why is spatial stereoacuity so poor?

64
hy is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor Martin S. Banks School of Optometry, Dept. of Psychology UC Berkeley Sergei Gepshtein Vision Science Program UC Berkeley Michael S. Landy Dept. of Psychology, Center for Neural Science NYU Supported by NIH

Upload: nakia

Post on 18-Feb-2016

32 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

DESCRIPTION

Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?. Martin S. Banks School of Optometry, Dept. of Psychology UC Berkeley. Sergei Gepshtein Vision Science Program UC Berkeley. Michael S. Landy Dept. of Psychology, Center for Neural Science NYU. Supported by NIH. Depth Perception. Depth Perception. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Martin S. BanksSchool of Optometry, Dept. of Psychology

UC Berkeley

Sergei GepshteinVision Science Program

UC Berkeley

Michael S. LandyDept. of Psychology, Center for Neural Science

NYU

Supported by NIH

Page 2: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Depth Perception

Page 3: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Depth Perception

How precise is the depth map generated from disparity?

Page 4: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Precision of Stereopsis

Stereo precision measured in various waysA: Precision of detecting depth change on line of sightD: Precision of detecting spatial variation in depth

from Tyler (1977)

Page 5: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Precision of Stereopsis

Stereo precision measured in various waysA: Detect depth change on line of sightD: Precision of detecting spatial variation in depth

from Tyler (1977)

Page 6: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Precision of Stereopsis

Stereo precision measured in various waysA: Detect depth change on line of sightD: Detect spatial variation in depth

from Tyler (1977)

Page 7: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity

• Modulate disparity sinusoidally creating corrugations in depth.

• Least disparity required for detection as a function of spatial frequency of corrugations: “Disparity MTF”.

• Index of precision of depth map.

Page 8: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity MTF

•Disparity modulation threshold as a function of spatial frequency of corrugations.

•Bradshaw & Rogers (1999).

•Horizontal & vertical corrugations.

•Disparity MTF: acuity = 2-3 cpd; peak at 0.3 cpd.

Page 9: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Luminance Contrast Sensitivity & Acuity

• Luminance contrast sensitivity function (CSF): contrast for detection as function of spatial frequency.

• Proven useful for characterizing limits of visual performance and for understanding optical, retinal, & post-retinal processing.

• Highest detectable spatial frequency (grating acuity): 40-50 c/deg.

Page 10: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity MTF

Spatial stereoacuity more than 1 log unit lower than luminance acuity.

Page 11: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity MTF

Why is spatial stereoacuity so low?

Spatial stereoacuity more than 1 log unit lower than luminance acuity.

Page 12: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Likely Constraints to Spatial Stereoacuity

1. Sampling constraints in the stimulus: Stereoacuity measured using random-element stereograms. Discrete sampling limits the highest spatial frequency one can reconstruct.

2. Disparity gradient limit: With increasing spatial frequency, the disparity gradient increases. If gradient approaches 1.0, binocular fusion fails.

3. Spatial filtering at the front end: Optical quality & retinal sampling limit acuity in other tasks, so probably limits spatial stereoacuity as well.

4. The correspondence problem: Manner in which binocular matching occurs presumably affects spatial stereoacuity.

Page 13: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Likely Constraints to Spatial Stereoacuity

1. Sampling constraints in the stimulus: Stereoacuity measured using random-element stereograms. Discrete sampling limits the highest spatial frequency one can reconstruct.

2. Disparity gradient limit: With increasing spatial frequency, the disparity gradient increases. If gradient approaches 1.0, binocular fusion fails.

3. Spatial filtering at the front end: Optical quality & retinal sampling limit acuity in other tasks, so probably limits spatial stereoacuity as well.

4. The correspondence problem: Manner in which binocular matching occurs presumably affects spatial stereoacuity.

Page 14: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Sampling Limit: Nyquist Frequency

• Signal reconstruction from discrete samples.

• At least 2 samples required per cycle.

• In 1d, highest recoverable spatial frequency is Nyquist frequency:

where N is number of samples per unit distance.

12Nf N

Page 15: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Sampling Limit: Nyquist Frequency

• Signal reconstruction from 2d discrete samples.

• In 2d, Nyquist frequency is:

where N is number of samples in area A.

12NNfA

Page 16: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Methodology

• Random-dot stereograms with sinusoidal disparity corrugations.

• Corrugation orientations: +/-20 deg (near horizontal).

• Observers identified orientation in 2-IFC psychophysical procedure; phase randomized.

• Spatial frequency of corrugations varied according to adaptive staircase procedure.

• Spatial stereoacuity threshold obtained for wide range of dot densities.

• Duration = 600 msec; disparity amplitude = 16 minarc.

Page 17: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Stimuli

Page 18: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity as a function of Dot Density

Dot Density (dots/deg2)

Spat

ial S

tere

oacu

ity (c

/deg

)•Acuity proportional to dot density squared.

•Scale invariance!

•Asymptote at high density.

0.1

1.0

ModulationAmplitude

ViewingDistance

16 min 39 cm

JMA MSB

DMV TMG

0.1 1.0 10 100 0.1 1.0 10 1000.1

1.0

a d

1

0.1

1

0.110.1 10 100 10.1 10 100

Page 19: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

• Calculated Nyquist frequency for our displays.

Dot Density (dots/deg2)

Spat

ial S

tere

oacu

ity (c

/deg

)

0.1

1.0

ModulationAmplitude

ViewingDistance

16 min 39 cm

JMA MSB

DMV TMG

0.1 1.0 10 100 0.1 1.0 10 1000.1

1.0

1

0.1

1

0.110.1 10 100 10.1 10 100

Spatial Stereoacuity & Nyquist Limit

Page 20: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

• Calculated Nyquist frequency for our displays.

• Acuity approx. equal to Nyquist frequency except at high densities.

Dot Density (dots/deg2)

Spat

ial S

tere

oacu

ity (c

/deg

)

0.1

1.0

ModulationAmplitude

ViewingDistance

16 min 39 cm

JMA MSB

DMV TMG

0.1 1.0 10 100 0.1 1.0 10 1000.1

1.0

1

0.1

1

0.110.1 10 100 10.1 10 100

Spatial Stereoacuity & Nyquist Limit

Nyquist frequency

Page 21: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Types of Random-element Stereograms

Jittered-lattice: dots displaced randomly from regular latticeSparse random: dots positioned randomly

Page 22: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Sampling Limit: Nyquist Frequency

Same acuities with jittered-lattice and sparse random stereograms. Both follow Nyquist limit at low densities.

Page 23: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Likely Constraints to Spatial Stereoacuity

1. Sampling constraints in the stimulus: Stereoacuity measured using random-element stereograms. Discrete sampling limits the highest spatial frequency one can reconstruct.

2. Disparity gradient limit: With increasing spatial frequency, the disparity gradient increases. If gradient approaches 1.0, binocular fusion fails.

3. Spatial filtering at the front end: Optical quality & retinal sampling limit acuity in other tasks, so probably limits spatial stereoacuity as well.

4. The correspondence problem: Manner in which binocular matching occurs presumably affects spatial stereoacuity.

Page 24: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient

P1

P2

L R

Disparity gradient = disparity / separation

= (R – L) / [(L + R)/2]

Page 25: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient

Disparity gradient = 0

P1 & P2 on horopter

R = L, so disparity = 0

P1 P2

Page 26: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient

Disparity gradient =

P1 & P2 on cyclopean line of sight

R = -L, so separation = 0

P1

P2

Page 27: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient

Disparity gradient for different directions.

horizontaldisparity

sepa

ratio

n

P1

P2 (left & right eyes)

Page 28: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient Limit

• Burt & Julesz (1980): fusion as function of disparity, separation, & direction (tilt).

• Set direction & horizontal disparity and found smallest fusable separation.

disparity

sepa

ratio

n

P1

P2 (left & right eyes)

direction

Page 29: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient Limit

• Fusion breaks when disparity gradient reaches constant value.

• Critical gradient = ~1.

• “Disparity gradient limit”.

• Limit same for all directions.

Page 30: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient Limit

Page 31: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Disparity Gradient Limit

• Panum’s fusion area (hatched).

• Disparity gradient limit means that fusion area affected by nearby objects (A).

• Forbidden zone is conical (isotropic).

Page 32: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Horizontal Position (deg)

Dis

parit

y (d

eg)

highest gradient

peak-trough gradient

Disparity Gradient & Spatial Frequency

• Disparity gradient for sinusoid is indeterminant.

• But for fixed amplitude, gradient proportional to spatial frequency.

• We may have approached disparity gradient limit.

• Tested by reducing disparity amplitude from 16 to 4.8 minarc.

Page 33: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Disparity Gradient Limit

• Reducing disparity amplitude increases acuity at high dot densities (where DG is high).

• Lowers acuity slightly at low densities (where DG is low).

0.1

1.0

ModulationAmplitude

ViewingDistance

39 cm4.8 min

16 min 39 cm

JMA MSB

DMV TMG

0.1 1.0 10 100 0.1 1.0 10 1000.1

1.0

Nyquist frequency

Dot Density (dots/deg2)

Spat

ial S

tere

oacu

ity (c

/deg

)

1

0.1

1

0.110.1 10 100 10.1 10 100

Page 34: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Likely Constraints to Spatial Stereoacuity

1. Sampling constraints in the stimulus: Stereoacuity measured using random-element stereograms. Discrete sampling limits the highest spatial frequency one can reconstruct.

2. Disparity gradient limit: With increasing spatial frequency, the disparity gradient increases. If gradient approaches 1.0, binocular fusion fails.

3. Spatial filtering at the front end: Optical quality & retinal sampling limit acuity in other tasks, so probably limits spatial stereoacuity as well.

4. The correspondence problem: Manner in which binocular matching occurs presumably affects spatial stereoacuity.

Page 35: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Stereoacuity & Front-end Spatial Filtering

• Low-pass spatial filtering at front-end of visual system determines high-frequency roll-off of luminance CSF.

• Tested similar effects on spatial stereoacuity by:

1. Decreasing retinal image size of dots by increasing viewing distance.

2. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of retinal eccentricity.

3. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of blur.

Page 36: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Stereoacuity & Front-end Spatial Filtering

• Low-pass spatial filtering at front-end of visual system determines high-frequency roll-off of luminance CSF.

• Tested similar effects on spatial stereoacuity by:

1. Decreasing retinal image size of dots by increasing viewing distance.

2. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of retinal eccentricity.

3. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of blur.

Page 37: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity at Higher Densities

• Monocular artifacts at high dot densities.

• Reduce dot size to test upper limit.

• Increase viewing distance from 39-154 cm.

• Acuity still levels off, but at higher value.

0.1

1.0

JMA MSB

Spat

ialS

tere

oacu

ity(c

/deg

)

DMV TMG

Dot Density (dot/deg2)

0.1 1.0 10 100 0.1 1.0 10 1000.1

1.0

ModulationAmplitude

ViewingDistance

39 cm4.8 min154 cm4.8 min

Nyquist frequency

Dot Density (dots/deg2)

Spat

ial S

tere

oacu

ity (c

/deg

) 1

0.1

1

0.110.1 10 100 10.1 10 100

Page 38: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Stereoacuity & Front-end Spatial Filtering

• Low-pass spatial filtering at front-end of visual system determines high-frequency roll-off of luminance CSF.

• Tested similar effects on spatial stereoacuity by:

1. Decreasing retinal image size of dots by increasing viewing distance.

2. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of retinal eccentricity.

3. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of blur.

Page 39: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Retinal Eccentricity

8 deg

4 deg

fixation point

eccentricity

•Elliptical patch with sinusoidal corrugation.

•Patch centered at one of three eccentricities (subject dependent).

•Eccentricity random; duration = 250 ms.

•Same task as before.•Again vary dot density.

Page 40: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Retinal Eccentricity

Page 41: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Retinal Eccentricity

0.1

1.0

TMG

0.1 1.0 10 100

YHH

1.0 10 100 1.0 10 100

SSG

Dot Density (dots/deg2)

Spat

ial S

tere

oacu

ity (c

/deg

)

0 deg5.210.4

0 deg6.212.4

0 deg6.813.6

Retinal eccentricity

•Same acuities at low dot densities; Nyquist. •Asymptote varies significantly with retinal eccentricity.

1

0.1

10.1 10 100 1 10 100 1 10 100

Page 42: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Stereoacuity & Front-end Spatial Filtering

• Low-pass spatial filtering at front-end of visual system determines high-frequency roll-off of luminance CSF.

• Tested similar effects on spatial stereoacuity by:

1. Decreasing retinal image size of dots by increasing viewing distance.

2. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of retinal eccentricity.

3. Measuring stereoacuity as a function of blur.

Page 43: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Blur

• We examined effect of blur on foveal spatial stereoacuity.• Three levels of blur introduced with diffusion plate:

no blur ( = 0 deg)low blur ( = 0.12)high blur ( = 0.25)

Page 44: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Blur

• We examined effect of blur on foveal spatial stereoacuity.• Three levels of blur introduced with diffusion plate:

no blur ( = 0 deg)low blur ( = 0.12)high blur ( = 0.25)

Page 45: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Spatial Stereoacuity & Blur

•Same acuities at low dot densities; Nyquist. •Asymptote varies significantly with spatial lowpass filtering.

Page 46: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Likely Constraints to Spatial Stereoacuity

1. Sampling constraints in the stimulus: Stereoacuity measured using random-element stereograms. Discrete sampling limits the highest spatial frequency one can reconstruct.

2. Disparity gradient limit: With increasing spatial frequency, the disparity gradient increases. If gradient approaches 1.0, binocular fusion fails.

3. Spatial filtering at the front end: Optical quality & retinal sampling limit acuity in other tasks, so probably limits spatial stereoacuity as well.

4. The correspondence problem: Manner in which binocular matching occurs presumably affects spatial stereoacuity.

Page 47: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Binocular Matching by Correlation

1. Binocular matching by correlation: basic and well-studied technique for obtaining depth map from binocular images.

Computer vision: Kanade & Okutomi (1994); Panton (1978)

Physiology: Ohzawa, DeAngelis, & Freeman (1990); Cumming & Parker (1997)

2. We developed a cross-correlation algorithm for binocular matching & compared its properties to the psychophysics.

Page 48: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Left eye’s image Right eye’s image

• Compute cross-correlation between eyes’ images.

• Window in left eye’s image moved orthogonal to signal.

• For each position in left eye, window in right eye’s image moved horizontally & cross-correlation computed.

Binocular Matching by Correlation

Page 49: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Left eye’s image Right eye’s image

• Compute cross-correlation between eyes’ images.

• Window in left eye’s image moved orthogonal to signal.

• For each position in left eye, window in right eye’s image moved horizontally & cross-correlation computed.

Binocular Matching by Correlation

Page 50: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Left eye’s image Right eye’s image

• Compute cross-correlation between eyes’ images.

• Window in left eye’s image moved orthogonal to signal.

• For each position in left eye, window in right eye’s image moved horizontally & cross-correlation computed.

Binocular Matching by Correlation

Page 51: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Binocular Matching by Correlation

Plot correlation as a function of position in left eye (red arrow) & relative position in right eye (blue arrow); disparity.

Correlation (gray value) high where images similar & low where dissimilar.

X axis

Y axis

Position in left eye’s image

Posi

tion

in ri

ght e

ye’s

imag

e

Page 52: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

0.1 1 10 100Dot Density

Spat

ial F

requ

ency

1

0.1

Examples of Output

Dot density: 16 dots/deg2

Spatial frequency: 1 c/degWindow size: 0.2 deg

Disparity waveform evident in output

Page 53: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Window Size & Dot Density

Page 54: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Window Size & Dot Density

2 0 7.w d

Correlation window must be large enough to contain sufficient luminance variation to find correct matches

Page 55: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Window Size & Spatial Frequency

Page 56: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Window Size & Spatial Frequency

0 5.w f

When significant depth variation is present in a region, window must be small enough to respond differentially

Page 57: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Window Size, # Samples, & Spatial Frequency

0 5.w f

2 0 7.w d From two constraints:

then substitute for w, take log:

1 0 222

log log .f d

Page 58: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Disparity Gradient

• Fix spatial frequency, dot density, & window size.

• Increase disparity amplitude (which also increases disparity gradient: 0.21, 0.59, 1.77).

• As approach 1.0, disparity estimation becomes poor. Images too dissimilar in two eyes.

• Matching by correlation yields piecewise frontal estimates.

Page 59: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Disparity Gradient

• Fix spatial frequency, dot density, & window size.

• Increase disparity amplitude (which also increases disparity gradient: 0.21, 0.59, 1.77).

• As approach 1.0, disparity estimation becomes poor. Images too dissimilar in two eyes.

• Matching by correlation yields piecewise frontal estimates.

Page 60: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Low-pass Spatial Filtering

• Amount of variation in image dependent on spatial-frequency content.

• If proportional to w and inversely proportional to , variation constant in cycles/window.

• Algorithm yields similar outputs for these images.

• For each , there’s a window just large enough to yield good disparity estimates.

• Highest detectable spatial frequency inversely proportional to .

d

Page 61: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Effect of Low-pass Spatial Filtering

• Spatial stereoacuity for different amounts of blur. • : all filtering elements: dots, optics, diffusion screen.• Horizontal lines: predictions for asymptotic acuities.• Asymptotic acuity limited by filtering before binocular combination.

Page 62: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Correlation algorithm reveals two effects.

1. Disparity estimation is poor when there’s insufficient intensity variation within correlation window. a. when window too small for presented dot density b. when spatial-frequency content is too low.c. employ a larger window (or receptive field).

2. Disparity estimation is poor when correlation window is too large in direction of maximum disparity gradient.

a. when window width greater than half cycle of stimulus. b. employ a smaller window (or receptive field).

2 0.7w d

0.5w f

Summary of Matching Effects

Page 63: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Summary

1. Sampling constraints in the stimulus: Stereoacuity follows Nyquist limit for all but highest densities. Occurs in peripheral visual field and in fovea with blur.

2. Disparity gradient limit: Stereoacuity reduced at high gradients.

3. Spatial filtering at the front end: Low-pass filtering before binocular combination determines asymptotic acuity.

4. The correspondence problem: Binocular matching by correlation requires sufficient information in correlation window & thereby reduces highest attainable acuity. Visual system measures disparity in piecewise frontal fashion.

Page 64: Why is Spatial Stereoacuity so Poor?

Depth Map from Disparity

depth-varying scene

Disparity estimates are piecewise frontal.Only one perceived depth per direction.