a case study of the physics enhancement project for two year colleges, its effects and outcomes on...

Upload: red-kite

Post on 02-Jun-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    1/277

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    2/277

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    3/277

    Orde r Number 91S0457

    A concept learning and teaching approach to the instruction of linear motion in introductory college physics

    Chyuan, Jong-pyng Michael, Ph.D.

    The Ohio State University, 1991

    U M I300 N. Zeeb Rd.Ann Aibor, MI 48106

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    4/277

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    5/277

    A CONCEPT LEARNING A N D TEACHING APPROACH TO THE

    INSTRUCTION OF LINEAR MOTION

    IN

    INTRODUCTORY COLLEGE PHYSICS

    DISSERTATION

    Presented in Partial Fulfillment o f the Requirements for

    the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate

    School of The Ohio State University

    By

    Jong-pyng Michael Chyuan, B.S., M.S .

    * * * * *

    The Ohio State Universi ty

    1991

    Dissertation Committee:

    Dr. Arthur L. White

    Dr. Wil li am D. Ploughe

    Dr. Keith A. Hall

    Approved by

    Coll ege of Education

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    6/277

    To my parents, my wife, and my son

    ii

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    7/277

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    X wish to express my deep grati tude to Dr. Arthur L.

    White for his enthusiastic and expert guidance throughout

    this research project. Th e complet ion of this dissertation

    would no t have been possible w ithout support.

    Sincere appreciation and thanks is expressed to Dr.

    Will iam D. Ploughe for sharing his knowledge and experience

    teaching physics and for generously implementing the three

    teaching methods in his physics 101 classes.

    I would also like to thank Dr. Keith A. Hall fo r his

    valuable counsel and thoughtful suggestions throughout this

    research project.

    Finally, my deep appreciat ion and thanks to a dear

    friend and colleague, Evelyn Zei fman Becker, for her patience and help in edi ting manuscript.

    iii

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    8/277

    VITA

    November 13, 1952 ........... Born - Taiwan,Republic of China

    1974 ......................... B.S., National Taiwan NormalUniversity, Taiwan, Republic of China

    1974-1975 ................... Teaching Assistant,Provincial Tainan Teachers Co11ege, Ta iwan, ROC

    1979 ......................... M.S., National Taiwan NormalUniversity, Taiwan, Republic of China

    1979-1980 ................... Instructor National Defense Medicine

    College, Taiwan, ROC

    1979-1987 ................... InstructorProvincial Taipei Teachers College, Taiwan, ROC

    1987-present ................. Associate Professor,Taipei Teachers College,

    Taiwan, ROC1987-1988 . . . Director of Computer Center

    Taipei Teachers College Taiwan, ROC

    1990-present ................. Graduate Research AssociateThe Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA

    PUBLICATIONS

    Chyuan, J. M. (1979). The growth and the analysis of Cu-Zn

    alloy crystal . Unpublished mas ters thesis, National

    Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan, ROC.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1980). Physics of Matter, Temperature and Heat (chapter 6, 7). In J. Chou (Ed.), Teachers College Phvsics (I). Taipei: Cheng-jong Book Company.

    iv

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    9/277

    Chyuan, J. M. (1981). Waves and sound, Optics, Electricity and Magnetism, Electromagnetism, Modern Physics (chapter 8, 9, 10, 11, 12). In J. Chou (Ed.), Teachers College Physics (II). Taipei: Cheng-jong Book company.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1984). Water and energy. In T. Li (Ed.),Children Natural Science Research and Study Curriculum and Development (I). Taipei: Taipei Teachers College.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1985). The analysis and research of primary school natural science learning adaptation's problem in Taipei area . Taipei: Jong-shing Publishing.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1985). Energy and soil. In M. Kou (Ed.),Children Natural Science Research and Study Curriculum and Development (II). Taipei: Taipei Teachers College.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1986). Energy and soil. In J. Chen (Ed.), Children Natural Science Research and Study Curriculum and Development (III). Taipei: Taipei Teachers College.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1987). Energy and soil. In J. M. Chyuan(Ed.), Childrerr-i'Jatural Science Research and Study Curriculum and Development (IV^. Taipei: Taipei Teachers College.

    Chyuan, J. M. (1988). Taipei teachers college entrance examination information system (EEIS). In J. M. Chyuan (Ed.), Computer Education Symposium of Teachers colleges in Taiwan Area (pp. 97 - 129). Taipei: Taipei Teachers College.

    FIELDS OF STUDY

    Major Field: Education

    Studies in Science Education: Dr. Arthur White,Dr. Victor Mayer, Dr. Patricia Blosser.

    Studies in Educational Research: Dr. Arthur White,Dr. Keith Hall, Dr. John Kennedy.

    Studies in Technology in Science: Dr. William Ploughe.

    v

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    10/277

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    Page

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...................................... iii

    V I T A ..................................................... iv

    LIST OF T A B L E S ........................................ viii

    LIST OF FIGURES ....................................... xii

    CHAPTER

    I. INTRODUCTION .................................. 1

    Need for Study ................................ 3Statement of the Problem ...................... 6Definition of Terms ............................ 8

    A s s u m p t i o n s ....................................... 10Delimitations .. ................................ 10L i m i t a t i o n s .................................... 11

    II. LITERATURE REVIEW ............................... 13

    Misconception .................................. 13Concept Definition ............................ 20Concept Learning .............................. 22Concept M a p ....................................... 26Teaching Concept .............................. 32Concept Teaching Model ........................ 39General Instruction Model 4 6

    vi

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    11/277

    III. METHODS AND PROCEDURES ............................ 50

    Design ............................................. 50Sample and Population ........................... 59Instrumentation .................................. 65Research Design .................................. 80

    Analysis ........................................... 85Procedures ......................................... 86

    IV. R E S U L T S ............................................. 90

    Cognitive Learning Effect ....................... 90Cognitive Teaching Effect ....................... IllLearning Physics Attitude Effect ............... 116 Multivariate Regression Effect ................. 121S u m m a r y ............................................ 137

    V. SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS .................... 139

    Summary of Findings ......................... 139Discussion ........................................ 142Summary and Interpretation ..................... 149Specific Instructional Recommendations ........ 154Future Research .................................. 159

    BIBLIOGRAPHY .......................................... 162

    APPENDICES

    A. Physics Concept Instruction Design ............. 169

    B. Physics Concept Map T e s t .......................... 221

    C. Physics Misconception T e s t ........................ 226

    D. Physics Achievement T e s t .......................... 231

    E. Physics Attitude Test ............................. 236

    F. Course Copies for the Concept Teaching Method . . 246G. Students' Constructed Concept Maps ............. 255

    H. Frequency Distribution for Items of the Physics Misconception Test and the Physics Att itude Test. 258

    vii

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    12/277

    LIST OF TABLES

    Table Page1. Univariate ANOVA on the Physics Misconception

    Pretest by Retained/Deleted Group ............. 61

    2. Distribution by Gender among Three Classes . . . 62

    3. Distribution by Program Areas among three Groups. 62

    4. Distribution by Age among Three Classes . . . . 63

    5. Distribution by High School Physics Chemistry Courseamong Three Classes ............................ 64

    6. Items of the Achievement Test by Knowledge Level. 70

    7. Means, Standard Deviations, and Item-TotalCorrelations for the Physics Learning Concept

    Attitude Test .................................. 74

    8. Means, Standard Deviations, and Item-TotalCorrelations for the Physics Misconception Attitude T e s t ............................................ 75

    9. Means, Standard Deviations, and Item-TotalCorrelations for the Physics Teaching Concept

    Attitude Test .................................. 76

    10. Means, Standard Deviations, and Item-TotalCorrelations for the Physics Concept Map Attitude Test .......................................... 77

    11. The Mode of Physics Instruction ............... 82

    12. Sample Size, Means, and Standard Deviations of thePhysics Concept Map Pretest and Posttest, Physics

    Misconception Pretest and Posttest, and Physics Achievement Test among Three Groups ........... 92

    viii

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    13/277

    13. Multivariate Tests of Regression Effect for theRelationship between Dependent Variables, Physics Concept Map Posttest, Physics Misconception Posttest, and Physics Achievement Test, and Covariates, PhysicsConcept Map Pretest and Physics Misconception P r e t e s t .......... 93

    14. Multivariate and Univariate Analysis of Covariance: Treatment Effect on the Concept Map Posttest, the

    Misconception Posttest, and the Achievement Test with the Concept Map Pretest and the Misconception

    Pretest as Covariates ......................... 95

    15. Bryant-Paulson Comparisons on Physics Concept Map T e s t ............................................ 96

    16. Test of Significance for Discriminant Functions about Dependent Variables: Physics Misconception Posttest, Physics Concept Map Posttest, and Physics

    Achievement T e s t ................................ 98

    17. Canonical Discriminant Functions Evaluated at Group Centroids of the Physics Misconception Posttest, the Physics Concept Map Posttest, and the Physics Achievement Test ........................ 99

    18. Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients of Dependent Variables ............................. 100

    19. Sample Size, Means, and Standard Deviations of the Proposition, Hierarchy, Cross-Link, and Example of the Concept Map Posttest among Three Groups . . 102

    20. Multivariate Tests of Regression Effect for the Relationship between Dependent Variables, Four Parts of the Concept Map Test, and Covariates,Concept Map Pretest and Misconception Pretest . . 103

    21. Multivariate and Univariate Analysis of Covariance:

    Treatment Effect on the Concept Map Posttest with thePhysics Concept Map Pretest and the Physics Misconception Pretest as Covariates .......... 104

    22. Bryant-Paulson Comparisons on the Proposition of thePhysics Concept Map T e s t .......................... 106

    ix

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    14/277

    23. Bryant-Paulson Comparisons on the Hierarchy of the Physics Concept Map T e s t .......................... 106

    24. Test of Significance for Discriminant Functions about Dependent Variables: Proposition, Hierarchy,Cross-Link, and Example of the Concept Map Test . 107

    25. Canonical Discriminant Functions Evaluated atGroup Centroids of the Concept Map Test . . . . 109

    26. Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficientsof Dependent Variables ......................... 110

    27. Mult ivariate Tests of the Effect of Concept Map and Misconception Pre-Post-Tests and the Effectof Group by Pr e- Po st -T es ts .................... 113

    28. Within-Subject Effect on Misconception Test . . 114

    29. Within-Subject Effect on Concept Map Test . . . 114

    30. Means and Standard Deviations of Four AttitudeTests ............................................. 118

    31. Multivariate Tests of Regression Effect for theRelationship between Dependent Variables, Four

    Attitude Subtests, and Covariates, Concept Map Pretest and Misconception Pretest ............. 119

    32. Multivariate Analysis of Covariance: Treatment Group Effect Test of Four Parts of the Concept

    Map P o s t t e s t .................................. 119

    33. Means of the Physics Attitude T e s t ............ 120

    34. Correlation of the Dependent Variables and thePredictors for the Traditional Teaching Method (Group 1) 126

    35. Canonical Correlation, Coefficients, and Componentsfor Each Canonical Variate for the Group 1 . . . 127

    x

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    15/277

    36. Multivariate Regression Analysis of the Relation between the Concept Map Posttest, Misconception

    Posttest, and Achievement Test, and the Four Attitude Subtests for the Traditional Teaching Method . . 128

    37. Univariate F-Test of the Dependent Variables forthe Traditional Teaching Method ............... 128

    38. Multiple Regression Test on the Traditional Teaching Method ................................ 129

    39. Correlation of the Dependent Variables and the Predictors for the Example-NonexampleTeaching Method (Group 2) 130

    40. Canonical Correlation Mult ivariate Regression Analysis of the Relation between the Concept Map

    Posttest, Misconception Posttest, and Achievement Test, and the Four Attitude Subtests for the Example-Nonexample Teaching Method (Group 2) . . 131

    41. Correlation of the Dependent Variables and the Predictors for the Concept Teaching Method (Group 3 ) ........................................... 132

    42. Canonical Correlation, Coefficients, and Components for Each Canonical Variate for the Group 3 . . . 133

    43. Multivariate Regression Analysis of the relation between the Concept Map Posttest, Misconception

    Posttest, and Achievement Test, and the Four Attitude Subtests, for the concept Teaching Method . . . 134

    44. Univariate F-Test of the Dependent Variables forthe Concept Teaching Method .................. 134

    45. Multiple Regression Test on the Concept Teaching M e t h o d ............................................. 135

    46. The Overall Significant Correlation of the Dependent Variables and the Predictors Relating Group 1, 2, and 3 136

    47. Summary of the Significant Results in This Study. 137

    48. Concepts Taught in the Three Lecture Sessions . . 156

    xi

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    16/277

    LIST OF FIGURES

    Figure Page

    1. Probabil ity Levels of Examples and Nonexamples . 36

    2. A Concept Teaching-Model ( Tennyson and Cocchiarella, 1986) 47

    3. General Instructional Model (Berlin & White,1987) 49

    4. The Schema of the Physics Concept InstructionalDesign ........................................ 53

    5. Physics Instructional Design Model ............. 54

    6. The Teacher-Made Concept Map About Position . . 57

    7. The Instructional Design for the Concept,P o s i t i o n ..................................... 58

    8. The Name and Contents of Experiment Instruments . 79

    9. The Concept Teaching Sequence among ThreeTreatments .............................. 81

    10. Physics Concept Teaching and Learning ResearchD e s i g n ....................................... 83

    11. Data Collection Procedure ...................... 88

    12. Time T a b l e .................................... 89

    13. The Three Group Centroids of the Physics Misconception Posttest, the Physics Concept Map

    Posttest, and Physics Achievement Test in the Discriminant-Function Space ................... 99

    14. The Three Group Centroids of the Concept MapPosttest in the Discriminant-Function Space . . . 109

    15. Interaction between the Three Teaching Methodsand the Concept Map Pretest and Posttest . . . . 115

    xii

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    17/277

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    18/277

    2

    concepts, Novak and Gowin (1984) illustrate an educational

    tool referred to as concept mapping to help students learn

    and to help teachers organize teaching material. A concept

    map is a schematic device for representing a set of concept

    meanings embedded in a framework of propositions which are

    two or more concept labels linked by words in a semantic

    unit.

    As for teaching concepts in the classroom, Merrill and

    Tennyson (1977), based on the conceptual model of

    classification behavior (Woolley & Tennyson, 1972) and

    experimental research studies (Tennyson, 1973; Tennyson,

    Steve, & Boutwell, 1975), developed a concept-teaching model

    in which a set of instructional design guidelines is

    provided to enhance concept teaching. Tennyson and

    Cocchiarella (1986) update and extend the concept-teaching

    model which is composed of two fundamental components of

    instructional design: (1) the content structure of a given

    domain of information and (2) the organization of

    instructional design variables related to the use of

    specific content structures.

    In classroom teaching, the organization of teaching

    materials and activities is closely concerned with concept teaching. Berlin and White (1987) present an instructional

    model which provides for the infusion and integration of

    teaching technology into the instructional process, and it

    also assists teachers in effectively promoting the learning

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    19/277

    3

    of concepts in the classroom.

    Need for Study

    Understanding of kinematical concepts is the ability to

    apply them successfully in learning and interpreting simple

    motions of real objects. However, certain conceptual

    difficulties occur frequently and predictably among

    introductory physics students in college. Student

    understanding of physical concepts has been subject to

    descriptive analysis (Trowbridge & McDermott, 1980). Physics

    instructors generally share a common interpretation of the

    kinematical concepts based on operational definitions and

    precise verbal and mathematical articulation. On the other

    hand, students are likely to have a wide variety of somewhat

    vague and undifferentiated ideas about motion based on

    intuition, experience, and their perception of previous

    instruction. Thus students often have insufficient

    qualitative understanding of position, velocity, and

    acceleration (Trowbridge & McDermott, 1981). Moreover, in

    introductory physics teaching, Ploughe (1990, private

    communication) indicates that many college students have

    verbal problems in explaining the phenomena of motion and

    incorrectly use physics definitions to discriminate the

    concepts of motion.

    Frequently, many students taking introductory physics

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    20/277

    cannot apply what they have learned about graphs from their

    study of mathematics to physics. The difficulties

    experienced by students in connecting graphs to physical

    concepts includes the indecision as to whether to extract

    the desired information from the slope or the height of a

    graph. Students also find it more difficult to interpret

    curved graphs than straight-line graphs (McDermott,

    Rosenquist & Zee, 1987; White, 1987; Mokros & Tinker, 1987).

    Furthermore, many students are unable to translate back and

    forth from a position versus time graph to a velocity versus

    time graph (McDermott et al., 1987).

    Prior to the instruction of introductory college

    physics, many students have a set of protoconcepts for

    interpreting motion in the rea l world (Trowbridge &

    McDermott, 1980). McCloskey (1983) indicates that the

    protoconcepts, misconceptions, appear to be grounded in a

    systematic, intuitive theory of motion and they are not

    consistent with fundamental principles of Newtonian

    mechanics. Halloun and Hestenes (1985) also explain that a

    system of beliefs and intuitions about physical phenomena

    are possessed by each college student entering a first

    course (motion) in physics and the system is derived from extensive personal experience. This system functions as a

    common sense theory of the physical world which the student

    uses to interpret what he uses and hears in the physics

    course. Yet conventional physics instruction fails almost

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    21/277

    5

    completely to take this into account. Moreover, the level of

    mathematical competence is not sufficient for high

    performance in physics. McDermott, Rosenquist, and Zee

    (1987) express that students who have no trouble making

    physics graphs and computing slopes of graph cannot apply what

    they have learned about graphs from their study of

    mathematics to physics. Differences in gender, age, academic

    major, and high school mathematics showed no effect on

    physics achievement. Conventional instruction had little

    effect on the student's basic knowledge state and the basic

    knowledge gain under conventional instruction is essentially

    independent of the professor (Halloun & Hestenes, 1985).

    Thus, Clement (1982) suggests that development of innovative

    instruction techniques that emphasize rigorous understanding

    of qualitative physics principles should be encouraged.Based on using the conceptual model of classification

    behavior (Woolley & Tennyson, 1972), the example and

    nonexample teaching strategy had significant results for

    instructional procedures on the cognitive level of behavior

    in non-science areas (Tennyson, Woolley, & Merrill, 1972;

    Tennyson, 1973; Tennyson, Steve, & Boutwell 1975; Tennyson &

    Tennyson, 1975) and in a study of crystal structure

    (Tennyson, et al., 1975; Merrill & Tennyson, 1978). But,

    this teaching strategy has not been used for instructional

    procedures in the physics area.

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    22/277

    The concept-teaching model originally derived by

    Merrill and Tennyson (1977) and updated and extended by

    Tennyson and Cocchiarella (1986) is based on the theory of

    cognitive learning processes and presents strategies of

    concept instructional design. However, this model does not

    clearly infuse and integrate teaching technology in its

    strategies. On the other hand, the general instructional

    model designed by Berlin and White (1987) relates to

    organizing teaching materials and activities to combine with

    the concept-teaching model.

    Therefore, there is a need to (1) develop an

    instructional design, in accordance with a physics concept

    teaching model derived from learning theories and teaching

    models, that is related to the motion topic in introductory

    physics for college level students, (2) test whether the

    design can improve students' concept learning about motion,

    and (3) learn if it can be used as an instructional system

    for introductory college physics.

    Statement of the Problem

    The major tasks of this study are to : (1) develop and

    analyze a physics unit of physics instructional design

    derived from the physics concept teaching model for college

    non-science students, (2) test the design in regard to the

    students' learning physics concepts, students' physics

    misconceptions, students' physics achievement, and students'

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    23/277

    7

    attitude toward learning physics, (3) compare whether the

    design is better in helping students' concept learning than

    the teaching strategy using example and nonexample or the

    traditional teaching strategy, and (4) identify what should

    be done to further improve the design.

    Problems

    According to the statement of the main tasks in this

    study, three groups of college non-science students are

    randomly assigned to three teaching methods: physics concept

    teaching method, physics example-nonexample teaching method,

    and physics traditional teaching method. Thus the problems

    related to the major tasks are stated as follows:

    (1) Are there significant differences among the three

    groups of non-science students on measures of their learning

    physics concepts, their physics misconceptions, and their

    physics achievement after three teaching methods are

    implemented?

    (2) Are there significant difference in physics concept

    learning and physics misconception between before and

    after three different teaching methods being implemented by

    three groups of non-science students?(3) Are there significant differences among three

    groups of non-science students on measures of their

    attitudes toward learning physics concepts after three

    teaching methods are implemented?

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    24/277

    8

    (4) What is the predictive contribution of such

    variables to learning physics concepts, physics

    misconceptions, physics achievement, and attitude toward

    learning physics concepts for each of the three teaching

    methods?

    Definition of Terms

    Physics traditional teaching method An approach to

    physics concept instruction that involves the teaching of

    (1) definition and label of a concept, (2) example or

    examples about the concept, and (3) phenomena description or

    demonstration to a large group of students.

    Physics example-nonexample teaching method An

    approach to physics concept instruction that involves the

    use of example and nonexample after teaching of the definition of a concept in the traditional teaching method.

    Physics concept teaching method An approach to

    physics concept instruction that involves the presentation

    of a concept map designed by Novak (1984), the teaching

    technology of the general instructional model designed by

    Berlin and White (1987) , and the instructional strategy of

    the concept-teaching model designed by Tennyson and

    Cocchiarella (1986).

    Concept map A concept map is a schematic device for

    representing a set of concept meanings embedded in a

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    25/277

    9

    framework of propositions (Novak & Gowin, 1984).

    Misconception Misconception is the term commonly

    used to describe an unaccepted (but not necessarily "wrong")

    interpretation of a concept illustrated in the statement in

    which the concept is embedded (Novak & Gowin, 1984).

    Concept teaching Concept teaching contains five

    procedures: (1) present a definition, (2) provide an

    expository presentation, (3) provide attribute isolation,

    (4) provide an inquisitory practice presentation, and (5) provide a test of classification (Merrill & Tennyson, 1977).

    Concept learning Learning the meaning of a concept,

    tha t is, learning the meaning of its criteria attributes;

    includes concept formation and concept assimilation

    (Ausubel, Novak & Hanesian, 1978).

    Physics 101 Nature of Physical World An

    undergraduate physics course which is an elementary

    description of the physical world emphasizing scientific

    method and contemporary viewpoints. This course is the first

    quarter of two quarters sequence. Laboratory work and

    demonstrations are included in it.

    Physics 111 General Physics: Mechanics and Heat

    An undergraduate course in mechanics and heat for students majoring in the life sciences and in architecture. This

    course is the first of a series of three courses which

    presents major physical principle and concepts from a

    contemporary point of view. The sequence includes laboratory

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    26/277

    10

    work and demonstrations.

    Physics 131 Introductory Physics: Particles and

    Motion An undergraduate physics course that presents the

    major concepts of physics from a contemporary point of view,

    for students majoring in physical sciences, mathematics, or

    engineering. This course is the first quarter of three

    quarters sequence.

    Assumptions

    The assumptions of this study are stated as follows:

    1. The non-science students in this study, before

    attending the Physics 101 course, have at least studied

    Mathematics 075 (previously Math 102) or placement in

    mathematics course code R or higher.

    2. The non-science students over the duration of this

    study do not get any private physics tutorial except

    classroom teaching and recitation and appointments with the

    teaching professor and teaching assistant.

    3. The non-science students responses to the

    instruments in this study are a valid indication of their

    physics concepts, physics achievement, and physics attitude

    about motion.

    Delimitations

    1. The subjects of this study are college non-science

    students studying non-calculus (algebra only) physics

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    27/277

    11

    (Physics 101).

    2. The total of three sections of Physics 101 will be

    included in this study.

    3. The concept map test for assessing students'

    learning of physics concepts is related to Novak's learning

    theory. The scoring criteria of the test are from Novak's

    rules and include: (a) relationships, (b) hierarchy, (c)

    cross lines, and (d) specific examples. Also, the content of

    the concept map pretest and posttest is the same.

    4. The content of the items of the misconception test

    related to the specific physics topic in the design unit are

    concerned with position, velocity, acceleration, and graph,

    and the item sources of the misconception test are from

    research results. Also, the content of the misconception

    pretest and posttest is the same.

    5. The items of the physics achievement test are based

    on the objectives of Physics 101.

    6. The item sources of the learning attitude test are

    from the research results, students' written responses,

    classroom observation, and personal interview. The contents

    of the test are concerned with: (a) learning concepts, (b)

    misconception beliefs, (c) teaching concepts, and (d)

    concept mapping.

    Limitations

    1. The classroom teaching time of this study was

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    28/277

    12

    determined by the schedule of Physics 101.

    2. Results of this study apply only to the Physics 101

    course, not to Physics 111 and 131 series physics courses.

    3. Out-of-class physics learning is undetermined.

    4. Results of this study cannot be generalized to other

    units of study in the Physics 101 course, without further

    testing.'

    5. The tested results of any subject who misses one of

    the implemented cognitive tests will not be considered in

    this study.

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    29/277

    CHAPTER II

    LITERATURE REVIEW

    The relevant literature for this study encompasses

    seven main topics: misconception, concept definition,

    concept learning, concept map, teaching concept, concept

    teaching model, and general instruction model.

    Misconception

    Recent studies have indicated that many students

    construct their own informal theories to interpret a number

    of physics events. Those students7 intuitive ideas often

    conflict with Newtonian theory and so interfere with physics

    learning and teaching. The ideas are always labeled as

    misconceptions. Trowbridge and McDermott (1980) indicated

    that students have a set of "protoconcepts" before

    instruction which are a repertoire of procedures,

    vocabulary, associations, and analogies for interpreting

    motion in the real world. Students often fail to make connections between the protoconcepts and the concepts of

    kinematics. Some students are even remarkably persistent in

    certain preconceptions and they depend strongly upon the

    establishment of satisfactory connection between the new

    13

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    30/277

    14

    motion concepts and the protoconcepts.

    In observing a large number of college students taking

    introductory physics, Clement (1982) found that "conceptual

    primitives" which include key physics concepts and

    fundamental physics principles and models are misunderstood

    by many physics students at the qualitative level in

    addition to any difficulties that might occur with

    mathematical formulation. He explains that Newtonian ideas

    are more likely misperceived or distorted by students so as

    to fit their existing preconceptions; or they may be

    memorized separately as formulas with little or no

    connection to fundamental qualitative concepts.

    McCloskey (1983) indicates that many students have

    striking misconceptions about the motion of objects in

    apparently simple circumstances and the misconceptions are

    apparently grounded in a systematic, intuitive theory of

    motion which is not consistent with basic principles of

    Newtonian mechanics.

    Halloun and Hestenes (1985) also found that each

    student entering a first course in physics possesses a

    system of beliefs and intuitions about physical phenomena

    derived from extensive personal experience. These systems are referred to as a "common sense belief" of the physical

    world which the student uses to interpret his experience,

    including what he uses and hears in the physics course. The

    common sense beliefs about motion are generally incompatible

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    31/277

    15

    with Newtonian theory and they are very stable, and

    conventional physics instruction does not .successfully

    correct them. Halloun and Hestenes (1986) indicate that the

    common sense beliefs which are incompatible with established

    scientific theory are dismissed by most scientists, but

    students are not so easily disabused of the beliefs because

    their own beliefs are grounded in long personal experience.

    They also express that Aristotle was the first to

    systematically develop explicit formulations for the common

    sense beliefs about physical phenomena and organize them

    into a coherent conceptual system, and the beliefs systems

    of students untutored in physics are sometimes characterized

    as "Aristotelian." McClelland (1985) used "pre-Galilean

    ideas" to characterize these views of the world which differ

    from those ideas held by "scientists". Hewson (1985) indicated that students bring to the science classroom

    surprisingly extensive "student theories" or "alternative

    conceptions" about how the natural world works.

    Misconception about motion

    In learning introductory college physics, many students

    have misconceptions in studying the kinematical concepts, the first subject of classical mechanics. Trowbridge and

    McDermott (1980) found that college students with no

    previous study of physics thought of the word 'speed' as a

    relation between the distance traveled and the elapsed time

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    32/277

    16

    but not necessarily as a ratio; 'accelerate ' was used to

    indicate that an object 'speeded up'. From many exploratory

    interviews and a number of preliminary trials, Trowbridge et

    al. (1980) developed two speed comparison tasks and asked

    student to compare the simultaneous motions of two identical

    balls rol ling on parallel U channels. As a result, they

    found that failure on the speed comparison tasks was almost

    invariably due to improper use of a position criterion to

    determine relative velocity. Moreover, they also found

    evidence that some students' certain preconceptions may be

    remarkably persistent.

    About the understanding of the concept of acceleration

    among college students, Trowbridge and McDermott (1981)

    designed acceleration comparison tasks to find how students

    apply their concepts of acceleration in interpreting simple motions of real objects. They found that many students use

    different concepts as the criterion to compare accelerations

    and these concepts are position, speed, relative velocity,

    and average velocity.

    In the real world experience, it is illustrated that

    some students have the misconception in separating the

    concepts of velocity and position at a particular instant.

    Trowbridge and McDermott (1980) and Halloun and Hestenes

    (1985) reported that some students believe that if two cars,

    on the freeway, reach the same position at the same time,

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    33/277

    17

    then they must have the same speed at that instant.

    Misconception about motion graphs

    Graphing is a powerful and key symbol system for

    representation of data and scientific communication.

    McDermott, Rosenquist, and Zee (1987) point out that many

    undergraduates taking introductory physics seem to lack the

    ability to use graphs either for imparting or extracting

    information. The analysis of graphing errors identified in

    their study indicates that many are a direct consequence of

    an inability to make connections between a graphical

    representation and the subject matter it represents. For

    example, students frequently do not know whether to extract

    the desired information from the slope or the height of a

    graph, and many are unable to translate back and forth form

    a position versus time graph to a velocity versus time graph.

    Mokros and Tinker (1987) in their Microcomputer-Based

    Lab (MBL) project identified that two major types of

    graphing misconception are: (1) a strong graph-as-picture

    confusion, and (2) a weaker indication of slope/height

    confusion. White (1987) also indicates that students tend to

    confuse the picture of an event with the graph of an event.

    Implications for Instruction about Motion

    Because many undergraduate students are unable to

    discriminate between position and velocity and some students

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    34/277

    18

    seem to depend strongly upon the establishment of

    satisfactory connections between new physics concepts and

    the protoconcepts with which the student is already

    familiar, Trowbridge and McDermott (1980, 1981) suggest that

    a conscious effort should be made to try to help students

    relate physical concepts to their experience. For example,

    the relationship between the use of technical vocabulary and

    the understanding of physical concepts needs to be examined

    carefully and more attention at the introductory level might

    be devoted to the basic kinematical concepts. They also

    suggest more attention to the detailed information about

    conceptual understanding that can usefully help some college

    students overcome deficiencies in studying introductory

    college physics.

    Clement (1982) thinks that it is important to find

    teaching strategies that encourage students to articulate

    and become conscious of their own preconceptions by making

    predictions based on them and also encourage them to make

    explicit comparisons between preconceptions, accepted

    scientific explanations, and convincing empirical

    observations. Furthermore, class discussions and arguments

    between students are especially helpful in their qualitative

    understandings about the Newtonian point of view.

    Halloun and Hestenes (1985) from their diagnostic test

    results argue that a student's initial knowledge has a large

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    35/277

    19

    effect on his performance in physics, but conventional

    instruction produces comparatively small improvements in his

    basic knowledge. However, Hewson (1985) describes the use of

    a microcomputer program designed to diagnose students who

    use a position criterion for judging when two objects are

    moving with the same velocity and the program is effective

    in changing students' alternative conceptions of velocity.

    Rosenquist and McDermott (1987) have developed an

    approach to the teaching of kinematics. By means of specific

    examples, instruction based on the direct observation of

    motion can help students recognize key features of

    definitions, distinguish related concepts from one another,

    and make explicit connections among concepts, their

    graphical representation, and the real world. For example,

    Rosenquist and McDermott (1987) apply a limiting process of

    a curved position versus time graph to have a conceptual

    criterion at the limit. From the concrete experience of the

    magnified sections of a curved graph, students can deepen

    their understanding of both instantaneous velocity and the

    slope of a curved graph.

    When interpreting a graph in physics, McDermott,

    Rosenquist, and Zee (1987) express that the ability to draw and interpret graphs is perhaps one of the most important in

    the study of physics. They believe that facility with

    graphing can play a critical role in helping students deepen

    their understanding of the kinematical concepts.

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    36/277

    20

    McDermott (1990) and Trowbridge (1990) introduce the

    use of a computer software package, Graphs and Tracks, as

    one way for students to deepen their understanding by having

    a direct experience making connections between motion and

    its graphical representation.

    Concept Definition

    In science teaching and learning, concepts are always

    the important results of scientific processes. Pella (1966)

    indicates that concepts may be viewed initially as a summary

    of the essential characteristics of a group of ideas and

    facts that epitomize important common features or factors

    from a large number of ideas. Because of the comprehensive

    nature of concepts, Pella states that concepts are useful to

    the individual in order to gain some grasp of a much larger field of knowledge than he has personally experienced.

    The most fundamental meaning of concept is exhibited in

    individual behavior by responding to a class of observable

    objects or object qualities such as those implied by the

    names "color," "shape," "size," "heaviness," and so on, or

    by common objects such as "cat," "chair," "tree," and

    "house." Those concepts are concrete and they are concepts

    by observation. Then, abstract concepts can be described and

    even defined; [e.g., mass, temperature, and prime number.

    (Gagne, 1970)].

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    37/277

    Shumway (1971) suggests a concept is a partitioning of

    a class X, universal class over which the concept is

    defined, into two disjoint classes X1# positive instances,

    and X2 , negative instances of the concept. The class X is

    the union of the class X1 and the class X2 and the classes

    X1 and X2 are disjoint. To say that a student knows the

    concept over the class X is to say that given any object

    from the class X the student is able to identify the object

    as a member of the class X^ or the class X2 associated with the concept over the class X. Thus their relationships can

    follow from the above results:

    X = xi x2 , X = X - X2, and X2 = X - X ^

    According to the empirical research results by Merrill

    and Tennyson (1977), a concept can be defined as "a set of

    specific objects, symbols, or events which are grouped

    together on the basis of shared characteristics and which

    can be referenced by a particular name or symbol." Novak and

    Gowin (1984) simply define concept as a regularity in events

    or objects designated by some label. Gagne et al. (1988)

    defines "a concept is a capability that makes it possible

    for an individual to identify a stimulus as a member of a

    class having some characteristic in common, even though such stimuli may otherwise differ from each other markedly." He

    also identifies a concrete concept as an object property or

    object attribute and a defined concept as an individuals

    ability to demonstrate the meaning of some particular class

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    38/277

    22

    of objects, events, or relations.

    Concept Learning

    An important type of learning is cognitive learning,

    which is Ausubel's primary theory in cognitive processes.

    Cognitive learning results in organized storage of

    information in the learner's brain and this organized

    complex is referred to as cognitive structure. The most

    important concept in Ausubel's theory is what he describes

    as meaningful learning. Meaningful learning occurs when new

    information is linked to existing relevant concepts in the

    learner's cognitive structure. Rote learning, on the other

    hand, is also possible to learn new information with little

    or no linkage to existing elements in cognitive structure

    (Thorsland & Novak, 1974; Novak, 1976).

    In the course of meaningful learning, individuals must

    choose to relate new information to relevant concepts and

    propositions they already know. The existing relevant

    concept in cognitive structure is called the subsuming

    concept or subsumer. The linkage of new information with a

    relevant subsumer in the process of meaningful learning is

    the course of subsumption. For a period of time, the new

    information learned will no longer be dissociable from the

    subsuming concept. This case is called obliterative

    subsumption. After obliterative subsumption, the residual

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    39/277

    23

    concept remains and much of the growth that occurred during

    subsumption is retained. Therefore, the remaining concept is

    strengthened and more capable of facilitating new meaningful

    learning in the future (Novak,1976).

    As the subsumption process proceeds, existing concepts

    become more elaborated or more differentiated; that is,

    meaningful learning is a continuous process wherein new

    concepts gain greater meaning as new relationships with

    previously learned, relevant concepts are acquired. Thus,

    concepts are always being learned, modified, and made more

    explicit and more inclusive as they become progressively

    differentiated (Novak,1976; Novak & Gowin, 1984).

    In the process of learning and concept differentiation,

    conflicting meanings may arise and the process by which

    conflicting meanings between concepts are clarified is

    known as integrative reconciliation (Novak 1976, 1979; Novak

    & Gowin 1984).

    Meaningful learning incorporates new knowledge into the

    cognitive structure of our minds non-arbitrarily and

    substantively (Novak, 1979). However, each student will form

    his/her own idiosyncratic meaning for the concept, and most

    of the new concepts will be achieved through reception learning (Ausubel et al., 1978). If the reception learning

    is to be meaningful, the learner must form unique linkages

    between the concepts s/he already has and the new

    descriptions of regularities that are to be learned. Thus,

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    40/277

    24

    meaningful learning is always idiosyncratic, and in this

    sense the learner "discovers" the meaning of all concepts by

    the nonarbitrary way in which s/he learns the new concepts.

    As the study of human learning has proceeded, Gagne

    (1988) indicates that a whole set of factors that influence

    learning may be called the conditions of learning. Some of

    these conditions pertain to the stimuli that are external to

    the learner. Others are internal conditions that are sought

    within the individual learner.

    Advance Organizer

    Advance organizers are introductory material at a high

    level of abstraction, generality, and inclusiveness and they

    facilitate meaningful verbal learning and retention

    (Ausubel, 1960; Ausubel & Fitzgerald, 1961).

    Two different ways that advance organizers facilitate

    the incorporability and longevity of meaningful verbal

    material: (1) the organizers explicitly draw upon and

    mobilize whatever relevant subsumers are already established

    in the learner's cognitive structure and make them part of

    the subsuming entity, (2) the organizers at an appropriate

    level of inclusiveness provide optimal anchorage. Thus, the more unfamiliar the learning material, the more inclusive or

    highly generalized the subsumers must be in order to be as

    proximate as possible to the degree of conceptualization of

    the learning task. If appropriately relevant and proximate

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    41/277

    25

    subsuming concepts are not available, the most dependable

    way of helping retention is to introduce the appropriate

    subsumers and make them part of cognitive structure before

    the actual presentation of the learning task. The introduced

    subsumers become advance organizers for the reception of new

    material (Ausubel, 1960). Moreover, the organizer better

    enables the learners to put their background knowledge to

    effective use in structuring the unfamiliar new material

    (Ausubel & Fitzgerald, 1962).

    By using Ausubel's theory about the ideas of

    subsumption as a promising base for research formulation,

    Novak (1971) found that advance organizers can facilitate

    learning before students have the available subsumers, and

    effective instruction for meaningful reception learning

    could benefit by use of advance organizers in sequences with

    instruction. Thus, a hierarchical series of organizers would

    be planned into the instructional sequence. Novak (1976)

    also uses "cognitive bridges" to emphasize the "linking" or

    "bridging" function of "advance organizers". Short segments

    of learning material can be used to provide guidance to the

    student by establishing appropriate subsuming concepts so

    that new concepts can be assimilated in the cognitive

    structure for meaningful learning. Moreover, the key

    concepts in the new material and their subordinate or

    superordinate relationship to concepts the learner already

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    42/277

    has can be helpful as cognitive bridges.

    26

    Concept Map

    Concept mapping is a model to demonstrate the nature of

    concept learning to students (Novak, 1980, 1981; Novak,

    Gowin & Johansen, 1983). Concept maps are intended to

    represent meaningful relationships between concepts in the

    form of propositions, which are two or more concept labels

    linked by words in a semantic unit (Novak & Gowin, 1984). A

    simple method for constructing concept maps is to supply

    students with a list of related concepts and have them

    construct a map, placing the most inclusive, most general

    concept at the top and then showing successively less

    inclusive concepts at lower positions on a hierarchy. Novak

    et al. (1984) indicate that this idea of hierarchical

    structure incorporates Ausubel's concept of subsumption,

    namely that new information often is relatable to and

    subsumable under more general, more inclusive concepts. The

    hierarchical structure can also show the set of

    relationships between a concept and other concepts

    subordinate to it. If sections of a concept map are too

    general or too specific, the hierarchical structure of it indicates either misunderstanding or the need for more

    careful integration of superordinate and subordinate

    concepts.

    Concepts are always being learned, modified, and made

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    43/277

    27

    more explicit and more inclusive as they become

    progressively differentiated. Novak et al. (1984) state when

    concept maps for one topic are cross linked to concept maps

    for other related topics, progressive differentiation of

    concepts is enhanced. Because the positive emotional

    experience that derives from meaningful learning is a major

    source of sustained intrinsic motivation for learning, Novak

    et al. (1984) state that progressive differentiation of

    concepts through concept mapping can provide emotional as

    well as cognitive rewards, both in the short term and,

    especially, in the long term.

    When the learner recognizes new relationships between

    related sets of concepts or propositions, integrative

    reconciliation in the process of meaningful learning is

    occurring. Novak et al. (1984) suggest that concept maps

    that show valid cross links between sets of concepts that

    might otherwise be viewed as independent, can suggest

    learners' integrative reconciliation of concepts.

    Concept Mao and Misconception

    Because concept maps are an explicit, overt

    representation of the concepts and propositions a person

    holds, they allow teachers and students to exchange views on

    checking propositional linkage or recognizing missing

    linkages between concepts. More importantly, because concept

    maps contain externalized expressions of propositions, they

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    44/277

    28

    are effective tools for showing misconceptions.

    Misconceptions are usually signaled either by a linkage

    between two concepts that leads to a clearly false

    proposition or by a linkage that misses the key idea

    relating two or more concepts (Novak & Gowin, 1984) .

    Concept Map and Evaluation

    Concept mapping is a technique which allows the student

    to demonstrate what he or she know in a visual form. Concept

    maps help learners identify the key concepts to be learned,

    and show links between what is to be learned and what he or

    she already knows. Thus scoring systems have been designed

    for concept maps, and the basis for the systems is the

    quantification of meaningful learning relative to some

    discipline area. Credit is given not only for the number of

    valid propositions or concept linkages, but even more

    importantly, for the number of valid hierarchical levels

    (Novak & Gowin, 1984; Ridley & Novak, 1988).

    Concept Map and Teaching

    Using concept mapping techniques, in an introductory

    biology course for biology majors, Arnandin, Mintzes, Dunn,

    and Shager (1984) trained students to map, tested their pre

    instruction maps and post-instruction maps, and assessed

    their maps. They advanced the following claims made on behalf of

    concept mapping: (1) concept mapping helps students

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    45/277

    29

    understand what meaningful learning is, (2) concept mapping

    facilitates meaningful learning, (3) concept mapping is an

    effective study technique, (4) concept mapping provides a

    useful evaluation tool, and (5) concept mapping is a useful

    tool for organizing and sequencing instruction.

    Ault (1985) introduced concept mapping as a study

    strategy in an earth science course and indicated that

    concept mapping is one strategy for solving the problem of

    why students often learn so little. Judiciously used by

    either instructors planning lectures or students preparing

    for an examination, concept mapping enhances opportunities

    for meaningful learning. However, in nonsupportive settings,

    Ault indicates that students' tolerance for mapping

    exercises will fade rapidly. And, grading practices can

    thoroughly undermine the value of concept mapping by

    reinforcing rote learning. Ault cautions that students

    should not be asked to memorize instructor-prepared maps.

    Moreover, students often do not feel comfortable working

    within highly interconnected systems of thought and some

    believe they must remember information precisely in the form

    presented or be penalized. Thus the rote knowledge in novel

    contexts becomes painfully evident. Ault suggests that meaningful learning requires uncompromising commitment by

    both teachers and students to an understanding of structure

    in knowledge, and concept mapping skill leads in this

    direction.

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    46/277

    30

    In order to actively promote meaningful learning,

    Cliburn (1986, 1990) uses teacher-made concept maps as

    potential applications of the advance organizer. He uses

    whole-unit maps to represent the unit's conceptual framework

    and then forms this large-scale map. A series of more

    specific, higher-resolution maps can be drawn to show more

    detail, result ing in a nested set of conceptual maps for the

    unit. Furthermore, he makes a color-coded composite map

    joining all the individual concept maps and posts it on the

    classroom bulletin board. After the formal study of the

    concept map, Cliburn found students who were taught by using

    concept maps learned better and retained the material

    better. Thus the effectiveness of concept maps promoting

    long-term retention is strongly confirmed.

    Some Alternate Assessments of Cognitive Structure

    Shavelson (1974) presents a model of human information

    processing which can be divided into two general components:

    perception and memory. In the memory component, long-term

    memory (LTM) and a retrieval and decision process serve to

    define the cognitive structure. Two measurement methods that

    retrieve the student's representation of a subject-matter

    structure from his cognitive structure are Word-Association

    Method and Graph-Construction Method.

    In the Word-Association Method, the student's list of

    responses to each stimulus word in a test are scored through

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    47/277

    31

    their number, type (Shavelson, 1973), or overlap (Shavelson,

    1974). Garskof and Houston (1963) provided a method of the

    Relatedness Coefficient (RC) to define the relatedness of

    each two words in a set of associates. This method was used

    to judge the physics concepts in the subject matter

    (Johnson, 1967); Preece, 1976). Then the observed

    coefficients of all pairs of concepts can be analyzed by

    using factor analysis, multidimensional scaling (Kruskal,

    1964; Davison, 1983), or hierarchical cluster analysis

    (SPSS, 1990) to present the student's cognitive structure.

    Gussarsky and Gorodetsky (1988) use the method of

    constrained word associations to gain knowledge on the

    chemical equilibrium concept.

    In the Graph-Construction Method, the student is given

    a list of key words and asked to build a linear tree graph by connecting pairs of words. The distances between all

    pairs of words on the graph are analyzed by various scaling

    techniques the same as in the word-association method, in

    order to examine the student's cognit ive structure (Waern,

    1972; Shavelson, 1974).

    Diekhoff and Diekhoff (1982) state that an instructor's

    numerical judgments of all possible pairs of key concepts

    selected from a knowledge domain should be useful in

    conveying structural information to students. The

    instructor's judgments can be analyzed through principal

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    48/277

    32

    components analysis (SPSS, 1990) to translate relationship

    judgments into distance. These distances are used in

    creating a graphic array of concept-points in space, called

    a "cognitive map".

    Jonassen (1984) develops a technology, Pattern Notes,

    for analyzing and classifying the relational links between

    concepts. To construct a pattern note, a primary subject is

    first identified and written in block letters in the center

    of a blank sheet of paper, and a box drawn around it. Next,

    the student free associates about the subject, thinking

    about the key related concepts, and writes them on lines

    connecting them to the box. The number of lines between two

    concepts may be summarized in a distance matrix and then

    they are analyzed by using the method of multidimensional

    scaling. As a result, the structure of conceptual

    relationships derived by the Pattern Notes test and the Word

    Associat ion test are statistically and visually very similar

    (Jonassen, 1987).

    Teaching Concept

    Concept Classification

    Most studies of conceptual behavior have dealt mainly with the characterization of the specific concept to be

    learned. The general form of solution (rule) has been simple

    and familiar, e. g., a conjunction of attributes, and has

    been described for the learner during preliminary

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    49/277

    33

    instructions and/or practice problems. Under these

    circumstances, the learning task can be described as

    attribute identification and the rule relating the relevant

    attributes given has received little attention. Thus, a rule

    learning (RL) task based on the conceptual rules which are

    conjunction, inclusive disjunction, joint denial, and

    conditional is constructed and concerned with separable and

    unique behaviors. In the RL task, changes in rule difficulty

    are a function of the acquisition by learners of a stimulus-

    coding strategy, and the strategy reduces a large and

    potentially unlimited stimulus population to four classes:

    (a) both, (b) the first but not the second, (c) the second

    but not the first, and (d) neither of the two given relevant

    attributes. In general, these classes are referred to as TT,

    TF, FT, and FF in a bidimensional truth table. Each bidimensional rule can map the stimulus classes uniquely

    into the two categories of a concept, positive and negative

    instances. Once the strategy is mastered, learners are

    merely required to learn the assignment of these four

    classes to two response categories. Moreover, the

    acquisition of a stimulus-coding strategy could reduce

    differences in rule difficulty and produce apparent positive

    interrule transfer effects (Haygood & Bourne, 1965).

    In the study of the relationship between unknown

    relevant attributes which are conjunctive, inclusive

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    50/277

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    51/277

    35

    a concept class, and a nonexample (item) does not belong to

    the class. Then the items in a class are normally

    distributed based upon the ease in recognizing an item as

    being either an example or a nonexample.

    Example and Nonexample

    During the process of concept learning, Woolley et al.

    (1972) define three independent variables concerned with the

    distribution to the examples: probability, matching, and

    pairing. Probability levels of examples and nonexamples

    presented are determined by their ease of recognition. The

    relationships between high/low probability and examples/

    nonexamples can be found in Figure 1. That the relationship

    between examples and nonexamples is presented continuously

    is called matching; that is, an example and nonexample are

    matched when they share vir tually all of the same irrelevant

    attributes, and differ only in some relevant attributes.

    Pairing has two different types: divergent pairing refers to

    the case when two examples presented in a sequence differ as

    much as possible in their irrelevant attributes; convergent

    pairing means two sequential examples differ only slightly

    in irrelevant attributes.There are four concept learning outcomes specified in

    relationship to the independent variables. If the

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    52/277

    36

    High probability Low probability

    Examples An example would be

    recognized as belonging to a specified concept class.

    An example would be incorrectly classified as belonging to a specified concept class

    Nonexamples

    A nonexample would be easily rejected as

    belonging to a specified concept class.

    A nonexample would be incorrectly accepted as belonging to a specified concept class.

    Figure 1. Probability Levels of Examples and Nonexamples

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    53/277

    37

    respondents would correctly classify previously

    unencountered examples of all probability levels, it is the

    case of correct classification. However, when previously

    unencountered examples are correctly identified but some low

    probability nonexamples are also included as being examples

    is called overgeneralization; i. e. a respondent cannot

    discriminate between examples and nonexamples and indicates

    that the learner's range of acceptance is too great. On the

    other hand, when the range of rejection is too great is

    called undergeneralization. If the learner variously accepts

    or rejects examples and nonexamples of all probability

    levels based on their irrelevant attributes, it is

    misconception (Woolley & Tennyson, 1972).

    In investigating the relationship among examples and

    nonexamples, concept learning is most effectively

    facilitated as examples present a range from easy to

    difficult, subsequent examples are divergent in variable

    attributes (irrelevant attributes) from previous examples,

    and examples are matched to nonexamples on the basis of

    similarity of variable attributes (Tennyson, Woolley &

    Merrill, 1972; Tennyson, 1973; Tennyson & Park, 1980).

    Moreover, organized sequence of a presentation of examples is more effective than random presentation. (Tennyson, Steve

    & Boutwell, 1975).

    In a classroom study, Shumway (1971) found that

    nonexamples discouraged overgeneralization errors by eighth

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    54/277

    38

    grade students for concepts involving the properties of

    mathematical binary operations.

    In the study of the effects of an instructional

    sequence of all examples and a sequence of examples and

    nonexamples on the acquisition of the mathematical concepts

    of commutativity, accociativity, distributiv ity , and

    homomorphism, Shumway (1973, 1974) found that a sequence of

    examples and nonexamples was superior to a sequence of all

    examples for the acquisition of these concepts. The results

    of these studies are consistent with Tennyson, Woolley, and

    Merril l (1972) and Shumway (1972).

    Instructional research on concepts in mathematics

    (Shumway, 1974, 1977), Poetry (Tennyson, Woolley & Merrill,

    1972; Tennyson, Steve & Boutwell, 1975), sentence (Tennyson,

    1973), and chemistry crystal structure (Merrill & Tennyson,

    1978) suggests that mixtures of examples and nonexamples are

    favored over all examples in learning school-related

    concepts. In order to identify important variables

    influencing the role of nonexamples on logical thinking in

    conjunctive feature identification tasks, the all example

    treatment was favored over the mixed example and nonexample

    treatment with the 1:1 feature frequency condition; the

    mixed example and nonexample treatment was favored over the

    all example treatment where the frequency of one feature of

    the irrelevant dimensions was 9:1 compared to the other

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    55/277

    39

    feature (Shumway, White, Wilson & Brombacher, 1983) . By

    using Apple II microcomputer to design a program including

    graphics animated to represent chemical and physical changes

    which are often observed in qualitative chemical analysis,

    White, Wilson, and Shumway (1981) also found the same

    results.

    In addition to organizing examples and nonexamples in

    teaching concepts, additional improvements are suggested to

    include: (1) the use of attention focusing devices

    (attribute isolation) to focus the student's attention on

    the critical attributes, (2) the use of a step-by-step

    (algorithmic) presentat ion of the definition, and (3) the

    combination of expository presentations with feedback-

    accompanied practice (Merrill & Tennyson, 1978).

    As to the question of how many examples and nonexamples

    in concept teaching should be presented, Tennyson and Park

    (1980) indicated that the appropriate number of examples

    differs according to the learning characteristics of

    individual students.

    Concept Teaching Model

    In carefully controlled experimental research studies, Merrill and Tennyson (1977) express that most concepts do

    not exist in isolation but rather as part of a set of

    related concepts. Thus " a concept taxonomy is a diagram

    which is constructed to indicate the subordinate,

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    56/277

    40

    superordinate, and coordinate relationships among a set of

    related concepts." "When a superordinate concept is divided

    into subordinate concepts, the subordinate concepts for a

    single superordinate concept are called coordinate

    concepts." Using the coordinate concepts selected from

    psychology as a task wi th junior and senior high students,

    Tennyson, Tennyson and Rothen (1980) examined three

    presentation orders of sequencing examples of coordinate

    concepts: (1) Simultaneous order, all of the coordinate

    concepts were presented concurrently by grouping one example

    from each concept in a rational set. Within a rational set

    the representative examples had matched variable attributes

    and different critical attributes, while between rational

    sets the examples had divergent variable attributes; (2)

    Collective order, the coordinate concepts are clustered according to shared critical attributes. That is, certain

    critical attributes may be identical in various concepts,

    with differences between these concepts based on subordinate

    concepts; (3) Successive order, there were no groupings;

    instead, each concept was presented independently. In terms

    of concept learning, the results indicate that students

    learn generalization behavior when they are given a range of

    variable attributes between rational sets, and that they

    learn discrimination behavior when given examples of each

    concept within rational sets.

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    57/277

    41

    In the research of concept learning, Tennyson, Chao,

    and Youngers (1981) suggest that what is stored in memory is

    a bes t example (prototype or clear case) and that learners

    match newly encountered examples with that best example.

    Thus they used three presentation methods to test concept

    attainment: (1) expository, with labels and statements

    clearly identifying examples and nonexamples; (2)

    interrogatory, with questions requiring students to identify

    examples and nonexamples; and (3) expository-interrogatory,

    a combination of the first two. Data analyses showed that

    learning was facilitated for fourth-grade students by a

    presentation method that combined expository statements of

    best examples with interrogatives over presentations that

    were expository or interrogatory only.

    From the extensive research on concept learning in

    psychology and education, two processes should be involved

    in the concept learning: first, formation in the

    individual's memory of a best example and second,

    development of the skill to recognize specific attributes of

    similarity and difference between and among newly

    encountered examples. As to the teaching concept, a good

    concept lesson should include the following elements (Jassal & Tennyson, 1982):

    1. a concept definition;

    2. a best example;

    3. an expository set of examples;

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    58/277

    42

    4. an interrogatory set of examples;

    5. a classification test.

    For purposes of instructional design, the research

    presented above have shown that concept learning involves

    development of classification skills in generalization and

    discrimination and that learners can learn most effectively

    if the learners are presented with best examples of the

    concept in both expository and interrogatory presentation

    forms. In order to test (1) whether formation of conceptual

    information may be learned by an instructional presentation

    form that focuses attention on a best example or that

    clarifies the relationship of the critical attributes, and

    (2) the effect of expository examples in facilitat ing the

    transition between the encoding of conceptual information

    and the development of the classification skill, Tennyson,

    Youngers, and Suebsonthi (1983) used the concept of a

    regular polygon as a task with third-grade students. The

    results of the study show (a) that presentation of best-

    examples along with the definition facilitated prototype

    formation more than did a presentation of the definition

    along with a statement clarifying the relationship of the

    critical attributes, and (b) that classification skill was

    facilitated more by presentation of both expository and

    interrogatory examples compared to an interrogatory-only

    presenta tion .

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    59/277

    43

    Based on a programmatic line of research that has

    focused on the improvement of concept-learning through the

    enhancement of instructional design variables, Tennyson and

    Cocchiarel la (1986) present a concept-teaching model which

    extends the earlier version of a model presented by Merrill

    and Tennyson (1977). In this model, Tennyson et al. (1986)

    use current theory and research findings from instructional

    systems, cognitive science, and developmental psychology to

    indicate two cognitive processes in a concept-learning

    model: (a) formation of conceptual knowledge which is formed

    in memory by the integrated storage of meaningful dimensions

    selected from known examples and the connecting of this

    enti ty in a given domain of information; and (b) development

    of procedural knowledge which is developed by using

    conceptual knowledge to solve domain-specific problems.

    Based on this two-phase theory of concept learning, the

    concept-teaching model is composed of two fundamental

    components of design: (1) the content structure of a given

    domain of information and (2) the organization of

    instructional design variables related to the use of

    specific content structures.

    In the content structure variables of the concept-

    teaching model, Tennyson et al. (1986) suggest that they

    should be analyzed according to two conditions: (1) the

    relational structure between concepts in a given domain of

    information and (2) the variability of the attribute

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    60/277

    44

    characteristics of each concept in the domain. The

    relational structure of concepts are associated with two

    classification skills: generalization and discrimination.

    When engaging in a content analysis, successive and

    coordinate are determined as the basic relations of the

    domain's structures. With successive relationships, learning

    is limited primarily to the development of generalizations

    within a concept class. With coordination relationships,

    learning includes the development of skills to generalize

    within a concept class and discriminate between concepts.

    The second condition, attribute characteristics, affects the

    design of an instructional strategy for either coordinate or

    successive concepts. Attributes can be thought of as having

    constant or variable dimensions. When the definition of a

    concept does not change with the context in which it is learned, the concept may be considered as having constant

    attributes. On the other hand, a concept may be considered

    as having variable dimensions when its definition and

    examples tend to change with the context of instruction.

    The instructional design variables for concept-teaching

    suggested by Tennyson and Cocchiarella (1986) are directly

    related to specific cognitive processes in concept-learning.

    They are explained in the following:

    (1) Label, definition, and context. Labels and

    definitions seem to help the learner establish in memory the

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    61/277

    possible connection between existing necessary knowledge and

    the to-be-learned concepts, but definitions play a secondary

    role in concept learning because learners rarely learn well

    from only definitions and most often verbatim definitions

    are not encoded in memory. The relationship between a label

    and the concept represented by a label often in an

    unhelpful, arbitrary association established by cultural

    convention. However, concept labels can assist learners in

    conjuring up the concept in memory by relat ing the concept

    to what is already known. A concept may have one label but

    several definitions, a specific context for the presentation

    of a problem within the appropriate situation or domain can

    provide information to establish critical attributes of the

    concept.

    (2) Best examples. The best example represents an

    average, central, or prototypical form of a concept, but it

    is not necessarily a quantitative value. Because a best

    example sets up the initial encoding of conceptual

    knowledge, it should be an example that can conjure up in

    memory existing knowledge structures.

    (3) Expository examples. Expository examples provide

    the dimensionali ty or richness of conceptual knowledge. In the process of teaching the examples, learners also acquire

    the initial procedural knowledge for using the conceptual

    knowledge.

    (4) Interrogatory examples. Interrogatory examples

  • 8/11/2019 A Case Study of the Physics Enhancement Project for Two Year Colleges, Its Effects and Outcomes on the Teachin

    62/277

    46

    further enhance the development of procedural knowledge.

    (5) Attribute elaboration. This des ign variable assists

    the learner in establishing the conceptual knowledge

    structure of a given concept and its relationship in a

    schematic network. There are two devices used for attribute

    elaboration: (1) attribute prompting which is a tool to help

    learners establish the conceptual knowledge by focusing