disorders of skilled movement not caused by weakness

34
Apraxia 1 Apraxia Disorders of skilled movement not caused by weakness, deafferentiation (loss of sensory capacity), abnormality of tone or posture, abnormal movements, intellectual deterioration, poor comprehension, or uncooperativeness

Upload: nirmala-last

Post on 29-Nov-2014

1.570 views

Category:

Health & Medicine


0 download

DESCRIPTION

 

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 1

Apraxia

Disorders of skilled movement not caused by weakness, deafferentiation (loss of sensory capacity), abnormality of tone or posture, abnormal movements, intellectual deterioration, poor comprehension, or uncooperativeness

Page 2: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 2

Lashley’s Serial Order of Behavior Problem• The "only strictly physiological theory" to have been explicitly

formulated was associative chaining theory, in which it is postulated that each element in a series of actions provides the excitation of the next.

• But speech and piano playing is too fast.• Lashley postulated instead that the production of serial behaviour

involves the parallel activation of a set of actions, which together comprise some "chunk", so that responses are internally activated before being externally generated. This activation, in itself, does not contain the serial ordering of the actions. Superimposed on this activation is some kind of independent ordering system, a "schema for action", which selects which response, of those activated, to produce at which time. Unfortunately, Lashley was able to progress no further, writing that,

• “ ... that elements of the [sequence] are ... partially activated before the order is imposed upon them in expression suggest that some scanning mechanism must be at in regulating their temporal sequence. The real problem, however, is the nature of the selective mechanism by which the particular acts are picked out in this scanning process and to this problem I have no answer. “

Page 3: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 3

The Muscle Spindle

• Muscle spindles are found within the belly of muscles and run in parallel with the main muscle fibres. The spindle senses muscle length and changes in length. It has sensory nerve terminals whose discharge rate increases as the sensory ending is stretched. This nerve terminal is known as the ANNULOSPIRAL ending, so named because it is composed of a set of rings in a spiral configuration. These terminals (shown in blue) are wrapped around specialised muscle fibres that belongs to the muscle spindle (INTRAFUSAL FIBRES) and are quite separate from the fibres that make up the bulk of the muscle (EXTRAFUSAL FIBRES).

Page 4: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 4

Early Descriptions of Apraxia

• Steinthal (1871) called it "apraxia"

• Hughlings Jackson (1886) described

• Liepmann & Maas (1907) Because language mediated by left hemisphere

Because distal movements of left arm controlled by right motor cortex

Corpus Callosal damage disconnects language area from right motor area = patient cannot carry out motor commands with left hand.

His imitation was poor

When object was placed in his hand performance was poor

Patient was aware of poor performance

• Since he could not imitate Liepmann and Maas proposed that the left hemisphere contains "skilled motor engrams" necessary for skilled movement

Page 5: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 5

Liepmann : IDEOMOTOR APRAXIA

• Inability to perform acts to verbal commands, even though they might be performed spontaneously, or

when aroused emotionally

• An example of one of his cases:

– A man with left hemisphere stroke tried to complete commands with his left hand. Could not though it was not paralyzed (as right hand).

– Right-handed patients with right hemisphere stroke did not show apraxia with their right hands.

• Liepmann’s explanation:

– left hemisphere was the “organizer” for skilled movements

– suggested supramarginal gyrus of parietal was necessary = a patient with the corresponding section of the corpus callosum damaged also showed left hand apraxia.

• HOWEVER: there are several cases studies in the literature (Gazzaniga, Geshwind) in which patients with callosal damage cannot perform to command but can imitate.

RH motor control still present: THEREFORE = something in left hemisphere is cut off – some skilled motor organizing thing

Therefore they have both parts: control & skilled system (and it must be in left h.)

Suggesting a disconnection of verbal and visual routes

Page 6: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 6

Motor Engram

Paralyzed hand

Apraxic Hand

Therefore

Page 7: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 7

Motor Engram

Paralyzed hand

Therefore

No engram here

Page 8: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 8

Motor Engram

Paralyzed hand

Therefore

No engram here

Ipsilateral side must need the left-side engram

Page 9: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 9

In 1905 Liepmann reviewed 83 cases classified as right or left damaged on the basis of the side of hemiplegia. The syndrome of apraxia occurred in about half of the right hemiplegics with left hemisphere damage.

“A part of what the left hand can do is not a property borrowed from the right hemisphere. The right-sided hand center, which has generally learned all the more advanced activities after the left, remains throughout life in a certain dependence on the left hemisphere.” (page 34);” Our results show that event that which the left hand can do is for the most part not its own property (with respect to the right hemisphere).” (p 35.); ‘. . . the right-brained arm center remains constantly in a certain dependence on the left, and that the latter, through the mediation of the corpus callosum, is the leader of the right.” (P. 38). Cited in [Rothi, 1996 #2965]

Page 10: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 10

This early observation has been directly tested through examination of the transfer of learning from one hemisphere to another in intact subjects. [Heilman [Heilman, 1979] made the observation that if motor engrams are stored in the left hemisphere then the right hand should be better at learning new motor tasks and that tasks learned by the left hand should transfer more readily to the right hand (since the learning of a left-handed task must involve the laying out of new motor engrams in the left hemisphere). He cites two studies using either writing inverted or reversed letters [Hicks, 1974 #3083] or learning a button pressing sequence [Taylor, 1977 #3084] that support this prediction.]]

Heilman K, Van Den Abell T. 1979. Right hemisphere dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuropsychologia 17: 315-21

Page 11: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 11

1.Koskia, L., M. Iacobonia, and J.C. Mazziottaa, Deconstructing apraxia: understanding disorders of intentional movement after stroke. Current Opinion in Neurology, 2002. 15: p. 71-77.

Controversial: Ideational may be a severe form of ideomotor apraxia

Page 12: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 12

Ideational Apraxia• And, “ideational apraxia” in which the patient identifies the

wrong overall plan, despite preserving the elements. A common example is a patient who puts the match to their mouth to light with a cigarette in their hand. (large diffuse lesions and many question whether its not a severe form of ideomotor apraxia).

• Some authors suggest that “pantomiming” tasks versus “imitating” tasks separate these two disorders, ideational apraxics failing on pantomiming but not imitation, and ideomotor apraxics on both - but this is also consistent with the idea that ideomotor is just more severe.

• Assuming two syndromes - then ideational would mean a “loss of the memory engrams” but motor sequences can be imitated. In ideomotor the posterior areas (e.g. suprmarginal gyrus) are disconnected from the anterior motor control areas - thus the patient cannot pantomime or imitate.

Page 13: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 13

Motor engram damaged

Paralyzed hand

Ideational ApraxiaIdeomotor Apraxia

No imitation

Motor engram okay Diffuse

damage

Can imitate

Confused motor sequences

Page 14: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 14

ambiguity in defining ideational apraxia, & ideomotor apraxia.

Hécaen and Albert (Hécaen & Albert, 1978) define ideomotor apraxia as a disorder limited to simple, single gestures with preservation of the complex overall idea of the movement. They note the patient often uses a body part to represent the action. They define ideational apraxia as a disruption of the harmonious integration of independent actions into a coherent sequence.

Kenneth Heilman (Heilman, 1973) says that ideomotor apraxia is characterized by an inability to perform movements upon command, a little improvement in imitation, and significance improvement in actual manipulation of the object. He says that ideational apraxia is often defined by improvement in the actual handling of the object. (DeRenzi, Pieczuro, & Vignolo, 1968).

However, in this paper he “redefines” ideational apraxia. He presents three cases with lesions in the dominant parietal lobe. They all had anomic aphasia and were unable to respond at all to movement commands (they would look at their hands, appear perplexed, or make irrelevant movements. However, they performed imitations flawlessly and were able to use actual objects. (Kimura questions the cases??)

Roy and Squire (Roy & Squire, 1994) suggest that differentiating ideomotor from ideational apraxia is largely on the basis of comparing pantomiming and imitation. He states that both are impaired in ideomotor apraxia, whereas only pantomiming is impaired in ideational apraxia.

Page 15: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 15

Ideomotor Apraxia(Unilateral ideomotorapraxia)

Inaccurate pantomime of skilled movements on both sidesof body in response to verbal demand:

Incorrect but recognizable movement Partial movement = abridgement of target move Distorted movement Use of body part as object Incorrect orientation of arm, hand, or fingers for movement Substitution of verbal responseInaccurate imitation of pantomimed skilled movements onboth sides Evidenced by types of errors aboveInaccurate performance of skilled movements using objects Movement not appropriate for object Partial movement (abridgement) Incorrect orientation of arms, hands, limbs Incorrect orientation of object in space Use of body part as object

Ideational Apraxia Inaccurate sequencing of individual steps within a goal-directed sequence Confusion of sequential order of steps Omission of one or more steps Substitution of incorrect actions for one or more of the

actions in a series Inability to use a tool to act on another objectRelatively preserved performance of individuals actionswithin the series on verbal commandRelatively preserved imitation of individual actions withinthe series

Lesions:Primarily parietal,with possibleextension to frontal.Also, supplementalmotor cortex offrontal, possible tocorpus callosum,insular cortex andadjacent white matter,basal ganglia (caudatenucleus & lenticularnuclei)

Lesions:Frontal lobe, extensionto parietal & temporalLateral surface oftemporal lobeParietal lobe

Page 16: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 16

Dimensions of apraxic testing

• Motor Movements• Imitate

– meaningless - meaningful

• Follow command

– meaningless -meaningful

– with and without object

– Without object: body-as-part, or not

• Movement sequences

– meaningless-meaningful

– with and without objects

Page 17: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 17

Dimensions of apraxic testing: Constructional

• Drawing

– Imitation

– meaningful-meaningless

• Memory

– Familiar

– Unfamiliar

• Three-dimensional

– Imitation

– meaningful-meaningless

• Memory

– meaningful

• Symbolic

– Maps

– drawing, following

Page 18: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 18

MODELS OF APRAXIA: Roy’s Model

• Proposes two systems: conceptual & production

• Conceptual: knowledge for gesturing– .knowledge of objects and tools

– .knowledge of actions (hammering)

– .knowledge of serial order of actions

• Production system generates and controls movements

• The conceptual system controls the production system in a top-down fashion. (With attention at key choice points)

Roy EA, Squire PA. 1994. Neuropsychology of movement sequencing disorders and apraxia. In Neuropsychology, ed. DW Zaidel, pp. 183-218. New York: Academic Press

Page 19: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 19

Recognition impairments:• e.g. Humphreys: patient could accurately pantomime a gesture

with a particular object to a verbal command -- but not to the actual object. Suggests dissociation between object and action. [Why is this not a visual agnosia???]

• Ochipa, Rothi, & Heliman: patient could respond to verbal instruction but not imitate gesture displayed by examiner.

• A series of studies of gesture recognition. e.g. identify target gesture among set of gestures (video or model)

anterior apraxic subjects could identify correct gesture however presented.

posterior apraxic subjects could not. Heilman argued posterior apraxia = disruption of visuokinetic memory engrams.

• Apraxic patients do not recognize body as part pantomimes as incorrect. Since they did recognize spatially incorrect pantomimes as incorrect Roy suggests that these are part of production system but part-as-body are part of conceptual.

Page 20: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 20

Comprehension and apraxia:

• Debate between correlation or not between language dysfunction and apraxia and comprehension and apraxia.

• What does it mean that the worse apraxia is seen in the worse aphasic patients?

Page 21: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 21

Heilman, Kenneth, Coyle, John M., Gonyea, Edward, & Geshwind, Norman. (1973). Apraxia and agraphia in a left-hander. Brain, 96, 21-28. ---

• Previous reports of left-handers losing right hand writing ability without language loss, and of more cases of pure dysgraphia among left-handers.

• This case of a left-hander with left hemiplegia who lost right writing ability and has right apraxia. Brain scan & EEG suggest right hemisphere damage, probably middle.

• Left-hander - but wrote with right hand• left hemiparesis, slurred speech, but correct grammar & word

choice• first confused, then later alert and cooperative.• Stereognosis, two-point - grossly impaired on left• remote memory good, short-term poor• normal language with poor writing spontaneous or to dictation,

with improvement for copying. Could spell words out loud, could identify words spelt to him. Typing poor because left keyboard neglected.

• No buccofacial apraxia. Apraxia of right arm.

OR, Creative Diagram Making

1010

Page 22: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 22

Paralyzed ApraxicLanguage OK

Page 23: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 23

Heilman, Kenneth, Coyle, John M., Gonyea, Edward, & Geshwind, Norman. (1973). Apraxia and agraphia in a

left-hander. Brain, 96, 21-28.

• Cites cases of unilateral apraxia and agraphia with normal language with disconnection of cerebral hemispheres. But these cases were in left hand with inability to name objects in the left hand.

• And since the lab tests indicate right middle cerebral, how could this patient have right dominance for language and not be aphasic.

• Assumption is that in right-handers writing is controlled by the left hemisphere via the corpus callosum. But it needs both linguistic control and complex motor control. These are usually in the same hemisphere in right-handers and are more frequently housed in different hemispheres in left-handers.

Page 24: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 24

Heilman, M. Kenneth, John Coyle, Edward Gonyea & Norman Geshwind. (1973). Apraxia and agraphia in a left hander. Brain, 96, 21-28.

• “. . .Account for the presence of agraphia in the hand opposite a hemisphere which still serves speech, following a lesion in the hemisphere ipsilateral to that hand.”

• Apraxia & agraphia on the right• Case 1: Callosal lesion = right

sided agraphia and apraxia• Left hander with Right

Dominance for both handedness and speech.

• Left hand writing requires no callosal activity

SPEECHSPEECH

Skilled Skilled EngramsEngrams

Motor Motor ControlControl

Motor Motor ControlControl

Page 25: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 25

Heilman, M. Kenneth, John Coyle, Edward Gonyea & Norman Geshwind. (1973).

Apraxia and agraphia in a left hander. Brain, 96, 21-28.

• They discuss the Poeck & Kerschensteiner (1971) case

• Patient is Right Dominant for both speech and motor skills but writes with the right hand.

• So left hemisphere controls right hand via the corpus callosum

• Callosal lesion =• apraxic & agraphic with right

hand• Left paresis? Damage to

callosum at site?• Why not second lesion on left

parietal? No other Gerstmann symptoms

SPEECHSPEECH

Skilled Skilled EngramsEngrams

Motor Motor ControlControl

Motor Motor ControlControl

Paresis

Page 26: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 26

Poeck, K. & Kerschensteiner, M. (1971). Ideomotor

apraxia following right-sided cerebral lesion • In which a right sided lesion led to a left agraphia and

apraxia, but with aphasia, suggesting a right dominance for language.

• Don’t believe that the patient had a left parietal lesion and only part of the Gerstmann syndrome.

• This patient was not confused.

• Liepmann (1908) proposed that manual dominance is a reflection of one hemisphere’s ability to learn motor tasks more rapidly than the other. Damage to the corpus callosum, or the area where the fibers originate in the dominant hemisphere the patient fails to perform movements with the minor hand. Wyke (1971): patients with left hemisphere lesions do more poorly on bimanual tasks than do those with right hemisphere lesions.

• Writing with the left-hand, in right-handers, is under control of the left hemisphere via the corpus callosum.

Page 27: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 27

Poeck, K. & Kerschensteiner, M. (1971). Ideomotor apraxia following right-sided cerebral

lesion• So: For some left-handers with right dominance for handedness and right

dominance for speech = left hand writing requires no callosal activity. A callosal lesion would produce right-sided agraphia and apraxia.

• If the patient is right brained for speech and complex motor skills but writes with the right hand = then the left hemisphere controls the right hand via the corpus callosum. This patient, with a callosal lesion, would be apraxic and agraphic with the right hand but would be able to write with the left hand.

• If the patient is left-brained for speech and right-brained for motor skills and writes with the right hand = then the writing requires crossing the corpus callosum twice. He must relay linguistic information from left to right to arouse the appropriate motor sequence. Then he must relay the motor sequence back to the left hemisphere which controls the right hand. A callosal lesion in this patient = right apraxic and agraphic (disconnection of motor-dominant hemisphere), agraphic in left hand because of disconnection from speech-dominant hemisphere. Apraxic to verbal commands in the left hand, with preservation of object handling and imitation.

• In pathological left-handers the shift may not be complete, complicated the possible scenarios.

• Notes that Hecaen and Sauguet (1971) found apraxia occasionally after left-sided lesions and none after right-sided. But in cases such as this one the left-hander improves (improvement in apraxia) over time: left handers may show more improvement in apraxia, (also for aphasia).

Page 28: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 28

Heilman, M. Kenneth, John Coyle, Edward Gonyea & Norman Geshwind. (1973). Apraxia and agraphia in a left hander. Brain, 96, 21-

28.

• BACK TO THIS CASE:• Patient is left dominant for

speech and right dominant for motor skills and writes with the right hand.

• Then writing requires crossing the corpus callosum twice.

• Must relay linguistic information from left to arouse the appropriate motor right to sequence.

• Must relay the motor sequence back to left hemisphere which controls right hand.

• Right hemisphere lesion destroying R motor engrams

SPEECHSPEECH

Skilled Skilled EngramsEngrams

Motor Motor ControlControl

Paresis

Motor Motor ControlControl

Page 29: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 29

Heilman, Kenneth, Gonyea, Edward, & Geshwind, Norman. (1974). Apraxia and Agraphia in a right-hander. Cortex, 10(3), 284-288.

• Usually agraphia is reported with left hemisphere aphasia or Gerstmann’s syndrome. This is a case of a right-handed man with agraphia and apraxia - but not aphasia.

• Right hemiparesis, right homonymous hemianopsia, right-sided facial weakness. Neglect of visual, auditory, somesthetic stimuli to RIGHT side of space. Depressed with frequent crying spells.

• Reading = neglect of right side of space, could read sentence if moved to left visual field.

• Spontaneous writing bad, writing to dictation not much better, copying poor, better on the left.

• Movements, such as show how you use a key, were accompanied by adequate verbal description (“You put it into the lock, turn it a quarter-turn to the left, and open the door.”) but he raised his fist in the air and performed poor representations of these movements.

• Poor left middle cerebral artery perfusion and left internal carotid occlusion.

Page 30: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 30

Heilman, Kenneth, Gonyea, Edward, & Geshwind, Norman. (1974). Apraxia and Agraphia in a right-

hander. Cortex, 10(3), 284-288.• Notes previous cases (Liepmann and Maas, 1907; Geshwind & Kaplan,

1962) of unilateral left apraxia and agraphia without aphasia with damage to midline corpus callosum. They also show poor naming of objects held with the left hand.

• Notes Ajuriaguerra & Heacaen (1960) described patients who wrote on only one side of page and failed to form straight lines = called them spatial dysgraphics. But they could produce – this patient is less able to write than reflected in a spatial dysfunction.

• Chedru & Geshwind (1972). Described agraphics with little language disorder, but confusional states.

• This patient was not confused. Other components of Gerstmann’s syndrome not present.

• Previous case (Heilman, 1973) reported a left-handed patient with left hemiplegia, agraphia and apraxia in right hand, and no aphasia. They concluded left hemisphere speech and right hemisphere dominance for handedness,…crossed aphasia in left-handers.

• In this case they argue for dominance for engrams for complex motor acts in the left hemisphere and speech dominance in the right hemisphere.

Page 31: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 31

SPEECHSPEECH

Skilled Skilled EngramsEngrams

Right-Sided Neglect

Heilman, Kenneth, Gonyea, Edward, & Geshwind, Norman. (1974). Apraxia and Agraphia in a right-

hander. Cortex, 10(3), 284-288.

?

Page 32: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 32

Anatomical Considerations in the Motor Apraxias

• Gazzaniga, Bogen and Sperry (1967) found a wide variation in apraxic symptoms in nine patients with division of the cerebral commissures. However, they found that the hemispheres were able to exert voluntary motor control over both the homolateral and contralateral limbs. They suggest that the variations in reports of dyspraxia following callosal lesions may be due to differences in extracommissural damage, diaschisis, and so on. This is not a new claim. Kurt Goldstein had earlier objected to the “disconnectionist” models of apraxia and proposed that less localized disturbances accounted for the disorder.

Page 33: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 33

Kertesz, A. and J. Ferro (1984). “Lesion size and location in ideomotor apraxia.” Brain 107: 921-933.

• used CT scans to assess lesion size and location and the severity of apraxia in 177 acute and chronic right-handed left-hemisphere stroke patients.

– positive correlation between overall severity of apraxia and the size of the lesion. – In eight of nine cases of severe apraxia with small lesions the damage were

primarily subcortical in the frontal lobes. None of the lesions extended to the basal ganglia. Generally they were located in the white matter near the lateral ventricles (periventricular white matter). These patients also had severe nonfluent aphasia.

– A single case had damage in the left inferior temporo-occipital and splenial region causing moderate Wernicke’s aphasia, alexia with agraphia, and bilateral ideomotor apraxia. They note this is an unusual finding since neither damage to the splenium nor damage this far posterior are associated with apraxia. They suggest that the CT scan may not have picked up more extensive areas of damage.

– Nine acute patients and 14 chronic patients had large left hemisphere lesions without apraxia. Generally these patients had large frontocentral cortico-subcortical lesions and some also had inferior parietal lobe lesions. Eight out of 16 of these patients had larger left frontal lobes and larger right occipital lobes which is the reverse of the typical pattern of asymmetry seen in right-handers.

• Thus damage to the periventricular white matter, which could disrupt fronto-parietal connections and the occipital-frontal fasciculus, and the superior longitudinal fasciculus (which Geschwind, 1965, implicated in performing movements to verbal command). However, the superior longitudinal fasciculus was spared in some of the patients.

Page 34: Disorders Of Skilled Movement Not Caused By Weakness

Apraxia 34

Kertesz, A. and J. Ferro (1984). “Lesion size and location in ideomotor apraxia.” Brain 107: 921-933.

• Disrupting the parietal and frontal areas corresponds to theories proposed by both Liepmann and Geschwind; namely, it interrupts the visuo-kinesthetic patterns from reaching the motor programming areas of the frontal lobes. They propose that small lesions can disrupt the integration of information from a number of these critical pathways when they occur near the anterior fronto-parietal area subcortically. Alternatively, more posterior lesions would have to cover a wider area in order to create the same disturbance.

• They noted, however, that in some cases large left hemisphere lesions including the parietal lobe and/or the fronto-parietal connections failed to produce apraxia. They suggest that bihemispheric representation of function is a probable explanation for these cases. The observation that fifty-percent of these patients had unusual asymmetry in frontal and occipital lobe size supports this possibility. Though they caution that this asymmetry was also observed in two of the apraxic patients with small lesions.