relaţia dintre ie şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari - teză...

129
THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND BURNOUT AMONG POSTGRADUATE UNIVERSITY STUDENTS by MANDY WEINSTEIN Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree MASTER OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGY in the FACULTY OF HUMANITIES at the UNIVERSITY OF JOHANNESBURG SUPERVISOR: DR KARINA DE BRUIN JUNE 2010

Upload: dana250

Post on 09-Dec-2015

7 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND

BURNOUT AMONG POSTGRADUATE UNIVERSITY STUDENTS

by

MANDY WEINSTEIN

Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree

MASTER OF ARTS

IN PSYCHOLOGY

in the

FACULTY OF HUMANITIES

at the

UNIVERSITY OF JOHANNESBURG

SUPERVISOR: DR KARINA DE BRUIN

JUNE 2010

Page 2: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to whole-heartedly thank the following people and organisations:

The Almighty, G-d, who gave me the ability, strength and determination to complete this

thesis and provided me with the essential support I needed.

My parents, for their encouragement, support, guidance and unconditional love always.

My Grandmother, for her encouragement and concern throughout this time.

My husband, for his love and support in every area of my life.

My sister and brother-in-law for their support, understanding and encouragement.

My mother-in-law, sister-in-law and brother-in-law for their support.

My supervisor, Dr Karina De Bruin, whose patience, encouragement, support and

guidance was of the highest standard. I have truly learned so much from you.

The students who gave of their time and provided valuable data for this study.

All my friends and family, for their motivation and understanding.

STATKON for assisting me in capturing and analysing the data for this study.

The National Research Foundation (NRF), for assisting me with a bursary.

Page 3: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................ I

LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................................III

CHAPTER 1 .............................................................................................................................. 1

1. INTRODUCTION, PROBLEM STATEMENT AND FRAMEWORK OF THE STUDY ............. 1

1.1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 1

1.2 RESEARCH PROBLEM, RATIONALE AND AIMS OF THE STUDY .............................................. 2

1.3 RESEARCH QUESTIONS .................................................................................................. 4

1.4 DEFINITION OF KEY TERMS ............................................................................................. 4

1.4.1 Burnout .................................................................................................................... 4

1.4.1.1 Emotional exhaustion ........................................................................................ 5

1.4.1.2 Cynicism ........................................................................................................... 5

1.4.1.3 Professional efficacy ......................................................................................... 6

1.4.2 Emotional intelligence............................................................................................... 6

1.4.3 Student ..................................................................................................................... 6

1.5 OVERVIEW OF THE RESEARCH ........................................................................................ 7

CHAPTER 2 .............................................................................................................................. 8

2. EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ............................................................................................... 8

2.1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 8

2.2 EMOTIONS..................................................................................................................... 9

2.3 GENERAL INTELLIGENCE ...............................................................................................10

2.4 TOWARD A THEORY OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ..........................................................12

2.4.1 Adaptational processes as the basis for emotional intelligence ...............................12

2.5 EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE PERSPECTIVES .....................................................................14

2.5.1 Ability emotional intelligence....................................................................................14

2.5.2 Trait/mixed emotional intelligence ...........................................................................17

Page 4: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

2.5.2.1 Goleman ..........................................................................................................18

2.5.2.2 Bar-On .............................................................................................................21

2.5.2.3 Petrides and Furnham ......................................................................................22

2.6 EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND PREVIOUS RESEARCH FINDINGS ......................................25

2.7 EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND BURNOUT-RELATED RESEARCH .......................................27

2.7.1 Emotional intelligence and stress ............................................................................27

2.7.2 Emotional intelligence and depression ....................................................................28

2.8 CHAPTER SUMMARY......................................................................................................29

CHAPTER 3 .............................................................................................................................30

3. BURNOUT ............................................................................................................................30

3.1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................30

3.2 DEFINITION OF BURNOUT ...............................................................................................30

3.2.1 Freudenberger.........................................................................................................31

3.2.2 Pines .......................................................................................................................31

3.2.3 Cherniss ..................................................................................................................32

3.2.4 Maslach ...................................................................................................................32

3.2.5 Schaufeli and Enzmann ...........................................................................................35

3.3 MODELS OF BURNOUT ...................................................................................................36

3.3.1 Individual approaches..............................................................................................36

3.3.2 Interpersonal approaches ........................................................................................39

3.3.3 Organisational approaches ......................................................................................41

3.3.4 Societal approaches ................................................................................................43

3.4 INDIVIDUAL CONTRIBUTORS TO BURNOUT .......................................................................44

3.4.1 Personality ..............................................................................................................44

3.4.1.1 The big five personality traits ............................................................................44

3.4.1.2 Other personality characteristics ......................................................................47

Page 5: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

3.4.2 Coping skills ............................................................................................................49

3.4.3 Self-efficacy .............................................................................................................49

3.4.4 Demographic variables ............................................................................................50

3.5 IMPACT AND SYMPTOMS OF BURNOUT ............................................................................51

3.5.1 Affective/psychological symptoms ...........................................................................51

3.5.2 Cognitive symptoms ................................................................................................51

3.5.3 Physical symptoms ..................................................................................................52

3.5.4 Behavioural symptoms ............................................................................................52

3.6 BURNOUT IN THE WORK CONTEXT ..................................................................................53

3.7 BURNOUT AMONGST STUDENTS .....................................................................................55

3.8 BURNOUT AND EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE RELATED RESEARCH .......................................58

3.9 CHAPTER SUMMARY......................................................................................................61

CHAPTER 4 .............................................................................................................................62

4. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHOD ..................................................................................62

4.1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................62

4.2 RESEARCH DESIGN .......................................................................................................62

4.3 RESEARCH OBJECTIVES ................................................................................................63

4.4 RESEARCH QUESTIONS .................................................................................................63

4.5 RESEARCH METHOD......................................................................................................63

4.5.1 Procedure ................................................................................................................64

4.5.2 Ethical considerations..............................................................................................64

4.5.3 Participants .............................................................................................................64

4.6 INSTRUMENTS ..............................................................................................................67

4.6.1 Biographical Questionnaire .....................................................................................67

4.6.2 The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-TEIQue-Short Form .......................67

4.6.3 Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey .............................................................69

Page 6: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

4.7 DATA ANALYSES ...........................................................................................................70

4.8 CHAPTER SUMMARY......................................................................................................71

CHAPTER 5 .............................................................................................................................72

5. RESULTS .............................................................................................................................72

5.1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................72

5.2 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF THE SAMPLE ......................................................................72

5.3 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF THE MBI-SS ......................................................................72

5.4 THE RELIABILITY COEFFICIENTS OF THE MBI-SS .............................................................74

5.5 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR THE TEIQUE ....................................................................75

5.6 THE RELIABILITY COEFFICIENTS OF THE TEIQUE .............................................................76

5.7 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND BURNOUT ...........................76

5.8 SUMMARY OF THE RESULTS ...........................................................................................78

5.9 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................79

CHAPTER 6 .............................................................................................................................80

6. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION ......................................................................................80

6.1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................80

6.2 SUMMARY OF THE STUDY ..............................................................................................80

6.2.1 The aim of the study ................................................................................................80

6.2.2 Design, participants and procedure .........................................................................80

6.2.3 Research questions .................................................................................................81

6.2.4 Discussion of the results pertaining to the three dimensions of the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey ...................................................................................................81

6.3 EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ACCORDING TO THE TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE APPROACH

84

6.4 DISCUSSION OF THE RESULTS PERTAINING TO THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BURNOUT AND

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE .......................................................................................................85

6.5 LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY ...........................................................................................87

6.6 SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH..........................................................................88

Page 7: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

6.7 IMPLICATIONS OF THE STUDY .........................................................................................89

6.8 CONCLUSION ................................................................................................................90

REFERENCES .........................................................................................................................92

Page 8: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- I -

ABSTRACT

Burnout has been researched extensively within the work context, however, burnout

amongst the student population yielded a dearth of information. Burnout amongst

students can be considered as a loss of motivation to engage in academic study

(Mostert, Pienaar, Gauche & Jackson, 2007) and could place students’ academic

futures in jeopardy (Struthers, Perry & Menec, 20030). More research in this field was

required.

The purpose of this study was to ascertain the level of burnout and emotional

intelligence in a postgraduate university population. The study also aimed to assess

whether any relationship existed between burnout and emotional intelligence. The

sample consisted of 225 postgraduate participants from a large metropolitan university.

Each participant completed a biographical questionnaire, the Maslach Burnout

Inventory-Student Survey and the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire- Short

Form.

The Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey yielded three results. Professional

efficacy yielded the highest mean score, emotional exhaustion the second highest mean

score and cynicism obtained the lowest mean score. The Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire- Short Form yielded a relatively high mean for emotional intelligence.

The relationships between the scores on the measures of emotional intelligence and

burnout were investigated by means of Pearson’s product-moment correlation.

Significant correlations were found between the three dimensions of burnout and

emotional intelligence. There was a statistically significant negative correlation between

emotional intelligence and exhaustion (r = -0.257; p < 0.01). There was also a

statistically significant negative correlation between emotional intelligence and cynicism

(r = -0.366; p < 0.01). There was a significant positive correlation between professional

efficacy and emotional intelligence (r = 0.428; p < 0.01). It appears as if the higher the

level of emotional intelligence, the lower the levels of burnout specifically emotional

exhaustion and cynicism.

Page 9: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- II -

This study has implications for students who may suffer from burnout during their

university studies. It allows individuals who are involved with students to recognise the

huge impact that burnout may have on a student’s life; psychologically, physically,

cognitively and behaviourally. This study also provides information on how levels of

emotional intelligence can affect levels of burnout. Furthermore, an important aspect of

emotional intelligence is that certain areas of emotional intelligence can be learned and

increased. If students are taught to increase their levels of emotional intelligence, they

may be able to manage stress more efficiently.

Page 10: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- III -

LIST OF TABLES

Table 4.1 Gender and Racial Group Distribution of Participants ................................... 65

Table 4.2 Home Language of Participants .................................................................... 65

Table 4.3 Faculty Distribution of Participants ................................................................ 66

Table 4.4 Work Experience ........................................................................................... 66

Table 5.1 Mean Scores, Standard Deviations, Minimum and Maximum scores for the

MBI-SS .......................................................................................................................... 72

Table 5.2 Mean Score, Standard Deviation, Minimum and Maximum Scores for the

TEIQue .......................................................................................................................... 75

Table 5.3 Correlation Matrix of Emotional intelligence and three burnout component .. 77

Page 11: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 1 -

CHAPTER 1

1. INTRODUCTION, PROBLEM STATEMENT AND FRAMEWORK OF THE STUDY

1.1 Introduction

The term ‘burnout’ (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1988) was first coined by Freudenberger and

has been the subject of research since the 1970s. Freudenberger and Richelson (1980,

p.13) describe burnout as a “state of fatigue or frustration brought about by devotion to

a cause, way of life, or relationship that failed to produce the expected reward.”

Research on burnout has focused almost exclusively on the role of work characteristics

(Langelaan, Bakker, van Doornen & Schaufeli, 2006). Burnout has also been studied in

a variety of human service professions (Chan, 2006). Historically it was believed that

burnout only occurred in the human service fields (Morgan, 2009). However, research

and practice has shown that burnout also exists outside the realm of the human

services (Breso, Salanova & Shaufeli, 2007).

Some individuals may be more prone to burnout than others. Although emotions are

common to all human beings, individuals differ greatly in the extent to which they attend

to, process and utilise affect laden information of an intrapersonal and interpersonal

nature. This ability or characteristic can be termed emotional intelligence (Mikolajczak,

Menil & Luminet, 2007). There are two types of emotional intelligence: ability emotional

intelligence and trait emotional intelligence. Ability emotional intelligence is seen as a

cognitive emotional ability or a form of information processing (Murphy, 2006). Trait

emotional intelligence refers to a constellation of behavioural dispositions and self-

perceptions concerning one’s ability to recognise, process and utilise emotion-laden

information (Petrides, Federickson & Furnham, 2004). This research focused on trait

emotional intelligence. This study assessed individual perceptions of emotional

intelligence and thus the trait emotional intelligence approach corresponded with this

aim. Trait emotional intelligence has been found to be associated with lower levels of

stress (Mikolajczak et al., 2007) and may also be associated with lower levels of

Page 12: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 2 -

burnout.

1.2 Research problem, rationale and aims of the study

The transition from undergraduate to postgraduate studies often brings about

challenges for young adults (Parker, Hogan, Eastabrook, Oke & Wood, 2006). These

challenges include increased workload and responsibilities and a lack of balance

between part-time work and studies. Furthermore, students need to form new

relationships, modify existing ones and function independently as adults (Parker et al.,

2006). These challenges often result in stress, which may contribute to burnout. Burnout

can be described as “a prolonged response to chronic emotional and interpersonal

stressors on the job” (Maslach, Schaufeli & Leiter, 2001, p. 397). According to Farber

(1983, p. 3), burnout is characterised by “physical depletion, by feelings of helplessness

and hopelessness, by emotional drain and by the development of negative self-concept

and negative attitudes toward work, life and other people”.

Although students are not employed and do not hold jobs, from a psychological

perspective their core activities can be considered ‘work’. Students are engaged in

structured, coercive activities (such as attending classes) that are directed towards a

specific goal (such as passing exams) (Breso et al., 2007). Yang (2004, p. 287)

describes student burnout as “students in the learning process, because of course

stress, course load or other psychological factors, display a state of emotional

exhaustion, a tendency to depersonalisation, and a feeling of low personal

accomplishment”.

Burnout has been researched extensively within the work context (cf. Hakanen, Bakker

& Schaufeli, 2006; Martinussen, Richardsen & Burke, 2007; Maslach, Schaufeli &

Leiter, 2001; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998; Swider & Zimmerman, 2010). However, little

burnout research has focused extensively on the student population. Burnout amongst

students can manifest as a loss of motivation to engage in academic study (Mostert,

Pienaar, Gauche & Jackson, 2007) and can place students’ academic futures in

jeopardy (Struthers, Perry & Menec, 2000). The mental exhaustion experienced by such

Page 13: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 3 -

students may erode their sense of the value of university education. Furthermore,

student burnout may be an important predictor of professional burnout when students

enter the job market (Mostert et al., 2007). This is likely to cause losses to businesses

as well as a loss of self-confidence for the individual.

The negative consequences of student burnout extend beyond the negative experience

of the individual. Student burnout may also affect the general attractiveness of the

university for new students, with potential consequences for present and future

enrolment. Student burnout can also have a significant impact on the effectiveness of

the universities, which may in turn have distinct policy implications for higher education

institutions (Neumann, Finaly-Neumann & Reichel, 1990).

Studies have found that students exhibiting high stress reported significantly higher

consumption of alcohol, drugs and junk food, and exhibited lower self-esteem and

poorer sleeping habits than students who were not stressed (Weckwerth & Flynn,

2006). Other negative stress related behaviours in the student population include

suicidal ideation and smoking (Weckwerth & Flynn, 2006). When students become

burned out out these behaviours may increase and become fatal.

The dangers of burnout are well known and researchers have explored variables that

may contribute to an individual’s vulnerability to burnout. These variables include

personality (Bakker, Van der Zee, Lewig & Dollard, 2006; Morgan, 2009), temperament

(Langelaan et al., 2006), alexithymia (Mattila et al., 2007), self-efficacy (Brouwers &

Tomic, 2000), health (Montgomery, Mostert & Jackson, 2005) and depression

(Middeldorp, Cath & Boomsma, 2006). However, there is little research on the

relationship between emotional intelligence and burnout. According to Cipriano (2002),

burnout and emotional intelligence represent two ends of a continuum with emotional

intelligence at the high end of successful adaptation and burnout indicating a failure to

adapt to a harsh environment. It is possible that emotional intelligence represents a core

set of adaptive competencies that can protect the individual from the negative effects of

stressors and decrease the likelihood that an individual will suffer burnout.

Page 14: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 4 -

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between emotional intelligence and

burnout in postgraduate university students. This was accomplished through the use of

questionnaires assessing emotional intelligence and burnout. The Trait Emotional

Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue; Petrides & Furnham, 2003) was used

to assess emotional intelligence and the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey

(MBI-SS; Schaufeli, Martinez, Pinto, Salanova & Bakker, 2002) was used to assess

burnout. Postgraduate students from a large metropolitan university participated in the

study.

1.3 Research questions

The research questions in this research were:

1. What are the levels of emotional intelligence in a sample of postgraduate

university students?

2. What are the levels of burnout in a sample of postgraduate university students?

3. Is there is a statistically significant relationship between burnout and emotional

intelligence amongst postgraduate university students?

1.4 Definition of key terms

The following definitions are significant to this study.

1.4.1 Burnout

Burnout was first mentioned as a psychological phenomenon that occurred in the

helping professions by Bradley (1969 cited in Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998), who

proposed a new organisational structure in order to counteract ‘staff burnout’ among

probation officers. However, Herbert Feudenberger, an American Psychoanalyst, is

generally considered to be the originator of the burnout syndrome and provided the first

definition of burnout (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1988). Freudenberger and Richelson (1980,

Page 15: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 5 -

p.13) describe burnout as a “state of fatigue or frustration brought about by devotion to

a cause, way of life, or relationship that failed to produce the expected reward.” Various

other definitions of burnout also exist. The definition most acceptable and appropriate to

this study is Maslach and Jackson’s (1986 cited in Shaufeli & Enzmann, 1998, p. 31)

which states that “burnout is a syndrome of emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation,

and reduced personal accomplishment that can occur among individuals who do

‘people work’ of some kind”. Maslach and Goldberg (1998) state that the key

characteristics of burnout are an overwhelming exhaustion, feelings of frustration, anger

and cynicism and a sense of ineffectiveness and failure. Maslach (1993) further states

that burnout can occur in many occupational and non-occupational contexts.

Burnout amongst students can also be defined in various ways. Yang (2004, p. 287)

describes student burnout as “students in the learning process, because of course

stress, course load or other psychological factors, display a state of emotional

exhaustion, a tendency to depersonalisation, and a feeling of low personal

accomplishment”. Schaufeli et al. (2002) report that burnout among students’ manifests

as feeling exhausted because of study demands, having a cynical and detached attitude

toward one’s study and feeling incompetent as a student.

1.4.1.1 Emotional exhaustion

Exhaustion is a central concept of burnout and the most obvious manifestation of this

complex syndrome (Maslach et al., 2001). Emotional exhaustion involves feelings of

being emotionally overextended and depleted of one’s emotional resources (Maslach,

1998). The individual is incapable of performing (Montgomery et al., 2005) and cannot

‘give’ of the self at a psychological level (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). Specifically,

students feel exhausted because of study demands (Schaufeli et al., 2002).

1.4.1.2 Cynicism

Cynicism, also known as depersonalisation, refers to a negative, cynical or excessively

detached response to other people (Maslach & Goldberg, 1998). This response often

Page 16: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 6 -

includes a loss of idealism (Maslach, 1998). In the work setting depersonalisation is

marked by the display of negative and callous attitudes towards others. People are

often treated as objects rather than people (Furnell, 2007). Students are likely to

manifest a cynical and detached attitude towards their studies.

1.4.1.3 Professional efficacy

Lack of personal accomplishment or professional efficacy refers to a decline in one’s

feelings of competence and successful achievement (Van der Merwe, 2003). Maslach

(1993) states that reduced personal accomplishment/professional efficacy refers to

feelings of incompetency and lack of success and productivity at work. This is echoed in

the findings of Maslach et al. (2001). As a student, feelings of incompetence may arise

(Schaufeli et al., 2002).

1.4.2 Emotional intelligence

Emotional intelligence is defined by two broad approaches: ability emotional intelligence

and trait emotional intelligence. This research focused on trait emotional intelligence.

Trait emotional intelligence can be described as trait emotional self-efficacy (Mavroveli,

Petrides, Rieffe & Bakker, 2007). Petrides and Furnham (2003, p. 816) view trait

emotional intelligence as “a constellation of emotion-related self-perceived abilities and

dispositions located at lower levels of personality hierarchies”. The term ‘trait’ is useful

in describing the construct as it emphasises the strong relationship to the basic

dimensions of personality and the non-cognitive nature of the construct (Petrides &

Furnham, 2001).

1.4.3 Student

The term student refers to an individual who is participating in studies, often at a higher

education institution (Thompson, 1998). These studies cover the range of academic

programmes offered by the institution (Schaufeli et al., 2002). In this study, the sample

consisted of postgraduate students. These students have completed an undergraduate

degree and were in the process of completing an honours or masters degree at the time

Page 17: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 7 -

of the research.

1.5 Overview of the research

Chapter two provides an in depth and recent review of the concept of emotional

intelligence. It also contains an evaluation of constructs related to burnout and

emotional intelligence. Chapter three consists of a comprehensive review of recent and

relevant information pertaining to burnout. Research related to emotional intelligence

and burnout is also examined. Chapter four discusses the research method and design.

It provides details of the participants, the research method and the instruments used for

data collection. Chapter five presents the results of the data analyses. Chapter six

provides a discussion of the results and recommendations for future research. The

limitations of the study are also discussed.

Page 18: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 8 -

CHAPTER 2

2. EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE

2.1 Introduction

Cognition and emotion are frequently viewed as distinct and unrelated entities.

However, the way we feel is often heavily dependent on the way we think (Meyerhoff,

2007). Our brains make sense of what is happening and decide what it is that we are

experiencing. Individuals have different responses to the same situation. Some

individuals react in more adaptive ways than others. The capacity to react to a situation

in a healthy manner is referred to as emotional intelligence (Meyerhoff, 2007).

The concept of emotional intelligence was popularised by the work of Daniel Goleman,

a pioneer and critical proponent of research concerning this construct (Woitaszewski &

Aalsma, 2004). At the time of Goleman’s research, a movement known as Positive

Psychology was gaining broad acceptance. The basis of Positive Psychology is the

belief that in order to treat an individual it is necessary and useful to start by looking for

a skill or competency and build from there. An area in which an individual is functioning

well can serve as a basis for meaningful change. Positive Psychology emphasises

strengths that can be used to improve other areas of functioning. The concept of

emotional intelligence supports Positive Psychology as it has a number of clearly

delineated skills and strengths that could, with practice, be improved and enhanced

(Stein & Book, 2006).

Goleman argues that general intelligence (as represented by the Intelligence Quotient)

predicts approximately 20% of the variance in success in various areas of life. The

remaining 80% of variance can be explained by other factors (Woitaszewski & Aalsma,

2004), which are related to the construct of emotional intelligence. Goleman states that

emotional intelligence may at times be more significant than the Intelligence Quotient

(IQ) and that it can contribute greatly to many important life outcomes, such as

improved learning and better decision-making (Woitaszewski & Aalsma, 2004).

Page 19: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 9 -

Goleman further states that when people with high IQs flounder and those with modest

IQs do surprisingly well, the difference often lies in the individuals’ emotional

intelligence. Emotional intelligence includes abilities related to self-control, persistence

and the ability to motivate the self (Goleman, 1996).

In order to understand emotional intelligence, it is important that both emotion and

intelligence are understood. Intelligence refers to certain abilities, such as the capacity

to combine correlated ideas and separate unconnected ideas, to judge accurately, to

reason and to engage in abstract reasoning (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). The affective

(emotional) sphere of mental functioning includes the emotions themselves, moods,

evaluations and other feeling states, such as fatigue or energy (Mayer & Salovey,

1997). These two concepts are discussed in the sections below.

2.2 Emotions

Emotions are often regarded as primary motivating forces that arouse, direct and

sustain activity (Coetzee, Martins, Basson & Muller, 2006). Emotions can also be

defined as a “relatively short-lived positive or negative evaluative state that has

neurological and cognitive elements” (Lawler & Thye, 1999, p. 3). The biological basis

for emotions includes certain brain systems, such as the hippocampus and amygdala,

and neurotransmitters, such as catecholamines that are involved in most emotions

(Matthews, Zeidner & Roberts, 2004). In our emotional repertoire, each emotion plays a

unique role in accordance with its distinctive biological signature. For example, fear

causes blood to flow to the large skeletal muscles making it easier to flee. These

biological propensities to act are further influenced by our life experiences and our

culture (Goleman, 1996).

Ohman (2006) states that emotions are critical to humans and human cognition.

Emotions play an important role in the human rational thinking mechanism (Martinez-

Miranda & Aldea, 2005). The cognitive component of emotions includes affective

evaluations of the self and the situation (Matthews et al., 2004). Leget (2003) states that

emotions involve judgements about important things. For example, emotions influence

Page 20: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 10 -

how we act in social situations and contribute to optimal social functioning. Without the

ability to intelligently process and effectively manage emotional information, one cannot

effectively engage with others in meaningful relationships (Brackett, Rivers, Shiffman,

Lerner & Salovey, 2006).

In addition to affecting evaluations of the self and others, emotions also influence many

aspects of cognitive functioning, such as memory and attention (Brackett et al., 2006).

Emotion also alters thinking in various ways. For example, research indicates that

moods generally bias people’s thoughts (Mayer & Salovey, 1997).

The discussion above shows that emotions influence various areas of human life. A

human being is essentially emotional and therefore emotions influence everyday

behaviour (Martinez-Miranda & Aldea, 2005). When the amygdala (the area in the brain

where emotions are located) is damaged in any way an individual loses the ability to

perform essential human activities. An example of such an activity is the cognitive task

of decision-making. Individuals with damaged amygdalas cannot assign values to

different alternatives to prioritise them and thus cannot select the most appropriate

alternative (Martinez-Miranda & Aldea, 2005). Thus, emotions play an important role in

rational cognitive aspects of life. This link is important in order to understand the

foundation for the concept of emotional intelligence.

2.3 General intelligence

The concept of intelligence has been theorised for many, many years. W.L. Stern

(1912) postulated that the term ‘intelligence’ represents a global and constant entity

from a developmental point of view. His theory is based on the concept of ‘mental age’,

which refers to the typical intellectual developmental level of a person at any specific

age. In this theory a constant relationship exists between a person’s mental age and

chronological age. This ratio is multiplied by 100 and is known as an Intelligence

Quotient or IQ (Fancher, 1987).

Researchers have subsequently realised that there is more to intelligence than the

Page 21: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 11 -

cognitive abilities measured by traditional intelligence tests. Broader conceptualisations

and multiple components of intelligence have been proposed (Chan, 2007b). Gardner

(1993) suggested a theory of multiple intelligences in which he postulated that all

humans possess many types of intelligences, each located in a different part of the

brain. Included in this concept of multiple intelligences was the concept of a personal

intelligence, known as the interpersonal and intrapersonal intelligence. This personal

intelligence involves having an accurate insight into one’s own psychological processes

as well as understanding the psychological processes of others (Kaufhold & Johnson,

2005).

Following this development, Sternberg (1985, 1994) proposed a triarchic theory of

intelligence. This includes componential intelligence, experimental intelligence and

contextual intelligence. Componential intelligence refers to the cognitive components

required to perform certain intellectual operations. Experimental intelligence refers to a

person’s ability to deal effectively with novelty in new learning situations and to think in

terms of new concepts. Contextual intelligence relates to adapting to the socio-

cultural/physical environment and finding means of achieving goals (Sternberg &

Grigorenko, 2002).

The literature on intelligence describes many other theories of intelligence and cognitive

abilities, these theories include Spearman’s two factor theory of intelligence (Spearman,

1923). However, the theories of Gardner and Sternberg are linked to the concept of

emotional intelligence and as such are relevant to the present research. Emotional

intelligence is defined as an individual’s ability to monitor his or her own feelings as well

as the feelings and emotions of others, to discriminate among emotions and to use this

information to guide thinking and action (Chan, 2007b). This can be related to Gardner’s

concept of personal intelligence, which involves the ability to have knowledge of one’s

own and others’ emotions and inner processes in order to be fully self-aware and to

develop meaningful relationships. Sternberg’s concept of intelligence can be related to

emotional intelligence as defined by Bar-On (Stein & Book, 2006), which includes

components such as adaptation/flexibility (the ability to adjust to one’s environment,

Page 22: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 12 -

also referred to as contextual intelligence) and problem solving (the ability to think in

terms of new concepts, also known as experimental intelligence).

The conceptualisations above demonstrate that emotional intelligence can be defined in

different ways. Firstly, emotional intelligence can be defined as an ability, consisting of a

person’s actual ability to recognise, process and utilise emotional information (Petrides

et al., 2004). Secondly, as a trait/mixed approach involving a combination of behavioural

dispositions and self-perception about one’s ability to recognise, process and utilise

emotional information (Petrides et al., 2004).

2.4 Toward a theory of emotional intelligence

This section provides a conceptualisation of emotional intelligence as a competence for

successful adaptation to emotional events. Three aspects that have been found to

moderate emotion-relevant information processing, clarity, attention and intensity, are

also discussed.

2.4.1 Adaptational processes as the basis for emotional intelligence

Matthews et al. (2004) state that adaptation is a central process within emotional

intelligence. Adaptation processes are those processes that support the person’s

attempts to fulfil personal goals and minimise harm from external events within a

changing external environment. Emotional intelligence can be conceptualised as an

index of the person’s overall aptitude for success in adapting to encounters that provoke

emotion. Adaptive processes are found at different levels of biopsychological

organisation and are conceptualised in biological or cognitive-psychological terms.

Some processes, like the startle response to an unexpected stimulus, are controlled by

neural circuitry. However, other responses, such as the appraisal processes that

support evaluation of the personal significance of events, may relate to discrete

information-processing routines or to high-level cognitive processes. Individuals who are

high and low in emotional intelligence may differ in the way their major brain systems

respond to motivational stimuli, or in the way in which they interpret events (Matthews et

Page 23: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 13 -

al., 2004).

Gohm (2003) identifies three individual aspects that moderate emotion-relevant

information processing. These aspects are labelled clarity, attention and intensity.

Emotional clarity is the ability to identify and describe specific emotions and identify and

describe one’s own emotions and mood (Augusto Landa, Lopez-Zafra, Berrios Martos &

del Carmen Aguilar-Luzon, 2008). The ability to identify the emotions of the self and

others is essential to the construct of emotional intelligence (Mayer & Salovey, 1997).

Low clarity is associated with neuroticism, vulnerability to distress and unpredictability in

emotional situations (Gohm, 2003). Individuals with high scores on emotional clarity

have a greater ability to rebound from induced negative mood than their low clarity

counterparts. These individuals also show greater decline in ruminative thoughts

following a stressor (Extremera & Fernandez-Berrocal, 2005).

Attention to emotion is the tendency to observe, think about and value emotions

(Augusto Landa et al., 2008). Without some degree of attention to emotions, emotionally

intelligent functioning is not possible. Attention to emotions is positively associated with

public and private self-consciousness and empathy and negatively associated with

neuroticism. Individuals scoring high on attention show mood consistency on

judgements of risk (Gohm, 2003). People who do not pay attention to emotions and see

emotions as irrelevant are unlikely to engage in mood regulation. However, a person

who pays a lot of attention to his or her emotions and who sees emotions as very

valuable might also be unlikely to engage in mood management. Such an individual

appears to see emotions as relevant to everything. Individuals who report a moderate

level of attention to emotion are most likely to engage in mood regulation (Gohm, 2003).

In this regard Augusto Landa et al. (2008) refer to emotional repair, which they define as

an individual’s tendency to regulate his or her own feelings. Emotional repair is likely to

be important in adapting to emotional events; individuals who cannot adjust their

emotions may be unable to adjust to situations and are thus not able to achieve their

goals.

Finally, emotional intensity concerns the magnitude with which one typically

Page 24: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 14 -

experiences emotions. It is associated with measures of arousability/reactivity and

measures of somatic and neurotic symptoms (Larsen & Diener, 1987). Emotionally

intense individuals would seem to be the most likely to be aware of the influences of

emotions (Gohm, 2003). However, Lynch, Robins, Morse and Krause (2001) found that

affect intensity was not associated with stress but instead was often used by intense

individuals as an avoidance strategy. Intense individuals coped with stress by

attempting to suppress, inhibit or avoid relevant cognitive and emotional experiences.

These individuals would thus not be able to appropriately adapt to emotional situations.

The sections above explored the differences that people may have in experiencing

emotions. Two different emotional intelligence approaches are explored in the sections

below.

2.5 Emotional intelligence perspectives

Emotional intelligence can be viewed from two different perspectives. The first

perspective looks at emotional intelligence as an ability. This orientation states that

emotional intelligence should be seen as a cognitive emotional ability. The second

perspective, trait/mixed emotional intelligence, states that emotional intelligence can be

described as trait emotional self-efficacy (Mavroveli et al., 2007). Self-efficacy can be

described as being grounded in the theoretical framework of social cognitive theory

emphasising the evolvement and exercise of human agency; that people can exercise

some influence over what they can do (Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2010). These two

perspectives differ significantly and are discussed separately in the sections below.

2.5.1 Ability emotional intelligence

Ability emotional intelligence is viewed as a cognitive-emotional ability or a form of

information processing. It is related to traditional intelligence and measures optimal

behaviour using tests of ability (Murphy, 2006). This method for identifying ‘intelligent’

responses is based on the assumption that emotional knowledge is embedded in the

social context of interaction and communication and that the most common response is

Page 25: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 15 -

the correct response (Mayer, Salovey, Caruso & Sitarenios, 2001). With performance-

based emotional intelligence, the solution is not as definitive as with intelligence tests,

where there is generally assumed to be one correct answer for each question. Instead,

test takers are awarded points based on the percentage of respondents who endorse a

similar response (Van Rooy et al., 2005). Thus, according to this model of emotional

intelligence, it is possible to measure individual differences in emotional intelligence

skills. However, this measurement requires objectively normed tests of emotional

responding (Shulman & Hemenover, 2006).

Mayer and Salovey (1997) are the most prominent proponents of ability emotional

intelligence. They define emotional intelligence as the ability to perceive emotions, to

access and generate emotions to assist thought, to understand emotions and emotional

knowledge and to regulate emotions reflectively to promote emotional and intellectual

growth (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). This definition connects intelligence and emotion by

combining the ideas that emotion makes thinking more intelligent and that one thinks

intelligently about emotions. A person with these abilities is considered well-adjusted

and emotionally skilled. Deficiencies in these abilities render a person socially and

emotionally disabled (Tapia, 2001).

In order for ability emotional intelligence to be termed an intelligence, it must reflect

mental performance rather than simply preferred ways of behaving, or a person’s self-

efficacy, or non-intellectual attainment. Mental performance needs to measure emotion-

related abilities (Mayer et al., 2000). With ability emotional intelligence a productive

union of the cognitive and emotional systems exists. The cognitive system carries out

abstract reasoning about emotions, while the emotional system enhances cognitive

capacity. Individuals high in emotional intelligence perform four processes efficiently.

These processes are the ability to perceive emotions, the ability to understand

emotions, the ability to manage emotions and the ability to allow emotions to facilitate

thought (Mayer, Perkins, Caruso & Salovey, 2001). These four abilities are arranged so

that the more basic psychological processes, such as perceiving emotions, are at the

foundation and the more advanced processes are at the top of the model and are

Page 26: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 16 -

dependent on the lower level abilities. With each dimension, there is a developmental

progression of skills from basic to sophisticated (Brackett et al., 2006).

The first ability, emotional perception/identification, involves perceiving and encoding

information from the emotional system (Matthews et al., 2004). It involves accurately

noticing emotions in the self and the environment and expressing these emotions

appropriately in social settings. This facet may be important in adapting to stressors

because it directs attention toward stress-related cues in the environment (Lyons &

Schneider, 2005). The second ability, emotional facilitation of thought, involves further

processing emotion to improve cognitive processes with a view to complex problem

solving (Matthews et al., 2004). It is measured by assessing people’s ability to describe

emotional sensations and measuring an individual’s ability to incorporate mood into

thought processes (Brackett, Mayer & Warner, 2004). The third ability, emotional

understanding, involves cognitive processing of emotion (Matthews et al., 2004). It

involves emotions combining to form other emotions and the way in which emotional

reactions change over time (Lyons & Schneider, 2005). The final ability, emotion

management, concerns control and regulation of emotions in the self and others

(Matthews et al., 2004).

To summarise, Mayer and Salovey (1997) define emotional intelligence as the ability to

perceive emotions, to access and generate emotions to assist thought, to understand

emotions and emotional knowledge and to regulate emotions reflectively to promote

emotional and intellectual growth. According to this definition, people who are

emotionally intelligent are better able to identify their own and others’ emotions. These

individuals accurately express emotions, so others are not confused about the feelings

they are projecting. Emotionally intelligent individuals can use their emotions to aid their

thinking. They can also control their emotions in a way that promotes intellectual and

emotional growth (Epstein, 1998).

Petrides, Pita and Kokkinaki (2007) and Petrides, Niven and Mouskounti (2006) have

criticised the ability approach to emotional intelligence. They state that the

operationalisation of ability emotional intelligence is problematic because the

Page 27: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 17 -

subjectivity of emotional experiences undermines the development of maximum

performance tests. Petrides et al. (2006) add that the heart of the problem concerns the

inability to create items or tasks that can be scored according to truly objective criteria

and that can cover the domain of ability emotional intelligence comprehensively.

Other critiques concern the scoring methods used to assess ability emotional

intelligence. Two techniques are generally used, namely consensus (target) and expert

scoring. The emotional intelligence of a response is assessed according to either the

group (target) consensus or the correct response as identified by experts (Matthews et

al., 2004). Both target and expert scoring suffer from theoretical problems. In expert

scoring, evidence suggests that scores are higher for test-takers similar to the experts

(MacCann, Roberts, Matthews & Zeidener, 2004). Van Rooy, Viswesvaran and Pluta

(2005) state that another problem related to expert scoring concerns selecting the

experts– there are currently no criteria for establishing expertise in emotional

intelligence. Target scoring is problematic because the targets themselves may not be

able to express their emotions accurately, or they may report only pleasant emotions

when they are in fact feeling something else (MacCann et al., 2004).

In contrast to the criticism mentioned above, Daus and Ashkanasy (2005) argue that the

ability approach to emotional intelligence has clearly demonstrated solid psychometric

properties and can be used confidently in different arenas.

Those that criticise the ability emotional intelligence model are likely to subscribe to the

trait or mixed emotional intelligence models. The following subsection discusses

trait/mixed emotional intelligence.

2.5.2 Trait/mixed emotional intelligence

Petrides et al. (2007) describe the operationalisation of trait/mixed emotional

intelligence as straightforward. The construct consists of self-perceptions and

dispositions, which do not contradict the subjective nature of emotions. Trait emotional

intelligence consists of personality facets that are related to affect. Consequently, trait

Page 28: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 18 -

emotional intelligence can also be conceptualised as a broad construct of general

emotionality (Petrides et al., 2006).

Trait/mixed emotional intelligence is measured through self-report questionnaires. This

contrasts with ability emotional intelligence, which is measured through maximum-

performance tests that have correct and incorrect answers (Mavroveli et al., 2007).

Thus, the primary basis for discriminating between trait emotional intelligence and ability

emotional intelligence is the type of measurement approach used. However, the two

perspectives on emotional intelligence are not necessarily particularly different in terms

of their theoretical basis (Murphy, 2006).

Self-report emotional intelligence questionnaires measure broader concepts of

emotional intelligence than ability emotional intelligence tests. Concepts measured by

these self-report questionnaires include motivation, non-ability dispositions and traits

and global personal and social functioning (Tapia, 2001). Trait/mixed emotional

intelligence and self-report measures are also easy to use. It is also possible that the

perception of emotional intelligence may be as important as actual emotional

intelligence in predicting certain outcomes (Shulman & Hemenover, 2006).

Goleman (1995), Bar-On (Stein & Book, 2006) and Petrides and Furnham (2001) are all

proponents of the trait/mixed model of emotional intelligence. Their views on emotional

intelligence are discussed in the sections below.

2.5.2.1 Goleman

Daniel Goleman has been influential in bringing the concept of emotional intelligence to

the fore (Matthews et al., 2004). Goleman (1996) states that emotional intelligence

involves self-control, zeal, persistence and the ability to motivate oneself. His definition

includes the ability to regulate one’s moods and keep distress from preventing one’s

ability to think and to hope (Newsome, Day & Catano, 2000). Based on this definition,

Goleman proposed an emotional intelligence model that consists of five critical

competencies: (1) self-awareness, (2) self-regulation, (3) self-motivation, (4) social

Page 29: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 19 -

awareness (empathy) and (5) social skills (Brand, 2007).

Goleman’s conceptualisation of emotional intelligence is ultimately based on aspects

such as cognition, personality, motivation, emotions, neurobiology and intelligence.

Many researchers refer to this model as a mixed model of emotional intelligence

because it captures diverse psychological phenomena that embody both cognitive and

non-cognitive processes (Matthews et al., 2004). In support of the mixed model theory

Douglas, Frink and Ferris (2004) state that emotional intelligence is a hybrid construct

touching the domains of personality and social skills. Personality traits are defined as

enduring dispositions, while social skills can be learnt. Thus, emotional intelligence

contains elements that are stable as well as elements that can be learnt.

Goleman (1998) further suggests that emotional intelligence focuses on two sets of

personal qualities. The first set involves dispositional qualities, such as initiative and

empathy, and the second set involves trainable qualities, such as adaptability and

persuasiveness. Individuals are thus able to build and develop portions of emotional

intelligence. Goleman also believes that the balance and management of emotions

determine how intelligently individuals act and ultimate whether individuals succeed in

life (Pfeiffer, 2001).

Goleman suggests that the competencies associated with emotional intelligence relate

to four domains. These domains are defined by whether competence relates to self or

other, or recognition or regulation. The two aspects of self-competence are self-

awareness and self-management (Matthews et al., 2004). These aspects involve

knowing one’s emotions, managing emotions and motivating the self (Woitaszewski &

Aalsma, 2004). Competence with others can be broken down into social awareness and

relationship management (Matthews et al., 2004). These competencies involve

recognising emotions in others and handling relationships effectively (Woitaszewski &

Aalsma, 2004). Recognising emotions in others is also known as empathy. People who

are empathic are more attuned to the subtle social signals that indicate the needs and

wants of others (Goleman, 1995). The ability to regulate emotion in the self can be

viewed as adaptive or maladaptive. An example of an adaptive strategy is social

Page 30: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 20 -

support seeking. Maladaptive strategies include avoidance and substance abuse.

Emotion regulation in others refers to the ability to manage the emotions of others and

not be overwhelmed (Nelis, Quoidbach, Mikolajczak & Hansenne, 2009).

Goleman (1995) reports that a wide array of specific qualities, such as impulse control,

persistence, empathy, good moods, hope and optimism, are subsumed within these

broader components and are characteristic of emotionally intelligent individuals.

Emotional intelligence can thus be understood as “a master aptitude, a capacity that

profoundly affects all other abilities, either facilitating or interfering with them” (Goleman,

1995, p. 80). This statement reflects Goleman’s belief that emotional intelligence is

extremely powerful and impacts on people’s success.

Goleman’s theory has been criticised for being over inclusive and incorporating many

well-established personality constructs, such as empathy, motivation, warmth and social

skills (Murphy, 2006). The definition and the categories have been criticised for not

being related to each other (Murphy, 2006). Epstein (1998) states that although

Goleman's views appeal to people because of his emphasis on the importance of

abilities other than cognitive intelligence, they are not very useful for identifying a viable

construct of emotional intelligence.

Goleman’s theory draws attention to the limitations of cognitive intelligence in explaining

success in living. The theory emphasises the importance of several other abilities and

attributes, including emotional adjustment, social competence and ego strength. Ego

strength is seen as including the ability to tolerate frustration and regulate impulses. It is

unfortunate that the theory includes all of these aspects under the rubric of an undefined

concept labelled emotional intelligence. Goleman's theory is also limited in that it

ignores the concept of practical intelligence, which refers to solving problems in the real

world and which is different from both intellectual and emotional intelligence but equally

important for success in living (Epstein, 1998).

Page 31: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 21 -

2.5.2.2 Bar-On

Although Goleman’s name is rightfully associated with the popularisation of emotional

intelligence, Reuven Bar-On’s work has been equally influential (Woitaszewski &

Aalsma, 2004). Bar-On’s conceptualisation of emotional intelligence involves clusters of

established personality traits (Matthews et al., 2004). This conceptualisation correlates

highly with personality variables but not with cognitive ability (O’Connor & Little, 2003).

Bar-On characterises emotional intelligence as “an array of non-cognitive capabilities,

competencies and skills that influence one’s ability to succeed in coping with

environmental demands and pressures” (Bar-On, 1997, p.14). According to Bar-On,

these abilities should be conceptualised as a type of emotional competence rather than

an inherent intelligence (Brand, 2007).

Working from the mixed-model approach, Bar-On developed the Emotional Quotient

Inventory (EQ-i). The inventory is designed to measure awareness, understanding and

control over expressive emotions (Douglas et al., 2004). The EQ-i is a measure of the

non-cognitive abilities that seem to be the most critical for positive adjustment

(Cherniss, 2002). This model essentially focuses on an individual’s psychological well-

being and capacity to adapt to new situations (Ellis & Conboy, 2005).

The EQ-i assesses five broad types of emotional intelligence. Each of these higher-

order components is measured by various subcomponents defined by pools of items.

The subcomponents are subsequently summed to create each higher-order construct

(Matthews et al., 2004). The first type of emotional intelligence is measured by the

intrapersonal scale. This scale measures the extent to which respondents are in touch

with their own emotions, their self-confidence and their degree of self-satisfaction

(Newsome et al., 2000). People who score high in this area are able to express their

feelings and are independent, strong and confident in conveying their ideas and beliefs

(Bar-On, 1997). The interpersonal scale assesses the second type of emotional

intelligence, which involves how the respondent interacts with and understands other

people (Newsome et al., 2000). The third type of emotional intelligence is measured by

the adaptability scale, which measures how the respondent successfully solves

Page 32: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 22 -

problems and copes with demands (Newsome et al., 2000). This dimension assesses

how individuals adjust their emotions and behaviours to changing situations and

conditions (Parker, Summerfeldt, Hogan & Majeski, 2004). High scores on this

composite scale identify people who are generally flexible, realistic, effective in

understanding problematic situations and competent at arriving at adequate solutions

(Bar-On, 1997). The fourth dimension, the stress management scale, measures the

extent to which the individual can withstand stress (Newsome et al., 2000). People that

score high on this component can handle tasks that are stressful or anxiety provoking

efficiently (Bar-On, 1997). The fifth domain, the general mood scale, assesses a

respondent’s levels of stress and optimism (Newsome et al., 2000).

The EQ-i correlates with a wide range of existing personality constructs, as well as with

other theoretically relevant constructs, such as coping (Matthews et al., 2004). There is

also evidence that the EQ-i predicts other variables, such academic success in

university students (Parker, Hogan, Eastabrook, Oke & Wood, 2006), the presence of

clinical disorders and response to alcoholism treatment (Matthews et al., 2004).

Bar-On’s construct of emotional intelligence has been critiqued for being too broad and

containing too many aspects of personality. It has also been criticised for lacking

internal consistency and being difficult to evaluate (Murphy, 2006). Furthermore, Bar-

On’s model fails to include any cognitive abilities. The validity of such an approach is

questionable, since it reduces the concept of emotional intelligence to a metaphor.

Intelligence is understood as a cognitive feature associated with information processing.

If emotional intelligence is interpreted as a feature of personality alone, then the term

‘intelligence’ is no longer appropriate (Lyusin, 2006).

2.5.2.3 Petrides and Furnham

Petrides and Furnham (2003) view trait emotional intelligence as “a constellation of

emotion-related self-perceived abilities and dispositions located at lower levels of

personality hierarchies” (p. 816). Following the Eysenckian view (Eysenck, 1960; 1991),

Petrides and Furnham (2001, 2003) regard traits as dispositions and distinguish them

Page 33: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 23 -

from abilities. The term ‘trait’ is useful in describing the construct as it emphasises the

strong relationship to the basic dimensions of personality as well as the fact that this is

not a cognitive ability (Petrides & Furnham, 2001).

Petrides and Furnham (2001) state that trait emotional intelligence includes various

dispositions from the personality domain, such as empathy and assertiveness, as well

as elements of social intelligence, personal intelligence (Gardner, 1993) and ability

emotional intelligence (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). Social and personal intelligence and

ability emotional intelligence are included in the form of self-perceived abilities (Petrides

& Furnham, 2001).

Self-report emotional intelligence measures are more closely related to personality than

ability emotional intelligence measures (Bastian, Burns & Nettelback, 2005). Studies

have found low to moderate correlations between several of the Big Five personality

traits and emotional intelligence. Openness to Experience, Extroversion and

Conscientiousness have been positively correlated with emotional intelligence.

Neuroticism has been negatively correlated with emotional intelligence (Davies, Stankov

& Roberts, 1998; Warwick & Nettelback, 2004). Individuals measuring high on

Extroversion appear to have more knowledge about their emotional experiences, are

more accurate in their emotional perceptions and are more optimistic regarding their

abilities to repair their negative mood states. Individuals who measure high on

Neuroticism seem to focus more on their moods, yet do not understand their moods and

feel that they cannot regulate these moods (Shulman & Hemenover, 2006).

Individual differences in trait emotional intelligence appear to influence how people

respond to affect–laden emotional stimuli. A study using the Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire (TEIQue) found that high trait emotional intelligence participants showed

greater mood deterioration than their low trait emotional intelligence peers following

exposure to distressing stimuli (Sevdalis, Petrides & Harvey, 2007). These findings are

relevant to trait emotional intelligence theory and support the view that high trait

emotional intelligence is not always adaptive. The adaptive value of high trait emotional

intelligence will differ depending on the context. The findings of Sevdalis et al.’s (2007)

Page 34: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 24 -

research illustrate a basic tenet of trait emotional intelligence theory, in that trait

emotional intelligence is not viewed as a cognitive ability, competency or skill (Sevdalis

et al., 2007).

Although high trait emotional intelligence individuals show mood deterioration after

exposure to upsetting stimuli, these individuals are likely to be able to reduce their

negative moods more effectively than low trait emotional intelligence individuals. These

individuals have higher mood regulation and thus report more positive affect because

they are likely to be able to sustain their positive moods (Spence, Oades & Caputi,

2004).

In accordance with the idea that trait emotional intelligence is related to personality,

Petrides and Furnham (2001) conducted a study on the theoretical foundation of

emotional intelligence. Participants completed the Bar-On EQ-i and the Eysenckian

Personality Profiler. The Eysenckian Personality Profiler contains three factors, namely

Psychoticism, Extraversion and Neuroticism. The study found that the 15 EQ-i scales

did not disintegrate in the presence of the three Eysenckian factors. This provides

strong empirical evidence for the existence of a coherent and distinguishable trait

emotional intelligence factor. Therefore, the position of trait emotional intelligence in the

Eysenckian model of personality is theoretically meaningful (Petrides & Furnham,

2001).

Critics of trait emotional intelligence argue that the construct is strongly related to the

basic personality dimensions and often fails to account for criterion variance over and

above these dimensions. However, Petrides and Furnham’s (2001, 2003)

conceptualisation of trait emotional intelligence as a lower order personality trait renders

this criticism irrelevant (Petrides & Furnham, 2006). Shulman and Hemenover (2006)

state that dispositional emotional intelligence is not synonymous with personality but

instead predicts important outcomes above and beyond more generalised features of

personality. Thus, perceived emotional intelligence is a useful construct and predicts

meaningful outcomes independent of personality influences.

Page 35: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 25 -

This research made use of the trait approach to emotional intelligence. This concept

encompasses a broad range of dimensions, such as dispositions from the personality

domain as well as self-perceived abilities. The Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue) was administered to gain an understanding of the

participants’ levels of emotional intelligence.

2.6 Emotional intelligence and previous research findings

Emotional intelligence has been associated with a range of outcomes. In a broad sense

these outcomes can all be regarded as relating to quality of life (Austin, Saklofske &

Egan, 2004). Research has found emotional intelligence to be positively associated with

life satisfaction. To be more specific, trait emotional intelligence is conceptually and

empirically related to happiness and well-being (Furnham & Petrides, 2003). Trait

emotional intelligence also seems to be a stronger predictor of happiness than the Big

Five personality factors (Chamorro-Premuzic, Bennett & Furnham, 2007). Furnham and

Petrides (2003) report that a large amount of variance in happiness is determined by

people’s emotion-related self-perceptions and dispositions, including emotion

regulation, relationship skills and social competence.

Trait emotional intelligence has been negatively associated with psychological distress

(Austin et al., 2004) and depression (Dawda & Hart, 2000). Furthermore, correlations

have been found between trait emotional intelligence and borderline personality

disorder. Borderline personality disorder is characterised by impulsive behaviour,

unstable self-image, unstable interpersonal relationships and extreme difficulty in

emotion and mood management. Borderline personality disorder is negatively

associated with multiple aspects of trait emotional intelligence, including self-reported

emotion management (Gardner & Qualter, 2009).

Psychopathy and trait emotional intelligence are also related. Theoretically,

psychopathy is regarded as a heterogeneous concept consisting of primary

psychopathy, which is characterised by features such as cruelty and lack of affect and

secondary psychopathy, which is characterised by features such as impulsivity,

Page 36: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 26 -

neuroticism and aggression (Ali, Amorim & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2009). Psychopathic

people are infamous for their chronic and diverse failures of social adjustment despite

their adequate intellectual abilities (Malterer, Glass & Newman, 2008). These individuals

display a wide range of maladaptive antisocial behaviours that result in negative

consequences. These maladaptive behaviours may be attributed to deficient emotion

processing (Malterer et al., 2008). These individuals display lower levels of trait

emotional intelligence than controls (Ali et al., 2009). Individuals with primary

psychopathy are less likely to attend to emotion cues and less able to revise their mood

states once emotions are experienced (Malterer et al., 2008).

In terms of relationships, trait emotional intelligence has been found to be positively

associated with quality of interpersonal relationships (Palmer, Donaldson & Stough,

2002). According to Brackett, Warner and Bosco (2005), couples low in trait emotional

intelligence tend to have fewer positive relationship outcomes than couples in which at

least one partner shows high levels of trait emotional intelligence. Negative associations

have been found between trait emotional intelligence and loneliness (Saklofske, Austin

& Minski, 2003). Subjects who can manage others’ emotions seem to respond less

intensively to stressful situations and exhibit less suicidal ideation, less depression and

less hopelessness. These subjects also express more empathy and have better social

support that protects them from negative feelings (Hansenne & Bianchi, 2009).

High trait emotional intelligence individuals should also be more successful at meeting

the demands of stressful situations because they are better able to perceive, appraise

and regulate their emotions. More particularly, components of trait emotional

intelligence are related to a number of coping processes, such as rumination, social

support networks and the disclosure of trauma. This suggests that higher trait emotional

intelligence should be associated with better coping and the use of more effective

coping strategies (Bastian et al., 2005). Trait emotional intelligence is also positively

associated with task-focused coping and negatively associated with emotion-focused

coping (Saklofske, Austin, Galloway & Davidson, 2007). Individuals unable to perceive

and appraise their own feelings, thus measuring low on trait emotional intelligence, have

Page 37: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 27 -

difficulty directing their attention toward adaptive coping and thus employ more passive

coping strategies when faced with stressful life events. High trait emotional intelligence

individuals use adaptive-rational and detached coping strategies (Rogers, Qualter,

Phelps & Gardner, 2006).

High trait emotionally intelligent individuals have more adaptive coping strategies and

are likely to handle stress more effectively. Thus, high trait emotional intelligence is

related to academic success (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2009; Lyons & Schneider, 2005).

A study found that students who obtained high marks in university scored significantly

higher on trait emotional intelligence measures (Austin, Evans, Goldwater & Potter,

2005). Research findings also show that trait emotional intelligence is positively

associated with success in occupations that involve considerable reasoning using

emotional information (Palmer et al., 2002). In general, performance is enhanced with

high trait emotional intelligence. This includes performance in interviewing and

management as well as performance on academic and cognitive tasks (Lyons &

Schneider, 2005). Trait emotional intelligence is also likely to have an impact on

perceived job stress and the consequences of experienced stress and burnout (Brand,

2007).

This research focused on the relationship between emotional intelligence and burnout.

In the following section, research findings pertaining to emotional intelligence and

concepts closely related to burnout are presented.

2.7 Emotional intelligence and burnout-related research

Burnout consists of certain related concepts, such as stress, depression and fatigue.

Studies have shown that these concepts are also associated with emotional

intelligence. These associations are discussed below.

2.7.1 Emotional intelligence and stress

The link between emotional intelligence and stress relates to the idea that negative

emotions and stress are the result of some dysfunctional relationship between aspects

Page 38: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 28 -

of the self and the environment. The ability (emotional intelligence) to ‘read’ and

manage emotions in the self and others is a moderator in this process. In essence,

emotional intelligence accounts for individual differences in the capacity to process

information of an emotional nature and to be able to relate this information to wider

cognitions. Emotional intelligence is not about emotions as such but is more about the

way in which individuals effectively integrate emotions with thoughts and behaviour and

act to reduce aversive emotional experiences (Slaski & Cartwright, 2003). According to

Shih et al. (2009), if individuals could cope with difficulties through self-regulation their

problems could gradually be solved.

Ramesar, Koortzen and Oosthuizen (2009) view stress management as a component of

emotional intelligence. Certain forms of emotional intelligence may protect people from

stress and lead to better adaptation. Individuals with emotion management skills seem

to be able to better maintain a positive mood (Ciarrochi, Deane & Anderson, 2002).

Emotional intelligence has been found to be useful in reducing stress and improving

health and well-being (Slaski & Cartwright, 2003). Mikolajczak and Luminet (2008) state

that trait emotional intelligence is a significant predictor of both subjective and

neuroendocrine responses to stress. Students with high trait emotional intelligence

scores displayed less increase in psychological symptoms and somatic complaints

during exams than their lower trait emotional intelligence counterparts (Mikolajczak,

Roy, Luminet, Fillee & de Timary, 2007). Therefore, trait emotional intelligence appears

to moderate psychological and somatic resistance to chronic stressors or stressful

occupations (Mikolajczak et al., 2007). Augusto Landa et al. (2008) found that nurses

who score high on clarity and emotional repair report less stress, whereas those with

high scores in attention to emotions experience high levels of stress. Emotional

intelligence appears to be a protective factor against stress and its many

consequences.

2.7.2 Emotional intelligence and depression

According to Petrides, Pe´rez-Gonza´lez and Furnham (2007) and Dawda and Hart

(2000), trait emotional intelligence is negatively related to depression. Hansenne and

Page 39: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 29 -

Bianchi (2009) state that there are positive correlations between emotional intelligence

and subjective happiness and life satisfaction among undergraduate students. Subjects

that can manage others' emotions seem to respond less intensively to stressful

situations and exhibit less suicidal ideation, less depression, and less hopelessness.

These subjects also express more empathy and they have better social support to

protect them from negative feelings. In contrast, subjects that scored high on emotional

perception reported greater depression, hopelessness and suicidal ideation (Hansenne

& Bianchi, 2009). The study thus found that self-reported emotional intelligence is

related to emotional adjustment. In a different study, adolescents reporting higher ability

to discriminate clearly among feelings and to regulate emotional states showed less

anxiety and depression (Fernandez-Berrocal, Alcaide, Extremera & Pizzaro, 2006). The

tendency to not think about thoughts and feelings is associated with lower anxiety,

depression and paranoia and with higher self-esteem (Ciarrochi et al., 2002).

Emotional intelligence has also been negatively associated with fatigue. The

psychosocial variables of depression, anxiety, optimism, internal locus of control and

amount of social support each partially mediate between emotional intelligence and

fatigue (Brown & Schutte, 2006).

The research relating to emotional intelligence and burnout related constructs suggests

that a relationship does exist between burnout and emotional intelligence.

2.8 Chapter summary

This chapter discussed the concept of emotional intelligence. The two concepts that

make up emotional intelligence (emotions and intelligence) were discussed first. Next,

theories of emotional intelligence were discussed and ability and trait/mixed emotional

intelligence were defined. The different types of emotional intelligence were then

discussed. Finally, research findings concerning emotional intelligence and the

relationship between emotional intelligence and burnout were presented. In the

following chapter the discussion turns to the construct of burnout and traces the origins,

measurement and theoretical development of this construct.

Page 40: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 30 -

CHAPTER 3

3. BURNOUT

3.1 Introduction

Schaufeli and Enzmann (1988) describe burnout as a state of exhaustion similar to the

smothering of a fire or the extinguishing of a candle. Darkness replaces the vital spark.

This metaphor aptly describes the emotional state of being burned out. Burnout was first

understood to be a psychological phenomenon that occurred in the helping professions.

However, the concept has been extended to all professions as well as to the student

population.

In this chapter the concept of burnout is discussed. Many different definitions of burnout

are provided. Different theoretical models of burnout are explored. These models

include individual models, interpersonal models, organisational models and societal

models. The factors that contribute to burnout are discussed, with special reference to

personality factors, coping skills, self-efficacy and demographic variables. Symptoms of

burnout are also discussed in four different domains. The chapter then focuses on

burnout in the work context and burnout among students. Lastly, research related to

burnout and emotional intelligence is reviewed.

3.2 Definition of burnout

Burnout can be described as the state of utter depletion experienced by people in the

service and helping professions who work long hours and have an excessive and

intense workload (Yu, 2005). Various other definitions of burnout exist. However, there

is a general consensus among researchers that the symptoms of burnout have

attitudinal, emotional and physical components (Farber, 1983). These definitions refer to

symptoms such as mental or emotional exhaustion, fatigue and depression. The

emphasis is on mental and behavioural symptoms. These symptoms manifest in normal

persons who have not previously suffered from any psychopathology (Schaufeli,

Maslach & Marek, 1993).

Page 41: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 31 -

3.2.1 Freudenberger

Burnout can be considered to be a mental disorder that results from personal

characteristics such as intra-personal conflicts, dysfunctional personality traits and

ineffective coping mechanisms (Brand, 2007). Freudenberger and Richelson (1980, p.

13) describe burnout as a “state of fatigue or frustration brought about by devotion to a

cause, way of life, or relationship that failed to produce the expected reward.”

Furthermore, Freudenberger states that burnout cannot be understood through the

medical model. The medical model states that ill-health is biologically based pathologies

originating in the malfunctioning of genes, organs and cells in the individual body

(Clarke & Everest, 2006). A comprehensive understanding of burnout requires a

framework within which antecedent variables, of both a personal and social nature, are

explored in terms of how they impact on a person and in turn change the individual’s

view of the future. Therefore, it is important to understand the psychosocial context in

which burnout occurs (Freudenberger & Richelson, 1980).

3.2.2 Pines

Pines and Aronson (1988) report that burnout is a state of physical, emotional and

mental exhaustion caused by long-term involvement in situations that are emotionally

demanding. It is characterised by physical depletion, feelings of helplessness and

hopelessness, emotional drain and the development of a negative self-concept and

negative attitudes towards work, life and people.

Burnout tends to affect those individuals who were previously idealistic and enthusiastic.

Individuals entering a given profession with a cynical attitude are unlikely to burn out.

Individuals entering the profession with a strong desire to give of themselves to others

and feelings of helpfulness and idealism during their early years on the job are more

susceptible to severe burnout. In order to burn out, a person needs to have been very

passionate and motivated at one time. Thus, one of the great costs of burnout is the

diminution of the effective service of the very best people in a given profession (Pines,

Aronson & Kafry, 1981).

Page 42: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 32 -

Pines (2002) believes that the root cause of burnout lies in people’s need to believe that

their lives are meaningful and that the things they do, and consequently they

themselves, are important and significant.

3.2.3 Cherniss

Cherniss (1980) was one of the first to define burnout as a process in which

professionals’ attitudes and behaviour change in negative ways in response to job

strain. This process is more accurately explained as consisting of three stages. The first

stage involves an imbalance between resources and demands (stress). The second

stage involves the immediate, short-term emotional tension, fatigue and exhaustion.

The third stage consists of a number of changes in attitude and behaviour (Cherniss,

1980; Van der Merwe, 2003).

Cherniss (1980) introduces an important new element into the third stage. This element

involves the individual’s way of coping with stress. Although excessive job demands are

the root cause of burnout, a defensive coping strategy characterised by avoidance and

withdrawal fosters the development of burnout (Storm, 2001). The changes in attitude

and behaviour associated with burnout provide a psychological escape and ensure that

further stress will not be added to the strain already being experienced (Cherniss,

1980).

3.2.4 Maslach

Maslach and Jackson (1986, cited in Shaufeli & Enzmann, 1998, p.31) state that

“burnout is a syndrome of emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation, and reduced

personal accomplishment that can occur among individuals who do ‘people-work’ of

some kind”. Maslach and Goldberg (1998) state that the key characteristics of burnout

are an overwhelming exhaustion, feelings of frustration, anger, cynicism and a sense of

ineffectiveness and failure. However, the third edition of the Maslach Burnout Inventory

test manual broadens the concept of burnout and defines it as a crisis in one’s

relationship with work in general and not necessarily as a crisis in one’s relationship

Page 43: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 33 -

with people at work (Van der Merwe, 2003). The burnout concept has been extended to

various occupations as well as to non-occupational areas of life (Maslach, 1993).

Exhaustion is a central component of burnout and is the most obvious manifestation of

this complex syndrome (Maslach et al., 2001). Emotional exhaustion involves feelings of

being emotionally overextended (Maslach, 1988), emotional depletion and over-

involvement, as well as feelings of being overwhelmed by others and emotional

demands (Jordaan, 2005). The individual is incapable of performing (Montgomery et al.,

2005) and cannot ‘give’ of the self at a psychological level (Schaufeli & Enzmann,

1998). This exhaustion can also manifest in physical experiences such as waking up as

tired as when one went to bed or lacking the required energy to take on another task

(Van der Merwe, 2003).

Depersonalisation, also referred to as cynicism, refers to a negative, cynical or

excessively detached response to other people (Maslach & Goldberg, 1998). This

dimension often includes a loss of idealism (Maslach, 1998). In the work setting

depersonalisation is marked by the display of negative and callous attitudes towards

people and treating people as objects rather than people. Visible symptoms of

depersonalisation include derogatory language when referring to clients and withdrawal

from the job by taking longer breaks (Furnell, 2007). Depersonalisation is a type of

mental distancing that indicates that the employee is no longer willing to perform

(Montgomery et al., 2005). According to Cordes and Dougherty (1993), cynicism is

viewed as a type of coping, which is an acceptable and professional response to the

stressful situation. However, when this coping strategy becomes a habitual pattern the

person becomes dysfunctional because it disrupts adequate task performance

(Montgomery et al., 2005).

The dimensions of emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation are considered to be

the core dimensions of burnout (Pienaar & Sieberhagen, 2005). Distancing or

depersonalisation is such an immediate reaction to exhaustion that a burnout research

has consistently found a strong relationship between exhaustion and cynicism (Maslach

et al., 2001). In a study by Lee and Ashforth (1990) exhaustion and depersonalisation

Page 44: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 34 -

were found to be more strongly associated with psychological and physiological strain

than was the personal accomplishment dimension. The study found that helplessness

was also more strongly related to emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation than the

personal accomplishment dimension (Lee & Ashforth, 1990).

The third dimension of burnout is labelled lack of personal accomplishment and refers to

a decline in one’s feelings of competence and successful achievement (Van der Merwe,

2003). Maslach states that reduced personal accomplishment/ professional efficacy

involve feelings of incompetence and lack of success and productivity at work (Maslach,

1993; Maslach et al., 2001). Individuals experiencing a lack of personal accomplishment

view themselves negatively in terms of their ability to perform the job and their ability to

have personal interactions (Cordes & Dougherty, 1993). These individuals trivialise the

things at which they are successful and no longer feel they are able to make a

difference through their work or personal interaction. These feelings of inadequacy

directly affect an individual’s self-efficacy (Van der Merwe, 2003). This component of

burnout reflects the self-evaluation dimension of burnout (Maslach, 1998).

A lowered sense of self-efficacy has been linked to depression, stress and an inability to

cope with the demands of a job (Maslach, 1998). Depression and stress are linked to

burnout but the terms are not synonymous. Burnout involves physical or psychological

tension derived from demands that exceed available resources (Garland, 2002). Stress

is a temporary adaptation process that is accompanied by mental and physical

symptoms and caused by an imbalance between job demands and the response

capability of the worker. In contrast, burnout can be considered to be the final stage in a

breakdown in adaptation that results from the long-term imbalance of demands and

resources. Burnout is also accompanied by chronic malfunctioning at work. It is thus

possible to define burnout as a particular kind of prolonged job stress or the

consequence of chronic, ongoing stress (Storm & Rothmann, 2003b).

The concepts of burnout and depression are related but are differentiated in a number

of ways (Brenninkmeyer, VanYperen & Buunk, 2001). According to Schaufeli and

Enzmann (1998), burnout tends to be job related and situation specific rather than

Page 45: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 35 -

pervasive. In contrast, depression is generalised across situations and different life

areas. Depression is most often accompanied by guilt, whereas burnout generally

occurs in a context of anger. However, emotional exhaustion and depression overlap to

a certain extent (Storm & Rothmann, 2003b). Both concepts imply negative perceptions

regarding the environment, the self and future (Jordaan, 2005).

3.2.5 Schaufeli and Enzmann

Schaufeli and Enzmann (1998) propose an overarching definition of burnout that

includes the process characteristics of burnout. These authors define burnout as “a

persistent, negative, work-related state of mind in ‘normal’ individuals that is primarily

characterised by exhaustion, which is accompanied by distress, a sense of reduced

effectiveness, decreased motivation, and the development of dysfunctional attitudes

and behaviours at work” (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998, p. 36). Schaufeli and Buunk

(2003) state that this condition develops gradually and may go unnoticed for a long

time. Burnout is the result of a misfit between intentions and reality at the job. Burnout

can often be self-perpetuating because of the inadequate coping strategies that are

associated with the syndrome. Schaufeli and Enzmann’s (1998) definition of burnout

specifies its general symptomatology, its preconditions as well as the domain in which it

occurs. More specifically, the definition highlights one core indicator (exhaustion) and

four accompanying general symptoms: distress (affective, cognitive, physical and

behavioural symptoms), a sense of reduced effectiveness, decreased motivation and

dysfunctional attitudes and behaviours at work. Frustrated intentions and inadequate

coping strategies also play a role as preconditions in the development of burnout. The

burnout process is also considered to be self-perpetuating and may not be recognised

immediately. Schaufeli and Enzmann’s (1998) definition also specifies that the

symptoms must be work-related and that burnout occurs in ‘normal’ individuals who do

not suffer from psychopathology.

In conclusion, most definitions of burnout state that burnout starts with stress.

Individuals then feel that they do not have the emotional resources to cope with the

situation. They feel emotionally and mentally strained and their attitudes begin to

Page 46: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 36 -

change, eventually resulting in burnout. The way the individual deals with stress is

crucial to the development of burnout.

The current research made use of Maslach’s three dimensional explanation of burnout.

This theory provides measurable factors (emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation and

reduced personal accomplishment) with which to assess the concept of burnout. The

Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey, which consists of questions assessing

these three components, was utilised in the data collection process. Maslach (1993)

also recognises that burnout can occur in non-occupational settings. This research

investigated burnout in postgraduate university students.

3.3 Models of burnout

An increasing number of research initiatives have focused on the burnout construct,

however “a comprehensive theoretical model is still lacking” (Schaufeli & Enzmann,

1998, p. 101). Schaufeli and Enzmann (1998) distinguish four sets of theoretical

approaches to burnout: individual processes that emphasise the role of individual

processes within the person; interpersonal approaches focusing on demanding

relationships with others at work; organisational approaches that emphasise the

relevance of the organisational context; and societal approaches that focus on the

broader social and cultural dimensions of burnout.

3.3.1 Individual approaches

Individual approaches attempt to analyse burnout from a general psychological

perspective. One of these approaches, which views burnout as a failure to retain one’s

idealised self-image (Freudenberger & Richelson, 1980) states that burnout develops

when individuals believe in idealised images of themselves as super-competent and

inexhaustible. As a result of these idealised images they lose touch with their more

fallible or real selves (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). These individuals are very hard on

themselves for not doing and achieving more. They have a difficult time accepting

themselves and fear that others will also not accept them. They strive beyond the limits

Page 47: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 37 -

of their own health, constantly driving themselves and are never satisfied (Gold & Roth,

1993). According to this approach burnout results from unmet needs and unfulfilled

expectations (Lemyre, Hall & Roberts, 2008). In trying to live up to their idealised self-

images, burnout candidates typically use the wrong strategies, further depleting their

emotional resources (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

Another individual approach to burnout is based on a cognitive-behavioural approach

and grounded in social learning theory. This approach states that burnout results from

incorrect expectations regarding reinforcement, outcomes and efficacy (Schaufeli &

Buunk, 2003). Dysfunctional thinking habits are present (Blonk, Brenninkmeijer,

Lagerveld & Houtman, 2006). More specifically, this approach predicts that incorrect

outcome expectations may trigger learned helplessness (a condition akin to burnout),

whereas incorrect efficacy expectations may affect the individual’s sense of

accomplishment (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

The Conservation of Resources (COR) theory (Hobfoll, 1988, p. 25) states that “people

have an innate as well as learned desire to conserve the quality and quantity of their

resources and to limit any state that may jeopardise the security of their resources”. The

theory suggests that people have a deeply rooted motivation to obtain, retain and

protect that which they value (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998; Taris, Schreur & Van Iersel-

Van Silfhout, 2001). The COR theory identifies four categories of resources. The

categories are objects (e.g. houses), conditions (e.g. relationships, steady job), personal

characteristics (e.g. self-esteem) and forms of energy (e.g. money). It is argued that the

loss of resources threatens individuals and result in stress (Brand, 2007). Work stress

occurs when an individual’s resources are threatened or lost or when an investment of

resources fails to produce an anticipated level of return in resources (Hobfoll & Freedy,

1993). Burnout develops when resources are lost or when resources are inadequate to

meet the burden the individual faces (Brand, 2007). This theory is akin to the

transactional model of stress proposed by Lazarus (Brand, 2007).

Lazarus (2006) states that psychological stress and an individual’s ability to cope with

that stress impact on an individual’s well-being. This theory examines how individuals’

Page 48: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 38 -

own perceptions of their circumstances play a major role in explaining their emotional

experiences (Van Dick & Wagner, 2001). Stress is seen as a product of the way an

individual appraises and constructs a relationship with the environment. In this

relationship environmental demands, cognitive appraisals, coping efforts and emotional

responses are interrelated in reciprocal ways so that each has an effect on the others.

The transactional model recognises that different people experience stress in different

ways (Muldary, 1983). Burnout is explained as the result of triggering environmental

variables and intra-personal traits that may facilitate or inhibit the manifestation of

burnout (Kokkinos, 2007). Persons with lower levels of burnout may perceive the event

as amenable to change or may perceive their coping resources to be adequate

(Ceslowitz, 2006).

Taris et al. (2001) highlight certain important differences and critiques in relation to the

two theories discussed above. Lazarus’s (2006) model only accounts for the indirect

influence of resources, in that one’s personal resources will affect the appraisal of a

particular event, whereas in the Hobfoll (1988) framework resources directly affect

outcomes and coping behaviours. Taris et al. (2001) further state that COR theory is

better suited to explaining the effects of occupational stress.

The psychoanalytic individual approach to burnout considers burnout to be a narcissistic

personality disorder (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998) or narcissistic trauma that manifests

as a significant change in functioning attributable to a marked lowering of the

individual’s sense of self-esteem (Fischer, 1983). Individuals that have idealised their

jobs and suffered subsequent disillusionment can either reduce their ideals or leave the

situation. However, these options are not available to the burnout candidate. Instead

these individuals redouble their efforts in order to attain their unrealistic objectives. They

are motivated by the fear of having to give up their narcissistic ‘illusion of grandiosity’.

The burnout candidate’s sense of self-esteem is grounded in this narcissistic illusion

(Schaufeli & Buunk, 2003).

Pines proposes an individual approach to burnout that is in existential nature. In this

approach the root cause of burnout lies in the existential need to believe that our lives

Page 49: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 39 -

are meaningful and that the things we do are useful and important (Pines & Aronson,

1988; Pines & Keinan, 2005). Burnout combines physical, emotional and mental

aspects and it is a state that is difficult to escape (Pines & Aronson, 1988). This

existential model of burnout is a motivational model. Its underlying assumption is that

only highly motivated individuals can burn out (Schaufeli et al., 1993).

3.3.2 Interpersonal approaches

Initial theories of burnout viewed emotional strains resulting from daily interactions with

demanding, difficult or troubled recipients as the root cause of burnout (Schaufeli &

Enzmann, 1998). Various interpersonal approaches to burnout have been proposed and

some of these approaches are discussed in this section.

The social competence approach assumes that burnout is a function of perceived social

or interpersonal competence (Harrison, 1983). This theory pays attention to the way

individuals perceive, interpret and construct the behaviours of others at work (Buunk &

Schaufeli, 1993). Attention is also paid to individuals’ relationships with others, the way

in which individuals compare their own responses and feelings with those of others at

work and the way in which they are influenced by burnout symptoms in their colleagues

(Schaufeli & Buunk, 2003). Such comparisons may have consequences for the

development and persistence of burnout symptoms (Buunk & Schaufeli, 1993).

A second interpersonal approach conceptualises burnout as emotional overload

(Schaufeli et al., 1993). Maslach defines burnout as a psychological syndrome of

emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation and reduced personal accomplishment

(Schaufeli et al., 1993). Maslach assumes that burnout is a sequential process that

starts with emotional exhaustion as a result of the emotional demands of dealing with

other people. Depersonalisation develops as part of the attempt to cope with this

exhaustion. Depersonalisation is a dysfunctional coping strategy that further damages

the relationship with individuals and as a result more and more failures are experienced

so that a sense of diminished personal accomplishment gradually develops (Schaufeli &

Enzmann, 1998).

Page 50: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 40 -

This three factor sequential model is slightly superior to models that assume an

alternative sequence (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). This model views burnout as a

continuous variable and individuals are considered as positioned on a burnout

continuum (Ozan, 2009).

Maslach’s burnout inventory is used to assess the three dimensions discussed above.

However, the inventory has been criticised for having categories that are not mutually

exclusive (Isaksson, Gude, Tyssen & Aasland, 2010). There is also ongoing debate

concerning ‘lack of efficacy’ as the third dimension of burnout (Breso, et al., 2007).

Schaufeli et al. (2002) have shown that professional efficacy does not load on burnout

but instead loads on the positive concept of work engagement together with vigour,

dedication and absorption. This finding leaves exhaustion and cynicism as core burnout

dimensions.

A third interpersonal approach regards burnout as a lack of reciprocity (Buunk &

Shaufeli, 1993). In addition to working too long, too hard and with too difficult

individuals, this approach assumes that the balance between investments and

outcomes is crucial for the development of burnout (Schaufeli & Buunk, 2003).

Individuals experience insufficient rewards, involving a lack of appropriate rewards for

the work done, including both external (such as salary) and internal rewards (such as

pride) (Rothmann, 2003). High investment with low return can lead to burnout.

A fourth interpersonal approach refers to burnout as an emotional contagion (Rountree,

1984 cited in Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). Colleagues may act as models whose

symptoms are imitated through a process of emotional contagion (Schaufeli & Buunk,

2003). Rothmann (2004) states that managers suffering from burnout may spread their

symptoms to their subordinates and this could harm the organisation as a whole. This

perspective focuses on the way in which interpersonal relationships with colleagues

may contribute to burnout (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

The final interpersonal approach to be discussed defines burnout as emotional labour

(Hochschild, 1983 cited in Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). Emotional labour is defined as

Page 51: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 41 -

the act of displaying appropriate emotion. It can be defined as the performance of

various kinds of emotion work in the context of paid employment (Kim, 2008). According

to Brotheridge and Grandey (2002), burnout and emotional labour have occupational

differences. In emotional labour the focus is on customer service where interactions are

less spontaneously emotional, yet emotional control is needed to maintain positive

relations with customers across time and situations. Burnout is likely to develop as a

result of surface acting, which involves simulating emotions that are not actually felt and

thus identification with those emotions is low. Surface acting involves deliberate

emotional displays that are intended to deceive others (Gardner, Fischer & Hunt, 2009).

Surface acting leads to an increase in emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation and

a decrease in personal accomplishment (Kim, 2008). In contrast to surface acting, deep

acting involves an attempt to feel the emotions that are displayed. Deep acting can also

cause burnout through over-identification with emotions (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

However, deep acting produces more favourable outcomes than surface acting. For

example, deep acting has been positively correlated with a sense of personal

accomplishment (Kim, 2008).

3.3.3 Organisational approaches

Organisational approaches to burnout interpret the syndrome in terms of undesired

organisational behaviour that affects the individual and the organisation. Golembiewski

and Munzenrider (1988 as cited in Schaufeli & Buunk, 2003) consider burnout to be a

virulent or dynamic process in which the professional’s attitudes and behaviours change

in negative ways in response to job strain. Burnout has negative consequences for the

individual and the organisation (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). It is viewed as a process

that develops progressively through eight stages (Schaufeli & Buunk, 2003). The

process begins when functional detachment, which is necessary in certain professions,

gives way to dysfunctional depersonalisation. Dysfunctional depersonalisation then

interferes with job-related performance, thus affecting the individual’s evaluation of

personal accomplishment. Finally, depersonalisation results in diminished personal

accomplishment, which ultimately culminates in emotional exhaustion in chronic cases

(Lewin & Sager, 2007). Golembiewski and Munzenrider’s (Golembiewski, 2000) model

Page 52: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 42 -

is similar to Maslach’s burnout model, but different priorities are assigned to the three

subscales of the Maslach Burnout Inventory. This model views depersonalisation as the

least important contributor to burnout, and emotional exhaustion as the most important

contributor (Golembiewski, 2000). This phase model implies that burnout becomes

more evident as the individual moves through depersonalisation to a reduced sense of

personal accomplishment to emotional exhaustion. Hence, individuals in the more

advanced phases experience more severe symptoms and consequences than those in

the earlier phases (Brand, 2007).

A second organisational model of burnout views burnout as a mismatch between the

person and his or her job. In this model burnout results from a chronic imbalance in

which the job demands more than the employee can give and provides less than what

he or she needs (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). The greater the gap or mismatch

between the person and the job, the greater the likelihood of burnout (Rothmann, 2003).

The Job Demands-Resources Model (JD-R; Demerouti, Nachreiner, Bakker &

Schaufeli, 2001) is a prominent organisational model of burnout. This model proposes

that employee well-being is related to a wide range of workplace variables that can be

conceptualised as either job demands or job resources (Lewig, Xanthopoulou, Bakker,

Dollard & Metzer, 2007). Specifically, job demands refer to the physical, social or

organisational aspects of a job that require sustained physical or mental effort and are

therefore associated with certain physiological or psychological costs, such as

exhaustion (Demerouti et al., 2001). Job demands also include situational factors such

as role ambiguity, role conflict, stressful events, heavy workload, work pressure,

pressure to make decisions, being assigned additional responsibility and having to meet

deadlines (Rothmann & Joubert, 2007). Job resources refer to those aspects of a job

that reduce job demands, facilitate achievement of work goals and stimulate personal

growth and development (Lewig et al., 2007). Job resources include social support

(supervisory and collegial), job enhancement opportunities in the form of increased

control and autonomy, participation in decision making, reinforcement contingencies as

well as opportunities for advancement and rewards (Rothmann & Joubert, 2007).

Page 53: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 43 -

In a study of police officers, job demands and job resources were related to all three

burnout dimensions identified by the JD-R model (Martinussen et al., 2007). The JD-R

model predicts that job demands are primarily and positively related to exhaustion, while

job resources are primarily and negatively related to disengagement from work

(Demerouti et al., 2001). The exhaustion component of burnout is predicted by

overload, job insecurity and a lack of resources (Rothmann & Joubert, 2007). Work

engagement is predicted by job resources, specifically organisational support (including

managerial support, communication, role clarity and the extent of work autonomy).

Organisation support also has a strong influence on both the vigour and dedication

components of work engagement (Rothmann & Joubert, 2007). Therefore, according to

the JD-R model, burnout is a result of excessive job demands and diminished job

resources (Rothmann & Joubert, 2007).

The JD-R model neatly synthesises the concepts of job demands and job resources and

the literature concerning the relationship between these variables and work-related well-

being and withdrawal behaviours into one overarching model. The model is flexible in its

specification of demands and resources and can thus be applied across all occupational

groups (Lewig et al., 2007).

3.3.4 Societal approaches

Strategies to cope with stress differ across social contexts (Meyerson, 1994). According

to Handy (1988 cited in Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1988), burnout can be described as a

discrepancy between surface and latent functions of organisations. Thus, burnout may

be caused by the discrepancies between these manifest and latent functions and the

surface and deep structures of organisations that profoundly influence the actions and

understandings of individual employees (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

Burnout can also be seen as a cultural product. Meyerson (1994) states that the symbol

of ’stress’ captures a culturally central duality of the individual as product and agent of

his or her social condition. Meyerson (1994) also highlights the relevance of the

institutional and cultural context, suggesting that each side of this duality may become

Page 54: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 44 -

more or less salient in specific cultural conditions. In some cultural contexts people

interpret burnout, and by implication stress, as an individual problem while in other

cultural context it is interpreted as a social phenomenon.

The interpersonal approach to burnout was used in this research. This approach

focuses on demanding relationships with people at work (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

In this study the theory was applied to the university context. The Maslach Burnout

Inventory that was used as a data collection instrument is based on the interpersonal

theory of burnout.

3.4 Individual contributors to burnout

Many personal factors have been linked to burnout. Individual characteristics have been

postulated as important in the development of burnout. According to Kokkinos (2007),

burnout is the product of both organisational–environmental factors as well as personal

qualities of the individual. Personality has been explored quite extensively in the context

of burnout. This section examines the big five personality factors, hardy personality,

type-A behaviour, dependent personalities, locus of control, optimism and sense of

coherence. Other factors such as coping skills, self-efficacy and demographic factors

are also discussed.

3.4.1 Personality

The existence of a correlation between a personality trait and burnout does not mean

that the trait directly causes burnout. Individuals are likely to put themselves in

situations that match their personality and such situations may foster burnout (Garden,

1989). Furthermore, personality characteristics can moderate the relationship between

stressful situations and burnout in such a way that certain traits may buffer or,

conversely, enhance negative outcomes (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

3.4.1.1 The big five personality traits

Relationships between the big five personality traits and burnout have been reported in

Page 55: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 45 -

various studies. Storm and Rothmann (2003b) found a negative relationship between

Extroversion and burnout. Extroversion is also negatively associated with emotional

exhaustion and depersonalisation and positively associated with personal

accomplishment (Bakker et al., 2006; Jonker, 2004). Extroversion reflects individual

traits such as being sociable, gregarious, assertive, talkative and active (Kim, Shin &

Umbreit, 2007). People measuring high on this trait show positive emotions, have higher

frequency and intensity of personal interactions, have a tendency to be optimistic and

reappraise problems positively (Bakker et al., 2006). Zellers, Perrewe and Hochwarter

(2000) argue that the optimism, positive affect, energy and increased interpersonal

communication experienced by individuals scoring high on this factor may result in

reduced experiences of emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation.

Agreeableness generally shows a negative relationship with emotional exhaustion and

depersonalisation (Bakker et al., 2006; Kim et al., 2007) and a positive relationship with

personal accomplishment (Bakker et al., 2006). Agreeableness reflects individual

differences in warmth, friendliness, kindness and empathy in social interactions (Kim et

al., 2007). Low scores in Agreeableness are indicative of a distrustful attitude. This

attitude can easily become the dehumanised handling that is indicative of

depersonalisation (Cano-Garcia, Padilla-Munoz & Carrasco-Ortiz, 2005). Individuals

high on Agreeableness report less confrontational coping and more support seeking

(DeLongis & Holtzman, 2005). This factor, which encompasses sympathy, trust,

cooperation and altruism (McCrae & Costa, 1989), may allow an individual to manage

the frustration of dealing with people. This may result in reduced feelings of

depersonalisation and emotional exhaustion and augmented experiences of personal

accomplishment (Zellars et al., 2000).

Conscientiousness has been associated with problem-solving coping, self-discipline,

achievement striving and competence (Bakker et al., 2006). Individuals high on

Conscientiousness engage in more empathic responding. They report less avoidance

and self-blame strategies and more planned problem-solving (DeLongis & Holtzman,

2005). Conscientiousness shows a strong relationship with low emotional exhaustion

Page 56: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 46 -

and depersonalisation (Jonker, 2004). It is also associated with increased feelings of

personal accomplishment (Kokkinos, 2007). The trait has also been linked to

involvement, persistence, fulfilment and efficacy, which pave the way for goal fulfilment

(Cano-Garcia et al., 2005).

A consistent relationship has been reported between burnout and Neuroticism (Morgan,

2009). Numerous studies have found that Neuroticism is related to all three burnout

factors (Bakker et al., 2006). Neuroticism includes trait anxiety, hostility, depression,

self-consciousness and vulnerability (Maslach et al., 2001). High scores on Neuroticism

tend to indicate increased emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation. Individuals

measuring high on this trait tend to express more negative emotions, be more emotional

unstable and have more extreme stress reactions than their lower scoring counterparts.

These individuals are therefore vulnerable to burnout (Kokkinos, 2007). Neuroticism has

been found to be related to the use of coping strategies that are related to poorer

outcomes. These coping strategies include avoidance, interpersonal withdrawal and

self-blame (Lee-Baggley, Preecen & DeLongis, 2005).

Individuals high in Neuroticism expect the worst from a situation and underestimate their

own ability to cope (Zellers et al., 2000). Zellars et al. (2000) argue that the effect of

anxiety and negative affect combined with increased vulnerability to situations could

lead these individuals to blame others for their feelings of anxiety. This would be likely

to increase the experience of depersonalisation. In addition, the tendency to anticipate

the worst may result in reduced professional efficacy. Morgan (2009) further states that

because Neuroticism is related to negativity in thinking, mood and coping, it is likely to

be related to emotional exhaustion.

Openness to Experience is positively related to personal accomplishment (Bakker et al.,

2006). Deary et al. (1996) found that there is evidence that individuals scoring higher on

Openness to Experience have a decreased probability of experiencing emotional

exhaustion. Zellers et al. (2006) found that this personality trait is negatively related to

depersonalisation. Openness to Experience involves adaptive and flexible coping and

the ability to engage with others and the world. It is also related to lower levels of

Page 57: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 47 -

distancing in coping with stress (DeLongis & Holtzman, 2005). These individuals have a

good awareness of their surroundings and are thus likely to be attentive to stressors in

their environment. These individuals are also more likely to attempt new experiences

(Zellars et al., 2000). It is possible that these individuals may view stressful situations as

challenges, leading to an experience of an increased sense of personal

accomplishment and diminished emotional exhaustion (Zellars et al., 2000).

3.4.1.2 Other personality characteristics

As discussed in the previous section many studies have reported relationships between

the big five personality traits and burnout. However, similar relationships have been

found between other personality characteristics and burnout. Research has suggested a

negative relationship between hardiness and burnout. A ‘hardy personality’ is

characterised by involvement in daily activities, a sense of control over events,

openness to change (Garrosa, Moreno-Jimenez, Liang & Gonzalez, 2008) and the

capacity to commit to an undertaking (Kobasa, Maddi & Kahn, 1982). Having a hardy

personality appears to decrease burnout (Garrosa et al., 2008). It is also consistently

related to all three dimensions of the Maslach Burnout Inventory. Hardier individuals

appear to be less emotionally exhausted, less depersonalised and have stronger

feelings of personal accomplishment (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998; Toscano &

Ponterdolph, 1998).

The relationship between Type-A behaviour and burnout had also been investigated.

Storm (2001) reports a positive relationship between these two variables, especially in

relation to emotional exhaustion. Type-A individuals tend to be competitive, prefer a

time-pressured lifestyle and show an excessive need for control (Maslach et al., 2001).

Individuals with dependent personalities are likely to experience more symptoms of

burnout than less dependent people. Dependent individuals tend to experience

considerable stress that may have a cumulative effect over time. These individuals have

learned to assume submission under others and are inherently unable to perform tasks

with the same efficacy that others demonstrate (Muldary, 1983).

Page 58: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 48 -

Another personality related construct, locus of control, concerns the degree to which

individuals believe that they control events in their lives (internal locus of control) or that

the environment or fate controls events (external locus of control) (Judge, Locke,

Durham & Kluger, 1998). People who feel in control respond to stress in a different way

than people who feel that they have no control (Muldary, 1983). Individuals with an

internal locus of control have more effective coping strategies that lead to better

psychological adjustment. These characteristics reduce the negative effects associated

with stress (Graffeo & Silvestri, 2006). Individuals with an internal locus of control

experience less anxiety and handle pressure better than those with an external locus of

control (Van der Merwe, 2003). Individuals with an external locus of control are more

vulnerable to stress and burnout (Schmitz, Neumann & Oppermann, 2000). Individuals

with an external locus of control appear to be more emotionally exhausted,

depersonalised and experience reduced feelings of personal accomplishment (Schaufeli

& Enzmann, 1998).

The personality characteristic of optimism can be defined as a generalised expectancy

that the future will be good (Rothmann & Essenko, 2007). Optimism and pessimism

influence people’s subjective experiences when confronting problems. They also

influence the actions people engage in to try and deal with these problems (Rothmann

& Essenko, 2007). Individuals with optimistic dispositions rely on coping strategies that

could help to control or modify aspects of stressors. They also seek information and are

involved in planning and positive reframing (Rothmann, 2003). Optimism is negatively

related to depression, anxiety and job burnout (Strassle, 1999) and has also been found

to be negatively related to scores on the exhaustion and cynicism scales of the Maslach

Burnout Inventory (Chang, Rand & Strunk, 2000). According to Strassle, McKee and

Plant (1999), optimism shows a positive relationship with life satisfaction and mental

health. Therefore, optimism plays an important role in a person’s physical and emotional

well-being (Strassle et al., 1999) and acts as a buffer to stress (Jacobs & Dodd, 2003).

Sense of coherence refers to the extent to which an individual has a pervasive and

enduring feeling of confidence that his or her internal and external environments are

Page 59: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 49 -

predictable and believes that there is a high probability that things will work out as well

as can reasonably be expected (Rothmann, 2004). A strong sense of coherence

enables one to mobilise effective coping resources in the face of tension. Individuals

with a low sense of coherence may be predisposed to experiencing the negative

emotions associated with burnout regardless of the stressors. Individuals with a high

sense of coherence may be predisposed to experiencing ‘health proneness’ as opposed

to burnout, regardless of the stressors (Levert, Lucas & Ortlepp, 2000).

3.4.2 Coping skills

Coping is defined as an individual’s attempt to prevent, reduce or eliminate negative

experiences (Mostert & Joubert, 2005). Coping also refers to perceptual, cognitive or

behavioural responses that are used to manage, avoid or control situations that are

regarded as difficult (Storm & Rothmann, 2003b).

Certain coping strategies can serve as a buffer against burnout (Jordaan, Spangenberg,

Watson & Fouche, 2007). Poor coping skills appear to be a significant factor in

determining the intensity with which stress is experienced (Van der Merwe, 2003). High

levels of stress and burnout are associated with ineffective coping strategies such as

emotion-focused coping strategies (Wiese, Rothmann & Storm, 2003). Low levels of

burnout are associated with active coping strategies (Storm & Rothmann, 2003b; Van

der Merwe, 2003) such as confronting (Rothmann, 2004). The use of problem-focused

coping strategies, which can also be regarded as active strategies, may trigger feelings

of personal accomplishment (Rothmann, 2004), therefore reducing the likelihood of

burnout. The use of passive forms of coping, such as behavioural and mental

disconnection from the situation, has been linked to greater feelings of emotional

exhaustion (Mostert & Joubert, 2005; Wiese et al., 2003) and depersonalisation

(Jordaan et al., 2007).

3.4.3 Self-efficacy

Self-efficacy refers to beliefs in one’s capabilities to organise and execute the course of

Page 60: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 50 -

action required to produce the expected attainment. Self-efficacy is not concerned with

the skills an individual possesses but rather the individual’s judgement of what he or she

can do with these skills (Perrewe et al., 2002). Self-efficacy can be viewed as reflecting

an individual’s perceptions of his or her fundamental ability to cope with life’s exigencies

and as such it represents a core self-evaluation (Judge et al., 1998). People who

believe in their abilities and potential are less susceptible to stress and consequently

less likely to suffer from burnout (Rothmann, 2003). A significant positive relationship

was found between generalised self-efficacy and personal accomplishment (Chan,

2007a). A significant negative relationship was found between generalised self-efficacy

and cynicism (Chan, 2007a). Emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation are

negatively related to self-efficacy (Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2009).

3.4.4 Demographic variables

The demographic variable age is most consistently related to burnout. Burnout is

observed most often in employees aged younger than 30 or 40 years old (Maslach et

al., 2001; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). Burnout seems to occur at the beginning rather

than at the end of a person’s career (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

In terms of gender, burnout appears to be related to typically ‘female’ issues, such as

the desirability of attending to and expressing emotions. Males and females may well

differ in important ways relevant to the experience of stress and burnout (Lackritz,

2004). Women tend to score slightly higher on emotional exhaustion, whereas men

score significantly higher on depersonalisation (Grayson & Alvarez, 2008; Schaufeli &

Enzmann, 1998).

In terms of marital status, unmarried men and to a lesser extent unmarried women,

seem to be more prone to burnout than their married counterparts (Maslach et al.,

2001). This may be the result of social support. The efficient and creative use of a social

support system is among the most effective ways of coping with burnout (Pines et al.,

1981).

Page 61: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 51 -

3.5 Impact and symptoms of burnout

Many symptoms have been associated with burnout. These symptoms come from a

range of different areas such as affective, cognitive, physical and behavioural domains.

These symptom areas are discussed below.

3.5.1 Affective/psychological symptoms

Burnout represents a form of psychological imbalance that results from the inefficiency

appraisal or circumstances or the unavailability of adequate coping resources or a

combination of these two factors (Muldary, 1983). The components of burnout have

been linked to a variety of mental health problems. Deterioration in mental health is

characterised by a decrease in self-esteem and an increase in depression, irritability,

helplessness or anxiety (Cordes & Dougherty, 1993). Burnout also seems to harm an

individual’s well-being (Pienaar & Willemse, 2008). Individuals suffering from burnout

appear resigned and discouraged and experience anxiety about failure (Kraft, 2006).

Burnout can also lead to depression (Ahola & Hakanen, 2007). The individual’s

emotional resources are exhausted and emotional control might be decreased, which

can lead to undefined fears, anxiety and nervous tension (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).

There is also a link between burnout and feelings towards others. In a study conducted

by Holmqvist and Jeanneau (2006), burnout was connected with negative feelings of

psychiatric staff towards patients. The same study found that a low incidence of burnout

was correlated with positive feelings towards patients. Tedium, emotional exhaustion

and depersonalisation were associated with unhelpful and rejecting feelings, whereas

personal accomplishment was associated with accepting, helpful and close feelings

(Holmqvist & Jeanneau, 2006).

3.5.2 Cognitive symptoms

An individual suffering from burnout might experience impaired cognitive skills (Storm,

2001). Professionals who experience burnout feel helpless, hopeless and powerless

(Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). They find it difficult to concentrate, have fewer creative

Page 62: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 52 -

ideas and experience memory problems. They also begin to make mistakes (Kraft,

2006). Thinking becomes more rigid, schematic and detached and decision-making

becomes increasingly difficult (Storm, 2001).

Burned out practitioners experience increasing difficulty in finding proper solutions to

problems. Instead of actively solving problems there is a tendency to avoid reality by

daydreaming and fantasising (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). When problem-solving

mechanisms are not functioning properly, appraisal and coping are impaired and stress

continues to exacerbate the burnout (Muldary, 1983).

3.5.3 Physical symptoms

Burnout has also been linked to a variety of physical health problems (Cordes &

Dougherty, 1993). Physical symptoms of burnout can be grouped into three categories.

The first category involves physical distress such as headaches, nausea, dizziness,

restlessness and muscle pains. The individual may experience anxiety and can be

afraid of losing control over his or her body. Sexual problems, sleep disturbances and

weight changes can also occur. Chronic fatigue, drowsiness and bodily weakness are

all common physical symptoms of burnout (Storm, 2001). The second category of

physical health problems related to burnout consists of psychosomatic disorders like

ulcers, gastric-intestinal disorders and coronary heart disease. Less serious symptoms

in this category include prolonged colds and flu that can be considered a consequence

of prolonged stress (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). The third category concerns

physiological reactions and includes increased heart rate and respiration rate as well as

hypertension (Storm, 2001). The presence of burnout also predicts increased risk for

diseases of the circulatory, respiratory and musculoskeletal systems (Ahola et al.,

2008).

3.5.4 Behavioural symptoms

Behavioural symptoms are the result of the individual’s increased level of arousal

(Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998) and include hyperactivity, an inability to concentrate and

Page 63: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 53 -

acting directly and impulsively (Storm, 2001). Individuals who experience burnout may

also engage in behaviours such as smoking and drug and alcohol use (Cordes &

Dougherty, 1993).

In conclusion, psychological, cognitive, behavioural and physical symptoms can occur

as a result of burnout. Burnout can affect many areas of human life and is therefore

regarded as a pervasive disorder.

3.6 Burnout in the work context

The large number of employees suffering from burnout indicates that this occupational

disorder is a major problem in modern organisations (Brummelhuis, van der Lippe,

Kluwer & Flap, 2008). The concept of burnout was originally confined to individuals who

work in the helping professions. However, researchers currently acknowledge that

employees in almost any job can develop burnout (Jonker, 2004).

Burnout is related to the work environment and job demands such as time pressure and

work overload. Role overload, which is an individual’s perception that insufficient time

and resources are available to accomplish work-related tasks, is also related to burnout.

The employee may spend excessive energy in an attempt to maintain performance

standards, leading to feelings of emotional exhaustion (Lewin & Sager, 2007). Lack of

role clarity can also enhance the burnout risk (Isaksson et al., 2009). Burnout is also

linked to conflicting demands and lack of autonomy in the work place. Specific job

characteristics, such as shift work, may affect workers’ experience of burnout

(Martinussen et al., 2007).

When psychological outcomes such as state anxiety, moods, fear, depression,

psychological distress, fatigue, negative emotions and emotional job demands are

experienced over prolonged periods they represent risk factors for burnout (Awa,

Plaumann & Walter, 2010). Burnout may also occur when the pursuit for significance

and meaning in life and identity is unsuccessfully pursued through work (Gustaffson &

Strandberg, 2009). Furthermore, when work interferes with family life, levels of burnout

Page 64: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 54 -

increase (Innstrand, Langballe, Espnes, Falkum & Aasland, 2008).

According to Halbesleben and Buckley (2006), individuals’ perceptions of how burned

out they are compared to others may also have an effect on actual burnout. Individuals

engage in comparisons with those who they believe are worse off (downward

comparison) or better off (upward comparison) than themselves. Comparisons with

those who are worse off can result in positive affect. If individuals feel that they are in a

better situation than their peers they may reassess their perceptions of work-related

stressors and coping abilities in a manner that would lead to reduced burnout in the

future. Upward comparison reflects a social comparison situation characterised by a

belief that others are less burned out. Upward comparison plays a role in burnout.

Burnout might be exacerbated by the perception that others are less burned out

(Halbesleben & Buckley, 2006). Perception also influences burnout in the sense that

burnout may be due to a perceived imbalance between a person’s contribution to his or

her work and the organisation’s contribution to him or her (Salmela-Aro, Naatanen &

Nurmi, 2004).

Freudenberger and Richelson (1980) state that exhaustion plays a major role in

burnout. According to these authors burnout is often precipitated by the work situation.

Employees have been overworked and their resources depleted for a long time. These

employees become agitated and eventually they can no longer repress the agitation.

In the workplace burnout can lead to serious consequences for professionals, their

clients and the larger setting in which they interact. Burnout is related to decreased job

performance, withdrawal from clients, the job and the organisation (Lewin & Sager,

2007), high employee turnover, absenteeism and low employee morale. Burnout can

also lead to early retirement and may increase morbidity and mortality (Borritz et al.,

2006). It affects an employee’s job satisfaction, self-esteem, social life and morale (van

der Merwe, 2003). It also has a negative influence on personal functioning at home and

thus has an influence on others. People with high work pressure and emotional

demands have trouble combining work and family life (Demerouti, Bakker & Schaufeli,

2005).

Page 65: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 55 -

Certain variables may protect against burnout. Empowerment is an important protective

factor. Psychological empowerment at the workplace is a four-dimensional concept

consisting of meaning (a fit between the requirements of the job tasks and the subject’s

own values), competence (the subject’s belief that he or she possesses the skills and

abilities necessary to perform a job or task well), self-determination (the subject’s feeling

of having control over his or her own work) and impact (the belief that the subject has a

significant influence over strategic, administrative or operational outcomes at work). A

negative association exists between empowerment and burnout (Hochwalder, 2007).

Social support from co-workers and supervisors can also serve as a protective factor

against burnout (Awa et al., 2010).

The discussion in this section has made it clear that burnout in the work environment

has negative consequences for the individual’s professional and personal worlds.

3.7 Burnout amongst students

Although it was previously believed that students cannot experience burnout, recent

research has found this belief to be invalid (Pienaar & Sieberhagen, 2005). Experts

have reported that the number of college students with severe psychological problems

is increasing (Bernhard, 2007). Burnout can certainly be included in these psychological

problems. Yang (2004, p. 287) defines student burnout as “students in the learning

process, because of course stress, course load or other psychological factors, display a

state of emotional exhaustion, a tendency to depersonalisation, and a feeling of low

personal accomplishment.”

The transitory nature of post-secondary school life, leaving home, family and friends for

the first time, and adjusting to a vastly different environment, leaves students

particularly prone to stress and its potentially negative effects. Students must adjust to a

new social climate, attempt to build new support systems and achieve a high academic

standing (Weckwerth & Flynn, 2006).

Social support is very important for students (Jacobs & Dodd, 2003) and may also

Page 66: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 56 -

extend to romantic relationships. Willcock, Daly, Tennant and Allard (2004)

demonstrated that medical students who were not in a relationship were more likely to

experience emotional exhaustion. Greater social support is associated with less

emotional exhaustion, less depersonalisation and a greater sense of personal

accomplishment (Jacobs & Dodd, 2003).

Concerns about finances are also related to stress in student populations. Ross,

Cleland and Macleod (2006) report that students who worried about money had higher

debts and performed less well than their peers in degree examinations. Some students

in this group also had mental health problems.

Demographic factors also influence students’ levels of stress. Ried, Motycka, Mobley

and Meldrum (2006) established that students in their second year of study were more

prone to experiences of burnout. These students expressed more emotional exhaustion

than students in other years. However, this finding may be specific to their sample to

pharmacy students (Ried et al., 2006). For medical students the prevalence of burnout

was higher for students in more advanced years of training (Dyrbye et al., 2006).

A positive relationship exists between workload and burnout. Some studies have found

this relationship to only be true for emotional exhaustion (Male & May, 1997, 1998) but

other studies have found that workload is correlated to all three dimensions of burnout

(Greenglass, Burke & Fiksenbaum, 2001). In a study by Jacobs and Dodd (2003)

subjective experiences of being overworked predicted emotional exhaustion and

depersonalisation.

Gender differences are also related to burnout in students. Weckwerth and Flynn (2006)

found that female students demonstrated lower scores on personal accomplishment

than male students. However, male students scored higher on the depersonalisation

scale than their female counterparts. Similarly, Ried et al. (2006) found that female

students were more likely to express emotional exhaustion, while male students were

more likely to express symptoms of depersonalisation.

Page 67: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 57 -

Research suggests that students confront many challenges in pursuit of their

educational goals. University students may experience burnout due to learning

conditions that demand excessively high levels of effort and do not provide supportive

mechanisms to facilitate effective coping (Neumann et al., 1990). When these

experiences are perceived as negative, they can have adverse effects on students’

motivation and performance (Struthers, Perry & Menec, 2000). These individuals’

quality of life is dependent upon their ability to adjust to and cope with various demands.

Failure to adjust and cope may result in the impairment of behaviour and health.

Individuals who do not cope effectively initially experience behavioural problems such

as rage, anger, withdrawal and depression. If the stressors continue and are not dealt

with serious health problems can develop. Prolonged stress can result in burnout (Van

der Merwe, 2003).

Weckworth and Flynn (2006) state that in a study documenting stress and poor health

habits in college students, it was found that students exhibiting the greatest stress levels

reported significantly higher consumption of alcohol, drugs and junk food. These

students also exhibited lower self-esteem and poorer sleeping habits than students who

were not stressed. Other negative behaviours that have been linked to the experience

of stress in the student population include suicidal ideation and smoking (Weckwerth &

Flynn, 2006). When students reach the burnout level these behaviours may increase

and become fatal.

Postgraduate students who are also employed have to cope with demands that arise

from fulfilling the roles of student and employee at the same time. Non-working

postgraduate students have to learn the rules of being an adult and adjust to the

changes of not being a child anymore. All these factors impact on coping ability (Van

der Merwe, 2003). Furthermore, Morgan (2009) states there may be a relationship

between fear of unemployment (which is similar to job insecurity) and burnout in student

populations. Thus, the perception of possible unemployment after the completion of

higher education studies could be an antecedent to burnout in the student population.

Prior research indicates that the burnout syndrome in students is similar to that of

Page 68: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 58 -

service employees. Student burnout can lead to higher absenteeism, lower motivation

to complete required assignments and higher dropout rates (Yang, 2004). Burnout

among students manifests as feeling exhausted because of study demands, having a

cynical and detached attitude toward one’s studies and feeling incompetent as a student

(Schaufeli et al., 2002). Burnout influences students’ academic performance and

accomplishments and can threaten their academic futures (Struthers et al., 2000).

Student burnout is also an important predictor of later professional burnout (Mostert et

al., 2007).

Student burnout has negative consequences for individuals as well as for society at

large. It impacts on the general allure of the university for new students, with potential

consequences for present and future enrolment. Student burnout can also have a

significant impact on the effectiveness of the universities, which may in turn have

distinct policy implications for higher education institutions (Neumann et al., 1990).

3.8 Burnout and emotional intelligence related research

As discussed previously emotional intelligence appears to be closely related to

personality. Stable personality characteristics predispose individuals to view adverse

events in specific ways that can either impair or facilitate the adaptation process and

thereby impact psychological and physical health outcomes (Kokkinos, 2007). In terms

of the big five personality factors negative relationships have been found between

Extroversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and burnout. However, Neuroticism

has been positively linked to burnout (Bakker et al., 2006).

The most consistent findings regarding the relationship between personality and

burnout concern the relationship between Neuroticism and Extroversion and burnout

(Bühler & Land, 2003). Neuroticism is closely related to experiences of negative

affectivity. Individuals who score high on Neuroticism are predisposed to experience

negative emotions such as anxiety and depression and experience more distress in

general (Zellers et al., 2000). Numerous studies have found that Neuroticism is related

to all three burnout factors. In particular, Neuroticism has demonstrated a significant

Page 69: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 59 -

positive relationship with emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation (Morgan, 2009).

Neuroticism has been found to be the most influential predictor of burnout (Kim, Shin &

Swanger, 2009; Watson, Deary, Thompson & Li, 2008). Individuals scoring low on

Neuroticism may be more calm and relaxed, enabling them to cope better with the

demands of university life and allowing the use of more effective coping strategies in

dealing with stress (Morgan, 2009).

People who score high on Extroversion are generally people-orientated. In addition,

they frequently experience optimism and enthusiasm and have high levels of energy.

Thus, Extroversion is related to the experience of positive affect (Watson & Clark,

1997). Students scoring high on Extroversion tend to engage in social activities more

frequently. This social support may act as a buffer to the negative effects of stress.

Extroverts tend to be optimistic concerning possible outcomes. This optimism could

result in lower levels of exhaustion (Zellers et al, 2000). Extroversion has a negative

relation with emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation, and a positive association

with personal accomplishment (Morgan, 2009). Extroversion is the best predictor of

personal accomplishment (Miner, 2007).

With regards to the personality dimension of Openness to experience, it is associated

with lower emotional exhaustion and depersonalisation (Storm & Rothmann, 2003b) and

higher personal accomplishment (Bakker et al., 2006).

Agreeableness has been linked to burnout. Agreeable individuals are trusting and

influenced more by their feelings than by reason. This dimension reflects tendencies to

care and nurture and may help individuals cope with people and difficult situations.

Therefore, individuals high in Agreeableness should report lower levels of emotional

exhaustion. In addition, these individuals tend to focus on the needs and well-being of

others and it is therefore less likely that they would see others as objects. This should

result in fewer reports of depersonalisation (Zellers et al., 2000). Agreeableness has a

positive relationship with personal accomplishment and is negatively related to

depersonalisation and emotional exhaustion. Demerouti et al. (2001) state that students

scoring high on Agreeableness also tend to be more receptive to social support. This

Page 70: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 60 -

increased support may serve to increase feelings of personal accomplishment.

Conscientiousness appears to be related to greater feelings of personal

accomplishment (Hochwalder, 2006). This factor is positively associated with personal

accomplishment. Kokkinos (2007) established is the existence of a negative relationship

between Conscientiousness and depersonalisation. Students scoring high on

Conscientiousness may be more organised and thus experience less stress at exam

time (Zellers et al, 2000). This in turn may result in increased experiences of personal

accomplishment and reduced feelings of depersonalisation and emotional exhaustion

(Morgan, 2009)

The construct of Alexithymia is also closely related to emotional intelligence.

Alexithymia is characterised by impoverished fantasy, poor capacity for symbolic

thought and difficulties in experiencing and verbalising emotions. Alexithymia is

associated with impaired coping with stress and poor academic performance in first

year university students. Alexithymia may be a predisposing factor for burnout due to

inadequate coping with stress (Mattila et al., 2007).

Optimism, which is a component of trait emotional intelligence, is associated with all

three dimensions of burnout (Chang et al., 2000). Dispositional optimism can be

described as the generalised expectancy that good things will happen in the future and

bad things will be minimal (Hayes & Weathington, 2007). Optimism has been negatively

correlated with scores on the Exhaustion and Cynicism scales of the Maslach Burnout

Inventory among working college students. Greater optimism is associated with less risk

to job burnout (Chang et al., 2000).

Self-efficacy refers to the beliefs in one’s ability to organise and execute the course of

action required to produce given attainments (Perrewe et al., 2002). Self-efficacy is

closely related to self-esteem, which is a facet of trait emotional intelligence. Many

researchers have used self-efficacy theory in burnout research. Burnout has even been

described as “a crisis in self-efficacy” (Brouwers & Tomic, 2000, p. 242). Teachers who

scored low in self-efficacy reported a higher degree of burnout than their high scoring

Page 71: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 61 -

counterparts (Brouwers & Tomic, 2000).

The constructs of personality, alexithymia, optimism and self-efficacy are closely related

to the construct of emotional intelligence. Their relationship with burnout leads to the

hypothesis that a relationship exists between burnout and emotional intelligence.

3.9 Chapter summary

This chapter focused on the concept of burnout. First, a few relevant definitions of

burnout were provided. Different models of burnout were presented on individual,

interpersonal, organisational and societal levels. Individual causes of burnout were then

discussed.

Burnout in the workplace and amongst students was investigated. The symptoms of

burnout were considered in accordance with the different areas of human functioning,

namely affective, cognitive, physical and behavioural functioning. Lastly, research

related to burnout and emotional intelligence was presented. In the next chapter the

research design and method are discussed.

Page 72: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 62 -

CHAPTER 4

4. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHOD

4.1 Introduction

This chapter focuses on the research design and method of research. Firstly, the

quantitative research paradigm, descriptive statistics, correlational design and survey

research are discussed. The research objectives and questions are then presented. The

research method is also presented. This includes ethical considerations, an description

of the participants used and the instruments employed. Lastly, data analyses

procedures are discussed.

4.2 Research design

The research design constitutes the planning stage of the study. Important decisions

include the development of research questions, the operationalisation of constructs, the

choice of sampling method and the use of appropriate statistical tests (Black, 1999).

Hardy (2006) states that a research design has two purposes: first, to promote well-

planned research and; second, to enable future researchers to replicate the research if

necessary.

This research made use of a quantitative research paradigm. With quantitative

research, the data are collected and presented in the form of numbers (Goodwin, 2008).

The quantitative paradigm is concerned with the generalisations of results from a

sample group to the population as a whole (Morgan, 2009). Quantitative data can be

analysed by means of descriptive or inferential statistics (Dyer, 1995).

A cross-sectional survey design was used in this study. A survey involves asking people

for information, usually using a completely structured questionnaire with mostly fixed-

choice questions (Coolican, 1996). In this research, the aim of the questionnaires was

to determine whether a relationship exists between emotional intelligence and burnout.

Correlational research was used to achieve this objective.

Page 73: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 63 -

A correlation exists when two variables are associated or related to each other in some

fashion (Goodwin, 2008). The correlational design allowed for an investigation of a

relationship between emotional intelligence and burnout (Goodwin, 2008). Pearson

product-moment correlation coefficients were determined. This procedure provides a

measure of the extent of co-variation between any two variables based on sets of

interval or ratio scale data (Dyer, 1995).

4.3 Research objectives

This research’s main objective was to obtain information concerning postgraduate

university students’ levels of emotional intelligence and burnout and to determine

whether a relationship exists between these two variables. Burnout was assessed on

three dimensions: emotional exhaustion, cynicism and depersonalisation. Emotional

intelligence was assessed as a global trait. The burnout dimensions were correlated

with the global emotional intelligence scores.

4.4 Research questions

The research questions for this study were:

1. What are the levels of emotional intelligence in a sample of postgraduate

university students?

2. What are the levels of burnout in a sample of postgraduate university students?

3. Is there is a statistically significant relationship between burnout and emotional

intelligence amongst postgraduate university students?

4.5 Research method

In this section the research procedure, ethical considerations and the participants’

biographical details are discussed.

Page 74: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 64 -

4.5.1 Procedure

Individual postgraduate lecturers were approached to obtain permission to administer

the questionnaires to their students. The research proposal was sent via email to all the

lecturers who were contacted. Once the lecturers had approved the proposal, times

were organised to administer the questionnaires. The questionnaires were administered

at the end of lecture times with prior permission from the lecturer. The researcher, who

is a registered independent practice psychometrist, administered the questionnaires.

The research was explained to the participants. Participants were allowed to refuse

participation. Anonymity was ensured as the participants did not provide their names or

signatures. The ethical considerations are discussed below.

4.5.2 Ethical considerations

The proposal for the research was submitted to the Faculty of Humanities Higher

Degrees Committee at the University of Johannesburg. The proposal was given ethical

approval. Participants were informed of the nature of the study. Anonymity was ensured

as no names or signatures were provided. Participants were allowed to refuse

participation and the refusal to participate involved no negative consequences.

Participants who wanted to speak about the research were able to contract the

researcher via email. Participants were informed that the results would be made

available through the University of Johannesburg library and they were encouraged to

view these results.

4.5.3 Participants

Participants were selected through purposive sampling. Lecturers involved in

postgraduate lecturing were approached to obtain permission for data collection. A total

of 225 postgraduate students enrolled at a large metropolitan university participated in

this study. Most of the participants were aged between 21 and 25. The minimum age

was 21 and the maximum age 62. This student population was older than the normal

student population because the participants were all registered for postgraduate

Page 75: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 65 -

studies. Those students who were 21 years of age were thought to have finished grade

12 (matric) at age 17 and continued with their postgraduate studies immediately after

their undergraduate studies. Those students who were significantly older than the mean

age probably worked for a number of years and then returned to postgraduate studies

on a full time or part time basis. The tables below provide further information regarding

the participants.

Table 4.1 Gender and Racial Group Distribution of Participants

N Gender Race

Female Male Missing Black Coloured White Asian Missing

225 172 45 8 86 8 112 12 7

One hundred and seventy two participants were female (76.4 %) and 45 male (20.0%).

The study used a cross-racial sample and four racial groups were identified. Eighty six

(38.2%) participants were black, 112 (49.8%) participants were white, 12 (5.3%)

participants were Asian and 8 (3.6%) participants were coloured.

Table 4.2 Home Language of Participants

Language N = 225

English 84

Afrikaans 49

African Language 66

Other

Missing

18

8

The largest percentage of students (37.3%) were English speaking. Forty nine (21.8%)

students were Afrikaans speaking. In terms of African languages 19 (8.4%) students

spoke IsiZulu, 2 (0.9%) students spoke IsiXhosa, 18 (8.0%) students spoke Setswana,

16 (7.1%) students spoke Northern Sotho and 11 (4.9%) students spoke Southern

Sotho. Eighteen (8.0%) participants indicated that they spoke another language not

Page 76: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 66 -

indicated on the biographical questionnaire. These languages included African

languages such as Siswati and Xitsonga as well as languages not indigenous to South

Africa.

Table 4.3 Faculty Distribution of Participants

Faculty N = 225

Art, Design and Architecture 2

Science 45

Education 4

Health Sciences 86

Law 1

Humanities 50

Management 30

Missing value 7

Students were selected from a variety of faculties in order to prevent sample bias. The

majority of participants (38.2%) were students in the Faculty of Health. Fifty (22.2%)

participants were students in the Humanities Faculty, 45 (20.0%) participants were

students in the Science Faculty and 30 (13.3%) participants were students in the

Management Faculty. There were very few participants (3.1%) from the Faculties of

Education, Art, Design and Architecture and Law.

Table 4.4 Work Experience

Work experience N = 225

Yes 104

No 109

Missing 12

An almost equal number of participants had part-time work (46.2%) and did not have

part-time work (48.4%).

Page 77: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 67 -

4.6 Instruments

The study used two psychometric instruments, the Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire (TEIQue; Petrides & Furnham, 2003) and the Maslach Burnout Inventory

- Student Survey (MBI-SS; Schaufeli et al., 2002). A biographical questionnaire was

also included in order to obtain the necessary biographical data for the study. These

three questionnaires are discussed in this section.

4.6.1 Biographical Questionnaire

Students were given a biographical questionnaire to complete. The information on the

questionnaire included age, gender, faculty, ethnic group, home language, year of study

and part-time experience. This information allowed the researcher to describe the

group. It also allowed the researcher to ascertain whether the sample was biased

towards any particular group of students.

4.6.2 The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-TEIQue-Short Form

Trait emotional intelligence is assessed through self-report. It is regarded as an emotion

related dispositional trait that is a lower level component of personality (Austin, 2009).

Trait emotional intelligence is distinguished from ability emotional intelligence, which

concerns actual abilities measured with maximal performance tests (De Raad, 2005). In

lay terms, trait emotional intelligence is people’s own perceptions of their emotional

abilities (Petrides & Sevdalis, 2009).

The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF) is a self-report

30-item questionnaire designed to measure global trait emotional intelligence (Petrides

& Furnham, 2006). It is based on the long form of the TEIQue (Petrides & Furnham,

2003), which consists of 153 items, rated on a 7-point Likert scale, and 13 facets,

organised under the 4 factors of well-being, self-control, emotionality and sociability

(Freudenthaler, Neubauer, Gabler, Scherl & Rindermann, 2008; Mikolajczak, Luminet,

Leroy & Roy, 2007). Two items from each of the 15 subscales of the original TEIQue

were selected for inclusion. These items were selected based primarily on their

Page 78: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 68 -

correlations with the corresponding total subscale scores. This procedure was followed

in order to ensure adequate internal consistencies and broad coverage of the sampling

domain of the construct. Items were responded to on a 7-point Likert scale. The TEIQue

was constructed with the aim of providing comprehensive coverage of the trait

emotional intelligence domain (Petrides & Furnham, 2001).

In order to validate trait emotional intelligence Petrides and colleagues (Petrides &

Furnham, 2001; Petrides et al., 2007) have demonstrated the isolation of an oblique trait

emotional intelligence factor in both Eysenckian and Big Five factor space.

Consequently, trait emotional intelligence has provided evidence of its discriminant

validity in terms of well-established personality dimensions. In addition, there is

accumulating evidence regarding the incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence

measures in predicting a wide range of variables such as life satisfaction coping styles,

depression and loneliness. These findings apply specifically to the TEIQue

(Freudenthaler et al. 2007).

A study conducted by Freudenthaler et al. (2007) found that the internal consistencies

of the German TEIQue’s twenty variables (15 facets, four factors and global score) were

generally excellent and similar to those reported for the original TEIQue and its various

translations. Importantly, this study also replicated the four factor structure of the

TEIQue (Freudenthaler et al., 2007). Freudenthaler et al. (2007) provide conclusive

evidence that the TEIQue is a valid and reliable inventory for the comprehensive

measurement of trait emotional intelligence.

According to Mikolajczak et al. (2007) 10 of the 15 TEIQue subscales had Cronbach’s

alphas indicative of acceptable to excellent reliability (varying from 0.71 to 0.91) for both

men and women. Two subscales, Self-Motivation and Empathy, had acceptable

reliability among men but lower reliability among women. Finally, three subscales,

Impulsiveness, Relationship Skills and Adaptability, had Cronbach’s alphas below 0.70

for both men and women. Internal consistencies at the factor level were excellent for

both men and women, and this was also the case for the global score (Mikolajczak et

al., 2007). Significant gender differences exist with women scoring higher on

Page 79: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 69 -

emotionality and men scoring higher on self-control, sociability and the global score

(Mikolajczak et al., 2007).

The TEIQue-SF has demonstrated high internal consistency (Sevdalis et al., 2007) for

both males and females (Petrides & Furham, 2006). Cronbach alpha coefficients for the

TEIQue-SF have been indicated as 0.84 for males and 0.89 for females (Petrides &

Furnham, 2006). Petrides and Furham (2006) state that, in terms of incremental

predictive validity, it has been repeatedly demonstrated that trait emotional intelligence

can account for variance over and above the giant three or the big five personality

factors. The same authors found a mean of 158.1 for males (SD 17.5) and 156.9 for

females (SD 19.8) in their sample of 167 participants. 87 of these participants were

female and education levels ranged from high school diplomas to postgraduate

qualifications (Petrides & Furnham, 2006).

A study conducted by Hardy (2006) investigated the association between sense of

coherence, emotional intelligence and health behaviour. Participants in the study were

staff members from the University of Johannesburg, who were over the age of 22.

Hardy (2006) found that the mean for the total score of the TEIQue was 155.96 and the

standard deviation was 28.41. Scores were also calculated separately for male and

female participants. The mean score for men was 161.52 and standard deviation was

23.23. The mean score for women was 158.05 and standard deviation was 23.87.

4.6.3 Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey

The Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey (MBI-SS) is an adapted version of the

Maslach Burnout Inventory-General Survey (MBI-GS). It has been adapted for use

among students (Schaufeli et al., 2002). For instance, the item “I feel emotionally

drained from my work [italics added]” was changed to “I feel emotionally drained from

my study [italics added].” (Schaufeli et al., 2002, p. 467) The MBI-SS consists of 15

items that constitute three scales: exhaustion, cynicism and professional efficacy. Five

items measure exhaustion, four items measure cynicism and six items measure

professional efficacy. All items are scored on a 7-point frequency rating scale ranging

Page 80: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 70 -

from 0 (never) to 6 (always). As with other versions of the MBI, there no norms are

available for the MBI-SS. However, high scores on exhaustion and cynicism and low

scores on professional efficacy are indicative of burnout (Schaufeli et al., 2002).

The MBI-SS has been researched in South Africa (Mostert et al., 2007; Pienaar &

Sieberhagen, 2005). Mostert et al. (2007) administered the assessment to 353 students

from a recently merged university in South Africa. This sample included Afrikaans and

Setswana speaking students from different year groups enrolled in various courses in

the Economic and Management Sciences Faculty at the two campuses of the relevant

university. The study found Cronbach alpha coefficients of 0.74 for exhaustion and 0.68

for cynicism. The study did not investigate professional efficacy. Although these

reliabilities are not considered high, Mostert et al. (2007) did provide evidence for the

construct validity and reliability of the MBI-SS for South African university students. One

item from the cynicism scale (“I have become more cynical about the potential

usefulness of my studies”) was found to be statistically insignificant (Mostert et al.,

2007).

A study by Pienaar and Sieberhagen (2005) made use of the MBI-SS in the assessment

of 154 student leaders. Most of the student leaders had satisfactory academic records.

Most of the student leaders were white and Afrikaans speaking. The study found

Cronbach alpha coefficients of 0.79 for exhaustion, 0.73 for cynicism and 0.76 for

professional efficacy. Morgan (2009) found Cronbach alphas for the MBI-SS of 0.87 for

emotional exhaustion, 0.88 for personal accomplishment and 0.78 for professional

efficacy. These reliability alphas are considered satisfactory.

4.7 Data analyses

Based on the research problem, the general purpose of this study was to ascertain

whether a relationship exists between emotional intelligence and burnout. The data

analysis in the study was performed by using the Statistical Package for Social

Sciences (SPSS, version 18). Descriptive statistics were used to analyse the data.

Frequencies, mean scores and standard deviations were used to describe the sample’s

Page 81: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 71 -

scores. The reliabilities of the subscales and total scores were estimated by means of

Cronbach alpha coefficients.

The relationships between the scores on the measures of emotional intelligence and

burnout were investigated by means of Pearson’s product-moment correlations

(Kerlinger & Lee, 2000). The levels of significance of the correlations were considered

at p ≤ 0.01 and p ≤ 0.05. Correlation coefficients of less than 0.10 are considered

insignificant (Kerlinger & Lee, 2000). Correlations of 0.30 are seen as moderate and

correlations of 0.50 and higher are considered to be large.

4.8 Chapter summary

This chapter discussed the research design and method employed in the study. The

research instruments were also discussed. The following chapter includes the results of

the study.

Page 82: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 72 -

CHAPTER 5

5. RESULTS

5.1 Introduction

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between emotional intelligence and

burnout among postgraduate university students. In this chapter results of the study are

presented. All statistical procedures were performed using SPSS (version 18).

5.2 Descriptive statistics of the sample

The sample consisted of 225 postgraduate students aged between 21 and 62 years.

There were more women (N= 172) in the sample than men (N= 45). The largest portion

of the sample consisted of white participants (N=112). There were 86 black participants,

12 Asian participants and 8 coloured participants. Almost 60% of the participants were

English and Afrikaans speaking. Half the sample reported having part-time work.

5.3 Descriptive statistics of the MBI-SS

The means, standard deviations and minimum and maximum scores were ascertained

for each of the three dimensions of the MBI-SS. The table below provides this

information.

Table 5.1 Mean Scores, Standard Deviations, Minimum and Maximum scores for

the MBI-SS

MBI-SS scale Min Max Mean Std Dev Standardised mean

Emotional exhaustion 0 30 15.56 7.07 3.11

Cynicism 0 24 6.65 5.64 1.66

Professional efficacy 0 36 25.94 5.90 4.32

The burnout scales have different ranges. Emotional exhaustion has a possible

minimum score of 0 and a possible maximum score of 30. Cynicism has a possible

Page 83: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 73 -

minimum score of 0 and a possible maximum score of 24. Professional efficacy has a

possible range of 0 to 36. Due to the differences in range a decision was taken to

standardise the mean scores obtained for these three subscales. The standardised

maximum score for each scale is 6 and the minimum score remains 0. The

standardised mean scores allowed the participants’ scores on the subscales to be

compared.

Schaufeli et al. (2002) state that burnout amongst students’ manifests as feeling

exhausted because of study demands, having a cynical and detached attitude toward

one’s study, and feeling incompetent as a student. Therefore, high scores on the

emotional exhaustion and cynicism scales and low scores on professional efficacy are

indicative of burnout.

In this study professional efficacy had the highest standardised mean score (M = 4.32).

Professional efficacy refers to being effective at one’s job and feeling able to contribute

in a meaningful way (Pienaar & Sieberhagen, 2005). These participants appear to feel

relatively competent as students (Schaufeli et al., 2002). They are likely to feel that they

are good students and are able to make an effective contribution to the classes they

attend (Schaufeli et al., 2002). In reference to the professional efficacy component of

Schaufeli et al.’s (2002) definition of burnout these students do not appear to be

suffering from burnout.

The second highest score was emotional exhaustion (M = 3.11). This score is average

in relation to the standardised maximum score of 6. This indicates that some students

may have average levels of emotional exhaustion. These individuals may sometimes

feel emotionally depleted (Jordaan, 2005), incapable of performing (Montgomery et al.,

2005) or unable to ‘give’ of themselves at a psychological level (Schaufeli & Enzmann,

1998). Some students in this research may show no signs of emotional exhaustion.

Lastly, cynicism yielded a low standardised mean (M = 1.66). Cynicism refers to having

a cynical or detached attitude towards studies (Schaufeli et al., 2002). These students

do not seem to have a sceptical or distrustful attitude towards studies, lecturers and

Page 84: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 74 -

other students.

The scores discussed above indicate that the students in the sample do not generally

appear to be suffering from burnout. Instead, they tend to feel competent as students,

have average levels of emotional exhaustion and do not have cynical attitudes towards

their studies and the individuals involved in their studies.

Mean scores were used to compare this study to that of Pienaar and Sieberhagen’s

(2005). The mean obtained for emotional exhaustion (M = 15.56) in this study was

higher than the mean (M = 12.97) obtained by Pienaar and Sieberhagen (2005).

However, the mean obtained in this study for cynicism (M = 6.65) was lower than the

mean (M = 9.94) obtained in Pienaar and Sieberhagen’s (2005) study. This study

reported a mean of 25.94 for professional efficacy, which is significantly higher than the

mean (M = 18.62) obtained in Pienaar and Sieberhagen’s (2005) study. The students in

Pienaar and Sieberhagen’s (2005) study were student leaders and it is possible that

these students reported less emotional exhaustion as a result of their ability to manage

multiple roles effectively. The students in this study possibly have a higher sense of

professional efficacy because they are postgraduate university students. These

postgraduate students may have learned to effectively solve problems that arise in their

studies and to feel confident that they are effective in completing tasks (Schaufeli et al.,

2002).

The study conducted by Morgan (2009) found a significantly lower mean for emotional

exhaustion (M = 14.22) than the emotional exhaustion mean found in this study (M =

15.56). Morgan’s (2009) mean of 6.15 for cynicism is comparable with the cynicism

mean obtained in this study (M = 6.65). Lastly, the mean of 26.10 for professional

efficacy in Morgan’s (2009) study compares well to the mean of 25.94 obtained in this

study.

5.4 The reliability coefficients of the MBI-SS

The reliability coefficients of the MBI-SS were determined through the use of Cronbach

Page 85: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 75 -

alpha coefficients. The Cronbach alpha coefficient provides an actual estimate of

reliability (Nunnaly & Bernstein, 1994). The Cronbach alpha coefficients for this study

were 0.85 for emotional exhaustion, 0.79 for cynicism and 0.76 for professional efficacy.

These reliability coefficients are considered satisfactory (Cohen, 1988; Nunnaly &

Bernstein, 1994).

A study by Morgan (2009) also assessed the reliability and validity of the MBI-SS in the

South African university context. The study found the instrument to be valid and reliable

for use in the South African context. The reliability coefficients were satisfactory with all

alpha coefficients exceeding 0.70 (emotional exhaustion = 0.87; cynicism = 0.88;

professional efficacy = 0.78). The reliability coefficients calculated in the current study

compare favourably with the ones reported by Morgan (2009).

5.5 Descriptive statistics for the TEIQue

Table 5.2 Mean Score, Standard Deviation, Minimum and Maximum Scores for the

TEIQue

Minimum Maximum Mean Standard Deviation

TEIQue 76 208 153.48 21.99

In this study the mean for the TEIQue total score was 153.48 and the standard deviation

was 21.99. The maximum score for this scale is 208. Thus, the mean score indicates

that the students in the sample appear to have relatively high levels of emotional

intelligence. This could be due to the fact that the students in the sample were all

postgraduate students. These students have already completed an undergraduate

degree and have possibly learned to regulate their emotions, use support in an effective

manner and have an adequate belief in their own abilities.

A study by Hardy (2006) obtained a mean of 155.96 for the TEIQue. This mean

compares favourably to the mean obtained in this study (M = 153.48). A study

conducted by Petrides and Furnham (2006) assessed 167 participants, of whom 87

Page 86: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 76 -

were female. The TEIQue was used to measure levels of emotional intelligence in

females and males. The study found a TEIQue mean for males of 158.1 and a mean of

156.9 for females. These means compare relatively well to the mean obtained in this

study (M = 153.48).

Furnham and Petrides (2003) completed a study with a sample of 88 individuals whose

mean age was 19 years. The mean obtained for the TEIQue was 143.93, which is lower

than the mean (M = 153.48) obtained in this study. The mean age of the participants in

this study was 29.34. According to Kafetsios (2004), older individuals score higher on

most of the emotional intelligence components. This is indicative of a developmental

criterion for emotional abilities.

5.6 The reliability coefficients of the TEIQue

The TEIQue measures four factors: emotionality, well-being, self-control and sociability.

Reliability coefficients were obtained for the TEIQue by means of Cronbach alpha

coefficients. The Cronbach alpha coefficient for the well-being scale in this study was

0.71, which can be regarded as acceptable (Nunnaly & Bernstein, 1994). However, the

reliability coefficients obtained for the other three factors are not considered acceptable

(self control scale = 0.60; emotionality scale = 0.65; sociability scale = 0.63). The

reliability coefficient for the global emotional intelligence scale was 0.87, which can be

viewed as acceptable. Based on these results a decision was taken to use only the

global score in the interpretation of data. This decision was in line with suggestions

provided by Petrides (2006). The TEIQue-SF was primarily designed to yield global trait

emotional intelligence scores and is an efficient measure of global trait emotional

intelligence (Petrides, 2006). It is recommended that researchers interested in factor or

subscale scores should use the original longer version of the TEIQue (Petrides, 2006).

5.7 The relationship between emotional intelligence and burnout

The relationships between the three dimensions of burnout and emotional intelligence

were investigated through determining Pearson product-moment correlation

Page 87: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 77 -

coefficients. According to Tabachnick and Fidell (2001), coefficients greater than 0.30

may be regarded as practically meaningful. The significance of the correlations was

considered at the p ≤ 0.05 and p ≤ 0.01 levels. Analysis of the data in this study

revealed several statistically significant correlations at the 0.01 level of significance.

Table 5.3 presents the correlation matrix.

Table 5.3 Correlation Matrix of Emotional intelligence and three burnout

component

Exhaustion Cynicism

Professional

Efficacy

Exhaustion 1

Cynicism 0.540** 1

Professional efficacy 0.65 0.300** 1

TEIQue -0.257** -0.366** 0.428**

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

The table indicates significant correlations between the three dimensions of burnout and

emotional intelligence. A statistically significant negative correlation was found between

emotional intelligence and exhaustion (r = -0.257; p < 0.01). This can be regarded as

small to medium correlation (Kerlinger & Lee, 2000). There was also a statistically

significant negative correlation between emotional intelligence and cynicism (r = -0.366;

p < 0.01). In accordance with Tabachnick and Fidell’s (2001) recommendation that

coefficients > 0.30 may be regarded as meaningful, this correlation was regarded as

practically meaningful. There was a significant positive correlation between professional

efficacy and emotional intelligence (r = 0.428; p < 0.01). This relationship can also be

regarded as practically meaningful (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).

Mikolajczak et al. (2007) conducted a study investigating the protective effect of trait

emotional intelligence in relation to occupational stress amongst nurses. The Trait

Page 88: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 78 -

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-SF (TEIQue) was used to assess emotional

intelligence and the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) was used to assess levels of

burnout. Mikolaiczak et al. (2007) reported a statistically significant negative correlation

(r = -0.49; p < 0.001) between emotional exhaustion and trait emotional intelligence.

There was also a statistically significant negative correlation (r = -0.30; p < 0.001)

between depersonalisation and trait emotional intelligence. Finally, a statistically

significant negative correlation (r = -0.45; p < 0.001) was found between diminished

accomplishment and trait emotional intelligence. These findings support the results of

the current study.

5.8 Summary of the results

The three scales of the MBI-SS showed acceptable Cronbach alpha reliability

coefficients. The reliability coefficient of the global emotional intelligence can also be

regarded as acceptable (Nunnaly & Bernstein, 1994).

The relationships between the three components of burnout and emotional intelligence

were also determined. There was a small negative correlation between emotional

intelligence and exhaustion. This indicates that the higher the levels of emotional

intelligence, the lower the levels of exhaustion. In contrast, lower levels of emotional

intelligence are associated with higher the levels of exhaustion. There was also a

significant negative correlation between emotional intelligence and cynicism. This

indicates that the higher the levels of emotional intelligence, the less cynical and

detached the individual will be. However, the lower the levels of emotional intelligence,

the more cynical and detached an individual will be. Finally, there was a significant

positive correlation between professional efficacy and emotional intelligence. This

suggests that the higher the levels of emotional intelligence, the more individuals feel

that they are competent and successful. In contrast, the lower the levels of emotional

intelligence, the less individuals feel that they are competent and successful at what

they are doing.

Page 89: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 79 -

5.9 Conclusion

In this chapter, the results of the research were discussed in terms of descriptive

statistics and correlations. In the following chapter the discussion of the results

continues. The implications of the study, limitations and further research

recommendations are also discussed.

Page 90: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 80 -

CHAPTER 6

6. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION

6.1 Introduction

This chapter presents a discussion of the results of the study. Recommendations for

future research are made and the limitations of the study are highlighted.

6.2 Summary of the study

6.2.1 The aim of the study

This study aimed to evaluate the levels of emotional intelligence and burnout amongst

postgraduate university students. In addition, the study aimed to explore whether a

relationship exists between emotional intelligence and burnout amongst these students.

Emotional intelligence was understood in terms of trait emotional intelligence. Trait

emotional intelligence refers to the extent to which an individual is able to deal with

emotion-laden information (Petrides, Furnham & Frederickson, 2004). Individuals with

higher trait emotional intelligence are able to perceive, process and apply emotion-laden

information better and are considered psychologically more stable (Hardy, 2006). The

study made use of Maslach and Jackson’s (1986 cited in Shaufeli & Enzmann, 1998, p.

31) definition of burnout which states that “Burnout is a syndrome of emotional

exhaustion, depersonalisation, and reduced personal accomplishment that can occur

among individuals who do ‘people work’ of some kind”. This definition has previously

been used in various work contexts and non-occupational settings (Maslach, 1993).

6.2.2 Design, participants and procedure

This study implemented a survey design. The sample consisted of 225 postgraduate

students from a large South African metropolitan university. The participants

represented different faculties as well as different racial and language groups.

Postgraduate lecturers were contacted and permission was obtained to administer the

questionnaires at the end of lectures. The research was explained to the participants.

Page 91: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 81 -

Students were allowed to refuse participation and anonymity was ensured. Participants

completed the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey (MBI-SS; Schaufeli et al.,

2002) and the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue; Petrides

& Furnham, 2006).

6.2.3 Research questions

The research questions were:

1. What are the levels of emotional intelligence in a sample of postgraduate

university students?

2. What are the levels of burnout in a sample of postgraduate university students?

3. Is there is a statistically significant relationship between burnout and emotional

intelligence amongst a sample of postgraduate university students?

6.2.4 Discussion of the results pertaining to the three dimensions of the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey

The results pertaining to the three dimensions of burnout – (emotional exhaustion,

cynicism and professional efficacy). Due to the differences in the ranges of the burnout

scales it was decided to standardise the mean scores for these three scales in order to

compare the scores. However, the original mean scores are used when the results of

this study are compared to previous research.

Emotional exhaustion is a central component of burnout and the most obvious

manifestation of this complex syndrome (Maslach et al., 2001). The maximum score for

emotional exhaustion on the MBI-SS is 30. In this study the mean score for emotional

exhaustion was 15.56. This mean is higher than desired but still falls in the average

range. It appears that this group of students has standard levels of exhaustion. Certain

individuals may feel emotionally depleted (Jordaan, 2005), incapable of performing

(Montgomery et al., 2005) or unable to ‘give’ themselves at a psychological level

Page 92: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 82 -

(Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). However, most students are likely to feel normal levels of

exhaustion and emotional fatigue appropriate to their position as postgraduate students.

According to the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey (MBI-SS; Schaufeli et al.,

2002), many of these students do not generally feel emotionally drained by their studies

and they do not feel ‘used up’ at the end of a university day. They are unlikely to

become disinterested and unenthusiastic about their studies or to feel that attending

class is a drain (Schaufeli et al., 2002). The higher than desired level of emotional

exhaustion may be due to the postgraduate nature of the studies and the stringent

academic requirements of the courses. Furthermore, half the sample reported working

part-time or full-time in addition to studying. This dual role can increase levels of

emotional exhaustion. In addition, the sample in this study consisted of more women

than men. Women tend to score slightly higher on emotional exhaustion (Grayson &

Alvarez, 2008; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998) and thus the high score may in part be

attributed to this gender distribution.

This study’s mean for emotional exhaustion compares well to the mean (M = 14.22) for

emotional exhaustion achieved by Morgan (2009). Morgan’s (2009) study included

participants completing undergraduate and postgraduate degrees at the same

metropolitan university as the present study. Pienaar and Sieberhagen’s (2005) study

included all members of the Students’ Representative Council and members of the

House Committees of all residences of a university. The mean (M = 12.97) for emotional

exhaustion in Pienaar and Sieberhagen’s (2005) study is slightly lower than the mean

reported in this study.

Cynicism, which is similar to depersonalisation, had a mean score of 6.65 in the present

study. This mean seems to be low in relation to the maximum score of 24. Thus, the

students in this study did not generally present with a cynical and detached attitude

towards their studies (Schaufeli et al., 2002). Cordes and Dougherty (1993) view

cynicism as a type of coping, which is an acceptable and professional response to the

stressful situation. However, when this coping strategy becomes a habitual pattern the

person becomes dysfunctional and the coping strategy disrupts adequate task

Page 93: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 83 -

performance (Montgomery et al., 2005). It is possible that the individuals in the present

study do not use cynicism as a coping strategy. This would indicate that the participants

are usually able to perform tasks effectively. The students who participated in the study

are likely to have a positive attitude towards study as they have decided to pursue

further academic studies. It is possible that they value academia and believe in the

significance of studying.

Morgan’s (2009) study found a mean score of 6.15 for cynicism, which compares well

with the mean (M = 6.65) in this study. Pienaar and Sieberhagen (2005) obtained a

slightly higher mean (M = 9.94) than the mean obtained in this study. According to

Pieanaar and Sieberhagen (2005) the high levels of cynicism in their study might be due

to the fact that their participants were student leaders who constantly have to deal with

high job demands, in the form of dealing with students’ problems and working under

pressure. These job demands may have resulted in their sample becoming more

cynical.

A mean score of 25.94 was presented for professional efficacy. This mean score seems

to be high in relation to the maximum possible score of 36. These students do not

appear to have a reduced sense of competency (Maslach, 1998). According to Pienaar

and Sieberhagen (2005) individuals can be professionally efficacious if they have

adequate resources at their disposal. Resources include availability of support,

adequate pay and opportunities for training. It is likely that the students in the present

study had financial (and perhaps emotional) support as they were able to continue on to

postgraduate study.

Morgan’s (2009) study reported a mean score of 26.10 for professional efficacy, which

compares well with the mean in this study. Pienaar and Sieberhagen (2005) obtained a

mean score of 18.62 for professional efficacy, which is slightly lower than the mean

reported in this study. Pienaar and Sieberhagen (2005) state that the dependent

variable of professional efficacy was best described by the independent variable of

resources. This could be due to the fact that if one has adequate resources it is possible

to be effective at one’s job. It is possible that the student leaders in Pienaar and

Page 94: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 84 -

Sieberhagen’s (2005) study did not have ample resources and therefore could not do

their jobs effectively.

Professional efficacy has the highest standardised mean score (M = 4.32) of the three

burnout components. Emotional exhaustion (M = 3.11) had the second highest

standardised mean score. Cynicism had a standardised mean score of 1.66. These

results indicate that the postgraduate students feel professionally efficient in their

abilities as academic individuals and experience a normal sense of emotional

exhaustion. Emotional exhaustion arises from feelings of tension and frustration

resulting from an individual’s fears that they will be unable to provide previous levels of

work performance (Swider & Zimmerman, 2010). The students in this sample were not

emotionally exhausted but average levels of tension and stress were present. This is

possibly related to the fact that these students are postgraduate students and therefore

experience significant amounts of pressure.

6.3 Emotional intelligence according to the trait emotional intelligence approach

In this study, the mean score for the TEIQue was 153.48. The range of this scale is from

30 to 210. Hardy (2006) obtained a total mean score of 155.96, which is similar to the

mean score obtained in this study. The mean score for the TEIQue in this study was

relatively high. This indicates that the participants have relatively high levels of

emotional intelligence.

Trait emotional intelligence has many different facets. The construct includes individual

dispositions related to the perception, processing, regulation and utilisation of emotional

information (Mikolajczak, Nelis, Hansenne & Quoidbach, 2008). Petrides and Furnham

(2001) state that trait emotional intelligence includes various constructs from the

personality domain such as empathy and assertiveness as well as elements of social

intelligence, personal intelligence (Gardner, 1993) and ability emotional intelligence

(Mayer & Salovey, 1997). The facets of trait emotional intelligence are: adaptability;

assertiveness; the perception, expression, management and regulation of emotions;

Page 95: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 85 -

self-esteem; low impulsiveness; relationship skills; self-motivation; stress management;

social competence; trait empathy; trait happiness; and trait optimism (Petrides &

Furnham, 2001; Smith, Heaven & Ciarrochi, 2008).

Research has found trait emotional intelligence scores to be strong predictors of the

impact of stressful life events (Mikolajczak & Luminet, 2008). Trait emotional intelligence

might influence the choice of coping strategies, which are the behavioural and

psychological approaches that people use to deal with negative events (Mikolajczak &

Luminet, 2008). A high level of trait emotional intelligence predisposes individuals to

think and act in ways that encourage positive emotional experiences while discouraging

negative emotional experiences. Trait emotional intelligence is negatively associated

with burnout (Mikolajczak et al., 2007).

The participants’ high levels of emotional intelligence could be due to the acquisition of

facets of trait emotional intelligence during their years of study. In order to successfully

reach a postgraduate level of study students need resources and traits that will assist

them in being effective. These traits and resources include assertiveness, self-esteem,

self-motivation and stress management. These are all qualities of trait emotional

intelligence.

These postgraduate students are also likely to utilise effective coping strategies in order

to manage stressful periods such as examinations. Effective and productive coping

strategies could have developed during undergraduate studies or during part or full-time

employment. The participants may employ these techniques in their postgraduate

studies, leading to high levels of emotional intelligence.

6.4 Discussion of the results pertaining to the relationship between burnout and emotional intelligence

The relationship between emotional exhaustion and emotional intelligence yielded a

small to moderate negative relationship (r = -0.257, p < 0.01). This means that those

individuals with high levels of emotional intelligence were less likely to be emotionally

Page 96: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 86 -

exhausted. Those individuals with lower levels of emotional intelligence were more likely

to be emotionally exhausted. Previous research has shown that individuals with high

levels of emotional intelligence are able to reduce negative moods more effectively than

low emotional intelligence individuals. These individuals have higher mood regulation

and thus report more positive affect because they are likely to be able to sustain their

positive moods (Spence et al., 2004). Emotional exhaustion involves a feeling that

emotional resources have been depleted (Maslach, 1998). This indicates that when

individuals with high emotional intelligence experience exhaustion they are likely to be

able to reduce these feelings effectively. Negative associations have been found

between psychological distress (Austin et al., 2004) and high emotional intelligence

levels. High trait emotional intelligence individuals are more successful at meeting the

demands of stressful situations because they are better able to perceive, appraise and

regulate their emotions (Bastian et al., 2005). Therefore, individuals with high levels of

emotional intelligence are less likely to become emotionally exhausted than their low

emotional intelligence counterparts.

Cynicism and emotional intelligence had a moderate negative relationship (r = -0.366, p

< 0.01). This correlation is stronger than the correlation between emotional exhaustion

and emotional intelligence. Individuals with lower levels of emotional intelligence are

likely to feel more cynical and detached towards their studies. However, individuals who

are better able to regulate and utilise emotional information are unlikely to use this

coping strategy.

The components of emotional intelligence are related to a number of coping techniques,

including rumination, social support networks and the disclosure of trauma. This

indicates that higher emotional intelligence should be associated with better coping and

the use of more effective coping strategies (Bastian et al., 2005). Cynicism is a negative

coping strategy and therefore individuals who use this strategy are more prone to

burnout.

Professional efficacy and emotional intelligence had a significant positive relationship (r

= 0.428, p < 0.01). This indicates that individuals with a higher level of emotional

Page 97: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 87 -

intelligence are more likely to feel a sense of personal accomplishment and believe in

their ability to perform. Students who have lower levels of emotional intelligence are

likely to view themselves negatively in many areas and suffer from feelings of

inadequacy.

Professional efficacy relates to feelings of competence and belief in one’s own ability to

complete tasks (Schaufeli et al., 2002). Individuals experiencing a lack of professional

efficacy view themselves and their abilities negatively (Cordes & Dougherty, 1993). Self-

efficacy, which is closely related to the self-esteem facet of trait emotional intelligence,

refers to belief in one’s ability to organise and execute the required course of action

(Perrewe et al., 2002). Self-efficacy is related to professional efficacy and higher

degrees of burnout (Brouwers & Tomic, 2000).

6.5 Limitations of the study

All studies have certain limitations. One of the limitations of this research is that

students from only one of the campuses at one metropolitan university were included in

the sample. Therefore the results are not generalisable to the university population of

South Africa. Some universities may place more responsibility and pressure on their

students to achieve. Higher and more stringent standards of accomplishment may also

be in place. The students in this study also had a limited language distribution. Students

of all languages were included in the research, but most of the students who

participated were English and Afrikaans speaking. This means that the results of this

study cannot be generalised cross-culturally.

All the measures used in this study were self-report measures. Social desirability bias is

therefore a possible threat to the validity of these measures and may impact on the

accuracy of the measurement of the constructs (Hardy, 2006). The social desirability

effect is a reflection of research participants’ desire to be seen to conform to general

social norms. Participants may be unwilling to report negative feelings or they may

falsify any information or behaviour that might be interpreted in a negative or

unfavourable manner (Dyer, 1995). It is possible that participants answered the

Page 98: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 88 -

questions in a manner that would seem favourable to the researcher rather than in an

honest and truthful way. The participants answered the burnout questionnaire to

indicate that they feel confident and effective as students. However, they also stated

that they feel emotionally exhausted and burned out from their studies. It is possible that

the students experienced anxiety concerning admitting to feelings of incompetence and

instead provided answers to the questionnaire that indicate that they feel competent.

Survival bias constitutes another limitation of the present research. Survival bias refers

to the fact that individuals who generally have reduced experiences of burnout remain at

work, while those individuals experiencing high levels of burnout tend to leave work

(Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1988). In the context of this research it is possible that students

who experienced burnout at the undergraduate level did not register for a postgraduate

degree. These students are also likely to have dropped out of their programmes if

burnout was severe.

A further limitation relates to the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue).

This instrument has not been standardised for the South African population. This may

have impacted on the validity of the TEIQue for measuring emotional intelligence in the

South African population. However, this instrument was used because the researcher

chose to assess trait emotional intelligence rather than ability emotional intelligence.

The TEIQue has demonstrated high internal consistency (Sevdalis et al., 2007) for both

males and females (Petrides & Furham, 2006) in international studies.

6.6 Suggestions for future research

Future research should include students from various South African universities in order

to obtain a more standardised result that can be generalised to all postgraduate

students in South Africa. Students from various language groups should also be

included. There should be a relatively equal balance of English, Afrikaans and African

language students.

Furthermore, studies should explore burnout in different demographic and cultural

Page 99: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 89 -

groups within South Africa in order to reflect the multi-racial and multi-cultural nature of

the country. Thus, students from various provinces should participate in future research,

allowing individuals from different ethnic and language groups to be involved.

Research into factors influencing student burnout is currently limited. The large body of

research on burnout within the work context cannot be generalised to the student

population. Studies focusing specifically on student burnout may lead to prevention

strategies being instituted at universities and thereby limit the amount of student

burnout.

Management and interventions programmes for student burnout should also be

explored. Burnout amongst students is a real problem and action needs to be taken in

order to assist students that are experiencing symptoms. Support groups should be

established for sufferers of burnout. Greater social support is associated with less

emotional exhaustion, less depersonalisation and a greater sense of personal

accomplishment (Jacobs & Dodd, 2003).

A further recommendation for future research involves the standardisation of the Trait

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue) for the South African population in order

to ensure its reliability and validity for the South African context. This will allow for more

accurate results when assessing trait emotional intelligence in the South African

population.

A qualitative study exploring the experience of individuals with high levels of emotional

intelligence may allow a more in depth understanding of these individuals. This

understanding could assist in developing programmes to increase aspects of emotional

intelligence such as use of effective coping strategies. This may assist in curtailing

levels of student burnout.

6.7 Implications of the study

This study informs lecturers, parents and peers of the existence of burnout in the

Page 100: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 90 -

student population. It also allows individuals who are involved with students to

recognise the huge impact that burnout may have on a student’s life. Burnout can affect

a student psychologically, physically, cognitively and behaviourally (Schaufeli &

Enzmann, 1998). If burnout is undetected it can lead to dangerous consequences such

as drug addiction and suicidal ideation (Weckwerth & Flynn, 2006).

Relatively few studies have looked at burnout amongst students and none of these

studies has investigated the relationship between burnout and emotional intelligence.

This study provides information on how levels of emotional intelligence can affect levels

of burnout.

It is important that a valid and reliable measure is used to measure burnout. Morgan

(2009) has reported that the MBI-SS has been shown to be valid and reliable for South

African students. This is important in terms of the ethical requirements set out by the

Health Professions Council of South Africa and is also in line with best test practice and

use in South Africa.

An important aspect of emotional intelligence is that certain areas of emotional

intelligence can be learned and increased. Some emotional abilities and habits can be

improved effectively, even through relatively short training (Nelis et al., 2009). A study

conducted by Clarke (2010) found that the emotional intelligence ability ‘understanding

emotions’ can be increased if a specific training programme is utilised. If we teach

individuals to be more emotionally intelligent, they can learn to adapt to and deal with

stress more effectively.

6.8 Conclusion

This study investigated the relationship between burnout and emotional intelligence in

postgraduate students. It was previously believed that only those in the human service

professions could burn out (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1988). However, this concept has

been extended to all professions as well as to the student population.

Page 101: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 91 -

Students experience many changes when entering university and this leaves them

particularly prone to stress and its negative effects (Weckwerth & Flynn, 2006). Student

burnout can have negative consequences in future areas of the students’ lives. Student

burnout is an important predictor of professional burnout when students become

professionals after graduation (Mostert et al., 2007). Thus student burnout has a

harmful effect on the student and on the economy and well-being of South Africa as a

whole (Morgan, 2009).

Due to these deleterious consequences variables that may impact on burnout need to

be examined and interventions should be implemented. This study explored the

correlation between emotional intelligence and burnout. There were significant

correlations between emotional intelligence and all three components of burnout.

More research is required concerning the role emotional intelligence plays in burnout.

Furthermore, research into how to increase emotional intelligence in the student

population is essential. Universities focus on increasing the intellectual capacity of

students but the emotional element is often ignored. Training programmes need to be

established at universities to assist students in improving their levels of emotional

intelligence.

Page 102: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 92 -

REFERENCES

Ahola, K. & Hakanen, J. (2007). Job strain, burnout and depressive symptoms: A

prospective study among dentists. Journal of Affective Disorders, 104, 103110.

Ahola, K., Kivimaki, M., Honkonen, T., Virtanen, M., Koskinen, S., Vahtera, J., et al.

(2008). Occupational burnout medically certified sickness absence: A population

based study of Finnish employees. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 64, 185-

193.

Ali, F., Amorim, I.S. & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy deficits and trait

emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and

Individual Differences, 47, 758-762.

Augusto Landa, J.M., Lopez-Zafra, E., Berrios Martos, M.P. & Del Carmen Aguilar-

Luzon, M. (2008). The relationship between emotional intelligence, occupational

stress and health in nurses: A questionnaire survey. International Journal of

Nursing Studies, 45, 888-901.

Austin, E.J. (2009). A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional

intelligence test items. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 381-383.

Austin, E.J., Evans, P.E., Goldwater, R. & Potter, V. (2005). A preliminary study of

emotional intelligence, empathy and exam performance in first year medical

students. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1395-1405.

Austin, E.J., Saklofske, D.H. & Egan, V. (2004). Personality, well-being and health

correlates of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences,

38, 547-558.

Awa, W.L., Plaumann, M. & Walter, U. (2010). Burnout prevention: A review of

intervention programs. Patient Education & Counseling, 78, 184-190.

Page 103: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 93 -

Bakker, A., Van der Zee, K., Lewig, K.A. & Dollard, M.F. (2006). The relationship

between the big five personality factors and burnout: A study among volunteer

counselors. The Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 31-50.

Bar-On, R. (1997). The Bar-On emotional quotient inventory (EQ-i): Technicalmanual.

Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

Bar-On, R. (2005). The impact of emotional intelligence on subjective well-being.

Perspectives in Education, 23(2), 41-62.

Basson, M., Martins, N., Basson, J.S. & Muller, H. (2006). The relationship between

personality preferences, self-esteem and emotional intelligence. The South

African Journal of Industrial Psychology, 32(2), 64-73.

Bastian, V.A., Burns, N.R. & Nettelback, T. (2005). Emotional intelligence predicts life

skills, but not as well as personality and cognitive abilities. Personality and

Individual Differences, 39, 1135-1145.

Bernhard, H.C. (2007). A survey of burnout among college music majors. College

Student Journal, 41, 392-402.

Black, T.R. (1999). Doing quantitative research in the social sciences: An integrated

approach to research design, measurement and statistics. London: Sage.

Blonk, R.W.B., Brenninkmeijer, V., Lagerveld, S.E. & Houtman, I.L.D. (2006). Return to

work: A comparison of two cognitive behavioural interventions in cases of work-

related psychological complaints among the self-employed. Work and Stress, 20,

129-144.

Borritz, M., Rugulies, R., Bjorner, J.B., Villadsen, E., Mikkelsen, O.A. & Kristensen, T.S.

(2006). Burnout among employees in human service work: Design and baseline

findings of the PUMA study. Scandanavian Journal of Public Health, 34, 49-58.

Page 104: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 94 -

Brackett, M.A., Mayer, J.D. & Warner, R.M. (2004). Emotional intelligence and its

relation to everyday behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1387-

1402.

Brackett, M.A., Rivers, S.E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N. & Salovey, P. (2006). Relating

emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and

performance measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 91, 780-795.

Brackett, M.A., Warner, R.M. & Bosco, J.S. (2005). Emotional intelligence and

relationship quality among couples. Personal Relationships, 12, 197-212.

Brand, T. (2007). An exploration of the relationship between burnout, occupational

stress and emotional intelligence in the nursing industry. Unpublished MA

dissertation. University of Stellenbosch.

Brenninkmeyer, V., VanYperen, N.W. & Buunk, B.P. (2001). Burnout and depression

are not identical twins: Is superiority a distinguishing feature? Personality and

Individual differences, 30, 873-880.

Bresó, E., Salanova, M. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2007). In search of the ‘third dimension’ of

burnout: Efficacy or inefficacy? Applied Psychology: An International Review, 56,

460-478.

Brotheridge, C.M. & Grandey, A.A. (2002). Emotional labour and burnout: Comparing

two perspectives of people work. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 17-39.

Brouwers, A. & Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of teacher burnout and

perceived self-efficacy in classroom manangement. Teaching & Teacher

Education, 16, 239-253.

Brown, R.F. & Schutte, N.S. (2006). Direct and indirect relationships between emotional

Page 105: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 95 -

intelligence and subjective fatigue in university students. Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 60, 585-593.

Brummelhuis, L.L.T., Van der Lippe, T.V.D., Kluwer, E.S. & Flap, H. (2008) Positive and

negative effects of family involvement on work-related burnout. Journal of

Vocational Behavior, 73, 387-396.

Bühler, K.E. & Land, T. (2003). Burnout and personality in intensive care: An empirical

study. Hospital Topics, 81, 5-12.

Buunk, B.P. & Schaufeli, W.B. (1993). Burnout: A perspective from social comparison

theory. In W.B. Schaufeli, C. Maslach and T. Marek (Eds), Professional burnout.

Recent developments in theory and research (pp. 53-66). Washington, DC:

Taylor & Francis.

Cano-Garcia, F.J., Padilla-Munoz, E.M. & Carrasco-Ortiz, M.A. (2005). Personality and

contextual variables in teacher burnout. Personality and Individual Differences,

38, 929-940.

Ceslowitz, S.B. (1989). Burnout and coping strategies among hospital staff nurses.

Journal of Advanced Nursing, 14, 553-557.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Bennett, E. & Furnham, A. (2007). The happy personality: the

meditational role of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual

Differences, 42, 1633-1639.

Chan, D. (2006). Emotional intelligence and components of burnout among Chinese

secondary school teachers in Hong Kong. Teaching and Teacher Education, 22,

1042-1054.

Chan, D.W. (2007a). Burnout, self-efficacy, and successful intelligence among Chinese

prospective and in-service school teachers in Hong Kong. Educational

Page 106: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 96 -

Psychology, 27, 33-49.

Chan, D.W. (2007b). Leadership competencies among Chinese gifted students in Hong

Kong: The connection with emotional intelligence and successful intelligence.

Roeper Review, 29, 183-189.

Chang, E.C., Rand, K.L. & Strunk, D.R. (2000). Optimism and risk for job burnout

among working college students: Stress as a mediator. Personality and Individual

Differences, 29, 255-263.

Cherniss, C. (1980). Staff burnout: Job stress in the human services. Beverly Hills, CA:

Sage.

Cherniss, C. (2002). Emotional intelligence and the good community. American Journal

of Community Psychology, 30, 1-11.

Ciarrochi, J., Deane, F.P. & Anderson, S. (2002). Emotional intelligence moderates the

relationship between stress and mental health. Personality and Individual

Differences, 32, 197-209.

Cipriano, J.F. (2002). Burnout and emotional intelligence: A consultation model for

emotional competency training. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Institute for

Graduate Clinical Psychology, Widener University, Pennsylvania, United states of

America

Clarke, N. (2010). The impact of a training programme designed to target the emotional

intelligence abilities of project managers. International Journal of Project

Management, 28, 461-468.

Clarke, J.N. & Everest, M.M. (2006). Cancer in the mass print media: Fear, uncertainty

and the medical model. Social Science and Medicine, 62, 2591- 2600.

Page 107: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 97 -

Coetzee, M., Martins, N., Basson, J.S. & Muller, H. (2006). The relationship between

personality preferences, self-esteem and emotional competence. South African

Journal of Industrial Psychology, 32(2), 64-73.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power for the behavioral sciences. (2nd ed.). NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum.

Coolican, H. (1996). Introduction to research methods and statistics in psychology (2nd

ed.). London: Hodder Arnold.

Cordes, C.L. & Dougherty, T.W. (1993). A review and an integration of research on job

burnout. Academy of Management Review, 18, 621-656.

Daus, C.S. & Ashkanasy, N.M. (2005). The case for the ability-based model of

emotional intelligence in organisational behaviour. Journal of Organisational

Behavior, 26, 453-466.

Davies, M., Stankov, L. & Roberts, R.D. (1998). Emotional intelligence: In search of an

elusive construct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 989-1015.

Dawda, D. & Hart, S.D. (2000). Assessing Emotional Intelligence: Reliability and validity

of the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) in university students.

Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 797-812.

De Raad, B. (2005). The trait-coverage of emotional intelligence. Personality and

Individual Differences, 38, 673-687.

Deary, I.J., Blenkin, H., Agius, R.M., Endler, N.S., Zealley, H. & Wood, R. (1996).

Models of job-related stress and personal achievement among consultant

doctors. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 3-29.

DeLongis, A. & Holtzman, S. (2005). Coping in context: The role of stress, social

Page 108: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 98 -

support, and personality in coping. Journal of Personality, 73, 1633-1656.

Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2001). The job demands-

resources model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 499-512.

Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2005). Spillover and crossover of

exhaustion and life satisfaction among dual-earner parents. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 67, 266-289.

Di Fabio, A. & Palazzeschi, L. (2009). An in-depth look at scholastic success: Fluid

intelligence, personality traits or emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual

Differences, 46, 581-585.

Douglas, C., Frink, D.D. & Ferris, G.R. (2004). Emotional intelligence as a moderator of

the relationship between conscientiousness and performance. Journal of

Leadership and Organisational Studies, 10, 2-11.

Dyer, C. (1995). Beginning research in psychology: A practical guide to research

methods and statistics. Cambrige, MA: Blackwell.

Dyrbye, L.N., Thomas, M.R., Huntington, J.L., Lawson, K.L., Novotny, P.J., Slaon, J.A.,

et al. (2006). Personal life events and medical student burnout: A multicentre

study. Academic Medicine, 81, 374-384.

Ellis, R. & Conboy, E. (2005). Healing the wounded organisation: A role for

psychotherapists. Annals of the American Psychotherapy Association, 8(1), 36-

48.

Epstein, S. (1998). Constructive thinking: The key to emotional intelligence.Westport:

Praeger.

Extremera, N. & Fernandez-Berrocal, P. (2005). Perceived emotional intelligence and

Page 109: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 99 -

life satisfaction: Predictive and incremental validity using the Trait Meta-Mood

Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 937-948.

Eysenck, H.J. (1960). The structure of human personality. London: Methuen.

Eysenck, H.J. (2001). Dimensions of personality: The biosocial approach to personality.

In H.S. Friedman and M.W. Schustack (Eds.). Readings in personality. Classic

theories and modern research (pp. 69-77). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Fancher, R.E. (1987). The intelligence men: Makers of the IQ controversy. New York:

W.W. Norton.

Farber, B.A. (Ed.). (1983). Stress and burnout in the human service professions. New

York: Pergamon.

Fernandez-Berrocal, P., Alcaide, R., Extremera, N. & Pizzaro, D. (2006). The role of

Emotional Intelligence in anxiety and depression among adolescents. Individual

Differences Research, 4, 16-27.

Fischer, H.J. (1983). A psychoanalytic view of burnout. In B.A. Farber (Ed), Stress and

burnout in the human service professions (pp. 40-45). New York: Pergamon.

Freudenberger, H.J. & Richelson, G. (1980). Burnout: The high cost of high

achievement. New York: Anchor.

Freudenthaler, H.H., Neubauer, A.C., Gabler, P., Scherl, W.G. & Rindermann, H.

(2008). Testing and validating the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire

(TEIQue) in a German-speaking sample. Personality and Individual Differences,

45, 673-678.

Furnell, B. (2007). Exploring the relationship between emotional labour, emotional

intelligence and burnout: A study on call centre representatives. Unpublished

Page 110: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 100 -

master’s thesis, University of Stellenbosch, Stellenbosch, South Africa

Furnham, A. & Petrides, K.V. (2003) Trait emotional intelligence and happiness. Social

Behaviour and Personality, 31, 815-824.

Garden, A.M. (1989). Burnout: The effect of psychological type on research findings.

Journal of Occupational Psychology, 62, 223-234.

Gardner, H. (1993). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York:

Basic.

Gardner, W.L., Fischer, D. & Hunt, J.C. (2009). Emotional labour and leadership: A

threat to authenticity. The Leadership Quarterly, 20, 466-482.

Gardner, K. & Qualter, P. (2009). Emotional intelligence and borderline personality

disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 94-98.

Garland, B. (2002). Prison treatment staff burnout: Consequences, causes and

prevention. Corrections today, 64(7), 116-121.

Garrosa, E., Moreno-Jimenez, B., Liang, Y. & Gonzalez, J.L. (2008). The relationship

between socio-demographic variables, job stressors, burnout and hardy

personality in nurses: An exploratory study. International Journal of Nursing

Studies, 45, 418-427.

Gohm, C.L. (2003). Mood regulation and emotional intelligence: Individual differences.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 594-607.

Gold, Y. & Roth, R.A. (1993). Teachers managing stress and preventing burnout: The

professional health solution. London: Farmer.

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.

Page 111: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 101 -

Goleman, D. (1996). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. London:

Bloomsbury.

Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.

Golembiewski, R.T. (2000). The handbook of organisational behaviour. Retrieved

August, 17, 2008, from

http://www.books.google.co.za/books?isbn=0824703936.

Goodwin, C.J. (2008). Research in psychology: Methods and design (5th ed.). Hoboken:

John Wiley.

Graffeo, L.C. & Silvestri, L. (2006). Relationship between locus of control and health

variables. Education, 126, 593-596.

Gravetter, F.J. & Wallnau, L.B. (2004). Statistics for the behavioral sciences (6th ed.).

Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.

Grayson, J.L. & Alvarez, H.K. (2008). School climate factors relating to teacher

burnout: A mediator model. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24, 1349-1363.

Greenglass, E.R., Burke, R.J. & Fiksenbaum, L. (2001). Workload and burnout in

nurses. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 211-215.

Gustaffson, G. & Stranberg, G. (2009). Meanings of staying healthy in a context where

others developed burnout- phenomenological-hermeneutic interpretation of

healthcare personnel’s narratives. Scandanavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 23,

456-464.

Hakanen, J.J., Bakker, A.B. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2006). Burnout and engagement among

teachers. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 495-513.

Page 112: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 102 -

Halbesleben, J.R.B. & Buckley, M.R. (2006). Social comparison and burnout: The role

of relative burnout and received social support. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 19,

259-278.

Hansenne, M. & Bianchi, J. (2009). Emotional intelligence and personality in major

depression: Trait versus state effects. Psychiatry Research, 166, 63-68.

Hardy, A. (2006). The association between sense of coherence, emotional intelligence

and health behaviour: A salutogenic perspective. Unpublished master’s thesis,

University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa.

Harrison, W.D. (1983). A social competence model of burnout. In B.A. Farber (Ed.),

Stress and burnout in the human service professions (pp. 29-39). New York:

Pergamon.

Hayes, C.T. & Weathington, B.L. (2007). Optimism, stress, life satisfaction and job

burnout in restaurant managers. The Journal of Psychology, 141, 565-579.

Heppner, P.P. & Heppner, M.J. (2004). Writing and publishing your thesis, dissertation

& research. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole-Thomson Learning.

Hobfoll, S.E. (1988). The ecology of stress: Series in health psychology and behavioural

medicine. New York: Hemisphere

Hobfoll, S.E. & Freedy, J. (1993). Conservation of resources: A general stress theory

applied to burnout. In W.B. Schaufeli, C. Maslach and T. Marek (Eds),

Professional burnout. Recent developments in theory and research (pp. 115-

129). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

Hochwalder, J. (2006). An empirical exploration of the effect of personality on general

and job-related mental ill health. Social Behavior and Personality, 34, 1051-1070.

Page 113: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 103 -

Hochwalder, J. (2007). The psychosocial work environment and burnout among

Swedish resident and assistant nurses: The main, mediating and moderating role

of empowerment. Nursing and Health Sciences, 9, 205-211.

Holmqvist , R. & Jeanneau, M. (2006). Burnout and psychiatric staffs’ feelings towards

patients. Psychiatry Research, 145, 207-213.

Innstrand, S.T., Langballe, E.M., Espnes, G.A., Falkum, E. & Aasland, O.G. (2008).

Positive and negative work-family interaction and burnout: A longitudinal study of

reciprocal relations. Work and Stress, 22, 1-15.

Isaksson, K.E., Gude, T., Tyssen, R. & Aasland, O.G. (2010). A self-referral preventive

intervention for burnout among Norwegian nurses: One-year follow-up study.

Patient Education and Counseling 78, 191-197.

Jacobs, S.R. & Dodd, D.K. (2003). Student burnout as a function of personality, social

support, and workload. Journal of College Student Development, 44, 291-303.

Jonker, B.E. (2004). Burnout, job stress and personality traits in the South African

Police Service. Unpublished master’s thesis, North West University,

Potchefstroom, South Africa.

Jordaan, I. (2005). Stress, burnout and coping strategies of South African clinical and

counselling psychologists. Unpublished master’s thesis, Nelson Mandela

Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa.

Jordaan, I., Spangenberg, J., Watson, M. & Fouche, P. (2007). Burnout and its

correlates in South African clinical and counselling psychologists. Acta

Academia, 39(1), 176-201.

Judge, T.A., Locke, E.A., Durham, C.C. & Kluger, A.N. (1998). Dispositional effects of

job and life satisfaction: The role of core evaluations. Journal of Applied

Page 114: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 104 -

Psychology, 83, 17-34.

Kafetsios, K. (2004). Attachment and emotional intelligence abilities across the life

course. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 129-145.

Kaufhold, J.A. & Johnson, L. R. (2005). The analysis of the emotional intelligence skills

and potential problem areas of elementary educators. Education, 125, 615-627.

Kerlinger, F.N. & Lee, H.B. (2000). Foundations of behavioral research (4th ed.). Fort

Worth: Harcourt.

Kim, H.J. (2008). Hotel service providers’ emotional labour: The antecedents and

effects of burnout. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 27, 151-161.

Kim, H.J., Shin, K.H. & Swanger, N. (2009). Burnout and engagement: A comparative

analysis using the big five personality dimensions. International Journal of

Hospitality Management, 28, 96-104.

Kim, H.J., Shin, K.H. & Umbreit, W.T. (2007). Hotel Job burnout: The role of personality

characteristics. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 26, 421-434.

Kobasa, S.C., Maddi, S.R. & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and health: A prospective

study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 168-177.

Kokkinos, C.M. (2007). Job stressors, personality and burnout in primary school

teachers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 229-243.

Kraft, U. (2006, June/July). Burned out. Scientific American Mind Monthly, 29.

Lackritz, J. (2004). Exploring burnout among university faculty: incidence, performance

and demographic issues. Teaching and Teacher Education, 20, 713-729.

Langelaan, S., Bakker, A.B., Van Doornen, L.J.P. & Schaufeli, W.B. (2006). Burnout

Page 115: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 105 -

and work engagement: Do individual differences make a difference? Personality

and Individual Differences, 40, 521-532.

Larsen, R.J. & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual difference

characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 1-39.

Lawler, E.J. & Thye, S.R. (1999). Bringing emotions into social exchange theory. Annual

Review of Sociology, 25, 217-245.

Lazarus, R. (2006). Emotions and interpersonal relationships: Toward a person-

centered conceptualisation of emotions and coping. Journal of Personality, 74, 9-

47.

Lee, R.T. & Ashforth, B.E. (1990). On the meaning of Maslach’s three dimensions of

burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 743-747.

Lee-Baggley, D., Preece, M. & DeLongis, A. (2005). Coping with interpersonal stress:

Role of Big Five traits. Journal of Personality, 73, 1141-1180.

Leget, C. (2003). Marth Nussbaum and Thomas Equinas on emotions. Theological

Studies, 64, 558-580.

Lemyre, P.N., Hall, H.K. & Roberts, G.C. (2008). A social cognitive approach to burnout

in elite athletes. Scandanavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, 18,

221-234.

Levert, T., Lucas, M. & Ortlepp, K. (2000). Burnout in psychiatric nurses: Contributions

of the work environment and sense of coherence. South African Journal of

Psychology, 30(2), 36-43.

Lewig, K.A., Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A.B., Dollard, M.F. & Metzer, J.C. (2007).

Burnout and connectedness among Australian volunteers: A test of the job

Page 116: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 106 -

demands-resources model. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 71, 429-445.

Lewin, J. E. & Sager, J.K. (2007). A process model of burnout among salespeople:

Some new thoughts. Journal of Business Research, 60, 1216-1224.

Lyons, J.B. & Schneider, T.R. (2005). The influence of emotional intelligence on

performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 693-703.

Lynch, T.R., Robins, C.J., Morse, J.Q. & Krause, E.D. (2001). A meditational model

relating affect intensity, emotion inhibition, and psychological distress. Behavior

Therapy, 32, 519-536.

Lyusin, D.B. (2006). Emotional intelligence as a mixed contrast: Its relation to

personality and gender. Journal of Russian & East European Psychology, 44(6),

54-68.

MacCann, C., Roberts, R.D., Matthews, G. & Zeidner, M. (2004). Consensus scoring

and empirical option weighting of performance-based emotional intelligence (EI)

tests. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 645-662.

Male, D.B. & May, D.S. (1997). Burnout and workload in teachers of children with

severe learning disabilities. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 117-121.

Male, D.B. & May, D.S. (1998). Stress and health, workload and burnout in learning

support coordinators in colleges of further education. Support for Learning, 13,

134-138.

Malterer, M.B., Glass, S.J. & Newman, J.P. (2008). Psychopathy and trait emotional

intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 735-745.

Martinez-Miranda, J. & Aldea, A. (2005). Emotions in human and artificial intelligence.

Computers in Human Behaviour, 21, 323-341.

Page 117: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 107 -

Martinez-Pons, M. (1997). The relation of emotional intelligence with selected areas of

personal functioning. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 17, 3–13.

Martinussen, M., Richardsen, A.M. & Burke, R.J. (2007). Job demands, job resources,

and burnout among police officers. Journal of Criminal Justice, 35, 239-249.

Maslach, C. (1993). Burnout: A multidimensional perspective. In W.B. Schaufeli, C.

Maslach and T. Marek (Eds), Professional burnout. Recent developments in

theory and research (pp. 19-32). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

Maslach, C. (1998). A multidimensional theory of burnout. In C.L. Cooper (Ed.),

Theories of organisational stress (pp. 68-85). New York: Oxford University

Maslach, C. & Goldberg, J. (1998). Prevention of burnout: New perspectives. Applied &

Preventive Psychology, 7, 63-74.

Maslach, C., Shaufeli, W.B. & Leiter, M.P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of

Psychology, 52, 397-422.

Matthews, G., Zeidner, M. & Roberts, R.D. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Science &

myth. London: MIT.

Mattila, A., Ahola, K., Hankonen, T., Salminen, J., Huhtala, H. & Joukamau, M. (2007).

Alexithymia and occupation burnout are strongly associated in working

population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 62, 657-665.

Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K.V., Rieffe, C. & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence,

psychological well-being and peer-rated social competence in adolescence.

British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 263-275.

Mayer, J.D., Perkins, D.M., Caruso, D.R. & Salovey, P. (2001). Emotional intelligence

and giftedness. Roeper Review, 23, 131-138.

Page 118: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 108 -

Mayer, J.D. & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey and

D.J. Sluyter (Eds), Emotional development and emotional intelligence:

Educational implications. (pp. 3-31). New York: Basic Books.

Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D.R. & Sitarenios, G. (2001). Emotional intelligence

as a standard intelligence, Emotion, 1, 232-242.

McCrae, R.R. & Costa, P.T., Jr. (1989). Reinterpreting the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator

from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality. Journal of

Personality, 57, 17-40

Meyerhoff, M.K. (2007). Perspectives on parenting. Pediatrics for Parents, 23(5), 8-9.

Meyerson, D.E. (1994). Interpretations of Stress in Institutions: The Cultural Production

of Ambiguity and Burnout. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39, 628-653.

Middeldorp, C.M., Cath, D.C. & Boomsma, D.I. (2006). A twin-family study of the

association between employment, burnout and anxious depression. Journal of

Affective Disorders. 90, 163-169.

Mikolajczak, M. & Luminet, O. (2008). Trait emotional intelligence and the cognitive

appraisal of stressful events: An exploratory study. Personality and Individual

Differences, 44, 1445-1453.

Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., Leroy, C. & Roy, E. (2007). Psychometric properties of the

trait emotional intelligence questionnaire: Factor structure, reliability, construct

and incremental validity in a French-speaking population. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 88, 338-353.

Mikolajczak, M., Menil, C. & Luminet, O. (2007). Explaining the protective effect of trait

emotional intelligence regarding occupational stress: Exploration of emotional

labour processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1107-1117.

Page 119: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 109 -

Mikolajczak, M., Nelis, D., Hansenne, M. & Quoidbach, J. (2008). If you can regulate

sadness, you can probably regulate shame: Associations between trait emotional

intelligence, emotion regulation and coping efficiency across discrete emotions.

Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1356-1368.

Mikolajczak, M., Roy, E., Luminet, O., Fillee, C. & de Timary, P. (2007). The moderating

impact of emotional intelligence on free cortisol responses to

stress.Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 1000-1012.

Miner, M.H. (2007). Burnout in the first year of ministry: Personality and belief style as

important predictors. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 10, 17-29.

Montgomery, A., Mostert, K. & Jackson, L. (2005). Burnout and health of primary school

educators in the North West Province. South African Journal of Education, 25,

266-272.

Morgan, B. (2009). The relationship between the big five personality traits and burnout

in South African university students. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of

Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa.

Mostert, K. & Joubert, A.F. (2005). Job stress, burnout and coping strategies in the

South African Police Service. South African Journal of Economic

andManagement Sciences, 8, 39-53.

Mostert, K., Pienaar, J., Gauche, C. & Jackson, L.T.B. (2007). Burnout and engagement

in university students: A psychometric analysis of the MBI-SS and UWES-S.

South African Journal of Higher Education, 21, 147-162.

Muldary, T.W. (1983). Burnout and health professionals: Manifestations and

management. California: Capistrano.

Murphy, A. (2006). A comparison of the emotional intelligence and thinking styles of

Page 120: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 110 -

students in different university study fields. Unpublished master’s thesis,

University of South Africa, Pretoria, South Africa.

Nelis, D., Quoidbach, J., Mikolajczak, M. & Hansenne, M. (2009). Increasing emotional

intelligence: (How) is it possible? Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 36-

41.

Neumann, Y., Finaly-Neumann, E. & Reichel, A. (1990). Determinants and

consequences of students’ burnout in universities. Journal of Higher Education,

61, 20-31.

Newsome, S., Day, A.L. & Catano, V.M. (2000). Assessing the predictive validity of

emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1005-1016.

Nunnally, J.C. (1975). Introduction to statistics for psychology and education. New York:

McGraw-Hill.

Nunnally, J.C. & Bernstein, I.H. (1994) Psychometric theory (3rd ed). New York:

McGraw- Hill.

O’Connor, R.M. & Little, I.S. (2004). Revisiting the predictive validity of emotional

intelligence: self-report versus ability-based measures. Personality and Individual

Differences, 35, 1893-1902.

Ohman, A. (2006). Making sense of emotion: Evolution, reason and the brain.

Daedalus, 135(3), 33-45.

Ozan, M.B. (2009). A study on primary schoolteacher burnout levels: The northern

Cyprus case. Education, 129, 692-703.

Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS survival manual ( 3rd ed.). Berkshire: Open University.

Page 121: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 111 -

Palmer, B., Donaldson, C. & Stough, C. (2002). Emotional intelligence and life

satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1091-1100.

Parker, J.D.A., Hogan, M.J., Eastabrook, J.M., Oke, A. & Wood, L.M. (2006). Emotional

intelligence and student retention: Predicting the successful transition from high

school to university. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1329-1336.

Parker, J.D.A., Summerfeld, L.J., Hogan, M.J. & Majeski, S.A. (2004). Emotional

intelligence and academic success: Examining the transition from high school to

university. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 163-172.

Perrewe, P.L., Hochwater, W.A., Rossi, A.M., Wallace, A., Maignan, I., Castro, S.L., et

al. (2002). Are work stress relationships universal: A nine region examination of

role stressors, general self-efficacy and burnout. Journal of International

Management, 8, 163-187.

Petrides, K.V. (2006). Deriving Factor Scores from the TEIQue-SF. Webnote #2.

Retrieved July, 15, 2009, http://www.ioe.ac.uk/schools/phd/kpetrides/teique1.htm.

Petrides, K.V., Frederickson, N. & Furnham, A. (2004). The role of trait emotional

intelligence in academic performance and deviant behaviour at school.

Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 277-293.

Petrides, K.V. & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric

investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journalof

Personality, 15, 425-448.

Petrides, K.V. & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioural validation

in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. European

Journal of Personality, 17, 39-57.

Petrides, K.V. & Furnham, A. (2006). The role of trait emotional intelligence in a gender-

Page 122: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 112 -

specific model of organisational variables. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,

36, 552-569.

Petrides, K.V., Furnham, A. & Frederickson, N. (2004). Emotional intelligence. The

Psychologist, 17, 574-577.

Petrides, K.V., Niven, L. & Mouskounti, T. (2006). The trait emotional intelligence of

ballet dancers and musicians. Psicothema, 18, 101-107.

Petrides, K.V., Pe´rez-Gonza´lez, J.C. & Furnham, A. (2007). On the criterion and

incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 26-

55.

Petrides, K.V., Pita, R. & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional

intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 273-

289.

Petrides, K.V. & Sevdalis, N. (2009). Emotional intelligence and Nursing: Comment on

Bulmer-Smith, Profetto-McGrath and Cummings. International Journal of Nursing

Studies, 46, 1624-1636.

Pfeiffer, S.I. (2001). Emotional intelligence: Popular but elusive construct. Roeper

Review, 23, 138-142.

Pienaar, J. & Sieberhagen, C. (2005). Burnout and engagement of student leaders in a

higher education institution. South African Journal of Higher Education, 19,155-

167.

Pienaar, J. & Willemse, S.A. (2007) Burnout, engagement, coping and general health of

service employees in the hospitality industry. Tourism Management, 29, 1053-

1063.

Page 123: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 113 -

Pines, A.M. (2002). The changing psychological contract at work and employee

burnout. Journal of Health and Human Services administration, 25(1), 11-32.

Pines, A.M. & Aronson, E. (1988). Career burnout: Causes and cures. New York: The

Free Press.

Pines, A.M., Aronson, E. & Kafry, D. (1981). Burnout. New York: The Free Press.

Pines, A.M. & Keinan, G. (2005). Stress and burnout: the significant difference.

Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 625-635.

Ramesar, S., Koortzen, P. & Oosthuizen, R.M. (2009). The relationship between

emotional intelligence and stress management. South African Journal of

Industrial Psychology, 35(1), 1-10.

Ried, L.D., Motycka, C., Mobley, C. & Meldrum, M. (2006). Comparing self reported

burnout of pharmacy students on the founding campus with those at distance

campuses. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 70(5), 1-12.

Rogers, P., Qualter, P., Phelps, G. & Gardner, K. (2006). Belief in the paranormal,

coping and emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences,

41,1089-1105.

Ross, S., Cleland, J. & Macleod, M.J. (2006). Stress, debt and undergraduate medical

student performance. Medical Education, 40, 584-589.

Rothmann, S. (2003). Burnout and engagement: A South African perspective. South

African Journal of Industrial Psychology, 29(4), 16-25.

Rothmann, S. (2004). Burnout, psychological strengths and coping strategies of senior

managers in a manufacturing organisation. Management Dynamics, 13(4), 26-36.

Page 124: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 114 -

Rothmann, S. & Essenko, N. (2007). Job characteristics, optimism, burnout and ill

health of support staff in a higher education institution in South Africa. South

African Journal of Psychology, 37, 135-152.

Rothmann, S. & Joubert, J.H.M. (2007). Job demands, job resources, burnout and work

engagement of managers at a platinum mine in the North West Province. South

African Journal of Business Management, 38(3), 49-61.

Saklofske, D.H., Austin, E.J., Galloway, J. & Davidson, K. (2007). Individual correlates

of health-related behaviours: Preliminary evidence for links between emotional

intelligence and coping. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 491-502.

Saklofske, D.H., Austin, E.J. & Minski, P.S. (2003). Factor structure and validity of a trait

emotional intelligence measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 707-

721.

Salmela-Aro, K., Naatanen, P. & Nurmi, J.E. (2004). The role of work-related personal

projects during two burnout interventions: A longitudinal study. Work and Stress,

18, 208-230.

Schaufeli, W.B. & Buunk, B.P. (2003). Burnout: An overview of 25 years of research

and theorizing. In M. Schabracq, J.A.K. Winnubst and C.C. Cooper (Eds), The

handbook of work and health psychology (2nd ed, pp. 383-428). Chichester: John

Wiley.

Schaufeli, W. & Enzmann, D. (1998). The burnout companion to study & practice: A

critical analysis. London: Taylor & Francis.

Schaufeli, W.B., Martinez, I.A., Pinto, A.M., Salanova, M. & Bakker, A.B. (2002).

Burnout and engagement in university students. A cross-national study. Journal

of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 33, 464-481.

Page 125: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 115 -

Schaufeli, W.B., Maslach, C. & Marek, T. (Eds) (1993). Professional burnout: Recent

developments in theory and research. New York: Taylor & Francis.

Schmitz, N., Neumann, W. & Oppermann, R. (2000). Stress, burnout and locus of

control in German nurses. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 37, 95-99.

Schutte, N.S., Malouff, J.M., Bobik, C., Caston, T.D., Greeson, C. & Jedlicka, C. (2001).

Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. Journal of SocialPsychology,

141, 523-536.

Sevdalis, N., Petrides, K.V. & Harvey, N. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence and

decision-related emotions. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1347-1358.

Shih, F-J., Turale, S., Lin, Y-S., Gau, M.L., Kao, C-C., Yang, C-Y. et al., (2009).

Surviving a life threatening crisis: Taiwan’s nurse leaders’ reflections and

difficulties fighting the SARS epidemic. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 18, 3391-3400

Shulman, T.E. & Hemenover, S.H. (2006). Is dispositional emotional intelligence

synonymous with personality? Self and Identity, 5, 147-171.

Skaalvik, E.M. & Skaalvik, S. (2010). Teacher self-efficacy and teacher burnout: A study

of relations. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26, 1059-1069.

Slaski, M. & Cartwright, S. (2003). Emotional intelligence training and its implications for

stress, health and performance. Stress and Health, 19, 233-239.

Smith, L., Heaven, P.C.L. & Ciarrochi, J. (2008). Trait emotional intelligence, conflict

communication patterns, and relationship satisfaction. Personality and Individual

Differences, 44, 1314-1325.

Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of intelligence and the principles of cognition.

London: Macmillan.

Page 126: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 116 -

Spence, G., Oades, L.G. & Caputi, P. (2004). Trait emotional intelligence and goal self-

integration: important predictors of emotional well-being? Personality and

Individual Differences, 37, 449-461.

Stein, S.J. & Book, H.E. (2006). The EQ edge: Emotional intelligence and your success.

Mississauga: Wiley.

Stern, W. (1912). The psychological methods of intelligence testing. Retrieved July 29,

2010, from http://psychology.wikia.com/wiki/William_Stern.

Sternberg, R.J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge:

Cambridge University.

Sternberg, R.J. (1994). Encyclopaedia of human intelligence. New York: MacMillan.

Sternberg, R.J. & Grigorenko, E.L. (Eds.). (2002). The general factor of intelligence:

How general is it? Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Storm, K. (2001). The relationship between personality dimensions, coping and burnout

of pharmacists in a corporate pharmacy group. Unpublished master’s thesis,

Potchefstroom, South Africa.

Storm, K. & Rothmann, S. (2003a). A psychometric analysis of the Maslach Burnout

Inventory-General Survey in the South African Police Service. South African

Journal of Psychology, 33, 219-226.

Storm, K. & Rothmann, S. (2003b). The relationship between burnout, personality traits

and coping strategies in a corporate pharmaceutical group. South African Journal

of Industrial Psychology, 29(4), 35-42.

Strassle, C.G., McKee, E.A. & Plant, D.D. (1999). Optimism as an indicator of

psychological health: Using psychological assessment wisely. Journal of

Page 127: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 117 -

Personality Assessment, 72, 191-201.

Struthers, C.W., Perry, R.P. & Menec, V.H. (2000). An examination of the relationship

among academic stress, coping, motivation, and performance in college.

Research in Higher Education, 41, 581-592.

Swider, B.W. & Zimmerman, R.D. (2010). Born to burnout: A meta-analytic path model

of personality, job burnout, and work outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior,

76, 487-506.

Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics. (3rd ed.). Northridge:

Harper Collins.

Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). NewYork:

Harper Collins.

Tapia, M. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence. Psychological Reports, 88, 353-

364.

Taris, T.W., Schreur, P.J.G. & Van Iersel-Van Silfhout, I.J. (2001). Job stress, job strain,

and psychological withdrawal among Dutch university staff: towards a dual

process model for the effects of occupational stress. Work and Stress, 15, 283-

296.

Thompson, D. (Ed.) (1998). Oxford dictionary of current English. Oxford: Oxford

University.

Toscano, P. & Ponterdolph, M. (1998). The personality to buffer burnout: What makes

some nurses more resistant to daily stressors in critical care? Nursing

Management, 29(8), 32L-32R.

Van der Merwe, M. (2003). Burnout, engagement, coping and locus of control of

Page 128: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 118 -

postgraduate students. Unpublished masters thesis, North West University,

Potchefstroom, South Africa.

Van Dick, R. & Wagner, U. (2001). Stress and strain in teaching: A structural equation

approach. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 243-259.

Van Rooy, D.L., Viswesvaran, C. & Pluta, P. (2005). An evaluation of construct validity:

What is this thing called emotional intelligence. Human Performance, 18, 445-

462.

Warwick, J. & Nettelback, T. (2004). Emotional intelligence is..? Personality and

Individual Differences, 37, 1091-1100.

Watson, D. & Clark, L.A. (1997). Extroversion and its positive emotional core. In R.

Hogan, J. Johnson and S.R. Briggs (Eds), Handbook of personality psychology

(pp. 767-793). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Watson, R., Deary, I., Thompson, D. & Li, G. (2008). The study of stress and burnout in

nursing students in Hong Kong. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 45,

1534-1542.

Weckwerth, A.C. & Flynn, D. (2006). Effect of sex on perceived support and burnout in

university students. College Student Journal, 40, 237-250.

Wiese, C., Rothmann, S. & Storm, K. (2003). Coping, stress and burnout in the South

African Police Service in Kwazulu-Natal. South African Journal of Industrial

Psychology, 29(4), 71-80.

Willcock, S.M., Daly, M.G., Tennant, C.C. & Allard, B.J. (2004). Burnout and psychiatric

morbidity in new medical graduates. Medical Journal of Australia, 181, 357-360.

Woitaszewski, S. A. & Aalsma, M.C. (2004). Social and emotional intelligence: The

Page 129: Relaţia dintre IE şi epuizarea profesională ȋn rândul studentilor postuniversitari -  Teză master 2010.pdf

- 119 -

contribution of emotional to the social and academic success of gifted

adolescence as measured by the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale-

Adolescent Version. Roeper Review, 27, 25-30.

Yang, H-J. (2004). Factors affecting student burnout and academic achievement in

multiple enrolment programs in Taiwan’s technical-vocational colleges.

International Journal of Educational development, 24, 283-301.

Yu, S. (2005). Burnout in higher education “two-course” teachers and some suggested

approaches to the problem. Chinese Education and Society, 38(6), 53-6

Zellers, K.L., Perrewe, P. & Hochwarter, W.A. (2000). Burnout in health care: The role

of the five factors of personality. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1570-

1598.