 98: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

86

presence of a problem that must be addressed and compensated for, rather than indicating

an area of rigid dysfunction that implies therapist should help bolster and mobilize other,

already stronger capacities (Cheavens, Strunk, Lazarus, & Goldstein, 2012). We therefore

tested baseline values of each change variable as moderators of the impact of change in

both BBSIQ and PSRF.

We also examined the role of co-morbid personality disorder (PersD) in the

change processes for these therapies. A little less than 50% of panic patients meet criteria

for a SCID-II diagnosable comorbid personality disorder (Friborg, Martinussen, Kaiser,

Øvergård, & Rosenvinge, 2013; Keefe, Milrod, Gallop, Barber, & Chambless, in press),

and anxiety patients with PersD experience worse trajectories of symptom improvements

over time generally (Ansell et al., 2011; Skodol, Geier, Grant, & Hasin, 2014) and in

CBT for panic specifically (Porter & Chambless, 2015). Studied mechanisms of change

in personality disorder are generally based on personality change (or, in the case of DBT,

acquisition of skills) rather than the change processes studied in this investigation (Keefe

& DeRubeis, 2018). Psychotherapy with PersD-comorbid patients may sometimes

require adaptations in technique (Keefe, Webb, & DeRubeis, 2016). Taken together, we

hypothesized that the personality factors giving rise to PersD may also help contribute to

panic experiences, and that thus the presence of PersD could attenuate and render less

primary the relationship between BBSIQ and PSRF improvements and symptomatic

response.

Method

Participants

Page 99: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

87

The present study is a secondary analysis of 138 of 201 patients randomized to

three psychotherapy treatments in a two-site randomized controlled trial comparing CBT,

applied relaxation training, and PFPP among patients with primary DSM-IV panic

disorder with or without agoraphobia. Patients were recruited at Weill Cornell Medical

College and the University of Pennsylvania. Participants received study treatment gratis.

Participants gave informed written consent. Both sites’ institutional review boards

approved the protocol, and the study is registered with ClinicalTrials.gov (identifier:

NCT00353470).

Patients were included in the trial if they had the spontaneous occurrence of one

or more panic attacks for the month before trial entry, and qualified for a primary DSM-

IV panic disorder with or without agoraphobia diagnosis determined as per the ADIS-IV

version (DiNardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1995). Cross-site agreement on ADIS ratings for

panic severity (with a “4” indicating the diagnostic threshold) was excellent (ICC = 1.00).

See Milrod et al., 2016 for further details.

Additional ongoing psychotherapy was prohibited. Medications were permitted if

stable for at least two months at presentation, and were recorded, held constant, and

monitored during the trial. Exclusion criteria were active substance dependence (of less

than 6 month’s remission), a history of psychosis or bipolar disorder, acute suicidality, or

organic mental syndrome. Additional details on trial design, diagnostician training, and

therapy adherence can be consulted in the primary outcome paper (Milrod et al., 2016).

Therapies

PFPP is based on the central assumption that panic symptoms have a partly

unconscious psychological meaning. It explores feelings and subjective content of panic

Page 100: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

88

episodes, so the patient can begin to address these meanings rather than experiencing

conflicts physically as somatic anxiety leading to panic (Busch et al., 2012; Milrod et al.,

1997). The therapy helps patients understand and alter core conflicts (e.g., regarding

attachment and dependency) to avert future panic vulnerability. CBT for PD followed a

modified version of the Panic Control Therapy protocol (Craske et al., 2000), entailing

education about panic, correction of maladaptive thoughts about anxiety and body

sensations, and both in-session and homework interoceptive exposures to bodily

sensations designed to mimic those experienced during panic (Craske et al., 2000). Both

psychotherapies comprised 24 sessions delivered twice weekly (12 weeks). Additional

information on treatments, including training, supervision, and adherence monitoring, can

be found in Milrod et al. (2016).

Outcome Index

Panic Disorder Severity Scale (PDSS; (Shear et al., 1997)). The PDSS is a

diagnosis-based, composite, global rating of panic disorder severity, with acceptable

psychometric properties. The PDSS was administered by trained, master’s-level

diagnosticians who were uninformed as to treatment condition. Interrater reliability on

the PDSS was excellent (ICC[2,1] = 0.95). The PDSS was administered five times during

treatment: at baseline (Week 0), Week 1, Week 5, Week 9, and termination (Week 12).

The mediational analyses in this manuscript concern PDSS change between Week 5 to

termination (Week 12), controlling for early PDSS change from Week 0 to Week 5.

Psychological Change and Moderator Measures

Brief Bodily Sensations Interpretation Questionnaire (BBSIQ; Clark et al.,

1997). The BBSIQ is a 7-item measure of catastrophic misinterpretation of panic-related

Page 101: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

89

bodily sensations. Respondents read seven scenarios about bodily sensations, and rank

three provided interpretations in the order by which they would most likely come to the

respondent’s mind in that scenario. A score for each item is determined as a function of

the reported order and the level of catastrophizing of the interpretation, and the item

scores are summed. The BBSIQ exhibits psychometric consistency conforming to a one-

factor solution, in addition to strong construct validity and test-retest reliability over three

months. It showed adequate internal consistency in this trial (Cronbach’s α = 0.86).

Panic-Specific Reflective Functioning (PSRF; Rudden et al., 2006). PSRF is

an interview-based measure that assesses the degree to which a patient can recognize the

psychological contributions to panic symptoms. Respondents are queried as to their

theory of panic, how it has changed over time, and whether they notice any concordance

between panic and particular emotional or interpersonal states. Each response is rated for

its psychological mindedness and complexity, and an overall score is assigned based on

the item scores. An example of a more impaired PSRF answer might be saying “It’s the

heat, the heat brings them on” in response to “reasons for your panic attacks.” A less

impaired response to that same question might be “I notice that I get them when I am

feeling a lack of control in my personal relationships. I fear that others will leave me, or

that I may want to leave them.” PSRF narratives were reliably scored by three trained

raters at the Weill-Cornell Medical College (ICC = 0.80).

Structured Clinical Interview for Axis-II Disorders (SCID-II; First, Gibbon,

Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997). The Structured Clinical Interview for Axis-II

disorders (First et al., 1997) was used to assess the presence of PersD criteria and

Page 102: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

90

diagnoses as defined by DSM-IV. SCID-II criteria counts rather than diagnostic status

were employed as a moderator variable for reasons of statistical power and validity

(MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, 2002), following research suggesting that

DSM-IV PersD reflects a continuum of illness rather than categorical taxonomies

(Harford, Chen, & Grant, 2014; Harford et al., 2013; Haslam, Holland, & Kuppens,

2012).

Trained, independent masters’ level diagnosticians, uninformed to treatment

condition administered the interviews. Cross-site inter-rater reliability for number of total

PersD criteria scored as present was excellent (ICC[2,1] = 0.92).

Statistical Analyses

Missing data. Due to the nonrandom nature of dropout in this trial, wherein

patients with worse symptom improvement trajectories tended to drop out significantly

more, and because early psychological changes on the BBSIQ and PSRF were the crucial

predictors in this study, we could not use early dropout patients in our investigation (n =

24 in CBT/PFPP; 14.8% dropout rate), resulting in a final sample size of n = 138 across

the two arms. Attempts to multiply impute their outcome and predictor values would

likely be statistically biased due to the nonrandom dropout (White, Royston, & Wood,

2011). As a result, 138 CBT and PFPP patients who performed assessments through at

least Week 5 (Assessment 3) were the focus of this study.

Modeling. Statistical analyses were conducted in SAS using the PROC MIXED

command. Repeated measures mixed models were employed on the three PDSS

evaluations between Week 5 and Termination (Week 12). Random slopes of PDSS

symptom change across the time unit of week of treatment during the evaluation period

Page 103: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

91

were nested in random person-specific intercepts, with an unstructured covariance

structure. Covariance structures were allowed to vary between the two sites of the trial. In

every model, the baseline PDSS score and early PDSS change (between baseline and

Week 5) were entered as covariates.

Within a mixed model, the primary analyses predicted slopes of PDSS change

between Week 5 and termination as a function of early change in psychological change

mechanisms (i.e., on the BBSIQ or PSRF) between baseline and Week 5. Time was

modeled as linear, following the pattern of change detected in the parent trial. Moderators

were tested as an interaction between the moderator value, early psychological change,

and time (p <.05 threshold). Significance of this interaction indicates that the degree to

which early psychological change predicts subsequent symptom change is dependent on

baseline levels of the moderator variable. Covariates included baseline PDSS score and

early PDSS change occurring between baseline and Week 5 (when the mediator was

measured the second time). Separate models were run for each of the combinations of

two psychological mechanism variables (BBSIQ, PSRF) and three baseline moderator

variables (BBSIQ, PSRF, total SCID-II criteria).

We separately tested whether any of the above three potential moderators found to

be statistically significant further interacted with site or treatment at p <.10. However, no

such interactions emerged, indicating that moderation effects were not reliably different

across sites or treatments.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Page 104: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

92

Information on patient demographics and clinical characteristics can be found in

Table 1. Patients with better PSRF were somewhat less, but not significantly, likely to

have a catastrophic, body-focused interpretation style as measured by the BBSIQ (r[134]

= -0.16, p = 0.064). Early improvements on the PSRF (Baseline to Week 5) did not

correlate with early improvements on the BBSIQ (r[70] = 0.19, p = 0.111). The

relationship between baseline values in mediator and moderator measures at baseline and

early mediator change is reported in Table 2.

Moderators of the impact of PSRF improvements

As reported in the Barber et al. (in press) paper on mediation in the trial, patients

who experienced more PSRF improvements during the first five weeks of treatment

exhibited greater panic symptom improvements over the subsequent seven weeks (B = -

0.47 [95% CI: -0.82, -0.13], t[166] = -2.69, p = 0.008).

Two of the three tested variables moderated the impact of early PSRF

improvements on PDSS change. Our primary hypothesis concerning baseline PSRF

severity was not supported, as baseline PSRF was not related to the degree to which

improvements in PSRF predicted subsequent symptom change (B = 0.10 [95 CI: -0.20,

0.39], t[159] = 0.64, p = 0.520). Contrary to our expectation, however, patients who

engaged in more severe catastrophic interpretations at baseline on the BBSIQ

experienced more subsequent symptomatic benefits from early improvements in PSRF (B

= -1.05 [95% CI: -2.06, -0.03], t[151] = -2.04, p = 0.043) (see Figure 1).

Nevertheless, as hypothesized, personality pathology was a significant moderator,

as patients with more personality pathology as measured by the SCID-II were less likely

to experience symptom improvement after gains in PSRF (B = 0.06 [0.00, 0.11], t[158] =

Page 105: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

93

2.00, p = 0.047), whereas patients with less personality pathology burden experienced

relatively more improvement after gains in PSRF. The critical value of this interaction, at

which point early PSRF change was no longer significantly related to subsequent PDSS

improvement, was 12 SCID-II criteria, as identified with the Johnson-Neyman technique

(Johnson & Fay, 1950; see Figure 2).

Moderators of the impact of BBSIQ improvements

Again, as was found in the Barber et al. (in press) paper on overall mediation in

this trial, early improvements in the BBSIQ (between baseline and Week 5) predicted

greater subsequent symptom change on the PDSS (B = 0.13 [0.0, 0.25], t[142] = 2.05, p =

0.042), controlling for baseline PDSS symptom severity and early symptom

improvement.

In a test of our moderation hypothesis, we found that patients with higher BBSIQ

scores at baseline experienced more symptom relief following early improvements on the

BBSIQ (B = 0.27 [95% CI: 0.05, 0.49, t[140] = 2.43, p = 0.016). Similar to the pattern

shown in Figure 1 for PSRF improvements, patients with relatively lower baseline

BBSIQ exhibited no extra symptomatic improvement subsequent to early decreases on

the BBSIQ, while those with increasingly higher BBSIQ scores at baseline were

predicted to benefit more and more from BBSIQ improvements. As expected, baseline

levels of PSRF did not moderate the relationship between BBSIQ change and symptom

improvement (B = -0.06 [95% CI: -0.16, 0.03], t[131]=-1.28, p = 0.203). The test of the

hypothesis that a patient’s level of comorbid personality pathology would affect

negatively the impact of improvements in catastrophic cognitions on symptomatic benefit

was not supported (B = 0.01 [95% CI: -0.01, 0.03], t[134] = 1.17, p = 0.244).

Page 106: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

94

Discussion

A panic patient’s degree of symptomatic improvement after experiencing a

particular psychological change in psychotherapy differs significantly as a function of

their psychological characteristics. For certain patients, focus on helping stimulate

particular psychological changes might improve the efficacy of their treatment. From a

theoretical perspective, these results align with models of psychopathology that posit that

different psychological variables may contribute to what appear to be identical symptoms

for different patients (Fisher et al., 2017). As such, ameliorating the same putatively

pathological process or helping a patient gain an apparently adaptive capacity may be

more helpful for some patients than those same changes will be for others. Moreover, our

findings align with the viewpoint that patients’ characteristics may meaningfully

contribute to which kind of treatment they should receive to optimize their chances of

success (Cohen & DeRubeis, 2018).

Possessing a more catastrophic style of interpreting bodily sensations indicated

that patients experienced greater subsequent symptom change from early improvements

in both catastrophic interpretations as measured by BBSIQ, or insight into the possible

psychological meanings surrounding panic as assessed by the PSRF. For these patients,

the cognitive model of panic may be especially applicable (i.e., that such interpretations

spark and fuel the cycle into panic attacks; Clark, 1986), such that restructuring those

interpretations more reliably helps patients thwart the panic cycle. By contrast, patients

without a strong tendency to catastrophize may not benefit as much from attempts to

reduce this tendency further.

Page 107: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

95

We did not expect to detect the apparent moderating influence of catastrophic

cognitive style on the impact of gains in PSRF. In PFPP, tendency to somatize is

understood as a defense mechanism. From a psychodynamic perspective, focus on body

sensations can arise due to a discomfort with the underlying emotions and fantasies

contributing to physiological arousal—somatization avoids direct acknowledgement of

these unacceptable, sometimes partially unconscious contents (Busch et al., 2012; Busch

et al., 1995). Shifting from understanding anxiety and arousal as implying somatic danger

to deriving from central psychological meanings may help patients to recognize and work

with those meanings rather than to ascribe arousal to frightening somatic causes,

especially given a heightened sense of catastrophe.

By contrast, patients with lower versus higher insight about possible

psychological meanings of their panic (i.e., PSRF) evidenced equal apparent benefit from

either type of psychological change. Ergo, patients beginning psychotherapy with little or

no insight as to possible emotional or interpersonal links to their panic appear to

nonetheless benefit from work fostering early improvements in PSRF.

Taken together, these findings imply that patients with a highly catastrophic,

body-focused interpretation style may need either to ameliorate this cognitive style or

gain insight into their emotional-interpersonal triggers in order to experience greater

relief from panic disorder. Both types of changes may commonly reflect patients

recognizing that bodily sensations do not indicate danger and catastrophe, but rather that

they are harmless (BBSIQ improvement) or may signify other psychological meanings

(PSRF improvement). This may also reflect that, while BBSIQ indexes severity of a

cognitive style that specifically distinguishes and potentially causes panic disorder (Clark

Page 108: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

96

et al., 1997), low levels of PSRF may reflect a lack of understanding of symptom links

that can be gained to help with panic, rather than a deficit particular to panic patients that

must be improved. It is possible that individual differences concerning the severity of

more general emotional awareness deficits (e.g., alexithymia; experiential avoidance)

documented in panic patients may be a better moderator for panic mediational models.

Unfortunately, it was not possible to meaningfully investigate the case of simultaneous

improvement in both constructs, as the type of changes a patient experienced was

dependent on the therapy they received (CBT vs. PFPP), and simultaneous improvements

were thus uncommon.

Patients with more personality pathology burden benefitted less from

improvements in PSRF, while the converse was true for patients with few such traits.

Indeed, patients without any personality pathology whatsoever exhibited the greatest

benefit from PSRF improvements. Nevertheless, in this subsample, there was no

indication that patients meeting more PersD criteria had significantly worse symptomatic

outcomes in PFPP as compared to CBT. Furthermore, in this trial PFPP was superior to

CBT in improving comorbid severe PersD symptoms on the SCID-II (Keefe et al., 2018).

It may be that PFPP’s capacity to treat severely PersD-comorbid patients is due to other

psychological changes targeted by psychodynamic therapies, such as improvements in

defensive functioning or attachment security (Johansson et al., 2010; Keefe & DeRubeis,

2018; Perry & Bond, 2012; Rossouw & Fonagy, 2012). On the other hand, BBSIQ

improvements were predictive of symptom change regardless of patients’ personality

pathology, which is consistent with an account that PersD is not a clear contraindication

to cognitive restructuring for panic symptom relief.

Page 109: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

97

This investigation was not designed to elucidate specific techniques or therapeutic

foci within the two treatments that best promote the psychological changes we examined.

Most psychological treatments consist of multiple interventions targeting multiple types

of changes. CBT for panic disorder includes teaching relaxation (a coping skill),

cognitive restructuring concerning catastrophic thoughts about the body, and both

interoceptive exposure to feared bodily sensations and in vivo exposures to phobic, panic-

engendering spaces (facilitating extinction and potentially fueling cognitive change).

Panic-focused psychodynamic psychotherapy also entails several foci, including the

exploration of dysregulated attachment, the interpretation of transference and of defenses,

and efforts to enhance the patient’s awareness and tolerance of emotions and fantasy life.

Different psychological interventions within a given therapy may best encourage the

types of psychological changes most likely to help a particular patient (Keefe et al., 2016;

Sasso, Strunk, Braun, DeRubeis, & Brotman, 2015). These kinds of questions require

further investigation.

Limitations and future directions

Due to the nonrandom nature of dropout in this trial such that patients with worse

trajectories of symptom change were more likely to drop out, only patients with observed

data up to at least Week 5 were analyzed in this investigation (14.8% of CBT and PFPP

patients randomized to treatment).

For this analysis, we had the option to explore either a wide range of potential

moderators of proposed change mechanisms, or to limit our analyses to a select few on

the basis of theoretical predictions. In the interest of limiting Type I error, we elected to

test a smaller set of moderators. It is possible that other individual traits exist that would

Page 110: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

98

help envisage how each type of psychological change might predict subsequent

symptomatic improvements. In addition, only two psychological changes relevant to

panic disorder were examined in the primary mediator analyses for this trial (catastrophic

interpretations and PSRF), whereas other putative mediators may exist, such as self-

efficacy (Fentz et al., 2013) and improvements in attachment security (Milrod, 2015),

neither of which were assessed in this trial. In addition, as this is the first investigation of

its kind in panic treatment, these relationships should be explored in other treatment trials

with relevant data.

Despite our goals to identify which psychological changes might be most

important for individual patients, our study takes a nomothetic, between-patients

approach to examining “what mechanism works for whom.” An alternative, personalized

approach would be to identify these patterns for individual patients, and to examine the

differential effects of attempting to intervene upon the core problems that sustain an

individual’s panic (Fisher & Boswell, 2016). For example, if a patient’s tendency to

catastrophize bodily sensations was observed to reliably predict their experiences of

panic and anxiety across time, cognitive interventions targeting that tendency may be

found to be more efficacious in improving panic than other cognitive-behavioral

interventions (e.g., relaxation training).

References

Ansell, E. B., Pinto, A., Edelen, M. O., Markowitz, J. C., Sanislow, C. A., Yen, S., . . .

Grilo, C. M. (2011). The association of personality disorders with the prospective

7-year course of anxiety disorders. Psychol Med, 41(5), 1019-1028. doi:

10.1017/s0033291710001777

Page 111: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

99

Busch, F. N., Milrod, B. L., Singer, M. B., & Aronson, A. C. (2012). Manual of panic

focused psychodynamic psychotherapy - extended range (Vol. 36). New York,

NY: Routledge.

Busch, F. N., Shear, M. K., Cooper, A. M., Shapiro, T., & Leon, A. C. (1995). An

empirical study of defense mechanisms in panic disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis,

183(5), 299-303.

Chambless, D. L. (2010). Interpersonal aspects of panic disorder and agoraphobia. In J.

G. Beck (Ed.), Interpersonal processes in the anxiety disorders: Implications for

understanding psychopathology and treatment (pp. 209-233). Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

Cheavens, J. S., Strunk, D. R., Lazarus, S. A., & Goldstein, L. A. (2012). The

compensation and capitalization models: a test of two approaches to

individualizing the treatment of depression. Behav Res Ther, 50(11), 699-706.

doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.08.002

Clark, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

24(4), 461-470. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(86)90011-2

Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Ost, L. G., Breitholtz, E., Koehler, K. A., Westling, B.

E., . . . Gelder, M. (1997). Misinterpretation of body sensations in panic disorder.

J Consult Clin Psychol, 65(2), 203-213.

Cohen, Z. D., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2018). Treatment Selection in Depression. Annual

Review of Clinical Psychology, 14(1), 209-236. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-

050817-084746

Page 112: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

100

Craske, M. G., Barlow, D. H., & Meadows, E. (2000). Master Your Own Anxiety and

Panic: Therapist Guide for Anxiety, Panic, and Agoraphobia (MAP-3). San

Antonio, TX: Graywind/Psychological Corporation.

DeRubeis, R. J., Cohen, Z. D., Forand, N. R., Fournier, J. C., Gelfand, L. A., & Lorenzo-

Luaces, L. (2014). The Personalized Advantage Index: Translating Research on

Prediction into Individualized Treatment Recommendations. A Demonstration.

PLOS ONE, 9(1), e83875. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083875

DeRubeis, R. J., Gelfand, L. A., German, R. E., Fournier, J. C., & Forand, N. R. (2014).

Understanding processes of change: How some patients reveal more than others –

and some groups of therapists less – about what matters in psychotherapy.

Psychotherapy research : journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research,

24(3), 419-428. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2013.838654

DiNardo, P. A., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Anxiety Disorders Interview

Schedule for DSM-IV: Current Version (ADIS-IV). New York, NY: Graywinds

Publications.

Fentz, H. N., Hoffart, A., Jensen, M. B., Arendt, M., O'Toole, M. S., Rosenberg, N. K., &

Hougaard, E. (2013). Mechanisms of change in cognitive behaviour therapy for

panic disorder: The role of panic self-efficacy and catastrophic misinterpretations.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(9), 579-587. doi:

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.06.002

Page 113: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

101

First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997).

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders, (SCID-

II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Fisher, A. J., & Boswell, J. F. (2016). Enhancing the Personalization of Psychotherapy

With Dynamic Assessment and Modeling. Assessment, 23(4), 496-506. doi:

doi:10.1177/1073191116638735

Fisher, A. J., Reeves, J. W., Lawyer, G., Medaglia, J. D., & Rubel, J. A. (2017).

Exploring the idiographic dynamics of mood and anxiety via network analysis. J

Abnorm Psychol, 126(8), 1044-1056. doi: 10.1037/abn0000311

Friborg, O., Martinussen, M., Kaiser, S., Øvergård, K. T., & Rosenvinge, J. H. (2013).

Comorbidity of personality disorders in anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of 30

years of research. Journal of Affective Disorders, 145(2), 143-155. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.004

Galderisi, S., Mancuso, F., Mucci, A., Garramone, S., Zamboli, R., & Maj, M. (2008).

Alexithymia and cognitive dysfunctions in patients with panic disorder.

Psychother Psychosom, 77(3), 182-188. doi: 10.1159/000119738

Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Barber, J. P., Wiltsey Stirman, S., Gallop, R.,

Goldstein, L. A., . . . Ring-Kurtz, S. (2009). Unique and common mechanisms of

change across cognitive and dynamic psychotherapies. J Consult Clin Psychol,

77(5), 801-813. doi: 10.1037/a0016596

Hofmann, S. G., Curtiss, J., & McNally, R. J. (2016). A Complex Network Perspective

on Clinical Science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(5), 597-605. doi:

10.1177/1745691616639283

Page 114: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

102

Izci, F., Gultekin, B. K., Saglam, S., Koc, M. I., Zincir, S. B., & Atmaca, M. (2014).

Temperament, character traits, and alexithymia in patients with panic disorder.

Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat, 10, 879-885. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s62647

Johansson, P., Hoglend, P., Ulberg, R., Amlo, S., Marble, A., Bogwald, K. P., . . .

Heyerdahl, O. (2010). The mediating role of insight for long-term improvements

in psychodynamic therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol, 78(3), 438-448. doi:

10.1037/a0019245

Johnson, P. O., & Fay, L. C. (1950). The Johnson-Neyman technique, its theory and

application. Psychometrika, 15(4), 349-367. doi: 10.1007/bf02288864

Kallestad, H., Valen, J., McCullough, L., Svartberg, M., Høglend, P., & Stiles, T. C.

(2010). The relationship between insight gained during therapy and long-term

outcome in short-term dynamic psychotherapy and cognitive therapy for cluster C

personality disorders. Psychotherapy Research, 20(5), 526-534. doi:

10.1080/10503307.2010.492807

Keefe, J. R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2018). Changing character: A narrative review of

personality change in psychotherapies for personality disorder. Psychotherapy

Research, 1-18. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1425930

Keefe, J. R., Milrod, B. L., Gallop, R., Barber, J. P., & Chambless, D. L. (2018). What is

the effect on comorbid personality disorder of brief panic-focused psychotherapy

in patients with panic disorder? Depression and Anxiety, 35(3), 239-247. doi:

10.1002/da.22708

Page 115: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

103

Keefe, J. R., Webb, C. A., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2016). In cognitive therapy for depression,

early focus on maladaptive beliefs may be especially efficacious for patients with

personality disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol, 84(4), 353-364. doi:

10.1037/ccp0000071

Kendler, K. S., Zachar, P., & Craver, C. (2011). What kinds of things are psychiatric

disorders? Psychol Med, 41(6), 1143-1150. doi: 10.1017/s0033291710001844

Klass, E. T., Milrod, B. L., Leon, A. C., Kay, S. J., Schwalberg, M., Li, C., & Markowitz,

J. C. (2009). Does Interpersonal Loss Preceding Panic Disorder Onset Moderate

Response to Psychotherapy? An Exploratory Study. The Journal of clinical

psychiatry, 70(3), 406-411.

Lorenzo-Luaces, L., Keefe, J. R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2016). Cognitive-Behavioral

Therapy: Nature and Relation to Non-Cognitive Behavioral Therapy. Behavior

Therapy, 47(6), 785-803. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.012

Milrod, B. (2015). An Epidemiological Contribution to Clinical Understanding of

Anxiety. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(7), 601-602. doi:

10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15030312

Milrod, B., Chambless, D. L., Gallop, R., Busch, F. N., Schwalberg, M., McCarthy, K.

S., . . . Barber, J. P. (2016). Psychotherapies for Panic Disorder: A Tale of Two

Sites. J Clin Psychiatry, 77(7), 927-935. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09507

Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Acklin, M. W. (1993). Alexithymia in panic

disorder and simple phobia: a comparative study. American Journal of Psychiatry,

150(7), 1105-1107. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.7.1105

Page 116: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

104

Perry, J. C., & Bond, M. (2012). Change in Defense Mechanisms During Long-Term

Dynamic Psychotherapy and Five-Year Outcome. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 169(9), 916-925. doi: doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091403

Porter, E., & Chambless, D. L. (2015). A systematic review of predictors and moderators

of improvement in cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disorder and

agoraphobia. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 179-192. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.09.004

Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing Moderated Mediation

Hypotheses: Theory, Methods, and Prescriptions. Multivariate Behav Res, 42(1),

185-227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316

Rossouw, T. I., & Fonagy, P. (2012). Mentalization-Based Treatment for Self-Harm in

Adolescents: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of the American Academy

of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(12), 1304-1313.e1303. doi:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.09.018

Rudden, M., Milrod, B., Target, M., Ackerman, S., & Graf, E. (2006). Reflective

functioning in panic disorder patients: a pilot study. J Am Psychoanal Assoc,

54(4), 1339-1343.

Sasso, K. E., Strunk, D. R., Braun, J. D., DeRubeis, R. J., & Brotman, M. A. (2015).

Identifying moderators of the adherence-outcome relation in cognitive therapy for

depression. J Consult Clin Psychol, 83(5), 976-984. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000045

Scocco, P., Barbieri, I., & Frank, E. (2007). Interpersonal Problem Areas and Onset of

Panic Disorder. Psychopathology, 40(1), 8-13.

Page 117: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

105

Shear, M. K., Brown, T. A., Barlow, D. H., Money, R., Sholomskas, D. E., Woods, S.

W., . . . Papp, L. A. (1997). Multicenter collaborative panic disorder severity

scale. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(11), 1571-1575. doi:

10.1176/ajp.154.11.1571

Skodol, A. E., Geier, T., Grant, B. F., & Hasin, D. S. (2014). Personality disorders and

the persistence of anxiety disorders in a nationally representative sample. Depress

Anxiety, 31(9), 721-728. doi: 10.1002/da.22287

Strunk, D. R., DeRubeis, R. J., Chiu, A. W., & Alvarez, J. (2007). Patients' competence

in and performance of cognitive therapy skills: relation to the reduction of relapse

risk following treatment for depression. J Consult Clin Psychol, 75(4), 523-530.

doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.75.4.523

Teachman, B. A., Marker, C. D., & Clerkin, E. M. (2010). Catastrophic

misinterpretations as a predictor of symptom change during treatment for panic

disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol, 78(6), 964-973. doi: 10.1037/a0021067

Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2007). Emotion regulation difficulties associated with the

experience of uncued panic attacks: evidence of experiential avoidance, emotional

nonacceptance, and decreased emotional clarity. Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 378-

391. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.006

Ulberg, R., Amlo, S., Dahl, H.-S. J., & Høglend, P. (2017). Does Insight Mediate

Treatment and Enhance Outcome? Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 37(3), 140-152. doi:

10.1080/07351690.2017.1285184

Page 118: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

106

Wallace, M. L., Frank, E., & Kraemer, H. C. (2013). A novel approach for developing

and interpreting treatment moderator profiles in randomized clinical trials. JAMA

Psychiatry, 70(11), 1241-1247. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1960

White, I. R., Royston, P., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Multiple imputation using chained

equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine, 30(4), 377-

399. doi: 10.1002/sim.4067

Zilcha-Mano, S., Chui, H., Dolev, T., McCarthy, K. S., Dinger, U., & Barber, J. P.

(2016). Changes in causal attributions and relationship representations: Are they

specific or common mechanisms in the treatment of depression? J Affect Disord,

193(Supplement C), 73-80. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.073

Page 119: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

107

Table 3.1

Descriptive data for baseline characteristics and for symptom change

Baseline Measure Mean (SD) or # (%)

Baseline PDSS 13.8 (3.6)

Week 5 PDSS 9.6 (4.2)

Termination PDSS 6.5 (4.3)

Week 5 to Termination PDSS Change -3.4 (3.5)

Baseline PSRF 3.4 (1.4)

Baseline to Week 5 PSRF Change (CBT / PFPP) 0.0 (1.0) / 0.5 (1.3)

Baseline BBSIQ 1.9 (0.4)

Baseline to Week 5 BBSIQ Change (CBT / PFPP) -0.2 (0.4) / 0.0 (0.3)

SCID-II Criteria Count 7.5 (6.4)

Agoraphobia Diagnosis (% Yes) 112 (81.2%)

Age 40.00 (13.5)

Gender (Female) 86 (58.1%)

Ethnicity (Hispanic) 23 (16.7%)

Race (Caucasian) 101 (73.2%)

Concurrent Psychopharmacology 42 (30.4%)

Note. BBSIQ = Brief Bodily Sensation Interpretation Questionnaire; PDSS = Panic

Disorder Severity Scale; PSRF = Panic-Specific Reflective Functioning; SCID-II =

Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnosis of Axis-II Disorders

Page 120: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

108

Table 3.2

Correlations of baseline moderator values to improvement in BBSIQ and PSRF from

Baseline (Week 0) to Week 5 of treatment.

Baseline BBSIQ Baseline PSRF Baseline SCID-II

BBSIQ

Improvement

0.49*** -0.23* -0.02

PSRF Improvement 0.06 -0.57*** -0.04

Notes. Higher scores on the BBSIQ indicate a more pronounced tendency to

catastrophically interpret body sensations. Higher PSRF scores indicate greater symptom

insight. * = p<.05; *** = p<.001

Page 121: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

109

Figure 3.1. Predicted degree of Week 5 to Termination (Week 12) symptom change

attributable to early (Baseline to Week 5) improvements in PSRF, as a function of

baseline severity of catastrophic interpretive style as measured by the BBSIQ (interaction

p = 0.043).

Page 122: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

110

Figure 3.2. Predicted degree of Week 5 to Termination (Week 12) symptom change

attributable to early (Baseline to Week 5) improvements in PSRF, as a function of

baseline level of personality disorder pathology as measured by number of criteria met on

the SCID-II interview (interaction p = 0.047).

Page 123: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

111

APPENDICES

Page 124: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

112

Appendix A

Additional notes on statistical analyses (Study 1)

Random forest imputation. Random forest imputation is a single-dataset

imputation method that can produce nominally equivalent or more accurate imputations

compared to standard multiple imputation by chained equations, and can natively handle

simultaneous continuous/categorical variable imputation and datasets with interactions

and nonlinear relationships between variables (Liao et al., 2014; Stekhoven & Bühlmann,

2011; Waljee et al., 2013). Random forest algorithms produce nested prediction or

decision trees based on splits in continuous or categorical predictor variables (King &

Resick, 2014). Each individual tree selects a bootstrapped subset of the main data set, and

at each potential split point allows a random subset of possible predictors to be used, until

there are no more possible splits to be made. This process is repeated across many

different forests of trees, wherein predictions for the missing variables are iteratively

updated and improved with successive models, until a stopping criterion is reached. A

given imputed data value is based on the average prediction made across several decision

trees built from multiple subsets of the data and potential predicting variables.

All baseline data, technique ratings from a rated session not used in this

manuscript (Session 5), and termination and pre-to-post treatment change scores on the

PDSS, Sheehan Disability Scale, Inventory of Interpersonal Problems, and Hamilton

Rating Scale for Depression were allowed to predict missing data in the set, in addition to

all rated process measures and their constituent items.

Additional references

Page 125: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

113

King, M. W., & Resick, P. A. (2014). Data mining in psychological treatment research: A

primer on classification and regression trees. J Consult Clin Psychol, 82(5), 895-

905. doi: 10.1037/a0035886

Liao, S. G., Lin, Y., Kang, D. D., Chandra, D., Bon, J., Kaminski, N., . . . Tseng, G. C.

(2014). Missing value imputation in high-dimensional phenomic data: imputable

or not, and how? BMC Bioinformatics, 15(1), 346. doi: 10.1186/s12859-014-

0346-6

Waljee, A. K., Mukherjee, A., Singal, A. G., Zhang, Y., Warren, J., Balis, U., . . .

Higgins, P. D. (2013). Comparison of imputation methods for missing laboratory

data in medicine. BMJ Open, 3(8). doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002847

Page 126: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

114

Appendix B

Additional checks on robustness of obtained findings (Study 1)

Controlling for therapist effects

The packages “lme4” and “lmerTest” in the R statistical language were used

(Bates et al., 2016; Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2016). Therapists were

specified as a 2nd-level random effect in which patients were nested. When therapist

effects were examined without fixed effects, there was no statistically significant random

effect of therapist on PDSS symptom change (ICCT Week 1 to 5 = 0.05, p = 0.600; ICCT

Week 5 to 9 = 0.00, p = 1.000; ICCT Total Pre-Post Change = 0.08, p = 0.300). The lack

of estimable effect or statistical significance is likely due to the low sample size and poor

patient/therapist ratio leading to issues in estimation. On the other hand, the estimate of

reliable therapist-level variance for total pre-post symptom change (8%) was comparable

to the range of 6-9% often reported in the therapist effects literature (Lutz & Barkham,

2015).

For PDSS symptom change between Weeks 1 and 5 and Weeks 5 to 9, we

conducted an exploratory analysis to examine whether our technique-outcome findings

differed notably when simultaneously modeling therapist effects, using the same model

specifications as the primary analyses (e.g., outcome indices, covariates, technique terms)

but as applied to a mixed model framework. Unremarkably, there were no p-value shifts

across the significance level of p <.05, and effect size estimates were comparable.

Functioning and interpersonal outcomes

In our primary analyses, we found that panic-focused interpretations at Session 10

predicted panic symptom improvement on the PDSS occurring subsequent to Session 10.

Page 127: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

115

One possibility is that panic-focused interpretations are conducive to symptomatic

improvements, but that they do not help other aspects of functioning. As a secondary,

post-hoc analysis, we explored whether these findings extended to improvements on the

Sheehan Disability Scale (Leon, Shear, Portera, & Klerman, 1992), a measure of

psychosocial dysfunction due to mental illness, and the Inventory of Interpersonal

Problems. Unlike the PDSS, which was assessed several times during the trial, the SDS

and IIP were administered only pre- and post-therapy.

Our overall modeling strategy was very similar, with the following exceptions: (1)

the predicted variable was the final SDS or IIP score; and (2) baseline levels of the SDS

or IIP were included as a covariate in lieu of baseline PDSS. We retained the degree of

PDSS symptom change between baseline and Week 5 as a covariate, such that early

symptomatic progress occurring during the process measurement period could not

contribute to the apparent relationships (or lack thereof) between process variables and

non-symptomatic outcomes measured at treatment termination.

Panic-focused interpretations at Session 10 predicted significantly lower IIP (B =

-0.27 [95% CI: -0.45 to -0.09], SE = 0.09, t[58] = -2.99, p = 0.004) scores at treatment

termination, and, at the level of a statistical trend, lower SDS scores (B = -2.42 [95% CI:

-5.21 to 0.38], SE = 1.40, t[59] = -1.73, p = 0.089). By contrast, panic-focused

interpretations at Session 2 did not relate to improvements on either the IIP (B = -0.20

[95% CI: -0.47 to 0.08], SE = 0.14, t[58] = -1.44, p = 0.156) or the SDS (B = -2.64 [95%

CI: -6.29 to 1.01], SE = 1.82, t[59] = -1.45, p = 0.152).

These findings are consistent with a perspective that panic-focused interpretations

at mid-therapy may also encourage improvements in interpersonal functioning, in

Page 128: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

116

addition to panic symptom response. However, it should be noted that these analyses are

less analytically rigorous than our primary analyses, as temporal precedence between

technique use and symptom change cannot be assured because the SDS and IIP were

measured pre- to post-treatment.

Additional References

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., Singmann, H., . . .

Green, P. (2016). lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4

(Version 1.1-12). Retrieved from https://cran.r-

project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2016). lmerTest: Tests in

Linear Mixed Effects Models (Version 2.0-30). Retrieved from https://cran.r-

project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html

Leon, A. C., Shear, M. K., Portera, L., & Klerman, G. L. (1992). Assessing impairment in

patients with panic disorder: the Sheehan Disability Scale. Soc Psychiatry

Psychiatr Epidemiol, 27(2), 78-82.

Lutz, W., & Barkham, M. (2015). Therapist Effects The Encyclopedia of Clinical

Psychology: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Page 129: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

117

Appendix C

Supplementary tables (Study 1)

Table S1

Examples of Panic-focused Clarification and Interpretation

Technique Example

Panic-focused

Clarification

Patient: It’s just that I know I might be having a heart attack

whenever I get that anxious. Everything in my chest is so tight, I’m

so wound up, I’m so clenched, I just feel like my heart is going to

give out and die.

Therapist: You think this even though you’ve been to specialists

several times, and all tests show you as being in not only OK, but

apparently remarkably good health for your or any age.

Patient: Yes, yes, but what if they’re wrong? I’m the one who pays

the price.

Therapist: Jane, it’s interesting. Somehow, it seems as though it’s

easier for you to believe you have a hidden, undiagnosable heart

defect impenetrable to modern medical science, than think that

there may be any psychological reason you may be panicking. Part

of you would rather feel like you’re on the razor’s edge than think

about whatever it is you’re avoiding.

Panic-focused

Interpretation Therapist: When you finally saw [your boyfriend], did you feel any

of that disappointment or anger you described to me last session,

about his lack of communication?

Patient: Maybe just a little. But, I know that he’s been so busy

recently, I can’t consider myself the most important thing all the

time in his life. That would be so selfish of me.

Therapist: You’re putting in all this grand effort to welcome him,

at the same time he’s been treating you poorly. I wonder if it is

easier for you to try to feel excited, rather than experience your

disappointment, which might lead you to have to reevaluate the

relationship. I think that considering the relationship feels very

dangerous to you, you might break up. I wonder if trying to ignore

that anger and concern about separation leaves you feeling anxious

and panicky.

Page 130: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

118

Appendix D

Supplementary tables (Study 2)

Table S2.

Correlations between emotional expression ratings within the same 5-minute segment.

Overall

Expression

Grief/Sadness Anger/Assertion Anxiety/Distress

Overall

Expression

1 0.48*** 0.38*** 0.65***

Grief/Sadness 1 0.10† 0.24***

Anger/Assertion 1 0.01

Anxiety/Distress 1

† = p <.10; *** = p <.001

Table S3.

Average levels of emotional expression per 5-minute segment across sampled sessions.

Session 2 (M / SD) Session 5 (M / SD) Session 10 (M /

SD)

Overall Expressivity 1.10 (0.43) 0.99 (0.36) 1.04 (0.39)

Grief/Sadness 0.33 (0.29) 0.19 (0.17) 0.17 (0.19)

Anger/Assertion 0.30 (0.21) 0.32 (0.26) 0.36 (0.29)

Anxiety/Distress 0.68 (0.38) 0.66 (0.32) 0.74 (0.37)

Note. As estimated in a mixed model using the R package lme4 (Bates et al., 2017),

grief/sadness expression was estimated to significantly decrease within-person as patients

progressed from sessions 2 to 5 to 10, coded as 0, 1, and 2 time indicators (B = -0.08

[95% CI: -0.13 to -0.04], SE = 0.02, t[74.8] = -3.67, r = 0.39, p <.001). There were no

other reliable patterns of change in emotional expression experienced by the average

patient.

Page 131: What Process Works For Whom: Individual Differences And

119

Table S4.

Sources of variance in emotional expression per 5-minute segment.

Patient-Level

Variance

Session-Level

Variance (in

Patient)

Unique/Residual

Variance

Overall Expressivity 29.8% 17.9% 52.2%

Grief/Sadness 15.2% 14.7% 70.1%

Anger/Assertion 10.0% 20.6% 69.3%

Anxiety/Distress 23.6% 27.8% 48.6%

Note. Patient-level variance indicates the degree to which a given patient was reliably

more versus less expressive across all of their observed ratings and sessions. Session-

level variance indicates the degree to which patients sometimes have entire sessions that

are more versus less expressive. Unique/residual variance indicates variability in

expression that is not predicted by the fact that a rating comes from a given patient, or

which session of a given patient is being rated. Variance components were extracted from

a mixed model (Bates et al., 2017) including a random effect of session nested within a

random effect of patient.