using a problem-solving approach in the primary grades

5
Using a Problem-solving Approach in the Primary Grades Author(s): Patricia F. Campbell Source: The Arithmetic Teacher, Vol. 32, No. 4 (December 1984), pp. 11-14 Published by: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/41194008 . Accessed: 15/06/2014 19:02 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Arithmetic Teacher. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 91.229.229.162 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 19:02:25 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: patricia-f-campbell

Post on 16-Jan-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Using a Problem-solving Approach in the Primary GradesAuthor(s): Patricia F. CampbellSource: The Arithmetic Teacher, Vol. 32, No. 4 (December 1984), pp. 11-14Published by: National Council of Teachers of MathematicsStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/41194008 .

Accessed: 15/06/2014 19:02

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extendaccess to The Arithmetic Teacher.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.162 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 19:02:25 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Using a Problem-solving Approach

in the Primary Grades By Patricia F. Campbell

According to the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1980), the focus of school mathematics in the 1980s must be on problem solving. Furthermore, computation is to be a tool for problem solving. The impor- tance of problem solving as a goal in mathematics education cannot be dis- puted; however, the de-emphasis of computation may cause feelings of uneasiness for many primary-level teachers. These feelings can be accen- tuated by such statements as "Pri- mary-level curricula contain practical- ly no mathematical problem-solving experiences" (Greenes 1981). Where does this dilemma leave the typical primary-level teacher, given the exist- ing primary mathematics curriculum and the demands from parents and school administrators that young chil- dren develop a mastery of addition and subtraction?

The source of this anxiety over a focus on problem solving may be the lack of a universally accepted defini- tion of problem solving. Krulik and Rudnick (1980) define problem solv- ing as "a process . . . the means by which an individual uses previously acquired knowledge, skills, and un- derstanding to satisfy the demands of an unfamiliar situation" (p. 4). During problem solving, the student must take what has been learned and apply it to a new situation. If the student knows a procedure that can guarantee

Patricia Campbell teaches mathematics educa- tion and computer education courses at the University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. She is currently doing research on the learning of mathematics by elementary school- children.

a solution and if this technique can be easily applied, then it could be said that there was "no problem." How- ever, if this skill is still being acquired, then deciding what process to use and how to use it may be quite a problem (see Heller and Greeno 1978).

It is often tacitly assumed that chil- dren must acquire computational

Students must comprehend the problem, then conceptualize it in a general setting.

skills before they can apply them to the solution of problems. However, preschool children invent strategies for solving mathematical problems in- dependent of instruction (Ginsburg 1977; Groen and Resnick 1977). This behavior suggests that computational skills are not prerequisites for prob- lem solving. Consider the so-called simple, one-step story problem. For example:

Three children were playing in a sandbox. Two more children came and played with them. How many children were in the sandbox?

Primary teachers may be able to use this problem setting as a means of developing an understanding of addi- tion rather than using addition as a means of modeling the problem, as typically done in the middle grades. Rather than emphasizing only the ac- quisition of computational skill for its own sake, teachers may take advan- tage of the young child's problem- solving strategies. In this way, addi- tion and subtraction have meaning

from the beginning and are not simply part of a complex scheme involving the writing of numerals and the state- ment of facts.

This approach is conducive to the development of an understanding of the operations of addition and sub- traction as well as practice with the computational skills involved. Recent research (Carpenter, Hiebert, and Moser 1981) supplies evidence sup- porting this method. At the same time, if these one-step problems are used for problem solving (where stu- dents need to understand the problem and plan for a solution), then they may serve as a foundation for devel- oping skills needed to solve nonrou- tine problems (Carpenter et al. 1980).

What Influences Young Children's Interpretation and Solution of Problems? Before using a problem as the setting for developing or clarifying the mean- ing of addition or subtraction, we should consider what influence the structure of the problem and its mode of presentation may have on the stu- dent's ability to solve it. Generally, teachers should consider the structure of the problem, the use of manipula- tives or- diagrams to represent the problem, and the choice of pictures or oral statements to communicate the problem setting.

Carpenter, Hiebert, and Moser (1981) classified the structure of addi- tion and subtraction problems as join- ing/separating, part-part-whole, com- parison, and equalizing. The joining/

December 1984 И

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.162 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 19:02:25 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

separating problem has an initial set and an action (either direct or implied) that causes a change in the quantity of the initial set. The part-part- whole class consists of problems in which two amounts are considered indepen- dently or as part of a whole and action does not take place. Comparison problems involve the comparison of two amounts. Equalizing problems in- volve comparison, with an implied action on one quantity.

The data collected by Carpenter, Hiebert, and Moser indicate that first- grade children can recognize different problem situations and use that recog- nition to determine what solution strategy to apply to a given problem.

With respect to the presentation of the problem, research offers some guidelines that teachers should keep in mind. An oral problem with no

We can foster computational skills and problem-solving skills in the same setting.

manipulative is, as expected, the most difficult for young children to under- stand. An oral problem with concur- rent physical movement of objects to depict the action in the problem gen- erally prompts the greatest number of correct responses (Ibarra and Lind-

it has been suggested that approach- ing the one-step problems in a prob- lem-solving setting may provide a base for the growth of skills in solving nonroutine problems as the student matures. Research has been cited to support the feasibility of such an ap- proach as well as to indicate the varia- bles within problems that seem to influence young children's percep- tion. How can all these factors be combined for effective instruction?

Initially the student must under- stand the problem. This comprehen- sion may be indicated by the child's ability to tell the story of the problem in a paraphrased form when asked, "Now you tell me the story." All that

Fig. 1 Two sketches to describe the same problem

The children in their study did not attempt to use or rely on one solution strategy; rather, their strategy seemed related to problem type. Because the mathematics curriculum of these stu- dents had not yet introduced formal instruction in symbolic representa- tions of addition and subtraction, they relied on their natural problem-solv- ing skills, including counting, and their experience in using objects to represent problem settings. The high level of success evidenced by these students supports the use of problem settings as a means of developing ad- dition and subtraction concepts rather than simply as a means of applying previously learned algorithms.

vail 1979). When pictures depict prob- lems rather than oral statements, Campbell (1981) noted a progression in the young child's interpretation of the pictures from set recognition to additional recognition of motion to perception of mathematical relation- ships between the sets.

Problem Solving in the Primary Grades The premise outlined thus far has been that primary-level children may gain an understanding of the concepts of addition and subtraction as well as practice in computational skills through one-step problems. Further,

is required at this stage is that children be able to translate the problem into their own words. To help their com- prehension, students could act out the events or watch them being acted out as the problem is read aloud by the teacher. Instead of reading the entire problem aloud and then expecting the children to enact it, the teacher should probably read only one portion of the problem (e.g., "David, Sarah, and Gregory were drawing pictures on the blackboard") and have that portion acted out before reading the next por- tion ("Then Nancy and Ramon also came up to the blackboard to draw pictures"). The conclusion of the en- actment prompts the question for the

12 Arithmetic Teacher

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.162 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 19:02:25 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

class to answer ("Now how many children are drawing at the black- board?"). At this point the actors may return to their seats and the teacher may ask the other students to tell what happened in their own words.

Next the child must take from the story the essentials of the problem. The student must conceptualize the problem in a general setting. For ex- ample, in the problem above, the child must come to see that "this is a prob- lem where there were some people and more came. So now I have to find how many people there are altogeth- er." Having the students describe, as in a time line, the events of the prob- lem can encourage this conceptualiza- tion. For example, the teacher may ask, "What was the very first thing that happened? Then what? . . ."The children may also be led to see the structure of the problem. For exam- ple, five other children could enact this problem. There could be four girls and one boy (rather than two girls and three boys). They could draw differ- ent pictures on the blackboard, but the enactment would be similar and the solution would be the same. By discussing which parts of the acted- out problem are useful for answering the question, the children begin to learn what part of the enactment, and hence what elements of the problem, deserve their attention and what infor- mation or actions may be disregarded. In this way the children are encour- aged to see the structure of the prob- lem. Having conceptualized the prob- lem, the students may represent it with fingers, counters, or other ma- nipulatives. Using objects as the mod- el, counting will generally yield a solu- tion. At this point, the students should be encouraged to consider whether the solution makes sense.

The student may then symbolize the general form that has been described (either verbally or with objects) by writing a number sentence and solving it. This activity calls for the student to use numbers to record an event. To emphasize the use of the symbols as a recording device and to aid students in attributing meaning to abstract symbols, the class may practice re- cording each phase of the action as it occurs or recording each part of the

problem as it is read (e.g., "Three children came up to the blackboard, so we can write a 3 to remember that is what happened first"). If children have difficulty at this stage, it may first be necessary to symbolize the problem by using a diagram to repre- sent the story or enactment (see fig. 1). The diagram provides a more con- crete model related to the enactment or manipulatives used previously and, at the same time, introduces numerals to indicate the quantities.

By acting out problems and discussing them, the children can learn a language of communicating about solving problems.

Additional activities to aid symbol- ization may call for another skill. Stu- dents may be presented with an ab- stract number sentence and asked to invent a situation to accompany the symbols. This task requires the chil- dren to create a story or problem to go with the number sentence. In this way the children are encouraged to consid- er the structure of the problem and the meaning of the arithmetic operation. As students compare their problems and see that one number sentence may represent many different events, their conceptualization of the opera- tions of addition and subtraction will be enhanced.

The number sentence is the mathe- matical model of the one-step prob- lem. Lindvall and Ibarra (1979) note that the typical first-grade student does not construct the incomplete number sentence (3 + 2) as a step in the problem solution. Rather, the number sentence is only a way of representing the solution process that has already taken place. The young child cannot determine whether a so- lution makes sense from the number sentence. That determination should be made before the symbolization oc- curs. Using a number sentence to solve a problem does not occur until much later when problems are more complex and the conceptualization of

the operations is already developed. Primary teachers may realize that

they use many of these ideas when they work individually with students who are having difficulty solving a computation or workbook problem. Frequently teachers explain 3 + 4 = by stating, "Suppose you had three candies and I gave you four more. Let's use the counters to see how many candies you would have." The problem-solving approach places this technique in the forefront as a means of developing students' under- standing of the addition of three units and four units rather than using it as an afterthought following the formal- ization of the number sentence 3 + 4 =

Conclusion Can the primary-level teacher take the existing curriculum, develop stu- dents' understanding of arithmetic op- erations, and teach problem solving? Yes, by using a problem-solving ap- proach from the beginning of the in- struction process. This approach does not isolate addition and subtraction concepts from their application in the children's experience. When teachers encourage students to analyze the problem's structure by modeling the essential elements of the problem through physical representation, young children can gain further under- standing of the problem and the mean- ing of arithmetic operations. Symbol- izing the problem and writing the solution fosters their skill in computa- tion. Furthermore, this method may help prevent children from developing the idea that mathematics is a collec- tion of arbitrary rules or meaningless procedures that must be memorized and executed. Although this idea may not be evident at the primary level except in students' emerging depen- dence on the teacher to check the correctness of their answers (Brandau and Easley 1979), it has been verified at the middle school level (Erlwanger 1973; 1974). The problem-solving ap- proach at the primary level will also help students develop the language to communicate about solving problems, a skill that many students lack today (Zweng 1979). In addition, students

December 1984 13

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.162 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 19:02:25 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

will be acquiring a problem-solving style that may serve as the foundation for developing the skills that are need- ed to solve nonroutine problems in the future.

References

Brandau, Linda, and Jack Easley. Understand- ing the Realities of Problem Solving in the Elementary School. Columbus, Ohio: ERIC/ SMEAC, 1979.

Campbell, Patricia F. "What Do Children See in Mathematics Textbook Pictures?" Arith- metic Teacher 28 (January 1981): 12-16.

Carpenter, Thomas P., Mary Kay Corbitt, Hen- ry S. Kepner, Jr., Mary Montgomery Lind- quist, and Robert E. Reys. "Solving Verbal Problems: Results and Implications from Na- tional Assessment." Arithmetic Teacher 28 (September 1980):8-12.

Carpenter, Thomas P., James Hiebert, and James M. Moser. "Problem Structure and First-Grade Children's Initial Solution Pro- cesses for Simple Addition and Subtraction Problems." Journal for Research in Mathe- matics Education 12 (January 1981):27-39.

Erlwanger, Stanley H. "Benny's Conception of Rules and Answers in IPI Mathematics." Journal of Children s Mathematical Behavior 1 (Autumn 1973):7-26.

. "Case Studies of Children's Concep- tions of Mathematics." Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana, 1974.

Ginsburg, Herbert. Children's Arithmetic: The Learning Process. New York: D. Van Nos- trand Co., 1977.

Greenes, Carole E. "Beyond the Textbook." Arithmetic Teacher 28 (March 1981):2.

Groen, Guy, and Lauren B. Resnick. "Can Preschool Children Invent Addition Algo- rithms?" Journal of Educational Psychology 69 (1977):645-52.

Heller, Joan I., and James G. Greeno. "Infor- mation Processing Analyses of Mathematical Problem Solving." Paper presented at the National Science Foundation Seminar on Ap- plied Problem Solving, March 1978, at North- western University.

Ibarra, Cheryl G., and C. Mauritz Lind vail. "An Investigation of Factors Associated with Children's Comprehension of Simple Story Problems Involving Addition and Subtraction Prior to Formal Instruction on These Opera- tions." Paper presented at the 57th Annual Meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, April 1979, Boston.

Krulik, Stephen, and Jesse A. Rudnick. Prob- lem Solving: A Handbook for Teachers. Bos- ton: Allyn & Bacon, 1980.

Lindvall, C. Mauritz, and Cheryl G. Ibarra. The Development of Problem-solving Capabili- ties in Kindergarten and First-Grade Chil- dren. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and De- velopment Center, University of Pittsburgh, 1979.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. An Agenda for Action: Recommendations for School Mathematics of the 1980s. Reston, Va.: The Council, 1980.

Zweng, Marilyn J. "The Problem of Solving Story Problems." Arithmetic Teacher 27 (September 1979):2-3. m

SPECIAL OFFER -$19.95 Basic Operations & computation

Diskette The disk contains six programs: Computation with - 1) Whole Numbers 4) Fractions 2) integers 5) Order of Operations 3) Decimals 6) Estimation

Ch4C™,, TRS-80DOS1.3 Sendcheckor

TRS-80DOS1.3 school purchase Apple DOS 3.3 orderto:

DHS Publications satisfaction P.O. Box 506 Guaranteed Normal, il 61 761

PROFESSIONALDATES- NCTM 63d Annual Meeting

17-20 April 1985, San Antonio, Tex.

NCTM 64th Annual Meeting 2-5 April 1986, Washington, D.C.

NCTM 65th Annual Meeting 8-11 April 1987, Anaheim, Calif.

For a listing of local and regional meetings, contact NCTM, Dept. PD, 1906 Association Dr., Reston, VA 22091, Telephone: 703-620-9840; CompuServe: 75445,1161; The

k Source: STJ228.

' >♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦♦#♦»#»♦»♦<

;: KINDER KONCEPTS '' i » 30 Action-packed programs for ages 4-7 4 ,

* > Concepts covered include more, less, i *

, , matching, order, longest shortest, etc. on 4 ,

' [ math disk and perception, shapes, < < ► sequencing, etc. on reading disk. < *

; ; SENT ON APPROVAL ¡ ] TO SCHOOLS FOR 30 DAYS <

¡ ' Math disk only (15 programs) $65.00

' [

, , Reading disk only (15 programs) $65.00 < >

« > Complete set, all 30, $125.00 Free backup ' '

, , disks included. For Apple П + , He or « »

; ' Commodore PET 16K/32K. ' '

, , (Commodore Business Machines, Inc., is < >

[ handling the Commodore 64 versions.) ' > * • Pitas« add $2.00 for postaga and handling. ;

* ' Other excellant useful software available. | J

' [ VISA and Mastercard accepted. [ I

; MIDWEST SOFTWARE ' ¡ ¡! A DIVISION OF ZERO-ONE, LTD. !> < ■ Box 214 Farmington, Ml 48024 ' >

¡ | Phone (313) 477-0897 ' ¡

14

üH mm m ^Н Mathematics 1

шщ Шт

This content downloaded from 91.229.229.162 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 19:02:25 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions