measuring variability in color semantics

Upload: georgia-kleoudi

Post on 01-Jun-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    1/30

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    2/30

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    3/30

    MEASURING VARIATION

    IN COLOR SEMANTICS

    The main

    topic yields

    four

    spin-offs.

    First,

    methods

    are

    presented

    here

    in

    detail

    (?3),

    and there

    is

    a

    review of

    questions regarding reliability

    in the

    Ap-

    pendix.

    Second,

    intercommunity

    variation and

    category change

    are

    addressed

    with a

    dynamic cognitive

    model, which,

    in

    turn,

    is based

    on

    a

    distinction

    be-

    tween

    cognition

    and

    perception

    (?4).

    The difference is

    readily

    definable

    in

    the

    color

    domain,

    because its

    neurological

    bases are

    known. Without this

    distinc-

    tion,

    an

    explanation

    of

    change

    within universal constraints

    would

    be

    impossible.

    The model attributes both the structure and the

    process

    of

    categorization

    to

    a

    selective

    emphasis

    on different

    perceptions;

    but a further

    construct of

    social

    variables

    completes

    the

    account

    of

    intercommunity

    differences

    (?5.4). Third,

    the data substantiate in detail the incremental nature

    of

    category change

    and

    variation

    (?5).

    And

    fourth,

    the

    explanation

    of

    individually

    compelled change

    unites Berlin & Kay's BASICCOLORTERMwith Rosch's BASICLEVEL,which has

    been

    a

    long-standing

    desideratum

    in

    the

    theory

    of

    categorization.

    Its

    resolution

    deflates the

    mounting

    pressure

    to redefine 'basic color

    terms',

    and it

    recognizes

    that the basic level of

    categorization

    is mutable

    (?6).

    THEORY

    2.1. The data

    from

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal and

    Navenchauc Tzotzil

    offer an

    op-

    portunity

    to

    assess

    the

    explanatory capacities

    of two

    theories that model the

    relation between

    linguistic

    variation and

    change. One theory, articulated in

    Weinreich et al. 1968, holds that change is socially motivated: any language

    harbors a

    range

    of

    variation,

    and

    change progresses when particular variants

    are

    assigned prestige

    and

    favored as markers

    of

    social

    identity. The other theory

    was

    developed

    in

    MacLaury

    1986

    to

    account for variation among basic color

    categories:

    each

    individual

    adopts

    a

    private strategy

    for

    coping with the novelty

    that

    impinges upon

    him

    or her

    throughout daily life;

    the

    strategy always involves

    attendance

    both

    to the

    similarity

    and

    to the

    distinctiveness of the entities that

    he

    or she

    categorizes

    and

    names;

    some

    individuals attend more strongly to

    similarity

    and

    others

    attend

    more

    strongly

    to

    distinctiveness. The latter group

    of

    individuals will

    name more

    color

    categories during standardized experi-

    ments, such as the one that is used to determine how a subject categorizes and

    names the

    330-chip

    Munsell color

    array.

    The

    INDIVIDUALIST HEORY

    was

    devised

    to

    address

    situations encountered

    widely

    and

    repeatedly by

    the

    Mesoamerican

    Color Survey (MCS).2 The survey

    found that

    as

    a

    rule

    people

    who

    interact

    daily

    differ

    in

    the ways that they

    categorize color,

    even

    though

    shared

    color

    categories would facilitate the ex-

    and

    The

    American

    Heritage Dictionary (p. 263) explains

    it

    briefly.

    See

    MacLaury (1987a,

    n.

    2)

    for

    technical

    specification

    of stimulus

    materials

    used

    in this

    study.

    2

    The MCS was conducted during 1978-81 in Mexico and Guatemala in collaboration with eighty

    teams of

    language specialists

    who interviewed

    900

    speakers

    of

    116

    languages.

    Some data were

    pooled

    with

    the

    World

    Color

    Survey (WCS)

    of

    1976-78,

    in

    which 25

    speakers

    in each

    of 100

    American, African, Austronesian,

    and Asian

    languages

    were interviewed

    by

    members of the Sum-

    mer

    Institute

    of

    Linguistics

    under an NSF

    grant

    to

    Berlin, Kay,

    and Merrifield

    (see

    Berlin

    et al.

    1985).

    35

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    4/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER

    1

    (1991)

    change

    of

    information.

    To

    illustrate with a

    typical

    case,

    speakers

    A

    and

    B

    are

    adults

    in

    middle

    age

    who have conversed

    throughout

    their

    lives;

    speaker

    A

    has

    three basic color terms and

    speaker

    B

    has

    ten,

    neither

    seems

    to

    be

    aware of

    the

    difference,

    and

    they

    appear

    to talk

    past

    each

    other when

    casually handling

    the

    Munsell color

    chips. Rampant intracommunity

    variation

    was

    described

    pre-

    cisely by

    the

    Munsell metric when it was

    systematically applied

    over a

    wide

    region by

    putting

    hundreds of

    subjects,

    one at a

    time,

    through

    a

    formal two-

    hour

    interview.

    It is

    essential to draw

    upon

    both

    theories to

    explain

    variation

    and

    change

    in

    Tzeltal

    and Tzotzil color

    categorization.

    The social

    theory explains

    the dra-

    matic

    difference between the

    ways

    that

    color is

    categorized

    in

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal

    and

    Navenchauc

    Tzotzil,

    but the

    individual

    theory

    addresses the

    astounding

    range of nonagreement in each community.

    2.2.

    PRIOR

    WORK. The

    body

    of

    theory

    has roots in two

    traditions.

    Weinreich,

    Labov,

    and

    Herzog

    (1968)

    are

    among

    the

    most recent

    scholars who

    demonstrate

    that

    linguistic change depends

    on

    variation.

    Labov,

    in

    his classic

    study

    of

    vowel

    raising

    on

    Martha's

    Vineyard

    (1963),

    shows

    that

    change

    focuses on a

    prestigious

    variant

    that native

    Vineyarders

    adopted

    as

    a

    symbol

    of social

    membership.

    In

    an

    independent tradition,

    Berlin &

    Kay (1969)

    define

    the

    concept

    of

    BASIC

    COLOR

    ERMS

    nd

    show that their focal

    referents

    and

    order

    of evolution

    are

    universal; any language

    increases

    its stock of

    basic

    color terms in

    response to

    societal

    complexity

    or culture

    contact

    (cf.

    Berlin & Berlin

    1975, Dougherty

    1975). Kay (1975) and Dougherty (1977) further note the relation of variation

    to

    basic

    color-term

    evolution:

    different

    speakers

    of one

    language can manifest

    distinct

    stages

    of that

    process,

    with

    younger speakers usually showing later

    stages; yet

    each

    individual

    system represents

    some

    stage

    of the

    predictable

    sequence.

    But

    Kay

    and

    Dougherty

    further

    imply

    that

    each

    individual passes

    through

    the

    sequence

    at a

    different

    pace,

    sharing cognition only partially with

    other

    speakers

    of the

    language.

    That

    is,

    either

    color-term evolution

    is privately

    motivated or

    the

    shared

    social

    motive,

    whatever

    it

    might

    be, does not

    foster

    uniform

    cognition.

    DESCRIPTION

    ND

    DISPLAY

    OF DATA

    3. Before I

    discuss

    data,

    it is

    essential

    to outline how

    they

    were

    collected,

    how

    they

    are

    displayed

    in

    a

    black-and-white

    medium,

    and

    why they

    constitute

    an

    advance over

    prior

    collections.

    Earlier

    fieldworkers

    placed

    acetate over

    the Munsell

    spectrum

    and

    asked an

    interviewee

    to circle with a

    grease pencil

    the

    range

    of each

    native

    color term

    from

    a

    previously

    elicited

    list; further,

    the interviewee

    marked

    the

    focus

    of

    each term

    (Lenneberg

    &

    Roberts

    1956,

    Berlin

    &

    Kay 1969, Dougherty 1975,

    Berlin & Berlin 1975). Other researchers upgraded the method or experimented

    with

    options (Collier

    et al.

    1976, Hage

    &

    Hawkes 1975,

    Berlin et al.

    1985).

    During

    the

    Mesoamerican

    survey,

    a

    final

    procedure

    was added

    and

    refined

    (Burgess

    et al.

    1983,

    Greenfeld

    1986, MacLaury

    1986,

    1987a-b).

    36

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    5/30

    MEASURING

    VARIATION

    IN

    COLOR

    SEMANTICS

    The

    combinedefforts

    have resulted

    n a

    particularpackage

    of

    equipment

    and

    procedures

    for its use. Stimulus materials consist

    of 330

    Munsell

    colors

    or

    'chips'.

    Oneset

    comprises

    oose

    chips

    thatare

    separately

    numberedand

    aligned

    in random order. Two additionalsets are glued to a neutralmatte in spectral

    order of lettered rows from

    light

    to

    dark, B-I,

    and numbered olumns

    through

    the

    hues,

    1-40;

    each is called an

    'array'.

    One

    arrayplaces green-blue

    at

    center;

    the

    other centers

    red-yellow.

    Both include achromatic

    white-grey-black

    n

    an

    unnumbered

    eft

    column, A-J,

    in which

    row

    A is

    pure

    white,

    row J is

    pure

    black,

    and

    rows B-I match the

    eightbrightness

    evels of the hue

    columns

    (see

    n.

    1).

    Data are

    elicited from an individual

    speaker by

    three

    independent

    proce-

    dures.

    First,

    an

    interviewee names the 330 loose

    chips

    one

    by

    one

    in

    their

    random order. The investigatorrecordson a numberedsheet each nameas a

    head lexeme with

    any qualifiers

    that

    might accompany

    it.

    Second,

    the inter-

    viewee selects from one

    of the

    arrays

    he best

    example

    or 'focus' of

    each head

    lexeme.

    Third,

    the interviewee

    maps

    the

    range

    of each head lexeme

    by

    placing

    a

    grain

    of rice on each

    color of

    the

    array

    that the lexeme can name.

    When the

    interviewee

    stops

    mapping

    he

    range

    of a

    term,

    the

    investigator

    requests

    that

    he

    or she

    place grains

    on more of

    the

    colors that

    the

    term

    can

    name. The

    request

    is

    repeated

    for one

    term until

    the

    interviewee insists that it can name

    no color

    chips

    that

    have not

    already

    been

    covered with

    rice.

    Thus,

    a

    mapping

    can

    progressthrough

    one or more

    'steps',

    which the

    investigator

    ecords

    separately

    in reference to the row letters and columnnumbers hat borderthe array.

    Ultimately,

    the

    three

    types

    of

    independently

    elicited

    data are

    derandomized

    and

    organized

    on

    graphs

    that

    represent

    the

    Munsell

    array.

    In

    their

    organized

    form, they

    are

    called, respectively, (1)

    NAMING

    RANGES

    and

    QUALIFIER

    DISTRIBUTIONS, (2)

    FOCI,

    and

    (3)

    MAPPINGS

    nd

    MAPPING

    STEPS.

    A

    mappingcan

    consist

    of

    only

    one

    step

    or of several. It

    shows the full

    extent of a

    color category;

    and,

    if it

    consists of

    more than one

    step,

    the

    order in which

    the

    steps were

    executed

    suggests

    how the

    category

    s

    internallyorganizedby

    the

    interviewee.

    Correspondence

    between

    independently

    elicited orders of

    data

    verifies the

    accuracyof each in any set froman individual.Matchescan occur between a

    naming range

    and

    its

    mapping,

    a focus and

    a

    first

    mappingstep, a particular

    mappingstep

    and

    the

    distributionof a

    qualifier,

    and

    many

    other

    possible pairs.

    Analyses

    of

    individual

    cognition

    are

    always

    based

    on such correspondences,

    never

    on

    naming ranges alone,

    foci

    alone,

    or

    mappings alone. The various

    responses

    to

    the

    330 stimuli

    produceamplequantificationor statisticalanalysis

    of

    patterns

    shared

    between

    individuals.

    (See

    the

    Appendix.).

    Figure

    1

    provides

    the

    English-speaking

    eader

    with a reference by which to

    gauge

    the

    Munsell

    system

    and

    to

    assess

    how

    speakers

    of

    Tzeltal and Tzotzil

    have

    named it.

    The

    figure

    shows

    derandomized

    namingranges and the foci of

    one English speakerwho used eleven termsto namethe 330 chips one by one.

    Figures

    2

    and

    3

    show that two

    Tzeltal

    speakerseach used five terms to name

    the

    same

    chips during

    he

    same

    procedure.

    Their

    data can be directlycompared

    with that

    of

    Fig.

    1.

    37

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    6/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER 1

    (1991)

    ^~~~~~1 1 1

    1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 22 22

    22 2 2223

    33 3 333 3

    334

    :..

    A

    B

    7 -4

    greg

    orange

    ~~~p i

    *

    F 2

    _Ya3*

    ---LLII B Xa

    H IIred

    _ ____ _l11

    v

    1

    1

    1

    1 1

    1

    1 1 1 1222222222233333333334

    11234567890123456789012345678901234567890

    D1 E I Illi

    _F]IE

    *

    ah

    1111

    1 1 1 1222222 222 333333334

    +1 2345678901234567890 12345678901234567

    890

    C

    '

    D

    *~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~

    G

    H

    i

    J

    ?J

    ocus

    FIGURES -3.

    (1)

    American

    English

    color

    categories: naming ranges

    and

    foci; male,

    age

    35,

    1980.

    (2)

    Tzeltal color

    categories,

    San Ildefonso

    Tenejapa, Chiapas,

    Mexico;

    naming ranges

    and

    foci;

    female, age

    60,

    1980.

    (3) Tzeltal,

    Tenejapa; male,

    age

    65,

    1980.

    Figures 4a-d show all naming ranges, foci and mappings from one

    Tzeltal

    speaker.

    The

    figures fully

    exemplify techniques of elicitation and

    display, and

    they

    reveal

    characteristics of

    early-stage color categorization that turned up

    time

    and again during the Mesoamerican survey. Figs. 4b-d represent

    mappings

    with lines, which allows the

    display of two mappings in one diagram

    so as to

    show their

    relationship and overlap. Mapping steps are bracketed by

    numbers

    embedded in

    each line.

    Mappings can also be depicted by shaded quadrates,

    as in

    Figs. 8b-c below.

    Usually the mapping of a category covers more colors than its naming range

    does;

    a

    category has greater

    breadth than the normal use of its name

    would

    suggest,

    and

    the

    mapping brings out this fact. Often a few uses of a

    name fall

    outside

    of

    the normal range,

    where they correspond to the peripheral steps of

    the

    broad mapping. For

    example, in Fig. 4a sak names bright red

    (Gl); k'an

    38

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    7/30

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    8/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER 1

    (1991)

    trials

    during

    the interview.

    This

    semantic relation is discussed in another

    study,

    where

    it

    is called

    COEXTENSIVITY.3

    ost

    Tzeltal and Tzotzil consultants

    mapped

    both red-focused

    ?ah

    or

    ?oh

    and

    yellow-focused

    k'an or k'on

    throughout

    one

    warm category, although some did not commingle the terms throughout red

    and

    yellow

    colors

    during

    the

    naming

    procedure.

    Coextensivity

    occurs in

    many

    languages

    of the Mesoamerican

    sample;

    it

    is

    reported

    in color

    naming by

    other

    investigators

    both in Mesoamerica

    and elsewhere

    (Merrifield

    1971:264;

    Hage

    &

    Hawkes

    1975:297),

    and it is not confined

    to the warm

    category.

    (See

    Figs.

    8b-c below for coextensive

    mappings

    of the cool

    category.)

    For reasons

    that

    are

    explained

    in

    the work on

    coextensive

    ranges

    (cited

    in n.

    3),

    one

    range

    is

    always

    slightly

    smaller

    and more skewed than the

    other;

    the smaller

    range

    has

    a less central focus or even a

    polarized

    focus,

    which occurs

    on the

    periphery

    of the category-as in Figs. 4a-b at 140-or even outside of the category

    margin,

    as in

    Figs.

    8a-b at H38.

    Fig.

    4c shows the

    mappings

    of sak and

    2ihk',

    'light-warm'

    versus

    'dark-cool',

    which

    together

    cover all but ten colors of the Munsell

    array

    in a

    relation

    of

    complementation;

    neither

    range

    covers

    the

    opposite

    focus,

    unlike the

    mappings

    of k'an

    and

    ?ah.

    The

    light-warm mapping

    of

    sak

    is

    unique

    to Tzeltal

    speaker

    #4

    in this

    sample

    from

    Tenejapa.

    Yet

    speakers #2, #3,

    #4,

    and #5 all rendered

    the

    dark-cool

    mapping

    of

    2ihk',

    two of

    the three

    Tzeltals who were interviewed

    in

    Amatenango mapped

    2ihk'

    in

    the same

    way,

    and

    five of the

    six Navenchauc

    Tzotzil

    speakers

    rendered

    a

    dark-cool

    mapping

    of

    2ik'.

    Fig. 4d shows that the mapping of 2ihk' encompasses the mapping of yas,

    revealing

    a relation

    of inclusion between the black-focused

    dark-cool

    category

    and the

    green-focused

    cool

    category. Yet,

    in

    Fig. 4a,

    the

    naming range

    of

    yas

    is

    highly salient;

    the

    green

    and blue

    portion

    of the

    mapping

    of

    pihk'

    is not

    corroborated

    by marginal

    uses of

    that

    name.

    However, Figs. 2, 3,

    and 5a

    pro-

    vide

    better evidence

    in

    naming

    data of the Tzeltal

    dark-cool

    category. For

    example, speaker

    #5

    (Fig. 5a) applies

    2ihk' to

    J0, F17,

    and

    F29,

    the colors on

    which the

    English speaker (Fig. 1)

    focuses

    black, green,

    and

    blue.

    The

    Tzeltal

    dark-cool

    category

    is

    more salient

    in

    dark than in

    cool colors.

    In

    sum, Figs. 4a-d demonstrate three types of semantic relation: (1) coex-

    tensivity (Fig. 4b), (2)

    inclusion

    (Fig. 4d),

    and

    (3) complementation (Fig. 4c).4

    Identification

    of Tzeltal basic color

    categories

    is far

    from straightforward,

    3

    During

    the

    Mesoamerican Color

    Survey,

    the

    coextensive relation between color terms first

    emerged

    in

    Uspantec

    data

    (MacLaury

    et

    al. 1979), and it seemed very odd at the time. It took

    three

    years

    thereafter to

    develop the hypothesis that coextensive terms each name the same cat-

    egory,

    but

    from

    different mental

    vantages.

    It

    took even longer to devise a numerical test (MacLaury

    1987b).

    The

    results

    eventually

    led to

    a

    theory regarding the role of spatial analogy and viewpoint

    in

    categorization

    and

    cognitive change (MacLaury 1992).

    4

    MacLaury

    1987b

    relates these

    semantic types to a continuum of change in the order stated

    here-(l), (2), and (3)-and adds a fourth type, 'near synonymy', to the earliest end of the con-

    tinuum. All

    four

    types

    are most

    exhaustively demonstrated in the warm category as it evolves

    internally

    toward a

    complete

    division

    into basic red and yellow categories, although the types

    pertain

    to

    categories

    of

    other

    colors as

    well. For the immediate purpose of comprehending the

    Tzeltal

    data,

    it

    is

    sufficient to

    note

    that

    each type of semantic relation differs from the others and

    that

    one of

    them, coextensivity, has not been recognized as a distinctive type in prior literature.

    40

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    9/30

    MEASURING VARIATION

    IN

    COLOR

    SEMANTICS

    A

    ia /ESr

    1 1 1 1 1 1

    t T i l l

    t i t r t t

    | I

    @ / / / / / / / X S _ g : :

    l T -

    fl-

    1w

    R

    IIIS

    W J

    1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2

    33333333 3 3 4

    e

    1234567890123456789012345678901234567890

    C

    7+4 111111

    1

    111 1111

    1111111111

    45

    7 _ G

    6 _ ' 8

    7 x 1 1 3

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1m

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 i 3 1 - 4 4 - 5

    B

    E

    . .... .

    -

    _

    _^

    +

    ?

    *

    .

    ___ll

    "

    1,

    ' '

    5

    . . .

    7 | W 3

    E

    F

    7-|2%1111

    1111111

    1I11111 11111111111 I

    ZC11

    7 1 l l 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1

    1 1 1 * - 5

    GL

    3

    H

    3

    7 ^ . 1 1 . 1 1 1 1 ' 1 . 1 . 1 1 1 . 1 l 1 1 1 1 1 . 1

    1 t - 5

    J

    B^ b

    r

    -J

    71ihk'

    *

    naming --mapping

    ya?

    mB

    naming

    111111111

    mapping

    1

    1

    focus

    FIGURES 5a-b.

    Tzeltal,

    Tenejapa,

    (a)

    naming ranges

    and

    foci,

    (b)

    mappings;

    male,

    age

    90,

    1980.

    using

    Berlin&

    Kay's

    criteriaof

    (a)

    salience

    and

    (b)

    not

    being

    encompassed

    by

    another

    category

    (superordination, nclusion,

    nonhyponymy).

    Tzeltal

    speaker

    #4

    (Fig.

    4)

    probably

    has three

    basic

    color

    categories

    of

    white,

    dark-cool,

    and

    warm,

    although

    she names

    color with

    five

    salient

    terms.

    Two

    terms coexten-

    sively

    name

    one

    warm

    category,

    k'an

    and

    ?ah. The cool

    category

    yas

    is not

    basic,

    because the

    basic

    dark-cool

    category

    pihk'

    ncompasses

    all

    of

    yas;

    how-

    ever,

    the

    interpretation

    s

    complicated

    by

    the

    low

    salience of

    2ihk'

    amonggreen

    and blue.

    The

    white-focused

    light-warm

    category

    might

    be

    vestigial;

    at

    least,

    unlike

    dark-cool

    2ihk',

    the

    light-warm

    range

    is not

    shared

    with

    other

    Tzeltal

    interviewees.It

    is

    interpreted

    as

    a basic

    white

    category

    n

    spite

    of its

    nonsalient

    range

    throughout

    yellow

    and red.

    The

    interpretation

    f

    three

    basic

    categories

    classifies Tzeltal speaker#4 at Berlin& Kay's StageII, andspeakers#2, #3,

    and

    #5 are also

    at

    Stage

    II.

    (See

    n.

    14

    below

    for a

    description

    of the

    stages.)

    But

    this

    classification s not

    clear-cut,

    becauseall

    the

    individuals

    are

    advancing

    to the

    next

    stage,

    IIIa,

    and

    each shows

    a

    different

    degree

    of

    progress.Likewise,

    Tzeltal

    speakers

    #6,

    #7,

    and

    #8

    represent

    distinct

    phases

    of

    Stage

    IIIa.

    Before

    further

    analysis

    of the

    Tzeltal

    data

    (in

    ?5.3),

    there

    should

    be some

    attempt

    to

    account

    for these

    dynamics.

    COGNITIVE

    DYNAMICS

    4.

    Why

    do

    languages

    differ n

    number

    of

    basic

    color

    categories?

    For

    example,

    why does English have eleven basic color categories while Tzeltal only has

    about

    three?

    Although

    'societal

    complexity' might

    have

    much to

    do

    with

    it,

    that

    notion in

    itself

    does

    not

    provide

    a

    cognitive

    explanation

    of the

    differences.

    Why

    do the

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal

    speakers

    differ

    from

    each

    other,

    even

    when all

    reside in

    the

    same

    small

    hamlet?

    m

    41

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    10/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER 1

    (1991)

    During analysis

    of results

    from

    the Mesoamerican Color

    Survey,

    a

    dynamic

    model

    of color

    cognition

    was

    developed

    to address the

    question

    of

    category

    change

    and a host of

    related observations

    (MacLaury

    1986)-including

    differ-

    ences

    among

    individuals

    in

    single

    communities

    as well as more dramatic

    dif-

    ferences

    between related

    languages,

    such as Tzeltal and

    Tzotzil,

    and also

    including

    maximal

    differences,

    such

    as those between

    Tzeltal and

    English.

    The model rests on four axioms:

    (1)

    people perceive

    six

    purest-possible

    colors,

    fifteen

    potential pairs;

    (2)

    the members of each

    pair

    are to

    an

    extent

    similar and

    to

    an extent

    distinct;

    (3)

    each

    pair

    differs

    in these

    extents;

    and

    (4)

    an individual attends

    simultaneously

    to

    similarity

    and to

    distinctiveness,

    and

    can

    reciprocally

    shift the

    strength

    of

    the

    attendances.

    For the

    purposes

    of this

    model,

    PERCEPTIONs the automatic

    registration

    by

    the visual cortex

    of

    pre-

    cortical neural response to wavelength, including the similarities and differ-

    ences that inhere

    among

    those

    signals.

    COGNITIONs

    the

    active

    devotion of

    attention

    both

    to

    similarity

    and to

    distinctiveness,

    and

    it

    is

    the

    selective em-

    phasis

    that

    a

    person places

    on the two attendances.5 The

    cognition

    is

    ceaselessly

    asserted and unconscious.

    Axioms 1-3

    pertain

    to

    perception.

    They

    are

    actually supported independently

    by research

    in

    visual physiology.

    Evidence

    in

    their

    support

    and their

    effect

    on

    human color categorization are detailed

    in De

    Valois

    & De Valois

    1975, Kay

    &

    McDaniel

    1978,

    and

    MacLaury

    1987a.

    In

    short,

    the

    purest imaginable

    ex-

    amples of red, yellow, green, blue, white,

    and black are the

    only unique

    color

    percepts; others, such as brown, purple, orange, pink, and grey, are perceptual

    blends

    of unique hues.6 Colors

    of some

    pairs, e.g. red/green

    and

    yellow/blue,

    cannot

    be

    seen

    in the same

    place

    at

    once

    and, thus, they

    contrast more

    than

    other colors. Among the

    hues

    of lesser contrast, yellow/red differ more

    than

    green/blue; green

    and blue are the most similar of

    all

    unique pairs.

    Since human

    visual physiology is invariant throughout

    the

    species,

    axioms

    1-3

    explain why

    there

    are

    universals of human color

    categorization (Berlin

    &

    Kay 1969, Kay

    &

    McDaniel 1978).

    Axiom

    4

    is

    cognitive,

    since

    only

    the

    mind

    elects to attend 'more or

    less'

    to

    what one sees. The axiom is truly a postulate, because it is supported only by

    its

    success

    in

    uniting disparate observations regarding the composition, se-

    mantics, variation, and change of color categories (MacLaury 1986).

    Individuals divide broad color categories into two or three narrow categories

    as

    they foreground perceived differences and suppress similarities; the pro-

    cesses are

    necessarily reciprocal. Thus broad categories, such as Tzeltal dark-

    cool, red-with-yellow, and green-with-blue, divide into categories equivalent

    to

    English black, red, yellow, green,

    and blue.

    Coupling

    of axioms 1-3

    (invariant perception)

    with

    axiom

    4

    (mutable cog-

    5

    'Attend'

    and 'attendance' have a precedent in psychological literature as technical terms (see

    e.g. laccino & Sowa 1989, abstract). The more widely used equivalent, 'attention' (e.g. Wright et

    al.

    1990), cannot be pluralized as 'attendance' can be, which is why the rarer term is used here.

    6

    'Unique

    hue' is

    used

    loosely.

    The

    term strictly pertains only to yellow, green, and blue; pure

    white and pure black are extremes of reflectance, and purest red contains a slight input of yellow.

    42

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    11/30

    MEASURING

    VARIATION IN COLOR

    SEMANTICS

    nition)

    allows

    change

    that,

    nevertheless,

    progresses

    according

    to

    universal

    reg-

    ularities.

    Berlin &

    Kay

    note that the

    maximally

    contrasting

    colors,

    such as

    red/

    green

    and

    yellow/blue,

    are never

    categorized

    under the same name in

    any

    language.

    A

    language

    that includes red and

    yellow

    in one

    category

    will

    always

    categorize green

    and

    blue

    together, although

    the reverse

    prediction

    does

    not

    hold;

    since

    red and

    yellow

    look

    more

    distinct,

    they

    will be

    separately

    cate-

    gorized

    prior

    to

    green

    and

    blue

    in an

    implied evolutionary

    order as

    speakers

    of a

    language increasingly

    attend

    to distinctiveness.

    The same

    engagement

    of

    perception

    and

    cognition

    explains

    other

    regularities

    of color

    categorization.

    First,

    change

    in

    color

    categorization

    is

    always

    contin-

    uous;

    it never

    progresses

    by quantum

    jumps.

    Although

    Berlin

    &

    Kay

    classify

    evolving

    color-category systems

    in seven

    stages,

    an individual can show

    a

    system that is between stages; for example, in Figs. 4a-d, superordinate light-

    warm and dark-cool

    categories

    are less salient

    than the cool and warm

    cate-

    gories

    that

    they

    subsume.

    Smoothly advancing change

    conforms to the

    model,

    which

    stipulates

    that

    people

    decrease

    their

    emphasis

    on

    similarity

    as

    they

    em-

    phasize

    distinctiveness

    more;

    the counterbalance of

    strength

    between the

    two

    attendances

    prevents

    change

    from

    surging

    ahead

    by

    a

    leap

    from

    an

    old

    system

    to

    a radically

    different

    new

    system.

    Second, as

    systems

    evolve

    by division

    of broad

    categories

    into

    narrow

    ones,

    every

    category undergoes

    internal

    reorganization.

    For

    example, the focus

    and

    judgments

    of higher

    membership

    within

    a category

    gradually

    migrate from

    its

    center to its edge; as attendance to distinctiveness is enhanced, half of a cat-

    egory

    is increasingly

    contrasted

    against the

    other.

    This

    process is seen in

    Figs.

    4a,c-d,

    where

    the lighter

    half

    of the

    dark-cool

    category

    is

    nonsalient.

    It is

    also

    seen in

    Figs.

    4a-b, where

    the focus

    of ?ah

    is placed

    at the

    outer extreme

    of

    the warm

    category-at

    140 in

    dark

    maroon.

    Third,

    reorganization

    involves

    an evolution

    of

    semantic

    relationships

    within

    a

    category

    from

    near-synonymy

    to coextensivity

    to

    inclusion

    to

    an

    eventual

    division and subsequent

    complementation.

    Each

    semantic type

    requires

    stronger

    attendance

    to distinctiveness

    than

    its predecessor.

    Evolution

    of

    se-

    mantic relationships passes through a continuum of variants that are inter-

    mediary to ideal

    types.

    Good

    examples

    of

    ideal types

    are

    rare (see

    n. 3

    for

    references).7

    Fourth,

    individuals

    shift

    toward stronger

    attendance

    to

    distinctiveness

    when

    they

    are

    exposed

    to novelty

    at an

    increasing

    rate.

    They

    emphasize

    distinc-

    tiveness to sort

    out new

    experience

    and

    are

    thereby

    motivated

    to evolve

    color

    7

    As

    a

    category gradually

    divides,

    it

    is not compulsory

    that

    it be named

    with

    two terms

    at

    any

    specific point

    in

    its development.

    But

    whenever a

    second

    name is

    adopted to

    supplement

    an

    older

    name,

    the two

    names must assume

    a particular

    semantic

    relation

    on the

    continuum

    of types.

    The

    segment of the continuum that the relation represents-that is, the type of relation-will be de-

    termined

    by

    the

    cognition

    of the individual

    who uses

    the names,

    that is,

    by the

    balance

    of attend-

    ances that he

    or she

    maintains.

    For example,

    Figs.

    8b-c

    below show

    that speaker

    #8 uses seleste,

    which is

    derived

    from Spanish

    celeste

    'sky-blue'.

    But she

    accommodates

    the loan

    word to a coex-

    tensive relation throughout

    the cool

    category.

    The

    other Tenejapa

    Tzeltal speakers

    of this

    sample

    have

    opted

    not

    to apply

    a second

    term

    to the cool

    category.

    43

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    12/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER

    1

    (1991)

    categories

    as

    part

    of a

    general adaptive

    strategy

    of

    regarding

    the

    world

    more

    analytically.

    Exposure

    to

    novelty accompanies

    societal

    complexity

    and

    culture

    contact,

    although

    it

    can

    also arise in small societies under harsh

    conditions

    where

    people

    must

    improvise

    by many

    means from one

    day

    to

    the

    next. Novelty

    directly

    links all such situations

    with

    the

    cognition

    that

    propels

    change

    in

    color

    categorization.8

    Fifth,

    different

    members of

    a

    society

    are

    exposed

    to

    different

    amounts

    of

    novelty,

    and some will notice

    novelty

    more

    than others.

    Thus,

    individuals

    are

    motivated

    nonuniformly

    to

    attend to distinctiveness such that each

    will

    attend

    at a

    different

    strength.

    Although

    each

    person

    is constrained

    by physiology

    to

    conform to

    a universal

    order

    in

    subdividing

    broad color

    categories,

    each

    will

    fall

    out at a different

    point

    along

    the

    evolutionary

    path.

    The foregoing provides a very 'individualist' account of the rampantvariation

    that is attested in the color

    domain,

    throughout

    Mesoamerica

    and

    elsewhere.

    Under that

    explanation,

    the

    only

    coherence within the domain

    is

    determined

    by physiology.

    Social factors

    do not stabilize

    color-category

    evolution

    at

    a

    predominantly

    uniform

    stage throughout

    a

    community.

    Only

    unvarying

    ex-

    posure

    to

    novelty might

    tend to stabilize at one

    stage

    a

    large

    proportion

    of

    a

    particular

    population.

    However,

    the

    individualist account

    does not

    explain

    the

    qualitative

    differ-

    ences

    among

    some societies.

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal and

    Navenchauc Tzotzil

    exhibit

    that

    type

    of

    difference,

    as

    will

    be shown

    in

    ?5.3

    and

    ?5.4.

    Their

    disparity

    is

    of

    interest, because it does not follow from axioms 1-4. The difference suggests

    that,

    in

    some

    cases,

    the individualist model

    might

    be combined with a

    socially

    oriented

    account of

    the kind

    proposed

    in

    Weinreich et al.

    1968.

    ANALYSIS

    5.

    Before

    describing

    color

    categorization

    in

    the two

    communities,

    and

    before

    attempting

    to

    integrate

    individual and

    social

    models,

    I will

    provide

    some

    rel-

    evant

    ethnographic

    information

    in the

    form of a

    sketch of

    social

    differences

    between

    Tenejapa

    and

    Navenchauc

    that

    seemed

    to

    be

    apparent

    during

    my

    visits

    there. These

    differences

    might

    influence the

    process

    of

    attending

    increasingly

    to distinctiveness.

    5.1. DIFFERENT OCIETIES.

    an

    Ildefonso

    Tenejapa

    and

    Navenchauc

    are

    corn-farming

    communities located about

    twenty

    miles

    apart

    in

    the state

    of

    Chia-

    pas,

    Mexico. Tzeltal

    is

    spoken

    in

    Tenejapa

    and

    Tzotzil is

    spoken

    in

    Naven-

    chauc. The

    two

    languages

    are

    closely-related

    members of

    the

    Mayan

    family

    8

    Languages

    in

    harsh

    environs, such as

    Eskimo

    (Heinrich

    1974)

    and

    Kung

    Bushman

    (Berlin

    &

    Kay

    1969:33,

    75),

    have

    more basic color

    terms than attendant societal

    complexity

    might

    predict.

    The MCS found that

    languages

    in

    isolated,

    backland

    villages

    have more

    basic

    color

    terms

    than

    languages of large, prosperous farming towns in the orbit of a city; the isolated areas are of poor

    soil,

    which

    forces

    people

    to

    improvise

    diverse

    livelihoods. Baines

    (1985:283)

    makes the amazing

    observation that

    Egyptian

    color terms

    remained at

    Stage

    lia 'from the

    mid-3rd millennium

    B.C.

    to the Middle

    Ages',

    in

    spite

    of

    vast societal transformation

    over the

    3000-year

    period.

    However,

    although

    the

    change

    was

    great

    and

    the

    society complex,

    the

    pace

    of

    change probably

    was slow

    in

    comparison

    to that which

    pervades contemporary

    Mesoamerica.

    44

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    13/30

    MEASURINGVARIATION N

    COLOR

    SEMANTICS

    which have

    been

    in situ for

    at least 1500

    years

    and

    which sharea

    pre-Columbian

    heritage. Radically

    distinct circumstances

    mpinge

    on the two

    communities.

    Tenejapa

    s

    at

    the

    end of

    a

    twenty-mile

    road

    (paved

    the

    year

    after

    this

    field

    work

    was

    carried

    out)

    from the nearest

    Hispanic

    center,

    San Cristobalde las

    Casas.

    Althoughmany

    outsiders

    visit,

    the

    remoteness

    of

    Tenejapa

    has

    shielded

    it

    from other

    parts

    of the

    country.

    In the

    context of the heretofore slow

    pace

    of

    change,

    its

    inhabitants

    are

    unlikely

    to sense

    destabilization

    and are

    either

    neutral n their

    attitudetoward

    ncorporating ovelty

    or

    eager

    to do so.

    During

    this

    fieldwork,

    a

    preservation

    officer from the

    National Institute of

    Anthro-

    pology

    and

    History

    informed

    disappointed

    ocal officials that

    they

    could

    not

    pave

    asphalt

    over cobbled streets or

    replace

    the

    beam-and-Spanish-tile

    oof of

    the colonial

    church with

    two-by-fours

    and

    corrugated

    tin.

    Middle-aged

    and

    older people are monolingualand wear indigenousdress, althoughmen have

    shed

    broad-brimmed ibboned

    headgear

    or commercialhats.

    People

    under

    age

    thirty

    are

    bilingual

    and wear

    commercial

    clothing.

    Old and

    young

    women

    alike

    comfortably

    underwent

    the color interview

    without

    seeking

    consent

    from

    a

    husband or father. A few

    men and women have

    married

    oreigners

    without

    even

    minor social

    consequence.

    Thirty years

    before this field

    work, the Pan-American

    highway was cut

    through

    Navenchauc; the village was

    catapulted rom the

    isolation of

    a

    moun-

    tain

    valley into the

    twentieth-centurymainstream.

    Interviews

    were

    arranged

    by

    a

    native

    interpeter-spokesman,

    who

    had

    troublefindinganyonewho would

    participate. He summonedfive of his young female relatives, two of whom

    would not

    volunteer until

    others had gone first.

    The man'smother,

    about sixty

    yearsold,

    steadfastly

    declined to be interviewed n

    spite of

    the

    generous

    wage,

    and

    a fifty-year-oldwidow

    consented

    hesitantlywith an eye

    toward hereward.

    Business was

    conductedin a house

    compound

    within close earshot

    of diesel

    trucks passing on the

    highway.

    Despite the presence of a

    primaryschool in

    Navenchauc, all but the

    spokesman

    and

    youngest

    interviewee were monolin-

    gual, and all

    wore traditional

    dress to the last

    stitch. Elsewhere all

    but a few

    young construction workers wore

    indigenous clothing,

    including the broad

    Tzotzil hat. In sum, it appears that the people of Navenchauc are trying to

    maintain

    inguistic and cultural

    continuity while

    undergoing

    ntensive, unso-

    licited

    contact with

    national society.

    5.2. EMBLEMS F

    IDENTITY.

    avenchauc is more closely

    engaged with the

    world at large

    than is Tenejapa.

    Inhabitantsof

    Tenejapamight still

    take their

    society for

    granted,but outside

    influence ntrudeson

    Navenchaucto the

    extent

    that

    no one can

    ignore it or turn it

    away. Its inhabitants

    must

    self-consciously

    observe

    tradition f they

    are to maintain t at all.

    Such a view can foster

    socially

    shared

    emblems or symbolic practices

    that shapethe

    cognitive

    organizationof

    color categorization.

    Tenejapadoes not broadcastthe

    presence

    of

    any particular

    value or shared

    point

    of reference that would

    channel

    the

    organization

    of color

    cognition,

    al-

    though

    an

    acceptanceof

    the

    outside world

    might encourage

    some Tzeltals to

    develop color

    categories

    toward the

    number

    used

    in

    Spanish.

    Color

    categori-

    45

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    14/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER

    1

    (1991)

    zation

    is

    evolving

    in

    Tenejapa

    in much

    the same

    way

    that it

    is observed in

    most

    other

    communities

    visited

    by

    the

    Mesoamerican

    survey,

    with neither

    clear

    incentive

    to

    speed

    it

    along

    nor desire to

    hold it back. It

    might

    be that

    the

    individualist

    model

    pertains

    fully

    to

    Tenejapa.

    In Navenchauc

    the emblem

    probably

    is tradition itself. The

    socially

    shared

    conservativism

    might

    encourage preservation

    of an archaic

    system

    of

    three

    essential

    categories

    and

    thereby shape

    the

    cognition

    of color.

    But the

    shaping

    of

    cognition

    involves

    further

    complexity:

    abundant

    novelty

    has fostered

    strong

    attendance

    to

    distinctiveness,

    which is

    uniformly

    evident

    among

    the data

    from

    all five Tzotzil interviewees

    under

    age

    thirty.

    The enhanced attendance to

    dis-

    tinctiveness

    is in conflict

    with the social

    requirement

    to

    maintain

    three

    super-

    ordinate terms. The

    result is

    a

    shared

    system

    of color

    categorization

    that

    is

    unparalleled by other survey data: highly salient Tzotzil categories named with

    Spanish

    loanwords

    are subordinated

    to the archaisms at a taxonomic

    depth

    of

    four

    levels,

    two levels

    deeper

    than

    any

    known

    system

    in Mesoamerica.

    Broad

    mappings

    of

    archaic

    categories

    are executed

    fully,

    but with a

    multitude

    of

    tiny

    steps-often

    a dozen

    or

    more-and,

    again,

    the

    numbers are

    unprecedented

    elsewhere.

    The

    tension

    between

    a

    cognitive adaptation

    and

    a

    socially

    pre-

    scribed emblem

    gives

    rise

    to an

    extraordinarily complicated system

    of

    color

    categories,

    described

    in ?5.4.

    5.3.

    TZELTAL

    COLOR

    ATEGORIES.

    ata

    from seven Tzeltal speakers,

    dis-

    played in Figs. 2-8c, represent the way that color-category evolution usually

    proceeds.

    That

    is,

    they

    show

    the

    range of

    variation and taxonomic depth

    that

    was observed in

    most

    of the

    communities

    that

    were surveyed

    in

    Mesoamerica.

    The

    demonstration

    is valuable

    for two

    reasons:

    it

    exemplifies

    the five

    principles

    of

    change

    that are

    outlined

    in

    ?4; and

    this

    typical case from Tenejapa

    contrasts

    with the

    extraordinary

    case

    from

    Navenchauc,

    where

    the same

    principles

    op-

    erate

    under a socially

    enforced

    distortion.

    In

    review, the

    mainspring

    of change

    is shift

    in strength

    of attendance

    from

    similarity

    to

    distinctiveness,

    a balance

    that each

    person

    manipulates

    in a

    private

    effort to

    accommodate

    novelty

    in

    general.

    In

    the color

    domain,

    the

    chosen

    balance concentrates ease of categorization at a particular point either on one

    level or

    between two

    levels

    in

    a perceptual

    hierarchy

    of

    potential

    degrees

    of

    inclusion.

    Among

    dark-cool

    colors,

    the

    potential

    levels

    of inclusion

    are,

    from

    broad to

    narrow, BLACK-WITH-GREEN-WITH-BLUE

    t

    level

    1, BLACK

    gainst

    GREEN-WITH-BLUE

    t level

    2,

    and

    BLACK

    gainst

    GREEN

    gainst

    BLUE

    t level

    3.9

    9

    A

    reviewer asks,

    'What does

    it mean

    to be

    at level

    1 or level

    2? How

    does one

    demonstrate

    that

    there

    is

    a

    hierarchy,

    and what

    exactly

    constitutes

    inclusion?'

    Inclusion

    means that one

    category

    encompasses

    the entire range

    (or 'contents')

    of another

    category

    plus more.

    In the

    cognitive

    branches of

    anthropology,

    psychology,

    and

    linguistics,

    relations

    of inclusion

    are

    conventionally

    discussed and diagrammed as a taxonomic hierarchy, with the most inclusive category highest

    (Berlin

    et al.

    1974, figs.

    2.1-6; Rosch

    et al. 1976:386;

    Taylor

    1989, fig.

    3.1). In that

    view,

    'level

    1'

    is

    highest,

    'level 2'

    is next down,

    and so forth.

    Although

    representation

    of a taxonomy

    as a

    hierarchy

    displays

    relations of inclusion more

    perspicuously

    than does

    any

    other

    schema, it

    is not

    proven

    to

    be

    more

    psychologically

    real than

    the alternatives,

    such as

    horizontal

    nesting

    of categories.

    At

    this

    point

    in the

    discussion of color,

    the levels

    consist

    of

    the potential

    groupings

    and contrasts that

    are

    46

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    15/30

    MEASURING

    VARIATION

    IN COLOR SEMANTICS 47

    A

    B

    ._ =

    B

    E-

    111111111111111111111111llllllllllllllllllll

    11lall

    llllll

    2

    16

    D

    4

    E

    4424111111lBlB 11111iii

    1i1iBiBiai Biiii 111i1i1,iBi

    iiiii

    2i .a-a.2a5

    4^

    D

    4a.a211111111111aa

    a

    11111111111111111,,

    1111111

    i

    1

    Ia

    1111

    11111

    (aa2

    5

    F.a.. a**

    *

    *

    B^

    I

    ll 11'111111'iii'iiBIBu11

    n

    1111'n

    BIn Ba la

    I' I

    B'lnBi B

    i'

    2..a.t-5.__

    i

    ........................................

    ---

    4_F

    BBIB II111111 I BIB 1111 11111 BIB laial BIl 11111 lBial

    2...a-aa.255

    1- G3 .

    4"'""""""""""""""""""5

    1 H.

    __

    ^ ______ 41-43.

    '1

    j

    111

    n11111

    >11111 m11111

    II B IlB

    1111

    1111n11111111BaB

    Il

    i

    iB

    3.>5

    - H 3

    rr;n";;" ";";";";"; ;";"?

    _ 1 2

    2

    .. 4 s

    J

    ihk'

    U

    aming

    mapping

    ya

    B

    naming

    111a11111111

    apping

    (3

    focus

    FIGURES a-b.

    Tzeltal, Tenejapa, (a) naming ranges

    and

    foci, (b) mappings;

    male, age 75,

    1980.

    An individual who strongly attends

    to

    similarity

    maintains

    one

    basic

    category

    at level 1, a dark-cool category as in Figs. 4b-c. An individual who attends

    less to similarity and more to distinctiveness

    maintains two

    basic categories

    at

    level 2, one black and the other

    cool,

    as do the Tzeltal

    speakers

    of

    Figures

    6a-

    8c. An

    individual who

    attends

    weakly

    to

    similarity

    and

    very strongly

    to dis-

    tinctiveness maintains

    three

    basic categories

    at level

    3; so,

    for

    example, English

    speakers

    contrast

    basic

    black, green,

    and blue.

    By establishing

    a

    certain

    balance

    of

    attendances,

    an individual

    determines where in the

    hierarchy

    he

    or she will

    make basic contrasts most

    naturally.

    Since

    the two attendances are

    recipro-

    cal-one weakens while

    the other

    strengthens-the preference

    for basic cat-

    egorization can slide smoothly through the perceptual hierarchy and can reside

    between the

    levels at

    any particular

    time. In that

    case,

    Berlin &

    Kay's

    char-

    acteristics of a basic category, such as superordination and salience, will be

    split

    between levels.

    For

    example,

    a

    superordinate

    dark-cool

    category

    will be

    named with less salience than

    the

    cool

    category

    that it

    encompasses,

    as

    in

    Figs.

    4a,c-d.

    based

    on

    perceived

    likenesses and differences

    among black, green,

    and

    blue; they

    are

    perceptual

    levels to which

    categories

    can

    pertain. However,

    the 'basic

    level'

    of

    categorization

    is

    cognitive,

    because it is the

    point

    within the

    hierarchy

    where an individual

    categorizes

    with least effort. Some

    Tzeltals situate their basic level between perceptual levels 1 and 2. Furthermore, 'level' in Figs.

    9-10

    below refers

    to

    actual relations

    of inclusion between

    categories,

    not to

    perceptual

    levels

    per

    se.

    Since these relations of inclusion are

    cognitive, they

    do

    not

    match the three

    perceptual

    levels

    one-to-one

    in

    every

    case.

    For a

    distinction

    between

    perception

    and

    cognition,

    see

    ?4.

    Since

    'per-

    ceptual

    levels' and

    'cognitive

    levels' can be

    differentiated,

    I

    call

    them both 'levels'

    throughout

    the

    discussion rather than

    designating

    them

    by separate

    terms.

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    16/30

    48

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER

    1

    (1991)

    +

    .1A

    E

    jua;

    '~

    G

    +?i~~?~?~

    ,: H i |.s.s.ska pel.s.

    W

    .

    *ihk?

    l

    1

    1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1 12222222222

    3 3 rm

    ^

    +

    1 2345678901234567890

    1 2345678901

    11I

    rosado

    asul

    B

    54

    im

    '1

    111''lllllElE

    '*lll5 a aai aa ii11*

    111ia' B

    '

    aa 11 11 115 DII

    E

    516111114

    II

    lIIII

    11111111111 111111111111 1 11111

    ii1ii i

    i

    4 11111 111111

    15

    4'

    11''"|4

    iiiii

    ii

    liiiiiiiiii

    i

    i iii ii

    liiliii

    5

    F

    6- 4

    111.. . . .

    .1 1 1 111111111111

    4

    aa

    l .ai .i .i

    a

    i .i

    a

    a . . .a.a a

    111111116a

    3-

    G 7>

    11?~~:111115 11iiiiiiii>11111f1) iiiiaiiaiai::ia::iii:i:iiiiiiiiiaiiiip1111111111

    11a1111'6aa

    3

    -

    H

    7111111111116

    11111111111111111111111111111111a

    11

    a

    ia~ 111111111111

    1 1 1

    11

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    16

    F

    4

    7

    ...................................................................

    ?

    ihk'

    naming

    mapping

    ya

    ffj

    naming

    111i1111a11

    apping

    ?

    focus

    FIGURES a-b.

    Tzeltal,

    Tenejapa,

    (a)

    naming ranges

    and

    foci, (b)

    mappings;

    female,

    age

    70,

    1980.

    @

    1111111111222222222233333333334

    1

    A

    1 2345678901234567890

    1 234567890

    1

    234567890

    B

    W?Iz

    focus

    H iQA3rsaoO oad

    seleste

    ?~

    @

    H38

    C

    -

    Bseeste

    mapped|

    seleste

    :

    .

    D. . .

    -

    five steps

    |

    ............4g

    rH

    ?:

    h

    mapped

    ^

    @

    J

    1111111111222222222233333333334

    +

    1234567890123456789012345678901234567890

    r

    age

    apped0.

    E..

    . .... ...~ - 2 - ,.,--_>-....

    G

    Hl 7.S

    *-

    nI-g

    ~ZZZC

    ~~r~C

    I I I . ......................., ;

    C

    |if

    age

    37 1980

    E~~~~~~~~~~~fiiiiPi

    i

    el est

    tiiiiiiiiiiiiiifemale,g 7 90

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    17/30

    MEASURING VARIATION

    IN COLOR SEMANTICS

    The Tzeltal

    speakers

    constitute two

    groups:

    speakers

    #2,

    #3,

    #4,

    and

    #5

    maintain a

    black-focused

    dark-cool

    category,

    whereas

    #6,

    #7,

    and

    #8

    do

    not.

    The first

    group,

    as mentioned above

    (?3),

    represents

    Stage

    II,

    the second

    Stage

    IIIa.

    Data from another

    individual,

    Tzeltal

    #A,

    are

    not

    shown;

    she

    falls

    into

    the second

    group,

    although

    she alone

    represents

    Stage

    IV.

    Speaker

    #8

    differs

    from

    the

    other members of

    the second

    group,

    because

    only

    she names the

    cool

    category

    with coextensive terms.

    A

    numerical

    correspondence

    among

    different data

    suggests

    that

    they

    accu-

    rately

    reflect

    cognition.

    The four who

    mapped

    the black-focused

    category

    2ihk'

    throughout

    dark-cool colors

    designated

    the cool

    category yas

    with a

    smaller

    naming

    range

    than those who

    mapped

    the black-focused

    category

    over

    only

    dark

    colors.

    Speakers

    #2, #3, #4,

    and

    #5

    named the cool

    category,

    respec-

    tively, 80, 101, 102, and 86 times-an average of 92 color chips per individual

    that

    were named with the

    term

    yas during

    the

    first

    part

    of the

    interview.

    Speak-

    ers

    #6,

    #7, #8,

    and

    #A

    named the

    cool

    category

    124,

    152,

    119,

    and 160

    times,

    an

    average

    of 139 uses

    of

    yas

    (or

    coextensive

    seleste)

    per

    individual.

    The

    between-group

    difference in the

    size

    of the

    cool-category

    naming

    range

    is

    explained

    by

    the

    model

    of a

    sliding

    preference

    for basic

    categorization:

    the

    four

    individuals

    of the first

    group

    named

    the

    cool

    category

    fewer

    times

    because

    it was more

    difficult and

    less

    natural for them to differentiate at that level

    than

    it

    was for

    the

    four members

    of

    the

    second

    group.

    The

    cognitive shift

    between

    attendances

    to

    similarity

    and to

    distinctiveness accounts for

    variation in the

    level of basic

    categorization

    and

    the

    consequent presence

    or absence

    of the

    dark-cool

    category;

    and

    the

    shift

    simultaneously accounts

    for the further

    cor-

    ollary

    of a

    difference

    in

    size

    of

    the

    cool-category

    naming range.

    This

    model

    of

    cognitive

    shift

    predicts,

    further, that a

    degree of precision

    that

    is

    sufficient

    for one

    individual who attends strongly

    to similarity

    is

    deficient

    for

    another

    individual

    who

    attends

    strongly

    to distinctiveness.

    The difference

    in

    overview

    surfaces

    in

    the

    color naming of the

    Tzeltal

    speakers at earlier

    and

    at

    later

    stages,

    who

    appear

    to name

    colors

    sloppily

    versus

    neatly; compare

    Figs. 2, 3,

    4a,

    and 5a with

    Figs.

    6a and 7a.10

    'Sloppiness',

    however,

    is

    an

    interpretation that would stem from the assumption that individuals directly

    categorize

    perception

    and name what

    they perceive,

    either accurately

    or less

    so. But

    the

    stronger

    attendance

    to distinctiveness that

    is characteristic

    of later

    evolutionary stages

    simultaneously promotes

    a sharper notion

    of accuracy,

    even

    when all

    speakers

    name

    colors with what seems

    to be

    equal care for

    exactitude.

    Finally,

    the

    model

    addresses the grades

    of

    variation within

    each group. In

    the

    first

    group,

    only speaker #4

    maps the white-focused

    category

    with

    a ves-

    tigial

    light-warm

    range,

    and

    speakers #2,

    #3,

    and #4 map the

    dark-cool cat-

    egory

    over

    lighter

    colors

    (rows B-J) than

    does speaker

    #5 (rows

    D-J). Speaker

    #5 appears to be retracting his dark-cool category toward the restricted dark

    range

    that

    characterizes the

    second group.

    10

    The

    naming ranges

    of

    speaker

    #8,

    like those

    of

    speakers

    #2, #3, #4,

    and

    #5,

    also

    appear

    'sloppy',

    but for

    a different reason.

    Speaker

    #8

    adopts

    seleste

    and asul

    as additional

    names

    of

    the

    cool

    category,

    and she

    applies

    coextensive

    tah

    to

    yellow

    more

    than

    do the other

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltals.

    49

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    18/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER

    1

    (1991)

    Speaker

    #6,

    who

    is at

    Stage

    la,

    belongs

    to the second

    group.

    However,

    his data show

    qualities

    that are intermediate to the two

    groups:

    he

    has

    retracted

    the

    black-focused

    category

    to

    exclusively

    dark

    colors,

    but he

    has

    not

    added

    terms

    in

    excess

    of the five

    that are shared

    by

    all

    speakers.

    Speakers

    #7,

    #8,

    and

    #A

    have added words for blends

    of

    color,

    such

    as

    kape

    'brown'

    (from

    Spanish

    cafe

    'coffee/brown'),

    rosa

    'pink'

    (Sp.

    rosa

    'rose/

    pink'),

    'in

    'purple'

    (Tzel.

    'yucca

    root'),

    and

    limun

    'chartreuse'

    (Sp.

    limon

    'lime/chartreuse')-a

    further

    corollary

    of

    stronger

    attendance

    to

    distinctive-

    ness.

    Speakers

    #7

    and

    #8

    are more

    discriminating

    than

    #6;

    they

    map

    the

    black-

    focused

    category

    over

    fewer

    colors.

    Speaker

    #8

    shows

    exceptional

    discrimi-

    nation

    by polarizing

    all foci

    except

    that of coextensive

    k'an;

    she

    placed

    her

    other six

    chromatic foci

    in the darkest

    rows,

    H-I.

    In sum, the cognitive model of reciprocal strengths of attendance to similarity

    and to distinctiveness accounts for the smooth

    gradient

    of variation

    throughout

    stages

    and

    substages

    of Tzeltal color

    categorization.

    Theoretically,

    different

    individuals have

    balanced the

    strengths

    of attendances as a

    personal

    adjustment

    to

    the

    novelty

    that each confronts

    in a

    day.

    Differences between

    the

    privately

    attained

    balances cause each

    person

    to fall out at a different

    point

    along

    a

    continuous

    sequence

    of

    color-category

    evolution that is

    constrained only

    by

    physiology.

    The

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal

    might

    aspire

    to some emblem

    of social

    identity

    in

    their

    use of

    color

    names,

    although

    such

    a

    construct

    is not essential

    to

    explain

    the

    current variation in Tenejapa.

    5.4.

    TZOTZIL

    COLOR

    CATEGORIES.Color

    categorization

    in

    Navenchauc

    Tzotzil

    is

    very

    different from that of

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal.

    Four

    of

    the six Tzotzil

    interviewees

    preserve

    the

    three-category system,

    and five retain

    the

    dark-cool

    category.

    But

    they preserve

    it at

    low salience and

    as

    the

    highest

    level of

    a

    four-

    tier

    taxonomy.

    All three

    levels of subordinate

    categories

    are

    of

    greater

    salience

    (see

    n. 9).

    The

    Navenchauc

    system

    is

    unique

    among

    the

    Mesoamerican

    data. An

    ex-

    planation of its unusual character requires some special construct in addition

    to

    those of

    the

    individualistic

    variety,

    perhaps

    a

    social variable. Labov

    (1963)

    puts

    forth the

    notion of

    a

    prestigious

    variant in

    his model

    of

    socially

    motivated

    phonological

    change.

    The

    concept

    of a

    socially

    valued

    linguistic

    form

    fits well with

    the hypothesis

    that the

    people

    of

    Navenchauc value the

    maintenance of

    many

    sorts of

    tradition,

    including

    early-stage

    color

    categorization.

    Traditional

    practices

    might

    provide

    a

    source of social

    identity

    under

    the

    relentless

    encroachment of

    outside

    influ-

    ences.

    Labov

    notes

    that

    'the

    linguistic

    form

    which

    began

    to

    shift was

    often

    a

    marker of

    regional

    status'

    (1972:178).

    His idea

    is

    amplified

    here to

    construe

    any time-honored behavior as a potential marker; the Navenchauc Tzotzil

    might

    seek to

    stabilize

    such markers as

    islands of

    identity

    in

    a

    world

    that

    shifts

    around

    them.

    Tzotzil

    data

    are

    represented by

    Figures

    9-11,

    speakers

    #9, #10,

    and

    #11;

    data

    from the

    other

    three

    speakers

    (#C,

    #D,

    and

    #E)

    are

    omitted.

    Speaker

    50

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    19/30

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    20/30

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    21/30

    MEASURING

    VARIATION IN

    COLOR

    SEMANTICS

    53

    +;;

    A

    Ba

    B

    H

    | ,dh I

    : .

    7

    D

    7

    jF

    1 1_

    -

    1111111111

    S F

    1234567890123456789 iS

    3

    (

    focus

    asul

    d

    kafe

    morero

    .

    B

    . : : : : : : :

    : : : : : : ; ;

    : : : : : : : : ; : : : : : : : : :

    : : : : : : .

    b

    C

    8

    Ilf4-I4w4.4

    3

    Illll

    lllllll

    1 6

    il

    1111111

    111

    3

    DH11lf

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    22/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER 1

    (1991)

    uba,

    kafe,

    selestre, limun,

    and

    asul

    (whose

    forms

    derive from

    Spanish

    uva

    'grape/purple',

    'cafe

    'coffee/brown',

    celeste

    'sky-blue',

    limon

    'lime/char-

    treuse',

    and

    azul

    'blue').

    According

    to this

    model,

    if the broad

    categories

    had

    not

    been

    adopted

    as emblems of

    identity,

    by

    now

    they

    would

    have

    retracted

    to narrower

    ranges;

    that

    is,

    dark-cool and

    coextensively

    named

    warm

    would

    have retracted to

    black, red,

    and

    yellow.

    Since

    the retraction is

    discouraged

    by

    a social

    value,

    other

    terms have

    been innovated to name the

    finer

    order

    of

    differentiation mandated

    by

    current

    thinking.

    Tzotzil

    speakers

    #10

    and

    #11

    show variants of

    the

    emblematic

    dark-cool

    category

    that is retracted to row

    D

    or

    E,

    and

    #10

    shows

    a variant

    of

    the

    subordinate

    complexity.'3

    Speaker

    #11,

    age

    50,

    shows

    only

    two

    taxonomic

    levels;

    she

    established color

    categories

    as

    an adult before intrusion

    of

    the

    high-

    way.

    5.5.

    INDEPENDENTEVIDENCE

    OF

    COGNITIVE

    IFFERENCE.

    The

    Tzotzil

    speak-

    ers

    map

    color

    categories

    with

    more

    steps

    than do the Tzeltal

    speakers,

    as

    is

    shown

    by

    the

    following comparison.

    The

    comparison presents,

    first,

    the

    range

    of each

    category

    that is shared

    between the

    two

    languages

    (e.g.

    'dark-cool');

    second,

    the

    name of the

    category

    in

    Tzotzil

    and in

    Tzeltal

    (e.g.

    2ik'/2ihk');

    and

    third,

    the

    average

    number of

    steps

    with

    which

    the

    speakers

    of

    each language

    mapped

    the

    category (e.g.

    13.5:8.5),

    always

    in the order

    Tzotzil:

    Tzeltal

    for

    each name and

    each

    average:

    'dark-cool'

    2ik'/2ihk'

    13.5:8.5;

    'cool'

    yo?/yas

    7.4:5.0; 'warm' with red focus?ohl/ah 5.6:4.0; 'warm' with yellow focus k'on/

    k'an

    7.8:4.0; 'white

    sak/sak

    4.5:2.8.

    The

    consistently larger

    number of Tzotzil

    mapping steps

    indicates

    that,

    on

    the

    average

    and within the

    present

    sample,

    the Tzotzil

    speakers

    attend to

    dif-

    ferences

    among

    colors

    more

    strongly

    than do the Tzeltal

    speakers.

    These

    num-

    bers are

    consistent with the

    greater

    taxonomic

    depth

    of Tzotzil

    color

    categorization,

    which

    I

    have also attributed

    to

    a

    stronger

    attendance to

    dis-

    tinctiveness. The

    taxonomic

    depth

    and

    the

    average

    number

    of

    mapping

    steps

    per category

    are

    independent

    orders

    of

    data.

    The

    averages

    are based

    on

    the

    Tzotzil data

    from

    Navenchauc and

    on

    the

    Tzeltal data both from

    Tenejapa

    and from

    Amatenango

    del

    Valle,

    where three

    individuals

    were interviewed. The

    differences

    in

    averages

    between

    Tenejapa

    and

    Amatenango

    are as

    follows:

    'dark-cool'

    2ihk'

    9.0:7.5;

    'cool'

    yas

    5.75:4;

    'warm'

    with red

    focus

    ?ah

    4.5:3.0;

    'warm'

    with

    yellow

    focus

    k'an

    4.25:3.5;

    'white' sak

    2.75:3.0.

    The

    Amatenango

    averages

    are

    lower

    than

    those

    of

    Te-

    nejapa,

    except

    for

    the

    sak

    averages.

    However,

    each

    Tenejapa

    Tzeltal

    average

    is lower

    than

    its Navenchauc Tzotzil

    counterpart.

    The

    averages

    of

    mapping

    steps

    for

    'dark-cool',

    'warm',

    and

    'cool'

    can

    be

    calculated

    only

    from the data

    representing

    the

    stages

    that

    contain

    those

    cate-

    gories, basic or secondary.'4 Two speakers at Stage IV, Tzotzil #E (with a

    13

    Tzotzil

    speakers

    #C

    and

    #D,

    whose data are

    not diagrammed,

    show

    similar

    complexity

    to

    a

    depth

    of

    four and three

    levels

    respectively,

    and

    speaker

    #E

    shows a

    Stage-IV

    system

    with

    two

    taxonomic

    levels.

    14

    Stages

    II-IV

    contain

    the

    following

    basic

    categories:

    II [white,

    warm,

    dark-cool],

    as

    mapped

    54

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    23/30

    MEASURING VARIATION

    IN

    COLOR

    SEMANTICS

    total of

    37

    mapping

    steps)

    and Tzeltal

    #A

    (with

    17

    steps),

    are

    excluded

    from

    the

    calculations,

    although

    their

    relevant numbers

    are: 'cool'

    yoslyas

    15

    steps:

    7

    steps;

    'white'

    saklsak

    2:1; 'black' 2ik'/2ihk'

    11:3. The three

    Stage-IIIa

    Tzeltals

    mapped

    black

    2ihk'

    with

    3, 2,

    and

    2

    steps, respectively.

    DISCUSSION

    6.

    The

    cognitive

    model of

    gradual change

    has

    implications

    for the

    growing

    theory

    of

    categorization

    (e.g.

    Rosch et al.

    1976,

    Medin & Smith

    1984,

    Taylor

    1989).

    These

    implications

    concern

    the

    basic

    level of

    categorization

    and

    the

    definition

    of basic color

    terms.

    6.1.

    MOBILITY

    OF THE BASICLEVEL

    OF CATEGORIZATION.erlin

    et al. 1974

    find that

    biological

    folk taxonomies

    can

    have as

    many

    as

    five

    major

    levels. But

    the middle level-which Berlin calls FOLKGENERICCATEGORIES is learned first

    by

    children. It

    pertains

    to concrete

    and

    imageable meanings,

    and it is

    named

    with

    simplest

    terminology,

    named with the

    largest

    inventory

    of

    terms,

    most

    frequently

    named,

    and

    named with least effort. This is

    the level

    of

    cognition

    on which

    people

    achieve

    basic

    partitions

    of their environment

    and

    interact most

    directly

    and

    comfortably

    with

    it;

    for

    example,

    tomato

    is a

    'folk-generic'

    cat-

    egory,

    as

    opposed

    to

    the

    superordinate

    category vegetable

    or a

    subordinate

    category

    such as

    Roman tomato.

    Rosch

    et al.

    (1976; cf. Taylor

    1989:46-51)

    rename

    this concept

    the BASIC

    EVEL

    f categorization.

    They

    couple

    the

    concept

    with prototype

    theory by

    showing

    that a

    BASICOBJECT

    ategory

    constitutes

    the

    most

    abstract

    level on

    which

    prototypical

    members

    manifest

    the maximum of

    attributes that typify

    the category

    and

    a minimum

    of attributes

    that

    characterize

    prototypes

    of other

    categories.

    Berlin

    and his

    coworkers

    further

    show that

    the

    'generic'

    or basic level is not

    immutably

    pegged

    to physical

    form,

    and

    others

    have

    repeated

    the

    finding (Rosch

    et al

    1976:432,

    Dougherty

    1978,

    Mervis

    &

    Rosch 1981:93).

    Thus,

    some items

    that are originally

    classified

    as subtypes

    of

    basic concepts

    are later

    promoted

    to basic

    status.

    For example,

    a few

    Tzeltals

    have

    promoted

    a divergent

    species

    of oak to the

    generic level,

    while

    the majority

    of

    speakers

    classify

    that

    taxon on

    the specific

    level

    (Berlin

    1972:74-79).

    Or,

    from another perspective, it could be said that some Tzeltals have moved their

    basic level 'downward'

    to incorporate

    the

    divergent

    taxon.

    The

    data regarding

    Tzeltal color

    categorization

    shows that

    in this domain

    the

    basic level

    is

    highly mobile.

    As individuals

    shift

    the

    strength of

    cognitive

    attendances from

    similarity

    to distinctiveness,

    the

    basic level

    of color

    cate-

    gorization

    moves toward

    greater differentiation

    and specificity.

    Superordinate

    categories

    fall into disuse and

    diminish

    in range;

    for example,

    dark-cool

    pihk'

    in

    Fig. 9b;

    IlIa [white,

    warm, black,

    cool]; IIIb [white,

    red,

    yellow, dark-cool];

    IV [white,

    black,

    red, yellow,

    cool].

    Stages II

    and

    IIIb

    can include

    cool as a nonbasic

    category,

    as mapped

    in Fig.

    4d. For these reasons, mapping steps are counted for dark-cool categories only from Stage-lI and

    Stage-IIIb

    data,

    for warm categories

    only from

    Stage-lI and Stage-IIIa

    data, and

    for white and cool

    categories

    from data of

    Stages

    II, liIa, and

    IIb. Likewise,

    mapping

    steps of

    black categories are

    counted

    only

    from

    Stage-IlIa

    and Stage-IV

    data. Data

    regarding

    Stage-IV

    cool and white

    categories

    are

    counted separately,

    and data regarding

    red and yellow

    categories

    of Stage

    IIIb

    and Stage

    IV

    are not

    counted.

    55

  • 8/9/2019 Measuring Variability in Color Semantics

    24/30

    LANGUAGE,

    VOLUME

    67,

    NUMBER

    1

    (1991)

    retracts from

    green

    and from

    blue toward its black focus. Subordinate cate-

    gories

    gain

    in

    usage

    and

    expand

    in

    range;

    for

    example,

    cool

    yas

    broadens its

    range

    and is named

    more often

    during

    the Munsell interview. In

    effect,

    the

    basic

    categorization

    shifts

    from the level of

    combining

    BLACK-WITH-GREEN-

    WITH-BLUE to

    the level of

    contrasting

    BLACK

    against

    GREEN-WITH-BLUE. In

    the

    future,

    Tzeltal

    speakers might

    move

    their

    basic color

    categorization

    to the level

    of

    maximum

    contrast

    of

    BLACK

    against

    GREEN

    against

    BLUE,

    as have

    speakers

    of other

    Mesoamerican

    languages

    after

    they

    innovated a term for a

    separate

    basic

    category

    of

    BLUE

    MacLaury

    1986,

    figs.

    8.12a-c).

    6.2.

    SPLITTING

    CRITERIAOF BASICCOLORCATEGORIES.Berlin

    &

    Kay

    identify

    the

    basic

    status of

    a

    color

    category-a

    BASIC COLOR

    TERM-with

    operational

    criteria:

    (1)

    monolexemic,

    (2)

    non-context

    specific,

    (3)

    not included in the

    range

    of another term, and (4) highly salient.'5 Criteria (1) and (2) are not proble-

    matical

    here,

    but

    (3)

    and

    (4)

    warrant

    discussion.

    In

    Tzeltal,

    as the basic

    level

    of

    categorization

    moves from the level of

    BLACK-WITH-GREEN-WITH-BLUE

    o the

    level

    of BLACK

    against

    GREEN-WITH-BLUE,

    riterion

    (3)

    continues to

    pertain

    at

    the broader level

    while criterion

    (4)

    gains prominence

    at the

    more

    specific

    level.

    That

    is,

    'dark-cool'

    2ihk'

    of uneven salience

    continues

    to

    encompass

    consis-

    tently

    salient 'cool'

    yas.

    When

    does one

    stop

    calling

    'dark-cool' a

    basic

    category

    and start

    thinking

    of 'cool' as

    basic?

    This question has no clear answer, because change

    occurs

    gradually as the basic level continuously moves. Many Tzeltals place the basic

    level somewhere

    between

    the

    tight

    grouping of

    BLACK-WITH-GREEN-WITH-BLUE

    and the unequivocal

    contrast

    of BLACKagainst GREEN-WITH-BLUE;

    few

    have

    arrived at the

    latter,

    Stage

    lia.

    For this reason, it is difficult

    to specify

    the

    exact

    number

    of

    basic

    color

    categories that each Tzeltal speaker

    maintains.

    The fact that

    there

    is flux

    in

    basic status is not

    an

    indictment

    of the