university of nevada, reno | school of medicine | school of … · 2020. 10. 2. ·...

1
One of the greatest obstacles that maletofemale (MTF) transgender individuals face in being perceived as female is their voice. The voice is one of the few changes that transgender MTFs cannot change with surgery or hormones. The best option to achieve the perception of a more feminine voice is through receiving voice therapy. Introduc)on Methods PICO Question The purpose of this study was to determine whether raising fundamental frequency in combination with other speech strategies (I) results in the perception of a more feminine voice (O) in maleto female transgender individuals (P) than when fundamental frequency is the only strategy utilized (C). Search Terms:: Transgender voice therapy, transgender speech, and transgender feminine. Databases: PubMed and ScienceDirect; thirty articles were found. Validity: Critical Appraisal of Treatment Evidence (CATE form) was used to appraise validity and clinical signiJicance for ten articles. Reliability: Four articles were chosen to make an evidence based practice decision and each study had innterater reliability of at least 85%. A review of therapy techniques to feminize the voice of male-to-female transgender individuals Selah Sullivan, B.A. & Abbie Olszewski, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Nevada, Reno I am a speech language pathology graduate student at the University of Nevada, Reno and I anticipate that I will work with clients who are transgender in the future. I have learned from previous coursework that transgender voice clients are more likely to be male tofemale because estrogen supplements do not have an effect on the biologically male larynx. The most common type of voice therapy focuses on raising fundamental frequency, however many transgender maletofemale individuals have reported that their voice is still perceived as male post therapy. I would like to know what are the most effective voice therapy technique(s) for increasing the perception of a feminine voice. Discussion Borsel, J., Janssens, J., & De Bodt , M. (2009). Breathiness as a feminine voice characteris=c: A perceptual approach. Journal of Voice, 23(3), 291294. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.08.002 Carew, L., Dacakis, G., & Oates, J. (2007). The effec=veness of oral resonance therapy on the percep=on of femininity if voice in maletofemale transsexuals. Journal of Voice , 21(5), 591603. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.05.005 Gelfer, M., & BenneV, Q. (2012). Speaking fundamental frequency and vowel formant frequencies: Effects on percep=on of gender. Journal of Voice , 27(5), 556566. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.11.00 Hancock, A., Colton, L., & Douglas, F. (2013). Intona=on and Gender Percep=on: Applica=ons for Transgender Speakers. Journal of Voice, 28, 203209. doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.08.009 A multitherapy approach to voice therapy for male tofemale transgender individuals may be more effective in achieving a more feminine voice than raising fundamental frequency alone. Although all tests showed increased perception of feminine voice the MTF voice was still distinguishable from the biologically female voice. More research is needed, and more therapy techniques need to be developed, as no one therapy approach or combination of approaches has been found to successfully achieve the perception of a female voice at this point in time. Purpose Clinical Scenario References Authors & Research Design Validity & Importance Purpose Participants Results Borsel, Janssens, & De Bodt (2009) QuasiExperimental Compelling To investigate the effect that breathiness, as a quality of speech, has on the perception of a feminine voice in transgender maletofemales as compared to raising fundamental frequency alone. N = 12 Age: 2160 (mean 27.7) Biologically Female Experiment 1: The breathy samples always received higher femininity scores than their corresponding normal samples. Statistical analysis using Wilcoxon signed rank test showed that the difference was signiJicant (z =2.371; p = 0.018) Experiment 2: For each speaker the number of judges considering the breathy sample more feminine than the normal sample was higher than the number of judges having the opposite opinion. A sign test showed the difference was signiJicant (p = 0.016) Carew, Dacakis, & Oates (2007) QuasiExperimental Suggestive To determine if oral resonance therapy may be an effective treatment increasing femininity of voice in maletofemale transgender clients. N = 10 Age: 2564 (mean 40) Transgender maleto female All participants saw gains in vowel formant frequency posttreatment (F1: p = .02, F2: p = 0.01, FF: p = 0.013) Gelfer, & Bennett (2012) Multivalent Experimental Study Suggestive To investigate the contribution of vowel formant frequencies to gender identiJication in connected speech, the distinctiveness of vowel formants in males versus females, and how ambiguous speaking fundamental frequencies (SFFs) and vowel formants might affect perception of gender. N = 30 Age: 2035 Biologically Male (N=15) Biologically Female (N=15) The correlation between the perception of gender and each vowel formant was statistically signiJicant (p = 0.0038) Correlation coefJicients (r) varied between 0.569 and 0.836. The higher the percentage of “perceived male” judgments, the lower the average formant frequency of /i/ and /a/. Hancock & Helenius (2011) QuasiExperimental Compelling To determine how intonation relates to gender perception of transgender speakers in connected speech. N = 44 Age: Not speciJied Biologically Male: (N=12) Biologically Female: (N=12) Transgender maleto female: (N=14) Transgender femaleto male: (N=6) KruskalWallis test revealed statistically signiJicant differences in femininity ratings across the four gender groups, χ2 (4, n = 44) = 34.208, p = 0.000. The female group had the highest median score (Md = 870), followed by MTF (Md = 446), FTM (Md = 225), and male groups (Md = 59). MannWhitney U post hoc tests with Bonferroni adjustment (alpha = .008) show that the femininity rating was signiJicantly higher for the females compared with the males (U = 0.000, Z = −4.157, p = 0.000), FTMs (U = 0.000, Z = −3.372, p = 0.001), and MTFs (U = 5.5, Z = −4.038, p = 0.000). The males were also signiJicantly lower than the MTF (U = 5, Z = −4.063, p = 0.000) but not different from the FTMs (U = 15, Z = −1.967, p = 0.049). The FTM group was not statistically different from the MTF group (U = 19, Z = −1.897, p = 0.058). Results

Upload: others

Post on 18-Nov-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: University of Nevada, Reno | School of Medicine | School of … · 2020. 10. 2. · One$of$the$greatest$obstacles$that$maletofemale$(MTF)$ transgender$individuals$face$in$being$perceived$as$female$

One  of  the  greatest  obstacles  that  male-­‐to-­‐female  (MTF)  transgender  individuals  face  in  being  perceived  as  female  is  their  voice.  The  voice  is  one  of  the  few  changes  that  transgender  MTFs  cannot  change  with  surgery  or  hormones.  The  best  option  to  achieve  the  perception  of  a  more  feminine  voice  is  through  receiving  voice  therapy.  

Introduc)on   Methods  

PICO  Question    The  purpose  of  this  study  was  to  determine  whether    raising  fundamental  frequency  in  combination  with  other  speech  strategies  (I)  results  in  the  perception  of  a  more  feminine  voice  (O)  in  male-­‐to  female  transgender  individuals  (P)  than  when  fundamental  frequency  is  the  only  strategy  utilized  (C).  

•  Search  Terms::    Transgender  voice  therapy,  transgender  speech,  and  transgender  feminine.    

•  Databases:  PubMed  and  ScienceDirect;  thirty  articles  were  found.    

•  Validity:  Critical  Appraisal  of  Treatment  Evidence  (CATE  form)  was  used  to  appraise  validity  and  clinical  signiJicance  for  ten  articles.  

•  Reliability:  Four  articles  were  chosen  to  make  an  evidence  based  practice  decision  and  each  study  had  innterater  reliability  of  at  least  85%.  

A review of therapy techniques to feminize the voice of male-to-female transgender individuals

Selah Sullivan, B.A. & Abbie Olszewski, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Nevada, Reno

•  I  am  a  speech  language  pathology  graduate  student  at  the  University  of  Nevada,  Reno  and  I  anticipate  that  I  will  work  with  clients  who  are  transgender  in  the  future.  

•  I  have  learned  from  previous  coursework  that  transgender  voice  clients  are  more  likely  to  be  male-­‐to-­‐female  because  estrogen  supplements  do  not  have  an  effect  on  the  biologically  male  larynx.    

•  The  most  common  type  of  voice  therapy  focuses  on  raising  fundamental  frequency,  however  many  transgender  male-­‐to-­‐female  individuals  have  reported  that  their  voice  is  still  perceived  as  male  post  therapy.  

•  I  would  like  to  know  what  are  the  most  effective  voice  therapy  technique(s)  for  increasing  the  perception    of  a  feminine  voice.  

Discussion  

Borsel,  J.,  Janssens,  J.,  &  De  Bodt  ,  M.  (2009).  Breathiness  as  a  feminine  voice  characteris=c:    A                              perceptual  approach.  Journal  of  Voice,  23(3),  291-­‐294.  doi:  10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.08.002  Carew,  L.,  Dacakis,  G.,  &  Oates,  J.  (2007).  The  effec=veness  of  oral  resonance  therapy  on  the                          percep=on  of  femininity  if  voice  in  male-­‐to-­‐female  transsexuals.  Journal  of                                Voice  ,  21(5),  591-­‐603.  doi:  10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.05.005  Gelfer,  M.,  &  BenneV,  Q.  (2012).  Speaking  fundamental  frequency  and  vowel  formant                          frequencies:  Effects  on  percep=on  of  gender.  Journal  of  Voice  ,  27(5),  556-­‐566.  doi:                                  10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.11.00  Hancock,  A.,  Colton,  L.,  &  Douglas,  F.  (2013).  Intona=on  and  Gender  Percep=on:  Applica=ons  for                          Transgender  Speakers.  Journal  of  Voice,  28,  203-­‐209.  doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.08.009      

   

•  A  multi-­‐therapy  approach  to  voice  therapy  for  male-­‐to-­‐female  transgender  individuals  may  be  more  effective  in  achieving  a  more  feminine  voice  than  raising  fundamental  frequency  alone.    

•  Although  all  tests  showed  increased  perception  of  feminine  voice  the  MTF  voice  was  still  distinguishable  from  the  biologically  female  voice.  

•  More  research  is  needed,  and  more  therapy  techniques  need  to  be  developed,  as  no  one  therapy  approach  or  combination  of  approaches  has  been  found  to  successfully  achieve  the  perception  of  a  female  voice  at  this  point  in  time.      

Purpose  

Clinical  Scenario    

References    

Authors  &  Research  Design  

Validity  &  Importance  

Purpose   Participants   Results  

 Borsel,    Janssens,  

 &  De  Bodt    (2009)  

       

Quasi-­‐Experimental    

           

Compelling  

To  investigate  the  effect  that  breathiness,  as  a  quality  of  speech,  has  on  the  perception  of  a  feminine  voice  in  transgender  male-­‐to-­‐females  as  compared  to  raising  fundamental  frequency  alone.  

N    =  12      Age:  21-­‐60  (mean  27.7)        Biologically  Female        

•  Experiment  1:  The  breathy  samples  always  received  higher  femininity  scores  than  their  corresponding  normal  samples.  Statistical  analysis  using  Wilcoxon  signed  rank  test  showed  that  the  difference  was  signiJicant                          (z  =-­‐2.371;  p  =  0.018)    

 

•  Experiment  2:  For  each  speaker  the  number  of  judges  considering  the  breathy  sample  more  feminine  than  the  normal  sample  was  higher  than  the  number  of  judges  having  the  opposite  opinion.  A  sign  test  showed  the  difference  was  signiJicant  (p  =  0.016)  

   

Carew,  Dacakis,    &  Oates  (2007)  

   Quasi-­‐Experimental    

           

Suggestive    

To  determine  if  oral  resonance  therapy  may  be  an  effective  treatment  increasing  femininity  of  voice  in  male-­‐to-­‐female  transgender  clients.    

N  =  10    Age:  25-­‐64  (mean  40)    Transgender  male-­‐to-­‐female      

•  All  participants  saw  gains  in  vowel  formant  frequency  post-­‐treatment              (F1:  p  =  .02,  F2:  p  =  0.01,  FF:  p  =  0.013)  

 Gelfer,  &    Bennett  (2012)  

   Multivalent  

Experimental  Study  

           

Suggestive  

To  investigate  the  contribution  of  vowel  formant  frequencies  to  gender  identiJication  in  connected  speech,  the  distinctiveness  of  vowel  formants  in  males  versus  females,  and  how  ambiguous  speaking  fundamental  frequencies  (SFFs)  and  vowel  formants  might  affect  perception  of  gender.  

N  =  30  Age:  20-­‐35      Biologically  Male  (N=15)    Biologically  Female  (N=15)        

•  The  correlation  between  the  perception  of  gender  and  each  vowel  formant  was  statistically  signiJicant  (p  =  0.0038)    

 •  Correlation  coefJicients  (r)  varied  between  -­‐0.569  and  -­‐0.836.    •  The  higher  the  percentage  of  “perceived  male”  judgments,  the  lower  the  average  formant  frequency  of  /i/  and  /a/.        

 Hancock  &    Helenius    (2011)  

   Quasi-­‐Experimental    

           

Compelling  

 To  determine  how  intonation  relates  to  gender  perception  of  transgender  speakers  in  connected  speech.  

N  =  44    Age:  Not  speciJied        Biologically  Male:  (N=12)    

Biologically  Female:  (N=12)    

Transgender  male-­‐to-­‐female:  (N=14)    

Transgender  female-­‐to-­‐male:  (N=6)        

•  Kruskal-­‐Wallis  test  revealed  statistically  signiJicant  differences  in  femininity  ratings  across  the  four  gender  groups,  χ2  (4,  n  =  44)  =  34.208,  p  =  0.000.  The  female  group  had  the  highest  median  score  (Md  =  870),  followed  by  MTF  (Md  =  446),  FTM  (Md  =  225),  and  male  groups  (Md  =  59).  

•  Mann-­‐Whitney  U  post  hoc  tests  with  Bonferroni  adjustment  (alpha  =                          .008)  show  that  the  femininity  rating  was  signiJicantly  higher  for  the              females  compared  with  the  males  (U  =  0.000,  Z  =  −4.157,  p  =  0.000),              FTMs  (U  =  0.000,  Z  =  −3.372,  p  =  0.001),  and  MTFs  (U  =  5.5,  Z  =  −4.038,  p              =  0.000).    •  The  males  were  also  signiJicantly  lower  than  the  MTF  (U  =  5,  Z  =  −4.063,  p  =  0.000)  but  not  different  from  the  FTMs  (U  =  15,  Z  =  −1.967,  p  =  0.049).  

•  The  FTM  group  was  not  statistically  different  from  the  MTF  group  (U  =  19,  Z  =  −1.897,  p  =  0.058).  

Results