effects of stress and balance of options on decision...

32
Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Master Thesis Effects of stress and balance of options on decision-making and associated physiological responses in laying hens Mia Persson LiTH-IFM-A-Ex--12/2634--SE Supervisor: Christine Nicol, Bristol University; Per Jensen, Linköping Univesrity Examiner: Dominic Wright, Linköping University Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Linköpings universitet SE-581 83 Linköping, Sweden

Upload: others

Post on 11-Oct-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology

Master Thesis

Effects of stress and balance of options on

decision-making and associated physiological

responses in laying hens

Mia Persson

LiTH-IFM-A-Ex--12/2634--SE

Supervisor: Christine Nicol, Bristol University; Per Jensen,

Linköping Univesrity

Examiner: Dominic Wright, Linköping University

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology

Linköpings universitet

SE-581 83 Linköping, Sweden

Page 2: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

Rapporttyp

Report category

Examensarbete

D-uppsats

Språk/Language

Engelska/English

Titel/Title:

Effects of stress and balance of options on decision-making and associated physiological responses in

laying hens

Författare/Author:

Mia Persson

Sammanfattning/Abstract:

Animal preferences in choice tests have frequently been used within animal welfare research to make

recommendations about animal handling and husbandry. It is therefore important that these results are

obtained in a way that, as far as possible, respects the behavioural capabilities of the animal. Stress has

been shown to affect cognitive processes in animals and could therefore affect the decision making

process. To examine the effects of stress on decision making, 16 laying hens were trained to distinguish

between two different quantities of a food reward. A decision balance point was found, by increasing the

cost of reaching the large reward, in lines with the theory of demand curves. Hens were then tested in a t-

maze choice test with both balanced and unbalanced sets of options, with and without prior stress

treatment. Choice, latency to choose, heart rate and temperatures were recorded. Hens that received stress

treatment prior to their first test session were affected by this even in subsequent sessions where they did

not receive stress treatment. This effect was not found in hens that first received stress treatment prior to

their second test session. This shows the influence of previous experiences on animal decision making.

Also, a decrease in heart rate during the decision making period was found, when making a choice

between balanced options, indicating anticipation of difficulty. Additionally, this shows that physiological

measurements such as heart rate could be of importance for future studies and greater understanding of

underlying processes of animal decision making.

ISBN

LITH-IFM-A-EX—12/2634—SE

__________________________________________________

ISRN

__________________________________________________

Serietitel och serienummer ISSN

Title of series, numbering

Handledare/Supervisor Per Jensen

Ort/Location: Linköping

Nyckelord/Keyword:

Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door, stress, t-maze

Datum/Date

2012-05-25

URL för elektronisk version

Institutionen för fysik, kemi och biologi

Department of Physics, Chemistry and

Biology

Avdelningen för biologi

Instutitionen för fysik och mätteknik

Page 3: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

Contents 1 Abstract ................................................................................................... 1

2 Introduction ............................................................................................. 1

3 Materials and Methods ............................................................................ 6

3.1 Animals, Housing and Husbandry ..................................................... 6

3.2 Habituation, training and defining individual decision difficulties ..... 7

3.3 Experimental Setup ........................................................................... 8

3.3.1 T-maze choice test ....................................................................... 9

3.3.2 Stress Treatment ........................................................................ 10

3.4 Data Collection................................................................................ 11

3.4.1 Choice ....................................................................................... 11

3.4.2 Latency...................................................................................... 11

3.4.3 Heart Rate Monitoring ............................................................... 11

3.4.4 Thermal Imaging ....................................................................... 13

3.5 Ethical Note .................................................................................... 13

3.6 Statistical Methods .......................................................................... 13

4 Results................................................................................................... 13

4.1 Choice ............................................................................................. 14

4.2 Latency............................................................................................ 16

4.3 Heart Rate ....................................................................................... 17

4.4 Thermal Images ............................................................................... 18

5 Discussion ............................................................................................. 18

6 Acknowledgements ............................................................................... 22

7 References ............................................................................................. 22

Appendix 1 ............................................................................................... 25

Appendix 2 ............................................................................................... 27

Appendix 3 ............................................................................................... 28

Page 4: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

1

1 Abstract

Animal preferences in choice tests have frequently been used within animal

welfare research to make recommendations about animal handling and

husbandry. It is therefore important that these results are obtained in a way that,

as far as possible, respects the behavioural capabilities of the animal. Stress has

been shown to affect cognitive processes and could therefore affect the decision

making process. To examine the effects of stress on decision making, 16 laying

hens were trained to distinguish between two different quantities of a food

reward. A decision balance point was found, by increasing the cost of reaching

the large reward, in lines with the theory of demand curves. Hens were then

tested in a t-maze choice test with both balanced and unbalanced sets of options,

with and without prior stress treatment. Choice, latency to choose, heart rate and

temperatures were recorded. Hens that received stress treatment prior to their

first test session were affected by this even in subsequent sessions where they

did not receive stress treatment. This effect was not found in hens that first

received stress treatment prior to their second test session. This shows the

influence of previous experiences on animal decision making. Also, a decrease

in heart rate during the decision making period was found, when making a

choice between balanced options, indicating anticipation of difficulty.

Additionally, this shows that physiological measurements such as heart rate

could be of importance for future studies and a greater understanding of the

underlying processes of animal decision making.

1.1 Keywords Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door, stress, t-maze.

2 Introduction

Decision making in animals has been described as a process whereby an animal

chooses one specific behavioural response from a set of potential alternatives

(Dill, 1987). Animals make decisions on a daily basis in a wide range of

contexts including when and where to feed, sleep or court or when to engage in

activities which could potentially expose them to a predation risk (McFarland,

1977; Martel and Boivin, 2011). These decisions are very likely to influence the

animals’ life span and fitness (Blumstein and Bouskila, 1996).

Understanding the process of animal decision making is invaluable in

behavioural ecology, to understand and in some cases even predict how animals

may respond to environmental changes (Blumstein and Bouskila, 1996).

Behavioural ecology models describe how an animal should choose to

maximally utilize e.g. energy resources (McNamara and Houston, 1987).

However, when studying animal decision making, inconsistencies can be seen

where animals do not always make the rational decisions predicted by the

model. This occurs even though the animal is considered to be able to evaluate

Page 5: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

2

trade-offs and make a rational decision (Blumstein and Bouskila, 1996; Schuck-

Paim et al. 2004). It is important to take in consideration that animals’

motivational priorities are not fixed and can therefore result in behavioural

flexibility shown as inconsistency in their choices (Bateson, 2004; Browne et al.,

2010). Motivational priorities and preferences may vary with species, breeds and

sexes. Even within the same sex of the same species, preferences can change

with the time of the year, experiences, age and reproductive state. Also, other

factors like energy and time budgets, and if the animal is stressed or not, can

affect motivational priorities, especially if the costs associated with acquiring a

resource is altered e.g. the positioning of the cost or if the length of time

accessing the resource is increased (Warburton and Nicol, 1998). Availability of

cues (e.g. visual, tactile, odours) can also have an effect on preferences in

animal choice tests. In other words, there are many factors that can affect

animals’ choices and therefore need to be taken into account when studying

animal decision making.

To better understand how fluctuation in motivational priorities can affect

decision making, a model like the one presented by Blumstein and Bouskila

(1996) can be used. They use the term assessment, defined as the process when

animals evaluate perceived stimuli and convert these into what they call an

informational state. In other words, assessment is the process of acquiring the

information. What is important to remember is that assessment results in an

informational state. Decision making is what follows the process of assessment.

An animal evaluates the informational state according to its current state as

explained earlier (age, experience, etc.) and follows up with a behavioural

response that we see as the decision. The process of decision-making involves

evaluating trade-offs where the animal weighs benefits and costs against each

other to, possibly, make a rational decision which we can record as the

behavioural response. When two animals react with different behavioural

responses to the same stimuli, the model presents us with two different possible

explanations. Either the animals are in two different states and therefore assess

the stimuli in two different ways and end up at different informational states

from the same stimuli. The second explanation is that the animals perceive the

stimuli in a similar way and end up with the same informational state but for

some other reason make different decisions, resulting in two different

behavioural responses. So far there is no way to indicate which one of these two

explanations is more or less relevant because there is no method for empirically

measuring the informational state of an animal. Instead, animals have been

treated as a “black box” where we put in a stimulus and the outcome is a

behavioural response. However, these processes of assessment of informational

states underlying decision making in animals may be the key mechanisms that

we need to understand and predict behavioural responses. It has, however, been

discussed whether physiological measurements may be the way to evaluate the

Page 6: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

3

process of assessment and the informational state in animals (Bechara and

Damasio, 2005) but so far no studies have been able to show this.

It is believed that choices made by domestic animals are affected by the

individuals’ subjective experiences of suffering or pleasure (Dawkins, 1990).

Therefore, animal decision making can be used to gather important information

about animal welfare (Browne et al., 2010). Choice tests are commonly used in

animal welfare research for evaluating animal preferences (Bateson, 2004).

Choice tests can involve two or more options between e.g. different

environmental conditions, food items or the access to perform certain

behaviours. An option is considered to be preferred by the animal if it chooses it

more often, spends more time with it or approaches it with a shorter latency than

the other options (Brown et al., 2011). The strength of the behavioural

preference for a specific option is thought to show the strength of the animals’

motivation. When an animal cannot perform a behaviour that it is motivated to

do, suffering may occur, affecting animal welfare. Results from preference tests

can be used to make recommendations about animal handling and husbandry

(Bateson, 2004; Fraser and Nicol, 2011). It is therefore essential that these

results are interpreted in a way that, as accurately as possible, reflects the value

of the option for the animal from a welfare point of view.

Since choice tasks and preference tests are popular methods of evaluating

animal preferences and welfare it is important to know which factors that may

influence the outcome of these tests. The disruptive effect of stressors on

cognitive processes, such as decision-making, is an important research area as

this may lead to husbandry and welfare problems (Mendl, 1999). If housing and

husbandry cause stress to laboratory, farmed or zoo animals, this may impair

their cognitive capabilities such as memory and learning. Stress has previously

been shown to affect several cognitive functions where the effect on learning

and memory are the most studied topics in both animals and humans (Graham,

Yoon and Kim, 2010; McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995). A commonly encountered

explanatory model of the link between stress and cognition is the Yerkes-

Dodson law (Mendl, 1999). The Yerkes-Dodson law explains the general

relationship between an individual’s state of arousal or stress and cognitive task

performance as an inverted U-shaped curve. According to this law, a certain

amount of stress or arousal could be helpful in solving a cognitive task while too

high levels of stress or arousal could have the opposite effect. This biphasic

effect is thought to be partly triggered by the two different types of receptors for

the glucocorticoid corticosterone produced as a part of the stress response

(Gross, Siegel and DuBose, 1980; McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995). Type I

receptors have high affinity and react to the everyday fluctuations of plasma

corticosterone while the Type II receptors with lower affinity only react to stress

Page 7: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

4

levels. The corticosterone peak in laying hens, after being exposed to a stressor,

has been shown to occur after 15 minutes (Fraisse and Cockrem, 2006).

There is no definite definition of stress and different scientists use different

explanations (Moberg, 2000; von Borell, 2001). Some authors distinguish

between good or bad stress, some focus on mental stress while others use a

broader definition saying that stress is the activation of the Hypothalamic

Pituitary Adrenal – axis (HPA-axis). I will use the latter, broader, definition of

stress due to the difficulty of segregating stress and arousal since the

physiological response is very similar. Therefore, a stressor is defined as

something causing the activation of the HPA-axis in an animal. In addition to

the problem of defining stress it can also be difficult to measure since many of

the physiological stress responses that are recorded also occur in other situations

(von Borell, 2001). For example, an increase in heart rate could be the

autonomic stress response of the cardiovascular system or it could be the result

of an increase in metabolic rate due to e.g. exercise (Broom and Johnson, 1993).

When studying the stress response it is therefore important to use several

measurements.

One way in which stress can affect decision-making in animals is by its effect on

the animals’ attention (Mendl, 1999). Stress has been shown to cause attention

narrowing, shifts and lapses e.g. in a detour test in chicks, random responses are

observed due to stress caused by social isolation (Regolin et al., 1995). A

possible explanation for this random behaviour is that the stress causes a shift in

attention from the task which results in poor performance. Attention narrowing

or shifting can lead to decision-making based on inadequate information

(Mendl, 1999). This is thought to be due to the state of stress or arousal causing

a speed-accuracy trade off, where an increase in response speed results in a

decision being made before all relevant information has been processed.

Another possible effect of stress on decision making is that it may cause a shift

in the animals’ motivational priorities which could result in it preferring another

option than if it were not stressed. Thus far, few studies have focused on the

effect of stress on decision-making in animals and most studies involving choice

tests use the “black box” concept, only studying behaviour and not physiology.

To investigate the effects of stress on decision-making we can use an

experimental model consisting of two different types of decisions that here will

be referred to as “easy” and “difficult” decisions. Easy and difficult decisions

can be defined in several different ways. For example, a difficult decision can be

one which involves a critical outcome, requires processing a large amount of

information where the options are finely balanced. For the present study a

difficult decision was defined as a choice where the options were finely

balanced and, correspondingly, an easy decision where the options were

Page 8: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

5

unbalanced (Figure 1). As long as the two resources being compared satisfy the

same need, the substitutability can be evaluated by measuring the cross-point

between the two demand curves (Sørensen et al. 2004). This study was carried

out alongside another which aimed to develop a method for defining decision

difficulty by finding the empirical cross point between two demand curves in

laying hens.

In addition to this, physiological responses were studied by measuring heart rate

(HR) and temperatures. An increase in heart rate (HR) can be an autonomic

response to stress (Moberg, 2000). HR can increase when the level of physical

activity increases or when the metabolic rate increases (von Borell, 2001).

Incidentally, HR can also increase or decrease before the action occurs as an

emotional response where the sympathetic nervous system causes an increase

(tachycardia) and the parasympathetic nervous system causes a decrease

(bradycardia) (Broom and Johnson, 1993). Additionally, a depression in

behaviour can be seen when animals fail to cope with a stressor which can be

accompanied with a decrease in HR (Henry, 1993). Furthermore, core body

temperature can increase as a result of stress, while peripheral body temperature

decreases (Broom and Johnson, 1993). This response is called stress-induced

hypothermia (SIH) and is a result of peripheral vasoconstriction caused by the

sympathetic nervous system. SIH has been shown to occur in many different

species of animals as well as humans and is caused by a wide variety of

stressors, both physical and psychological (Bouwknecht et al., 2007).

Figure 1. Predicted demand curve showing how often an option is chosen with increasing cost to reach Option 1. A and C are unbalanced options, defined as an easy decision. B is the point where the options are finely balanced, defined as

a difficult decision.

Page 9: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

6

The aim of this study is to investigate the effects of stress on decision-making in

laying hens. By looking at both physiological and behavioural responses during

both “easy” and “difficult” decisions, this may result in a deeper understanding

of the underlying processes of decision-making.

3 Materials and Methods

3.1 Animals, Housing and Husbandry Sixteen Columbian Blacktail layers were obtained at point of lay (16-18 weeks

of age). On arrival hens were treated with preventative red mite powder,

weighed and given leg tags for identification. Hens were housed in groups of

four and all four groups were housed in the same room in individual pens (1.02

x 1.25 m and 2 m high). The home room was set up as shown in Figure 2, there

were four pens on each side and the hens were held in every other pen so that

the opposite pen was empty. During weekly cleaning the hens were switched

over, to the opposite pen, to avoid housing side bias. One hen was housed

separately, in the opposite pen to her original housing pen, after four weeks due

to injurious pecking behaviour.

Figure 2. Home room (left) with pens and group habituation tunnel set up and procedure room (right) with reward pens, t-maze and logging equipment.

The hens were kept at a light schedule of 12 L : 12 D (light period between 7 am

and 7 pm) in a temperature of 20 ± 2 ˚C. Feed (Farmgate Layers Mash, BOCM

Pauls, Ipswich, Suffolk, UK) was provided ad libitum through two separate

external feeders (0.5 m respectively 0.27 m in length) reached through an

opening at the height of 0.15 m from the pen floor. Water was provided from a

hanging drinker at height 0.2 m from the pen floor. The pens were also provided

with a round perch at height 0.25 m that stretched through the width of the pen,

and a cardboard nest box (0.39 x 0.38 x 0.47 m). In addition, the hens were

provided with a bedding of wood shavings (approximately 10 cm depth).

Page 10: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

7

3.2 Habituation, training and defining individual decision difficulties After a settling in period of five days, the hens were gradually habituated to the

different elements of the test procedure. Habituation criteria were set (see

Appendix 1) which had to be fulfilled before progressing to the next stage. This

was assessed on an individual basis. The total habituation time ranged from four

to five weeks before the training phase began. All hens were ready for testing

after eight to nine weeks of training (including push door calibration) and could

then be tested to evaluate the individual decision difficulties.

The theory of demand curves was used to define unbalanced and balanced

options for food acquisition (Sørensen et al. 2004). The balanced options were

estimated by finding the point where two different quantities of the same food

reward became substitutes for one another by altering the cost of reaching the

larger quantity (Figure 1).

The training, calibration and testing phases, described in this paragraph, were

carried out as parts of another experiment which aimed to individually identify

easy and difficult decisions. The hens were individually trained to distinguish

between two different quantities of sweetcorn presented in different coloured,

different sized bowls (Figure 3). The colours and sizes of the bowls were

counterbalanced so that four hens had each of the different combinations. The

large quantity consisted of six pieces of sweetcorn while the small quantity

represented only one piece. When the hens had successfully completed the

training phase and were considered able to distinguish between the two

different quantities of food, they moved on to the calibration phase. Hens were

first calibrated for the "easy" decision (unbalanced options, un-weighted push

doors). Since there were no additional costs to obtain the larger reward hens

were predicted to prefer this option and the calibration threshold were therefore

set to choosing the large quantity 90-100% over at least 12 consecutive trials as

some divergent choices can be expected. When this criterion had been reached,

the calibration phase for the "difficult" decision began (balanced options,

weighted push door). This involved gradually increasing the weight on the push

door to the large quantity. The weight of the push door was adjusted until hens

began to switch their preference from the large quantity to the small quantity. A

balance point was defined as the point which hens chose the large quantity 25-

75 % of the time. Hen 8 however, fell outside of this range and chose the large

quantity 16.7 % of the time but was still tested and included in the analysis since

she performed within the range during testing. After calibration of both the

balanced ("difficult") and unbalanced ("easy") options, the hens were tested

(separate experiment) to confirm the cross-over point of the demand curves. The

push door weights obtained here were then used in the present experiment for all

individuals with the exception of hen 6 who did not push through any of the

Page 11: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

8

weighted doors during the testing period and were therefore down regulated to a

slightly lower weight previously used during training.

Figure 3. The different set ups of food bowl size, colour and sweetcorn quantities (A-D).

3.3 Experimental Setup A test was designed to investigate the effects of stress on decision-making in

laying hens. Every individual was tested during both "easy" and "difficult"

choices (unbalanced and balanced) with and without stress treatment (Table 1).

Every hen had four test sessions, one for each combination (control treatment,

easy choice; control treatment, difficult choice; stress treatment, easy choice;

stress treatment, difficult choice). The order of the combinations were

systematically alternated and counterbalanced.

Tabel 1. Option balance and stress treatment combinations.

Option Balances Treatments Combinations

Balanced X Stress

Balanced Stress Balanced X No Stress

Unbalanced No Stress Unbalanced X Stress

Unbalanced X No Stress

The same animals and experimental setup were used in both the current study

and the previous experiment when individual decision difficulty was defined.

Choice tests were carried out using a transparent t-maze tunnel with built in

push doors on each side of the maze, regulated by externally powered

electromagnets (Figure 4). At the end of each arm of the t-maze, the tunnel exits

into a pen, of the same type used for housing, containing shavings and a drinker.

The food bowls containing the reward was placed in the pens, one on each side,

Page 12: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

9

25 cm from the entrance and 10 cm from the side wall, clearly visible from

inside the start box.

Figure 4. Experimental setup: the start box with closed side doors and tunnel slide door, t-maze with two transparent sides, push doors and the openings into the pens where the rewards were placed on both sides.

3.3.1 T-maze choice test

The t-maze is commonly used in preference tests where the animal gets to

choose between two options. The animal is placed in the middle of the maze and

can then choose to go either down the right or the left arm, of the maze, to reach

the options. Two experimenters were required to perform the test procedure. The

hen was placed in a start box with sliding doors on the sides facing the openings

of the pens allowing the hen see the reward on each side. The side doors were

closed when the hen was placed in the start box and two further screen doors

were placed either side of the start box to occlude the hens view on entering the

start box. When Experimenter 1 started the HR monitor logging Experimenter 2

simultaneously started the stopwatch and was then responsible for time

recording throughout the test. The actual test started after the thermal imaging

camera was focused, at the onset of thermal imaging recording (approximately

15 seconds after the hen entered the start box) (Figure 5). Ten seconds were

given to record baseline heart rate and thermal images before the first side door

was removed, allowing the hen to see the reward on one side. After five

seconds, the side door was replaced and the second side door was removed,

Page 13: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

10

allowing the hen to see the reward on the other side of the maze. When the

second side door had been open for five seconds, both side doors were removed,

allowing the hen to see both options. When the hen had been presented with

both options for the duration of ten seconds the tunnel slide door was opened,

allowing them to enter into the t-maze tunnel. The order of the side door

removal as well as the reward side placement was systematically alternated and

counterbalanced. If the hen had not entered the tunnel within 60 seconds she was

gently pushed inside and the tunnel door were closed. The hen was allowed a

maximum of four minutes in the tunnel. If a decision had not been made within

the duration, the hen was gently removed through the start box and were then

handled in the same way as if removed from the pen. Upon reaching either of

the pens, the hen was given 90 seconds to consume the reward and to regain

baseline heart rate before tested again. Each of the four sessions for each hen

consisted of: two forced tests, one to each side (for the hens to experience the

weight of the push doors), followed by five consecutive free choices before

switching the reward sides and repeating the same procedure. In total, each of

the four test sessions had 14 tests (four forced and ten free). The day before the

testing period started, the hens were each given four free choices as a reminder

of the test procedure used in the previous experiment.

Figure 5. Time line describing the test procedure from the start of HR logging until the tunnel door opening specifying the role for both experimenters.

Before each test session, the hens were food deprived according to a schedule

obtained during the previous experiment (see Appendix 2). It was determined

during the previous experiment that different individuals displayed different

motivational levels to perform the test, therefore, the optimal food deprivation

time was established on an individual basis. The duration of food deprivation

varied between one hour twenty minutes and six hours thirty minutes.

3.3.2 Stress Treatment

The hens were stressed using a form of physical restraint where the hen was

placed in a free hanging net for 5 minutes directly before testing. The hen was

removed from the net and directly brought into the procedure room where a

harness with heart rate monitor was fitted and the test started. This stress method

has previously been shown to cause a significant increase in plasma

Page 14: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

11

corticosterone in chickens (Karlsson et al., 2011). Stress treatment was carried

out in a separate room, opposite to the procedure room, to avoid disturbing other

hens.

3.4 Data Collection

3.4.1 Choice

Two different choices were recorded for each test. The first choice which

represents the side of the first push on a push door and the final choice which is

recorded as the side of the pen the hen actually entered.

3.4.2 Latency The latency for an animal to approach an option is a well known behavioural

measure of motivation used in preference tests (Dawkins, 1990; Bateson, 2004).

If an animal approaches an option with a shorter latency than the other, it can be

interpreted as if the animal prefers this option. Also, latency to choose has been

shown to be positively correlated with consistency of choice in hens (Browne et

al., 2010). Two latencies were measured in this study; the latency from the

opening of the tunnel door until the hen first pushed on a door and the latency

until the hen entered a pen.

3.4.3 Heart Rate Monitoring Heart rate (HR) was monitored using the telemetric logging system presented by

Lowe et al, 2007. A Lycra harness, with an integrated pouch, was used to attach

the heart rate monitor to the hens (Figure 6). The harness was specifically

designed for this purpose so as not to restrict the crop or the hens’ movement.

The base unit, which was attached to the computer received the radio signal

from the logging unit and was placed next to the start box for the duration of

each test.

Page 15: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

12

Figure 6. One of the hens wearing a harness with a heart rate monitor, standing beside the base unit connected to a computer logging the heart rate.

ECG pads were placed on each of the

featherless patches on both sides of the

keel bone (Figure 7). When needed,

downy feathers were removed in

accordance with home office licence

procedures. The patches were wiped

with Surgical Spirits to remove dead

cells to ensure good contact between

the ECG pad and the surface of the

skin. RVC Telemetry Logger Control

version 1.5 was used for logging the

heart rate. These files were later

analysed using Spike 2 6.10. Three

different ten seconds heart rate

intervals were analysed. The first

interval was basal heart rate when the

hen was in the start box but had not

yet been presented with the options.

The second interval was the decision

Figure 7. Placement of the ECG pads (blue) on either sides of the keel bone.

Page 16: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

13

period when the hen could see both options at the same time before allowed to

enter the tunnel. The third interval analysed was the first ten seconds after the

hen entered the chosen pen.

3.4.4 Thermal Imaging

In the current study, stress-induced hypothermia was studied with a non-

invasive method using a thermal imaging camera FLIR SC305, FLIR System

AB, Sweden. The camera was placed as shown in Figure 1 and the back of the

start box was made of black plastic (emissivity 0.292) allowing thermal imaging

to take place through the plastic so the hens could not see the camera. Maximum

head and comb temperatures as well as eye temperatures were analyzed using

FLIR R&D ResearchIR. Basal temperatures were recorded for the same ten

seconds interval as the basal heart rate (in the start box before presented with the

options). The second ten seconds interval was recorded during the decision

phase when the hen was presented with both options at the same time before

allowed to enter the tunnel. Thermal images were recorded with nine frames per

second. From each ten second interval, one good image with a side shot of the

head in focus was chosen for analysing.

3.5 Ethical Note All work was conducted under UK Home office licence (PPL number: 30/2779).

3.6 Statistical Methods

The statistical analysis was carried out using the statistical package IBM SPSS

Statistics 19. Percentage differences between basal values and decision phase

and pen heart rate (HR) were calculated. However, mean HR values were used

when studying the variation of HR over the ten consecutive free choices.

Individual slope values were calculated and analysed using univariate general

linear model. Percentage differences between basal and decision phase values

were calculated for head, eye and comb temperatures. One-way repeated

measurements ANOVA was used to explore and analyse the obtained data.

Preliminary analysis were made looking at possible effects of session order, side

placement and comparing the first five free choices and the second five free

choices. If no significant effects of these factors were found they were excluded

from the statistical models used to examine the effects of stress and decision

difficulty.

4 Results All data was normally distributed. Factors were addressed individually before

taken into the model. No significant differences were found between the side

placements of the rewards and there were no differences between the first five

choices and the second five choices within each session. Also, no differences

were found between the individual test sessions.

Page 17: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

14

4.1 Choice When looking at towards which side the first push were made during each test

(ten test per session, 40 tests in total), on average 78.7±4.4(SE) % of these were

towards the large quantity reward. Furthermore, when looking at the pen entered

during each test, on average 76.3±4.5(SE) % of these were large quantity reward

choices.

Two hens did not make all of their choices after receiving stress treatment in

combination with the difficult decision. Hen 2 made six of ten first pushes and

entered a pen five out of ten times with stress treatment and the difficult

decision. During her next session where she did not receive stress treatment but

still had a difficult decision she made eight of ten complete choices. Hen 10

made five of ten first pushes and entered a pen three out of ten times after

receiving stress treatment in combination with the difficult decision. On her

upcoming session where she did not receive stress treatment but had the difficult

decision she did not make any choices at all. On her second upcoming session,

hen 10 had the easy decision and no stress treatment and she made eight out of

ten complete choices.

The stress treatment did not have any significant effect on either the latency to

the first push or until the pen was entered. On the other hand, the percentage of

large quantity choices was significantly higher when the decision was between

unbalanced options (easy decision) than when the decision was between

balanced options (difficult decision). This applies both for the latency until the

first push and the entering of the pen (Figure 8) (first push: F(1)= 5.931; P<0.05;

pen entered: F(1)=19.794; P<0.05).

Figure 8. Difference in direction of the first push (F(1)= 5.931; P<0.05) and pen entered (F(1)=19.794; P<0.05) depending on decision difficulty. Error bars displays standard error.

Page 18: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

15

Statistically significant differences were found between the decision difficulty of

the first test session and towards which option the first push was directed

throughout all four sessions (Figure 9). Hens that had a difficult decision during

the first test session had a higher frequency of choosing the large quantity food

reward as direction of their first push (F(1)=12.215; P<0.05). Correspondingly, a

statistically significant difference was found between the pen entered and the

decision difficulty of the first test session (Figure 9). Hens that had a difficult

decision during their first test session had a higher frequency of choosing the

pen containing the large quantity food reward (F(1)=7.746; P<0.05).

The stress treatment that the hens received immediately before their first test

session had a significant effect on both towards which option the first push were

directed and pen entered throughout the entire experiment. Hens that received

stress treatment immediately before their first session chose the large quantity

reward less frequently (Figure 10), as their first choice, than hens that did not

have stress treatment on their first session (F(1)=7.269; P<0.05).

Correspondingly, hens that received stress treatment immediately before their

first session chose the larger quantity reward less frequently (Figure 10), as their

final choice, than hens that did not have stress treatment on their first session

(F(1)=8.099; P<0.05).

Figure 9. Difference in direction of the first push (F(1)=12.215; P<0.05) and pen entered (F(1)=7.746; P<0.05) depending on whether the hens experienced easy or difficult choices during their first test session. Error bars displays standard error.

Page 19: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

16

Figure 10. Differences in first (F(1)=7.269; P<0.05) and final (F(1)=8.099; P<0.05) choices depending on whether the hens received stress treatment or not during their first test session. Error bars display standard error.

4.2 Latency Average latency to the first push was 6.2±1.4(SE) seconds respectively

9.6±1.8(SE) seconds on average latency to enter a pen. The latency until the first

push and until entrance of either of the pens were not affected by the stress

treatment (P>0.1). Latency to first push was not affected by decision difficulty

(P>0.1), however, decision difficulty had a significant effect on the latency to

enter a pen (Figure 11) where it took the hens longer to enter the chosen pen if

the decision was difficult (F(1)=21.282; P<0.05).

Figure 11. Mean latencies (s) to enter a pen for easy and difficult decisions (F(1)=21.282; P<0.05). Error bars display standard error.

Figure 12. Mean latencies (s) to enter a pen depending on whether the hens received stress treatment or not during their first test session (F(1)=7.491; P<0.05). Error bars display standard error.

Page 20: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

17

First session decision difficulty and treatment did not have an effect on latency

to first push (P>0.1). First session decision difficulty did not have an effect on

latency to enter pen (P>0.1). However, the stress treatment given immediately

before the first test session had a significant effect on latency to enter pen

(Figure 12) where hens that received stress treatment on their first test session

had longer latency to enter pen (F(1)=7.491; P<0.05).

4.3 Heart Rate

Neither the stress treatment nor the

decision difficulty affected mean

basal heart rate (HR), decision

phase HR or pen HR (P>0.1).

However, there was a strong

quadratic relationship between these

three different HR measures (Figure

13) showing a significant decrease

in mean decision phase HR in

comparison to basal HR and a

significant increase in pen HR in

comparison to basal HR

(F(1)=40.644; P<0.05).

When looking at the overall pattern

of mean heart rate over the ten free

choices there was a negative

gradient slope independent of decision difficulty and treatment (see Appendix 3

Figure 15-18). When looking at the mean decision phase HR over ten free

choices there is a significant linear relationship for both easy decision and

difficult decision HR (Figure 14) (F(1)=4.816; P<0.05). The negative slopes are

significantly different between easy (mean: -1.309) and difficult (mean: -2.389)

choices where the mean HR during the decision phase has a greater decline

during difficult decisions (F(1)=4.815; P<0.05).

Figure 13. Quadratic relationship between mean basal, decision phase and pen heart rate (F(1)=40.644; P<0.05).

Page 21: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

18

4.4 Thermal Images

Neither the stress treatment nor the decision difficulty resulted in any

differences in head, eye or comb temperatures which only had a great individual

variation.

5 Discussion To summarise the results, hens that received stress treatment immediately before

their first test session chose the large quantity reward less frequently than hens

that did not receive stress treatment before their first test session. Also, hens that

received stress treatment before their first test session showed a longer latency to

enter either of the chosen pens in comparison to hens that did not receive stress

treatment at their first test session. Decision difficulty, on the other hand, did

have an effect on the overall choices made where the larger quantity reward was

chosen less frequently for difficult choices but still chosen more often than

expected. In addition, latency to enter pen was longer for difficult decisions than

for easy decisions. Also, the decrease in decision phase heart rate (HR) over ten

free choices was greater for difficult choices than easy choices. Hens that

experienced a difficult decision during their first test session chose the larger

quantity reward more frequently than hens that had an easy decision during their

first test session. Looking at how HR varies over single tests, there is a quadratic

relationship between the three different intervals analysed. There is a decrease in

Figure 14. Mean decision phase heart rate over ten free choices for easy and difficult decisions (filled lines). A linear relationship was found for both easy and difficult decisions (F(1)=4.816; P<0.05). There is a statistical significant difference between slopes (dotted lines) for easy (-1.309) and difficult (-2.389) decisions (F(1)=4.815; P<0.05). Error bars display standard error.

Page 22: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

19

mean HR from the basal values to decision phase values and a great increase in

mean HR when the hen reached the pen with the reward.

Here, a difficult decision was defined as one which presented two balanced

options. In contrast, an easy decision was defined as one which presented two

unbalanced options. Here, “difficult” decisions resulted in a more balanced

choice distribution than the “easy” decision. This indicates that the difficult

choice had more balanced options than the easy choice. The choice distribution

of the balanced options was within the range used as a criterion during training.

On the other hand, the unbalanced options had a more balanced choice

distribution than what was defined during training but not as balanced as what

was defined as a balanced choice distribution which is very important for the

interpretation of the results from this experiment. It has previously been shown

that hens have the cognitive capacity of making rational choices (Browne et al.

2010). However, hens do not give fixed values to options and so their

preferences can change. Even though we can expect the hens to overall have a

preference for the reward that is the most valuable for them, we cannot expect

every individual to always choose the optimal option (McFarland, 1977).

Preferences may vary with motivational priorities and these are not fixed but can

be altered by e.g. altering the cost to reach a resource (Bateson, 2004). Here we

have caused a situation where the hens have to weigh the trade off between

reward size and the cost of reaching it which has resulted in a more balanced

option set.

Furthermore, hens that were stressed immediately before their first test session

had a more balanced choice distribution, throughout the entire experiment, than

hens that were not stressed before their first session. Stress can impair optimal

behaviour (Graham, 2010), due to its effect on the decision making process

(Mendl, 1999). When an animal is stressed by a stressor that is not only

physiologically stressful but also emotionally stressful it may experience

fearfulness (Fraisse and Cockrem, 2006). Fear as well as stress can alter the

motivational priorities of an animal (Bateson, 2004). A stressed or fearful

animal may prioritise a quick escape path instead of evaluating the options of

reward size. Also, stress can cause shifts in attention compromising animals’

ability to make a decision from the given stimuli which could result in them

choosing e.g. a preferred side instead of a preferred option. In this case, the

stress treatment given during the first test session affected the choices made by

hens throughout the entire experiment. This could indicate that the hens

associate the test with being stressed or fearfulness and this persists all the way

through their four separate test sessions. Even so, most of the hens still made

choices and the effect of prior stress treatment did not have major effects on the

outcome of this choice test. This very importantly points out that that prior

handling should not affect choice tests used in animal welfare context.

Page 23: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

20

Additionally, hens that had a difficult decision in their first session ended up

choosing the large quantity reward more frequently than hens that had an easy

decision during their first test session. As previously mentioned, it is possible

that the hens’ expectations of the test are influenced by what they experienced at

their first session. If that is the case, hens that expect a difficult choice may be

more confident in pushing through the weighted doors than hens that expect an

easy choice.

Furthermore, there were no effects of decision difficulty or treatment on latency

to first push. However, latency to enter the chosen pen was longer for difficult

decisions which is most likely a result of the fact that the hens had to push

through a weighted door. Hens that were stressed during their first test session

had a longer latency to enter the chosen pen than hens that were not stressed

during their first test session. In addition, these hens had a longer latency to

enter the chosen pen, even though they had a lower frequency of choosing the

larger quantity reward. Previous studies looking at motivation to reach a food

reward in laying hens, found that when hens are less motivated to feed they took

longer time to push through the weighted push doors (Olsson et al. 2002). This

indicates that hens receiving stress treatment in their first session were less

motivated to push through the push doors than hens who did not receive stress

treatment during their first session. This could be a further indication towards

the hens associating the test procedure with stress or fearfulness if they

experienced stress treatment in their first test session.

Moving on, the quadratic relationship in heart rate (HR) over single tests can

most likely be explained as an effect of handling. Basal HR was measured

before the decision phase HR. Therefore, basal HR is likely to be higher due to

the recent handling of the hen. As the hen remains inactive in the start box a

decrease in HR can be expected as a result of relaxation (Henry, 1993). Pen HR

is expected to be significantly increased in comparison with basal values due to

the physical effort of walking through the maze and pushing through the door

causing an increase in sympathetic activity (Moberg, 2000).

Additionally, mean heart rate (HR) over the ten free choices is represented by a

negative linear relationship. This could indicate that hens get more used to the

choice test and relax as a result of confidence in how to control their situation

(Henry, 1993). Unexpectedly, the decrease in HR over the ten free choices

seems more profound when hens are presented with a balanced or “difficult”

choice. A study on learning in dwarf goats found a decrease in HR as a result of

what they call frustration in the beginning of a learning task before the animals

had learned how to cope with the problem of the task (Langbein et al, 2004).

Likewise, Crone et al. (2004) found an anticipatory decrease in HR, in humans,

Page 24: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

21

prior to making risky choices. According to the concept of coping by Henry

(1993), the individual cognitive evaluation of a challenging situation can lead to

an activation of the HPA axis due to an uncontrollable situation. This could lead

to a depression in behaviour accompanied with a decrease in HR. It is believed

that the ability to cope with a stressor is mainly controlled by the sympathetic

nervous system and is therefore generally accompanied by an increase in HR

(Porges, 1995; von Borell 2001). This could possibly indicate that the hens did

not find a coping strategy for the balanced choice thus the slowing of HR in

anticipation of difficulty.

Another explanation for the decrease in decision phase heart rate (HR) could be

that what we here defined as a “difficult” and an “easy” decision is actually not

perceived that way by the hens. A decision between two balanced options may

actually be easier than a decision between two unbalanced options. The

differences in decision phase HR could therefore result from the decision

between two unbalanced options being more stressful than the balanced options.

These findings can be interpreted in line with the decision theory presented by

Blumstein and Bouskila (1996) previously mentioned in the introduction.

Behavioural differences (choice and latency) were seen even though the same

stimuli were presented. This could mean that the hens differ in their assessment

of the stimuli and therefore end up with different informational states and so

make different decisions leading to different behavioural outputs. Another

explanation is that the hens do not differ in their assessment of the stimuli and

end up with the same informational state, but when evaluating the informational

state in comparison to their own state (e.g. physiological state) they differ in

their decision making process which results in different behavioural outputs.

From this study we cannot say if the difference is in the informational state or

the hens’ evaluation of the informational state (decision making process).

However, it could be argued that the decrease in decision phase heart rate,

possibly a sign of anticipation of difficulty, could be an indication of differences

in the process of assessment or decision making in the hens. It is therefore

possible that heart rate measures could be one of the keys to solving the “black

box”.

In conclusion, recent experiences related to the test procedure, can have an

effect on hens behaviour in a choice test. This appears to apply even if the hens

do not have any direct negative experiences of the options. Hens expecting a

difficult choice from their first session generally performed better in subsequent

sessions. Hens experiencing stress treatment immediately before their first

session showed a more balanced choice distribution possibly due to a shift in

motivational priorities. Another theory is that the stress treatment prior to the

first test session affected the hens’ informational state, altering evaluation of the

Page 25: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

22

informational state, resulting in more balanced choice distribution. However, the

stress treatment did not have a major effect on the outcome of the choice test

indicating the resilience of animal choice test to mild stress such as prior

handling. Additionally, decrease in heart rate during the decision making period,

of a balanced or decision could indicate anticipation of difficulty in hens. This

physiological measurement could be one of the keys to obtain a more detailed

picture of the “black box” that we often face in animal behaviour and welfare

research. Nevertheless, there is still substantial research to be done, on the

effects of stress on animal decision making, to understand underlying processes.

A possible next step could be to investigate the effect of stress on other forms of

“difficult” decisions.

6 Acknowledgements

I thank Prof. Christine Nicol, Bristol University, and Prof. Per Jensen,

Linköping University, for their involvement and good advice. I also thank PhD

Anna Davies, Bristol University, for all the help and experience.

7 References Bateson M (2004) Mechanisms of decision-making and the interpretation of

choice tests. Animal Welfare 13, 115-120

Bechara A, Damasio AR (2004) The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural

theory of economic decision. Games and Economic Behavior 52, 336-372

Blumstein DT, Bouskila A (1996) Assessment and decision making in animals:

a mechanistic model underlying behavioural flexibility can prevent ambiguity.

Oikos 77, 569-576

Von Borell EH (2001) The biology of stress and its application to livestock

housing and transportation assessment. Journal of Animal Science 79, E260-

E267

Bouwknecht JA, Olivier B, Paylor RE (2007) The stress-induced hypothermia

paradigm as a physiological animal model of enxiety: A review of

pharmacological and genetic studies in the mouse. Neuroscience and Behavioral

Reviews 31, 41-59

Broom DM, Johnson KG (1993) Stress and animal welfare. pp 92-95. Kluwer

Academic Publisher, Dordrecht

Browne WJ, Caplen G, Edgar J, Wilson LR, Nicol CJ (2010) Consistency,

transitivity and inter-relationships between measures of choice in environmental

preference tests with chickens. Behavioural Processes 83, 72-78

Page 26: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

23

Browne WJ, Caplen G, Statham P, Nicol CJ (2011) Mild environmental

aversion is detected be a discrete-choice preference testing method but not by a

free-access method. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 134, 152-163

Crone EA, Somsen RJM, Van Beek B, Van der Molen MW (2004) Heart rate

and skin conductance analysis of antecendents and consequences of decision

making. Psychophysiology 41, 531-540

Dawkins MS (1990) From an animal’s point of view: motivation, fitness, and

animal welfare. Behavioral and Brain Science 13, 1-61

Dill LM (1987) Animal decision making and its ecological consequences: the

future of aquatic ecology and behaviour. Canadian Journal of Zoology 65, 803-

811

Fraisse F, Cockrem JF (2006) Corticosterone and fear behaviour in white and

brown caged laying hens. British Poultry Science 47, 110-119

Fraser D, Nicol CJ (2011) Preference and motivation research. In Animal

Welfare, 2nd

edition. CAB International, Wallingford UK

Graham LK, Yoon T, Kim JJ (2010) Stress impairs optimal behavior in a water

foraging choice task in rats. Learning & Memory 17, 1-4

Lowe JC, Abeyesinghe SM, Demmers TGM, Wathes CM, McKeegan

DEF(2007) A novel telemetric logging system for recording physiological

signals in unrestrained animals. Computer and Electronics in Agriculture 57, 74-

79

McEwen BS, Sapolsky RM (1995) Stress and cognitive function. Current

Opinion in Neurobiology 5, 205-216

McFarland DJ (1977) Decision making in animals. Nature 269, 15-20

Mendl M (1999) Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function.

Applied Animal Behaviour Science 65, 221-244

Moberg GP (2000) Biological responses to stress: Implications for animal

welfare. pp 1-21 in The biology of animal stress. CAB International,

Wallingford UK

Page 27: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

24

Olsson EAS, Keeling LJ, McAdie TM (2002) The push-door for measuring

motivation in hens: an adaptation and a critical discussion of the method.

Animal Welfare 11, 1-10

Regolin L, Vallortigara G, Zanforlin M (1995) Detour behaviour in the domestic

chick: searching for a disappearing prey or a disappearing social partner. Animal

Behaviour 50, 203-211

Schuck-Paim C, Pompilio L, Kacelnik A (2004) State-dependent decisions

cause apparent violations of rationality in animal choice. PLoS Biology 2, 2305-

2315

Sørensen DB, Ladewig J, Ersboll AK, Matthews L (2004) Using the cross point

of demand functions to assess animal priorities. Animal Behaviour 68, 949-955

Warbuton HJ, Nicol CJ (1998) Position of operant costs affects visits to

resources by laboratory mice, Mus musculus. Animal Behaviour 55, 1325-1333

Page 28: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

25

Appendix 1

Table 1. Rough schedule of the habituation, training, calibration and testing.

Week Summary of each week

1 Habituation

2 Habituation

3 Habituation

4 Habituation + Training

5 Training

6 Training

7 Calibration easy choice

8 Push door calibration

9 Calibration difficult choice

10 Testing Experiment 1 (decision

difficulty)

11 Testing Experiment 1 (decision

difficulty)

12 Christmas Holiday

13 Testing Experiment 2 (stress)

14 Testing Experiment 2 (stress)

Page 29: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

26

Table 2. Habituation, training and calibration elements and their criteria as well as the approximate number of days and tests needed to fulfil these.

Elements of

habituation,

training and

calibration

Criterion/criteria to fulfil

Number of

days spent on

each criterion

Number

of tests

needed

per

individual

Sweet corn Eats the sweet corn.

1 to 2

Food bowl

Not obviously distressed by the presence

of the bowls, eats sweet corn from the

bowls

3 to 4

Harness plain

Moves around freely without being

obviously distressed by it. Walks forward

without stopping and pecking at the

harness.

4 to 5, 1 hour

sessions

Harness +

HR monitor

Moves around freely without being

obviously distressed by it. Walks forward

without stopping and pecking at the

harness.

1 to 2 in pen, 3

to 4 with test procedure

Tunnel

(group)

Every individual has gone through the

tunnel into the opposite pen and back at

least once.

3 days, 1-1.5

hours a day

Tunnel

(individual)

Walks freely from the start box through the

tunnel without stopping, showing fearful

behaviour or hesitating.

7 to 8 20

Start box

Able to wait for the required duration

without showing fearful behaviour or

distress when removal of side doors and

tunnel door.

6 with gradually

increasing

duration

23

Push door Pushes through the unweighed door using

a correct technique without hesitation. 8 to 10 30-40

Push door

calibration Pushes through the weighted door using a

correct technique without hesitation. 2 to 3 15-20

Easy choice

calibration

Chooses the larger quantity of sweet corn

100-90% over at least 12 consecutive

trials.

4 to 6, some

needed more

training

30-40

Difficult

choice

calibration

Chooses the larger quantity of sweet corn

75-25% over at least 12 consecutive trials.

9 to 10 50-70

Page 30: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

27

Appendix 2 Table 3. Duration of food deprivation for all subjects ranging from 1 hour 20 min to 6 hours 30 min.

Hen Food deprivation Hen Food deprivation

1 2 hours 10 min 9 1 hour 20 min

2 2 hours 10 min 10 4 hours 30 min

3 3 hours 11 5 hours 50 min

4 6 hours 30 min 12 1 hour 40 min

5 1 hour 20 min 13 2 hours 10 min

6 2 hours 10 min 14 4 hours 30 min

7 2 hours 30 min 15 1 hour 20 min

8 3 hours 16 2 hours 30 min

Page 31: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

28

Appendix 3

Figure 15. Mean basal heart rate for stressed and non stressed hens over ten free choices (P>0.1). Error bars display standard error.

Figure 16. Mean decision phase heart rate for stressed and non stressed hens over ten free choices (P>0.1). Error bars display standard error.

Page 32: Effects of stress and balance of options on decision ...liu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:529370/FULLTEXT01.pdf · Choice test, decision making, heart rate, laying hen, push-door,

29

Figure 17. Mean pen heart rate for stressed and non stressed hens over ten free choices (P>0.1). Error bars display standard error.

Figure 18. Mean pen heart rate for easy and difficult decisions over ten free choices (P>0.1). Error bars display standard error.