brandon, farrington - effects of cctv on crime

27
American Academy of Political and Social Science Effects of Closed-Circuit Television on Crime Author(s): Brandon C. Welsh and David P. Farrington Source: Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 587, Assessing Systematic Evidence in Crime and Justice: Methodological Concerns and Empirical Outcomes (May, 2003), pp. 110-135 Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. in association with the American Academy of Political and Social Science Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1049950 . Accessed: 03/08/2014 07:26 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Sage Publications, Inc. and American Academy of Political and Social Science are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: kenseironin

Post on 20-Jul-2016

15 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

American Academy of Political and Social Science

Effects of Closed-Circuit Television on CrimeAuthor(s): Brandon C. Welsh and David P. FarringtonSource: Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 587, AssessingSystematic Evidence in Crime and Justice: Methodological Concerns and Empirical Outcomes(May, 2003), pp. 110-135Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. in association with the American Academy of Political andSocial ScienceStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1049950 .

Accessed: 03/08/2014 07:26

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

Sage Publications, Inc. and American Academy of Political and Social Science are collaborating with JSTORto digitize, preserve and extend access to Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 2: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

Effects of Closed-Circuit Television on

Crime

By BRANDON C. WELSH

and DAVID P. FARRINGTON

This article reports on the findings of a systematic review-incorporating meta-analytic techniques-of the available research evidence on the effects of closed- circuit television (CCTV) on crime in public space. A number of targeted and comprehensive searches of the published and unpublished literature and contacts with leading researchers produced twenty-two CCTV evalu- ations that met our criteria for inclusion in this review. CCTV had a significant desirable effect on crime, although the overall reduction in crime was a rather small 4 percent. All nine studies showing evidence of a desirable effect of CCTV on crime were carried out in the United Kingdom. Conversely, the other nine studies showing no evidence of any desirable effect of CCTV on crime included all five North American studies. CCTV was most effective in reducing crime in car parks. It had no effect on violent crimes but had a significant desir- able effect on vehicle crimes.

Keywords: closed-circuit television; situational crime prevention; surveillance; systematic review; meta-analysis

losed-circuit television (CCTV) serves

many functions and is used in both public and private settings. The prevention of personal and property crime is among its primary objec- tives in public space, and this is the focus of the

present article. As an intervention targeted at crime, CCTV is a type of situational crime pre- vention (Clarke 1995). According to Clarke and Homel's (1997) classification of situational crime prevention, CCTV is viewed as a tech-

nique of "formal surveillance." In this regard, CCTV cameras are seen to enhance or take the

place of security personnel. The mechanisms by which CCTV may pre-

vent crime are numerous. These have been

Brandon C. Welsh is an assistant professor in the Department of Criminal Justice, University of Massa- chusetts, Lowell. His research interests include the pre- vention of delinquency and crime and economic analysis of prevention programs and policies. He is an author or editor of three books, including Evidence-Based Crime Prevention with Lawrence Sherman, David Farrington, and Doris MacKenzie (2002, Routledge), and has served

DOI: 10.1177/0002716202250802

ANNALS, AAPSS, 587, May2003 110

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 3: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

articulated by Armitage, Smyth, and Pease (1999, 226-27), and the main ones are as follows:

* Caught in the act-perpetrators will be detected and possibly removed or deterred. * You've been framed-CCTV deters potential offenders who perceive an elevated risk of

apprehension. * Nosy Parker-CCTV may lead more people to feel able to frequent the surveilled places.

This will increase the extent of natural surveillance by newcomers, which may deter poten- tial offenders.

* Effective deployment-CCTV directs security personnel to ambiguous situations, which may head off their translation into crime.

* Publicity-CCTV could symbolize efforts to take crime seriously, and the perception of those efforts may energize law-abiding citizens and/or deter crime.

* Time for crime-CCTV may be perceived as reducing the time available to commit crime, preventing those crimes that require extended time and effort.

* Memory jogging-the presence of CCTV may induce people to take elementary security precautions, such as locking their car, by jogging their memory.

* Anticipated shaming-the presence of CCTV may induce people to take elementary secu- rity precautions for fear that they will be shamed by being shown on CCTV.

* Appeal to the cautious-cautious people migrate to the areas with CCTV to shop, leave their cars, and so on. Their caution and security mindedness reduce the risk.

CCTV may cause reported and actual crime to increase. For example, CCTV may encourage increased reporting to police and recording by police. The pres- ence of CCTV may give people a false sense of security and cause them to stop tak- ing precautions that they would have taken in the absence of this intervention, such as not wearing jewelry or walking in groups when out at night. It may also cause crime to be displaced to other locations, times, or victims.

In recent years, there has been a tremendous growth in the use of CCTV to pre- vent crime in public space, especially in Britain (Norris and Armstrong 1999) and,

as an editor of a special issue of the Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Sci- ence titled 'What Works in Preventing Crime: Systematic Reviews of Experimental and Quasi- Experimental Research" (2001). He received his Ph.D. in criminology from the University of Cambridge. David P. Farrington is a professor of psychological criminology at the Institute of Criminology, University of Cambridge. He has published twenty-nine books and monographs and about three hundred papers on criminological and psychological topics. He is the director of the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development and coinvestigator on the Pittsburgh Youth Study. He is also the chair of the Campbell Collaboration Crime and Justice Coordinating Group and is aformer president of the American Society of Criminology, the British Society of Criminology, and the European Association of Psychology and Law.

NOTE: The authors would like to thank Hugh Arold, Trevor Bennett, Jason Ditton, John Eck, Deborah Friedman, Lorraine Mazerolle, Sara McLafferty, David Skinns, Peter Squires, and Pierre Tremblay for providing assistance in obtaining copies of evaluation studies; Jennifer Wylie for translation services; and Gwendolyn Brandon, Martin Gill, Ross Homel, Cynthia Lum, and David Weisburd for helpful comments on previous versions of this article. This research was commissioned by the U.K. Home Office, which is publishing a more detailed report ("Crime Prevention Effects of Closed-Circuit Television: A Systematic Review") as a Home Office research study.

11

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 4: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

surprisingly to a much lesser extent, in the United States (Nieto 1997). In Britain, CCTV is the single most heavily funded non-criminal justice crime prevention measure. During the three-year period of 1999 through 2001, the British govern- ment made available ?170 million (approximately $250 million) for "CCTV schemes in town and city centres, car parks, crime hot-spots and residential areas" (Home Office Policing and Reducing Crime Unit 2001, 8). In previous years (1996 through 1998), CCTV accounted for more than three-quarters of total spending on crime prevention by the British Home Office (Koch 1998, 49).

In recent years, there has been a tremendous

growth in the use of CCTV to prevent crime in public space.

During this time, there has been much debate about the effectiveness of CCTV to prevent crime and, hence, on the wisdom of spending such large sums of money. A key issue is how far funding for CCTV in Britain has been based on high-quality scientific evidence demonstrating its efficacy in preventing crime. There is con- cern that this funding has been based partly on a handful of apparently successful schemes that were usually evaluated using simple one-group (no-control-group), before-after designs, done with varying degrees of competence (Armitage, Smyth, and Pease 1999, 226) and with varying degrees of professional independence from the Home Office (Ditton and Short 1999, 202). Recent reviews that have examined the effectiveness of CCTV against crime (Eck 1997, 2002; Phillips 1999) have also noted the need for higher quality, independent evaluation research.

This article reports on the findings of a systematic review-incorporating meta-

analytic techniques-of the available research evidence on the effects of CCTV on crime in public space.

Method

Criteriafor inclusion of evaluation studies

In selecting evaluations for inclusion in this review, we used the following criteria:

1. CCTV was the focus of the intervention. For evaluations involving one or more other interventions, only those evaluations in which CCTV was the main intervention were included. The determination of the main intervention was based on the author's identify-

112

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 5: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

ing it as such or, if the author did not do this, the importance of CCTV relative to the other interventions. For a small number of included evaluations with multiple interventions, the main intervention was not identified, but it seemed clear from the report that CCTV was the most important. It is desirable to include only evaluations wherein CCTV was the main intervention because in other cases, it is impossible to disentangle the effects of CCTV from the effects of other interventions.

2. There was an outcome measure of crime. The most relevant crime outcomes were violent and property crimes (especially vehicle crimes).

3. The evaluation design was of adequate or good methodological quality, with the mini- mum design involving before-and-after measures of crime in experimental and control areas. According to Cook and Campbell (1979), this is the minimum design that is inter- pretable. The unit of interest is the area (including carparks and underground stations).

4. There was at least one experimental area and one reasonably comparable control area. Studies involving residential, business, or commercial areas (e.g., city centers) and other public and private areas (e.g., underground stations, car parks) were eligible for inclusion.

5. The total number of crimes in each area before the intervention was at least twenty. The main measure of effect size was based on changes in crime rates between the before-and- after time periods. It was considered that a measure of change based on an N less than twenty was potentially misleading. Also, any study with less than twenty crimes before would have insufficient statistical power to detect changes in crime. The criterion of twenty is probably too low, but we were reluctant to exclude studies unless their numbers were clearly inadequate.

It is worth saying a few more words about criterion 3. Ideally, the gold standard of the randomized experiment, which is the most convincing method of evaluating crime prevention programs (Farrington 1983; Weisburd, Lum, and Petrosino 2001), would have been used. The key feature of randomized controlled trials is that the experimental and control groups are equated before the experimental intervention on all possible extraneous variables. Hence, any subsequent differ- ences between them must be attributable to the intervention. Technically, ran- domized experiments have the highest possible internal validity in unambiguously attributing an effect to a cause (Shadish, Cook, and Campbell 2002).

The randomized experiment, however, is only the most convincing method of evaluation if a sufficiently large number of units is randomly assigned to ensure that the experimental group is equivalent to the control group on all possible extrane- ous variables (within the limits of statistical fluctuation). As a rule of thumb, at least

fifty units in each category are needed. This number is relatively easy to achieve with individuals but very difficult to achieve with larger units such as areas, as in the evaluation of CCTV schemes. For larger units such as areas, the best and most fea- sible design usually involves before-and-after measures in experimental and con- trol conditions together with statistical control of extraneous variables (Farrington 1997). The use of a control condition that is comparable to the experimental condi- tion is necessary to exclude threats to internal validity.

Search strategies The following four search strategies were carried out to identify CCTV evalua-

tions meeting the criteria for inclusion in this review:

113

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 6: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

1. Searches of online databases. The following databases were searched: Criminal Justice Abstracts, National Criminal Justice Reference Service Abstracts, Sociological Abstracts, Social Science Abstracts, Educational Resources Information Clearinghouse, Govern- ment Publications Office Monthly Catalog, Psychology Information, and Public Affairs Information Service International. These databases were selected because they had the most comprehensive coverage ofcriminological, criminal justice, and social science liter- atures. They are also among the top databases recommended by the Campbell Crime and Justice Coordinating Group, and other systematic reviews of interventions in the field of crime and justice have used them (e.g., Petrosino, Turpin-Petrosino, and Finckenauer 2000; Braga 2001).

The following terms were used to search these databases: "closed circuit televi- sion," "CCTV," "cameras," "social control," "surveillance," and "formal surveil- lance." When applicable, "crime" was then added to each of these terms (e.g., "CCTV and crime") to narrow the search parameters.

2. Searches of literature reviews on the effectiveness of CCTV in preventing crime (Eck 1997, 2002; Nieto 1997; Phillips 1999; Poyner 1993).

3. Searches of bibliographies of CCTV reports. 4. Contacts with leading researchers.

Both published and unpublished reports were included in the searches. Fur- thermore, the searches were international in scope and were not limited to the

English language (one non-English-language evaluation report is included in the review). Searches 1 through 3 were completed in January 2001 and reflect material

published or known up to 31 December 2000. The search strategies resulted in the identification of forty-nine CCTV evalua-

tions. Of these, forty-six were obtained and analyzed. Of these forty-six evaluations, twenty-two met the criteria for inclusion and twenty-four did not; thus, they have been excluded from this review. These twenty-four evaluations were excluded because they did not use a control area (n = 17); they had a noncomparable control area, such as the rest of the city (n = 4); or they did not report crime data (n = 3). The

remaining three evaluations that we identified, which may or may not have met the criteria for inclusion, could not be obtained; information on the unobtainable and excluded evaluations is available from the first author.

Results

In summarizing results, the focus was on the most relevant crime outcomes for this review (i.e., total, property, and violent crime types) and comparisons between

experimental and control areas. The results of significance tests are listed, but they were rarely provided by researchers. Similarly, few effect size measures were pro- vided. The problem with significance tests is that they depend partly on sample size and partly on strength of effect. A significant result in a large sample could corre-

spond to a rather small effect size, and conversely, a large effect size in a small sam-

ple may not be statistically significant. Consequently, this article relies on measures of effect size (and associated confidence intervals) where possible.

114

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 7: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

Each of the evaluations was rated on its effectiveness in reducing crime. Each evaluation was assigned to one of the following four categories: desirable effect (marked decrease in crime), undesirable effect (marked increase in crime), null effect (evidence of no effect on crime), or uncertain effect (unclear evidence of an effect on crime).

The final category was assigned to those evaluations in which methodological problems (i.e., small numbers of crimes or contamination of control areas) con- founded the reported results to the point that the evaluation could not be assigned to one of the other three categories. It was difficult to rate those evaluations that

reported the percentage change in crime (from before to after the program was

implemented) but did not provide data on the number of crimes in the before-and- after periods. Instead of giving these evaluations a rating of uncertain effect, they were rated subjectively on the basis of the reported percentage change in crime.

CCTV had a significant desirable effect on crime, although the overall reduction in

crime was a rather small 4 percent.

We also examined the important issues of displacement of crime and diffusion of

crime-prevention benefits. Displacement is often defined as the unintended increase in targeted crimes in other locations following the introduction of a crime- reduction scheme (for a discussion of "benign" or desirable effects of displace- ment, see Barr and Pease 1990). Five different forms of displacement have been identified by Reppetto (1976): temporal (change in time), tactical (change in method), target (change in victim), territorial (change in place), and functional (change in type of crime). Diffusion of benefits is defined as the unintended decrease in crimes following a crime-reduction scheme, or the "complete reverse" of displacement (Clarke and Weisburd 1994).

City center or public housing

Thirteen evaluations were identified that met the methodological criteria for inclusion in this review and assessed the impact of CCTV on crime in the setting of a city center (n = 11) or public housing (n = 2). Three of the evaluations are

reported in Mazerolle, Hurley, and Chamlin (2000). Of the three settings, this set- ting contains the largest number of evaluations. Table 1 presents summary infor- mation on each of the thirteen evaluations.

(text continues on p. 122)

115

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 8: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

TABLE 1

CCTV EVALUATIONS IN CITY CENTERS OR PUBLIC HOUSING

Outcome Author, Measure of Publication Type and Interest Research Design Results and Date, Context of Duration of Other and and Before-After Displacement/ and Location Intervention Intervention Sample Size Intervention Data Source Time Period Diffusion

Public housing CCTV moni- E = 3 buildings, 1 toring system C = 3 buildings; (cameras in note that project lobby and had 26 high-rises elevators, with 53 apartments monitors in in each apartments); 3 months

City or town CCTV; 15 center months

E = 4 beats of central area, C = 7 remaining beats of city cen- ter; note that there are 2 other Cs, but each is less comparable to E

None

None; note that 14 of 16 cameras are in E; remaining 2 are in C

Crime (multiple offenses), victim survey

Crime (multiple offenses); police records

Before-after, experimental- control; before = 3 months, after = 3 months

Before-after, experimental control; before = 26 months, after = 15 months

E versus C: total crimes: -9.4% (32 to 29) versus -19.2% (26 to 21) (uncertain effect); displacement/diffusion not measured

E versus C (monthly average): total crimes: -21.6% (343 to 269) versus -29.7% (676 to 475), burglary: -57.5% (40 to 17, p < .05) versus -38.7% (75 to 46, p < .05), theft of vehicles: -47.1% (17 to 9, p < .05) versus -40.5% (168 to 100, p < .05), theft from vehicles: -50.0% (18 to 9, p < .05) versus -38.9% (106 to 65, p < .05) (undesirable effect); some displacement and diffusion occurred

Musheno, Levine, and Palumbo (1978), Bronxdale Houses, New York City

Brown (1995), Newcastle- upon-Tyne, England

I

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 9: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

Brown (1995), City or town Birmingham, center England

CCTV; 12 months

Sarno (1995, Town center CCTV, 12 1996), months London Borough of Sutton, England

Short and Town center CCTV, 24 Ditton (1995, months 1996) and Ditton and Short (1998, 1999), Airdrie, Scotland

E = area 1 (streets None with good coverage), Cl = area 2 (streets with partial coverage), C2 = area 4 (other streets in zone A of division F), C3 = area 5 (streets in zones B-G of division F)

E = part of Sutton None town center, C1 = rest of Sutton town center, C2 = all of

Borough of Sutton

E = 6 police beats, None C1 = rest of 6

police beats (not in camera vision), C2 = rest of police subdivision, C3 = rest of police division

Crime (total and most serious offenses), victim survey

Crime (total and selected offenses), police records

Before-after, experimental control; before = 12 months, after = 12 months

Before-after, experimental control; before = 12 months, after = 12 months

Crime Before-after, (total and experimental multiple control; before = categories), 24 months, police after = 24 months records

E versus Cl: total crimes: -4.3% (163 to 156) versus +131.6% (19 to 44), E versus C2: total crimes: -4.3% versus +130.8% (26 to 60), E versus C3: total crimes: -4.3% versus +45.5% (33 to 48) (desir- able effect); displacement occurred

E versus C1: total crimes (not including vehicle crime): -12.8% (1,655 to 1,443) versus -18% (data N/A), E versus C2: total crimes: -12.8% versus -30% (data N/A/) (undesirable effect); displacement/diffusion not measured

E versus C3: total crimes: -35% (data N/A) versus -12% (data N/A) (desirable effect); note that data were not provided to allow for comparisons of E with C1 or C2; diffusion occurred

(continued)

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 10: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

TABLE 1 (continued)

Outcome Author, Measure of Publication Type and Interest Research Design Results and Date, Context of Duration of Other and and Before-After Displacement/ and Location Intervention Intervention Sample Size Intervention Data Source Time Period Diffusion

Skinns (1998a, Town center 1998b), Doncaster, England

Squires (1998), Town center CCTV, 7 Ilford, months England

CCTV, 12 E = all or parts of months streets in vision

of cameras in commercial area, C = commercial areas of 4 adjacent townships

E = town center, C = areas adjacent to town center

"Help points" for public to contact CCTV control rooms

None

Crime (total and selected offenses), police records

Crime (total, violent, and selected offenses), police records

Before-after, experimental- control; before = 24 months, after = 24 months; note that there were 2 Es and 6 C used, and the C used here is because the authoi says it was the most comparable to E; note that this E has been used because it includes the other E

Before-after, experimental- control; before = 6

months, after = 7 months; note that 2 other Cs were used but were less likely to be compa rable to E

E versus C: total police- recorded crimes: -21.3% (5,832 to 4,591) versus +11.9% (1,789 to 2,002) (desirable effect); no displacement occurred

s

r

E versus C: total crimes: -17% (data N/A) versus

5 +9% (data N/A) (desirable effect); displacement occurred

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 11: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

CCTV, 20 E = police beats None months with CCTV,

Cl = beats having a common boundary with CCTV beats, C2 = other beats in police division

Crime (total and multiple offenses), police records

Before-after, E versus Ci: total crimes: experimental- -28% (1,805 to 1,410) control; before = versus -1% (6,242 to 6,180), 12 months, violence: -35% (117 to 87) after = 12 monthsa versus -20% (267 to 223),

vehicle crimes: -48% (375 to 253) versus -8% (1,842 to 1,706), burglary: -41% (143 to 101) versus +9% (2,208 to 2,426), E versus C2: total crimes: -28% versus +9% (1,069 to 1,175), violence: -35% versus 0% (32 to 32), vehicle crimes: -48% versus -8% (309 to 285), burglary: -41% versus +34% (366 to 555) (desirable effect); diffusion occurred

City center CCTV, 3 months

E = 1 site with CCTV, C =

1,000-foot radius BZ

None Calls for Before-after, service experimental- (weekly control; before = average), 23 months, police after = 6 months; records note that 2 other

Cs of 200- and 500-foot radii were used and are included in the 1,000-foot radius (

E versus C (weekly average): +1.8% (901 to 917) versus 0.0% (36 to 36) (null effect); little or no displacement occurred

(continued)

Town center Armitage, Smyth, and Pease (1999), Burnley, England

Mazerolle, Hurley, and Chamlin (2000), Cincinnati (Northside)

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 12: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

TABLE 1 (continued)

Outcome Author, Measure of Publication Type and Interest Research Design Results and Date, Context of Duration of Other and and Before-After Displacement/ and Location Intervention Intervention Sample Size Intervention Data Source Time Period Diffusion

Mazerolle, Hurley, and Chamlin (2000), Cincinnati (Hopkins Park)

City center/ CCTV, 3 park months

Mazerolle, City center CCTV, 2 Hurley, and months Chamlin (2000), Cincinnati (Findlay Market)

E = 1 site with CCTV, C =

1,000-foot radius BZ

E = 1 site with CCTV, C = 1,000-foot radius BZ

None

None

Calls for Before-after, service (weekly average), police records

Calls for service (weekly average), police records

experimental-con- trol; before = 23 months, after = 4 months; note that 2 other Cs of 200- and 500-foot radii were used and are included in the 1,000-foot radius C

E versus C (weekly average): +9.8% (1,062 to 1,166) versus 0.0% (22 to 22) (null effect); displacement/ diffusion not measured

Before-after, E versus C (weekly average): experimental- +16.9% (1,005 to 1,175) control; before = versus +17.1% (111 to 130) 24.5 months, (null effect); some after = 3.5 months; displacement occurred note that 2 other Cs of 200- and 500-foot radii were used and are included in the 1,000-foot radius C

]

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 13: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

?ublic CCTV, 18 E = 9 buildings housing months (1,220 apartments,

Albany project), C = number of buildings N/A (Roosevelt project)

None Crime Before-after, (total and multiple categories) inside housing projects and inside zones of 0.1- to 0.5-mile radii around projects, police records

experimental- control with matching; before = 18 months, after = 18 months

E versus C: change in total crimes inside projects: 0.0% versus -5.3%, change in total crimes inside 0.1-mile BZ: 0.0% versus -4.0%, change in major felonies inside projects: -22.8% versus -14.5%, change in major felonies inside 0.1-mile BZ: -6.4% versus -8.6% (data N/A) (null effect); displacement and diffusion did not occur

Farrington, City center CCTV, 11 Bennett, and months Welsh (2002), Cambridge, England

E = city center, C = secondary center

None Crime Before-after, (total and experimental- multiple control; before =

categories), 11 months, police after = 11 months records, also victim survey data on crime and disorder

E versus C: total crimes: -13.8% (2,600 to 2,242) versus -26.9% (1,324 to 968), violent crimes: -6.0% (151 to 142) versus -33.8% (77 to 51), vehicle crimes: -53.1% (224 to 105) versus -54.0% (250 to 115), percentage victimized: +8.0% (26.4% to 28.5%) versus +19.3% (11.4% to 13.6%) (undesirable effect); displacement/diffusion not measured

NOTE: CCTV = closed-circuit television; E = experimental area; C = control area; N/A = not available; BZ = buffer zone (area surrounding experi- mental area). a. There were an additional eight months of follow-up, but the authors reported crime data as percentage changes relative to the twelve-month before

> period, so it was not possible to accurately calculate the number of incidents for the additional eight months. t-o

Williamson I and McLafferty (2000), Brooklyn, New York

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 14: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

TABLE 2

META-ANALYSIS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION EVALUATIONS IN CITY CENTERS OR PUBLIC HOUSING

Evaluation Odds Ratio

Musheno, Levine, and Palumbo (1978), New York City 0.89 Brown (1995), Newcastle-upon-Tyne 0.90* Brown (1995), Birmingham 1.91* Skinns (1998a), Doncaster 1.42* Armitage, Smyth, and Pease (1999), Burnley 1.27* Mazerolle, Hurley, and Chamlin (2000), Cincinnati (Northside) 0.98 Mazerolle, Hurley, and Chamlin (2000), Cincinnati (Hopkins Park) 0.91 Mazerolle, Hurley, and Chamlin (2000), Cincinnati (Findlay Market) 1.00 Farrington, Bennett, and Welsh (2002), Cambridge 0.85* All nine studies 1.02 Five U.K. studies 1.04* Four U.S. studies 0.98

*p < .05.

Seven of the thirteen evaluations were carried out in England, five in the United States, and one in Scotland. On average, the follow-up period in the evaluations was 10.9 months, ranging from a low of 3 months in the evaluation by Musheno, Levine, and Palumbo (1978) to a high of 24 months in the evaluations by Short and Ditton (1995) and Skinns (1998b). Only one of the evaluations (Skinns 1998a) included interventions in addition to the main intervention of CCTV. Many of the evaluations used multiple experimental areas (e.g., police beats, apartment build-

ings), meaning that the coverage of the CCTV intervention was quite extensive in the city or town center. Multiple control areas (e.g., adjacent police beats, remain- der of city) were also used by some of the evaluations.

As shown in Table 1, the city center or public housing CCTV evaluations showed mixed results in their effectiveness in reducing crime. Five of the thirteen evalua- tions were considered to have a desirable effect on crime, while three were consid- ered to have an undesirable effect (increased crime). The remaining five evalua- tions were considered to have a null (evidence of no effect, n = 4) or uncertain (unclear evidence of an effect, n = 1) effect on crime. An equal number of schemes showed evidence of diffusion of benefits and displacement.

Table 2 presents the results of a meta-analysis of the CCTV evaluations in city centers or public housing. To carry out a meta-analysis, a comparable measure of effect size is needed in each project (Lipsey and Wilson 2001; Wilson 2001). This has to be based on the number of crimes in the experimental and control areas before and after the CCTV intervention because this is the only information that is

regularly provided in these evaluations. Here, the odds ratio is used as the measure of effect size. For example, in Doncaster, the odds of a crime after, given a crime before in the control area, were 2,002/1,780 or 1.12. The odds of a crime after, given a crime before in the experimental area, were 4,591/5,832 or 0.79. The odds

122

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 15: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

ratio therefore was 1.12/0.79 or 1.42. This was statistically highly significant (z = 9.24, p < .0001).

The odds ratio has a very simple and meaningful interpretation. It indicates the

proportional change in crime in the control area compared with the experimental area. In this example, the odds ratio of 1.42 indicates that crime increased by 42

percent in the control area compared with the experimental area. An odds ratio of 1.42 could also indicate that crime decreased by 30 percent in the experimental area compared with the control area, since the change in the experimental area

compared with the control area is the inverse of the odds ratio, or 1/1.42 here. The odds ratio could be calculated for only nine evaluations because numbers of

crimes were not reported in the Airdrie, Ilford, Brooklyn, or (for the control area) Sutton evaluations. Table 2 shows that CCTV had a significant effect on crime in five evaluations: three desirable (Birmingham, Doncaster, and Burnley) and two undesirable (Newcastle and Cambridge). CCTV had no effect on crime in the four North American evaluations.

CCTV can be most effective in reducing crime in car parks.

To produce a summary effect size in a meta-analysis, each effect size has to have a standard error. This was one reason for choosing the odds ratio, which has a known standard error. The average effect size (weighted according to the standard error of each study) was an odds ratio of 1.02, which was not statistically significant (z = 1.40, n.s.). Thus, pooling the data from the nine studies, there was no evidence that CCTV led to a reduction in crime.

The nine effect sizes were significantly variable (Q = 164.9, df = 8, p < .0001). This means that they were not randomly distributed about the average effect size. The four American studies showed a null effect on crime (odds ratio = 0.98, z = 0.79, n.s.), and they were homogeneous (Q = 0.62, df= 3, n.s.). The five U.K. stud- ies showed a small but significant effect on crime (odds ratio = 1.04, z = 2.51, p = .012), but they were significantly heterogeneous (Q = 157.5, df = 4, p < .0001).

Public transport

Four evaluations were identified that met the methodological criteria for inclu- sion in this review and assessed the impact of CCTV on crime in public transporta- tion systems. All of the evaluations were conducted in subway systems: three in the London Underground (Burrows 1979; two by Webb and Laycock 1992) and one in

123

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 16: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

the Montreal Metro (Grandmaison and Tremblay 1997). The follow-up periods ranged from a low of twelve months to a high of thirty-two months.

With the exception of the program by Grandmaison and Tremblay (1997), all of the programs involved interventions in addition to CCTV. In the program by Bur- rows (1979), notices were posted to alert people to the presence of CCTV cameras, and special police patrols were in operation prior to the installation of CCTV. (In the evaluation of this program, Burrows controlled for the effect of the police patrols by using as the before period the twelve months prior to the patrols' coming into operation. The police patrols were discontinued at the time the CCTV was implemented, so there was no direct influence of the patrols during the after period.) For the two other London Underground programs, some of the other interventions that were used included passenger alarms, kiosks to monitor CCTV, and mirrors (see Table 3). It is important to note that the two evaluations by Webb and Laycock (1992) involved the expansion rather than the introduction of CCTV services. For each of these three Underground programs, CCTV was, however, the main intervention.

Overall, CCTV programs in public transportation systems present conflicting evidence of effectiveness: two had a desirable effect, one had no effect, and one had an undesirable effect on crime. However, for the two effective programs in the London Underground, the use of other interventions makes it difficult to say with certainty that it was CCTV that produced the observed crime reductions, although in the program by Burrows (1979), CCTV was more than likely the cause. Only two of the studies measured diffusion of benefits or displacement, with one showing evidence of diffusion and the other displacement.

Table 4 shows the results of a meta-analysis of the CCTV evaluations in public transport settings. In all cases, the most comparable control area is used. The odds ratio was significant only in one case: the evaluation by Burrows (1979) (odds ratio = 2.58, z = 6.39, p < .0001). When all four odds ratios were combined, the overall odds ratio was 1.06 (z = 1.37, n.s.), corresponding to a 6 percent reduction in crimes in experimental areas compared with control areas.

All four of these evaluations provided information about the effects of CCTV on violent crimes, but the numbers of violent crimes afterward were very small in the evaluation by Burrows (1979). The Cambridge and Burnley evaluations in Table 1 also provided information on violent crimes. Combining these five evaluations (excluding the Burrows study), the overall odds ratio for the effect of CCTV on vio- lent crimes was 0.96 (z = 0.59, n.s.), meaning that CCTV had no effect on violent crimes.

Car parks

We identified five CCTV evaluations that met the criteria for inclusion and were conducted in car parks or parking lots (see Table 5). Three of the evaluations are reported in Tilley (1993). All of the programs were implemented in England between the early 1980s and the mid-1990s. The follow-up periods ranged from a

124

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 17: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

TABLE 3

CCTV EVALUATIONS IN PUBLIC TRANSPORT

Outcome Author, Measure of Publication Type and Interest Research Design Results and Date, Context of Duration of Other and and Before-After Displacement/ and Location Intervention Intervention Sample Size Intervention Data Source Time Period Diffusion

Public transport CCTV, (subway) 12 months

Public transport (subway)

CCTV (expansion of), 26 months

E = 4 stations on southern sector, C1 = 15 other stations on southern sector, C2 = 228 other

Underground stations

E = 6 stations on south end of Northern line, Cl = 6 stations on north end of line, C2 = 236 other Under- ground stations

Notices of CCTV (also special police patrols preceded CCTV)

Personal theft and robbery, BTP records

Passenger Robbery, alarms, visi- BTP ble kiosk to records monitor CCTV, mir- rors, and improved lighting

Before-after, experimental- control; before = 12 months, after = 12 months

Before-after, experimental- control; before = 46 months, after = 26 months; note that special policing was used in E stations during first 3 years (1985-1987) of before period (i.e., first 36 of 46 months of before period), and in 1988 (remaining 10 months of before period), policing activity reduced in E stations

E versus C1: robbery: -22.2% (9 to 7) versus +23.1% (13 to 16), theft: -72.8% (243 to 66) versus -26.5% (535 to 393), E versus C2: robbery: -22.2% versus +116.3% (43 to 93), theft: -72.8% versus -39.4% (4,884 to 2,962) (desirable effect); some displacement occurred

E versus C1 (monthly aver- age): -62.3% (5.3 to 2.0) versus -50.0% (7.8 to 3.9), E versus C2: -62.3% versus -12.2% (69.6 to 61.1) (desirable effect); note that for C2, Guardian Angels patrols began in May 1989 (7 months into 26 months of after period); diffusion occurred

(continued)

Burrows (1979, 1980), Under- ground, London, England

Webb and Laycock (1992), Under- ground, London, England

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 18: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

TABLE 3 (continued)

Outcome Author, Measure of Publication Type and Interest Research Design Results and Date, Context of Duration of Other and and Before-After Displacement/ and Location Intervention Intervention Sample Size Intervention Data Source Time Period Diffusion

Public transport CCTV E = 1 station, (subway) (expansion C = 1 station

of), 32 months

Public transport CCTV, 18 (subway) months

E = 13 stations, C = 52 stations

Passenger Personal alarms, theft, visible kiosk robbery, to monitor and CCTV, and assault; BTP patrols BTP

records

None Crime (total and multiple offenses), police records

Before-after, E versus C (monthly aver- experimental-control; age): robbery: +47.1% (1.7 before = 28 months, to 2.5) versus +21.4% (1.4 after = 32 months to 1.7), theft: +11.0% (31.0

to 34.4) versus -1.9% (20.8 to 20.4), assault: +29.4% (1.7 to 2.2) versus +36.4% (1.1 to 1.5) (undesirable effect); displacement/ diffusion not measured

Before-after, E versus C: total crimes: experimental-control -20.0% (905 to 724) versus with statistical analy- -18.3% (1,376 to 1,124), ses; before = robbery: -27.0% (141 to 18 months, after = 103) versus -30.8% (312 to 18 months 216), assault: -27.5% (178

to 129) versus +5.6% (233 to 246), total theft and fraud: -15.5% (388 to 328) versus -16.0% (507 to 426) (null effect); displacement/ diffusion not measured

NOTE: CCTV = closed-circuit television; E = experimental area; C = control area; BTP = British Transport Police.

0-' NcO o^

Webb and Laycock (1992), Oxford Circus station, Under- ground, London, England

Grandmaison and Tremblay (1997), Metro, Montreal, Canada

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 19: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

TABLE 4

META-ANALYSIS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION EVALUATIONS IN PUBLIC TRANSPORT OR CAR PARKS

Evaluation Odds Ratio

Public transport Burrows (1979), Underground 2.58* Webb and Laycock (1992), Underground 1.32 Webb and Laycock (1992), Underground (Oxford Circus) 0.89 Grandmaison and Tremblay (1997), Montreal 1.02 All four studies 1.06

Car parks Poyner (1991), Guildford 0.23 Tilley (1993), Hartlepool 1.78* Tilley (1993), Bradford 2.67* Tilley (1993), Coventry 1.95* Sarno (1995), Sutton 1.49* All five studies 1.70* All eighteen studies 1.04*

*p < .05.

low of ten months to a high of twenty-four months. All of the programs supple- mented CCTV with other interventions, such as improved lighting, painting, fenc-

ing, payment schemes, notices about CCTV, and security personnel. In each pro- gram, however, CCTV was the main intervention.

As shown in Table 5, four of the programs had a desirable effect, and one had an undesirable effect on vehicle crimes, which was the exclusive focus of each of the

impact evaluations. Most studies did not measure either diffusion of benefits or

displacement. Table 4 shows the results of a meta-analysis of the five CCTV evaluations in car

parks. In four cases, the odds ratios showed a significant and desirable effect of CCTV. In the other case (Poyner 1991), the effect was undesirable, but the small numbers meant that the odds ratio was not significant (z = 1.35). When all five odds ratios were combined, the overall odds ratio was 1.70 (z = 7.45, p < .0001). Thus, crime increased by 70 percent in control areas compared with experimental areas, or conversely, crime decreased by 41 percent in experimental areas compared with control areas.

All five evaluations provided information about the effects of CCTV on vehicle crimes, as did the Cambridge, Newcastle, and Burnley studies in Table 1. Com-

bining these eight evaluations, the overall odds ratio for the effect of CCTV on vehicle crimes was 1.38 (z = 7.63, p < .0001). Thus, CCTV increased vehicle crimes

by about 38 percent in control areas compared with experimental areas or con-

versely decreased vehicle crimes by about 28 percent in experimental areas com-

pared with control areas.

127

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 20: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

TABLE 5 CCTV EVALUATIONS IN CAR PARKS

Outcome Author, Measure of Publication Type and Interest Research Design Results and Date, Context of Duration of Other and and Before-After Displacement/ and Location Intervention Intervention Sample Size Intervention Data Source Time Period Diffusion

Poyner(1991), Parking lot University of Surrey, Guildford, England

Tilley (1993), Car park Hartlepool, England

CCTV, E = 1 parking lot Improved 10 months (no. 4), lighting and

C = 1 parking foliage cut lot (no. 1) back (for

both E and C, only E received CCTV)

CCTV, E = CCTV cov- Security 24 months ered car parks, officers,

C = non-CCTV notices of covered car CCTV, and parks; note that payment number of E and scheme C car parks or spaces N/A

Theft from Before-after, vehicles, pri- experimental- vate security control; before = records 24 months,

after = 10 months

Theft of and from vehicles, police records

Before-after, experimental control; before = 15 months, after = 30 months

E versus C (monthly average): theft from vehicles: -73.3% (3.0 to 0.8) versus -93.8%(1.6 to 0.1) (undesirable effect); diffusion occurred

E versus C: theft of vehicles: -59.0% (21.2 to 8.7 per quarter year) versus -16.3% (16.0 to 13.4 per quarter year), theft from vehicles: -9.4% (6.4 to 5.8 per quarter year) versus +3.1% (16.0 to 16.5 per quarter year) (desirable effect); displacement occurred

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 21: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

Tilley (1993), Car park Bradford, England

Tilley (1993), Car park Coventry, England

Sarno (1995, Car park 1996), London Borough of Sutton, England

CCTV, 12 E = 1 car park, Notices of months C1 - 2 adjacent CCTV,

car parks, C2 = adjacent street parking

CCTV, E = 3 car parks, various C = 2 car parks

CCTV, 12 E = 3 car parks months in part of Sutton

police sector, C1 = rest of Sutton sector, C2 = all of

Borough of Sutton

1

improved lighting, and painting; note that C1 received some CCTV coverage for last 4 months

Lighting, 1 painting, and fencing

Multiple (e.g., locking overnight, lighting)

Theft of and from vehicles, police records

Theft of and from vehicles, police records

/ehicle crime, police records

Before-after, E versus Ci: theft of vehicles: experimental -43.5% (23 to 13) versus control; before = +5.9% (17 to 18), theft from 12 months, vehicles: -68.8% (32 to 10) after = 12 months; versus +4.5% (22 to 23), note that a third E versus C2: theft of C is used but is vehicles: -43.5% versus less comparable +31.8% (22 to 29), theft than C1 or C2 from vehicles: -68.8% versus

+6.1% (33 to 35) (desirable effect); displacement/ diffusion not measured

Before-after, E versus C: theft of vehicles: experimental -50.5% (91 to 45) versus - control; before 53.6% (56 to 26), theft from and after = 8 vehicles: -64.4% (276 to 101) months (E) and versus -10.7% (150 to 134) 16 months (C) (desirable effect);

displacement/diffusion not measured

Before-after, E versus Cl: -57.3% (349 to experimental 149) versus -36.5% (2,367 to control; before = 1,504), E versus C2: -57.3% 12 months, versus -40.2% (6,346 to after = 12 months 3,798) (desirable effect);

displacement/diffusion not measured

NOTE: CCTV = closed-circuit television; E = experimental area; C = control area; N/A = not available.

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 22: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

FIGURE 1 CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION EVALUATIONS

Study

Bradford Underground S Coventry Birmingham Hartlepool Sutton Doncaster Underground N Burnley Montreal Cincinnati F Cincinnati N Cincinnati H Newcastle Underground C New York Cambridge Guildford Mean

0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10

0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10

Crime Up Crime Down

NOTE: Odds ratios on logarithmic scale.

Pooled meta-analysis results

Figure 1 summarizes the results of eighteen studies in a Forest graph. This shows the odds ratio for total crime measured in each study plus its 95 percent con- fidence interval. The eighteen studies are ordered according to magnitudes of their odds ratios. It can immediately be seen that half of the studies (nine out of

eighteen) showed evidence of a desirable effect of CCTV on crime, with odds ratios of 1.27 or greater (from Burnley upward). All nine studies were carried out in the United Kingdom. Conversely, the other nine studies (including Guildford) showed no evidence of any desirable effect of CCTV on crime, with odds ratios of 1.02 or less. All five North American studies were in this group. The overall odds ratio of 1.04 (95 percent confidence interval = 1.01-1.06, z = 2.97, p = .003) indi- cates a significant but small overall reduction of 4 percent in the crime rate in these

eighteen studies. The eighteen CCTV evaluation studies were significantly heterogeneous in

their effect sizes (Q = 267.9, df= 17, p < .0001). The five North American studies were homogeneous in showing no desirable effect (odds ratio = 0.99, Q = 0.33, df=

130

0

a

a

-0-

0

-6-

1--

A

--I

I-+

--,I

-IF

A

*_

*

I

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 23: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

4, n.s.). The thirteen U.K. studies showed a desirable effect (odds ratio = 1.07, 95

percent confidence interval = 1.04-1.11, z = 4.42, p < .0001), but they were signifi- cantly heterogeneous even within the three context categories of city centers/public housing, public transport, and car parks.

Discussion and Conclusion

A number of targeted and comprehensive searches of the published and unpub- lished literature and contacts with leading researchers produced twenty-two CCTV evaluations that met our criteria for inclusion in this review; twenty-four evaluations did not meet the inclusion criteria (mainly because they had no control condition) and were excluded. The criteria for inclusion called for CCTV programs that employed rigorous evaluation designs to assess effects on crime, with the mini- mum design involving before-and-after measures of crime in experimental and

comparable control areas.

Setting the threshold any higher-for example, requiring randomized experi- mental designs-was impractical because no CCTV program has been evaluated with this degree of scientific rigor. Therefore, the methodological criteria used here sought to achieve a balance between weak (e.g., simple one-group, no-control-

group, before-after designs) and strong science. Faced with a similar dilemma, Sherman and his colleagues (1998) adopted the same approach: "The report [Pre- venting Crime] takes the middle road between reaching very few conclusions with

great certainty and reaching very many conclusions with very little certainty" (p. 6). Across the studies, mixed results were found for territorial displacement of

crime and diffusion of crime-prevention benefits. To investigate these topics, the minimum design should involve one experimental area, one adjacent area, and one nonadjacent control area. If crime decreased in the experimental area, increased in the adjacent area, and stayed constant in the control area, this might be evidence of

displacement. If crime decreased in the experimental and adjacent areas and

stayed constant or increased in the control area, this might be evidence of diffusion of benefits. Very few of the included evaluations had both adjacent and nonadjacent but comparable control areas. More had an adjacent control area and the remainder of the city as another (noncomparable) control area, for example.

Advancing knowledge about the crime prevention benefits of CCTV programs should begin with attention to the methodological rigor of the evaluation designs. The use of a comparable control group by all of the twenty-two included evalua- tions went some way toward ruling out some of the major threats to internal valid- ity, such as selection, maturation, history, and instrumentation (Cook and Camp- bell 1979; Shadish, Cook, and Campbell 2002). The effect of CCTV on crime can also be investigated after controlling (e.g., in a regression equation) not only for prior crime but also for other community-level factors that influence crime, such as neighborhood poverty and poor housing. Another possible research design is to match two areas and then to choose one at random to be the experimental area. Of course, several pairs of areas would be better than only one pair.

131

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 24: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Also important in advancing knowledge about the effectiveness of CCTV in pre- venting crime is attention to methodological problems or changes to programs that take place during and after implementation. Some of these implementation issues include statistical conclusion validity (adequacy of statistical analyses), construct validity (fidelity), and statistical power (to detect change). For some of the included evaluations, small numbers of crimes made it difficult to determine whether the program had an effect on crime. It is essential to carry out statistical power analyses before embarking on evaluation studies (Cohen 1988). Few studies attempted to control for regression to the mean, which happens if an intervention is imple- mented just after an unusually high crime-rate period. A long time series of obser- vations is needed to investigate this. The contamination of control areas (i.e., by the CCTV intervention) was another, albeit less common, problem that faced the evaluations.

Advancing knowledge about the crime

prevention benefits of CCTV programs should

begin with attention to the methodological rigor of the evaluation designs.

Beyond evaluation design and implementation issues, there is also the need for

longer follow-up periods to see how far the effects persist. Of the twenty-two included schemes, four were in operation for six months or less prior to being eval- uated. This is a very short time to assess a program's impact on crime or any other outcome measure, and for these programs, the following question can be asked: Was the intervention in place long enough to provide an accurate picture of its observed effects on crime? Ideally, time series designs are needed with a long series of crime rates in experimental and control conditions before and after the introduction of CCTV. In the situational crime-prevention literature, brief follow- up periods are the norm, but "it is now recognized that more information is needed about the longer-term effects of situational prevention" (Clarke 2001, 29). Ideally, the same time periods should be used in before-and-after measures of crime.

Research is also needed to help identify the active ingredients of effective CCTV programs and the causal mechanisms linking CCTV to reductions in crime. One-third of the included programs involved interventions in addition to CCTV, and this makes it difficult to isolate the independent effects of the different compo- nents and the interactional effects of CCTV in combination with other measures. Future experiments that attempt to disentangle elements of effective programs are

132

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 25: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

needed. Also, future experiments need to measure the intensity of the CCTV dose and the dose-response relationship and need to include alternative methods of

measuring crime (surveys as well as police records). Research is also needed on the financial costs and benefits of CCTV programs.

We had hoped to be able to examine this issue, but it was not possible because only one (Skinns 1998a) of the twenty-two programs presented data on financial costs and benefits or conducted a cost-benefit analysis. Skinns (1998b) found that the criminal justice costs saved from fewer prosecutions and sentences (the benefits) were greater than the costs of running the CCTV program by more than three times, for a benefit-cost ratio of 3.5:1. Previous work (Welsh and Farrington 1999, 2000) has shown that situational crime prevention generally is an economically efficient strategy in preventing crime. It is important to measure the cost effective- ness of CCTV in preventing crime compared with or in conjunction with other alternatives and complementary measures such as improved street lighting.

The studies included in this systematic review show that CCTV can be most effective in reducing crime in car parks. Exactly what the optimal circumstances are for effective use of CCTV schemes is not entirely clear at present and needs to be established by future evaluation research. But it is interesting to note that the success of the CCTV schemes in car parks was limited to a reduction in vehicle crimes (the only crime type measured) and that all five schemes included other interventions, such as improved lighting and notices about CCTV cameras. Con-

versely, the evaluations of CCTV schemes in city centers and public housing mea- sured a much larger range of crime types, and the schemes did not involve, with one exception, other interventions. These CCTV schemes, and those focused on

public transport, had only a small effect on crime. Could it be that a package of interventions focused on a specific crime type is what made the CCTV-led schemes in car parks effective? The research evidence on the effectiveness of situational crime prevention in general is replete with such examples (e.g., for the prevention of convenience store robbery, see Hunter and Jeffery 1992).

Overall, it might be concluded that CCTV reduces crime to a small degree. In

light of the successful results, future CCTV schemes should be carefully imple- mented in different settings and should employ high-quality evaluation designs with long follow-up periods.

References

Armitage, Rachel, Graham Smyth, and Ken Pease. 1999. Burley CCTV evaluation. In Surveillance ofpublic space: CCTV, street lighting and crime prevention. Crime prevention studies, Vol. 10, edited by Kate Painter and Nick Tilley. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.

Barr, Robert, and Ken Pease. 1990. Crime placement, displacement, and deflection. In Crime andjustice: A review of research, Vol. 12, edited by Michael Tonry and Norval Morris. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Braga, Anthony A. 2001. The effects of hot spots policing on crime. Annals ofthe American Academy of Polit- ical and Social Science 578:104-25.

Brown, Ben. 1995. CCTVin town centres: Three case studies. Crime Detection and Prevention Series paper no. 68. London: Home Office.

133

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 26: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY

Burrows, John N. 1979. The impact of closed circuit television on crime in the London Underground. In Crime in public view, Home Office Research Study no. 49, edited by Patricia Mayhew, Ronald V. G. Clarke, John N. Burrows, J. Michael Hough, and Simon W. C. Winchester. London: HMSO.

.1980. Closed circuit television on the London Underground. In Designing out crime, edited by Ron- ald V. G. Clarke and Patricia Mayhew. London: HMSO.

Clarke, Ronald V. 1995. Situational crime prevention. In Building a safer society: Strategic approaches to crime prevention. Crime andjustice: A review of research, Vol. 19, edited by Michael Tonry and David P. Farrington. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

.2001. Effective crime prevention: Keeping pace with new developments. Forum on Crime and Soci- ety 1:17-33.

Clarke, Ronald V, and Ross Homel. 1997. A revised classification of situational crime prevention techniques. In Crime prevention at a crossroads, edited by Steven P. Lab. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.

Clarke, Ronald V., and David Weisburd. 1994. Diffusion of crime control benefits: Observations on the reverse of displacement. In Crime prevention studies, Vol. 2, edited by Ronald V. Clarke. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.

Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2d ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cook, Thomas D., and Donald T. Campbell. 1979. Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issuesfor field settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.

Ditton, Jason, and Emma Short. 1998. Evaluating Scotland's first town centre CCTV scheme. In Surveillance, closed circuit television and social control, edited by Clive Norris, Jade Moran, and Gary Armstrong. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.

1999. Yes, it works, no, it doesn't: Comparing the effects of open-street CCTV in two adjacent Scot- tish town centres. In Surveillance ofpublic space: CCTV, street lighting and crime prevention. Crime pre- vention studies, Vol. 10, edited by Kate Painter and Nick Tilley. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.

Eck, John E. 1997. Preventing crime at places. In Preventing crime: What works, what doesn't, what's prom- ising. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of Justice.

.2002. Preventing crime at places. In Evidence-based crime prevention, edited by Lawrence W Sherman, David P. Farrington, Brandon C. Welsh, and Doris L. MacKenzie. London: Routledge.

Farrington, David P. 1983. Randomized experiments on crime and justice. In Crime andjustice: A review of research, Vol. 4, edited by Michael Tonry and Norval Morris. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

.1997. Evaluating a community crime prevention program. Evaluation 3:157-73.

Farrington, David P., Trevor H. Bennett, and Brandon C. Welsh. 2002. Rigorous evaluations of the effects of CCTV on crime. Unpublished manuscript, University of Cambridge, UK.

Grandmaison, Rachel, and Pierre Tremblay. 1997. Evaluation des Effets de la Tel6-Surveillance sur la Criminalite Commise dans 13 Stations du M6tro de Montreal. Criminologie 30:93-110.

Home Office Policing and Reducing Crime Unit. 2001. Invitation to tender: Evaluation of CCTV initiatives.

Unpublished manuscript, London. Hunter, Ronald D., and C. Ray Jeffery. 1992. Preventing convenience store robbery through environmental

design. In Situational crime prevention: Successful case studies, edited by Ronald V. Clarke. Albany, NY: Harrow and Heston.

Koch, Brigitte C. M. 1998. The politics of crime prevention. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate. Lipsey, Mark W, and David B. Wilson. 2001. Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mazerolle, Lorraine, David C. Hurley, and Mitchell Chamlin. 2000. Social behavior in public space:An anal-

ysis of behavioral adaptations to CCTV. Unpublished manuscript, Griffith University, Queensland, Australia.

Musheno, Michael C., James P. Levine, and Denis J. Palumbo. 1978. Television surveillance and crime pre- vention: Evaluating an attempt to create defensible space in public housing. Social Science Quarterly 58:647-56.

Nieto, Marcus. 1997. Public video surveillance: Is it an effective crime prevention tool? Sacramento: Califor- nia Research Bureau, California State Library.

Norris, Clive, and Gary Armstrong. 1999. The maximum surveillance society: The rise of CCTV. Oxford, UK:

Berg.

134

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 27: Brandon, Farrington - Effects of CCTV on Crime

EFFECTS OF CLOSED-CIRCUIT TELEVISION ON CRIME

Petrosino, Anthony, Carolyn Turpin-Petrosino, and James 0. Finckenauer. 2000. Well-meaning programs can have harmful effects! Lessons from experiments of programs such as Scared Straight. Crime & Delin-

quency 46:354-79.

Phillips, Coretta. 1999. A review of CCTV evaluations: Crime reduction effects and attitudes towards its use. In Surveillance of public space: CCTV, street lighting and crime prevention. Crime prevention studies, Vol. 10, edited by Kate Painter and Nick Tilley. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.

Poyner, Barry. 1991. Situational crime prevention in two parking facilities. Security Journal 2:96-101. 1993. What works in crime prevention: An overview of evaluations. In Crime prevention studies, Vol. 1,

edited by Ronald V. Clarke. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press.

Reppetto, Thomas A. 1976. Crime prevention and the displacement phenomenon. Crime & Delinquency 22:166-77.

Saro, Christopher. 1995. Impact of CCTV on crime. In Towards a safer Sutton? Impact of closed circuit tele- vision on Sutton town centre, edited by Marjorie Bulos. London: London Borough of Sutton.

.1996. The impact of closed circuit television on crime in Sutton town centre. In Towards a safer Sutton? CCTV one year on, edited by Marjorie Bulos and Duncan Grant. London: London Borough of Sutton.

Shadish, William R., Thomas D. Cook, and Donald T. Campbell. 2002. Experimental and quasi-experimen- tal designsfor generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Sherman, Lawrence W, Denise C. Gottfredson, Doris L. MacKenzie, John E. Eck, Peter Reuter, and Shawn D. Bushway. 1998. Preventing crime: What works, what doesn't, what's promising. Research in

brief. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of Justice. Short, Emma, and Jason Ditton. 1995. Does closed circuit television prevent crime? An evaluation of the use

of CCTV surveillance cameras in Airdrie town centre. In Crime and criminaljustice researchfindings. Vol. 8. Edinburgh: Central Research Unit, Scottish Office.

. 1996. Does closed circuit television prevent crime? An evaluation of the use of CCTV surveillance cameras in Airdrie town centre. Edinburgh: Central Research Unit, Scottish Office.

Skinns, David. 1998a. Crime reduction, diffusion and displacement: Evaluating the effectiveness of CCTV. In Surveillance, closed circuit television and social control, edited by Clive Norris, Jade Moran, and

Gary Armstrong. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate. 1998b. Doncaster CCTV surveillance system: Second annual report of the independent evaluation.

Doncaster, UK: Faculty of Business and Professional Studies, Doncaster College. Squires, Peter. 1998. An evaluation of the Ilford town centre CCTVscheme. Brighton, UK: Health and Social

Policy Research Centre, University of Brighton. Tilley, Nick. 1993. Understanding car parks, crime and CCTV: Evaluation lessonsfrom safer cities. Crime

Prevention Unit Series paper no. 42. London: Home Office. Webb, Barry, and Gloria Laycock. 1992. Reducing crime on the London Underground: An evaluation of three

pilot projects. Crime Prevention Unit Series paper no. 30. London: Home Office. Weisburd, David, Cynthia M. Lum, and Anthony Petrosino. 2001. Does research design affect study out-

comes in criminal justice? Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 578:50-70. Welsh, Brandon C., and David P. Farrington. 1999. Value for money? A review of the costs and benefits ofsit-

uational crime prevention. British Journal of Criminology 39:345-68. .2000. Monetary costs and benefits of crime prevention programs. In Crime andjustice: A review of

research, Vol. 27, edited by Michael Tonry. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Williamson, Douglas, and Sara McLafferty. 2000. The effects of CCTV on crime in public housing: An appli-

cation of GIS and spatial statistics. Paper presented at the American Society of Criminology meeting, San Francisco, 15-19 November.

Wilson, David B. 2001. Meta-analytic methods for criminology. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 578:71-89.

135

This content downloaded from 164.15.128.33 on Sun, 3 Aug 2014 07:26:57 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions