rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in mapudungun · mapudungun and the mapuche mapudungun is the...

30
RHYTHMIC VS. DEMARCATIONAL STRESS IN MAPUDUNGUN Benjamin Molineaux THE UNIVERSITY OF EDINBURGH 24th Manchester Phonology Meeting 26-28 May 2016 – The University of Manchester Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Upload: others

Post on 24-Apr-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

RHYTHMIC VS. DEMARCATIONAL STRESS IN

MAPUDUNGUN

Benjamin Molineaux

THE UNIVERSITY OF EDINBURGH

24th Manchester Phonology Meeting26-28 May 2016 – The University of Manchester

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 2: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

What I will be doing

Telling you about Mapudungun , aka ‘Araucanian’ and howthe literature has got its stress system wrongProviding new data on Mapudungun stressClaiming Mapudungun’s stress system is not purelyphonological, but is actually conditioned by morphologyComparing Mapudungun to other languages that seem tobe ‘less phonological’ in their stress assignmentDrawing attention to an underexplored function of stresssystems, namely demarcation, and its relation tomorpho-syntactic structure

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 3: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Introduction – what is stress for?

Contemporary (generative) theories focus on structuralproperties of stress placement:

phonological goalbased on phonological unitsparametericalternation: rhythm; hierarchical organisation:culminativity

Some interaction with morphology is acknowledged, but isseen as external to the system itself (cf. Hayes 1995: 32).Earlier theories (Prague School – Grade, 1967, Martinet,1964) focus on stress’ functional properties.

morphological goalparsing words and phrases demarcational role

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 4: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Mapudungun and the Mapuche

Mapudungun is the ancestral tongueof the Mapuche people

Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zuniga2007)Argentina: c. 8,400 (INEC, 2005)

It is considered endangered, due topoor transmissionMonolingualism is vanishingly rareMost speakers are elderly and live intraditional, rural communitiesIt is presumed to be an isolatePolysynthetic, agglutinating andhead-marking

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 5: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

The literature on Mapudungun stress

Mapudungun-specific literature presents stress as trochaic,quantity sensitive and right-left (cf. Valdivia 1606; Febres1765; Lenz 1895-1897; Augusta 1903; Salas 2006; Zuniga2006; Sadowsky et al. 2013)

Right-edge stress (from Salas, 1976, 2006):a. [wa.Ni."len] ‘star’ b. [we.jul.-k1."le-j] ‘swim-PROG-IND.3’c. [ma."wi.Ta] ‘woodland’ d. [le.li.-"fi.-m-i] ’watch-INV.3SP.IND-2-S’

None of these studies is specifically focused on stressNo phonetic data or formal analysis available

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 6: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

The literature on Mapudungun stress

Typological studies take Mapudungun to be iambic,quantity insensitive and left-right (cf. Hyman 1977; Kager1993; Hayes 1995; Gordon 2002; Tesar 2004;Martınez-Paricio & Kager 2015)All based on Echeverrıa & Contreras (1965)

Presumed Quantity Insensitive Iamba. [wu."le] b. [úùi."pan.to] c. [e."lu.-mu.-­j-u]

tomorrow year give-INV.2-IND.1-D

‘tomorrow’ ‘year’ ‘you give us (both)’

d. [e."lu.-a-.­e-.n-ew] f. [ki."mu.-fa.­lu.-wu-.­la-j]give-FUT-INV-1-3 know-SIM-RFX-NEG-IND.3‘s/he will give me x’ ‘s/he (her/himself) pretended not to know’

Paul de Lacy (2014) has thoroughly critiqued this evidence

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 7: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

The literature on Mapudungun stress

Language-specific and typological approach differ in allparameters (e.g. Salas vs. Martınez-Paricio & Kager)!

FOOT WEIGHT DIRECTION ITERATION

LANG-SPECIFIC Trochaic Sensitive Right-Left NoTYPOLOGICAL Iambic Insensitive Left-Right Yes

We might need a fresh look...

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 8: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

New Data

Gathered near Cholchol, in Chile’s Araucanıa RegionSeven native speakers interviewedWords recorded in context and isolationNative intuitions elicited

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 9: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Acoustics and perception of stress

Acoustic analysis of stress cues (in monomorphemes):duration, intensity and pitch maxima were analysed.only F0 significantly related to stress (Molineaux 2014)

Additional study on native and non-native stress perception(Molineaux 2016)

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 10: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Stress patterns: morphologically simplex words

Mono-, di- and trisyllabic nouns:a. ["f1n”] ‘seed’ b. ["we] ‘young’c. [n”a."m1n”] ‘foot’ d. ["ma.pu] ∼ [ma."pu] ‘land’e. [l”af."ken”] ‘sea’ f. ["piw.ke] ∼ [piw."ke] ‘heart’g. [wa.Ni."len] ‘star’ h. [ma."wi.Ta] ‘woodland’i. [a.tSuL."peñ] ‘floating ash’ j. [puñ."pu.ja] ‘armpit’

Final closed syllables are stressed (a, c, e, g, i)Otherwise, the penult (h,j) or the sole syllable (b) isstressedSounds like a right-aligned moraic trochee!

([µµ]); ([µµ] µ); ([µ] µ)

But: Vowel-final disyllables alternate stress position (d, f)No evidence for secondary stress

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 11: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Stress patterns: multi-suffix verbs

Complex words may have two stressesStress falls on:

word-final (ω) moraic trochee ([µµ]); ([µµ] µ); ([µ] µ)stem-final (s) syllable

No clash:a. [[úùe."ka.]s-ja."w-a-j]ω b. [[1."úù1f.]s-tu.-pu.-ke."la-j.-m-i]ω

walk-AMB-FUT-IND.3 throw-REST-TRLOC-HAB-NEG-IND-2-S

‘s/he will walk around’ ‘You don’t usually throw x back here’c. [["lef.]s-pu."le-j]ω d. [[úùi."pa.]s-ke."la-n.-m-i]ω

run-TRSLOC-PROG-IND.3 exit-HABIT-NEG-IND-1S

‘s/he is running here’ ‘I don’t usually go out’

No clear word-level stress hierarchy (no culminativity)

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 12: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Stress patterns: multi-suffix verbs

Clash:

a. [[a.mu]s-"la-j.-m-i]ω b. [[le.li.]s-"fi.-m-i]ωgo-NEG-IND-2-S look-DIR.3SP-IND-2-S

‘You didn’t go’ ‘you looked at him/her/it’c. [[e.lu-ñ"ma.]s-fi-j.-m-i]ω d. [l”a."N-1m]s-fi-j]ω

give-APPL-3.OBJ-IND-2-S die-CAUSE-3SP-IND.3‘You give him/her/it x for y’ ‘s/he killed him/her/it’

In most cases, stem stress is demoted, and only the ω-finaltrochee is stressed (a, b)‘Extended’ stems (c, d), take stress, while the ω-final stressis lost

Extended stems have a valency-changing suffix such as:-Ne ‘PASS’; -ñma ‘APPl’; -(l)el ‘APPL’; -(1)m ‘CAUSE’; (1)l‘CAUSE’

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 13: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Stress patterns: Nominal compounds

Stress is on the final syllable of the first root, and on thefinal moraic trochee of the second

No Clash:

a. [tSa."fo]D-[ku."úùan]H b. [tSa."NuL]H-[n”a."mun”]D‘cough-disease’(a cold) ‘finger-foot’(toe)

In clash, the head of the compound retains stressHead (H) and dependant (D) roots bracketed

Clash:

a. [ku.Ti]D-["fo.ro]H b. [fo."ro]H-[tSaL.wa]D‘morter-bone’(spine) ‘bone-fish’(fishbone)

c. [we.nu]D-["ma.pu]H d. [i."lo-]H-[úùe.wa]D‘morter-bone’(spine) ’meat-dog’ (dog-meat)

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 14: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Accounting for previous accounts

Echeverrıa & Contreras (1965) and the typologists:Focus on the first morpheme, usually a disyllableInitial stress (stem-stress) seems quantity insensitiveNO-CLASH means at least one syllable intervenes betweenstem- and word-stress:

Language-specific literatureFocuses on the right-edge, trochaicAllows for a ‘two-syllable stress window’ on left edge of verbSalas (2006); Zuniga (2006)

Both analyses overlook the morphology

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 15: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Phonology of Mapudungun stress

Stress refers to prosodic units:morae (weight), feet, PRWDS

NOCLASH plays a role at the morpheme boundaryPossibly a rhythmic constraint

But,Native speakers have no intuitions as to stress hierarchy

culminativity is not definitional (at the PRWD-level)

Stress is signalled by pitch alone, not lengtheningNo evidence for vocalic reduction in unstressed position(Sadowsky et al. 2013)No stress-based phonotactic asymmetries (Salas 2006;Zuniga 2006)No attested stress-based processes in Mapudungun’ssynchronic or diachronic phonology (Molineaux 2014)

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 16: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Stress and the morphology

Barring clash, stress is a reliable cue for the stem edgeIn clash, it signals compound heads, and valency changesIt signals the word’s right edge as coextensive with a foot

Stress-based demarcation helps disambiguate Mapudungunstems among abundant, highly agglutinating morphology

[[ke."Lu.]s-pu.-tu.-ke.-"fu-n]help-TRLOC-REST-HABIT-BI-IND.1S

‘I used to go back there to help’

Rhythm (clash avoidance) is subordinateto the morphology

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 17: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Stress and the morphology II

Paucity of stress-based phonological asymmetries isadvantageous to parsing of agglutinative morphology:

a. [Tu."Nu.-ke.-"la.-j.-m-i] ‘speak-HABIT-NEG-IND-2-S’b. [Tu."Nu.-ke.-"le.-j.-m-i] ‘speak-HABIT-PROG-IND-2-S’c. [Tu."Nu.-ke.-la.-"j-i-ñ] ‘speak-HABIT-NEG-IND-1-P’d. [Tu."Nu.-ke.-le.-"j-i-ñ] ‘speak-HABIT-PROG-IND-1-P’

Productive agglutinating morphology means the targetmorpheme for stress changes dynamicallyComputing enhancement and reductions online couldcreate processing difficulties

*[Tu."[email protected]"j-i-ñ] ‘speak-HABIT-???-IND-1-P’

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 18: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Mapudungun and stress typology

Mapudungun phonology seems to ‘care’ very little about stressAccording to (Hyman 2014: 59):

’Languages which exploit metrical structure for multiplepurposes... will exhibit the kind of “metrical coherence”found in Germanic (Dresher & Lahiri 1991) . . . Languagessuch as Hungarian or Turkish . . . seem different becausetheir metrical structure has little or no relevance outside thestress system itself. The contrast with English, whosephonology cares so much about stress, is quite striking.’

The morphology of Mapudungun does appear to ‘care’about stressWhat about languages like Hungarian and Turkish?

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 19: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Does Hungarian phonology care about stress?

Hungarian main stress is word-initialSecondary stress is...

a quantity sensitive feature: (Szinnyei 1912)a LR syllabic trochee: Kerek (1971); Varga (2002)in alternation with tertiary stress: Hammond (1987)

Blaho & Szeredi (2011) and Vogel et al. (in press) find nophonetic evidence for (impressionistic) secondary stressF0 cues primary stress, but is weak outside focus positionPhonological correlates to stress are conspicuously absent(Kalman & Nadasdy 1994; Blaho & Szeredi 2011)

“this putative rhythmic intensity alternation is phonologicallyirrelevant as it does not interact in any way with the rest ofthe phonology”(Siptar & Tokenczy 2000: 22)

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 20: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Does Hungarian morphology care about stress?

Hungarian is predominantly suffixing (Kenesei et al. 1998):so main stress does not interact with morphologyException: some compounds with stress on the firstsyllable of second element (Varga 2012)

utott-kopott ‘beaten-worn (battered)’tizen-egy ‘one-on-ten (eleven)’

Functionally, Hungarian stress demarcates the word levelvery clearly, and occasionally also the structure ofcompounds as well

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 21: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Does Turkish care about stress?

Default stress is claimed to be on a word-final syllableIts cueing is extremely subtle (F0) (Levi 2005)May be epiphenomenal (boundary tone?) (Vogel et al. inpress)predictability of the pattern may result in a degree ofdeafness to it (Domahs et al. 2013)

Nevertheless, the default prominence seems to have aword-demarcative function (Kabak & Vogel 2001)Non-final stress is lexically specified, relating to borrowednouns, pre-stressed or stressed suffixesCues for these lexical stresses are more robust (Levi 2005;Vogel et al. in press)There is no evidence for secondary stress overallNeither final nor non-final stress show any broaderphonological effects

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 22: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Conclusions: General

In Mapudungun, Hungarian and Turkish, stress has littlestructural value (rhythm, phonology)The three languages, however, show a clear functional rolefor stress (demarcation, morphology)They all signal default stress via F0 only, with littlephonological involvementEvidence for rhythmic, secondary stress is scanty if notaltogether absentThe three languages are also highly agglutinating

Morphemes don’t have a pre-established prosodicstructure/positionStress-based asymmetries would make morphologicalparsing sub-optimal

More typological work needed to assess the relationbetween function/acoustics of stress and morphologicalagglutination/fusion

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 23: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

Conclusions: Mapudungun

The default stress pattern for Mapudungun seems to be aword-level right-aligned trocheeA second main stress marks the right edge of the firstmorphemePrevious accounts fail to consider the role of morphology instress-assignmentEvidence for rhythmic, secondary stress is lackingaltogetherLack of major stress-based phonological asymmetriesconspires to maintain agglutinating morphologytransparentDemarcation is a valuable feature of stress which in thiscase trumps rhythm and culminativity.

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 24: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Selected references I

Augusta, Felix Jose (1903) Gramatica Araucana. Valdivia: Imprenta Central J. Lampert.

Blaho, Sylvia & Daniel Szeredi (2011) (The non-existence of) secondary stress in Hungarian. In Approaches toHungarian: Volume 12: Papers from the 2009 Debrecen conference, Tibor Laczko & Catherine O. Ringen, eds.,Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 39–62.

de Lacy, Paul (2014) Evidence for stress systems. In Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues, Harryvan der Hulst, ed., Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, in press.

Domahs, Ulrike, Safiye Genc, Johannes Knaus, Richard Wiese, & Barıs Kabak (2013) Processing (un-)predictableword stress: ERP evidence from Turkish. Language and Cognitive Processes 28(3): 335–354, URLhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.634590.

Dresher, Elan B. & Aditi Lahiri (1991) The Germanic Foot: Metrical coherence in Old English. Linguistic Inquiry22(2): 251–286.

Echeverrıa, Sergio Max & Heles Contreras (1965) Araucanian Phonemics. International Journal of AmericanLinguistics 31(2): 132–135.

Febres, Andres (1765) Arte de la Lengua General del Reyno de Chile. Lima.

Gordon, Matthew (2002) A factorial typology of quantity insensitive stress. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory20: 491–552.

Hayes, Bruce (1995) Metrical Stress Theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Hyman, Larry (1977) On the nature of linguistic stress. In Studies in Stress and Accent, vol. SCOPIL, Larry Hyman,ed., University of Southern California, 37–82.

Hyman, Larry (2014) Do All Languages Have Word Accent? In Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues,Harry van der Hulst, ed., Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

INEC: Instituto Nacional de Estadısticas y Censos (2005) Complementaria de Pueblos Indıgenas URLhttp://www.indec.mecon.ar/webcenso/ECPI/pueblos/Datos/0700c0302.xls.

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 25: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Selected references II

Kabak, Baris & Irene Vogel (2001) The phonological word and stress assignment in Turkish. Phonology 18:315–360.

Kager, Rene (1993) Alternatives to the Iambic-Trochaic Law. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 11: 381–432.

Kalman, Lazslo & Adam Nadasdy (1994) A hangsuly [stress]. In Strukturdlis magyar nyelvtan 2: Fonologia, F. Kiefer,ed., Budapest: Akademiai Kiado, 393–467.

Kenesei, Istvan, Robert Michael Vago, & Anna Fenyvesi (1998) Hungarian. London: Routledge.

Kerek, Andrew (1971) Hungarian metric: Some linguistic aspects of iambic verse. The Hague: Mouton.

Lenz, Rodolfo (1895-1897) Estudios Araucanos, vol. XCVII. Santiago: Anales de la Universidad de Chile.

Levi, Susannah (2005) Acoustic correlates of lexical accent in Turkish. Journal of the International PhoneticAssociation 35: 73–97.

Martınez-Paricio, Violeta & Rene Kager (2015) The binary-to-ternary rhythmic continuum in stress typology: layeredfeet and non-intervention constraints. Phonology 32(03): 459–504.

Molineaux, Benjamin J. (2014) Sychronic and Diachronic Morphoprosody: Evidence from Mapudungun and EarlyEnglish. Ph.D. thesis, The University of Oxford, Oxford.

Molineaux, Benjamin J. (2016) Native and Non-native Perception of Stress in Mapudungun: Assessing StructuralMaintenance in the Phonology of an Endangered Language. Language and Speech : online first.

Sadowsky, Scott, Hector Paniqueo, Gaston Salamanca, & Heriberto Avelino (2013) Mapudungun. Journal of theInternational Phonetic Association: Illustration of the IPA 43(1): 87–96.

Salas, Adalberto (2006) El Mapuche o Araucano. Santiago: Centro de Estudios Publicos (CEP).

Siptar, Peter & Miklos Tokenczy (2000) The Phonology of Hungarian. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Szinnyei, Joseph (1912) Ungarische Sprachlehre. Berlin: G. J. Goschen.

Tesar, Bruce (2004) Using inconsistency detection to overcome structural ambiguity in language learning. LinguisticInquiry 35(2): 219–253.

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 26: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Selected references III

Valdivia, Luis de (1606) Arte, y Gramatica General de la Lengua que Corre en Todo el Reyno de Chile, con unVocabulario y Confessionario. Seville: Thomas Lopez de Haro.

Varga, Laszlo (2012) Rhythmical Variation in Hungarian revisited. In Twenty Years of Theoretical Linguistics inBudapest, Ferenc Kiefer & Zoltan Banreti, eds., Budapest: Tinta Konyvkiado, 161–182.

Varga, Lazlo (2002) Intonation and stress : evidence from Hungarian. London: Palgrave MacMillan.

Vogel, Irene, Angeliki Athanasopuolou, & Nadya Pincus (in press) Prominece, Contrast and the Functional LoadHypothesis: an acoustic investigation. In Accent and Stress, Rob Goedemans, Jefferey Heinz, & Harry van derHulst, eds., Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Zuniga, Fernando (2006) Mapudungun: El habla mapuche. Santiago: Centro de Estudios Publicos.

Zuniga, Fernando (2007) Mapudunguwelaymi am? ‘¿Acaso ya no hablas mapudungun?’ Acerca del estado actualde la lengua mapuche. Estudios Publicos : 9–24.

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 27: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Thank You!

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 28: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Stress or accent?

Is it even stress? Could it just be a pitch-accent system?F0 is the key cue for Mapudungun stressHere we follow Hyman (2009) in characterizingMapudungun within a property-driven prosodic typology.Mapudungun prominence is obligatory (i.e., every lexicalword must have at least one stressed syllable)It is clearly assigned at the level of the output lexical word,and not at the input morpheme level.These two key traits place Mapudungun firmly within thespectrum of stressed languages.

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 29: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Disyllables, again

But what about those pesky vowel-final disyllables?It’s really only the nouns that alternate

N: ["ru.ka] ∼ [ru."ka] ‘house’Other word categories stress a final open syllable

Adj: [f1."úùa] ‘old/large’; [pi."tSi] ‘young/small’Adv: [we."lu] ‘tomorrow’, [pe."tu] ‘still/yet’

Adjs. and Advs. appear mostly as first elements in aphrase, since Mapudungun tends to pre-specify:

cf. [f1."úùa ma."wi.Ta]φ ‘old wodland’cf. [pe."tu k1."pa-j]φ ‘s/he is still coming’

In isolation they behave like nouns: [f1."úùa]∼["f1.úùa]Nouns don’t alternate within larger PRWDS (compounds)Adj.+N and Adv.+V look a lot like N+N compoundsPhrasal and word levels are somewhat blurred here

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun

Page 30: Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun · Mapudungun and the Mapuche Mapudungun is the ancestral tongue of the Mapuche people Chile: c.144,000 speakers (Zu´niga˜ 2007)

References

Stress patterns: multi-suffix verbs

Conflation:

a. [["pe]s-j]ω b. [[je-ñ."ma]s-j]ωsee-IND.3 carry-APPL-IND.3‘s/he sees’ ‘s/he watches x’

Patterns of stem- and word-level stress interaction (n=282)

Structure Pattern n Percentagea. [[(σ) σ]S σ1 σ(σ)]ω No interaction 114 (40.4%)b. [[(σ) σ]S(σ)]ω Conflation 68 (24.1%)c. [[(σ) σ]S σ(σ)]ω STEM de-stress 52 (18.4%)d. [[(σ) σ]S σ(σ) ]ω WORD de-stress 38 (13.5%)e. [[(σ) σ]S σ(σ)]ω Clash tolerated 10 (3.5%)

Stem-level stress faithful: 81.6%Word-level stress faithful: 86.5%

Benjamin Molineaux Rhythmic vs. demarcational stress in Mapudungun